You are on page 1of 18

Psychological Bulletin

1977, Vol. 84, No. 3, 570-587

Social and Cultural Aspects of Hallucinations


Ihsan Al-Issa
The University of Calgary, Calgary, Canada

A review of clinical and anthropological studies shows that social and cultural
factors affect both the definition of, and the sensory systems involved in, hallu-
cinations. It appears that the pejorative labeling of certain reported experiences
as hallucinatory is dependent on linguistic factors as well as social and cultural
beliefs (e.g., the consideration of hallucinations as an index of social maturity,
social competence, and social conformity). It is pointed out that the degree of
rationality in the culture is associated with attitudes, emotional response, and
the attribution of pathology to hallucinatory experiences. The implications of
social and cultural factors for the behavioral and psychoanalytical approaches
to hallucinations are also discussed.

Recently there has been an increasing in- investigations of stimulus deprivation and
terest in the study of mental imagery in drug-induced hallucinatory experiences.
general and in hallucinations in particular In the study of anomalous perception there
(Keup, 1970; Paivio, 1971; Pylyshyn, 1973; has been much emphasis on the investigation
Richardson, 1969; Segal, 1971a; Siegel & West, of the stimulus situation (e.g., presence or
1975; West, 1962b). Since hallucinations are absence of stimulus in psychophysics) and its
considered to be the result of an abnormal relationship to the mental state of the sub-
interaction of the senses with the physical jects (e.g., psychiatric diagnosis of psychotic
environment (i.e., misperception), they appear states). Textbooks of psychiatry and abnormal
to reflect the biological rather than the social psychology have characteristically defined
adaptation of the organism. It is therefore hallucinations as perception in the absence
not surprising that social and cultural aspects of external stimulation. However, recent
of hallucinations have not been given as much theory and research have rejected the concept
attention as have other clinical symptoms of hallucinations as misperception involving
such as delusion, in which social evaluation a pathological process. Studies have tended
and interpersonal relationships seem to be to postulate organic or psychological mecha-
clearly implicated (Kiev, 1963; Murphy, 1967; nisms common to both hallucinatory expe-
Weinstein, 1962a). The relative neglect of riences and imagery (Dement, 1965; Evarts,
social and cultural aspects of hallucinations 1962; Fischer, 1969; Hebb, 1968; Hefferline,
may well be contrasted with the numerous Bruno, & Camp, 1973; Klttver, 1965; Sarbin
& Juhasz, 1970; Stoyva, 1973; West, 1962a).
The evidence surveyed in this paper shows
This article was part of a research project entitled that the study of the social and cultural
Schizophrenic Symptoms: A Cross-Cultural Approach. situations in which hallucinations occur may
It was supported by The Canada Council, Grant have a substantial effect on the labeling of
No. S71-0171-S1. A shorter version of the article was
prepared for a symposium entitled Cross-Cultural these experiences as pathological. Data from
Factors Relevant to Symptoms, Diagnosis, and Treat- cross-cultural studies as well as from clinical
ment Factors in Promoting Mental Health, part of the and experimental research indicate that social
program of the 18th International Congress of Applied and cultural factors, and particularly atti-
Psychology, Montreal, July 28-August 2, 1974. The
author is grateful to Charles Costello, Ronald Schaub, tudes, affect the definition, frequency, and
David Schonfield, and John Yardley for their com- emotional response to hallucinations. These
ments on the paper. factors also seem to have some bearing on
Requests for reprints should be sent to Ihsan
Al-Issa, Department of Psychology, University of the development and termination of halluci-
Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada. natory experiences.
570
HALLUCINATIONS 571

Social and Cultural Factors in the Definition expected to know not only that the voices
of Hallucinations are in their heads, but also that they are
imaginary and not real. In auditory hallu-
A basic problem in the definition of hal- cinations, on the other hand, the schizophrenic
lucinations is to establish a criterion to dif- patient not only believes that the voices are
ferentiate psychotic or schizophrenic from real, but he also tends to attribute these
other hallucinatory experiences and normal voices to other people. It should be noted,
imagery. In psychiatric circles, much im- however, that when the individual (patient)
portance seems to have been assigned to the expresses some disbelief in the objective
situation, conditions, and circumstances in reality of his perceptual experiences (hearing
which hallucinations are reported. When the voices), they are labelled as pseudo-halluci-
precipitating conditions in which, or the nations (Buss, 1966; Strauss, 1969). These
process by which, the individual comes to criteria seem to be based more on the
have his hallucinations are not detectable, conviction of the individual concerning the
they are considered by the psychiatrist to be reality of his experience, rather than on his
an indication of functional psychosis and/or misperception of external stimuli. Whenever
schizophrenic disorder. Hallucinations with a he is unable to tell the difference between
known physical basis (acute brain syndrome, an image and a percept, he is likely to be
intoxication, fever, etc.) are, on the other considered out of touch with reality or to be
hand, considered to be a manifestation of hallucinating (Sarbin, 1967). Below, I deal
organic psychosis rather than due to unknown with some of the social conditions that might
schizophrenic process. Similarly, whether contribute to the confusion between images
dreams are differentiated from or confused and percepts. Suffice it to say here that
with visual hallucinations depends on whether investigations of the Perky effect show that
or not it is clearly indicated that the visions normal subjects under some conditions are
occurred in a state of wakefulness or sleep. liable to the confusion between vivid imagery
The individual's control of both the occur- and externally generated signals (Perky, 1910;
rence and content of the hallucinatory ex- Segal, 197 Ib).
perience is also used to differentiate waking Little research has been carried out to
imagery, as well as eidetic imagery, from the investigate the reliability and validity of
schizophrenic experience. It is generally be- these criteria (i.e., autonomy and spontaneity
lieved that schizophrenic hallucinations are of hallucinations, and belief in the reality of
spontaneous and beyond the control of the the experience), as applied by psychiatrists
patient. This belief is expected to pose prob- to schizophrenic patients. Strauss (1969), for
lems to the diagnostician, since these charac- example, investigated the frequency of hal-
teristics of hallucinations seem to overlap lucinations and pseudo-hallucinations in a
with those of normal imagery. For instance, group of hospitalized schizophrenic patients.
behavioral research reviewed below shows In this group he found almost three fourths
that hallucinations are not unrelated to in- as many questionable or pseudo-hallucina-
ternal and external cues. Furthermore, studies tions as definite hallucinations. This would
of the imagery of normal subjects show indicate that a large number of diagnosed
that imagery is by no means voluntary or schizophrenics tend to report imagery-reflect-
subject to personal control (Bugelski, 1971; ing experiences that are more ideational
McKellar, 1957; McLemore, 1972; Richard- (thoughts in the head of the patient) than
son, 1969;Stoyva, 1973). perceptual (objects in the outside environ-
The projection of the content of fantasy ment). A cross-national study by Edwards
and imagination and a belief in the reality (1972) has recently reported that American
of the experience by the individual are also psychiatrists seem to be less aware than their
considered as criteria for the determination British counterparts of the difference between
of the abnormality of the experience (Hare, schizophrenic hallucinations, pseudo-halucina-
1973). According to these criteria, normal tions, hypnagogic and hypnopompic imagery,
people may talk to themselves, but they are and other mental imagery. A broad concept
572 IHSAN AL-ISSA

of hallucinations that includes various kinds and imagination. All hallucinatory experiences,
of imagery would be expected to predispose imagery, dissociational states (fugue, hypnotic,
the American psychiatrist to see more hallu- hysterical states), and altered states of con-
cinations in his patients than would his sciousness are attributed to possession by
counterpart in Britain. In an attempt to spirits or to contact between the individual
explain these differences, Edwards considered and a spirit. Possession is considered as a
the possibility that British textbooks of psy- socially sanctioned role that serves public as
chiatry, which stress phenomenology (e.g., well as private needs and is accepted within
Anderson & Threthowan, 1967; Fish, 1962) a certain social context. Though experiences
as well as the influence of the European similar to the spontaneous hallucinations of
phenomenological schools, have led to the Western psychotic and schizophrenic patients
differentiation of more varieties of halluci- do occur in these cultures, the emphasis ap-
natory experiences than in the United States. pears to be on the social meaning of the
Hare (1973) has also recently reported that experience rather than on the conditions and
American textbooks of psychiatry tend to use the process by which hallucinations occur
hallucinations in a broad sense to cover (Bourguignon, 1970, 1973; R. Rabkin, 1970;
hallucinatory experience as well as other kinds Wallace, 1959). Furthermore, the content of
of imagery. these hallucinations tends to be similar from
In addition to British-American differences one individual to another within the tribe,
in the definition of hallucinations, Edwards but to differ between tribes (Benedict, 1934).
(1972) found American psychiatrists to be A question that should be of interest to
less aware than British psychiatrists of the Western investigators concerns the social and
occurrence of hallucinations in conditions cultural conditions that may affect the degree
other than schizophrenia (psychogenic, hys- to which those who are in a state of posses-
terical states, depression). In the rank order sion, and suffer from psychiatric disorder in
of importance of symptoms in the diagnosis the Western sense, derive social benefits from
of schizophrenia, American psychiatrists entering these states. There is some evidence
ranked hallucinations higher than did British to show that urban African patients who
psychiatrists. The orientation of psychiatrists report possession are still diagnosed as men-
in the United States and Britain toward tally ill, for example, hospitalized cases in
hallucinations and other symptoms may have, Nigeria described by A. H. Leighton, Lambo,
in part, brought about diagnostic differ- Hughes, J. M. Leighton, and Macklin (1963).
ences between the two countries, biasing the There are also certain conditions in which
Americans toward the diagnosis of schizo- some non-Western societies differentiate hal-
phrenia (Cooper, Kendell, Gurland, Sharpe, lucinations as part of a socially accepted state
Copeland, & Simon, 1972; Sandifer, Horden, of possession from those which are considered
Timbury, & Green, 1968). It may be con- pathological. Bourguignon (1970), for in-
cluded that British-American differences in stance, reported that among the Mohave
the definition of hallucinations, as well as in Indians, there is a taboo against the hunter
diagnosis, may not only reflect differences in eating his kill. A hunter who does eat his
training (Cooper et al., 1972; Edwards, 1972; kill tends to have hallucinations: He sees
Hare, 1973) but also in social beliefs about game everywhere. This is considered a type
these experiences, which will be discussed of pathology brought about by the infraction
below. of culturally accepted rules and seems to be
clearly linked to culturally derived concepts
Non-Western Definition of Hallucinations of guilt.
The tendency of some non-Western socie-
In contrast to the psychiatric definition of ties to ignore differences among hallucinations
hallucinations, many non-Western societies on the basis of precipitating conditions does
make no distinction between spontaneous not imply an inability to discriminate between
schizophrenic hallucinations, pseudo-halluci- hallucinations and veridical perception. It may
nations, dreams, and other products of fantasy be argued that it is their awareness of the
HALLUCINATIONS 573

difference between these two experiences The major emphasis would not be on the
(interaction with the physical environment in presence or absence of certain symptoms in
veridical perception versus interaction with various social and cultural groups (e.g., Al-
the spirits in hallucinations) that makes Issa, 1970; Wittkower & Rin, 1965), but on
hallucinations of distinctive value to these the study of beliefs, feelings, and behavior
societies. As will be noted in the next section, (behavior intentions, action tendencies) rele-
certain social and cultural attitudes might vant to these symptoms. The relevance of
increase the tendency of the individual to this approach to the study of hallucinations
give a reality status to his visual and auditory is that it indicates a need to explore the
images and thus to become pathological. "subjective culture" of the patient, the psy-
chiatrist, and representative samples of the
Attitudes Toward Hallucinations community (Triandis, Vassiliou, Vassiliou,
Tanaka, & Shanmugam, 1972).
In psychiatric practice it is implicitly as- In two important papers, Sarbin (1967)
sumed that hallucinations, as well as other and Sarbin & Juhasz (1967) have pointed out
symptomatology, are directly related to speci- that the labeling of reported experiences as
fic diagnostic categories. Psychiatric interest hallucinatory is influenced by both the beliefs
in the frequency of hallucinations seems to be and verbal sophistication of the diagnostician
motivated mainly by the belief that these and of the patient. The study of these two
symptoms are related to psychosis. Recent factors seems necessary for the understanding
evidence, however, is inconsistent with this of the process underlying the diagnosis of
belief. Zigler and Phillips (1961), for instance, hallucinations as pathological behavior.
found that among hospital patients, halluci-
nations occur in only 35% of schizophrenics Linguistic Factors
and to a lesser extent in patients suffering
from character disorders (12%), manic de- The first step in the process of labeling is
pressive reaction (11%), and psychoneurosis the inspection of the verbalization of the
(4%). More recently, McKellar (1968) has individual (e.g., I see . . ., I hear . . ., etc.).
cited two studies of normal subjects in whom Before the diagnostician can label the ex-
the occurrence of hallucinations, in the psy- perience as hallucinatory, he must decide
chiatric sense, was 10% and 25%, respec- whether the patient's statements are true or
tively.1 It thus appears that psychotic symp- false. If the diagnostician declares these state-
toms such as hallucinations not only occur ments false and therefore hallucinatory, many
at a low rate in the hospital population, but linguistic factors seem to affect this decision.
are also found in different diagnostic groups There is, for instance, the possibility that the
and in the general population. This tenuous statement may contain ellipses and that the
relationship between symptoms and mental individual is only foreshortening his utter-
illness has been highlighted by field studies ances. Indeed, the linguistic sophistication of
showing that psychiatric symptoms occur in the suspected hallucinator seems to play a
such large numbers in the general population dominant part in influencing the decision of
that it is difficult to accept them as the sole the diagnostician. His public report is, for
criterion for defining the true pathological example, unlikely to be declared false or1 con-
population (D. C. Leigh ton, Harding, Macklin, sidered hallucinatory if he includes in his
MacMillan, & A. H. Leighton, 1963; Srole, statements such qualifiers as "it is as if I
Langner, Michael, Opler, & Rennie, 1962).
The evidence seems to indicate that in the
1
study of factors in the diagnosis of mental If hallucinations are to be considered pathological,
illness, it is necessary to go beyond symptoms it is necessary to obtain data on base rates in the
general population. This work would parallel recent
to the study of social conditions and social investigations of base rates of hallucinations under
attitudes that may be crucial in determining hypnosis and waking states (Barber, 1970; Barber
whether or not these symptoms are patho- & Calverley, 1964; Bowers, 1967; Sarbin & Andersen,
logical (Draguns, Note 1; Hammer, 1972). 1963; Spanos & Barber, 1968).
574 IHSAN AL-ISSA

hear . . ., I thought I saw . . . ." That this Patently, one does not report all his imaginary ex-
linguistic problem interacts with the social periences to all possible auditors. People are selective
in what they report and to whom. And if a person
context is expressed by Sarbin (1967): inhibited the public reporting of all his imaginings,
there would be little basis for a judge to use the label
A special problem exists for the diagnostician when
hallucination.
the suspect lacks language sophistication and cannot
(3) What are the psychological and social criteria
indicate his ellipses with appropriate qualifiers. This
that lead a professional to declare that certain reported
is especially true of children, adults with limited
imaginings are hallucinations and others are not?
language skills, persons with meager vocabularies such
(p. 363)
as recent immigrants, etc. Expressing the concept of
as if requires a fair degree of linguistic skill. Thus In a series of papers, Sarbin (Sarbin, 1967;
when a four-year-old child insists on talking to an
imaginary playmate, the as if is supplied by the
Sarbin & Juhasz, 1967, 1970) has extensively
mother and the child is not likely to be referred to dealt with the first two questions. The next
a therapist for help in stamping out hallucinations. part of the present article deals with research
When reading the productions of great poets, the relevant to the third question posed by
readers supply the suppressed as if. In order to inter- Sarbin, about the social conditions associated
pret any sentence properly, the listener must consider
the total context, i.e., he must be ready to supply with hallucinations.
missing qualifiers, (p. 371)
Social and Cultural Beliefs
Another linguistic problem raised by Sarbin
(1967) is the use of metaphor. If a report Earlier in my discussion of hallucinations,
such as "I see . . ." is interpreted literally I have pointed out cross-cultural as well as
where it is metaphorically used, the indi- cross-national (American versus British) dif-
vidual's statement is automatically judged ferences in beliefs relating to hallucinations.
false. This situation may arise not only when I discuss here some of the social correlates
the listener mistakes the metaphorical intent of psychiatric beliefs and the reaction of the
of the speaker, but also when the latter is individual and his group to the report of
ignorant of "current norms regarding language subjective hallucinatory experience.
constraints" to be used when describing certain One aspect of psychiatric beliefs about
experiences (Sarbin & Juhasz, 1970, p. 66). hallucinations is the social implication and
Finally, when an individual states "I see relevance of hallucinatory experiences to the
an elephant," there are at least two ways social functioning of the individual. Phillips,
of reacting to this statement (Sarbin, 1967). Broverman, and Zigler (1966) have shown
The listener may concentrate either on the that hallucinations, more than other symp-
phenomenological reality of the reported toms, appear to reflect lower levels of social
object, "elephant," or on the predicate, competence and/or social maturity of pa-
"seeing an elephant." Although the first tients. One finding of particular importance
reaction follows the psychiatric tradition, that to the understanding of the social process
is, to find out whether the experience is involved in diagnosis is that the combination
pathological or not, the other is concerned of low level of social competence and symptom
with the meaning of what the individual is expression (hallucinations) is necessary in de-
trying to communicate by his statement. termining the diagnosis a patient receives
This latter reaction considers hallucinations (schizophrenia). As social competence is de-
to be in the realm of reported imagery, rather fined in terms of level of education, steady
than as misperceptions of the physical en- employment, and marital status, it is not
vironment. As Sarbin (1967) has pointed out, surprising that certain aspects of social com-
the appropriate questions to be asked by the petence are related to social class (Chapman
professional in this situation would be as & Baxter, 1963). Similarly, social competence
follows: as an underlying factor in the judgment of
severity of symptoms and in the diagnosis
(1) What biographical and stimulus conditions lead of patients may explain in part the excess
to covert imaginative behavior? How does the claimed of schizophrenia in the lower class (Hollings-
experience come about? In other words, what is the
background for imagining?
head & Redlich, 1958).
(2) What are the biographical and stimulus condi- The association of hallucinations with
tions that lead to public reporting of imaginings? severe pathology may also reflect the indi-
HALLUCINATIONS 575

vidual's social conformity and his suscepti- tendency toward misperception in the experi-
bility to social influence. Studies using the mental situation. It may be concluded from
Asch procedure in comparing schizophrenic these studies that the investigation of social
and normal subjects may elucidate such an factors such as social competence and social
association, since the experimental situation, conformity will increase understanding of the
in fact, involves social influence in the per- diagnostic process in general and of the rela-
ception of physical stimuli (i.e., length of tionship between hallucinations and psychi-
lines). The evidence is, however, inconsistent atric diagnosis in particular.
in this area of research, showing schizo- The differentiation between hallucinations
phrenics less susceptible to social influence in on the basis of the conditions and the process
some studies, but not in others (Diamond, involved (e.g., psychosis, drug, stimulus de-
1957; Gill, 1963; Schooler & Spohn, 1960; privation) seems to have affected beliefs and
Shean, 1973; Whitman, 1961). Since these expectations about the content of different
studies did not differentiate between halluci- hallucinatory experiences. Although psychotic
nating and nonhallucinating schizophrenics, and schizophrenic hallucinations have been
their data do not clarify the relationship be- expected to be predominantly meaningful in
tween social conformity and hallucinations. content, hallucinations induced by drugs and
If hallucinations reflect a generalized tendency stimulus deprivation have been described as
toward nonconformity to social pressure, hal- meaningless "form constants" (e.g., arcs,
lucinating schizophrenics would be expected circles, wheels, stars, spirals, etc.). Horowitz
to respond to the veridical length of the lines (1964) has, however, put forward the hy-
rather than to conform perceptually in the pothesis that these often-observed, meaning-
Asch situation. less visual experiences could serve as the
Consistent with the hypothesis that hallu- sensory core around which different types of
cinations are related to social conformity is imagery, including schizophrenic hallucina-
a study by Sarbin, Juhasz, and Todd (1971). tions, are elaborated. Horowitz presented evi-
They demonstrated that hallucinating schizo- dence that indicated that when schizophrenics
phrenics tended to "hallucinate" the taste of were carefully questioned and asked to draw
salt in distilled water significantly less often their visual hallucinations, simple forms were
than did college students. Similarly, these so common that they could be used to ex-
patients were less susceptible to hearing a plain the origin of the more elaborated and
tone that was not delivered by the experi- meaningful hallucinations. Summing up the
menter (an auditory hallucination). The same evidence, Richardson (1969) pointed out that
trends were found when they compared hal- the Horowitz hypothesis has some support in
lucinating with nonhallucinating schizo- that it explains the content of other imagery,
phrenics. It should be noted that in both such as hypnagogic imagery, meditation im-
experimental situations, subjects were given agery, pulse-current imagery, perceptual isola-
the strong expectation that they would taste tion imagery, and even everyday experiences
salt or hear a tone. It appears that halluci- of normal subjects. If the Horowitz hypothesis
nating schizophrenics were less likely to "hal- is taken into consideration to explain the
lucinate" when the physical stimulus condi- origin of different hallucinatory experiences,
tions called for a veridical perceptual report then social and cultural factors should play
but the social pressure called for mispercep- a predominant part in the elaboration of the
tion. When subjects in the Sarbin et al. study content of these experiences.2 In the study
were assessed for interest in the social en-
vironment on the basis of scores from the 2
A similar proposition has been put forward by
Motoric-Ideational Activity Preference Scale Segal (1971b) to explain the content of auditory
(Stein, 1968), hallucinating schizophrenics hallucinations, which tend to be common in both
were found to be lower in interest than were schizophrenic and alcoholic withdrawal psychoses.
nonhallucinating schizophrenics and normals. Segal cited the Saravay and Pardes (1967, 1970) hy-
It thus appears that the classification of these pothesis that local effects in the middle ear may
underlie auditory hallucinations in these two groups.
patients is associated with lack of interest in According to Saravay and Pardes, slight shifts in the
the social environment rather than with a states of contraction of the muscles of the middle
576 IHSAN AL-ISSA

of drug effects, psychological reaction to drugs Attitudes and Rationality


(placebo effect) has, of course, been widely
recognized, but no social or cultural controls The rationality of the culture and its dis-
have been used. If social and cultural factors tinction between reality and fantasy3 may
were important in the effects of drugs, one well result in different attitudes toward hal-
would expect wide deviations from the lucinations, and these in turn could affect
meaningless experiences observed in Western both the detection and report of hallucinatory
subjects. This has, in fact, been clearly dem- experiences. Rational cultures that make a
onstrated by anthropological studies of the rigid distinction between reality and fantasy
effects of hallucinogenic drugs on white North tend to consider hallucinations negatively, as
Americans and native Indians. Wallace (1959) they are expected to interfere with daily
has contrasted the auditory and visual dis- activities and interaction with the physical
tortion reported in a clinical research setting environment. Individuals are thus discouraged
with the socially meaningful visions and from assigning credibility to certain imagin-
voices experienced by the Indians who take ings. They even learn to ignore these ex-
drugs in a socially prescribed context. Wallace periences and to remain unaware of their
has also noted that the emotional reaction existence. These cultures would generally be
to the drug experience seems to be different. conducive to a high threshold in the ob-
The calm mood and personal satisfaction of servation of hallucinations and imagery and
the Indian after achieving hallucinations is to a negative attitude toward these experi-
quite the opposite of the mood shifts (agita- ences. The fact that the individual is un-
tion, depression, anxiety, euphoria) experi- familiar with his own imagery and defensively
enced by white subjects under the influence protected against it (i.e., it tends to arouse
of drugs. anxiety in the individual) may make him
Cultural beliefs concerning the origin of attribute a reality status to it when it is
hallucinations, discussed early in this article, occasionally experienced, and he is thus con-
would be predicted to play an important part sidered pathological.4 In a discussion of the
in the social control of both the occurrence adaptive function of the practice of day-
and content of the experience. Considering dreaming, Singer (1966) pointed out that
the schizophrenic hallucinations as random, the constant exploration of one's daydream world
spontaneous, and unpredictable, with un- makes it a familiar realm, less likely to evoke anxiety.
known causation, puts them beyond the con- It remains a distinct possibility, worthy of more
trol of the psychiatrist or the individual
himself. By attributing them to the spirits,
3
social expectation of their content and occur- Categorizing experience into reality and fantasy
rence tends to be prescribed. What the spirits is based on the scientific materialistic criterion used
by psychiatrists to evaluate an individual's report of
are expected to communicate to the individual his private imaginings. It is possible that individuals
may thus be socially stereotyped and could who act as if their imaginings are "real" are using
be predicted by the individual, his group, a different criterion. The question is, whose criterion
and the professionals. The general belief that is one expected to accept? How the criterion defining
external factors rather than individual ones reality changes with time and place has been exten-
sively discussed by Sarbin (1967) and Sarbin and
are responsible for behavior during possession Juhasz (1967).
4
may very well facilitate social influence and Relevant to this point of view is a perceptual
cultural controls (Lefcourt, 1972). isolation study reported by Silverman, Cohen, Shma-
vonian, and Greenberg (1961). They noted that field-
dependent subjects tended to report meaningful visual
imagery to which they gave reality status, whereas
ear may yield sound sources that ordinarily pass none of the field independent subjects reported this
unnoticed but under certain conditions do register in type of imagery. It would be of interest to find out
the brain. Segal (1971b) explained hallucinations with the relationship between social and cultural differences
meaningful content in schizophrenic and alcoholic in cognitive styles (Witkin, 1967; Witkin & Berry,
withdrawal psychoses as originating from these sounds, 1975) and the frequency of the confusion between
which are "modified by central expectancies, past fantasy and reality or the report of meaningful
memories, or current cognitions" (p. 89). hallucinations.
HALLUCINATIONS 577

thorough research exploration, that many persons who valued in relation to individual and group
have failed to indulge in fantasy play may misinter- daily activities, they tend to be frequently
pret vivid daydreams or images as hallucinations and
may be made more anxious by the sudden, seemingly noticed and communicated to others. Although
autonomous occurrence of a fantasy image or phrase the content of hallucinations may have posi-
of conversation, (p. 27) tive or negative connotations, the experience
The rational psychiatric approach to mental itself is not anxiety-arousing or disturbing to
illness may very well explain negative atti- the individual. Indeed, in some primitive soci-
tudes toward schizophrenic hallucinations and eties, the experience is actually sought, and a
the emotional response of the patient to his variety of methods (lack of sleep, fasting,
own experience. It may, for instance, partly prolonged physical pain, social isolation, drugs,
explain the recent finding that hallucinations special exercises, etc.) are used to achieve it
are reciprocally augmented by anxiety; they (Bourguignon, 1970; Wallace, 1959). Cultural
tend to be preceded as well as followed by conditions in such cultures seem to be con-
anxiety (Slade, 1972). Similarly, clinical ob- ducive to positive attitudes and a low
servation of the initial symptoms of anxiety threshold in the perception of hallucinations
and tension, shame, and guilt at the acute and imagery. These conditions may also
stage of mental breakdown may only indicate breed more introspective people than does a
a negative reaction of the patient to his rational culture. It is possible that in these
hallucinations and to other symptoms. It societies the high frequency of reported hal-
might also be observed that negative attitudes lucinations may not be because more of them
may reduce the patient's trust in his group occur, but because they come more often into
and thus make him unwilling to report his the public domain through self-description.
hallucinations. The prime example is the A flexible distinction between reality and
isolated, suspicious paranoid patient, who fantasy may not only encourage the report
seems to be more prevalent in technological and positive regard of hallucinations, it may
and Westernized societies (Al-Issa, 1970; also help absorb these experiences into the
Murphy, 1967). The isolation of the patient cultural stream and thus facilitate their social
and. his attempt to conceal his hallucinatory control. In this cultural context, hallucina-
experiences would be expected to make the tions may start as an individual spontaneous
experiences less amenable to the control of experience, but the duration and termination
social stimuli and perhaps more liable to of the experience may ultimately be brought
chronicity. under the control of social stimuli and be
Holt (1964) has succinctly summed up integrated with other ritualized activities (cf.
Western reaction to imagery and hallucinatory Murphy, 1967). Social control may be postu-
experiences: lated to explain, in part, the transitory nature
of psychotic behavior in general, and in
In a factually oriented, skeptical, anti-intraceptive, particular, hallucinations observed by African
brass-tacks culture like ours, where the para-normal psychiatrists (Collomb, 1965; Lambo, 1965).
is scoffed at and myth and religion are in decline,
the capacity for vivid imagery has little survival (Toxic psychoses and other organic conditions
value and less social acceptability. We live in an age may also explain transitory symptoms in
of literalism, an era that distrusts the imagination, African patients.) Hence, social and cultural
while at the same time it develops its beat fringe of factors would be expected to have certain
avid seekers after drugs that may artificially restore
the capacity for poetic vision. It is little wonder that implications in the therapeutic situation.
our children rapidly lose their eidetic capacity and
that adults are made uneasy by the admission that Therapeutic Implications
they can experience things that are not factually
present, (p. 262)
There are at least three aspects of the
In less rational cultures where the dis- social and cultural approach to hallucinations
tinction between reality and fantasy is more that might have some therapeutic implica-
flexible, individuals are encouraged to observe tions for the psychologist and psychiatrist.
their hallucinations, imagery, and other private First, there is the tendency of the psychiatric
events. Since these experiences are positively approach to give more attention to the ex-
578 IHSAN AL-ISSA

perience of hallucinations per se, with little sented by these authors in support of the
consideration of its meaning and content; association between the report of auditory
second, the tendency of the patient to react hallucinations and electromyographic activity
with anxiety and tension to his hallucina- in the speech musculature of the schizophrenic
tions; and finally, the patient's unwillingness patient suggests that the voices heard in
to reveal his hallucinations and private ex- auditory hallucinations may be the result of
periences. I would like to consider these three the subvocal speech of the patient himself.
aspects in relation to behavioral and psycho- The relevance of these data to the present
analytical approaches to hallucinations. analysis of the concept of hallucinations is
that the patient may sometimes report the
Behavioral Approach occurrence of a mediating event that is con-
sidered to be a response to a nonexistent
Early studies of sensory conditioning have external stimulus (Hefferline et al., 1973).
brought about a basic modification in the In addition to the experimental approach
concept of hallucinations (Ellson, 1941a, to hallucinations, there have been many at-
1941b; Seashore, 1895). Hallucinations are tempts to apply behavioral techniques to
not regarded as a response to nonexistent reduce or eliminate the report of hallucina-
stimuli (cf. Buss, 1966; Sarbin and Juhasz, tions. Systematic desensitization and asser-
1967), but as tiveness training, as originally used by Wolpe
(1958), were applied to deal with anxiety-
a (verbal) report of sensory perception made in the
absence of the adequate external stimulus (UCS) but arousing situations associated with halluci-
in the presence of another external stimulus (CS) nations, that is, being alone or interacting
that in the past has occurred in close temporal con- with family members at home (Slade, 1972),
tiguity to the UCS and probably to its accompanying smoking (Alumbaugh, 1971) and social situ-
report. (Hefferline et al., 1973, p. 311) ations requiring decision making (Nydegger,
This definition, unlike its psychiatric counter- 1972). Results of treatment indicate that a
part, does not distinguish between normal decrease of anxiety level and an increase in
and abnormal hallucinations, but attributes the social adaptation of the patient tend to
them all to the same conditioning process. reduce the report of hallucinations. These
One of its disadvantages, however, is to imply findings are consistent with my early ob-
that the hallucinations of the schizophrenic servation that anxiety-free attitudes toward
are only elicited in the presence of specific hallucinations and other imagery might con-
external stimuli. The reliance of the definition tribute to the speedy termination of these
on external stimulation has been recently cor- experiences in some non-Western cultures
rected by Hefferline and his associates (Hef- (Wallace, 1959).
ferline et al., 1973; Hefferline, Bruno & Da- Aversion therapy has also been used to
vidowitz, 1971; Hefferline & Perera, 1963) by deal with hallucinations. The evidence seems
seeking, within the organism itself, the ante- to show that the effect of self-administered
cedents of hallucinations. They have demon- shock in the treatment of hallucinations is
strated that covert events (proprioceptive either temporary (Bucher & Fabricatore, 1970)
responses) can, in fact, serve as conditioned or tends to be confounded by other factors,
stimuli that can be discriminated by the such as the patient's expectation for im-
individual and can come to elicit hallucinatory provement (Weingaertner, 1971). In the ap-
reports in the absence of external stimuli. plication of aversion therapy, the personal
A step in the same direction are the studies meaning of hallucinations may pose certain
reported by Stoyva (1973) that attempt to problems that are rarely encountered in the
show some of the physiological conditions treatment of neurotic symptoms. Behavior
under which hallucinations are likely to occur. therapists have been interested in the elimi-
These studies relating hallucinations to private nation of maladaptive neurotic behavior (e.g.,
events are not without clinical support (Gould, phobias) and this goal tends to be welcomed
1948, 1950; Inouye and Shimizu, 1970; by both patient and therapist. Schizophrenic
McGuigan, 1966, 1973). The evidence pre- patients may want to get rid of unpleasant
HALLUCINATIONS 579

hallucinations but might resent attempts to fighter that she was in communication with Martin
eliminate those that are positively self-rein- Luther King and other similar notables. To the
student whom she apparently identified as a Nazi
forcing (McKellar, 1957, 1968; Will, 1962). party member, she said she was not really a Negro,
Indeed, hallucinations may reflect evaluative but a Latin and that she hated Negroes for their
standards of self-reinforcement that mirror fifthy ways. She claimed that many of the rallies
the past experiences of the patient (cf. Rockwell conducted had actually resulted from con-
Bandura, 1971). Thus, any attempt to elimi- versations she had had with him in which he sought
her advice. To the girl, her hallucinatory references
nate hallucinations, particularly those with were at first about religious figures, then movie stars.
pleasant content, may be anxiety arousing to (Schaefer & Martin, 1969, pp. 145-146)
the patient. It is instructive to point out
The Schaefer and Martin case is reminiscent
that in the selection of the Weingaertner
of the ceremonial control of hallucinations
(1971) patients, 50% did not cooperate and
wherein the shaman and the audience in-
had to be excluded because they either denied
fluence the content of hallucinations (Bour-
hallucinations or were unwilling to take part
guignon, 1970, 1973; Kiev, 1972; Wallace,
in aversion therapy. The concealment of the
1959). The evidence thus far from the be-
report of hallucinatory experience by patients
havioral analysis of hallucinations, as well as
in the face of aversive stimuli may explain,
from anthropological data, indicates that
in part, the return of hallucinations after the
social control of hallucinations may, in fact,
termination of treatment or discharge from
partly involve an operant learning procedure
the hospital (Bucher & Fabricatore, 1970).
in which the individual (patient) learns to
Though operant conditioning seems to rep-
discriminate among audiences and different
resent one of the most promising areas in
situations or in which the audiences control
the behavioral study of hallucinations, the
the content of hallucinations.
early findings by Lindsley (1959, 1963) justi-
fied the conclusion that "vocal psychotic
symptoms are under some form of internal Psychoanalytical Approach
control that resists direct differential positive The interpretation of hallucinations and
reinforcement" (Lindsley, 1963, p. 269). Later, other imagery as related to delayed gratifica-
however, Ayllon and Haughton (1964) dem- tion (Freud, 1908/1962, 1911/1962) is not
onstrated that psychotic speech is amenable dissimilar to the emphasis on the inter-
to social reinforcement received on the ward, personal and social function, as well as on
for example, the attention given by the staff to the motivational aspects of hallucinatory ex-
the verbalization of symptoms. Two studies perience. In interpreting dreams as wish-
by Rutner and Bugle (1969) and Anderson fulfillment, Freud recognized that dreams are
and Alpert (1974) have demonstrated the motivated and that their content is a fulfill-
effects of social reinforcement and token ment of a motive. Dream analysis, in essence,
economy on hallucinatory reports. Just how is an attempt to uncover the underlying
much the report and content of hallucina- motivation or to locate the various elements
tions are under the control of social stimuli of the content of dreams among events in the
may be illustrated in the following case of a dreamer's past life. The analytical approach
36-year-old schizophrenic Negro female who of applying dream analysis procedures to
continually reported being in communication hallucinations seems to be as much a search
with famous personalities: for precipitating factors as is the behavioral
approach to hallucinations (McKellar, 1957).
Three student assistants assumed the role of listeners One important area of research that is
during daily twenty-minute visits with the patient.
One of the male students wore sandals, a beard, open relevant to both psychoanalytical and be-
collar, loose jacket and a SNCC button. Another havioral approaches to hallucinations is the
presented a picture of a clean-cut young man with controversial area of the cathartic effects of
military bearing who carried a Rockwell anti-Semite fantasy and imagination. Since hallucinations
and anti-Negro pamphlet quite visible to the patient.
The third, a friendly girl, tried to appear nondescript are considered by the analyst as the mani-
in both dress and demeanor. As predicted, the patient festation of delayed gratification, they are
reported to the student who emulated a freedom expected to be directly related to the reduc-
580 IHSAN AL-ISSA

tion of specific drives (Bandura, 1969; Fesh- In their pioneering research of schizophrenic
bach, 1970; Singer, 1970). The present social symptomatology of selected regions of the
approach would suggest that the concealment world, Murphy, Wittkower, Fried, and Ellen-
of hallucinations and the reluctance of the berger (1963) found that whereas visual hal-
individual to share these experiences with lucinations are the least frequently reported
others are expected to make their content in urban Euro-Americans, they are most fre-
unrelated to and somehow remote from the quently reported in Africa and the Near
immediate life situation. Hallucinations would East. It was, however, noted by Murphy
thus serve a generalized function rather than et al. that although there are variations in
one specific to the present drive state of the the frequency of different kinds of hallucina-
individual. As pointed out by Singer (1970), tions, in all countries studied, auditory hal-
fantasy and hallucinations are adaptive be- lucinations are in no instance reported as
cause of their flexibility for self-stimulation being infrequent by the psychiatrists. Though
to increase the affective state of the individual it is not used as frequently as vision by
and to maintain his general interest in a dull non-Western patients, it appears that audi-
and barren environment. It is only with tion is universally employed by schizophrenic
deliberate and extensive inquiry into the patients as a medium for the expression and
adaptive and role-taking function of halluci- communication of symptoms.
nations and imagery that they may become Another aspect of non-Western hallucina-
comprehensible within the context of the tions has been described by Collomb (1965).
patient's social and interpersonal situation Hallucinations of Senegalese patients tend to
(Juhasz, 1972; Sarbin & Juhasz, 1970; Singer, have an unstable and transitory quality. They
1970). consist of transformation of odors into sounds
and sounds into concrete shapes. A man, for
Cultural Variation in Hallucinations instance, hearing voices, especially the devil's
voices, gradually perceives these auditory
The content of hallucinations, like that of hallucinations as visible animals that want
dreams and other products of fantasy and to kill him.
imagination, is clearly based on the individu- Cross-temporal studies give the impression
al's past experience within a certain socio- that hallucinations of Western patients in the
cultural context and is therefore expected to past were predominantly visual, a finding
vary from one culture to another (Kiev, 1972; similar to those presently reported with non-
Weinstein, 1962b). Less obvious, however, is Western patients. Lenz (1964) has studied
the extent to which hallucinatory experience changes in symptomatology during the last
involves different senses across cultures. Audi- hundred years in Vienna. His data show that
tory hallucinations are the most frequently though visual hallucinations decreased during
reported by schizophrenic patients in the the period of study, there was an increase in
West, with visual hallucinations appearing in auditory hallucinations during the same period.
the most deteriorated patients (Buss, 1966; Similar findings were reported by Diethelm
Feinberg, 1962; Mayer-Gross, Slater & Roth, (cited in Opler, 1956). Since cross-temporal
1960; Straus, 1962). Feinberg, for instance, studies use case histories of patients, it is
found that in the admission ward of a large difficult to know whether the findings reflect
hospital there were only between 3% and 4% differences in the background of psychiatrists
of schizophrenic patients who reported visual throughout history, during which vision and
hallucinations. Hallucinations involving other audition have been differentially emphasized
senses are rarely reported by patients. Though in the observation of patients, or whether
in a hospital survey, American black schizo- they reflect true changes in symptomatology.
phrenic patients showed more hallucinations Although the validity of these cross-temporal
than did white patients, the prevalence of studies may be in doubt, it should be noted
auditory over visual hallucinations appears to that the religious concern with the report of
be true for both groups (Vitols, Waters, visions in the Middle Ages has been well
& Keeler, 1963). documented (Sarbin & Juhasz, 1967).
HALLUCINATIONS 581

Interpretation of Cultural Variation 1950; Buss, 1966). Though clinical observa-


tion indicates that visual hallucinations reflect
Social and cultural factors are not only more severe symptoms than do auditory hal-
involved in the definition and labeling of lucinations, McCabe, Fowler, Cadoret, and
certain experiences as hallucinatory, but they Winokur (1972) found that they are related
may also affect the report of different kinds to a good prognosis. This finding is in agree-
of hallucinations. The decreasing frequency of ment with an early study by Zubin, Sutton,
different kinds of hallucinations reported dy Salzinger, Salzinger, Burdock, and Peretz
Western patients seems to parallel the be- (1961), which indicates that although hallu-
creasing involvement of the senses in inter- cinating patients, regardless of modality, tend
personal and social interaction. The most to have a poor outcome, even so, visual
common type of communication between hallucinations tend to improve the outcome.
people is by means of speech (audition), next Jenkins (1952) has also observed that during
is communication by observing each other the recovery process, the course of improve-
(vision); the remaining senses tend to be ment of schizophrenic disorganization tends
relatively unimportant in social interaction to disappear first from the olfactory, gusta-
(Buss, 1966). If hallucinations constitute a tory, and visual senses, and remain longest
direct or symbolic expression of the indi- in the auditory sense. The differential resist-
vidual's social needs, these needs could be ance of hallucinations to treatment may reflect
more easily communicated by speech (auditory the relative use of the senses in interpersonal
hallucinations) than pictures (visual halluci- communication. Since audition is more easily
nations). Words are more ambiguous than used in communication, it is not surprising
visual images and hence more amenable to that auditory hallucinations are resistant to
distortion and reconstruction to suit the treatment. It thus appears that the involve-
individual's needs. When hallucinations are ment of the sense organ in interaction with
occasionally used by patients to present an the social environment seems to hinder rather
"objective" support of their delusional beliefs, than to facilitate treatment.
words are more suitable for the task than McCabe et al. (1972) found that visual
visions. It is, for instance, easier to hear an hallucinations tend to occur significantly more
insulting remark, "You are a wicked person," in the acute (less than 2-month duration)
than to conjure up a visual image that would than in the chronic (more than 2-year dura-
convey the same message. The general im- tion) schizophrenic state. Fischer (1972) also
plication of the present interpretation of pointed out that the chronicity process may
cross-temporal and cross-cultural data is not explain the frequency of different kinds of
that the rate of verbal discourse has changed, hallucinations in schizophrenia as well as
but that there may be cultural differences in during the drug experience. He stated that
the tendency to communicate with significant
authority figures; and words are more suitable the chronicity of the schizophrenic, as contrasted with
for communication with such figures (cf. the acuteness of the drug-induced state, may possibly
Lemert, 1962). account for the difference between auditory and visual
The social and interpersonal implication of hallucinations. Healthy volunteers who have lived
with the visual dimension of physical space-time
different kinds of hallucinations is also re- cannot denounce the dimension during the short
flected in diagnosis and treatment. Since the period of an acute drug-induced experience. It is well
patient is able to communicate to his social known that chronic abuse of amphetamine may pre-
environment more meaningful and explicit cipitate a psychosis which is almost indistinguishable
from schizophrenia, whereas the perceptual and
ideas through the voices he hears than through especially visual experiences of the incipient stages
other sensory impressions, it is not surprising of schizophrenia "are not unlike those induced by
that auditory hallucinations are considered a LSD or mescaline and auditory hallucinations only
sign of better contact with reality, indicating become prevalent after the process has lasted for
some weeks or months." (Fischer, 1972, p. 82)
good orientation of time and place. Confused
patients who are relatively disoriented tend Evidence showing similarities between hallu-
to have visual hallucinations (Bleuler, 1911/ cinations in schizophrenia and in chronic
582 IHSAN AL-ISSA

alcoholic psychosis give some support to the patients than other schizophrenics (Lowe,
Fischer proposition (Bagley & Binitie, 1970; 1973; Swanson, Bohmert, & Smith, 1970).
Freed, 1975; Norman, 1945). These studies The association of delusions with auditory
suggest that the clinical findings referred to hallucinations may reflect their proximity on
earlier, concerning the prevalence of auditory a conceptual-perceptual continuum. The ques-
hallucinations in schizophrenia, may not in- tion that arises here, however, is whether the
clude the acute early stage of the illness, relative use of symbolization and abstraction
particularly before the patient contacts psy- influences to some extent the prevalence of
chiatric services. auditory and visual hallucinations. It is not
There seems to be a striking similarity unreasonable, for instance, to suggest that
among the acute early stage of schizo- the prevalence of visual hallucinations in
phrenia, drug-induced states, and the episodic non-Western patients may be relevant to a
confusional psychotic states in African pa- general orientation toward the perceptual
tients (Fischer, 1972; Lambo, 1955). The (concrete) rather than the conceptual (ab-
sudden onset of these acute confusional states stract) aspects of the environment. Using a
tends to bring about visual rather than Middle Eastern sample, Al-Issa (1967) found
auditory hallucinations. Since physical and that illiterate schizophrenic and normal sub-
nutritional factors are more common in the jects gave more concrete definitions of words
psychotic reaction of preliterate primitive than did their literate counterparts. Price-
patients, the prevalence of visual hallucina- Williams (1970) also found a relationship
tions in non-Western societies could be attri- between literacy and concept formation in
buted in part to these factors. It is therefore Nigerian children. It is also possible that
important that organic factors in visual hal- in illiterate societies where people are taught
lucinations should be borne in mind when more through actions (interacting with the
considering any sociocultural hypothesis. physical environment) than through words
(as in Western school education), vision
Level of Abstraction and Hallucinations is more culturally emphasized than audition
in communication with the environment, and
The relationship between perception and this may bring about a tendency toward visual
conceptualization has long been documented
hallucinations and imagery. This tendency
(Bruner, 1957). In the study of abnormal may be enhanced by the cultural (social and
thinking and perception, if one considers a religious) connotation of vision in some non-
continuum ranging from concept to percept, Western societies.
auditory hallucinations and particularly de- The finding that visual eidetic imagery is
lusions could be located nearer to the con- more prevalent among unacculturated non-
ceptual and ideational end of the continuum. Western subjects than European or Western
On the other hand, visual hallucinations, as educated native subjects is compatible with
well as others involving contact receptors the present hypothesis. It is in accord with
(gustatory, olfactory, tactile), are bound to my interpretation of visual hallucinations that
physical stimuli and should be located near visual eidetic imagery is attributed to the
the perceptual end of the continuum. This dependence of the society on the develop-
suggestion is supported by descriptions by ment of visual skills and representations
patients of their own hallucinations. Jenkins rather than on verbalization and symboliza-
(1952), for instance, indicated that tion (Mountford, 1961; Porteus, 1931; Sheehan
the hallucinatory "voice" seems almost as close to & Stewart, 1972).
a conceptual as to perceptual disorder, as most pa- The present proposition of relating halluci-
tients, when queried, will describe it as something nations to different levels of conceptualization
heard in the mind or in the head, rather than as is not incompatible with recent theory and
something heard with the ears. (p. 743)
research in schizophrenia. On the conceptual-
Consistent with this point of view is the perceptual continuum, overinclusive patients
finding that auditory hallucinations tend to seem to function nearer to the conceptual
be more prevalent among paranoid delusional level and they tend to develop delusions and
HALLUCINATIONS 583

auditory hallucinations more frequently than attitudes toward these symptoms, for example,
do their underinclusive, withdrawn counter- social and cultural beliefs about hallucinations
parts (Lowe, 1973; Payne, Caird, & Laverty, may play a certain part in the consideration
1964). These two groups of overinclusive and of these symptoms as pathological in certain
underinclusive patients have been found to situations, but less pathological or even normal
attend differently to stimuli in the environ- in others. American-British comparisons and
ment (broad vs. narrow span of attention) studies relating hallucinations to social com-
and to use different strategies in processing petence or social conformity are important
information (low redundancy vs. high redun- steps in understanding the effect of social
dancy) (Cromwell, 1972; Silverman, 1964). factors on the relationship between pathology
The differentiation between conceptual and and symptoms.
perceptual functions in the study of thinking The investigation of factors involved in the
disorder in schizophrenia has also been em- tendency of the individual to attribute reality
phasized recently by Al-Issa (1972), Al-Issa, status to his imagery and/or to confuse reality
Bakal, and Larkin (1975), and Magaro (1974). with fantasy may elucidate the process under-
Though these approaches to schizophrenia lying hallucinations. The hypothesis that nega-
have not been validated in a non-Western tive attitudes might contribute to this con-
context, descriptive studies of Asian and fusion between reality and fantasy and to the
African hospitalized schizophrenics seem to pathological connotation of hallucinations
indicate that although paranoid schizophrenics needs to be investigated within different social
with auditory hallucinations are rare, with- and cultural contexts.
drawn schizophrenics with visual hallucina- Anthropological and behavioral data sug-
tions are the dominant type (Al-Issa, 1970; gesting a relationship between awareness of
Murphy, 1967). Systematic cross-cultural re- external conditions precipitating hallucinations
search is needed to validate these observa- (spirits, environmental factors) and the modifi-
tions and to provide evidence for the rela- cation or termination of the experience present
tionship between thinking and perception an interesting area of research (Bourguignon,
disorders in schizophrenia as investigated in 1970; Rutner & Bugle, 1969; Weingaertner,
the laboratory, and hallucinatory experiences. 1971). There seem to be some similarity be-
tween these data (e.g., non-Western interpreta-
Summary and Conclusions tion of hallucinations in terms of possession and
the social control of these experiences) and
Clinical and anthropological data indicate data from research on beliefs in the external
social and cultural differences in the frequency locus of control and the amenability of these
of, and attitudes toward, hallucinations. There beliefs to social influence (Lefcourt, 1972).
appears to be a paucity of data in this area Studies are needed to investigate the relation-
of research relating hallucinations to specific • ship between the disturbed reaction of the indi-
social and cultural factors, such as social vidual, and his faulty labeling of, or his at-
class, ethnicity, ecology, Westernization, and tribution of different causes to private ex-
acculturation. I have indicated that atti- periences, including imagery and hallucinations
tudes toward hallucinations may affect both (Singer, 1974).
the frequency and the content of hallucinatory Both reinforcement and drive-reduction
experience and emotional response to it. Un- (cathartic effects) theories of fantasy do not
fortunately, the attitudes of the professional, seem to explain the persistence and resistance
of the patient, and of his group toward hal- to treatment of schizophrenic hallucinations
lucinations have not been as systematically with aversive content. The present discussion
investigated as those toward mental illness suggests that sociocultural factors (e.g., nega-
(J. G. Rabkin, 1972). It has often been tive attitudes and the reluctance of patients
observed that hallucinations and other symp- to communicate their hallucinations) may
tomatology tend to be inconsistently related make hallucinations functionally autonomous
to specific psychiatric entities. This incon- and less relevant to the immediate social
sistency may, however, reflect differences in stimuli, though they may remain adaptive to
584 IHSAN AL-ISSA

the individual. It may be of interest to in- phrenias. New York: International Universities
vestigate the effects of institutionalization Press, 1950 (Originally published, 1911.)
Bourguignon, E. Hallucination and trance: An anthro-
and chronicity on the degree of accessibility pologist's perspective. In W. Keup (Ed.), Origin
of hallucinations to social stimuli and social and mechanisms of halhicinations. New York:
control. Plenum Press, 1970.
Bourguignon, E. Religion, altered stales of conscious-
ness, and social change. Columbus: Ohio State
Reference Note University Press, 1973.
Bowers, K. S. The effect of demands for honesty on
1. Draguns, J. G. Problems in comparing psycho- reports of visual and auditory hallucinations. Inter-
pathology across cultures. Paper presented at the national Journal of Clinical &r Experimental Hyp-
International Congress of Applied Psychology, nosis, 1967, 15, 31-36.
Montreal, July-August 1974. Bruner, J. S. On perceptual readiness. Psychological
Review, 1957, 64, 123-152.
Bucher, B., & Fabricatore, J. Use of patient-ad-
References ministered shock to suppress hallucinations. Be-
havior Therapy, 1970, 1, 382-385.
Al-Issa, I. Effects of literacy and schizophrenia on Bugelski, B. R. The definition of the image. In S. G.
verbal abstraction in Iraq. Journal of Social Psycho- Segal (Ed.), Imagery: Current cognitive approaches.
logy, 1967, 71, 39-43. New York: Academic Press, 1971.
Al-Issa, I. Cross-cultural studies of symptomatology Buss, A. Psychopathology. New York: Wiley, 1966.
in schizophrenia. In I. Al-Issa and W. Dennis Chapman, L. J., & Baxter, J. C. The process-reactive
(Eds), Cross-cultural studies of behavior. New York: distinction and patients' subculture. Journal of
Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1970. Nervous &• Mental Disease, 1963, 136, 352-359.
Al-Issa, I. Stimulus generalization and overinclusion Collomb, H. Bouffees delirantes en psychiatric Afri-
in normal and schizophrenic subjects. Journal of caine. Psychopathologie Africaine, 1965, 1, 167-237.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1972, 39, 182-186. Cooper, J. E., Kendell, R. E., Gurland, B. J., Sharpe,
Al-Issa, I., Bakal, D., & Larkin, N. Stimulus gen- L., Copeland, J. R. M., & Simon, R. Psychiatric
eralization and anxiety in schizophrenic and normal Diagnosis in New York and London. London:
subjects. British Journal of Social and Clinical Oxford University Press, 1972.
Psychology, 1975, 14, 371-378. Cromwell, R. L. Strategies for studying schizophrenic
Alumbaugh, R. V. Use of behavior modification behavior. Psychopharmacologia, 1972, 24, 121-146.
techniques toward reduction of hallucinatory be- Dement, W. C. An essay on dreams: The role of
havior: A case study. Psychological Record, 1971, physiology in understanding their nature. In New
21, 415-417. directions in psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Holt,
Anderson, E. N., & Threthowan, W. H. Psychiatry. Rinehart & Winston, 1965.
London: Bailliere, Tindall & Cassell, 1967. Diamond, M. D. The ability of schizophrenics to
Anderson, L. T., & Alpert, M. Operant analysis of modify responses in an interpersonal situation.
hallucination frequency in a hospitalized schizo- Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1957, 20, 441-444.
phrenic. Journal of Behavior Therapy 6* Experimental Edwards, G. Diagnosis of schizophrenia. An Anglo-
Psychiatry, 1974, 15, 13-18. American comparison. British Journal of Psychiatry,
Ayllon, T., & Haughton, E. Modification of sympto- 1972, L?0, 385-390.
matic verbal behavior of mental patients. Behaviour Ellson, D. G. Hallucinations produced by sensory
Research &• Therapy, 1964, 2, 87-97. conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
Bagley, C., & Binitie, A. Alcoholism and schizophrenia 1941,2*, 1-20. (a)
in Irishmen in London. British Journal of Addic- Ellson, D. G. Experimental extinction of an hallu-
tion, 1970, 65, 3-7. lucination produced by sensory conditioning. Journal
Bandura, A. Principles of behavior modification. New of Experimental Psychology, 1941, 28, 350-361. (b)
York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1969.
Evarts, E. V. A neurophysiologic theory of hallucina-
Bandura, A. Vicarious and self-reinforcement processes.
tions. In L. J. West (Ed.), Hallucinations. New
In R. Glaser (Ed.), The nature of reinforcement.
York: Grune & Stratton, 1962.
New York: Academic Press, 1971.
Barber, T. X. Hypnosis, suggestions and auditory- Feinberg, I. A comparison of the visual hallucina-
visual hallucinations. In W. Keup (Ed.), Origin tions in schizophrenia with those induced by mes-
and mechanisms of hallucinations. New York: caline and LSD-25. In L. J. West (Ed.), Hallu-
Plenum Press, 1970. cinations. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1962.
Barber, T. X., & Calverley, D. S. An experimental Feshbach, S. Aggression. In P. Mussen (Ed.), Car-
study of "hypnotic" (auditory and visual) hal- michael's manual of child psychology. New York:
lucinations. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psy- Wiley, 1970.
chology, 1964, 63, 13-20. Fischer, R. The perception-hallucination continuum.
Benedict, R. Anthropology and the abnormal. Journal Diseases of the Nervous System, 1969, 30, 161-171.
of General Psychology, 1934, 10, 59-82. Fischer, R. Schizophrenia: Research in biological
Bleuler, E. Dementia praecox or the group of schizo- perspective. In A. R. Kaplan (Ed.), Genetic factors
HALLUCINATIONS 585
in schizophrenia. Springfield, 111.: Charles C Thomas, Keup, W. (Ed.). Origin and mechanisms of hallucina-
1972. tions. New York: Plenum Press, 1970.
Fish, F. J. Schizophrenia. Bristol, England: John Kiev, A. Beliefs and delusions of West Indian immi-
Wright, 1962. grants to London. British Journal of Psychiatry,
Freed, E. X. Alcoholism and schizophrenia: The 1963, 109, 356-363.
search for perspectives review. Journal of Studies Kiev, A. Transcultural psychiatry. New York: Free
on Alcohol, 1975, 36, 853-881. Press, 1972.
Freud, S. Creative writers and daydreaming. In J. Klttver, H. Neurobiology of normal and abnormal
Strachey (Ed.), The Standard Edition of the Complete perception. In P. H. Hoch and J. Zubin (Eds.),
Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 12). Psychopathology of perception. New York: Grune
London: Hogarth, 1962. (Originally published, & Stratton, 1965.
1908.) Lambo, T. A. The role of cultural factors in paranoic
Freud, S. Formulation on the two principles of mental psychoses among the Yoruba tribe. Journal of
functioning. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The Standard Mental Science, 1955, 101, 239-266.
Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Lambo, T. A. Schizophrenic and borderline states.
Sigmund Freud (Vol. 12). London: Hogarth, 1962. In A. V. S. DeReuck (Ed.), Transcultural psy-
(Originally published, 1911.) chiatry. London: Churchill, 1965.
Gill, W. S. Interpersonal affect and conformity be- Lefcourt, H. M. Recent developments in the study
havior in schizophrenics. Journal of Abnormal and of locus of control. In B. Maher (Ed.), Progress
Social Psychology, 1963, 67, 502-505. in experimental personality research. New York:
Gould, L. N. Verbal hallucinations and activity of Academic Press, 1972.
vocal musculature: An electromyographic study. Leighton, A. H., Lambo, T. A., Hughes, C. C.,
American Journal of Psychiatry, 1948, 105, 367-372. Leighton, J. M., & Macklin, D. B. Psychiatric dis-
Gould, L. N. Verbal hallucinations as automatic order among the Yoruba. New York: Cornell Uni-
speech; the reactivation of dormant speech habit. veristy Press, 1963.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 1950, 107, 110-119. Leighton, D. C., Harding, J. S., Macklin, D. B.,
Hammer, M. Schizophrenia: Some questions and de- MacMillan, A. M., & Leighton, A. H. (Eds.),
finition in cultural perspective. In A. R. Kaplan The character of danger. New York: Basic Books,
(Ed.), Genetic factors in schizophrenia. Springfield, 1963.
111.: Charles C Thomas, 1972. Lemert, E. M. Paranoia and the dynamics of ex-
Hare, E. H. A short note on pseudo-hallucinations. clusion. Sociomelry, 1962, 25, 2-20.
British Journal of Psychiatry, 1973, 122, 469-476. Lenz, H. L. Verleichende Psychiatrie. Eine Studie uber
Hebb, D. O. Concerning imagery. Psychological Review, die Beziehung von Kultur, Soziologie and Psycho-
1968, 75, 466^77. pathologie. Vienna: Wilhelm Maudrich Verlag, 1964.
Hefferline, R. F., Bruno, L. J. J., & Camp, J. A. Lindsley, O. R. Reduction in rate of vocal psychotic
Hallucinations: An experimental approach. In F. J. symptoms by differential positive reinforcement.
McGuigan and R. A. Schoonover (Eds.), The psy- Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
chophysiology of thinking. New York: Academic 1959, 2, 269.
Press, 1973. Lindsley, O. R. Direct measurement and functional
Hefferline, R. F., Bruno, L. J. J., & Davidowitz, J. definition of vocal hallucinatory symptoms. Journal
Feedback control of covert behavior. In K. J. of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1963, 136, 293-297.
Connolly (Ed.), Mechanisms of motor skill develop- Lowe, G. R. The phenomenology of hallucinations
ment. New York: Academic Press, 1971. as an aid to differential diagnosis. British Journal
Hefferline, R. F., & Perera, T. B. Proprioceptive of Psychiatry, 1973, 123, 621-633.
discrimination of a covert operant without its ob- Magaro, P. A. Theories of the schizophrenic per-
servation by the subject. Science, 1963, 139, 834-835. formance deficit: An integration theory synthesis.
Hollingshead, A. B., & Redlich, F. C. Social class In B. Maher (Ed.), Progress in experimental per-
and mental illness. New York: Wiley, 1958. sonality research. New York: Academic Press, 1974.
Holt, R. R. Imagery: The return of the ostracized. Mayer-Gross, W., Slater, E., & Roth, M. Clinical
American Psychologist, 1964, 19, 254-264. psychiatry (2nd ed.). Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins,
Horowitz, M. J. The imagery of visual hallucinations. 1960.
Journal of Nervous &• Mental Disease, 1964, 138, McCabe, M. S., Fowler, R. C., Cadoret, R. J., &
513-523. Winokur, G. Symptom differences in schizophrenia
Inouye, T., & Shimizu, A. The electromyographic with good and poor prognosis. American Journal
study of verbal hallucination. Journal of Nervous of Psychiatry, 1972, 128, 1239-1243.
&• Mental Disease, 1970, 151, 415-522. McGuigan, F. J. Covert oral behavior and auditory
Jenkins, R. L. The schizophrenic sequence: With- hallucinations. Psychophysiology, 1966, 3, 73-80.
drawal, disorganization, psychotic reorganization. McGuigan, F. J. Electrical measurement of covert
American Journal of Orthopsychialry, 1952, 22, processes as an explication of "higher mental
738-748. events." In F. J. McGuigan and R. W. Schooner
Juhasz, J. B. An experimental study of imagining. (Eds.), The psychophysiology of thinking. New York:
Journal of Personality, 1972, 40, 588-600. Academic Press, 1973.
586 IHSAN AL-ISSA

McKellar, P. Imagination and thinking. London: Saravay, S. M., & Pardes, H. Auditory "elementary
Cohen & West, 1957. hallucinations" in alcohol withdrawal psychoses. In
McKellar, P. Experience and behavior. Middlesex, W. Keup (Ed.), Origin and mechanisms of hallu-
England: Penguin Books, 1968. cinations. New York: Plenum Press, 1970.
McLemore, C. W. Imagery in desensitization. Be- Sarbin, T. R. The concept of hallucination. Journal
haviour Research 6- Therapy, 1972, 10, 51-57. of Personality, 1967, 35, 359-380.
Mountford, C. P. The arts in Australia: Aboriginal Sarbin, T. R., & Andersen, M. L. Base-rate expectan-
art. London: Longmans, 1961. cies and perceptual alterations in hypnosis. British
Murphy, H. B. M. Cultural aspects of the delusion. Journal of Social &• Clinical Psychology, 1963, 2,
Studium Generate, 1967, 20, 684-692. 112-121.
Murphy, H. B. M., Wittkower, E. D., Fried, J., Sarbin, T. R., & Juhasz, J. B. The historical back-
& Ellenberger, H. A cross-cultural survey of schizo- ground of the concept of hallucination. Journal of
phrenic symptomatology. International Journal of
the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 1967, 3,
Social Psychiatry, 1963, 9, 237-249. 339-358.
Norman, J. P. Alcoholic hallucinatory states. Quarterly
Journal of the Study of Alcohol, 1945, 5, 563-574. Sarbin, T. R., & Juhasz, J. B. Toward a theory of
Nydegger, R. V. The elimination of hallucinatory imagination. Journal of Personality, 1970, 38, 52-76.
and delusional behavior by verbal conditioning and Sarbin, T. R., Juhasz, J. B., & Todd, P. The social
assertive training: A case study. Journal of Behavior psychology of "hallucinations." Psychological Record,
Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 1972, 3, 225-227. 1971, 21, 87-93.
Opler, M. K. Culture, psychiatry and human values. Schaefer, H. H., & Martin, P. L. Beltavioral therapy.
Springfield, 111.: Charles C Thomas, 1956. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1969.
Paivio, A. Imagery and verbal process. New York: Schooler, C., & Spohn, H. E. The susceptibility of
Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1971. chronic schizophrenics to social influence in the
Payne, R. W., Caird, W. K., & Laverty, S. G. Over- formation of perceptual judgments. Journal of
inclusive thinking and delusions in schizophrenic Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1960, 61, 348-354.
patients. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Seashore, C. E. Measurements of illusions and hal-
1964, 68, 562-566. lucinations in normal life. Slitdies from the Yale
Perky, C. W. An experimental study of imagination. Psychological Laboratory, 1895, 3, 1-67.
American Journal of Psychology, 1910, 21, 422-452. Segal, S. J. Imagery: Current cognitive approaches.
Phillips, L., Broverman, I. K., Si Zigler, E. Social New York: Academic Press, 1971. (a)
competence and psychiatric diagnosis. Journal of Segal, S. J. Processing of the stimulus in imagery
Abnormal Psychology, 1966, 71, 209-214. and perception. In S. J. Segal (Ed.), Imagery:
Porteus, S. D. The psychology of a primitive people: Current cognitive approaches. New York: Academic
A study of the Australian Aborigine. London: Edward Press, 1971. (b)
Arnold, 1931. Shean, G. Perceptual conformity and responsiveness
Price-Williams, D. R. Abstract and concrete modes to social reinforcement in chronic schizophrenics.
of classification in Nigeria. In I. Al-Issa & W. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1973, 82, 174-177.
Dennis (Eds.), Cross-cultural studies of behavior. Sheehan, P. W., & Stewart, S. J. A cross-cultural
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1970. study of eidetic imagery among Australian Ab-
Pylyshyn, Z. W. What the mind's eye tells the mind's original children. Journal of Social Psychology, 1972,
brain: A critique of mental imagery. Psychological 87, 179-188.
Bulletin, 1973, 80, 1-24. Siegel, R. K., & West, L. J. (Eds.) Hallucinations:
Rabkin, J. G. Opinions about mental illness: A review Behavior, experience, and theory. New York: Wiley,
of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 1972, 77, 1975.
153-171. Silverman, A. J., Cohen, S. I., Shmavonian, B. M.,
Rabkin, R. Do you see things that aren't there? & Greenberg, G. Psychophysiological investigations
Construct validity of the concept "hallucination." in sensory deprivation. Psychosomatic Medicine,
In W. Keup (Ed.), Origin and mechanisms of hal- 1961, 23, 48-61.
lucinations. New York: Plenum Press, 1970. Silverman, J. The problem of attention in research
Richardson, A. Mental imagery. London: Routledge and theory in schizophrenia. Psychological Review,
& Kegan Paul, 1969. 1964, 71, 352-379.
Rutner, I. T., & Bugle, C. An experimental procedure Singer, J. L. Daydreaming: An introduction to the
for the modification of psychotic behavior. Journal experimental study of inner experience. New York:
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1969, 33, Random House, 1966.
651-653.
Singer, J. L. Drives, affects and daydreams: The
Sandifer, M. G., Horden, A., Timbury, G. C., & Green, adaptive role of spontaneous imagery or stimulus-
L. M. Psychiatric diagnosis: A comparative study independent mentation. In J. S. Antrobus (Ed.)
in North Carolina, London and Glasgow. British
Journal of Psychiatry, 1968, 114, 1-9. Cognition and affect. Boston: Little, Brown, 1970.
Saravay, S. M., & Pardes, H. Auditory elementary Singer, J. L. Imagery and, daydream methods in psycho-
hallucinations in alcohol withdrawal psychosis. therapy and behavior modification. New York:
Archives of General Psychiatry, 1967, 16, 652-658. Academic Press, 1974.
HALLUCINATIONS 587

Slade, P. D. The effects of systematic desensitization Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1971,
on auditory hallucinations. Behaviour Research 6* 36, 422-429.
Therapy, 1972, 10, 85-91. Weinstein, E. A. Social aspects of delusions, a psy-
Spanos, N. P., & Barber, T. X. "Hypnotic" expe- chiatric study of the Virgin Islands. New York:
riences as inferred from subjective reports: Auditory Free Press, 1962. (a)
and visual hallucinations. Journal of Experimental Weinstein, E. A. Social aspects of hallucinations. In
Research in Personality, 1968, 3, 136-150. L. J. West (Ed.), Hallucinations. New York: Grune
Srole, L., Langner, T. S., Michael S. T., Opler, M. K., & Stratton, 1962. (b)
& Rennie, T. A. C. (Ed.), Mental health in the West, L. J. A general theory of hallucinations and
metropolis (Vol. 1). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1962. dreams. In L. J. West (Ed.), Hallucinations. New
Stein, K. B. Motoric and ideational dimensions: York: Grune & Stratton, 1962. (a)
A study of personality styles among prison inmates. West, L. J. (Ed.). Hallucinations. New York: Grune
Psychological Reports, 1968, 22, 119-129. & Stratton, 1962. (b)
Stoyva, J. Biofeedback techniques and the conditions Whitman, J. R. Responsiveness to social pressures
of hallucinatory activities. In F. J. McGuigan & in schizophrenia. Psychological Reports, 1961, 8, 238.
R. A. Schoonover (Eds.), The psychophysiology of Will, O. A. Hallucinations: Comments reflecting clini-
thinking. New York: Academic Press, 1973. cal observations of the schizophrenic reaction. In
L. J. West (Ed.), Hallucinations. New York: Grune
Straus, E. W. Phenomenology of hallucinations. In & Stratton, 1962.
L. J. West (Ed.), Hallucinations. New York: Grune
Witkin, H. A. Cognitive styles across cultures. Inter-
& Stratton, 1962.
national Journal of Psychology, 1967, 2, 233-250.
Strauss, J. S. Hallucinations and delusions as points Witkin, H. A., & Berry, J. W. Psychological differen-
on continua function: Rating scale evidence. Ar- tiation in cross-cultural perspective. Journal of
chives of General Psychiatry, 1969, 21, 581-586. Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1975, 6, 4-87.
Swanson, D. W., Bohmert, P. J., & Smith, J. A. Wittkower, E. D., & Rin, H. Trancultural psychiatry.
The paranoid. Boston: Little, Brown, 1970. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1965, 13, 387-394.
Triandis, H. C., Vassiliou, V., Vassiliou, B., Tanaka, Wolpe, J. Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition.
Y., & Shanmugam, A. V. The analysis of subjective Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1958.
culture. New York: Wiley, 1972. Zigler, E., & Phillips, L. Psychiatric diagnosis and
Vitols, M., Waters, H. G., & Keeler, M. H. Hallu- symptomatology. Journal of Abnormal and Social
cinations and delusions in white and Negro schizo- Psychology, 1961, 63, 69-75.
phrenics. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1963, 120, Zubin, J., Sutton, S., Salzinger, K., Salzinger, S.,
472-476. Burdock, E. I., & Peretz, D. A biometric approach
Wallace, A. F. C. Cultural determinants of response to prognosis in schizophrenia. In P. H. Hoch & J.
to hallucinatory experience. Archives of General Zubin (Ed.), Comparative epidemiology of the mental
Psychiatry, 1959, 1, 58-69. disorders. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1961.
Weingaertner, A. H. Self-administered aversive stimu-
lation with hallucinating hospitalized schizophrenics. Received February 9, 1976 •

You might also like