You are on page 1of 8

Psychopharmacology (1985) 86:392-399

Psychopharmacology
9 Springer-Verlag 1985

Comparative pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics


of lorazepam, alprazolam and diazepam
Everett H. Ellinwood, Jr., Douglas G. Heatherly, Arlene M. Nikaido,
Thorir D. Bjornsson, and Clinton Kilts
Behavioral Neuropharmacology Section, Department of Psychiatry, and Division of Clinical Pharmacology, Department of Medicine,
Duke University Medical Center, P.O. Box 3870, Durham, NC 27710, USA

Abstract. The contribution of differential absorption-dis- absorption-distribution pharmacokinetics to drug activity


tribution pharmacokinetics to drug activity can be partially can be partially determined by comparing simultaneous
determined by comparing simultaneous estimates of drug estimates of drug serum level with behavioral effects. In the
serum level with pharmacodynamic effects. In the present present study we have contrasted the effects of clinically
paper we have contrasted the effects of clinically equipotent equivalent doses of lorazepam, alprazolam, and diazepam
doses of lorazepam, alprazolam, and diazepam on the on the performance of cognitive-neuromotor tasks. We
performance of tracking and digit symbol substitution selected a high dose, i.e., twice the recommended higher
tasks. Eight young males were tested for 12 h after ingesting anxiolytic dosage, to obtain values closer to the maximum
the drug. The three benzodiazepines and placebo were effect and to be in the more linear segment of the
administered to each subject according to a balanced dose-response curve. The three benzodiazepines were
double-blind Latin square design. A model is presented chosen on the basis of their diversity in pharmacologic
that describes the relationship between drug concentration properties including elimination half-life, lipid solubility,
and the degree of impairment across time after the final plasma binding, and high-affinity receptor binding.
peak effect. Exponential rate constants were determined
for each drug using a Marquardt nonlinear fit of the pooled
Materials and methods
data. Basically, the constants relate offset serum drug
values to the impairment curves at a time when serum-brain Eight males, who were between the ages of 21 and 26 and
equilibrium is assumed to have occurred. The values within + 10% of ideal height-weight ratios, participated in
indicate markedly rapid acute tolerance for alprazolam and the study. The purpose and procedure were explained to
diazepam but relatively little acute tolerance for lorazepam. the subjects and written informed consent was
Whether these constants reflect adaptation or differential obtained.
association-dissociation receptor rate constants cannot be
determined, but they do highlight the need to consider Procedure
receptor kinetics as an important factor in benzodiazepine
Subjects were trained to a plateau level of performance on
pharmacodynamics.
the tasks in three 2-h sessions. Four drug sessions were
Key words: Lorazepam - Alprazolam - Diazepam - scheduled at 2-week intervals according tO a Latin square
Acute tolerance - Pharmacokinetics - Pharmacodynam- design balanced for drug order. Lorazepam, alprazolam,
ics diazepam, and placebo were administered using a dou-
ble-blind procedure at doses of 0.057, 0.028, 0.286, and
0.000 mg/kg body weight, respectively. The powered drug
was given PO in a starch lactose solution.
Benzodiazepine pharmacodynamic profiles are not pre- Each test session started at 7:30 AM with a predrug trial
dicted well by dispositional pharmacokinetics following followed by repeated performance testing on various
acute dosing. The offset of diazepam-induced psychomotor cognitive-neuromotor tasks. Two of the most frequently
impairment is much more rapid than the corresponding presented tasks, continuous subcritical tracking (SCT) and
decline of concurrent bound and unbound diazepam digit symbol substitution (DSS), are discussed in this paper.
concentration (Ellinwood et al. 1983a, b; MacLeod et al. Results of the other tests will be described in other papers
1977). In contrast, lorazepam has a prolonged period of (Heatherly et al. in preparation). To assess the time course of
impairment relative to diazepam (Seppala et al. 1976; the drug effect, particularly during the initial period, the SCT
George and Dundee 1977), which is clearly not a function task was presented at predrug and at 5, 20, 25, 30, 35, 65,
of drug elimination rates, since the elimination half-life of 80, 95, 110, and 155 min and 3.5, 5.5, 7.5, 9.5, 10.5, and
lorazepam is one-third that of diazepam. ll.5'h postdrug ingestion. The DSS task was performed
Either differential absorption-distribution pharmacoki- concurrently at the same time intervals as the SCT task,
netics (Arendt et al. 1983) or receptor kinetics appear to except at 20, 25, and 30 min; only the SCT task was done at
be a more likely explanation of the observed differences in the latter three time periods.
pharmacodynamics between the two drugs than their A 7-ml venous blood sample was drawn before and at
elimination kinetics. The contribution of differential 20, 35, 65, 85,100, and 135 min and at 3, 4, 6, 8, 10, 11, 24,
and 48 h after drug ingestion from an obturated indwelling
Offprint requests to: E. H. Ellinwood, Jr. catheter in the subject's left forearm. Lorazepam, diaze-
393

pam, and alprazolam serum levels were quantified after which was calculated from the terminal linear phase of the
extraction using electron-capture detection gas chromato- natural log concentration versus time plot. The K d for
graphy' as described by Greenblatt et al. (1978, 1981) and diazepam is a rough estimate since the last blood sample
Skinner (1979) with modifications. Only very small was taken at only 48 h. The total area under the serum
amounts of N-desmethyldiazepam were detected on test concentration versus time curve (AUC) was computed
sessions following the diazepam treatment and did not using the trapezoidal method for the observed data and
interfere with the assays of the other drugs. extrapolated to infinity from the last data point using the
Kel determined earlier. Apparent total clearance was
Psychomotor tasks determined from the ratio of dose administered to AUC.
Apparent volume of distribution was computed as the ratio
Apparatus. The experimental chamber was a sound-atten-
"uated room, containing a 98- x 128-cm rearview video of C1 to K d. The values reported as the maximum
display screen and an adjustable chair facing the screen. A concentration (C~x) and the time at which Cm~ occurs
car steering wheel and nine-digit keypad were attached in (tmax) are the actual observed values.
front of and to the right of the chair, respectively. All
subjects were right-handed. The images were controlled by Statistical treatment of the data
a Digital Equipment PDP 11/34 computer and an LPS 11 Analysis of the drug effect across all batteries was performed
Analog/Digital System interfaced to a custom-designed by using the repeated measures analysis of variance program
Raster Scan unit. The computer system recorded digital of the 1983 version of the Biomedical Statistics Package
data on-line. (BMDP). Battery was the repeated factor. Subsequent
contrasts of means were conduced with an ct level of 0.001
Continuous subcritical tracking. A 3-cm wide illuminated due to the number of multiple comparisons.
bar was displayed from top to bottom in the center of the
darkened TV projection screen. A central portion of the Pharmacodynamic/pharmacokinetic model. Since previous
bar, 34 cm in length, moved laterally across the entire studies suggest that drug serum concentrations account for
screen. The movement of the tracking bar was controlled by only a small percentage of the variance in. the performance
the subject manipulating the steering wheel and by the scores, we defined the following mathematical model to
computer. The wheel was connected to a 10-turn poten- describe more accurately the relationship between drug
tiometer of 100 g2, which was connected to the analog/di- concentration and the degree of performance impairment
gital input of the computer. The position of the poten- across time after the final impairment peak:
tiometer was controlled by the steering input, added to the
position of the bar by the computer and multiplied by a E = FF x P + A o x e(--KATc X t) Eq. (1)
constant value, termed the level of difficulty (2). A
simplified equation of this relationship would be: change in where E = performance score, P = serum drug concen-
bar position = (bar position + potentiometer position) x 2. tration, FF = flee fraction for drug, t = time after drug
Using this equation a new bar position was calculated 60 ingestion,/(AT c = a hybrid acute tolerance constant, Ao =
times/s. The duration of the tracking task was 3 rain; the 2 coefficient or intercept term for KaTC.
was 2 for the first 30 s and increased to 3 during the last Due to the nature of the impairment time course (fast
150 s. Subjects were instructed to maintain the moving bar rise, double peak, and offset), the model was applied only
in the center of the screen. The measure of merit was the to the offset phase from the final impairment peak at 95 min
root mean square (RMS) deviation of the bar from the for lorazepam and alprazolam and at 80 rain for diazepam
center. to the last measured time point at 690 rain postingestion.
Both parameters, /(ATc and Ao, were determined by
Digit symbol substitution. This task was an automated performing nonlinear least-squares regression analysis on
version of the DSS subtest from the Wechsler Adult all the known parameters. Since the free fraction (FF)
Intelligence Scale. A" code table, consisting of one row of change over time for each drug was negligible compared to
sequential numbers from 1 to 9 and a second row of the nine the change in serum concentration, the FF used in the
DSS symbols, was displayed on the screen. A symbol was equation was a constant determined from values reported
presented below the code table and the subject pressed on in the literature (Greenblatt et al. 1980, 1983; Johnson et al.
the keypad the number paired with the symbol in the code 1979). The nonlinear method used was the Marquardt
table. The number-symbol pairings were randomly rear- method of the NLIN procedure in the Statistical Analysis
ranged at the start of each battery. There were 12 System (SAS) package. Performance scores were repre-
presentations of each symbol for a total of 108 trials for the sented as delta scores (postdrug score minus predrug
entire task. The primary measure of merit was the power score). Regression analyses were performed to determine
score which was calculated by dividing the total number of how well the model predicted the observed performance
correct responses by the average reaction time. measures.

Pharmacokinetic analysis Results


The pharmacokinetic parameters, elimination half-life Pharmacokinetic parameters for the three benzodiazepines
(t 89 apparent total clearance (C1), and apparent volume are listed in Tables 1, 2, and 3. In general, they are within
of distribution (Vdarea), w e r e calculated from the serum the range of published results for PO dosing; however, tmax
concentration versus time data using conventional expres- is early, representing primarily the fast absorption of the
sions (Gibaldi and Perrier 1975). The elimination half-life suspension formulation. To define more precisely the time
was determined from the elimination rate constant (Kel), course of the drug effect during the initial period of rapid
394

Table 1, Pharmacokinetic parameters of diazepam after a PO dose of 0.286 mg/kg body weight

Subject Weight Age Dose tm C1 Vd~ea Cm,x t,n~


(ID) (kg) (years) (mg) (h) (ml/min/kg) (l/kg) (ng/ml) (h)

L1 -SK 71.8 25 20.5 19.4 0.41 0.69 740 1.583


L2-DB 84.1 25 24.1 22.7 0.39 0.76 1,143 1.333
L3-GH 61.4 25 17.6 36.8 0.16 0.50 1,039 1.083
L4-PZ 61.4 26 17.6 24.8 0.31 0.66 840 1.833
L5-CD 86.4 24 24.7 22.9 0.36 0.71 1,067 0.583
L6-DT 72.7 26 20.8 85.0 0.15 1.13 1,086 1.083
L8-TS 70.0 26 20.0 27.2 0.31 0.73 1,347 0.583
L9-RC 84.1 22 24.1 27.4 0.35 0.83 891 1.333
J~- 73.9 25 21.1 33.3 0.31 0.75 1,019 1.178
SD 9.9 1 2.9 21.5 0.10 0.18 191 0.442

Table 2. Pharmacoldnetic parameters of alprazolam after a PO dose of 0.028 mg/kg body weight

Subject Weight Age Dose tv2 C1 Vd.... Cmax tmax


(ID) (kg) (years) (rag) (h) (ml/min/kg) (1/kg) (ng/ml) (h)

L1-SK 71.8 25 2.05 13.6 1.03 1.21 26 1,333


L2-DB 84.1 25 2.41 14.7 1.09 1.39 25 2.583
L3-GH 61.4 25 1.76 13.5 0.72 0.85 62 0.333
L4-PZ 61.4 26 1.76 18.6 0.94 1.51 37 0.333
L5-CD 86.4 24 2.47 11.6 0.74 0.74 39 1.583
L6-DT 72.7 26 2.08 22.0 0.40 0.77 58 0.333
L8-TS 70.0 26 2.00 1.92 1.25 1.29 31 1.583
L9-RC 84.1 22 2.41 9.7 0.72 0.61 43 3.500
2( 73.9 25 2.11 14,5 0.86 1.05 40 1.448
SD 9.9 1 0.29 4.0 t3.27 0.34 14 1.15I

Table 3. Pharmacokinetic parameters of lorazepam after a PO dose of 0.057 mg/kg body weight

Subject Weight Age Dose tv2 CI Vd.... Cmax /max


(ID) (kg) (years) (mg) (h) (ml/min/kg) (1/kg) (ng/ml) (h)

L1-SK 71.8 25 4.10 19.9 1.20 2.05 46 1.583


L2-DB 84.1 25 4.80 20.0 1.08 1.87 81 0.583
L3-GH 61.4 25 3.50 19.4 1.08 1.81 78 0.583
L4-PZ 61.4 26 3.50 12.1 1.35 1.41 63 0.333
L5-CD 86.4 24 4.93 17.6 0.84 1.29 77 0.583
L6-DT 72.7 26 4.15 9.1 1.80 1.41 40 0.583
L8-TS 70.0 26 4.00 14.8 1.40 1.80 54 0.583
L9-RC 84.1 22 4.80 12.9 1.63 1.82 52 0.583
2( 73.9 25 4.22 15.7 1.30 1.56 61 0.670
SD 9.9 1 0.57 4.1 0.31 0.40 16 0.380

changes in p e r f o r m a n c e , SCT, the m o t o r coordination task, consistent relationship across drugs between times of p e a k
was p r e s e n t e d frequently during the first 2 h and is the drug concentrations and p e a k e f f e c t s .
principal test described in this report, DSS is also discussed A f t e r the second p e a k effect all three benzodiazepines
to d e m o n s t r a t e differential offset effects for a cognitive show a r a p i d offset of the SCT effect; d i a z e p a m and
task. a l p r a z o l a m rapidly return to baseline within 6 0 - 1 0 0 min.
T h e tracking profiles are shown in Fig. 1A. The L o r a z e p a m has both an initial r a p i d offset and a substantial
i m p a i r m e n t profile is quite similar for d i a z e p a m and late, shallow offset slope, and returns to baseline only after
alprazolam b u t different for lorazepam. O n e r e m a r k a b l e 500 rain. In general, l o r a z e p a m had m o r e p r o n o u n c e d and
feature of the tracking profiles for individual subjects is the p r o l o n g e d impairing effects, while alprazolam p r o d u c e d a
a p p e a r a n c e of two distinct m a j o r p e a k s of i m p a i r m e n t for level of i m p a i r m e n t b e t w e e n that of l o r a z e p a m and
d i a z e p a m and alprazolam. H o w e v e r , the difference diazepam. T h e time course of i m p a i r m e n t on the DSS task
b e t w e e n the m e a n of each p e a k and the lowest intervening was very similar for all three drugs to that of tracking.
m e a n p o i n t b e t w e e n the p e a k s (trough) was not significant H o w e v e r , the i m p a i r m e n t falloff for later periods was
for either drug, p r o b a b l y due to intersubject variability in slower for DSS than SCT (Fig. 1B).
the latencies of the p e a k s and the trough. T h e r e was no T h e r e p e a t e d measures analysis indicated signficant
395
275

drug [F (3,28) = 7.39, P < 0.0009], testing time [F (15,420)


250
= 24.14, P < 0.0001], and drug x testing time [F (45,420) =
4.63, P < 0.0001] effects. Significant differences between
2~'5 drugs on the SCT task are shown for different testing time
points in Fig. 1A. Similarly, the drug IF (3,27) =26.68, P <
T
2O0 0.0001], testing time [F (12,336) = 50.08, P < 0.0001], and
R drug x testing time [F (36,336) = 6.34, P < 0.0001] effects
A
C 175 were significant for the DSS task. As illustrated in Fig. 1B,
K
I differences between drugs for DSS impairment were signifi-
N
G IS0 cant during the first 4 h.
O To compare directly the time course of the drug serum
E
L 125 level to that of the drug effect, the performance of a subject
T
R for the SCT and DSS tasks was represented as a percent of
S 100
the maximum effect shown by that individual for each drug
C
0
condition (Figs. 2 A - C ) . Serum drug levels were similarly
R
E 75
transformed to a percent of the maximum blood levels
S achieved by each subject for the different drugs. In this way
5o
performance and drug measures were normalized for all
subjects within each drug condition allowing the compar-
ison of the time courses of effect and drug concentration for
25 Z
each drug. However, the transformed values do not validly
reflect magnitude of effect or drug levels for comparisons
~ across the three benzodiazepines.
0 }00 200 300 ~00 500 600 700 Over the 11.5-h testing period N-desmethyldiazepam
A M]N POST INGESTION constituted a very small fraction of the total diazepam
concentration. Based on the reported relative potency of
~oi ~ N-desmethyldiazepam (Palva and Linnoila 1978), the
diazepam serum levels in Figs. 2 and 3 reflect diazepam plus
55, half the measured metabolite concentration. To simplify
the data presentation in Fig. 2 and 3, the free fraction of the
5D. three drugs was not used in the calculation of the blood
O
$
values since protein binding would not vary substantially
S ~5. over the testing interval and thus would not alter the
0
V
time-dependent relationship between performance and
E qO-
R
serum levels.
B
L
The curves in Figs. 2A and 2C indicate that the
L 35-
diazepam impairment effect had a leading phase relation-
P
0 ship to serum drug level, whereas the opposite was the case
W 30-
E for lorazepam. As reported previously (Ellinwood et al.
R
1983a, b), acute tolerance is demonstrated for diazepam by
I 25-
M the precipitous drop in tracking impairment effect between
P
R
I 20-
110 and 155 rain, while serum levels fall more slowly.
R Alprazolam also demonstrated a 70% recovery of the peak
N 15-
impairment effect between 110 and 210 min during which
T time mean serum drug concentrations remained constant
S
io T
(Fig. 2B). In fact, serum values did not start to decline
) significantly until after the impairement effect had almost
S S- returned to baseline, suggesting the involvement of other
mechanisms before a decrement in drug level begins to
O2 affect performance.
The DSS task demonstrated a more prolonged effect
-5 for all three drugs, especially in the late phase offset. By
,' . . . .

I00
i , , .... , ....... i ...... , , - . , 20.0rain tracking impairment had returned to 15% of
200 300 qO0 500 600 700

B MIN POST INGESTION


maximum effect, whereas DSS performance improved at a
much slower rate. The varied time courses for the effect
Fig. 1. Impairment on subcritical tracking (A) and digit symbol falloff indicate that different underlying processes may be
substitution (B) tasks for diazepam (curve D), alprazolam (A), involved in the effect of the benzodiazepines on the SCT
lorazepam (L) and placebo (P). Performance is represented as unit and DSS tasks.
changes from the predrug score (delta score). A smooth spline
fitting procedure was used to generate the curves connecting all Similar concentration-response relationships are re-
time points. Symbols indicate significance levels for diazepam flected in the clockwise and counterclockwise hysteresis
versus alprazolam (A), alprazolam versus lorazepam (+), and curves for diazepam and lorazepam, respectively (Figs, 3A
diazepam versus lorazepam (C)) mean comparisons (one symbol and 3C). Only the tracking hysteresis is shown, since there
represents P < 0.05; two symbols P < 0.01; and three symbols P < were no early DSS measures. Compared to diazepam and
0.001) lorazepam, alprazolam had a very distinct hysteresis profile
396
lOO-I I O0-~ P~
r
90L
9o ~ i
oo-] ~1 N ',

7o

T 6D T 60

0
50
B
S !
E ~0 s ~o
R

D 30-

X 20.
1 T "-..~_ . . . . . . . . D. 2~ T ,, ,,,
M ~o- M I0 I'; " " ' " .... ""T-. ~ "I)
"..-.T- ..... "-'D

oZ "" ......... ~ ............. T---....T. __ ..-T O "'T ...... T

~]0 .,% -lO


0 I00 200 300 lO0 500 600 700 0 I00 200 300 ~00 500 600 700
A MIN POSTINOESTION B MIN POSTINGESTION

!/"~?'~$'"'\
I00--

gO-]

'lDl~"'~"
: %%

//
R 70 m

EPN I
T 60. i
0 ', ""
F ~\
SO. ~",, ~x\\
B$405" !
ER "T."'.........
O 30- T ""I x\ --B

X 20. i \
I "'" "~
M . "~..
M lo- "'"'-... Fig. 2. Time-dependent relationship between subcritical tracking
""'T'-.-._T.....~ " (curve T), digit symbol substitution (D) and drug serum concen-
tration (B) for diazepam (A), alprazolan (B), and lorazepam (C).
Task performance was measured as unit changes relative to
-I0 'qr predrug level. Task and blood values are expressed as percent of
0 lO0 200 SO0 400 500 600 700 maximum value and the curves were generated using a smooth
C HIN POSTINGESTION spline fitting procedure

(Fig. 3B); the onset and offset of the entire major unbound drug serum concentration alone. Alprazolam
impairment effect occurred within a very narrow average showed the largest increase in r2 from 16% to 52% for SCT
drug serum range, between 2 8 - 3 1 ng/ml. In contrast, there and from 32% to 57% for DSS. There was a more modest
was a much more systematic linear relation between the improvement for lorazepam from 33% to 43% for SCT and
offset time courses of the impairment and serum drug levels from 47% to 53% for DSS. Similarly, use of the KATC
of lorazepam. model changed to a lesser extent the r2 values for diazepam
The exponential constants (KATc) that represent the from 19% to 27% for SCT and from 30% to 34% for
best relationship of the offset of the impairment curves to DSS.
drug serum values were 1.06 h -1 for diazepam, 0.91 h -1 for
alprazolam, and a much slower constant of 0.23 h -1 for
lorazepam. The KATC constants for DSS were all consid-
erably lower, i.e., 0.08 h -1, 0.14 h -1 and 0.09 h -I, respec- Discussion
tively. Regression analyses indicated that for all three drugs The clockwise hysteresis loop observed for diazepam in
the model described by Eq. 1 accounted for more of the Fig. 3A has been designated as pathognomic of tolerance
performance variability during the offset phase than the (Holford and Sheiner 1981). Tolerance is defined as a
397

reduced drug effect at the same concentration that earlier more robust indicator of acute tolerance is the contrast
produced a greater effect. Clockwise hysteresis loops have between the onset and offset areas of the loop. For
been reported previously for the same tracking task example, diazepam impairment at 25 min is nine times the
following each of three different doses of diazepam level at 210 min, yet serum concentrations for both are
(Ellinwood et al. 1983a), demonstrating that the diazepam around 500 ng/ml. The KATCis an indicator of a late phase
acute tolerance profile is reproducible across dose levels9 of acute tolerance. The KATC values for diazepam and
Several components of the hysteresis loop are important in alprazolam quantitatively represent the faster effect offset
the formulation of the contributions to acute tolerance9 The as compared to the serum level offset. Both drugs thus

160

150 I

140 200.
i 110
130
100,
120 -

LiO - 160,
100 - 80, 15
T
R lqO
90- fl
C
B0 - I
M 120,
r.
70-
0
s
60- L 100, lS
T
Z5
$
C 90.
~~ 0
I
E
3O $
/9 ~0.
20'
q0.

0--
++go%+,~........ +" ""' 20,
-10 sTo.--. / .," /
-20
., 9 , .... J . . . . . . . . . + . . . . . . . . . i . . . . . . , 9 + . . . . . . . + . . . . . . . . , . . . . . . . . . ,

IO0 200 300 qO0 500 600 700 800 gO0 10 15 20 2S 30


A SERUM DIflZEPflH CONCENTRRTION CNG/ML) B SERUNALPRAZOLAMCONCENTRATION(NG/ML)

275 -
IP
/
110"
250.

225 9

2~ -
T
R

C 175-
K
!
N
S ISO-
D
s
L }.25
T +
fl
330
+ I00,
C
0
R
SE 75,
Fig. 3. Hysteresis curves of subcritical tracking performance versus
drug serum concentration for diazepam (A), alprazolam (B), and
lorazepam (C). Impairment is expressed as unit changes from the
predrug level (delta score). Mean values of both actual (curve A)
%;'~ and predicted (P) scores are shown. Predicted values are based on
the KATCmodel. Points represent testing time periods, which are
0 S 10 J$ ~0 2$ 30 35 q~ q~ $0 S$ indicated as number of minutes post ingestion. Arrows denote the
C SERUN LOP&ZEPANCONCE~ITP.ATIOtl(HG/NL) sequence of the observations
398

induce a very rapid initial offset of tracking impairment that phase in our data may indicate that receptor association
is not reflected in the shallower slope of the drug level fall rate constants m a y be involved significantly in the early
off. Between the onset and offset arms of the hysteresis process. In addition, the finding that for the SCT task the
loop there is a plateau period lasting 4 5 - 6 0 min for both diazepam KATC was three times that of lorazepam is
drug concentration level and effect. consistent with the Jack et al. (1983) hypothesis. However,
The alprazolam hysteresis curve does not fit classical there is no direct data to support this hypothesis.
notions about hysteresis since the delay in the onset drug The comparison between lorazepam and alprazolam
effect produces a counterclockwise hysteresis and the fast indicates that the prolonged effect of the former is not
offset of effect is consistent with a clockwise hysteresis simply a function of relative binding affinity (Kaplan and
indicating acute tolerance. Arendt et al. (1983)found in Jack 1983). Both drugs have similar binding affinity
dogs a late (28 min) latency time to peak cerebrospinal fluid (Baestrup and Nielsen 1983), but our findings demonstrated
(CSF) concentations for alprazolam in contrast to 3 min for a much shorter duration of impairment for alprazolam.
diazepam and 7 min for lorazepam. If confirmed, this lag in Similarly, ex vivo binding studies have shown that diazepam
central nervous system (CNS) entry for alprazolam may has a dissociation rate that is about nine times that of
prove to be the reason for the abrupt rapid onset of lorazepam (Jack et al. 1983). The structure of lorazepam, as
alprazolam-induced impairment following an initial onset well as triazolam and clonazepam, includes a chloride ion on
delay of 1 5 - 2 0 min. The alprazolam offset KATC, calcu- the ortho position of the ,,C" ring, which has been associated
lated from time periods when the equilibrium between with the very high affinity of these benzodiazepines and may
serum and CSF should be established, is very similar to that account for a specific long-lasting lorazepam effect.
of diazepam and probably reflects receptor kinetics or Recently emerging evidence has fostered the hypotheti-
adaptation. cal position that various areas of the CNS have differential
The counterclockwise hysteresis loop manifested with distributions of at least two benzodiazepine receptors with
lorazepam indicates either a reverse tolerance (sensitiza- differential binding and dissociation as a function of time,
tion) or the presence of a drug equilibrium delay between detergent solubilization, temperature, interaction with
the plasma and CNS receptor sites. However, an equili- triazolopyridazines, G A B A , and the presence of chloride
brium delay as the only contributor to the reverse tolerance ion (Dubnick et al. 1983; Lo et al. 1982; Squires et al. 1979).
is doubtful for this lipid-soluble drug, since Arendt et al. In contrast, on the basis of their research Gee and
(1983) have demonstrated peak CSF levels in dogs 7 min Yamamura (1982) and Chin and Rosenberg (1982) have
after intravenous dosing. Furthermore, while the onset suggested an alternative explanation, namely that there is a
hysteresis limb for lorazepam is counterclockwise, the single benzodiazepine receptor population with at least two
offset slope demonstrates no evidence of reverse tolerance. conformations. Thus, the hypothesis of multiple benzod-
In fact, the small positive KAXCconstant is one-third that of iazepine receptor subtypes or conformations with differ-
diazepam or alprazolam. Finally, although the desmethyl ential dissociation rates represent one possible explanation
metabolite of diazepam was the only active metabolite for the findings of fast and slow offset slopes for the
measured, there is no evidence in the literature that other different tasks.
metabolites of any of the three drugs contribute to the early Squires et al. (1979) have described two components of
acute effects. 3H-diazepam dissociation with half-lives of 2 and 9 min
The recovery curve for DSS is characterized by a late after addition of either diazepam or flunitrazepam. When
substantially slow offset component. The contrast to the CL-218-872, a 3-triazolopyridazine (a BZ1 type) anxiolytic,
tracking recovery profile was distinct for diazepam and is added, the fast component of the 3H-diazepam disso-
alprazolam and the KATCconstants for all three drugs were ciation is unaffected while the slow displacement compo-
below 0.14 h -1. The DSS task incorporates components of nent becomes much slower, suggesting to these authors that
learning and memory as well as psychomotor speed CL-218-872 has a lower affinity to "slow receptors" than
(Heatherly et al. in preparation), whereas tracking reflects "fast receptors." Recently, Reeves and Schweizer (1983)
mainly coordination ability. Elsewhere we have reported found evidence that in young rats acute treatment with
that the learning component of the DSS displays a more diazepam results in a 147% increase in BZ 1 (fast) receptor
prolonged impairment offset than the memory portion density while BZ2 (slow) receptors remain unchanged.
(Heatherly et al. in preparation). The data raise the intrigu- Thus, an acute adaptation of the BZ1 receptor type could be
ing question as to what mechanisms may be responsible for involved in diazepam's short pharmacodynamic half-life.
the differential profiles of the benzodiazepine effect demon- Dubnick et al. (1983) has also reported greater distribution
strated by the different types of tasks. of BZ1 receptors in the cerebellum in contrast to increased
Since it is difficult to conceive of a differential receptor distribution of BZ2 receptors in the hippocampus and
or effector mechanism operating for these drugs, the cortex. Thus, differential rate of recovery for learn-
present data are more consistent with the hypothesis of ing-memory versus coordination skills could be related to
differences in receptor association-dissociation rate con- anatomical distribution of receptor types. Although these
stants as recently proposed by Jack et al. (1983)~ These hypotheses are currently untestable in man, they do
authors argued that the pharmacodynamic half-life of highlight the point that receptor kinetics need to be
benzodiazepine may relate to the pharmacokinetic profile considered in the pharmacodynamic profiles of benzodia-
as well as drug differences in association-dissociation zepines.
receptor rate constants; they conclude on a basis of
differences in ex vivo binding studies that diazepam has a Acknowledgements. This research was supported by NIDA grant
dissociation rate approximately ten times faster than DA01883.
lorazepam. That SCT impairment for diazepam leads and
lorazepam lags serum drug concentration during the onset
399

References Holford NH, Sheiner LB (1981) Pharmacokinetic and pharma-


codynamic modeling in vivo. Crit Rev Biomed Eng
Arendt RM, Greenbaltt D J, DeJong RH, Bonin JD, Abemethy 5: 273- 322
DR, Ehrenberg BL, Gwynne Giles H, Sellers EM, Shader RI Jack ML, Corborn WA, Spirt NM, Bautz G, Zanki M, Horst W,
(1983) In vitro correlates of benzodiazepine cerebrospinal fluid O'Brien RA (1983) A pharmacokinetic/pharmacodynamic
uptake, pharmacodynamic action and peripheral distribution. J receptor binding model to predict the onset and duration of
Pharmacol Exp Ther 227: 98-106 pharmacological activity of the benzodiazepines. Prog Neu-
Braestrup C, Nielsen M (1983) Benzodiazepine receptors. In: ropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 7 : 5 - 6
Iversen LL, Iversen SD, Snyder SH (eds) Handbook of Johnson RF, Schenker RK, Roberts RK, Desmond PV, Wilkinson
psychopharmacology: Biochemical studies of CNS recpetors. GR (1979) Plasma binding of benzodiazepines in humans. J
Plenum, New York, pp 285-384 Pharm Sci 68:1320-1322
Chiu TH, Rosenberg AC (1982) Comparison of the kinetics of Kaplan SA, Jack ML (1983) Metabolism of the benzodiazepines:
[H-3]-labeled diazepam and [H-3]-labeled flunitrazepam bind- Pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic considerations. In:
ing to cortical synaptosomal membranes. J Neurochem Costa E (ed) Benzodiazepines: From molecular biology to
39:1716-1725 clinical practice. Raven, New York, pp 173-199
Dubnick B, Lippa AS, Klepner CA, Coupet J, Greenblatt EN, Lo MMS, Trifelleti RR, Snyder SH (1982) Physical separation and
Beer B (1983) The separation of 3H-benzodiazepine binding characterization of two central benzodiazepine receptors. In:
sites in brain and of benzodiazepine pharmacological proper- Usdin E (ed) Pharmacology of the benzodiazepines. MacMil-
ties. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 13:311-318 lan, New York, pp 165-173
Ellinwood EH Jr, Linnoila M, Easler ME, Molter DW (1983a) MacLeod SM, Giles HG, Patzalek G, Thiessen JJ, Sellers EM
Profile of acute tolerance to three sedative anxiolytics. (1977) Diazepam actions and plasma concentrations following
Psychopharmacology 79: 137-141 ethanol ingestion. Eur J Clin Pharmacot 11:345-349
Ellinwood EH Jr, Easier ME, Linnoila M, Molter DW, Heatherly Palva ES, Linnoila M (1978) Effect of active metabolites of
DG, Bjornsson TD (1983b) Effects of oral contraceptives on chlordiazepoxide and diazepam, alone or in combination with
diazepam-induced impairment of psychomotor activity in alcohol, on psychomotor skills related to driving. Eur J Clin
young females. Clin Pharmacol Ther 35:360-366 Pharmacol 13 : 345-350
Gee KW, Yamamura HI (1982) Benzodiazepine receptor hete- Reeves PM, Schweizer MP (1983) Aging, diazepam exposure and
rogeneity: A consequence of multiple conformational states of benzodiazepine receptors in rat cortex. Brain Res
a single receptor or multiple population of structurally distinct 270: 376-379
macromolecules. In: Usdin E (ed) Pharmacology of the Seppala T, Leino T, Linnoila M, Huttunen M, Ylikahri R (1976)
benzodiazepines. MacMillan, New York, pp 93-108 Effects of hangover on psychomotor skills related to driving:
George KA, Dundee JW (1977) Relative amnesic actions o f Modification by fructose and glucose. Acta Pharrnacol Toxicol
diazepam, fiunitrazepam and lorazepam in man. Br J Clin 38: 209-218
9 Pharmacol 4 : 4 5 - 5 0 Skinner TJ III (1979) A very sensitive gas chromatographic
Gibaldi M, Perrier D (1975) Pharmacokinetics. Dekker, New analytical procedure for the amitriptyline and nortriptyline
York tricyclic antidepressants. MS Thesis, Department of Pharma-
Greenblatt D J, Franke K, Shader RI (1978) Analysis of lorazepam cology, University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
and its glucuronide metabolite by electron-capture gas-liquid
Squires RF, Benson D, Braestrup C, Coupet J, Klepner CA,
chromatography. J Chromatogr 146:311-320
Myers V, Beer B (1979) Some properties of brain specific
Greenblatt D J, Ochs HR, Lloyd BL (1980) Entry of diazepam and
benzodiazepine receptors. Pharmacot Biochem Behav
its major metabolite into cerebrospinal fluid. Psychopharma- 10: 825-830
cology 70:89-93
Greenbaltt DJ, Divoll M, Moschitto LJ, Shader RI (1981)
Electron-capture gas chromatographic analysis of the triazo-
lobenzodiazepines, alprazolam and triazolam. J Chromatogr
225: 202-207
Greenblatt D J, Divoll M, Abernethy DR, Ochs HR, Shader RI
(1983) Clinical pharmacokinetics of the newer benzodiaze-
pines. Clin Pharmacokinet 8:233-252 Received August 27, 1984; Final version November 27, 1984

You might also like