You are on page 1of 7

Linguistic Analysisof Speech

in Affective Disorders
Nancy J. C. Andreasen, MD, PhD, Bruce Pfohl

\s=b\ Various aspects of speech and language were compared, these investigations are of two types: those that attempt to
using psycholinguistic techniques, in a group of 15 depressed examine formal elements of speech and writing such as
patients and 16 manic patients: lexical diversity, syntactical grammar and syntax, and those that attempt to examine
complexity, syntactical elements, and content analysis. Contrary aspects of content such as vocabulary word choice.
to anticipation, the manic patients did not show more varied
word choice or complexity of sentence structure than the depres- Analyses of form have grown increasingly sophisticated.
sives. In particular, they did not differ significantly in type-token
Newman and Mather1 made an early effort to examine
ratio. The greatest difference was in syntactical elements, with speech in patients suffering from mania or depression by
manics using more action verbs, adjectives, and concrete nouns, studying such variables as pitch, tempo, syntax, and
while the depressed patients used more state of being verbs, latency of response. Their results are largely descriptive,
modifying adverbs, first-person pronouns, and personal however, and provide no quantitative data. Lorenz and
pronouns. When compared by content analysis, the manics used Cobb have compared the speech of manies to that of
more words reflecting a concern with power and achievement. normals by using more rigorous quantitative methods such
These results imply that depressive speech tends to be more as determining the distributions of parts of speech or the
vague and qualified and to show considerable self-preoccupa- number of words spoken per minute.- Manies were found to
tion, while manic speech tends to be colorful and concrete and to have significantly different speech patterns. Lorenz and
show more concern with things than with people.
Cobb have also examined speech in neurotic patients.'·'
(Arch Gen Psychiatry 33:1361-1367, 1976) Determination of the verb-adjective (V-A) ratio was
developed in an effort to find a quantitative method of
patients' relating grammatical forms to psychopathology. Originally
Psychiatrists thought,
content of
frequently speak
disorder. Too often we
forget
stream of
about their
thought, or thought
that our only way of learning
developed by Busemann,'' it was considered to be an "action
quotient" or an index of emotional instability. Although
about a patient's thoughts is through his speech and the measure was developed for use in children, a variety of
language, either spoken or written. Spoken or written investigators have applied it to psychiatric patients. Fair¬
language may not be an accurate or reliable index of banks" and Mann7 have found schizophrenics to have a
thought, even when it is supposed to approximate it closely higher V-A quotient than normals, while Lorenz and Cobb-
as in free association, but is the only index we have have found manies and hysterics to have a higher quotient
available. Given this fact, it is something of an oddity that than normals. Benton et al8 obtained a higher quotient in
so much has been written by psychologists and psychia¬ people with high anxiety as compared to low anxiety, while
trists about thought and so little about language. Balken and Masserman" found anxiety states to have a
Linguistics is a relatively new field, of course, and higher quotient in comparison with conversion hysterics.
psycholinguistics is even newer. Nevertheless, a number of Very few studies have been able to replicate the actual
investigators have attempted to apply linguistic figures of the others, however; for example, the quotient in
techniques to a variety of psychiatric disorders. Basically, normals has been variously reported as 1.07, 1.98, and 3.43.
Such variability is probably due to differences in definition
of terms and in sample collection.
Accepted for publication Dec 19, 1975.
From the Department of Psychiatry, University of Iowa College of Spreen and Wachal have attempted to refine methodol¬
Medicine, Iowa City. ogy in syntactical analysis still further by describing a
Reprint requests to Department of Psychiatry, University of Iowa
Hospitals and Clinic, 500 Newton Rd, Iowa City, IA 52242 (Dr precise method of collecting and transcribing speech
Andreasen). samples and developing sophisticated statistical methods

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Manitoba User on 06/19/2015


for data analysis.'"'- Their techniques, developed for use in writing have been developed and applied over the years,
aphasies (perhaps the most difficult group for linguistic with somewhat varied results. With the possible exception
analysis), are too new to be widely replicated as yet. of Weintraub, no one has succeeded in developing a method
Other investigators have attacked the problem of de¬ that can be consistently repeated in a range of psychiatric
scribing and differentiating speech patterns by examining patients, and no one has as yet attempted to replicate
aspects of content rather than form. Perhaps the oldest Weintraub's data. Yet enough has been done to suggest
and simplest technique is the type-token ratio (TTR), that psycholinguistic studies are a potentially rich method
originally developed by Wendell Johnson as a way of for exploring language and ultimately thought in
describing vocabulary size of "lexical diversity."'1'4 This psychiatric patients.
measure is determined by dividing the total number of The present investigation was undertaken as a pilot
words (or tokens) into the total number of different words study. The overall plan was to examine a number of
(or types). This variable has been examined most often in linguistic variables. Based on the research just reviewed,
schizophrenia, with some indications that schizophrenics four different areas were selected for investigation: lexical
may have a lower TTR than normals. The range of values diversity, syntactical complexity, frequency of types of
has differed widely from study to study, however, with a syntactical elements, and content analysis. In order to
low of .26 and a high of .74."'7·'3 As Wachal and Spreen have increase precision, only variables that could be analyzed
pointed out, the TTR tends to be highly dependent on quantitatively were selected.
sample length and whether the sample is spoken or writ¬ METHOD
ten.
Other investigators have attempted to develop methods Within three to five days after admission, speech samples were
that focus more closely on aspects of content that may give obtained from 16 inpatients suffering from mania and 15 inpa-
a better index of actual cognitive style. Weintraub and tients suffering from depression. Although some patients were
Aronson""1 have developed a method of using word choice receiving medication at the time of testing, all were fully sympto¬
and phrase choice as an index of various psychological matic. Consecutive admissions were used, but only patients who
met previously defined diagnostic criteria (the Research Diag¬
defense mechanisms. This method identifies a variety of nostic Criteria) were included.-'' These criteria for mania and
factors considered to be of psychological significance in
depression are as follows:
psychiatric disorders: qualifying phrases and words, nega¬ For manic disorder, A through E are required for the episode of
tors, retractors, expressions of feeling, value judgments, illness being considered:
etc. Using these techniques, Weintraub and Aronson have A. One or more distinct periods with a predominantly elevated
found differences between normal controls and patients or irritable mood. The elevated or irritable mood must be a

suffering from a wide range of psychiatric disorders: prominent part of the illness and relatively persistent, although it
delusional behavior, impulsive behavior, compulsive behav¬ may alternate with depressive mood. If the manic symptom only
occurs during periods of alcohol or drug intake or withdrawal from
ior, and depression. Gottschalk and associates--"1 have
attempted to examine interpersonal and emotional aspects them, it should not be considered here.
B. If mood is elevated, at least three of the following symptom
of content by developing an elaborate weighted scoring
categories must be definitely present to a significant degree (four
system designed to evaluate anxiety, social alienation, if mood is only irritable) (for past episodes, because of memory
uncertainty, and feelings of being controlled. They have difficulty, one less symptom is required):
developed this primarily as a method of defining and 1. More active than usual—either socially, at work, sexually—or
differentiating schizophrenic thought. physically restless
The methods of Weintraub and Gottschalk and col¬ 2. More talkative than usual or felt a pressure to keep talking
leagues require individual scoring of phrases and clauses in 3. Flight of ideas or subjective experience that thoughts are
transcribed samples of speech. Thus they are necessarily racing
4. Inflated self-esteem (grandiosity, which may be delusional)
time-consuming and, to some extent, dependent on the 5. Decreased need for sleep
subjective judgment of the scorer. Other experiments in 6. Distractability, ie, attention is too easily drawn to unimpor¬
content analysis have developed as a way of surmounting
tant or irrelevant external stimuli
these problems. Several investigators have attempted to 7. Excessive involvement in activities without recognizing the
analyze content by developing "dictionaries" or sets of high potential for painful consequences, eg, buying sprees, sexual
word categories that group together words considered to indiscretions, foolish business investments, reckless driving
share a similar psychological significance.-4 For example, C. Overall disturbance is so severe that at least one of the
all words dealing with the concept of failure would be following is present:
enumerated under this category. After a series of these 1. Meaningful conversation is impossible
dictionaries have been developed and their reliability and 2. Serious impairment socially, with family, at home, or at
validity determined, they can be used to analyze vocabu¬ work
3. In the absence of No. 1 or 2, hospitalization
lary content through more accurate and efficient comput¬ D. Duration of manic features at least one week (or any
erized methods. Such computerized content analysis has duration if hospitalized).
been used to analyze a wide variety of speech samples,
E. None of the following, which suggest schizophrenia is pres¬
ranging from the political pronouncements of foreign ent:
statesmen to spontaneous productions by schizophrenics. 1. Delusions of being controlled or of thought broadcasting,
Its application to psychiatric populations has, however, insertion, withdrawal
or
been quite limited to date. 2. Hallucinations of any type throughout the day for several
Thus, a variety of methods for analyzing speech and days or intermittently throughout a one-week period, unless all of

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Manitoba User on 06/19/2015


the content is clearly related to depression or elation 6. Definite instances of formal thought disorder
3. Auditory hallucinations in which either a voice keeps up a Using these criteria, kappa coefficients of interrater reliability
running commentary on the patient's behavior as it occurs, or two have been calculated at .90 for manic disorder and .85 for major
or more voices converse with each other depressive disorder. The manies and depressives did differ slightly
4. At some time during the period of illness had delusions or in age or educational background. The manies ranged in age from
hallucinations for more than one week in the absence of prominent 16 to 64 years, with a mean of 33, and they had a mean of 12.6 years
affective (depressed or manic) symptoms of schooling. The depressives had 10.7 years of schooling, and they
5. At some time during the period of illness had more than one ranged from 16 to 76 years old, with a mean of 41. Nine of the
week when he exhibited no prominent manic symptoms but had manic patients were female, while 13 of the depressed group were
several instances of formal thought disorder female.
For major depressive disorder, A through E are required for the The speech samples consisted of a mixture of responses to
episode of illness being considered: proverb interpretation and spontaneous free speech in response to
A. Dysphoric mood characterized by symptoms such as the questions such as "What are you like?" or "Tell me about your
following: depressed, sad, blue, hopeless, low, down in the dumps, family." The proverbs were taken from a standard list in the
"don't care anymore," irritable, worried. The mood disturbance Mental Examiner's Handbook.-" The first 80 words of proverb
must be prominent and relatively persistent but not necessarily interpretation and the first 320 words of spontaneous speech were
the most dominant symptom. It does not include momentary shifts used for analysis. All speech samples were tape-recorded and later
from one dysphoric mood to another dysphoric mood, eg, anxiety transcribed.
to depression to anger, such as are seen in states of acute psychotic After transcription, each word in the sample was coded with a
turmoil. designation for part of speech and each sentence was coded for
B. At least five of the following symptoms are required to have sentence type, using a method worked out by one of the investi¬
appeared as part of the episode for definite and four for probable gators (N.J.C.A.) and available on request. Verbs were, for exam¬
(for past episodes, because of memory difficulty, one less symptom ple, classified into either action or state-of-being verbs and also by-
is required): tense into past, present, and future. Nouns were divided into
1. Poor appetite or weight loss or increased appetite or weight abstract (qualities such as beauty, truth) and concerte (objects
gain (change of 0.5 kg a week over several weeks or 4.5 kg a year such as book, street). Pronouns were divided into six classes: three
when not dieting) classes of personal pronouns (first, second, third person), and also
2. Sleep difficulty or sleeping too much relative (who, which, etc), demonstrative (this, that, etc), and
3. Loss of energy, fatigability, or tiredness indefinite (any, both, etc) pronouns. Adjectives were divided into
4. Psychomotor agitation or retardation (but not mere subjec¬ three types according to degree (positive, comparative, and
tive feeling of restlessness or being slowed down) superlative), and a fourth group was designated as indefinite
5. Loss of interest or pleasure in usual activities, including adjectives (a, the, this). Adverbs were divided into four groups:
social contact or sex (do not include if limited to a period when descriptive (forcefully, fast, etc), interrogative (how, when, etc),
delusional or hallucinating) modifying (rather, very, etc), and indicators of time and place
6. Feelings of self-reproach or excessive or inappropriate guilt (now, again, here, etc). Conjunctions were divided into coordinat¬
(either may be delusional) ing (and, or, etc), subordinating (because, if, etc), and transitional
7. Complaints or evidence of diminished ability to think or (accordingly, moreover, etc). Prepositions were not divided into
concentrate, such as slow thinking, or indecisiveness (do not subclasses. Thus, grammatical elements were divided into seven
include if associated with obvious formal thought disorder) major classes, which were further subdivided into 26 subclasses. In
8. Recurrent thoughts of death or suicide, or any suicidal an effort to examine possible differences in sentence structure,

behavior each sentence was also coded to indicate sentence type: simple,
C. Dysphoric features of illness lasting at least one week. compound, complex, compound-complex, and fragment. This
Definite if lasted more than two weeks, probable if one to two method of coding also permitted an examination of sentence
weeks. length.
D. Sought or was referred for help from someone during the Several speech samples were coded independently by each of the
dysphoric period or had impaired functioning socially, with family, investigators. Since codings differed on only 1.5% of the words,
at home, or at work. this was considered to represent suitable interrater reliability.
E. None of the following, which suggest schizophrenia is pres¬ Thereafter coding was divided between the two investigators,
ent: with each taking half the manic sample and half the depressed
1. Delusions of being controlled or of thought broadcasting, sample in order to randomize any systematic error that might
insertion, withdrawal
or occur.
2. Hallucinations of any type throughout the day for several Other methods were used to examine aspects of speech content,
days or intermittently throughout a one-week period, unless all of such as vocabulary and word choice, as opposed to the above
the content is clearly related to depression or elation syntactical aspects. At the simplest level, richness or convention¬
3. Auditory hallucinations in which either a voice keeps up a ality of vocabulary was examined by determining what
running commentary on the patient's behaviors or thoughts as percentage of words used by the patients were among the 1,000
they occur, or two or more voices converse with each other. most commonly used words as determined by the Lorge-Thorndike
4. At some time during the period of illness had delusions or Dictionary. Which particular words were used most frequently by
hallucinations for more than one month in the absence of promi¬ each subject was also determined. In addition, the lexical diversity
nent affective (manic or depressive) symptoms (although typical of each subject was determined by calculating the type-token
depressive delusions, such as delusions of guilt, sin, poverty, ratio. The number of negators (no, not, never, etc) and the number
nihilism, or self-deprecation or hallucinations of similar content of qualifiers (perhaps, if, maybe, etc) were also determined for
are permitted) each subject, using a standard predetermined list.
5. Preoccupation with a delusion or hallucination to the relative In an attempt to achieve a more sophisticated approach to
exclusion of other symptoms or concerns (other than delusions or vocabulary and word choice, techniques of content analysis were
guilt, sin, poverty, nihilism, or self-deprecation or hallucinations also used. The most recent revision of the Harvard Psychosociolog-
with similar content) ical Dictionary, the Harvard IV-3, was obtained from Phillip

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Manitoba User on 06/19/2015


Table 1.—Lexical Diversity in Depressives and Manies

Depressives ( 15) Manies ( =


16)

Lexical Diversity Mean SD Mean SD


No. of 1,000 most common words 338.13 15.07 330.88 11.37 NS
Type-token ratio .4098 .0333 .4280 .0259 2.63 NS

Table 2.—Syntactical Complexity in Depressives and Manies

Depressives ( 15) Manies ( =


16)

Mean SD Mean SD
No. of sentences
Simple 13.27 8.00 13.31 8.44 0.02 NS
Compound 280 1.90 2.13 1.15 1.16 NS
Complex 9.53 3.44 8.19 3.19 1.09 NS
Compound-complex 3.20 1.37 3.81 2.23 0.88 NS
No. of fragments 4.20 2.48 3.69 2.68 0.53 NS
No. of sentences in 400-word sample 33.00 9.54 31.13 10.62 0.50 NS

Stone and his associates. This dictionary divides 8,700 words or d. Personalization: It was predicted that manies would show
word senses into 170 content categories. Since using all 8,700 word more preoccupation with themselves, as reflected in increased use
senses and 170 content categories was thought to be too large an of first-person
pronouns.
undertaking for a pilot study of this type, 28 categories were Time orientation: It was predicted that depressives would be
e.
selected as being of possible relevance to affective disturbance. more preoccupied with the past, as reflected by the use of more
These categories contained words that tend to denote or connote past-tense verbs and more adverbs referring to time and place.
such ideas or feelings as achievement, failure, abandonment, /. Concrete vs abstract usage: It was predicted that manies
hostility, submission, strength, weakness, virtue, vice, disorder, or would be more concrete and depressives more abstract, as
negation. Each of these categories contained from 10 to 37 words. reflected by manies using more concrete nouns referring to places
The specific 28 categories used were: INCR, IMPLNEG, HOST, and things and depressives using more abstract nouns referring to
FEEL, ABANDON, EXPRS, EXERT, EVAL, EMOT, DISORDR, qualities or ideas.
DECR, CAUSAL, CHANGE, ACTV 3, ACH, FALL, WEAK 3, g. Verb-adjective ratio: This can be calculated as a verb-
VIRTUE, VICE, UNDRST, SUBM, STRNG 3, SOLVE, QUALIF, adjective, verb-noun, or verb-noun plus adjective ratio. If a higher
POWER, OVRST, NGTV 3, INDEF. ratio is associated with greater emotional instability, one would
Speech samples were transferred to computer cards, as were the expect a higher ratio in manic patients.
Lorge-Thorndike Dictionary and the 28 categories for content h. State of being-Action Verb Ratio: It was predicted that
analysis. The various frequency counts of word choice, and syntac¬ depressives would have a higher ratio, which would serve as an
tical elements, and vocabulary were all determined by computer¬ index of anergy.
ized analysis, using either stock programs locally available or 4. Content analysis: It was predicted that manies would use
programs written by one of the investigators. (B.P.) more words from the groups dealing with concepts such as
These data were used to test a number of hypotheses concerning strength or achievement, while depressives would choose words
several variables in structure and content of speech in affective reflecting a concern with weakness or failure.
disorder.
1. Lexical diversity: It was predicted that manies would have RESULTS
greater lexical diversity as reflected in higher type-token ratios The results of data analysis illustrate quite well how
and more words not occurring in the Lorge-Thorndike. difficult it is to predict language behavior on the basis of
2. Syntactical complexity: It was predicted that manies would
tend to use longer sentences and employ more complicated syntax
clinical experience. Subjective impressions were not always
as reflected in more compound, complex, and compound-complex supported by objective information, and a considerable
sentences and fewer simple sentences. It was also anticipated that number of the hypotheses were not proved.
they would use more fragments. As shown in Table 1, the depressives and manies did not
3. Frequency of syntactical elements: These were subdivided differ in lexical diversity. Although the manies did have a
into several groups that might reflect similar aspects of speech. slightly higher type-token ratio, suggesting a wider range
a. Vagueness: It was predicted that depressives would show of word choice, this was not statistically significant. Like¬
more vagueness as reflected by an increase in such syntactical
elements as indefinite adjectives, state of being verbs, and
wise, the manies used slightly fewer "commonly used"
relative, demonstrative, and indefinite pronouns.
words, as represented by the Lorge-Thorndike Dictionary,
but the difference was not statistically significant.
6. Colorfulness of speech: It was predicted that manies would
have more colorful or emotional speech, as reflected in more
As Table 2 demonstrates, the manies also did not show a
adverbs, adjectives, nouns, and action verbs. significant difference in syntactical complexity. Both
c. Use of qualifiers: It was predicted that depressives would manies and depressives used similar numbers of simple
tend to qualify their statements more often, as reflected by the sentences, and this tended to be the most common sentence
use of more modifying adverbs. type in both groups. They also did not differ significantly

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Manitoba User on 06/19/2015


Table 3.—Frequency of Syntactical Elements in Manies and Depressives
Depressives ( = 15) Manies ( =16)

Mean SD Mean SD
Vagueness
Indefinite adjectives 15.13 4.56 18.38 5.54 1.72 NS
Relative demonstrative and indefinite pronouns 25.60 6.63 24.38 5.73 0.53 NS
State of being verbs 61.13 13.35 49.44 9.45 2.74 .02
Colorfulness
Adverbs 46.60 8.25 39.44 7.19 2.50 .02
Action verbs 45.93 16.20 57.63 8.17 2.48 02
Adjectives 40.40 11.27 48.44 8.27 2.20 .05
Concreteness
Concrete nouns 31.20 7.35 43.94 11.73 3.48 .01
Abstract nouns 11.73 6.18 13.59 6.99 0.72 NS
Nouns 42.93 7.01 57.44 9.99 4.60 .001
Qualifiers
Modifying adverb 30.87 8.92 21.56 5.19 3.46 .01
Personalization
First-person pronouns 38.87 7.08 31.69 5.31 3.10 .01
Personal pronouns 69.93 9.17 61.50 11.06 2.25 .05
Time orientation
Past-tense verbs 24.53 14.35 17.81 14.43 1.26 NS
Adverb Indicating time 10.40 4.29 13.81 1.82 NS

in the numbers of compound, compound-complex, or difference significant at the .05 level. They used more past-
complex sentences used, and both used nearly the same tense verbs and therefore showed some preoccupation with
number of fragments. Depressives tended to use slightly the past, but the results did not reach statistical signifi¬
shorter sentences, but again the difference was not signif¬ cance, and they did not use significantly more adverbs
icant. referring to time and place.
Table 3 shows the frequency of syntactical elements in Table 4 contains a series of ratios that were calculated to
the two patient groups, and in this area the manies and permit comparison of our data with those of other studies.
depressives were often different, although not always in Each of these ratios is statistically significant, although
the direction predicted. As hypothesized, depressed slightly less so than if the various syntactical elements are
patients tended to show more vagueness, although only on compared singly. Contrary to prediction, the depressives
one index. They did not use significantly more indefinite have a higher verb-adjective ratio than the manies. They
adjectives or relative, demonstrative, and indefinite also have a higher verb-noun and verb-noun plus adjective
pronouns, but they did use significantly more state-of- ratios. These differences are significant at the .05, .02, and
being verbs at the .02 level. Manies, on the other hand, did .02 levels, respectively. If a ratio of state of being-action
tend to have more colorful language. Their use of action verbs is constructed, then the depressed patients also are
verbs and adjectives was greater at the .05 and .02 levels, higher at the .05 level. This index is probably relevant
respectively, but the depressed patients did use more because it indicates that the verb-adjective ratio cannot
adverbs with a difference significant at the .02 level. truly be considered an action quotient, since it does not
The issue of concrete vs abstract usage is somewhat reflect a high use of action verbs among the depressed
obscure. The manic patients tended to use considerably patients.
more nouns, and they therefore used both more concrete Table 5 reports the results of content analysis, which
nouns and more abstract nouns than the depressed were statistically significant. As expected, manies tended
patients. The difference in abstract nouns is not statisti¬ to choose words that reflected concern with power and
cally significant, however, while the differences in nouns achievement, but somewhat surprisingly the depressives
generally and in concrete nouns are significant at the .001 were higher in the overstatement category. Since the total
and .01 levels, respectively. The manies, therefore, were number of dictionaries examined was 28 and since only
definitely more concrete. three were found to differentiate the two groups signifi¬
As predicted, the depressed patients tended to qualify cantly, the differentiation may be purely on the basis of
their statements often, as reflected in their use of
more chance.
more qualifying adverbs, with the difference significant at Table 6 compares the results of our analysis of manic and
the .01 level. On the other hand, the hypotheses concerning depressive speech expressed in percentages with that of
personalization and time reference were not supported by Lorenz and Cobb. Since they reported their data in
the data. The depressed patients, rather than the manies, percentages and did not report means and standard devia¬
tended to show a greater preoccupation with themselves, as tions, statistical comparison is not possible, but the results
reflected in their greater use of the first person pronoun, for manic patients are strikingly similar and suggest that
with the difference significant at the .01 level. They also data of this type can be replicated. Their patient sample
tended to use more personal pronouns generally, with the size was smaller (ten) and their word sample size larger

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Manitoba User on 06/19/2015


Table 4.—Syntactical Ratios ¡n Manies and Depressives
Depressives ( =15) Manies ( =
16)
Ratio Mean SD Mean SD
Verb-adjective 2.88 0.92 2.29 0.51 2.18 .05
Verb-noun 2.57 0.56 1.93 0.44 2.47 .02
Verb-noun + adjective 1.33 0.30 1.04 0.20 3.18 .02
State of being-action verb 1.72 1.35 0.89 0.26 2.36 .05

Table 5—Content Analysis pretation and the relatively short length of the sponta¬
neous speech samples may well have produced an artifac-
Depressives Manies tual homogeneity in word choice and sentence type. Subse¬
(N 15)
=
(N = 16)
quent investigations in this area should probably be
Mean SD Mean SD limited to "spontaneous speech" only and increase the word
Power (18*) 9.26 2.67 13.19 3.86 3.17 .02 sample to at least 500 words per patient and possibly 1,000
Overstatement (19) 17.53 5.48 12.00 4.83 2.88 .02 words. Most of the content analysis dictionaries consist of
Achievement (37) 7.53 2.75 10.19 3.15 2.42 .05 20 to 30 words, and a 400-word sample is probably not long
'Numbers in parentheses are the number of words In the category.
enough to assess the use of this relatively small number of
words. Further, subsequent studies should probably control
for verbal intelligence quotient rather than using consecu¬
Table 6.—Syntactical Elements in tive admissions, since vocabulary size and intelligence may
Two Studies of Speech in Affective Disorders well play a significant role in syntactical complexity and
lexical diversity. The higher educational level of the manies
% of Words
may have distorted results in some way, although neither
Lorenz & Cobb2 Andreasen & Pfohl group is of a very high educational level.
(Current Study)
Nevertheless, manic and depressed patients did differ
Normals Manies Manies Depressives from one another substantial number of variables
on a
Nouns 16.7 14.9 13.3 9.9 probably not related to education. The
analysis of syntac¬
Adjectives 12.2 9.6 7.0 5.9 tical elements was particularly useful in distinguishing
Adverbs 12.5 11.7 9.1 10.8 between the two groups, and several working hypotheses
Verbs 16 8 22 3 24.8 24.8 were validated. The speech of the depressives was charac¬
Pronouns 13.8 18.2 19.9 22.1 terized by the use of more state of being verbs, adverbs,
Prepositions 10.4 8.3 5.6 5.9 modifying adverbs, first-person pronouns, and personal
Conjunctions 8.9 7.5 8 1 8.9 pronouns, while the manies used significantly more action
Articles 6.4 5.9 5.5 3.5 verbs, adjectives, concrete nouns, and nouns generally.
Although the characteristics of speech that these syntac¬
tical elements are assumed to represent are somewhat
(1,000 words), but nevertheless none of the categories arbitrary and theoretical, these results imply that depres¬
differ by more than 3%. Oddly, our depressed patients tend sive speech tends to be more vague, qualified, and person¬
to differ from Lorenz and Cobb's normal subjects in a more alized, while manic speech is more colorful and concrete. If
extreme way than the manies, suggesting that "normality" one wishes to reject the descriptive categories altogether
is not necessarily a mean between mania and depression. as too arbitrary, one nevertheless is left with the clear

COMMENT
implication that depressed patients tend to qualify more,
to talk more in terms of a "state of being," and to talk more
Because it is a pilot study, this investigation deals with a both about themselves and other people. Manies, on the
large number of linguistic variables. A considerable other hand, tend to talk more about things than about
number, thought to be potentially promising, were not of people, to discuss them in terms of action, and to use more
any value in discriminating between the two groups statis¬ adjectives to describe them. Because our data on manies
tically. In particular, measures of lexical diversity such as are so similar to those of Lorenz and Cobb, it seems likely
the type-token ratio have been of considerable value in that these positive findings can be replicated in future
other linguistic studies of psychiatric populations. The studies.
content analysis dictionaries have been described as a very The results of one familiar linguistic variable, the verb-
interesting method for examining the emotional tones adjective ratio, were particularly surprising. Originally
expressed in samples of language. The negative results described as an "action quotient" or an index of instability,
obtained with these two linguistic tools were particularly it has been consistently higher among psychiatric groups
disappointing. In addition, greater differences in syntac¬ than normals. If it were indeed either an action quotient or
tical complexity were also anticipated. an index of instability, one would anticipate that it would
However, these negative results may have been due to an be higher in manies than depressed patients, but the
error in experimental design, which should be remedied in reverse was true. Other aspects of the data analysis
subsequent studies. The use of 80 words of proverb inter- suggest that one must consider not only the quantity of

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Manitoba User on 06/19/2015


verbs used but also the types of verbs used in order to 6. Fairbanks H: The quantitative differentiation of samples of spoken
assess the meaning of the verb-adjective ratio. If the
language. Psychol Monogr 56:19-38, 1944.
7. Mann MB: The quantitative differentiation of samples of written
quotient is composed of all verbs, including both auxiliary language. Psychol Monogr 56:41-74, 1944.
and main verbs and a mixture of action and state of being 8. Benton AL, Hartman CH, Sarason IG: Some relations between speech
behavior and anxiety level. J Abnorm Soc Psychol 5:295-297, 1955.
verbs, as appears to be the case in most of the quotients 9. Balken ER, Masserman JH: The language of the phantasies of patients
calculated, then the varying quantity of those components with conversion hysteria, anxiety state, and obsessive compulsive neuroses.
J Psychol 10:75-86, 1940.
can certainly affect the meaning of the quotient. In this
10. Gosse A, Wachal RS, Spreen 0: Linguistic Analysis of Free Speech
study the quotient is loaded with a substantial quantity of Samples: Manual of Instructions for Transcription, Pre-editing, and
state of being verbs in the depressives and action verbs in Coding. Victoria, British Columbia, Neuropsychology Laboratory, Depart-
ment of Psychology, University of Victoria, 1974.
the manies, with the overall average number of verbs being 11. Spreen 0, Wachal RS: Psycholinguistic analysis of aphasic language:
similar in the two groups. Manies, however, use many more Theoretical formulations and procedures. Lang Speech 16:130-146, 1973.
12. Wachal RS, Spreen 0: Some measures of lexical diversity in aphasic
adjectives. This gives the manies a relatively lower and normal language performance. Lang Speech 16:169-181, 1973.
quotient than the depressives, although their speech 13. Johnson W: Studies in language behavior. Psychol Monogr 56:1-15,
contains more syntactical elements suggestive of action 1944.
14. Chotlos JW: A statistical and comparative analysis of individual
and color. Thus a high verb-adjective quotient in this study written language samples. Psychol Monogr 56:77-111, 1944.
appears to be an index of a rather drab colorless inactive 15. Maher B: The language of schizophrenia: A review and interpretation.
style of speaking. This dissecting out of the elements that Br J Psychiatry 120:3-17, 1972.
16. Weintraub W, Aronson H: The application of verbal behavior analysis
actually produce a particular quotient appears to be illumi¬ to the study of psychological defense mechanisms: Methodology and prelim-
nating and is recommended for future studies. inary report. J Nerv Ment Dis 134:169-181, 1962.
17. Weintraub W, Aronson H: The application of verbal behavior analysis
Thus, this pilot study has led to some positive results and to the study of psychological defense mechanisms: II. Speech pattern
suggested some methods for future improvement. It has associated with impulsive behavior. J Nerv Ment Dis 139:75-82, 1964.
not drawn heavily on transformational or generative 18. Weintraub W, Aronson H: The application of verbal behavior analysis
to the study of psychological defense mechanisms: III. Speech associated
grammar, since conventional grammar appeared to lend with delusional behavior. J Nerv Ment-Dis 141:172-179, 1965.
itself more readily to data analysis, but it has used other 19. Weintraub W, Aronson H: The application of verbal behavior analysis
to the study of psychological defense mechanisms: IV. Speech pattern
relatively sophisticated psycholinguistic techniques such as associated with depressive behavior. J Nerv Ment Dis 144:22-28, 1967.
content analysis and measures of lexical diversity. Oddly 20. Weintraub W, Aronson H: Application of verbal behavior analysis to
enough, at this point, analyzing grammar in conventional the study of psychological defense mechanisms: V. Speech pattern asso-
ciated with overeating. Arch Gen Psychiatry 21:739-744, 1969.
descriptive terms has been the most useful tool. Further 21. Weintraub W, Aronson H: Verbal behavior analysis and psychological
studies using larger samples are needed to determine defense mechanisms: VI. Speech pattern associated with compulsive behav-
definitively the usefulness of the other techniques. ior. Arch Gen Psychiatry 30:297-300, 1974.
22. Gottschalk LA, Glesner GC, Magliocco EB, et al: Further studies on
the speech patterns of schizophrenic patients. J Nerv Ment Dis 132:101-113,
References 1961.
23. Gottschalk LA, Glesner GC, Daniels RS, et al: The speech patterns of
1. Newman S, Mather VG: Analysis of spoken language of patients with schizophrenic patients: A method of assessing relative degree of personal
affective disorders. Am J Psychiatry 94:913-942, 1938. disorganization and social alienation. J Nerv Ment Dis 127:153-166, 1958.
2. Lorenz M, Cobb S: Language behavior in manic patients. Arch Neurol 24. Stone PJ, Dunphy DC, Smith MS, et al: The General Inquirer: A
Psychiatry 69:763-770, 1952. Computer Approach to Content Analysis. Cambridge, Massachusetts Insti-
3. Lorenz M, Cobb S: Language behavior in psychoneurotic patients. Arch tute of Technology Press, 1966.
Neurol Psychiatry 69:684-694, 1953. 25. Spitzer RL, Endicott J, Robins E: Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC)
4. Lorenz M: Language as expressive behavior. Arch Neurol Psychiatry for a Selected Group of Functional Disorders. New York, Biometrics
70:277-285, 1953. Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute, 1975.
5. Busemann A: Die Sprache der Jugend als Ausdruck der Entwicklungs- 26. Wells FL, Ruesch J: Mental Examiner's Handbook. New York,
rhythmik. Jena, Germany, Fisher Verlag, 1925. Psychological Corp, 1945.

Downloaded From: http://archpsyc.jamanetwork.com/ by a University of Manitoba User on 06/19/2015

You might also like