You are on page 1of 9

J Neurophysiol 94: 4002– 4010, 2005.

First published August 24, 2005; doi:10.1152/jn.00432.2005.

Serotonin Mediates Learning-Induced Potentiation of Excitability

Brian D. Burrell1 and Christie L. Sahley2


1
Neuroscience Group, Division of Basic Biomedical Sciences, University of South Dakota School of Medicine, Vermillion, South Dakota;
and 2Department of Biological Sciences and Graduate Neuroscience Program, Purdue University, West Lafayette, Indiana
Submitted 28 April 2005; accepted in final form 19 August 2005

Burrell, Brian D. and Christie L. Sahley. Serotonin mediates learning- is possible to record from single, identifiable neurons and to
induced potentiation of excitability. J Neurophysiol 94: 4002– 4010, monitor simultaneously learning-induced changes in behavior
2005. First published August 24, 2005; doi:10.1152/jn.00432.2005. and the response properties of individual neurons. An addi-
Sensitization potentiates excitability in an interneuron, the S-cell, that
is critical for this form of learning in the whole-body shortening reflex
tional advantage of the leech is that it is possible to focus on a
of the medicinal leech. Serotonin (5-HT) also increases S-cell excit- single interneuron type, the S-cell, which not only integrates a
ability, and serotonergic modulation is known to be critical for variety of afferent inputs and disseminates this information
sensitization of whole-body shortening, suggesting that 5-HT medi- throughout the CNS but also appears to be critical to certain
ates learning-induced enhancement of S-cell excitability. In this pa- forms of learning-related behavioral plasticity.
per, the role of 5-HT in mediating sensitization-induced potentiation The S-cells form a linear network of electrically coupled
of S-cell excitability was examined. Potentiation of S-cell excitability interneurons that extends throughout the leech CNS (Fig. 1A).
by 5-HT was blocked by the 5-HT receptor antagonist methysergide Each S-cell receives afferent input from touch, pressure, and
and by intracellular injection of the G-protein inhibitor GDP-␤-S, nociceptive mechanosensory neurons as well as photoreceptive
indicating that a metabotropic 5-HT receptor was involved. Bath neurons (Baccus et al. 2000; Muller and Scott 1981; Peterson
application of Rp-cAMP, an inhibitor of protein kinase A (PKA),
blocked 5-HT-induced potentiation of excitability, whereas db-cAMP,
1984), and action potentials that are elicited by this afferent
a cAMP analogue that activates PKA, mimicked the potentiating input propagate reliably throughout the entire S-cell network.
effects of 5-HT on the S-cell. During sensitization of the shortening The S-cell contributes to learning-related plasticity of the leech
reflex in semi-intact preparations, methysergide and Rp-cAMP pre- whole-body shortening reflex, a defensive withdrawal reflex
vented learning-induced potentiation of S-cell excitability, as well as that can be initiated by either mechanical or photo stimulation
the increase in S-cell activity that normally occurs during sensitiza- and involves the near-simultaneous contraction of the entire
tion. Furthermore, sensitization-induced increases in the shortening body. Although the S-cell receives input from afferents that
reflex did not occur in preparations treated with methysergide or elicit shortening, is active during this behavior, and has direct
Rp-cAMP. These results demonstrate that sensitization-induced en- input to at least one type of shortening motor neuron (the L
hancement of S-cell excitability is mediated by 5-HT and suggests motor neurons; see Fig. 1B), its activity alone is not sufficient
that these changes may contribute to this form of learning.
to trigger shortening, and lesions of the S-cell network do not
affect the animal’s capacity to shorten (Gardner-Medwin et al.
INTRODUCTION
1973; Shaw and Kristan 1995, 1999). However, lesions of the
S-cell chain completely disrupt sensitization and partially dis-
There has been increased appreciation for the role that rupt dishabituation of the leech shortening response (Burrell et
modulation of neuronal excitability plays during learning and al. 2003; Modney et al. 1997; Sahley et al. 1994). In addition,
memory (see reviews by Daoudal and Debanne 2003; Wu et al. S-cell activity during elicited shortening is enhanced after
2002; Zhang and Linden 2003). Increases in excitability during sensitization, which may be mediated, in part, by increases in
learning have been observed in a number of different regions of the excitability of the S-cell that also occur during sensitization
the mammalian brain including CA1 and CA3 pyramidal cells (Burrell et al. 2001). This increase in excitability may due to
in the hippocampus (Moyer et al. 1996, 2000; Oh et al. 2003; the effects of serotonin (5-HT), a modulatory neurotransmitter
Stackman et al. 2002; Thompson et al. 1996), cerebellar that is critical for sensitization, full dishabituation, and asso-
Purkinje neurons (Shreurs et al. 1997, 1998), and pyramidal ciative learning of leech shortening (Ehrlich et al. 1992; Sahley
cells in the pyriform cortex (Saar et al. 1998). Learning- 1994). Bath application of 5-HT directly increases S-cell ex-
induced increases in excitability also have been observed in a citability (Burrell et al. 2001) and the interneuron’s response to
number of invertebrate species, including sensory cells in mechanosensory stimuli (Belardetti et al. 1982; Burrell et al.
Hermissenda and Aplysia (Alkon et al. 1985; Antonov et al. 2002). S-cell excitability is also enhanced by stimulation of the
2001; Cleary et al. 1998), motor neurons in Lymnaea (Straub 5-HT-containing Retzius cells (Fig. 1B) (Burrell et al. 2001).
and Benjamin 2001), and interneurons in Helix and the medic- Although these results suggest that 5-HT contributes to
inal leech (Burrell et al. 2001; Gainutdinov et al. 1998). sensitization-induced increases in S-cell excitability, a direct
The medicinal leech is particularly useful for studying the link between 5-HT- and sensitization-induced changes in S-cell
mechanisms that underlie learning-related neuroplasticity. Like excitability has not been established. Therefore the major goal
many invertebrates, it has a well-characterized CNS in which it of this paper is to determine the role of 5-HT in sensitization-
mediated increases in S-cell excitability by blocking the effects
Address for reprint requests and other correspondence: B. D. Burrell, Asst.
Professor, Neuroscience Group, Div. of Basic Biomedical Sciences, University The costs of publication of this article were defrayed in part by the payment
of South Dakota School of Medicine, 414 E. Clark St. Vermillion, SD 57069 of page charges. The article must therefore be hereby marked “advertisement”
(E-mail: bburrell@usd.edu). in accordance with 18 U.S.C. Section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.

4002 0022-3077/05 $8.00 Copyright © 2005 The American Physiological Society www.jn.org
Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/jn at UNAM Fac De Med Dir Gen De Biblio (132.248.016.189) on March 10, 2020.
MODULATION OF EXCITABILITY DURING LEARNING 4003

FIG. 1. The S-cell network and organization of the whole-


body shortening neural circuit. A: leech CNS consists of a
chain of 21 ganglia (1 per body segment) plus fused ganglia
in the head and tail regions (not shown) that are linked by a
connective nerve, and each ganglion contains a single S
interneuron that projects an axon into the anterior and pos-
terior connective nerves. At the midpoint of each connective,
these axons form an S-to-S electrical synapse, resulting in a
chain or network that extends throughout the leech CNS. B:
whole-body shortening can be initiated by mechanosensory
stimuli that activate touch (T), pressure (P), and sometimes
nociceptive (N) neurons, which have synaptic input onto
local motor neurons (Nicholls and Purves 1970), i.e., those
that are in either the same segment or in the segment adjacent
to where the stimuli are applied (L, longitudinal motorneu-
ron; DE and VE, dorsal and ventral excitor motor neurons).
More distant motorneurons must be activated by an unknown
neuron or neurons that are responsible for triggering whole-
body shortening (these triggering elements presumably re-
ceive input from the T-, P-, and N-cells). The S-cell is also
activated during shortening by the mechanosensory neurons
and has synaptic output onto the L-motor neurons and the
serotonin (5-HT)-containing Retzius cell (Wang 1999). The
Retzius cell, in turn, has an excitatory (⫹) modulatory input
onto the S-cell (Burrell et al. 2001). Connections marked (*)
are polysynaptic.

of 5-HT on the S-cell during sensitization training in a behav- potential. Excitability was measured by recording the number of
iorally intact preparation. The results demonstrate that 5-HT- action potentials initiated during a 500 ms, 1 nA depolarizing current
induced potentiation of S-cell excitability is due to activation pulse (AP/pulse). Additional excitability measurements were made
of a metabotropic 5-HT receptor coupled to a cAMP/protein from the S-cell’s response to this stimulus. The membrane potential at
which action potential initiation occurred (membrane potential thresh-
kinase A (PKA) second-messenger pathway and that blocking old) was measured and defined as the point at which the velocity of
5-HT’s effect on the S-cell, either at the receptor or cAMP/ the change in membrane potential equaled 20 mV/s (Bekkers and
PKA level, prevents sensitization-induced increases in excit- Delaney 2001). The rate of the increase in membrane potential (slope)
ability. These experiments represent the first time, to our prior to the first action potential initiation was also measured by taking
knowledge, that learning-induced changes in excitability have the slope of the membrane potential depolarization (dV/dt) during a
been directly monitored and blocked at the same time that 5-ms window, 10 ms prior to action potential initiation (Cudmore and
learning was taking place. Turrigiano 2004). Finally, the interspike interval of the first pair of
action potentials initiated during the stimulus pulse was measured (1st
METHODS
ISI). The resting potential of the S-cell was kept constant for both pre-
and post-5-HT treatment measures of excitability.
Electrophysiology Changes in S-cell excitability were tested prior to (pretest) a 5-min
perfusion with 10 ␮M 5-HT (n ⫽ 9). This was followed by a 5-min
Medicinal leeches (Hirudo medicinalis) weighing 3 g were ob- wash in normal saline and then a second test of excitability (posttest).
tained from a commercial supplier (Leeches USA) and maintained in This treatment has been shown to produce robust increases in excit-
pond water (0.5 g/1 l H2O Hirudo salt (Leeches USA) at 18°C. Single ability that have reached a plateau by this time, although enhancement
ganglia were dissected in leech saline [which contained (in mM) 110 of excitability lasts ⱖ1 h (Burrell et al. 2001). The 5-HT-induced
NaCl, 5 NaOH, 4 KCl, 1.8 CaCl2, 1.5 MgCl2, and 10 HEPES] and changes in excitability were compared with groups of S-cells in which
placed in a recording chamber (chamber volume ⬇ 0.75 ml) under the 5-HT treatment was replaced with normal saline (n ⫽ 9), 5-HT
constant perfusion (⬇1 ml/min). Intracellular recordings were made plus 100 ␮M methysergide (a nonspecific antagonist of metabotropic
by impaling the cell with a sharp microelectrode made from borosili- 5-HT receptors; n ⫽ 7), methysergide alone (n ⫽ 3), 5-HT plus 1 mM
cate glass (1.0 mm OD, 0.78 mm ID; FHC) and pulled to a tip GDP-␤-S (a membrane impermeable competitive G-protein antago-
resistance of 20 –30 M⍀ using a Sutter P-97 puller (Sutter Instru- nist; n ⫽ 4), GDP-␤-S alone (n ⫽ 4), 5-HT plus 50 ␮M Rp-cAMP (a
ments). Electrodes were filled with 3 M potassium acetate (KAc). membrane permeable inhibitor of PKA; n ⫽ 5), Rp-cAMP alone (n ⫽
Recordings were made using a BA-1S amplifier (NPI). Either a Grass 5), or 50 ␮M dibutyryl cAMP (db-cAMP; a membrane permeable
S88 stimulator with SIU5 stimulus isolation units (Astromed) or an analogue of cAMP; n ⫽ 3). GDP-␤-S was dissolved in filtered 3 M
STG 1004 programmable stimulator (Multichannel Systems) was KAc and applied intracellularly by using the GDP-␤-S solution to fill
used to deliver current pulses. Electrophysiological data were viewed the recording electrode and then allowing it to diffuse into the
on a digital oscilloscope and stored on a computer using a Digidata interneuron. The remaining drugs were dissolved in leech saline and
1322A analog/digital interface and Axoscope 9.0 acquisition software applied via the perfusion through the recording chamber. All drugs
(Axon Instruments). were obtained from Sigma (St. Louis).
S-cells were identified by their position in the ganglion (variable,
but always in the central glial packet on the ventral side of the
ganglion), size (5–10 ␮m), and shape of their action potential (50 to Behavioral training and recordings from
60 mV amplitude, ⬇2-ms spike width). Measurements of intrinsic semi-intact preparations
electrical properties of the S-cell were made as follows. Input resis-
tance was measured by injecting a hyperpolarizing current pulse (1 Dissections were carried out in an ice-lined dissecting dish with
nA, 500 ms) and measuring the resulting change in membrane ice-cold leech saline. Segments 1–2 were left intact and pinned dorsal

J Neurophysiol • VOL 94 • DECEMBER 2005 • www.jn.org


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/jn at UNAM Fac De Med Dir Gen De Biblio (132.248.016.189) on March 10, 2020.
4004 B. D. BURRELL AND C. L. SAHLEY

side up in a silicone elastomer (Sylgard)-lined dish (Fig. 2). A 72– 4481; Harvard Apparatus) using a nylon monofilament so that
longitudinal incision was made down the dorsal midline of segments whole-body shortening could be recorded and quantified. The remain-
3– 8 so that the cylinder of the leech body could be opened up, and the ing portion of the preparation, segments 12–14, consisted of the CNS
resulting flat sheet of skin was rotated and pinned ventral side up (this only (ganglia and connective nerve), and the connective nerve poste-
will be referred to as the body-wall portion of the preparation). A rior of the segment 14 ganglion was pulled into a suction electrode for
small longitudinal incision was made on the ventral side of segment 4, extracellular recordings of S-cell activity. The S-cell action potential
exposing the segmental ganglion for S-cell intracellular recordings. produces the largest signal in such recordings (Frank et al. 1975), so
Segments 9 –11 were left intact and pinned dorsal side up with the S-cell activity during elicited shortening responses could be easily
posterior end connected to an isometric tension transducer (model monitored and recorded. The preparation was allowed to rest for 30
min in saline with 10 mM glucose before undergoing sensitization
training. All training was conducted at room temperature (22–24°C).
Whole-body shortening was elicited by delivering controlled elec-
troshocks to the skin using implanted Teflon-coated silver wire
electrodes (75-␮m bare diameter; A-M Systems). Each electrode
consisted of a pair of silver wires in which the Teflon was removed
from the portions of the wire that were in contact with the skin, and
two pairs of wire electrodes were implanted in the skin. One pair was
implanted at segment 4 and elicited whole-body shortening (test site)
using stimulation parameters from Shaw and Kristan (1995): five
stimulus pulses, 1 ms pulse duration, 10 Hz frequency. The stimulus
intensity at the test site was set at just above threshold for eliciting
shortening. The second set was implanted between segments 7 and 8
and was used to deliver the sensitizing stimulus (sensitizing site; 10
stimulus pulses, 1 ms pulse duration, 10 Hz frequency, intensity set at
⬇25% above threshold).
Immediately before training, intracellular recordings were made
from the segment 4 S-cell, and its excitability properties were mea-
sured in the same manner used for the 5-HT experiments described
previously. In preparations that would undergo sensitization in normal
saline (n ⫽ 7), training began by making two presensitization (pre-
test), see Fig 4A measurements of the shortening response and the
accompanying S-cell activity at an intertrial interval (ITI) of 2.5 min.
This was followed by the delivery of two trains of sensitizing stimuli,
again with a 2.5 min ITI. Five minutes after the last sensitizing trial,
two postsensitization (posttest) measurements of the shortening re-
flex/S-cell activity were made (2.5 min ITI). Postsensitization mea-
surements of S-cell excitability were made immediately after the last
behavioral posttest.
To test whether sensitization-induced potentiation of S-cell excit-
ability was mediated by 5-HT, sensitization training was carried in the
presence of 100 ␮M methysergide (n ⫽ 6) or 50 ␮M Rp-cAMP (n ⫽
5). Control experiments, in which delivery of the sensitizing stimuli
was omitted, were carried out in normal saline (n ⫽ 6), 100 ␮M
methysergide (n ⫽ 6) or 50 ␮M Rp-cAMP (n ⫽ 6). All drugs were
bath-applied 5 min before the first pretest. Injection of membrane
impermeable agents into the S-cell that might antagonize 5-HT-
induced changes in excitability (e.g., GDP-␤-S or PKA inhibitory
peptides) was not possible using these semi-intact preparations. Mech-
anosensory stimuli that elicit shortening cause multiple S-cells to fire
(2–5 adjacent interneurons) and increases in the S-cell network’s
response due to increased stimulus intensity, 5-HT, or sensitization
are distributed across multiple S-cells (Baccus et al. 2001; Burrell et
al. 2002; Cruz et al. 2003). Therefore using injectable drugs to block
potential serotonergic modulation of the S-cell would require finding
and injecting a minimum of three separate S-cells, by which time there
would have been a substantial reduction in the viability of the
semi-intact preparation.

Data analysis
FIG. 2. Diagram of the semi-intact preparation used for sensitization train-
ing. The ganglion in the 4th segment was exposed so that intracellular Changes in S-cell excitability properties from both the 5-HT and
recordings could be made from the S-cell before and after training. This sensitization training experiments were analyzed as follows. For each
portion of the preparation (segments 3– 8) was rotated so that it was ventral
side up during training to accommodate S-cell recordings. The rest of the
neuron, multiple measurements of a given excitability property (AP/
preparation was pinned dorsal side up. Changes in the whole-body shortening pulse, slope, input resistance, etc.) were made and an average calcu-
reflex were monitored using a tension transducer attached to segments 9 –11. lated for both the pre- and posttest excitability measurements. The
S-cell activity during shortening was monitored using a suction electrode averaged value from each neuron was then used for statistical analysis
applied to the connective nerve posterior of the segment 14 ganglion. and graphical representation. One-way ANOVA in which the post-

J Neurophysiol • VOL 94 • DECEMBER 2005 • www.jn.org


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/jn at UNAM Fac De Med Dir Gen De Biblio (132.248.016.189) on March 10, 2020.
MODULATION OF EXCITABILITY DURING LEARNING 4005

treatment levels had been normalized relative to the pretreatment level creases in S-cell excitability (Fig. 3E). Changes in AP/pulse
was used to detect differences between the various treatment groups. (post hoc P ⬍ 0.001), first ISI (P ⬍ 0.05), and slope (P ⬍ 0.05)
Behavioral and S-cell activity data from the sensitization training in the 5-HT⫹GDP-␤-S group were significantly different from
experiments were analyzed using a one-way ANOVA with repeated those treated with 5-HT alone but not significantly different
measures (presensitization trial 2 and postsensitization trials 1 and 2,
all normalized to the 1st presensitization trial).
from the saline group. The same result was observed in S-cells
treated with GDP-␤-S alone (Fig. 3E; post hoc results: AP/
pulse, P ⬍ 0.05; 1st ISI, P ⬍ 0.01; slope, P ⬍ 0.01). No
RESULTS significant difference in S-cell membrane potential threshold or
5-HT activates a metabotropic receptor coupled to a cAMP/ input resistance was observed among the 5-HT⫹GDP-␤-S,
PKA pathway GDP-␤-S, 5-HT, or saline groups (Fig. 3E).
Next, the involvement of a cAMP/PKA second-messenger
Application of 10 ␮M 5-HT induced a significant increase in cascade was examined. 5-HT-induced increases in S-cell ex-
the number of action potentials fired by the S-cell during citability properties were blocked when 50 ␮M Rp-cAMP was
current injection [Fig. 3A; AP/pulse 1-way ANOVA, F(8,36) ⫽ included in the perfusate (Fig. 3F; 5HT⫹Rp-cAMP). Changes
5.96, P ⬍ 0.0001]. This was accompanied by a significant in AP/pulse (post hoc P ⬍ 0.005), first ISI (P ⬍ 0.05), and
decrease in the interval between the first pair of action poten- slope (P ⬍ 0.01) in the 5-HT⫹Rp-cAMP group were signifi-
tials [Fig. 3B; 1st ISI 1-way ANOVA, F(8,34) ⫽ 3.15, P ⬍ cantly different from those treated with 5-HT alone but not
0.01] and an increase in the rate of depolarization prior to the significantly different from the saline group. The same result
first action potential [Fig. 3C; slope 1-way ANOVA, F(8,36) ⫽ was observed in S-cells treated with Rp-cAMP alone (Fig. 3F),
3.53, P ⬍ 0.005]. The 5-HT-induced changes in these excit- indicating that the drug did not affect S-cell excitability di-
ability properties were significantly different when compared rectly (post hoc results: AP/pulse, P ⬍ 0.0005; 1st ISI, P ⬍
with S-cells in which 5-HT application was omitted (saline 0.05; slope, P ⬍ 0.01). No significant difference in S-cell
group; Fig. 3D; post hoc test: AP/pulse, P ⬍ 0.0005; 1st ISI, membrane potential threshold or input resistance was observed
P ⬍ 0.05; slope, P ⬍ 0.01). No significant change was among the 5-HT⫹Rp-cAMP, Rp-cAMP, 5-HT, or saline
observed in the membrane potential threshold [1-way ANOVA, groups (Fig. 3F). If 5-HT modulation of excitability does
F(8,35) ⫽ 0.48] or in input resistance [1-way ANOVA, require the activity of a cAMP/PKA second-messenger system,
F(8,35) ⫽ 0.50]. 5HT-induced increases of S-cell excitability then increasing the intracellular level of cAMP should mimic
were blocked by co-application of 100 ␮M methysergide (Fig. the effects of 5-HT on this interneuron. A 5-min perfusion of
3D; 5-HT⫹methysergide). Excitability measures from the the S-cell with 50 ␮M db-cAMP produced increases in AP/
5-HT⫹methysergide group were significantly different com- pulse and slope and decreases in first ISI that were statistically
pared with the 5-HT group (post hoc test: AP/pulse, P ⬍ 0.001; indistinguishable from those of 5-HT (Fig. 3F) but statistically
1st ISI, P ⬍ 0.01; slope, P ⬍ 0.01) but were not significantly different from the saline group (post hoc results: AP/pulse, P ⬍
different from the saline group. S-cells treated with methyser- 0.001; 1st ISI, P ⬍ 0.05; slope, P ⬍ 0.05). No significant
gide alone were not significantly different from the saline or difference in S-cell membrane potential threshold or input
5-HT⫹methysergide groups but were significantly different resistance was observed between the db-cAMP, 5-HT, or saline
from the 5-HT-treated group in terms of AP/pulse (post hoc groups (Fig. 3F). Together, the results from the Rp-cAMP and
P ⬍ 0.001), first ISI (P ⬍ 0.01), and slope (P ⬍ 0.01). db-cAMP experiments demonstrate that 5-HT-induced in-
There was no significant difference in membrane potential creases in S-cell excitability require activation of a cAMP/PKA
threshold or input resistance in S-cells from the saline-, second-messenger cascade.
5-HT⫹methysergide-, methysergide-, and the 5-HT-treated
groups. Sensitization is blocked by treatments that inhibit 5-HT
Although methysergide was effective at blocking 5-HT- modulation of excitability
mediated increases in S-cell excitability, it is a relatively
nonspecific 5-HT receptor antagonist and does not help in To examine whether sensitization-induced increases in S-
identifying the 5-HT receptor subtype(s) involved in this mod- cell excitability are the result of serotonergic modulation,
ulation. It is difficult to identify 5-HT receptor subtypes in sensitization training was carried out under conditions that
invertebrates using vertebrate pharmacological tools (Tierney block potentiation of excitability by 5-HT. Specifically, semi-
2001). Therefore components of the signal transduction path- intact preparations were treated with either 100 ␮M methyser-
ways activated by the 5-HT receptors were examined instead. gide or 50 ␮M Rp-cAMP during sensitization training. In
This approach was used not only to characterize the cellular addition, the effects of these treatments on sensitization of
mechanisms mediating 5-HT modulation of the S-cell but also whole-body shortening and the accompanying increase in S-
to provide tools with which to disrupt this process during cell activity during shortening were examined.
sensitization training. The intensity of the whole-body shortening reflex more than
The first step was to identify whether a G-protein-coupled doubled after delivery of the sensitizing stimuli (Fig. 4, B and
receptor mediated 5-HT-induced changes in S-cell excitability C) in preparations that underwent sensitization training in
because there are both iono- and metabotropic 5-HT receptors normal saline (sens/saline group). S-cell activity during short-
(Hoyer et al. 2002). Therefore 1 mM GDP-␤-S was included in ening also increased relative to presensitization levels (Fig. 4,
the recording electrode used to monitor S-cell excitability B and D). No change in either the shortening reflex or S-cell
while the interneuron was perfused with 10 ␮M 5-HT activity was observed in preparations from sensitization control
(5HT⫹GDP-␤-S). GDP-␤-S prevented 5-HT-induced in- experiments in which delivery of the sensitizing stimuli was

J Neurophysiol • VOL 94 • DECEMBER 2005 • www.jn.org


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/jn at UNAM Fac De Med Dir Gen De Biblio (132.248.016.189) on March 10, 2020.
4006 B. D. BURRELL AND C. L. SAHLEY

omitted (Fig. 4, C and D; con/saline group). Sensitization-


induced increases in both the shortening reflex and S-cell
activity were blocked in preparations treated with either me-
thysergide (sens/methy group) or Rp-cAMP (sens/Rp-cAMP
group; Fig. 4, C and D).
Sensitization-induced changes in whole-body shortening
were confirmed using two-way ANOVA with repeated mea-
sures [treatment effect, F(5,29) ⫽ 4.00, P ⬍ 0.01; trial effect,
F(2,58) ⫽ 0.11; interaction effect, F(10,58) ⫽ 3.20, P ⬍
0.005]. Post hoc analysis confirmed that the postsensitization
shortening in the sens/saline group was significantly greater
compared with shortening in all the sensitization control
groups (con/saline, P ⬍ 0.001; con/methy, P ⬍ 0.005; con/
Rp-cAMP, P ⬍ 0.0005). Furthermore, shortening in the sens/
saline group was significantly greater than in preparations that
underwent sensitization training in the presence of either me-
thysergide (sens/methy P ⬍ 0.01) or Rp-cAMP (sens/Rp-
cAMP, P ⬍ 0.01). The level of shortening in the sens/methy
and sens/Rp-cAMP groups was not significantly different from
any of the sensitization control groups (con/saline, con/methy,
and con/Rp-cAMP). Similarly, sensitization-induced increases
in S-cell activity during the shortening response were con-
firmed statistically [treatment effect, F(5,29) ⫽ 3.02, P ⬍ 0.05;
trial effect, F(2,58) ⫽ 7.91, P ⬍ 0.001; interaction effect,
F(10,58) ⫽ 2.25, P ⬍ 0.05]. Post hoc analysis showed that
postsensitization S-cell activity in the sens/saline group was
significantly greater than activity in the preparations sensitized
in the presence of methysergide (sens/methy, P ⬍ 0.01) or
Rp-cAMP (sens/Rp-cAMP, P ⬍ 0.05) or any of the sensitiza-
tion control groups (con/saline, P ⬍ 0.001; con/methy, P ⬍
0.01; con/Rp-cAMP, P ⬍ 0.05). Postsensitization S-cell activ-
ity was not significantly different between any of the sensiti-
zation control groups or between these groups and the sens/
methy or sens/Rp-cAMP groups.
The lack of sensitization in methysergide- or Rp-cAMP-
treated groups was not due to any detrimental effect of these
drugs on the preparations during training. Shortening and
S-cell responses were stable during sensitization control exper-
iments in these preparations and were statistically indistin-
guishable from the con/saline group (Fig. 4, C and D). In
addition, the lack of sensitization in the sens/methy or sens/
Rp-cAMP groups was not due to any differences in the level of
the whole-body shortening response at the start of training.
There was no significant difference in the presensitization trial
1 shortening reflex from the sens/saline, sens/methy, and sens/
Rp-cAMP groups [1-way ANOVA, F(2,16) ⫽ 1.20]. Further-
more, the lack of sensitization was not due to any deficits in the

FIG. 3. 5-HT increases excitability via a metabotropic 5-HT receptor that


activates a cAMP/protein kinase A (PKA) second-messenger pathway.
Changes in excitability properties before and after application of 10 ␮M 5-HT:
action potentials (AP)/pulse (A), 1st interspike interval (ISI) in the train (B),
and slope of depolarization (dV/dT) prior to the 1st AP in the train (C). D:
average percent change (⫾SE) in excitability properties in 5-HT-treated (black
bars) and saline control (white bars) S-cells (same data used in E and F).
Co-application of methysergide blocked 5-HT’s effects on excitability (dark
gray-bars) but did not alter excitability itself (light gray-filled bars). E:
co-application of GDP-␤-S blocked 5-HT’s effects (fine right-hatched bars)
but had no effect by itself (coarse right-hatched bars). F: co-application of
Rp-cAMP block 5-HT’s effects (fine left-hatched bars) but had no effect by
itself (coarse left-hatched bars). Application of db-cAMP (cross-hatched bars)
mimicked the effects of 5-HT.

J Neurophysiol • VOL 94 • DECEMBER 2005 • www.jn.org


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/jn at UNAM Fac De Med Dir Gen De Biblio (132.248.016.189) on March 10, 2020.
MODULATION OF EXCITABILITY DURING LEARNING 4007

the sens/saline group, 21 ⫾ 2.43 and 18.8 ⫾ 3.68 for the


sens/methy group, and 21 ⫾ 4.13 and 15.3 ⫾ 3.78 for the
sens/Rp-cAMP group (average presensitization S-cell response
during shortening was 7.71 ⫾ 1.54 action potentials). One-way
ANOVA with repeated measures showed no significant effect
of treatment group [F(2,13) ⫽ 0.06] and no significant inter-
action effect [F(2,16) ⫽ 1.97]. There was a significant effect
for the repeated measures [F(1,13) ⫽ 8.73, P ⬍ 0.05] that was
the result of the first sensitizing stimulus consistently eliciting
more S-cell action potentials than the second sensitizing stim-
ulus in all three treatment groups.

Sensitization alters S-cell frequency distribution


Changes in the frequency distribution of S-cell activity as a
result of sensitization were also analyzed. The average fre-
quency from the two pre- and postsensitization trials was
calculated at 100 ms intervals following onset of the test
stimulus and then averaged across all preparations in the
sens/saline group. The frequency of S-cell activity from the
postsensitization trials was enhanced relative to the presensi-
tization trials with differences between the average activity
levels becoming greater at later poststimulus intervals (Fig. 5).
This sensitization effect was statistically significant based on
ANOVA [F(1,36) ⫽ 10.94, P ⬍ 0.005]. Not surprisingly, there
was also a significant effect of the poststimulus interval as a
result of the frequency decreasing at each time interval
[F(1,36) ⫽ 5.35, P ⬍ 0.001]. There was no interaction effect
[F(5,36) ⫽ 0.27]. The pre- versus postsensitization change in
overall S-cell frequency elicited during shortening is shown in
the Fig. 5, inset.

Sensitization-induced enhancement of S-cell excitability


is 5-HT dependent
In agreement with earlier observations (Burrell et al. 2001),
S-cell excitability increased after sensitization training, and the
pattern of sensitization-induced increases in excitability prop-
erties was identical to those produced by 5-HT in this study.
Sensitization produced an increase in AP/pulse [Fig. 6, A and
D; 1-way ANOVA, F(5,15) ⫽ 5.52, P ⬍ 0.005], a decrease in

FIG. 4. Methysergide and Rp-cAMP block sensitization-induced increases


the shortening reflex and S-cell activity. A: time line of training protocol. B:
sensitization training increased the intensity of the elicited shortening response
(top traces) and the S-cell activity during shortening (bottom traces). Average
(⫾SE) increase in the whole-body shortening reflex (C) and S-cell activity (D)
in the sensitized (F) vs. control (E) preparations trained in normal saline. Both
methysergide () and Rp-cAMP (■) blocked sensitization-induced increases in
behavior and S-cell activity. Neither methysergide nor Rp-cAMP had any
effect on shortening or S-cell activity when applied to preparations that did not
undergo sensitization training (ƒ and 䊐, respectively).
FIG. 5. Sensitization increases the rate of S-cell activity. Frequency distri-
ability of the sensitizing stimuli to activate the S-cell, which is bution of S-cell activity prior to (䊐) and after (■) sensitization. For each
preparation, the average S-cell frequency from both pre- and postsensitization
critical for the induction of sensitization (Burrell et al. 2003). tests was calculated for each interval and then those numbers were used to
The number of S-cell action potentials elicited during the two calculate the average and SE shown in the graph. Inset: average (⫾SE) overall
sensitizing stimulation trials was 20.4 ⫾ 1.5 and 19 ⫾ 3.44 for S-cell frequency during elicited shortening.

J Neurophysiol • VOL 94 • DECEMBER 2005 • www.jn.org


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/jn at UNAM Fac De Med Dir Gen De Biblio (132.248.016.189) on March 10, 2020.
4008 B. D. BURRELL AND C. L. SAHLEY

sens/Rp-cAMP (AP/pulse, P ⬍ 0.001; 1st ISI, P ⬍ 0.005;


slope, P ⬍ 0.01) groups. Excitability changes in the sens/saline
group were also significantly different from the con/saline
(AP/pulse, P ⬍ 0.005; 1st ISI, P ⬍ 0.005; slope, P ⬍ 0.001),
con/methy (AP/pulse, P ⬍ 0.005; 1st ISI, P ⬍ 0.01; slope, P ⬍
0.005), and con/Rp-cAMP (AP/pulse, P ⬍ 0.01; 1st ISI, P ⬍
0.05; slope, P ⬍ 0.05) groups. There were no significant
differences in the excitability properties between any of the
sensitization control groups or between the sens/methy, sens/
Rp-cAMP and any of the control groups (Fig. 6D).

DISCUSSION

These results confirm previous work (Burrell et al. 2001) in


which both sensitization training and 5-HT induced similar
increases in excitability in the S-cell and extends those findings
by demonstrating that 5-HT mediates sensitization-induced
potentiation of S-cell excitability. 5-HT-induced potentiation
of S-cell excitability is mediated by a metabotropic 5-HT
receptor that upregulates cAMP production and activates PKA,
although additional signaling pathways may be involved as
well. The fact that serotonergic modulation of the S-cell
requires a cAMP/PKA second-messenger cascade is consistent
with earlier findings showing that 5-HT-mediated increases in
the S-cell response to mechanosensory stimuli utilize a cAMP/
PKA pathway (Belardetti et al. 1982). Sensitization-induced
enhancement of S-cell excitability was blocked by treatments
that either antagonized 5-HT binding (methysergide) or pre-
vented cAMP activation of PKA (Rp-cAMP), demonstrating
that learning-induced potentiation of S-cell excitability is a
5-HT-dependent process.
Interestingly, treatments that blocked 5-HT-mediated poten-
tiation of S-cell excitability during sensitization training pre-
FIG. 6. Sensitization increases S-cell excitability and those changes are vented sensitization-induced increases in S-cell activity and
blocked by treatments that antagonized 5-HT’s effect on this interneuron. whole-body shortening. These results suggest that increased
Changes in excitability properties before and after application of sensitization
training: AP/pulse (A), 1st ISI in the train (B), slope of depolarization (dV/dT)
S-cell excitability contributes to sensitization of the shortening
prior to the first action potential in the train (C). D: as with 5-HT, S-cells from reflex but cannot be considered conclusive because the drugs
preparations that had undergone sensitization training in normal saline (sens/ used to block serotonergic modulation of the S-cell were
saline; black bars) exhibited an increase in AP/pulse and slope, a decrease in bath-applied (see METHODS) and may have prevented 5-HT- or
1st ISI, and no change in membrane potential threshold or input resistance. No cAMP/PKA-dependent changes in other cells that contribute to
significant change in these excitability properties was observed in S-cells from
the control (no sensitizing stimuli delivered) preparations in normal saline sensitization. For example, 5-HT decreases afterhyperpolariza-
(con/saline; white bars). Sensitization-induced increases in excitability were tion (AHP) in T and P mechanosensory neurons through a
not observed in preparations trained in methysergide (sens/methys; fine cAMP-dependent process (Catarsi and Brunelli 1991; Catarsi
hatched bars) or Rp-cAMP (Sens/Rp-cAMP; fine cross-hatched bars). No et al. 1995). Decreased AHP in these cells could enhance
changes in excitability were observed in control preparations treated with
either methysergide (con/methys; coarse hatched bars) or Rp-cAMP (con/Rp-
afferent input to the S-cell (or other neurons that contribute to
cAMP; coarse cross-hatched bars). shortening) by reversing branch point propagation failure of
action potentials (Mar and Drapeau 1996) or inducing action
the first ISI [Fig. 6, B and D; F(5,15) ⫽ 4.48, P ⬍ 0.05], and potential reflection at these branch points (Baccus et al. 2000,
an increase in slope [Fig. 6, C and D; F(5,15) ⫽ 4.72, P ⬍ 2001). In addition, 5-HT modulation of the S-to-S electrical
0.01] relative to S-cells from nonsensitized preparations, but synapses may increase the instantaneous frequency of action
there was no change in membrane potential threshold potentials as they propagate through the S-cell network (Moss
[F(5,15) ⫽ 0.29] or input resistance [F(5,15) ⫽ 1.56]. Further- et al. 2005), which could also contribute to increases in the rate
more, sensitization-induced potentiation of excitability was of S-cell activity. It is not known if 5-HT modulation of the
blocked by treatments that prevented 5-HT-induced enhance- S-to-S electrical synapses is also a PKA-dependent process.
ment of excitability, namely methysergide and Rp-cAMP (Fig. The S-cell appears to have two roles during sensitization.
6D), demonstrating that enhanced S-cell excitability during First, an intact S-cell network is necessary during the delivery
sensitization was mediated by 5-HT. Post hoc analyses con- of the sensitizing stimuli based on experiments in which
firmed that sensitization-induced changes in excitability prop- sensitization was prevented when the S-cell chain had been
erties were significantly different in the S-cells in the sens/ lesioned (Burrell et al. 2003; Modney et al. 1997; Sahley et al.
saline group compared with those in the sens/methy (AP/pulse, 1994). The precise role of the S-cell during this induction stage
P ⬍ 0.001; 1st ISI, P ⬍ 0.005; slope, P ⬍ 0.005) or the of sensitization is not known, but as the present results show,
J Neurophysiol • VOL 94 • DECEMBER 2005 • www.jn.org
Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/jn at UNAM Fac De Med Dir Gen De Biblio (132.248.016.189) on March 10, 2020.
MODULATION OF EXCITABILITY DURING LEARNING 4009

sensitizing stimuli strongly activate the S-cell. This may lead to induced changes in behavior were taking place. These results
the stimulation of 5-HT-containing Retzius cells throughout suggest, albeit not conclusively, that increased S-cell excitabil-
the leech CNS because these cells receive synaptic input from ity contributes to sensitization of the shortening reflex. It is
the S-cell (Wang 1999). Second, the S-cell itself may contrib- hypothesized that potentiation of S-cell excitability during
ute to producing the sensitized response, i.e., increased short- learning contributes to increases in the rate of S-cell activity
ening. Sahley et al. (1994) observed a strong correlation during the shortening response and that this increase in activity
between the level of S-cell activity and the intensity of the allows the interneuron to contribute to the reflex in a way that
sensitized shortening response, whereas no such correlation is does not under basal conditions (Lisman 1997). The S-cell
was detected in the nonsensitized shortening reflex. In addition, would be effectively “recruited” into the shortening neural
results presented here and from previous experiments (Burrell circuit as a result of learning, a process that has been observed
et al. 2001; Sahley et al. 1994) show that S-cell excitability and in both vertebrates and invertebrates (Daly et al. 2004; Moita et
activity during shortening are enhanced during sensitization. al. 2003). Such recruitment of a cell into a neural circuit may
The S-cell may also be incorporated into other behaviors as a represent a general mechanism by which modulation of excit-
result of sensitization. Debski and Friesen (1986) observed an ability re-configures neural networks during learning.
increase in S-cell activity during sensitization of leech swim-
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
ming behavior, which is also 5-HT- and cAMP-dependent
(Zaccardi et al. 2004), and suggested that increased S-cell The authors thank Drs. Brenda L. Moss and Kevin M. Crisp for helpful
activity might contribute to this behavior during sensitization discussions during the preparation of this paper and Rodney McPhail at the
Purdue University Department of Biological Sciences for the Fig. 2 drawing.
even though this interneuron is not part of the swimming neural
circuit. However, the necessity of the S-cell in the “expression” GRANTS
of the sensitization for shortening or swimming has not been This work was supported by National Institute of Neurological Disorders
conclusively demonstrated. and Stroke Grant R01-NS-34927 to C. L. Sahley with Kenneth J. Muller,
The biophysical basis for 5-HT-induced increases in S-cell National Science Foundation Grant IBN-0432683 to B. D. Burrell, and a
excitability is not known, but it likely involves the modulation subproject of National Institute of Research Resources Grant P20 RR-015567
to B. D. Burrell that is designated as a Center of Biomedical Research
of multiple membrane conductances (see reviews by Frick and Excellence.
Johnston 2005; Zhang and Linden 2003). The observed in-
crease in slope suggests an enhancement of voltage-gated Na⫹ REFERENCES
currents or a reduction of voltage-gated K⫹ currents. The Abraham WC and Bear MF. Metaplasticity: the plasticity of synaptic
increase in the AP’s/pulse (and possibly the decrease in 1st ISI) plasticity. Trends Neurosci 19: 126 –130, 1996.
suggests modulation of currents that regulate the repeated Alkon DL, Sakakibara M, Forman R, Harrigan J, Lederhendler I, and
firing of action potentials by a neuron, such as those mediated Farley J. Reduction of two voltage-dependent K⫹ currents mediates reten-
tion of a learned association. Behav Neural Biol 44: 278 –300, 1985.
by Ca2⫹-activated K⫹ channels or persistent Na⫹ channels Andersen P, Sundberg SH, Sveen O, Swann JW, and Wigstrom H.
(Sah and Faber 2002; Wu et al. 2004). Possible mechanisms for long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission
Potentiation of excitability is an important mechanism for in hippocampal slices from guinea pigs. J Physiol 302: 463– 482, 1980.
encoding and storing information during learning and memory. Antonov I, Antonova I, Kandel ER, and Hawkins RD. The contribution of
activity-dependent synaptic plasticity to classical conditioning in Aplysia.
Increased excitability has been observed after learning in both J Neurosci 21: 6413– 6422, 2001.
vertebrates and invertebrates (Alkon et al. 1985; Antonov et al. Baccus SA, Burrell BD, Sahley CL, and Muller KJ. Action potential
2001; Burrell et al. 2001; Cleary et al. 1998; Gainutdinov et al. reflection and failure at axon branch points cause stepwise changes in EPSPs
1998; Moyer et al. 1996, 2000; Oh et al. 2003; Saar et al. 1998; in a neuron essential for learning. J Neurophysiol 83: 1693–1700, 2000.
Shreurs et al. 1997, 1998; Stackman et al. 2002; Straub and Baccus SA, Sahley CL, and Muller KJ. Multiple sites of action potential
initiation increase neuronal firing rate. J Neurophysiol 86: 1226 –1236,
Benjamin 2001; Thompson et al. 1996) and dysfunctions in the 2001.
modulation of excitability may contribute to age-related defi- Bekkers JM and Delaney AJ. Modulation of excitability by alpha-dendro-
cits in learning and memory (Moyer et al. 2000; Wu et al. toxin-sensitive potassium channels in neocortical pyramidal neurons. J Neu-
2002). In addition, modulation of excitability interacts with rosci 21: 6553– 6560, 2001.
Belardetti F, Biondi C, Colombaioni L, Brunelli M, and Trevisani A. Role
activity-dependent forms of neuroplasticity that may contribute of serotonin and cyclic AMP on facilitation of the fast conducting system
to learning and memory, such as long-term potentiation (LTP) activity in the leech Hirudo medicinalis. Brain Res 246: 89 –103, 1982.
or long-term depression (LTD) (Andersen et al. 1980; Bliss Bliss TV and Lomo T. Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in
and Lomo 1973; Daoudal et al. 2002; Frick et al. 2004). the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the
Modulation of excitability may also act to change the threshold perforant path. J Physiol 232: 331–356, 1973.
Burrell BD and Sahley CL. Multiple forms of long-term potentiation and
for induction of LTP or LTD (Cohen et al. 1999; Le Ray et al. long-term depression converge on a single interneuron in the leech CNS.
2004; Morozov et al. 2003; Nolan et al. 2004), a process often J Neurosci 24: 4011– 4019, 2004.
referred to as metaplasticity (Abraham and Bear 1996). LTP Burrell BD, Sahley CL, and Muller KJ. Non-associative learning and
and LTD are observed in synapses formed by the mechanosen- serotonin induce similar bi-directional changes in excitability of a neuron
critical for learning in the medicinal leech. J Neurosci 21: 1401–1412, 2001.
sory T- and P-cells onto the S-cell (Burrell and Sahley 2004), Burrell BD, Sahley CL, and Muller KJ. Differential effects of serotonin
but it is not known if 5-HT modulation of S-cell excitability enhance activity of an electrically coupled neural network. J Neurophysiol
interacts with either form of synaptic plasticity. 87: 2889 –2895, 2002.
The results presented here show a relationship between Burrell BD, Sahley CL, and Muller KJ. Progressive recovery of learning
learning- and 5-HT-induced potentiation of excitability in the during regeneration of a single synapse in the medicinal leech. J Comp
Neurol 457: 67–74, 2003.
S-cell. To our knowledge, these experiments represent the first Catarsi S and Brunelli M. Serotonin depresses the after-hyperpolarization
time that learning-induced modulation of excitability has been through the inhibition of the Na⫹/K⫹ electrogenic pump in T sensory
directly monitored and blocked at the same time that learning- neurones of the leech. J Exp Biol 155: 261–273, 1991.

J Neurophysiol • VOL 94 • DECEMBER 2005 • www.jn.org


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/jn at UNAM Fac De Med Dir Gen De Biblio (132.248.016.189) on March 10, 2020.
4010 B. D. BURRELL AND C. L. SAHLEY

Catarsi S, Scuri R, and Brunelli M. Octopamine and Leydig cell stimulation Nicholls JG and Purves D. Monosynaptic chemical and electrical connexions
depress the afterhyperpolarization in touch sensory neurons of the leech. between sensory and motor cells in the central nervous system of the leech.
Neuroscience 66: 751–759, 1995. J Physiol 209: 647– 667, 1970.
Cleary LJ, Lee WL, and Byrne JH. Cellular correlates of long-term sensi- Nolan MF, Malleret G, Dudman JT, Buhl DL, Santoro B, Gibbs E,
tization in Aplysia. J Neurosci 18: 5988 –5998, 1998. Vronskaya S, Buzsaki G, Siegelbaum SA, Kandel ER, and Morozov A.
Cohen AS, Coussens CM, Raymond CR, and Abraham WC. Long-lasting A behavioral role for dendritic integration: HCN1 channels constrain spatial
increase in cellular excitability associated with the priming of LTP induction memory and plasticity at inputs to distal dendrites of CA1 pyramidal
in rat hippocampus. J Neurophysiol 82: 3139 –3148, 1999. neurons. Cell 119: 719 –732, 2004.
Cruz GE, Sahley CL, and Muller KJ. A neuronal network with multiple Oh MM, Kuo AG, Wu WW, Sametsky EA, and Disterhoft JF. Watermaze
learning enhances excitability of CA1 pyramidal neurons. J Neurophysiol
initiation sites in a behaving leech. Soc Neurosci Abstr 520.9, 2003.
90: 2171–2179, 2003.
Cudmore RH and Turrigiano GG. Long-term potentiation of intrinsic
Peterson EL. Photoreceptors and visual interneurons in the medicinal leech.
excitability in LV visual cortical neurons. J Neurophysiol 92: 341–348, J Neurobiol 15: 413– 428, 1984.
2004. Saar D, Grossman Y, and Barkai E. Reduced after-hyperpolarization in rat
Daly KC, Christensen TA, Lei H, Smith BH, and Hildebrand JG. Learning piriform cortex pyramidal neurons is associated with increased learning
modulates the ensemble representations for odors in primary olfactory capability during operant conditioning. Eur J Neurosci 10: 1518 –1523,
networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 10476 –10481, 2004. 1998.
Daoudal G and Debanne D. Long-term plasticity of intrinsic excitability: Sah P and Faber ES. Channels underlying neuronal calcium-activated potas-
learning rules and mechanisms. Learn Mem 10: 456 – 465, 2003. sium currents. Prog Neurobiol 66: 345–353, 2002.
Daoudal G, Hanada Y, and Debanne D. Bidirectional plasticity of excitatory Sahley CL. Serotonin depletion impairs but does not eliminate classical
postsynaptic potential (EPSP)-spike coupling in CA1 hippocampal pyrami- conditioning in the leech Hirudo medicinalis. Behav Neurosci 108: 1043–
dal neurons. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99: 14512–14517, 2002. 1052, 1994.
Debski EA and Friesen WO. Role of central interneurons in habituation of Sahley CL, Modney BK, Boulis NM, and Muller KJ. The S cell: an
swimming activity in the medicinal leech. J Neurophysiol 55: 977–994, interneuron essential for sensitization and full dishabituation of leech short-
1986. ening. J Neurosci 14: 6715– 6721, 1994.
Ehrlich JS, Boulis NM, Karrer T, and Sahley CL. Differential effects of Schreurs BG, Tomsic D, Gusev PA, and Alkon DL. Dendritic excitability
serotonin depletion on sensitization and dishabituation in the leech, Hirudo microzones and occluded long-term depression after classical conditioning
medicinalis. J Neurobiol 23: 270 –279, 1992. of the rabbit’s nictitating membrane response. J Neurophysiol 77: 86 –92,
Frank E, Jansen JK, and Rinvik E. A multisomatic axon in the central 1997.
nervous system of the leech. J Comp Neurol 159: 1–13, 1975. Schreurs BG, Gusev PA, Tomsic D, Alkon DL, and Shi T. Intracellular
Frick A and Johnston D. Plasticity of dendritic excitability. J Neurobiol 64: correlates of acquisition and long-term memory of classical conditioning in
100 –115, 2005. Purkinje cell dendrites in slices of rabbit cerebellar lobule HVI. J Neurosci
Frick A, Magee J, and Johnston D. LTP is accompanied by an enhanced 18: 5498 –5507, 1998.
local excitability of pyramidal neuron dendrites. Nat Neurosci 7: 126 –135, Shaw BK and Kristan WB Jr. The whole-body shortening reflex of the
2004. medicinal leech: motor pattern, sensory basis, and interneuronal pathways.
Gainutdinov KL, Chekmarev LJ, and Gainutdinova TH. Excitability J Comp Physiol [A] 177: 667– 681, 1995.
increase in withdrawal interneurons after conditioning in snail. Neuroreport Shaw BK and Kristan WB Jr. Relative roles of the S cell network and
9: 517–520, 1998. parallel interneuronal pathways in the whole-body shortening reflex of the
Gardner-Medwin AR, Jansen JK, and Taxt T. The “giant” axon of the medicinal leech. J Neurophysiol 82: 1114 –1123, 1999.
leech. Acta Physiol Scand 87: 30 –31, 1973. Stackman RW, Hammond RS, Linardatos E, Gerlach A, Maylie J, Adel-
Hoyer D, Hannon JP, and Martin GR. Molecular, pharmacological and man JP, and Tzounopoulos T. Small conductance Ca2⫹-activated K⫹
functional diversity of 5-HT receptors. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 71: channels modulate synaptic plasticity and memory encoding. J Neurosci 22:
533–554, 2002. 10163–10171, 2002.
Le Ray D, Fernandez DS, Belen PA, Fuenzalida M, and Buno W. Het- Straub VA and Benjamin PR. Extrinsic modulation and motor pattern
erosynaptic metaplastic regulation of synaptic efficacy in CA1 pyramidal generation in a feeding network: a cellular study. J Neurosci 21: 1767–1778,
neurons of rat hippocampus. Hippocampus 14: 1011–1025, 2004. 2001.
Lisman JE. Bursts as a unit of neural information: making unreliable synapses Thompson LT, Moyer JR Jr, and Disterhoft JF. Transient changes in
reliable. Trends Neurosci 20: 38 – 43, 1997. excitability of rabbit CA3 neurons with a time course appropriate to support
Mar A and Drapeau P. Modulation of conduction block in leech mech- memory consolidation. J Neurophysiol 76: 1836 –1849, 1996.
anosensory neurons. J Neurosci 16: 4335– 4343, 1996. Tierney AJ. Structure and function of invertebrate 5-HT receptors: a review.
Modney BK, Sahley CL, and Muller KJ. Regeneration of a central synapse Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol 128: 791– 804, 2001.
restores nonassociative learning. J Neurosci 17: 6478 – 6482, 1997. Wang Y. Myomodulin modulation of the Retzius cell and its role in the leech
Moita MA, Rosis S, Zhou Y, LeDoux JE, and Blair HT. Hippocampal place associative learning (PhD thesis). West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University,
cells acquire location-specific responses to the conditioned stimulus during 1998.
auditory fear conditioning. Neuron 37: 485– 497, 2003. Wang Y, Strong JA, and Sahley CL. Modulatory effects of myomodulin on
Morozov A, Muzzio IA, Bourtchouladze R, Van Strien N, Lapidus K, Yin the excitability and membrane currents in Retzius cells of the leech. J
D, Winder DG, Adams JP, Sweatt JD, and Kandel ER. Rap1 couples Neurophysiol 82: 216 –225, 1999.
cAMP signaling to a distinct pool of p42/44MAPK regulating excitability, Wu N, Enomoto A, Tanaka S, Hsiao CF, Nykamp DQ, Izhikevich E, and
synaptic plasticity, learning, and memory. Neuron 39: 309 –325, 2003. Chandler SH. Persistent sodium currents in mesencephalic V neurons
Moss BL, Fuller AD, Sahley CL, and Burrell BD. Serotonin modulates participate in burst generation and control of membrane excitability. J Neu-
axo-axonal coupling between neurons critical for learning in the leech. rophysiol 2004.
J Neurophysiol 94: 2575–2589, 2005. Wu WW, Oh MM, and Disterhoft JF. Age-related biophysical alterations of
Moyer JR Jr, Power JM, Thompson LT, and Disterhoft JF. Increased hippocampal pyramidal neurons: implications for learning and memory.
excitability of aged rabbit CA1 neurons after trace eyeblink conditioning. Ageing Res Rev 1: 181–207, 2002.
J Neurosci 20: 5476 –5482, 2000. Zaccardi ML, Traina G, Cataldo E, and Brunelli M. Sensitization and
Moyer JR Jr, Thompson LT, and Disterhoft JF. Trace eyeblink condition- dishabituation of swim induction in the leech Hirudo medicinalis: role of
ing increases CA1 excitability in a transient and learning-specific manner. serotonin and cyclic AMP. Behav Brain Res 153: 317–326, 2004.
J Neurosci 16: 5536 –5546, 1996. Zhang W and Linden DJ. The other side of the engram: experience-driven
Muller KJ and Scott SA. Transmission at a “direct” electrical connexion changes in neuronal intrinsic excitability. Nat Rev Neurosci 4: 885–900,
mediated by an interneuron in the leech. J Physiol 311: 565–583, 1981. 2003.

J Neurophysiol • VOL 94 • DECEMBER 2005 • www.jn.org


Downloaded from journals.physiology.org/journal/jn at UNAM Fac De Med Dir Gen De Biblio (132.248.016.189) on March 10, 2020.

You might also like