You are on page 1of 12

Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future Cole, M., & Levitin, K. (Eds.). (2005).

The autobiography
discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. of A. R. Luria: A dialogue with the making of mind. Mah-
wah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cole, M. (1998). Can cultural psychology help us think
about diversity? Mind, Culture and Activity, 5, 291–304. Cole, M., & Means, B. (1981). Comparative studies of how
doi:10.1207/s15327884mca0504_4 people think: An introduction. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Cole, M. (2006). Culture and cognitive development in
phylogenetic, historical, and ontogenetic perspective. In D. Cole, M., & Scribner, S. (Eds.). (1974). Culture and
Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychol- thought: A psychological introduction. New York, NY:
ogy: Vol. 2. Cognition, perception and language (6th ed., Wiley.
pp. 636 – 686). New York, NY: Wiley.
Gay, J., & Cole, M. (1967). The new mathematics and an
Cole, M. (2006). Internationalism in psychology: We need old culture. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

it now more than ever. American Psychologist, 61, 902–


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

917. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.61.8.902 Newman, D., Griffin, P., & Cole, M. (1989). The construc-
tion zone: Working for cognitive change in school. New
Cole, M. (2007). Giyoo Hatano’s analysis of psychological York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
tools. Human Development, 50, 73– 80. doi:10.1159/
000097687 Scribner, S., & Cole, M. (1981). The psychology of liter-
acy. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Cole, M., & Cole, S. (1989). The development of children.
New York, NY: Scientific American Press. Sharp, D., Cole, M., & Lave, C. (1979). Education and
cognitive development: The evidence from experimental
Cole, M., & Derry, J. (2005). We have met technology and research. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child
it is us. In R. Sternberg & D. Preiss (Eds.), Intelligence Development, 44(1–2, Serial No. 178).
and technology: The impact of tools on the nature and de-
velopment of human abilities (pp. 209 –228). Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Education as an Intergenerational Process of
Cole, M., & the Distributed Literacy Consortium (Eds.).
(2006). The Fifth Dimension. An after-school program
Human Learning, Teaching, and Development
built on diversity. New York, NY: Russell Sage Founda- Michael Cole
tion. University of California, San Diego

Cole, M., Dore, J., Hall, W. S., & Dowley, G. (1978). Sit-
uational variability in the speech of preschool children.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 318, 65–
105. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1978.tb16356.x In this article I argue that the future of psychological
research on educational processes would benefit from an
Cole, M., & Engeström, Y. (2007). Cultural-historical ap- interdisciplinary approach that enables psychologists to
proaches to designing for development. In J. Valsiner & A.
Rosa (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of sociocultural
psychology (pp. 484 –507). New York, NY: Cambridge Editor’s Note
University Press. Michael Cole received the Award for Distinguished Contri-
butions of Applications of Psychology to Education and
Cole, M., Gay, J., Glick, J. A., & Sharp, D. W. (Eds.). Training. Award winners are invited to deliver an award
(1971). The cultural context of learning and thinking. New address at the APA’s annual convention. A version of this
York, NY: Basic Books. award address was delivered at the 118th annual meeting,
held August 12–15, 2010, in San Diego, California. Arti-
Cole, M., John-Steiner, V., Scribner, S., & Souberman, E. cles based on award addresses are reviewed, but they dif-
(Eds.). (1978). L. S. Vygotsky, mind in society: The devel- fer from unsolicited articles in that they are expressions of
opment of higher processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni- the winners’ reflections on their work and their views of
versity Press. the field.

796 November 2010 ● American Psychologist


locate their objects of study within the cultural, social, and Psychological researchers seeking to develop effective
historical contexts of their research. To make this modes of education by expanding the unit of analysis, de-
argument, I begin by examining anthropological accounts spite differences among them, have often turned for inspi-
of the characteristics of education in small, face-to-face, ration to work in anthropology and sociology, those disci-
preindustrial societies. I then turn to a sample of plines that have traditionally been taxed with analysis of
contemporary psychoeducational research that seeks to the contexts toward which psychologists gesture. This shift
implement major, qualitative changes in modern in focus is often accompanied by invocation of processes
educational practices by transforming them to have the that generally go ignored in traditional psychological stud-
properties of education in those self-same face-to-face ies of the educational process. So, for example, Varenne
societies. Next I examine the challenges faced by these and McDermott (1998) argued that cultural and institu-
modern approaches and briefly describe a multi- tional contexts, as well as the social ecology of the school
institutional, multidisciplinary system of education that itself, play a crucial role in shaping the processes that lead
responds to these challenges while offering a model for to educational success or failure. In her well-known work
educating psychology students in a multigenerational on communities of practice, Jean Lave (1993) argued
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

system of activities with potential widespread benefits. forcefully that learning is a pervasive process in all of hu-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

man life; consequently, a narrow emphasis on deliberate


Keywords: educational practices, deliberate instruction, cogni- teaching in rigidly controlled classroom situations should
tive apprenticeship, community of learners, serious gaming be replaced by research acknowledging the fact that “de-
veloping an identity as a member of a community and be-
In recent decades, there has been a growing chorus of psy-
coming knowledgeably skillful are part of the same pro-
chologists interested in education calling for broadening the cess, with the former motivating, shaping, and giving
unit of psychological analysis from one in which an individ- meaning to the latter, which it subsumes” (p. 65). She and
ual person confronts a well-defined task to one that includes Etienne Wenger invoked the notion of “legitimate periph-
both the person and the task “in context” (Barab & Plucker, eral participation” in communities of practice as the crucial
2002; Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Brown & Campione, 1994; Cog- process inducing learning (Lave & Wenger, 1991).
nition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt [CTGV], 1992; Efforts by psychologists to reorganize formal schooling
Forman, Minnick, & Stone, 1993; Rogoff, 1990; Wertsch, on the basis of these ideas have taken a wide variety of
1998). Sasha Barab stated the issue clearly: forms. These include instituting pedagogical procedures
At the very core of the traditional pedagogy is the polariza- that permit children greater agency in planning and orga-
tion of the learner and the learning context. Such polarization nizing their own lessons (DePalma, Matusov, & Smith,
inevitably leads to the production of impoverished knowledge 2009; Rogoff, Turkanis, & Bartlett, 2001), organizing for
(inert knowledge), which is knowledge that can be recalled teachers to become personally familiar with community
when students are explicitly requested to do so, but is not “funds of knowledge” by arranging for them to spend time
something that can be readily applied to relevant real world in the community (Gonzalez, Moll, & Amanti, 2005), and
situations. (Barab, 2005, para. 1) creating computer virtual worlds and modes of small-group
educational activity that seek to bring the lifeworld into the
The term context ordinarily goes underspecified in such classroom (Barab, Thomas, Dodge, Carteaux, & Tuzun,
accounts, but the spirit of the call to take context seriously 2005; Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt,
is clear enough. For Bronfenbrenner (1979), the most in- 1992).
clusive level of context was culture, embodied in the domi- Below I return to examine specific examples of psycho-
nant ideology of the social group and its constituent insti- educational research in this tradition. But first I must back
tutions. Barab and Roth (2006) linked context with the up, historically, and branch out geographically and discipli-
ecology of the educational activities being studied, con- narily in order to explain what makes these intellectual
ceiving of ecology as a lifeworld, which they referred to as currents especially interesting. If the main argument of this
“the environment from the perspective of an individual” (p. article is correct, there is a striking similarity between the
3). Others have identified the relevant level of context with basic impulse of educational psychologists who wish to
a group of people who spend time together engaged in make persons in contexts their unit of analysis and the pro-
joint activities in some institutionalized setting, such as a cess of education observed by anthropologists and sociolo-
school, a classroom, or a work setting. This line of think- gists who have worked among people in nonliterate, nonin-
ing identifies the relevant unit of analysis as the commu- dustrial societies in relatively small, face-to-face groups.
nity in which the individual is located, as indicated by such The major issue is to determine how and if the educational
terms as communities of practice (Lave & Wenger, 1991), processes observed in those societies can be adapted to
community of learners (Brown & Campione, 1996), and achieve contemporary educational goals, by modern means
communities of inquiry (Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1991). under modern conditions.

November 2010 ● American Psychologist 797


Educational Practices in Small, Preliterate, range of experiences and behavior are controlled by his social
Preindustrial Societies space, and on the other, everything he learns causes it to ex-
pand and become more differentiated. In the lifetime of the
Children were not a major focus of early anthropologists, individual it changes pari passu with his psycho-physical and
but from the late 19th century onward, a steady trickle of social development. . . . In the evolution of an individual’s
reports, peaking in the 1930s, purported to describe the social space we have a measure of his educational develop-
content and process of education among “primitive” peo- ment. (p. 213)
ples (Chamberlain, 1896; Firth, 1936/1957; Hoernlé, 1931;
Malinowski, 1928; Whiting, 1941). My discussion draws What is special about the social sphere of the Tallensi,
on two major sources, Meyer Fortes’s (1938/1970) essay he wrote, is that
on education derived from his broader ethnographic work the social sphere of adult and child is unitary and undi-
in Ghana in the 1930s and Mariëtte de Haan’s (1999) care- vided. . . . As between adults and children, in Tale society,
ful ethnographic study of learning and teaching among the the social sphere is differentiated only in terms of relative
Mazahua people of Western and Northern Mexico in the capacity. All participate in the same culture, the same round
1990s.1 of life, but in varying degrees, corresponding to the stage of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Meyer Fortes published his essay “The Social and Psy- physical and mental development. . . . They are actively and
chological Aspects of Education in Taleland” in a special responsibly part of the social structure, of the economic sys-
supplement to the journal Africa: Journal of the Interna- tem, the ritual and ideological system. . . . [This] means that
tional Institute of African Languages and Cultures in 1938. the child is from the beginning oriented towards the same
reality as its parents and has the same physical and social ma-
At that time, the Talensi were a largely agricultural people
terial upon which to direct its cognitive and instinctual en-
living in the savannah land in the north of the country that dowment. The interests, motives, and purposes of children are
is now called Ghana. There were a few missionary schools identical with those of adults, but at a simpler level of organi-
in the area at the time, but by and large, people grew up zation. Hence the children need not be coerced to take a share
illiterate and monolingual in their native language. It is in economic and social activities. They are eager to do so.
education in this sociocultural context that was the focus of (Fortes, 1938/1970, p. 202)
Fortes’s interest.
Education among the Talensi, he wrote, In addition, Fortes (1938/1970) emphasized that
is a social process, a temporal concatenation of events in the training of the young is seldom regularized or system-
which the significant factor is time and the significant phe- atized, but occurs as a “by-product” of the cultural routine;
nomenon is change. Between birth and social maturity the that the kinsfolk, and particularly the family, are mainly re-
individual is transformed from a relatively peripheral into a sponsible for it; . . . it is conducted in a practical way in rela-
relatively central link in the social structure. (Fortes, 1938/ tion to the “actual situations of daily life.” (p. 202)
1970, p. 202)
These comments should not be taken to mean that there
The challenge is to understand this process of change. is no differentiation of educational experience among the
Fortes included in his view of education both knowledge Tallensi and, by well-documented extension, among “prim-
required as a practical matter to sustain oneself and the itive” peoples more generally. There are activities such as
social group in its local ecology as well as the beliefs and blacksmithing, bone setting, and midwifery that are spe-
customs at the center of moral life of the group that gov- cialized by kinship group and gender. It is in such special-
erns social relations. Neither of these forms of education is ized areas that the form of education referred to as appren-
“an unwitting process” he informed us, but they occur in ticeship arises, a topic to which I return. But the general
somewhat different ways. picture is very clearly that of children participating will-
Common to Fortes’s view of Tallensi educational pro- ingly in a wide variety of practical activities, while adults
cesses of both kinds is that they take place within what he adjust the difficulty of their participation, in a continual
refers to as the social sphere (or social space). Crucially, social process in which relative ability is the measure of
the participation of children and adults in the social sphere the forms of participation.
is distinctly different from that encountered in modern, in- De Haan’s (1999) research, despite its distance in time
dustrial societies. Fortes (1938/1970) defined the social and place, both confirms and extends Fortes’s (1938/1970)
sphere in the following, prescient, terms: account, in part because the teaching/learning process was
An individual’s social space is a product of that segment of
the focus of her work and in part because she had data-
the social structure and that segment of the habitat with which
he or she is in effective contact. To put it in another way, the 1
For a broad review of similar materials taken from a diverse set of so-
social space is the society in its ecological setting seen from cieties with special reference to the issues raised in this article, see Ro-
the individual’s point of view. The individual creates his so- goff, Paradise, Mejı́a Arauz, Correa-Chávez, and Angelillo (2003), to
cial space and is in turn formed by it. On the one hand, his which I refer later in the article.

798 November 2010 ● American Psychologist


recording tools that were unimaginable when Fortes did his Examples of Psychoeducational Research Modeled on
work. “Learning,” she wrote, “was not set apart, nor was a Traditional Educational Practices
completely different domain created for the sake of learn-
ing. . . . Learning was to take place by participating in the Clearly it is impossible deliberately to turn back the histor-
normal course of the activity and by meeting ‘authentic’ ical clock and undo all of the cultural/technological, social,
challenges” (de Haan, 1999, p. 224). and economic changes that distinguish the lifeworlds of
De Haan (1999) also documented how subtle adjust- children living in contemporary industrial societies from
ments are made by the parents for the sake of encouraging those of the Tallensi of the 1930s. Nor would most readers
learning: of this article wish to trade places with the Mazahua, or
the millions of people around the globe who live in near-
For instance, the activity was slowed down, “unnecessary” subsistence circumstances or worse. For such people, for-
risks were taken to involve children, modeling was organized mal schooling, years of formal schooling, is seen as the
and feedback given. However, these learning opportunities
road to progress measured in life expectancy, health, and
served the task activity at the same time and were an integral
part of it. The child was never oriented on a task without be- all of those material circumstances that make it hard to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ing actually an integral part of it. (p. 224) keep their children down on the farm once they have seen
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the lights of the city. So it is something of a paradox that a


The most striking aspect of the role a child was given when great many contemporary psychologists wish to reform
engaging in a learning situation is that she was not treated as
modern educational practices precisely by reinstituting the
a learner but a full participant. Children were treated, as far
as possible, as if they were already proficient and were ex- qualities of educational experience that I have just been
pected to learn from the demands this kind of situation pro- citing.
vokes. . . . Once engaged in an activity children’s proficiency I do not plan to rehearse here all of the negative fea-
is taken full advantage of although their level of competence tures of modern schooling that have created this apparently
is different from that of an adult. (pp. 224 –225) paradoxical attempt to reform modern educational practices
As a result of these arrangements, de Haan (1999) con- based on the model of small, nonliterate societies. The
cluded that such situations were arranged so that children painful shortcomings of modern schooling, including low
were given repeated opportunities to increase their compe- levels of achievement, widespread student disinterest, high
tence while at the same time subtly shifting their roles to dropout rates, and massive disparities among children ac-
become more and more responsible for more of the joint cording to ethnic and social class backgrounds are the daily
task. One consequence of this form of joint activity is that, fare of the news media (for a thoroughgoing critical analy-
although adults and children have different statuses, they sis, see Varenne & McDermott, 1998). Rather, I concen-
are both full-fledged performers, so the child is both able trate on contemporary work by psychologists who seek to
to identify with the adult role and to develop new skills refocus their research on a unit of analysis that includes
and acquire knowledge without such learning being the learners in their contexts, variously conceived. Individual
focus of joint attention. Another consequence is that when research efforts of this kind are by no means all of a piece;
the child cannot carry out a part of the joint activity, the they differ with respect to the particular units of analysis
adult takes over the child’s role and, without distracting they focus on, the particular qualities and kinds of joint
either party from the task at hand, thereby provides a activity they consider most important, the role they assign
model for the child to learn from without detracting from to modern technologies, their methods of research, and, of
the child’s status as a full member of the group or from the course, the particular theoretical traditions on which they
flow of the activity. draw to justify their methods and to interpret their results.
With these two accounts as my “data” concerning the How successful are such efforts, and what challenges do
educational practices of people living in two small, face-to- they face?
face communities where schooling plays a negligible role Cognitive Apprenticeship
in everyday life (and the extensive documentation of simi-
lar patterns from around the world summarized by Rogoff, In an influential early article, Collins, Brown, and Newman
Paradise, Mejı́a Arauz, Correa-Chávez, & Angelillo, 2003), (1989) coined the term cognitive apprenticeship to signal
I now return to my central argument: Contemporary schol- their desire to reorganize instruction on models of interac-
ars who seek to reorient educational research to study edu- tion derived from apprenticeship arrangements observed in
cation in its cultural, ecological context, and to re-embed small, face-to-face, nonliterate societies for specialized ac-
education in what they refer to as lifeworlds or communi- tivities. They took as their model the observations of Jean
ties of practice, are seeking precisely those qualities of ed- Lave (1977) among Vai tailors in Liberia (the work of
ucation that are characteristic of the learning of people for Maynard & Greenfield, 2006, on weaving among a group
whom formal schooling is absent from, or peripheral to, in south-central Mexico provides an even more elaborated
their everyday lives. case to think about).

November 2010 ● American Psychologist 799


Instruction based on the idea of cognitive apprentice- make visible the ordinarily invisible processes central to
ship, Collins and his colleagues (1989) argued, should cen- reading comprehension: formulating questions based on the
ter on students carrying out tasks in a content domain text, summarizing the text, clarifying difficulties in the text,
through observation and practice, combined with teacher and making predictions about what follows next in the text.
modeling, coaching, and dynamic support (“scaffolding” Reciprocal teaching is implemented by having the teacher
and “fading”). In principle, at the outset, cognitive appren- and students silently read a paragraph, which they then
tices, like Lave’s (1977) apprentice tailors or Maynard and discuss in terms of the four key procedures. Initially the
Greenfield’s (2006) apprentice weavers, should know the teacher models each procedure and the methods of discus-
end result of the work and have access to the entire pro- sion. Then, gradually, the structure of the activity morphs
cess of successful practice in the domain, which should as students take on the role of the teacher, at first with ex-
provide them with a coherent goal toward which to strive. tensive coaching that allows them to take over whatever
Traditional apprentices typically work in a group setting part of the overall task they can while filling in the remain-
where many levels of expertise are simultaneously on dis- der. The whole task always gets done, but the structure of
play, which provides further guidance for their own efforts the doing changes.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and feedback concerning their skills relative to those of Brown and Palincsar (1989) illuminated the affinities as
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

others. So, too, with cognitive apprentices. well as the differences between reciprocal teaching and
Concerned as they were with reading, writing, and traditional forms of apprenticeship. For example, the orga-
mathematics, Collins and his colleagues (1989) knew they nization of the activity makes clear that serious work is
must modify their apprenticeship approach while maintain- being conducted; the adult sets the overall goals, but all
ing the key properties they valued. As they noted, in focus- must contribute. When engaged in the group activity, the
ing on tasks such as farming or weaving, they were dealing adult teacher closely monitors students engaged in the
with tasks involving processes that were open to observa- teaching role while giving them room to control the discus-
tion, so that adjustment of behavior could be more or less sions as much as they can, provides feedback tailored to
transparently related to achievement of the desired out- their current levels of ability, and takes over the responsi-
come. By contrast, the processes involved, say, in reading ble role if progress falters, just as Fortes and de Haan de-
and comprehending a text in an alphabetic language are scribed for the learning activities they observed in the ev-
not, strictly speaking, even communicable, let alone exter- eryday activities of the communities they studied.
nally visible. As a consequence, successful use of cognitive Reciprocal teaching also differs from the cases de-
apprenticeship instruction must “bring these tacit [internal] scribed by Fortes and de Haan. For example, in traditional
processes into the open, where students can observe, enact, learning, the goals of the activity are shared by the adults
and practice them with help from the teacher and from and children from the start, whereas in reciprocal teaching,
other students.” (Collins et al., 1989, p. 458). adults initially understand the goal, but the children clearly
Another challenge to adopting a cognitive apprentice- do not and cannot precisely because they do not know how
ship approach is that in traditional apprenticeships, the to read. Moreover, teachers are not kin, and schools are not
problems and tasks arise from the practical work to be villages, making the emotional bonds between participants
done. Again, by contrast, when applying an apprenticeship as well as the motivations for engaging in the activity sig-
approach to modern school subject matter, manifest and nificantly different. Despite such differences, Brown and
measurable learning and conceptual change are the goal. her colleagues reported the effectiveness of this kind of
Moreover, it is not considered sufficient to master the task cognitive apprenticeship using a variety of content domains
at hand—learning should be generalized, transferable, and (Brown & Palincsar, 1989).
“decontextualized,” putting further constraints on the struc-
turing of joint activity that could easily distort the very From Cognitive Apprenticeships to Communities of
quality of interaction that one wants to achieve. Despite Learners and Intent Participation
these challenges, there are plausible examples of the effec- Over time, the approach dubbed cognitive apprenticeship
tive application of the significant aspects of the cognitive has expanded and morphed to include a wide variety of
apprentice approach. approaches that move beyond specialized activities to in-
clude a broad range of curricular contents and means of
Reciprocal Teaching structuring the roles of teachers and students.
A primary example invoked by Collins and his colleagues
The Jasper Program
(1989) is the procedure referred to as reciprocal teaching
(Palincsar & Brown, 1984). Reciprocal teaching was de- An important variation on the cognitive apprentice ap-
signed for students who had learned basic decoding skills proach was carried out over several years by the Cognition
but in so doing had failed to learn to read for meaning. and Technology Group at Vanderbilt (CTVG; (1997). Like
The method uses several procedures to externalize and others who complained that standard schooling produces

800 November 2010 ● American Psychologist


inert, encapsulated knowledge, the CTGV researchers searchers’ approach to organizing cognitive apprenticeships
sought a means of organizing what they referred to as gen- expanded to be group activities linked to problems beyond
erative learning, which results in flexible expertise and the classroom.
generalizable knowledge.
To implement this goal, the CTGV researchers used Communities of Learners
then-state-of-the-art technology to create a series of 15–20- Two major programs of research, each of which began
min videos that engaged middle school students in various with ideas similar to the idea of cognitive apprenticeship
problem-solving scenarios constructed something like a but were theoretically motivated by notions such as
children’s television series. Their design goals were (a) to “guided participation” and “zone of proximal develop-
immerse learners in a motivating environment using clear ment,” morphed into systemic approaches to classroom
but incomplete narrative structures in a manner that pro- design that highlight the qualities of interpersonal interac-
vided many opportunities for the students to generate novel tion and relegate the particular curricular content and the
problem solutions, seek new information, and engage in the tools to implement it to a subordinate, although important,
use of school-relevant knowledge to solve problems in the role. I describe two such programs that differ in their ap-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

simulated real world and (b) to provide various supports to proaches in instructive ways.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the students to deal with unusual (for school) levels of Fostering a community of learners. As described by
problem complexity. Brown and Campione (1996), the overarching goal of the
The adventures that constituted the Jasper programs Fostering a Community of Learners (FCL) program was to
posed sequences of problems whose solutions required var- engage students in authentic, generative inquiry into the
ious kinds of mathematics, planning, decision making, and “Big Ideas” of an academic discipline (their own focus was
so forth. For example, the first episode involved planning a on evolutionary biology, but the approach has been applied
trip up a river and required calculations of interrelation- to a variety of disciplinary topics including English and
ships between an ultralight plane’s fuel consumption, mathematics). Their guiding ideal was to transform the
weight limits, and other factors that had to be taken into classroom into a research community by organizing chil-
account to reach the group’s destination. The second pro- dren’s activities according to several distinct participation
gram continued from the first but introduced a new set of structures. These included benchmark lessons, reciprocal
challenges to be dealt with, culminating in the need to fig- teaching, and a cycle of activities in which students re-
ure out the fastest way to rescue a wounded eagle, which searched, discussed, and then performed (perhaps in a re-
involved students in the use of mapping skills, geometry, search report, perhaps in a play, etc.) the topic at hand in
and trade-offs in the use of resources of various kinds. order to develop their understandings of the “Big Ideas.”
The CTGV researchers studied this problem-solving ac- Benchmark lessons refer to whole-class lessons or dem-
tivity in a wide variety of classrooms, where teachers im- onstrations intended to activate children’s existing knowl-
plemented it in a variety of ways. Their own preferred edge, to introduce central new concepts, and to serve as
method was for the teacher to engage in anchored instruc- anchors to which participants refer throughout the project.
tion, which emphasizes procedures that help students to Reciprocal teaching, as I have already described, is in-
understand why it is important for them to learn the sub- tended to enhance reading comprehension in small-group
skills necessary to solve the problems embedded in the joint reading activities. To organize research-discussion-
overall program. They also sought to have teachers engage performance cycles, Brown and Campione (1996) em-
in flexible support of the students to ensure that all bene- ployed jigsaw groups (Aronson, Blaney, Stephin, Sikes, &
fited from engaging the processes of solving the complex Snapp, 1978) in which a “Big Idea” was divided into con-
problem, creating what they termed generative learning. stituent subconcepts. Working in small groups, students
The students sometimes encountered teachers who were so were helped to develop expertise in one constituent sub-
thoroughly imbued with a “basics first” approach to teach- concept; then the groups were reorganized so that as an
ing that they deconstructed the careful attempts to provide ensemble, each group had “all the pieces of the puzzle,”
an environment that would constantly motivate subskill which they then had to integrate through discussion. Fi-
learning in the service of larger, authentic goals. In such nally, at the end of a research cycle, students had to “per-
cases, students did learn subskills, but they proved unable form” their new knowledge in action. In sum, the FCL pro-
to use them in a meaningful way to deal with the complex gram was designed to create forms of participation that are
problem that motivated their use. Other teachers provided highly dialogic and anchored in core disciplinary concepts
worksheets to help students to minimize errors but at the and to foster authentic immersion in real-life concerns
cost of being able to generate successful subproblems or to (e.g., species survival) that provide natural linkages to the
monitor the relative success of different ways of organizing larger social ecology of the classroom and the students’
to solve the overall problem. When implemented as in- lives in their communities.
tended, evaluations supported the effectiveness of the re- As implemented by Brown and Campione (1996), FCL

November 2010 ● American Psychologist 801


proved to be an extremely effective form of education that also a teacher is evident from the ubiquity of activities
fulfilled the goals of linking individuals to their socioeco- when students are teaching each other or adults.
logical contexts and treating the community of students in It is difficult to do justice to the richness and complex-
their community as the unit of analysis. In experimental ity of this pedagogical strategy in a few paragraphs. As-
tests, students engaged in the program outperformed con- suming that the reader can glean the basic spirit of this
trol students to a striking degree. model of organizing the learning process, a natural, but
The Open Classroom community of learners. Barbara difficult-to-answer question is whether it works. Given the
Rogoff was not the originator of the Open Classroom (OC) nature of the school (there is no standardized testing, the
school in Salt Lake City, but she participated in it as a par- children’s parents self-select them into the school, etc.),
ent for several years while her children were enrolled two criteria of success stand out. First, the school became
there. By her own account, her ideas about the organization an accredited public charter school in the Salt Lake City
of students’ learning and her subsequent research on cul- school system and has been in operation for almost 40
tural variations in the organization of students’ learning years; it has community support and has been institutional-
were markedly shaped by the experience. The most com- ized. Second, children graduating from the school are suc-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

plete description of this work is to be found in a book that cessful when they move on to regular public schools at
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

she edited together with two of the school’s teachers (Ro- graduation time. Less conventional, perhaps, is the finding
goff, Turkanis, & Bartlett, 2001; contributors include the that the children left the school enthusiastic about going to
editors as well as parents, other teachers, and students). school, indicating that the school was achieving one of its
The OC school was opened in 1971 by a group of par- goals: to create lifelong learners. (Indirect evidence is pro-
ents dissatisfied with the curricula offered in either public vided by a study of a different school implementing a simi-
or private schools in their area. The originators wanted to lar philosophy in which students were studied as they tran-
create a child-centered curriculum, with significant daily sitioned to a traditional public high school; DePalma,
participation by parents, in which everyone would be con- Matusov, & Smith, 2009). The OC students were generally
sidered a learner. Child-centered is a term with many successful academically in their new schools, but they also
meanings, but as it evolved, child-centered at the OC came made a sharp distinction between what they considered
to mean “to build instruction on children’s interests in a authentic learning and the strategies they had to adopt to
collaborative way—learning activities are planned by chil- achieve academic success in the new, conventional school.
dren as well as adults, and adults learn from their own in-
“Serious” Gaming in Microworlds: Quest Atlantis
volvement as they foster children’s learning” (Rogoff et al.,
2001, p. 33). By serious gaming I refer to the burgeoning genre of digi-
A central principle underpinning the organization of the tal video games that have been designed for a primary pur-
OC community of learners is balancing, as in balancing pose that goes beyond pure entertainment. Such games
between a totally open, child-run classroom and an adult- have become prevalent in many domains of contemporary
run, tightly scripted classroom; balancing parent, teacher, life, including education, and the process of design is ordi-
and student roles; balancing the Utah state curriculum stan- narily guided by principles derived from psychological re-
dards with the interests of the students and parents; and search (Gee, 2007; Prensky, 2000). I have selected the
balancing the use of worksheets and hands-on projects. In game Quest Atlantis as an example because it has been
common with the FCL classrooms, the OC focuses on “Big specifically designed to go beyond individuals confronting
Ideas,” defined broadly as understanding key patterns in well-defined tasks as its unit of analysis and because its
the world and ways of learning about them. designer has explicitly drawn on the same family of theo-
The affinities between this mode of organizing the ries that has motivated the other examples described above.
teaching/learning process and those of prior examples Moreover, drawing heavily on design strategies used in the
should be clear if only from the overlap of vocabulary. commercial 3-D, massive multiuser gaming environments,
What appears to be different in this case is the insistence it has been used on an extraordinarily broad scale, involv-
that students engage in tasks of genuine interest to them; ing some 10,000 children in several countries.
the resulting diversity of activities that students, even in a The core activity in Quest Atlantis is to complete quests
single classroom, are engaged in on a daily basis; and the on the mythical planet of Atlantis, which is presented as a
difficulty of figuring out who is teaching and who is learn- place much like Earth. Quests are designed to engage chil-
ing. The idea that everyone is a learner in the OC is clear dren in simulated and real-world activities that are deemed
from the difficult learning process that parents, who spend to be socially and academically meaningful: for example,
three sessions a week at the school as the “price of admis- fish are dying in the local lake, a strange disease is killing
sion,” typically go through before they can contribute ef- people, a mayor needs information about how to solve the
fectively and comfortably to the ever-changing mixture of city’s problems. Children conduct research by a variety of
activities and their proper role in them. That everyone is means, develop plans of action, implement those actions,

802 November 2010 ● American Psychologist


find out about the consequences of their actions, and en- trialized country? The fact of the matter is that such pro-
gage with other participants in discussions about their ex- grams, despite the enormous efforts it takes to implement
periences. Quests are also designed to include standards- them and the equally arduous efforts made to sustain them,
based academic skills, and a number of evaluation studies are very difficult to find. Why? One place to look for an-
have demonstrated learning gains in science, language arts, swers is in the fate of the programs I have reviewed.
and social studies. At the same time, the real-world anchor- Consider reciprocal teaching. As implemented by
ing of the quest contents serves to link the child in the Palincsar and Brown (1984), there is no doubt that it mark-
context of the game with the game in the context of the edly improved comprehension skills. Moreover it proved a
real world and other participants, wherever they may live. successful component in the broader FCL program. But
Not only do children appear to find the game and its ancil- when others sought to implement the program, they often
lary activities attractive in school; they also seek to engage failed to obtain the same effects although they sought faith-
the game outside the classroom. Moreover, they and their fully to replicate the correct procedures. Why? Perhaps
teachers report increased levels of engagement and interest because the procedure itself requires flexible expertise for
in pursuing the curricular issues modeled in the game out- its implementation? Palincsar (2006) reported a case in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

side of school (see http://atlantis.crlt.indiana.edu/). which the four key strategies underpinning the activity
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

were treated mechanically as four discrete kinds of action


Is It Possible to Go Back to the Future? to be taught by direct instruction, not as tools to help shape
I hope that at this juncture I have established two basic genuine dialogue in a collaborative group process. The fail-
points. First, the variety of efforts to extend psychological ure to implement the underlying dialogic principle for
analyses of educational processes beyond children engaging which the four strategies were intended to serve as an in-
in well-defined tasks to include the larger context of the frastructure totally undermined the original design. Brown
child’s activity, situating it in the child’s community by a and Palincsar (1989) referred to such changes as “toxic
variety of means, bears a strong affinity to forms of educa- mutations.” Despite extensive efforts to support teachers in
tion that predominated in preliterate societies studied by their efforts to implement the procedure effectively and
anthropologists in the 19th and 20th centuries, forms that descriptions of this method in many leading educational
continue to dominate the educational practices of millions textbooks (see, e.g., http://edr1.educ.msu.edu/CompStrat/
of children today for whom formal schooling is, at most, a login.asp [username: demo, password: demo]), at present
peripheral part of their personal experience. Although only there is no information available on how widely reciprocal
one of the psychologists whose work I have reviewed re- teaching is used and/or abused (A. S. Palincsar, personal
ferred to Fortes (1938/1970), many actually did seem to be communication, December 19, 2009).
quoting him when he wrote that the process of develop- The same is true of the FCL program. Despite concerted
ment is one in which during development “the individual is attempts by Brown and Campione’s (1996) colleagues to
transformed from a relatively peripheral into a relatively replicate their achievement, generalization of this method
central link in the social structure” (p. 202) or that “in the beyond its initial implementation has proven elusive, with
evolution of an individual’s social space we have a mea- at most partial implementations being reported (see the
sure of his educational development” (p. 214), the social special issue of the Journal of Curriculum Studies, 2004,
space being “the society in its ecological setting seen from Vol. 36, No. 2).
the individual’s point of view” (p. 216). The OC program, based on principles that Rogoff has
Second, I hope to have established that by reasonable more recently referred to as “intent community participa-
criteria (sometimes including randomized experimental tion” (Rogoff et al., 2003), continues to exist and is a part
tests but more often by using less formal indicators such as of the Salt Lake City school system. Moreover, even a cur-
children’s meeting or exceeding state performance stan- sory search of the Internet turns up millions of entries on
dards or achieving success in later schooling), such alterna- open classrooms and more than a thousand mission state-
tive practices are successful. In several of the cases re- ments of specific schools implementing some form of this
ported, such practices also result in children who become strategy. As Cuban (2009) noted, there are many class-
self-motivated learners and who seek to engage in socially rooms adhering to some version of the principles of the
responsible activities related to the real-world problems OC school, and many more in which teachers use a mix-
they face in their communities. ture of teacher-directed and child-centered approaches, ac-
Granted these two points, new questions arise that take cording to their inclinations and circumstances.
us well beyond the confines of psychology, requiring us to Nonetheless, there are clearly restrictions on how wide-
consider issues most often addressed by political scientists, spread this form of education is likely to become. As Cu-
sociologists, historians, and economists. To begin with, if ban (2009) pointed out, the educational forms that domi-
such programs are indeed successful, why do they not per- nate a modern society are subject to myriad ideological and
vade education in the United States and every other indus- economic vicissitudes; it would be foolish to expect a sin-

November 2010 ● American Psychologist 803


gle form to dominate in a pluralistic, democratic society learning and development, we are faced with the uncom-
where one family’s virtuous education is another’s invita- fortable fact that the social ecologies that will support the
tion to perdition. It is significant, in this regard, that most forms of education our psychological theories urge upon us
schools that adopt open classrooms and require heavy adult are very difficult to find. The basic form of education dom-
participation are attended by children of highly educated, inating the classrooms of the United States and every other
generally professional people. This is true of both the Salt industrially advanced country is eerily similar to the form
Lake City OC as well as the similar program studied by of education to be found in ancient China or Sumeria.
DePalma, Matusov, and Smith (2009). The general pattern seems to be as follows: Exceptions
Such considerations should cause one to pause and think to what Rogoff (2003) referred to as the assembly-line
about for whom and under what conditions transmission/ method of mass education can work, but the socioeconom-
industrial-assembly-line education can be considered appro- ic-cultural ecologies that make them possible are limited.
priate. Since its inception in the ancient world, mass The existence proofs I have described in this article are
schooling has always been associated with large aggrega- like patches of soil in a rock pile. This situation restricts a
tions of people living in close proximity to each other as purely psychological approach to educational reforms to
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

part of something akin to a state political organization the building of tiny gardens that have to struggle to survive
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(Guthrie, 2003). It has always had as its primary mission against forces outside of the analytic lens of the discipline.
the teaching of literacy and numeracy (Goody, 1975). It We can write manuals galore on how to build these tiny
has also always been associated with the creation of social ecosystems, and we can spend years tending them. But we
hierarchy and economic privilege. And failure has always cannot break enough rocks to make them spread.
been one of its essential characteristics (Varenne & Mc-
Dermott, 1998), an idea often spoken about by those who So, What Is to Be Done?
consider schooling nothing more than a social “sorting ma- I do not intend to purvey a message of despair. Rather, I
chine” (Spring, 1989). think it is important to recognize that we provide the kinds
Elaborate hierarchy, marked economic inequality, and of education our social ecologies permit and promote, fail-
widespread failure are not characteristics of the forms of ures and all. It is changes in the modes of human life, in-
education associated with the education practices found cluding the role of education in promoting human adapta-
among the Tallensi, the Mazahua, or the myriad other tion, that will ultimately shape the forms that educational
small, face to face communities that have served as a activity takes. Modern mass education is the product of the
model for psychoeducational practices that seek to study socioeconomic forces that gave us the industrial revolution,
children in their sociocultural-ecological contexts. Conse- the assembly line, and McDonald’s. These forces also
quently, whatever the merits of the psychological theories brought about unprecedented welfare for many, savage ine-
of learning and development that seek to go beyond the qualities, a dependence on forms of energy that threaten
individual child confronting a predesigned task, the perfor- our long-term ability to live on this planet, and the poten-
mance of which will be assessed on an equally individual tial of exterminating our own species in a matter of min-
basis (and I firmly believe as a psychologist that these ap- utes.
proaches are scientifically correct), they will ineluctably But none of those circumstances have quenched human
fail to provide a theory of how to provide successful edu- inventiveness, nor did the circumstances facing the human
cation, as specified by educational policymakers, because beings who stared out of the caves of Lascaux seem any
they fail to look beyond the limits of psychology to incor- less challenging to them than our own situation seems to
porate the socioeconomic, historically conditioned factors us. Educational innovations, however limited, are variations
that limit their applicability. in modes of human life on which natural selection will
We seem to be caught in a double paradox. To begin work as it has always done. The challenge is to come up
with, I have argued, and I believe demonstrated, that con- with variations, shaped by the illusion that human life mat-
temporary psychological theorizing about the educational ters in the grand scale of things that do in fact have a fu-
process that adopts persons-in-their-contexts (communities, ture.
lifeworlds, ecologies) as the unit of analysis has overtly or To some, perhaps many, reading these words, the inno-
unconsciously sought to reinstitute forms of education that vations of people such as Sasha Barab and the CTGV re-
are characteristic of historically extinct or contemporane- search group may appear to be the answer. It seems incon-
ously marginalized peoples. To the extent that they are trovertible that new digital technologies offer enormous
successful in doing so, they appear to be successful in cre- potential for reorganizing human life, including the process
ating the kind of broadly educated, self-motivated, lifelong of education for that life. In the current enthusiasm for new
learner whom educational policymakers urge upon us as a technologies as the answer, my view is that we should take
national norm to be met or exceeded by all. But when we into account the lessons of history and approach such en-
go beyond existence proofs that substantiate our theories of deavors as part of an answer, an answer that will bring

804 November 2010 ● American Psychologist


with it unforeseen consequences with which we will have community. Together, with the children, they conduct a
to cope, just as we are having to cope with the problems wide range of projects that they jointly document in digital
currently facing us, which not long ago were viewed as the formats that are then placed on a website and that become
answer to the problems of prior generations. a growing record of the accomplishments of the local col-
In closing, I wish to put forward another strategy for reor- laborative community. In addition, the college students
ganizing educational activity that I believe to be a potentially document their experiences in the activities at the site in
productive way to supplement the efforts of those whose work detail following each visit and read academic research on
I have been discussing here. For the past quarter of a century, processes of learning and development, culture, class, and
my colleagues at the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cog- other topics that their professors think could be usefully
nition and myself, along with researchers in several parts of related to their field experiences; their notes then become
the world (see www.uclinks.org for an overview) have been the data for research papers and self-reflection on their
designing and implementing educational programs for imple- own development at the end of each academic quarter or
mentation during the after-school hours. This work shares semester.
several design characteristics with the psychoeducational re- An important outcome of this line of investigation is
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

search discussed above, but it also has some unique features evidence that participation in such “theory–practice” activi-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

that I believe are addressing several educational challenges at ties produces a marked change in the undergraduates’ ideas
the same time. about their own education. Difficult readings are not dis-
missed out of hand but interrogated to see if they can be
Shared Features
linked to any useful sort of practice. Students, being forced
The settings we are creating mix play and education, often by circumstances to take responsibility for the children in
using computers and computer networks as media for pre- collaboration with their peers, undergo a change in their
senting intriguing content that is a mixture of entertainment identities as college students and in their feeling of connec-
and academic content; we provide children ranging in age tion to cultural communities to which they would ordi-
from 4 to 16 years with a wide variety of potential activi- narily have no access.
ties to choose among and a quasi-structure that serves to As an ensemble, this interinstitutional collaborative ap-
coordinate involvement in the activities. We not only pro- proach creates a multigenerational community of practice
vide computer-based experiences, but we link these to a that makes visible to all participants a variety of futures
variety of lifeworld concerns. Children come to these activ- that they could not otherwise have imagined. It simulta-
ities voluntarily, and according to a variety of measures, neously addresses a range of educational challenges from
their academic achievement and social development are kindergarten through 12th grade, and it is fun, even if it is
promoted by their participation in these activities (see Cole a lot of hard work. It carries college students beyond a
and the Distributed Literacy Consortium, 2006, and data focus on individuals engaged in well-specified tasks in
presented at www.uclinks.org). carefully pared-down social situations and requires them to
engage the wider social ecology, motivating the inclusion
Features Unique to Our Approach of sociology, anthropology, and other social science disci-
The settings we are creating involve collaborations between plines as a central part of their education as psychologists.
institutions of higher learning and local communities that The “gardens for development” created by this form of
we have dubbed a U–C link (a handy acronym that began theory–practice college education face many challenges.
as a reference to the University of California but that Those my colleagues and I have created have both pros-
works well for universities and communities in general). pered and declined over the decades, depending on which
As part of an interinstitutional collaboration, each side is of the entire gamut of challenging conditions faced by the
obligated to provide the other with resources that they other persons-in-contexts projects discussed here that they
could not obtain outside of the collaboration. Community had to face. But wherever these “gardens” have emerged,
institutions (youth clubs, schools, churches, community they have provided a rich, multigenerational environment
learning centers) that already engage children during the for education. It is my belief that research strategies such
after-school hours provide space, a staff supervisor, and as this one, which solve several problems at once, seem to
whatever resources such as computers and telephone lines offer at least one promising way for educational programs
that they can afford. The college or university provides to survive both the scythe of natural selection processes
supervised undergraduates who spend several hours a week and the rocky ground with which our enormously pluralis-
with the children at the site, participating with the children tic society is currently dealing.
and local adults in carrying out the activities the commu-
nity deems most interesting and useful. The college stu- Author’s Note
dents, of course, bring their own enthusiasms and skills, I would like to acknowledge the extensive assistance pro-
but these are always subordinated to the wishes of the vided by Ray McDermott in helping me work through the

November 2010 ● American Psychologist 805


ideas presented here through repeated drafts of the manu- Chamberlain, A. F. (1896. The child in primitive cultures.
script on a very short time schedule. Consultations with New York, NY: Macmillan.
Barbara Rogoff and Sasha Barab are also gratefully ac-
knowledged. Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt. (1992).
Correspondence concerning this article should be ad- The Jasper experiment: An exploration of issues in learning
dressed to Michael Cole, Laboratory of Comparative Hu- and instructional design. Educational Teachnology Re-
man Cognition, University of California, San Diego, La search and Design, 40(1), 65– 80. doi:10.1007/BF02296707
Jolla, CA 92093-0506. E-mail: mcole@ucsd.edu
Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt. (1997).
References The Jasper project: Lessons in curriculum, instruction, as-
sessment, and professional development. Mahwah, NJ: Erl-
Aronson, E., Blaney, N., Stephin, C., Sikes, J., & Snapp, baum.
M. (1978). The jigsaw classroom. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Cole, M., & the Distributed Literacy Consortium. (Eds.).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(2006). The Fifth Dimension. An after-school program


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Barab, S. A. (2005). Research. Retrieved from http://


inkido.indiana.edu/barab/research.html. built on diversity. New York, NY: Russell Sage Founda-
tion.
Barab, S. A., & Plucker, J. A. (2002). Smart people or
smart contexts? Cognition, ability, and talent development Collins, A., Brown, J. S., & Newman, S. E. (1989). Cogni-
in an age of situated approaches to knowing and learning. tive apprenticeship: Teaching the craft of reading, writing
Educational Psychologist, 37, 165–182. and mathematics. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learn-
ing and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser (pp.
453– 494). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Barab, S. A., & Roth, W.-M. (2006). Curriculum-based
ecosystems: Supporting knowing from an ecological per-
Cuban, L. (2009). Fad or tradition: The case of the open
spective. Educational Researcher, 35(5), 3–13. doi:
classroom. Retrieved from http://larrycuban.wordpress.com/
10.3102/0013189X035005003
2009/12/05/fad-or-tradition-the-case-of-the-open-classroom/
Barab, S., Thomas, M., Dodge, T., Carteaux, R., & Tuzun, de Haan, M. (1999). Learning as cultural practice: How
H. (2005). Making learning fun: Quest Atlantis, a game children learn in a Mexican Mazahua community. Amster-
without guns. Educational Technology Research and De- dam, The Netherlands: Thela Thesis.
velopment, 53(1), 86 –107.
DePalma, R., Matusov, E., & Smith, M. (2009). Smuggling
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human develop- authentic learning into the school context: Transitioning
ment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. from an innovative elementary to a conventional high
school. Teachers College Record, 111, 934 –972.
Brown, A. L., & Campione, J. C. (1994). Guided discovery
in a community of learners. In K. McGilly (Ed.), Class- Firth, R. (1957). We the Tikopia: A sociological study of
room lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom kinship in primitive Polynesia. Boston, MA: Beacon Press.
practice (pp. 229 –272). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Brad- (Original work published 1936)
ford Books.
Forman, E., Minnick, N., & Stone, A. (Eds.). (1993). Con-
Brown, A. L., & Campione, C. (1996). Psychological the- texts for learning. New York, NY: Oxford University
ory and the design of innovative learning environments: On Press.
procedures, principles, and systems. In, L. Schauble & R.
Glaser (Eds.), Innovations in learning: New environments Fortes, M. (1970). Social and psychological aspects of edu-
for education (pp. 289 –325). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. cation in Taleland. In M. Fortes (Ed.), Time and social
structure and other essays (pp. 201–239). New York, NY:
Brown, A., & Palincsar, A. (1989). Guided, cooperative Humanities Press. (Original work published 1938)
learning and individual knowledge acquisition. In L. B.
Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning and instruction: Essays Gee, J. P. (2007). Good video games ⫹ good learning:
in honor of Robert Glaser (pp. 393-451). Hillsdale, NJ: Collected essays on video games, learning, and literacy.
Erlbaum. New York, NY: Peter Lang.

806 November 2010 ● American Psychologist


Gonzalez, N., Moll, L. C., & Amanti, C. (Eds.). (2005). Palincsar, A. S., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teach-
Funds of knowledge: Theorizing practices in households, ing of comprehension-fostering and comprehension-moni-
communities and classrooms. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. toring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1(2), 117–175.
doi:10.1207/s1532690xci0102_1
Goody, J. (1975). Schools, education, and the social sys-
tem: Some utopian suggestions. Interchange, 6(4), 1–5. Prensky, M. (2000). Digital game-based learning. New
doi:10.1007/BF02145342 York, NY. McGraw-Hill.

Guthrie, J. W. (Ed.). (2003). Encyclopedia of education. Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive
New York, NY: Macmillan. development in social context. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Hoernlé, A. W. (1931). An outline of the native conception
of education in Africa. Africa: Journal of the International Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human develop-
Institute of African Languages and Cultures, 4(2), 145– ment. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

163.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Rogoff, B., Paradise, R., Mejı́a Arauz, R., Correa-Chávez,


Lave, J. (1977). Tailor-made experiments and evaluating M., & Angelillo, C. (2003). Firsthand learning through in-
the intellectual consequences of apprenticeship training. tent participation. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 175–
Newsletter of the Institute for Comparative Human Devel- 203. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145118
opment, 7(2), 1–3.
Rogoff, B., Turkanis, C. G., & Bartlett, L. (2001). Learn-
Lave, J. (1993). Situating learning in communities of prac- ing together: Children and adults in a school community.
tice. In L. B. Resnick, J. M. Levine, & S. D. Teasley New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
(Eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp. 63–
82). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1991). Higher levels of
agency for children in knowledge building: A challenge for
Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legiti- the design of new knowledge media. Journal of the Learn-
mate peripheral participation. New York, NY. Cambridge ing Sciences, 1(1), 37– 68. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls0101_3
University Press.
Spring, J. H. (1989). The sorting machine revisited: Na-
Malinowski, B. (1928). The sexual life of savages in tional educational policy since 1945. New York, NY:
North-Western Melanesia. London, England: Routledge. Longman.

Maynard, A. E., & Greenfield, P. M. (2006). Cultural Varenne, H., & McDermott, R. P. (1998). Successful fail-
teaching and learning: Processes, effects, and development ure: The school America builds. Boulder, CO: Westview
of apprenticeship skills. In Z. Beerman, N. C. Burbules, & Press.
D. Silberman-Keller (Eds.), Learning in places: The infor-
mal education reader (pp. 139 –162). New York, NY: Peter Wertsch, J. (1998). Mind as action. New York, NY: Ox-
Lang. ford University Press.

Palincsar, A. S. (2006, November). Reciprocal teaching Whiting, J. W. M. (1941). Becoming a Kwoma. New Ha-
1982 to 2006: The role of research, theory, and represen- ven, CT: Yale University Press.
tation in the transformation of instructional research. Os-
car Causey Address presented at the National Reading
Conference, Los Angeles, CA.

November 2010 ● American Psychologist 807

You might also like