You are on page 1of 31

Vol 441|11 May 2006|doi:10.

1038/nature04676

LETTERS
Neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex encode
economic value
Camillo Padoa-Schioppa1 & John A. Assad1

Economic choice is the behaviour observed when individuals analyse each cell in relation to the choice pattern recorded in the same
select one among many available options. There is no intrinsically session.
‘correct’ answer: economic choice depends on subjective prefer- Our recordings focused on area 13 in the OFC. Figure 2 illustrates
ences. This behaviour is traditionally the object of economic the activity of one representative neuron. The cell’s activity does not
analysis1 and is also of primary interest in psychology2. However, depend on whether juice A is offered on the left or on the right
the underlying mental processes and neuronal mechanisms are (Fig. 2b). It also does not depend on whether the monkey chooses
not well understood. Theories of human and animal choice1–3 have the juice on the left or the juice on the right (that is, makes an eye
a cornerstone in the concept of ‘value’. Consider, for example, a movement to the left or to the right; Fig. 2c). However, the cell’s
monkey offered one raisin versus one piece of apple: behavioural activity varies with the offer type. This is consistent across
evidence suggests that the animal chooses by assigning values to the neuronal population. We recorded the activity of 931 cells
the two options4. But where and how values are represented in the and we analysed their neuronal responses in seven time windows
brain is unclear. Here we show that, during economic choice, (see Methods). We tested the activity of each cell in each time
neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex5–18 (OFC) encode the value of window with a three-way analysis of variance (ANOVA, with factors:
offered and chosen goods. Notably, OFC neurons encode value [position of juice A] £ [movement direction] £ [offer type],
independently of visuospatial factors and motor responses. If a P , 0.001). Rarely do responses depend on either the spatial con-
monkey chooses between A and B, neurons in the OFC encode the figuration of the offers or the motor output (,5% neurons). In
value of the two goods independently of whether A is presented on contrast, the activity of 505 (54%) neurons varies significantly
the right and B on the left, or vice versa. This trait distinguishes depending on the offer type in at least one time window. Pooling
the OFC from other brain areas in which value modulates activity time windows, a total of 1,379 responses are significantly modulated
related to sensory or motor processes19–25. Our results have broad by the offer type (Supplementary Fig. S2).
implications for possible psychological models, suggesting that The cell shown in Fig. 2d has a U-shaped response similar to that
economic choice is essentially choice between goods rather than hypothesized for a neuron encoding the chosen value. For this
choice between actions. In this framework, neurons in the OFC session V(A) ¼ 1.9V(B). Accordingly, the activity of the cell is
seem to be a good candidate network for value assignment
underlying economic choice.
In our experiments, monkeys choose between two types of juice
(A and B; where A is preferred) offered in different amounts. For
example, in the session shown in Fig. 1, the monkey chooses between
water (juice A) and unsweetened Kool-Aid (juice B). Offer types
include 1B:2A, 1B:1A, 2B:1A, 3B:1A, 4B:1A, 6B:1A, and 10B:1A, and
the ‘forced choices’ 0B:1A and 3B:0A. Behaviourally, we observe a
trade-off between juice type and juice quantity. The monkey chooses
A when 1B, 2B, or 3B are available as alternatives, it is roughly
indifferent between the two juices when offered 4B:1A, and it chooses
B when 6B or 10B are available.
We interpret this pattern of choice in terms of the ‘relative value’ of
the two juices4: in this case, the value of 1A is roughly equal to the
value of 4B. Fitting a sigmoid curve provides the better estimate
V(1A) ¼ V(4.1B), where V(x) indicates the value of x. Assuming a
linear value function, we obtain V(A) ¼ 4.1V(B). This equation puts
different quantities of juices A and B on the same value scale. On this
basis, we can compute for each trial the value of the juice chosen by
the monkey. Expressing values in units of V(B), the variable ‘chosen
value’ is about 4 when the monkey chooses 1A or 4B. When the
monkey chooses 2A, the chosen value is about 8. When the monkey Figure 1 | Experimental design. a, Trial structure (see Methods). b, Example
chooses 6B, 10B or 3B, the chosen value is respectively equal to 6, 10 of behavioural choice pattern. The plot shows the percentage of trials in
or 3. Hence, we can make specific hypotheses regarding the neuronal which the monkeys chose juice B (y axis) for various offer types (x axis). A
representation of juice values. In different sessions and with different sigmoid fit provides the measure of the relative value n* ¼ 4.1. The dotted
juices, we record different behavioural choice patterns. We then red circle indicates the saccade target chosen by the monkey.

1
Department of Neurobiology, Harvard Medical School, 220 Longwood Avenue, Boston, Massachusetts 02115, USA.

223
© 2006 Nature Publishing Group
LETTERS NATURE|Vol 441|11 May 2006

low when the monkey chooses 1A or 2B (in units of V(B), chosen Although many neurons seem to encode chosen value, offer value
value < 2), it is higher when the monkey chooses 2A or 4B (chosen or juice taste, the relation between their activity and these three
value < 4), and it is highest when the monkey chooses 3A or 6B variables could be subordinate to a correlation with other
(chosen value < 6). A linear regression of this response on the behavioural variables. For example, neurons in the OFC might
variable chosen value provides R 2 ¼ 0.86. Similar U-shaped encode the number of squares on the monitor (or variables pro-
responses are frequent in the OFC, and Fig. 3a–c illustrates three portional to number, such as juice quantity, or absolute luminance of
more examples. We also find other types of responses. For example, the visual stimulus). To cast a wide net, we examine the linear
neuronal responses often reflect the value of one of the two juices dependence of neuronal data on 19 possible variables (Supplemen-
alone. Figure 3d, e shows two cells in which activity covaries with tary Fig. S1). For example, we analyse the variables ‘chosen number’,
the value of A offered and the value of B offered, respectively. We ‘total number’ and ‘total value’. We include in this analysis 1,379
label these responses as related to the variable ‘offer value’. Other responses significantly modulated by the offer type, and we regress
frequently observed responses vary in a binary fashion depending each response separately on each variable. Collectively, the 19 vari-
on the type of juice chosen by the monkey, independently of the ables explain 1,227 (89%) neuronal responses. However, the 19
amount (Fig. 3f). We interpret these responses as related to the variables are often highly correlated.
variable juice ‘taste’. To identify a few variables that best describe the neuronal popu-
lation, we adapt procedures for variable selection commonly used in
multilinear regression in the presence of multi-collinearity. Both
the stepwise and the best-subset methods identify the variables offer
value, chosen value and taste, which explain well the large majority of
responses (1,085/1,379 ¼ 79% responses, with mean R 2 ¼ 0.63). A
post-hoc analysis indicates that the explanatory power of these three
variables is significantly higher than that of challenging alternatives.
Furthermore, data from the two monkeys analysed separately pro-
vide statistically indistinguishable results. Finally, a bilinear
regression analysis indicates that in 890/1,085 (82%) cases, adding
a second variable or a quadratic value term does not improve the
regression significantly (Supplementary Results S5 to S10 and Sup-
plementary Figs S4 to S11). We conclude that, as a population, OFC
responses indeed encode the variables offer value, chosen value and
taste.
We next turn to a specific analysis of U-shaped responses (Figs 2d,
3a–c). In our experiments, relative values were generally stable within
any recording session. However, the relative value of any given pair of
juices could vary from day to day. For example, the relative value of
apple juice versus peppermint tea varied between 1.5 and 3. This
variability provides a further opportunity to test the neuronal
encoding of value; specifically, U-shaped responses should reflect
this variability. For this analysis, we test the entire neuronal popu-
lation with the regression function a0 þ aA ðmA Þ þ aB ðmB Þ; where mA
and m B represent the amounts of juices A and B chosen by the
monkey. We define a response to be U-shaped if both a A and a B differ
from zero (P , 0.01). If U-shaped responses indeed encode the value
of the chosen juice, the slope ratio k* ¼ a A/a B should be, for each
U-shaped response, equal to the relative value (n*) measured in that
session. Most importantly, the slope ratio k* obtained for different
responses should covary with n*. To test this prediction, we compute
the regression k* ¼ b 0 þ b 1n* separately for every juice pair. Aver-
aging across juice pairs, we obtain b 0 (^s.e.m.) ¼ 20.13 (^0.15)
and b 1 ¼ 1.05 (^0.15), consistent with the identity k* ¼ n*. This
result demonstrates that U-shaped responses do not reflect the
quantity of any particular juice ingredient (for example, sugar).
Rather, they encode the value monkeys assign to the juice
Figure 2 | Activity of one neuron. a, Rasters. Each line represents one trial
and each small dot represents one spike. Trials, arranged by offer type, are
they choose to consume (Supplementary Results S10 to S12 and
aligned at the ‘offer’ (left) and at the ‘juice’ (right). The blue highlight marks Supplementary Figs S12 to S14).
the post-juice time window. ‘Sacc’ indicates the time of the saccade. The timing of neuronal activation is as follows: the average
b, Activity profiles shown separately for trials in which juice A is offered on neuronal activity peaks shortly after the offer, declines during the
the left (red) or on the right (green). The cell activity does not depend on the delay, is low before and during the eye movement, and has two
spatial configuration of the visual stimulus. c, Activity profiles shown secondary peaks at juice delivery (Supplementary Fig. S3). To
separately for trials in which the monkey chooses the juice offered on the left appreciate how the variables offer value, chosen value and taste are
(red) or on the right (green). The cell activity does not depend on the represented in the OFC over time, we analyse the activity of each cell
direction of the eye movement. d, The top panel shows the choice pattern in 50-ms, non-overlapping time bins. Figure 4 shows the number of
recorded in this session (n* ¼ 1.9). The bottom panel shows the activity of
cells encoding each of the three variables at different times. Remark-
the cell (^s.e.m.) recorded in the pre-offer (light grey, control) and post-
juice (black) time windows. Note that the response does not reflect simple ably, the time profile of different variables seems to reflect the mental
physical properties of the visual stimulus, such as the number of squares processes the monkey presumably undertakes during a trial. Shortly
displayed on the monitor. For example, offer types 1B:3A and 3B:1A, which after the offer, when the monkey assigns values to the two juices,
are visually identical except for the colour of the squares, elicit very different neurons encoding the offer value (that is, the value of one juice or the
activation. other) are most prevalent. Also during the delay, many neurons
224
© 2006 Nature Publishing Group
NATURE|Vol 441|11 May 2006 LETTERS

Figure 3 | Activity of six neurons. For each cell, the top panel shows the in which the monkey chose juice A (diamonds) or juice B (circles). The
choice pattern, with the relative value indicated on the top left. The bottom response reflects the chosen juice type independently of the amount.
panel shows the activity of the cell. a–c, Responses encoding the chosen Responses were recorded in the post-offer (a, d, e, blue), pre-juice (b, cyan),
value. The response in c is negatively correlated with the chosen value (high and post-juice (c, f, black) time windows. For each cell, the curves in light
activity for low value). d–e, Responses encoding the value of juice A offered grey show the activity in the pre-offer time window. Error bars represent
(d) and the value of juice B offered (e). We refer to these responses as related s.e.m.
to the offer value. f, Response encoding the juice taste. Here we separate trials

encode the chosen value (that is, the value of the juice the monkey will when the eye movement is associated with higher value20. On this
eventually consume), even though the choice is still covert (because basis, it has been proposed that parietal neurons encoding the value
the ‘go’ signal has not been given yet). Finally, after the monkey has of all possible courses of action form a common path for decision-
indicated its choice, before and after juice delivery, many neurons making, and that their activity is actually the subject of economic
encode the taste of the chosen juice. theory27. According to this ‘action-based’ model, economic choice is
Conceptually, responses encoding the chosen value are particularly fundamentally choice between actions. That neurons in the OFC
interesting because, in addition to being independent of the visuo- encode the economic value of offered and chosen goods per se, as
motor contingencies of the task, they are also independent of the opposed to reflecting value as a modulation of visuomotor processes,
specifics of the good, namely juice type and juice amount. These suggests an alternative ‘good-based’ model, according to which
responses encode economic value in a non-specific way. Further economic choice is fundamentally choice between goods. In this
research is necessary to establish whether this result generalizes to view, choice is made between goods, and a suitable motor action is
other kinds of goods, such as non-comestible goods26. The interpre- subsequently planned and executed.
tation of offer value responses is made more cautiously because, Several arguments seem to favour the good-based model. From a
assuming linear value functions, the value of a given amount of juice computational perspective, a modular design separating the mental
is proportional to the juice quantity. operations of ‘choosing’ and ‘moving’ is more parsimonious28,29. In
‘Value’ is known to modulate the activity of neurons in several addition, values processed in the OFC are logically sufficient for
sensory and motor areas19–25. For example, neurons in the lateral good-based choice. The action-based model would thus imply that,
intraparietal area activate when monkeys plan a saccade towards a during economic choice, the nervous system operates in a compu-
particular location of the visual field; their response is enhanced tationally inefficient way, while undertaking all the processes needed
to choose efficiently. Finally, a vast literature links choice in various
domains to the OFC. For example, human patients and monkeys
with OFC lesions can present eating disorders and hyperorality6–8,
abnormal risk-seeking and gambling behaviour9,10, and impulsivity,
altered personality and abnormal social behaviour6,11. In contrast,
parietal lesions typically result in visuospatial deficits such as
hemi-neglect or Balint’s syndrome30. In conclusion, together with
other lines of evidence, the present results support a good-based
psychological model of economic choice behaviour.

METHODS
Each trial begins with the monkey fixating the centre of a computer monitor
(Fig. 1a). After 1.5 s, two sets of squares appear on opposite sides of the fixation
point (‘offer’). The colour of the squares indicates the juice type and the number
of squares indicates the juice amount. For example, a monkey offered three blue
squares versus one yellow square chooses between three drops of peppermint tea
and one drop of grape juice. After a randomly variable delay (1–2 s), two saccade
targets appear near the offers (‘go’). The monkey indicates its choice with an eye
movement, and must maintain fixation on the target for an additional 0.75 s
Figure 4 | Time course. We assign each neuron to one of the three variables before juice delivery (‘juice’). The trial is aborted if the monkey breaks fixation
only if the regression slope is significantly different from zero (P , 0.01), before the go signal. The amounts of the two offered juices (0–10 drops) vary
and we include all 931 neurons in the analysis. The dotted line indicates pseudo-randomly. For a given offer type, left/right positions are counter-
chance level (9.31). balanced (that is, the monkey may be offered 1A on the left and 3B on the
225
© 2006 Nature Publishing Group
LETTERS NATURE|Vol 441|11 May 2006

right, or vice versa). A variety of different pairs of juices are used in different 17. Wallis, J. D. & Miller, E. K. Neuronal activity in primate dorsolateral and orbital
sessions (Supplementary Methods). prefrontal cortex during performance of a reward preference task. Eur.
We analyse cell activity in the following time windows: 0.5 s pre-offer (a J. Neurosci. 18, 2069–-2081 (2003).
18. Tremblay, L. & Schultz, W. Relative reward preference in primate orbitofrontal
control time window); 0.5 s post-offer; late delay (0.5–1.0 s after the offer); 0.5 s
cortex. Nature 398, 704–-708 (1999).
pre-go; reaction time (from go to saccade); 0.5 s pre-juice; and 0.5 s post-juice.
19. Kawagoe, R., Takikawa, Y. & Hikosaka, O. Expectation of reward
For the statistical analysis, we separate for each offer type trials in which the modulates cognitive signals in the basal ganglia. Nature Neurosci. 1, 411–-416
monkey chooses juices A and B (Supplementary Methods). (1998).
20. Platt, M. L. & Glimcher, P. W. Neural correlates of decision variables in parietal
Received 28 November 2005; accepted 24 February 2006. cortex. Nature 400, 233–-238 (1999).
Published online 23 April 2006. 21. Leon, M. I. & Shadlen, M. N. Effect of expected reward magnitude on the
response of neurons in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque.
1. Samuelson, P. A. Foundations of Economic Analysis (Harvard Univ. Press,
Neuron 24, 415–-425 (1999).
Cambridge, 1947).
22. Shidara, M. & Richmond, B. J. Anterior cingulate: single neuronal signals related
2. Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (eds) Choices, Values and Frames (Russell Sage
Foundation–-Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge/New York, 2000). to degree of reward expectancy. Science 296, 1709–-1711 (2002).
3. Kagel, J. H., Battalio, R. C. & Green, L. Economic Choice Theory: An Experimental 23. Ikeda, T. & Hikosaka, O. Reward-dependent gain and bias of visual responses
Analysis of Animal Behavior (Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge/New York, in primate superior colliculus. Neuron 39, 693–-700 (2003).
1995). 24. Roesch, M. R. & Olson, C. R. Impact of expected reward on neuronal activity in
4. Padoa-Schioppa, C., Jandolo, L. & Visalberghi, E. Multi-stage mental process prefrontal cortex, frontal and supplementary eye fields and premotor cortex.
for economic choice in capuchins. Cognition 99, B1–-B13 (2006). J. Neurophysiol. 90, 1766–-1789 (2003).
5. Ongur, D. & Price, J. L. The organization of networks within the orbital and 25. McCoy, A. N., Crowley, J. C., Haghighian, G., Dean, H. L. & Platt, M. L. Saccade
medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cereb. Cortex 10, reward signals in posterior cingulate cortex. Neuron 40, 1031–-1040 (2003).
206–-219 (2000). 26. Deaner, R. O., Khera, A. V. & Platt, M. L. Monkeys pay per view: adaptive
6. Pasquier, F. & Petit, H. Frontotemporal dementia: its rediscovery. Eur. Neurol. valuation of social images by rhesus macaques. Curr. Biol. 15, 243–-248 (2005).
38, 1–-6 (1997). 27. Glimcher, P. W., Dorris, M. C. & Bayer, H. M. Physiological utility theory and
7. Hodges, J. R. Frontotemporal dementia (Pick’s disease): clinical features and the neuroeconomics of choice. Games Econ. Behav. 52, 213–-256 (2005).
assessment. Neurology 56, S6–-S10 (2001). 28. Simon, H. A. The architecture of complexity. Proc. Am. Phil. Soc. 106, 467–-482
8. Butter, C. M., McDonald, J. A. & Snyder, D. R. Orality, preference behavior, and (1962).
reinforcement value of nonfood object in monkeys with orbital frontal lesions. 29. Pinker, S. How the Mind Works (Norton, New York, 1997).
Science 164, 1306–-1307 (1969). 30. Zigmond, M. J., Bloom, F. E., Landis, S. C., Roberts, J. L. & Squire, L. R.
9. Rahman, S., Sahakian, B. J., Hodges, J. R., Rogers, R. D. & Robbins, T. W. Fundamental Neuroscience Ch. 53 (Academic, San Diego, 1999).
Specific cognitive deficits in mild frontal variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain
122, 1469–-1493 (1999). Supplementary Information is linked to the online version of the paper at
10. Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H. & Damasio, A. R. Failure to respond www.nature.com/nature.
autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal
cortex. Cereb. Cortex 6, 215–-225 (1996). Acknowledgements We thank K. Irwin, T. LaFratta, D. Averbuch, J. LeBlanc,
11. Raleigh, M. J. & Steklis, H. D. Effect of orbitofrontal and temporal neocortical S. Peled and J. Harper for technical assistance and animal care; E. Brown for a
lesions of the affiliative behavior of vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops discussion on the statistical analysis; and T. Herrington, D. Freedman and E. Bizzi
sabaeus). Exp. Neurol. 73, 378–-389 (1981). for comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by postdoctoral
12. Izquierdo, A., Suda, R. K. & Murray, E. A. Bilateral orbital prefrontal cortex fellowships from the Lefler Foundation and from the Harvard Mind/Brain/
lesions in rhesus monkeys disrupt choices guided by both reward value and Behavior Initiative (to C.P.-S.) and by a grant from the National Institute of
reward contingency. J. Neurosci. 24, 7540–-7548 (2004). Neurological Disorders and Stroke (to J.A.A.).
13. O’Doherty, J., Kringelbach, M. L., Rolls, E. T., Hornak, J. & Andrews, C. Abstract
reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Author Contributions C.P.-S. performed all aspects of the study, including the
Nature Neurosci. 4, 95–-102 (2001). design of the experiment, collecting and analysing the data, and writing the
14. Knutson, B., Taylor, J., Kaufman, M., Peterson, R. & Glover, G. Distributed manuscript. J.A.A. assisted in experimental design, data analysis and manuscript
neural representation of expected value. J. Neurosci. 25, 4806–-4812 (2005). preparation.
15. Rolls, E. T., Sienkiewicz, Z. J. & Yaxley, S. Hunger modulates the responses to
gustatory stimuli of single neurons in the caudolateral orbitofrontal cortex of Author Information Reprints and permissions information is available at
the macaque monkey. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1, 53–-60 (1989). npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions. The authors declare no competing
16. Roesch, M. R. & Olson, C. R. Neuronal activity related to reward value and financial interests. Correspondence and requests for materials should be
motivation in primate frontal cortex. Science 304, 307–-310 (2004). addressed to C.P.-S. (camillo@alum.mit.edu).

226
© 2006 Nature Publishing Group
Neurons in Orbitofrontal Cortex Encode Economic We used the following juices, listed in roughly
Value decreasing order of preference (associated colors in
By Camillo Padoa-Schioppa and John A. Assad parenthesis): high-sugar lemon Kool-Aid (dark
yellow), grape juice (bright green), fruit punch
Supplementary Information (magenta), apple juice (diluted to 1/2 with water, dark
green), cranberry juice (diluted to 1/3 or 1/4 with
water, pink), water (white), milk (red), peppermint tea
(bright blue), tea (light brown), low-sugar agua frescas
Supplementary Methods Kool-Aid (light red), low-sugar tamarind Kool-Aid
(dark brown), slightly salted water (0.60 g/l or 0.65 g/l
Subjects, setup, and experimental design concentration, light gray).
One male (“V”, 9.5 Kg) and one female (“L”, 6.3 Kg) Recording sessions lasted typically 200-500 trials. The
rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) participated in the number of trials per offer type presented in each trial
experiments. Neither animal had been used for block was determined at the beginning of each session,
previous studies. During the experiments, monkeys sat and we generally tried to have more trials for offer
in a monkey chair in a darkened room. Their head was types near the animal’s (foreseen) indifference point.
restrained, and their eye position was monitored Typically, this resulted in 20-65 trials for each offer
continuously using a scleral eye-coil system1 (River- type in each recording session. The left/right
bend Instruments). A computer monitor was placed 57 configuration of each offer type was pseudo-
cm in front of the monkeys. The behavioral task was randomized and counterbalanced within each trial
controlled by custom-written software through a block. Each day, monkeys completed 2-8 sessions
computer interface (ITC-18, Instrutech Corporation). (typically 5-6), with different pairs of juices used in
different sessions.
Each trial began with the appearance of a fixation
point (0.2° of visual angle) placed in the center of the For the current experiments, we wanted to have a
monitor. The monkey directed gaze to the fixation stable relative value within any recording session.
point. After 1.5 s, two sets of squares appeared on the Indeed, the relative value was generally stable (though
left and on the right sides of the fixation point (offer). 3% of sessions were excluded from the analysis for
The color of the squares indicated the juice type, and unstable behavior). However, juice values could vary
the number of squares indicated the juice amount. considerably from session to session within each day.
After the offer, the monkey maintained fixation for a In particular, early in the day monkeys were thirstier,
1-2 s randomly variable delay, until a go signal. The and relative values tended to differ less from unity.
go was signaled by the appearance of two saccade Later in the day, monkeys generally became more
targets (0.2° of visual angle) placed 7° to the left and selective and the relative value of less sweetened
to the right of the center fixation point. After the go, juices decreased. As a result, some variability was also
the monkey had 1.5 s to indicate its choice by making present in the relative value measured for any given
an eye movement towards one of the saccade targets. pair of juices in different days. For any given juice
The monkey then maintained fixation onto the chosen pair, the range of variability was typically twofold or
target for an additional 0.75 s before juice delivery. less. The order of preference of different juices (i.e.,
The trial was aborted if the monkey broke center their ranking) remained fairly stable across sessions
fixation at any time before the go signal, or after target and across days for both monkeys (though it was not
acquisition but before juice delivery. Trials were exactly the same for the two monkeys).
separated by a 1.5 s inter-trial interval. Center fixation
was imposed within 1°. Training lasted 6-8 months. During the training,
monkeys initially indicated their choice by moving a
Sets of 0-10 squares indicating the offers were located bidirectional lever by hand. For most of the training,
around the (initially not visible) saccade targets, within we used only water offered in variables amounts (e.g.,
4.2° of visual angle. The side of each individual square 1 drop versus 2 drops). Monkeys spent most of the
subtended 1.05°. The spatial configuration of a given training learning non-specific aspects of the task, for
set of squares (for example, 3 squares on the right) example that objects on the monitor carried
remained the same throughout the experiments. The information about the to-be-received liquid, that a
same color was associated with any given juice type hand movement towards the right was associated to
throughout the experiments. the visual stimulus placed on the right, and, later in
training, that fixation should be maintained during the
S.1
delay. Monkeys were familiarized with each juice placed 1 mm apart. Electrical signals from each
separately for 1-3 single-juice sessions. The most channel were amplified and band-pass filtered
specific aspect of the task, namely the trade-off (custom-designed miniature amplifiers, low frequency
between juice type and juice quantity, did not require cutoff 400 Hz, high frequency cutoff 6 kHz) and
training, as monkeys showed it spontaneously and recorded at 20 kHz by a dedicated processor (Power
immediately. This was consistent with our previous 1401, Cambridge Electronic Design). Action potentials
observations in capuchins using solid foods in full were detected online by threshold crossing and
sight2. For the current experiments, we wanted to have waveforms were saved to disk for subsequent analysis.
relative values away from unity. At the same time, we Spike sorting was performed semi-manually using
wanted relative values to lie within the tested range of commercially available software (Spike 2, version 5,
offer types, so that, if the less preferred juice was Cambridge Electronic Design). We routinely used
offered in sufficiently large amount, the monkey multiple algorithms, including template matching,
would choose it. Prior to recordings, we tested clustering on waveform measurements, and principal
numerous juice types to find suitable pairs. component analysis.

Juices were delivered through a three-line juice tube. Structural MRI scans (1-mm sections) were obtained
Each juice line was controlled by a separate solenoid for both monkeys before and after placing the
valve. We routinely calibrated the three juice lines recording chambers. Scans were performed in a 3.0 T
individually so that the valve-opening times magnet (General Electric) and three-dimensional
corresponded to the desired multiple of juice quantum. reconstruction was performed off-line (Slicer-3D,
During recordings, we used quanta of 80 µl and 65 µl www.slicer.org).
for monkeys V and L, respectively.
Recordings areas
Surgery and recordings
Recordings were performed on the left hemisphere of
Under general anesthesia and aseptic surgery, we monkey V and on the right hemisphere of monkey L.
implanted a head-restraining device and a recording For their sulcal pattern, both hemispheres were
chamber on the skull of the monkeys, and a scleral eye classified as of type II of Chiavaras and Petrides3.
coil1. In both monkeys we used a large oval custom-
made chamber (main axes 50x30 mm). The chambers The position of the chamber allowed us to reach OFC
were centered on stereotaxic coordinates (A30, L0), through nearly straight dorso-ventral penetrations.
with the longer axis parallel to a coronal plane. Thus, Because the chambers were large, we were able to
chambers covered the frontal lobes bilaterally. record from an extended region of OFC. Based on
Following surgery, monkeys were given antibiotics exploratory recordings, we identified in both monkeys
(cefazolin, 20 mg/kg) and analgesics (buprenorphine, a region of interest where neuronal responses were
0.005 mg/kg; flunixin, 1 mg/kg) for 3 days. modulated by the offer type in our task. In monkey V,
Throughout the experiments, we strictly followed the the region of interest was centered in stereotaxic
NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory coordinates (A32.5, L–9.0) and extended for 6 mm
Animals and the guidelines of the Harvard Medical rostro-caudally and 5 mm medio-laterally. In monkey
School Standing Committee on Animals. L, the region of interest was centered in stereotaxic
coordinates (A33.5, L8.5) and extended for 6 mm
For recordings, we used tungsten electrodes (125-µm rostro-caudally and 2 mm medio-laterally. Based on
diameter, 5±1 MΩ initial impedance, Frederick Haer the MRI scans and on the sequence of gray and white
& Co.). Electrodes were advanced with custom-built matter encountered during electrode penetrations, we
motorized micro-drives. The micro-drives, consisting located the regions of interest in the lateral bank of the
of a titanium housing containing a 0-80 trapped screw, medial orbital sulcus and in the medial part of the
were anchored to a Teflon grid placed on the chamber. posterior orbital gyrus. Comparing our reconstruction
A brushless miniature precision gear motor (5.8-mm to the architectonic subdivision of Carmichael and
outer diameter, Micro Precision Systems) drove the Price4,5, we tentatively identified our regions of
trapped screw and was controlled remotely. The interest with area 13m.
system allowed up to 0.5 µm of depth resolution.
Typically four (and up to six) electrodes were used All the neurons described here were recorded from the
each day. We usually advanced electrodes by pairs regions of interest. Apart from this criterion and apart
(one motor for two electrodes), with the two electrodes from imposing good and stable electrical isolation,
neurons were not otherwise selected prior to data
S.2
collection or during the analysis. We did not attempt to (3B:1A, 3B). In principle, there are twice as many trial
identify the cortical layer of recordings. However, types as offer types in any given session. However,
because we reached OFC dorso-ventrally from the many trial types are “empty,” because for most offer
white matter, we presume that the lower layers are types the choice of the monkey is univocal (for
largely represented in our dataset. example, in a given session, a monkey offered 3B:1A
might always choose 3B). In subsequent analyses, we
Analysis of choice pattern and 3-way ANOVA include only trial types with two or more trials.

Unless otherwise specified, we always assume linear Neuronal activity is averaged across trials separately
value functions. Indicating with V(x) the value of x, for each trial type. Hereafter, the term “response”
the linear assumption means that a multiple q of a unit refers to the average activity of one neuron in one time
volume of juice X has a value V(qX) = q V(X). window, as a function of the different trial types. Note
that, for sake of clarity, in figures 2d and 3a-e of the
Data are analyzed in Matlab (PC version 6.5, Math- main text we grouped trials by offer type, not by trial
Works). In each session, we fit the choice pattern with type.
a sigmoid function, which for our data generally
provides an excellent fit (R2>0.95). The flex of the Neuro-econometric analysis: defined variables and
sigmoid determines the relative value n* such that correlation matrix
V(A) = n* V(B). From this equation, we can compute
the value of any amount of juice A and B, up to a What variables are encoded in OFC? As illustrated in
scaling factor. For example, expressing values in units the main text (figure 3), many responses seem quali-
of V(B), the value of b drops of juice B is b, and the tatively related to the variables chosen value, value A
value of a drops of juice A is n*a. Because the offered, value B offered, and taste. For a quantitative
analysis of neuronal responses in relation to the juice analysis, however, we want to consider alternative hy-
values is based on linear regressions (see below), the potheses, namely other variables with which neuronal
results do not depend on the particular units used to responses could a priori correlate. For example, we
express value. want to test the hypothesis that some responses might
encode the other value (i.e., the value of the non-
For quantitative analyses, neuronal firing is averaged chosen juice), or the total value, or the value A chosen;
over the following time windows: pre-offer (0.5 s be- etc. In addition, we want to test variables that might
fore the offer, a control time window); post-offer (0.5 s capture the decision process, such as the value
after the offer); late delay (0.5-1.0 s after the offer); difference (chosen–other) value and the value ratio
pre-go (0.5 s before the go); reaction time (RT; from (other/chosen) value. Most importantly, we want to
the go to the saccade); pre-juice (0.5 s before the distinguish between variables related to value and
juice); and post-juice (0.5 s after the juice). variables related to physical properties of the
juice/stimulus, such as quantity, volume, number, etc.
We first analyze single-trial data from each neuron These physical properties are all proportional to each
with a 3-way ANOVA (factors: [position of juice A] x other, and we collectively refer to them with the
[movement direction] x [offer type]), separately in variable number.(a) To put value and number on equal
each time window. We impose a significance level of
p<0.001. We choose a relatively conservative a
In our experiments, value is subjective but operationally-defined.
threshold because of the large number of responses The neuronal correlates of value and number cannot be dissociated
analyzed (931 neurons x 7 time windows). Because easily using a single good (e.g., a single juice) because in that case
factors [position of juice A] and [movement direction] the two variables are inextricably inter-related (assuming a linear
are rarely significant, we collapse data across these value function, value and number are in fact proportional to each
two factors in all subsequent analyses. We also use the other). Introducing uncertainty does not help, because the
confusion remains between expected value and expected number.
results of the ANOVA as a screening criterion for sub- However, value and number can be dissociated using two different
sequent analyses, which (unless otherwise indicated) juices, insofar as the relative value of the two juices (i.e., the ratio
we restrict to neurons and time windows for which the V(A)/V(B)) differs from unity. For example, in a session in which
factor [offer type] yields a significant effect. 1A=3B, a smaller number (say 2A) may have higher value than a
larger number (say 4B). Of course, a correlation between value and
number always remains (after all, a large quantity of any given
We divide trials into “trial types” based on the offer juice is always more valuable than a small quantity of that same
type and the choice. For example, a monkey facing the juice). For this reason dissociating definitively the neuronal
offer type 3B:1A can choose either 1A or 3B, correlates of value and number ultimately requires the “neuro-
corresponding to the two trial types (3B:1A, 1A) and econometric” analysis described below.
S.3
footing in the analysis, we want to test in the number Neuro-econometric analysis: variable selection
domain every variable tested in the value domain. In
summary, we want to analyze the 19 variables defined The variable-selection analysis is based on the
in figure s1. assumption that individual responses encode each (at
most) one variable. For this analysis, we regress each
These 19 variables are often highly correlated with response separately on each of the 19 variables. Each
each other. To estimate the typical correlation between regression provides a slope and the corresponding R2.
any two variables X and Y, we proceed as follows. For We consider a variable capable of “explaining” a
each session, we compute the correlation coefficient: given response if the regression slope is significantly
r r r r different from zero (p<0.05), and we conventionally
ρ ( X ,Y ) = x ⋅ y x2 ⋅ y2 set R2 = 0 for variables that do not explain the respon-
session
se. In general, any given response can be explained by
r r
where x and y are vectors of values taken by multiple variables. Largely, this is because variables
variables X and Y for different trial types. So defined, are often highly correlated with each other (for
the correlation coefficient varies between –1 and +1. example, this is the case for variables chosen value,
Most informative in this context is the absolute value, total value and chosen number), a situation referred to
which we compute and average across sessions: as multi-collinearity6,7.

In the presence of multi-collinearity, it is often


ρ ( X ,Y ) = ρ ( X ,Y ) possible to identify a small subset of variables that
session sessions
account for much of the data. However, the problem of
Repeating for all pairs of variables, we obtain a identifying the appropriate subset is in principle not
symmetric matrix ρ of elements ρ ( X ,Y ) that vary trivial. For our analysis we adapt two methods of
between 0 and 1. Hereafter, we refer to ρ as the variable selection routinely used in the case of multi-
linear regressions, namely the “stepwise selection”
“correlation matrix.”
method and the “best-subset” method6,7. Notably, our
The 19 variables defined in figure s1 cast a wide net. situation differs from that typically found in multi-
But ultimately we would like to test whether few linear regression, where any single response depends
variables can capture the neuronal activity of OFC, on multiple variables. Each of our responses is
and identify those that best do so. To achieve this goal, sampled in few data points (typically 8-10 trial types),
we proceed in two steps. First, we assume that many fewer than the number of variables we want to
individual OFC responses encode each only one test (19 variables). However, we can capitalize on the
variable and that value functions are linear. We large number of responses available (a total of 1379
perform independent linear regressions of each responses pass the ANOVA screening criterion) and
response on each variable, and we apply methods of identify the most relevant variables through a
variable selection. We thus identify value A offered, population analysis (see below).
value B offered, chosen value, and A|B chosen as the
In addition to the 19 original variables, we define two
variables that best account for the dataset. These
“collapsed” variables value A|B offered and value A|B
variables explain well the large majority of OFC
chosen, as follows. The variable value A|B offered is
responses. Second, having identified for each response
taken to explain a given response if at least one of the
the variable encoded “at the first order,” we relax the
two variables value A offered and value B offered
two initial assumptions. We test with standard multi-
explains the response. The R2 of the collapsed variable
regression methods whether adding a second variable
is equal to the higher R2 obtained from of the two
or a quadratic value term improves the regression, and
original variables (and equal to zero if none of the
we observe that for the large majority of responses this
original variables explains the response). Similarly, the
is not the case. In other words, both the “one response-
variable value A|B chosen is defined by collapsing
one variable” assumption and the assumption of linear
variables value A chosen and value B chosen.
value functions are, by and large, warranted. We
conclude that indeed OFC responses encode the
variables identified by the variable-selection Neuro-econometric analysis: second order encoding
procedures.
To explore the possibility that individual OFC
The next sections and the Supplementary Results responses might encode a mixture of variables, we
detail the methods and results of these analyses. proceed formally as follows8.

S.4
Consider a response encoding at the first order the counts mostly equal to 0 or 1. For this reason, we
variable X with R 2 = R X2 (i.e., a response explained choose a slightly more conservative threshold (p<0.01)
by X better than by any other selected variable, to identify regression slopes as significantly non-zero.
In each time bin, the number of neurons expected to be
with RX2 > 0 ). To establish whether adding a second assigned by chance to each class is Nchance = p* Ntot =
variable Y to the regression provides a significantly 9.31. For each time bin, we then compute:
better account, we compute
Nvalue A|B offered = Nvalue A offered + N value B offered – Nchance
FY | X = (n − 3) ∗ ( R XY
2
− R X2 ) (1 − R XY
2
)

In the equation, R X2 is obtained from the linear regres-


sion on X only; R XY 2
is obtained from the bi-linear Supplementary Results
regression on X and Y; and n is the number of trial Neuronal database, linear regressions, and
types (data points in the regression). We compute correlation matrix
FY | X for each of the variables Y we wish to test as
potentially encoded at the second order, and we take We recorded the activity of 931 neurons (375 from
{ }
the maximum F = max FY | X . The degrees of
monkey V; 556 from monkey L). Figure s2 reports the
number of cells for which each main factor in the
freedom of F are 1 for the numerator and n − 3 for the ANOVA has a significant effect. In particular, the
denominator. We then set a threshold F* corresponding factor [offer type] yields a significant effect in at least
to a desired p* = 0.01 (we choose this threshold be- one time window for 505 (54%) neurons in total (211
cause residuals are not pre-screened and because we = 56% from monkey V; 294 = 53% from monkey L).
test each response with multiple potential second order We refer to these as “task-related” cells. Figure s3
variables). If F passes the criterion, we identify the shows the average neuronal activity separately for
second order variable encoded by the response. If F task-related cells and for “other” (i.e., not task-related)
does not pass the criterion, we conclude that the cells. It can be observed that the average activity
response does not encode any second order variable (at profile of task-related cells peaks early after the offer,
least among those tested). slowly decays during the delay, and has two secondary
peaks before and after juice delivery. In contrast, the
For second order encoding, we test some of the average activity profile of other cells is lower and
variables tested for first order encoding. In addition, essentially flat throughout the trial.
we test value-encoding responses with quadratic value
terms. Hence, with this analysis we scrutinize the A total of 1379 responses are significantly modulated
validity of the two assumptions underlying the variable by [offer value] in the ANOVA (656 from monkey V;
selection procedures, namely that individual OFC 723 from monkey L). We further analyze these respon-
responses encode each only one variable and that ses in relation to the variables defined in figure s1. In
value functions are linear. total, 1227/1379 (89%) responses are explained by at
least one of the 19 variables (567/656 = 86% from
Analysis of time course monkey V; 660/723 = 91% from monkey L). Methods
for variable selection are applied to this dataset.
To analyze the time course by which neurons in OFC
encode variables value A offered, value B offered, Figure s4 illustrates the results of individual linear
chosen value, and A|B chosen, we define 50-ms non- regressions obtained for one particular response (i.e.,
overlapping time bins, aligning trials separately at the the activity of one neuron in one time window as a
time of the offer and at the time of juice delivery. For function of the different trial types). The two top left
each 50-ms time bin, we subject each cell in the panels show the behavioral choice pattern and the
population of Ntot = 931 neurons to independent linear neuronal response plotted with respect to offer type. In
regressions on the four selected variables. We assign a the other panels, the same neuronal response is plotted
neuron to a variable if the regression slope differs against each of the 19 variables. A blue regression line
significantly from zero in that time bin. If more than indicates that the slope is significantly non-zero
one variable has a non-zero slope (a rare case), we (p<0.05); if so, the respective R2 is indicated in the top
assign the neuron to the variable with highest R2. For left corner of the panel. In the top left panels, the
this analysis, we do not screen neuronal data prior to activity of the cell as a function of the offer type has a
regression, because small time bins result in spike U-shaped profile, qualitatively similar to that
S.5
hypothesized for a response encoding the chosen must analyze the entire population of responses and all
value. In fact, the variable chosen value explains the the variables at once.
response (R2 = 0.90). However, the response is also
explained by several other variables, for example total Figure s6 illustrates two complementary ways to
value (R2 = 0.72), chosen number (R2= 0.50), etc. derive a population analysis from the individual linear
regressions. In figure s6a, we compute for each time
The fact that multiple variables can explain the same window and for each variable the number of responses
response is not surprising, since the variables are in explained. For example (top left), in the post-offer
general not independent. This point is illustrated in time window, the variable total value explains 130
figure s5, which shows the correlation matrix. In the responses. Because, as we noted, more than one
figure, different shades of gray represent different variable may explain any given response, any given
values of correlation ( ρ ) with black corresponding to response may appear in multiple bins in this plot.
ρ = 1 and white corresponding to ρ = 0 . A small Some trends emerge clearly. First, as indicated by the
ANOVA, more responses are modulated by the trial
white circle or cross indicates ρ > 0.8 . For example,
type in early time windows (post-offer and late delay)
the variables chosen value and total value are highly and late time windows (pre-juice and post-juice), as
correlated with each other and with other variables compared to the peri-movement time windows (pre-go
(e.g., total number, max number, and chosen and RT). Second, focusing on the post-offer time
number)—a case of multi-collinearity. window we observe that a group of variables explain a
large number of responses. For example, the variables
Ideally, to provide a concise description of the dataset,
total value, chosen value, total number, max number,
we would like to achieve two goals: first, to assign
chosen number, (max–min) number, and value B
each response to only one variable; second, to identify
offered all explain more than 100 responses.
a small subset of variables that can explain as many
Inspection of figure s5 reveals that these variables are
responses as possible. Clearly, achieving the second
highly inter-correlated. So the picture in figure s6a
goal helps to achieve the first. But how can we select
clearly contains a high degree of redundancy, which
the “right” subset of variables? In the following
we may hope to resolve with further analysis. Third, in
sections, we first make a qualitative case for variable
addition to these variables, which are prevalent in the
selection and then we illustrate the results obtained
post-offer, pre-juice and post-juice time windows,
with statistically principled procedures.
there is another group of variables, including (chosen–
other) number, A|B chosen, and value B chosen, which
Variable selection: Qualitative analysis are common in the pre-juice and post-juice time
windows, but not in earlier time windows. Again,
Consider the response analyzed in figure s4. Our initial inspection of figure s5 reveals that these variables are
hypothesis that U-shaped responses might reflect the highly inter-correlated, a redundancy that we may
chosen value is bolstered by the fact that the variable hope to resolve with further analysis.
chosen value provides the best fit (i.e., the highest R2).
However, at this level of analysis, it would be A complementary way to derive a population analysis
unreasonable to rule out other variables (for example from the individual regressions is to consider for each
total value) that provide a slightly lower R2. A response only the variable providing the best fit (i.e.,
potentially powerful approach is to consider the entire the highest R2). By doing so, we essentially force each
population of responses. For example, we might response in only one bin. Figure s6b shows the result
discover that whenever (or most often when) both of this analysis. Note that much of the redundancy of
variables chosen value and total value provide a non- figure s6a seems naturally resolved. For example,
zero slope, the variable chosen value provides a many more responses are best fit by the variable
slightly higher R2. If that were the case, we could chosen value than by any of the variables total value,
conclude that neuronal responses genuinely encode the total number, etc. that are highly correlated with
chosen value, and that the variable total value has no chosen value. Likewise, many more responses are best
additional explanatory power. This argument is fit by the variable A|B chosen than by either (chosen–
obviously complicated by the fact that, as we observed other) number, or value B chosen. Thus, although the
qualitatively (figure 3, main text), not all responses in population picture obviously remains complex, figure
our dataset encode the same variable. In addition, s6b suggests that fewer variables than the 19 initially
variables have more than just pairwise correlation and considered may be sufficient to account for most
cannot be simply considered two at the time. So we responses.

S.6
In some sense, the pictures presented in figures s5a explains at least a certain percentage (for example 5%)
and s5b represent two opposite and extreme ways to of otherwise unexplained responses. At the end of the
analyze the population of responses. On the one hand, procedure, we classify responses based on the R2 (i.e.,
in figure s6a we forgo the possibility of ranking the we assign responses explained by more than one
quality of the linear fits (i.e., the R2) and, by doing so, selected variable to the variable with highest R2). This
we give up the possibility of a concise description. In method is called “stepwise selection.”6,7 (b)
fact, it could be argued that we spuriously added
redundancy by including “unreasonable” variables in We apply the stepwise selection method to our dataset.
the analysis. And, indeed, one could easily come up In figure s7, panels on the left represent the population
with more variables that would further complicate the of unexplained responses at different iterations of the
picture, to little or no advantage. On the other hand, in procedure. Tables of best fit are shown, so that the top
figure s6b we ignore that a given variable may panel is the same as shown in figure s6b. For each
sometimes provide a satisfactory explanation for a iteration, a red asterisk indicates the selected variable;
given response, even if another variable provides a blue dots indicate variables excluded by the 5%
better fit. That is clearly excessive: because neuronal selection criterion. The first four iterations select
data are noisy, responses that genuinely encode a variables A|B chosen, chosen value, value A offered,
given variable (say chosen value) might at times be and value B offered, and no other variable reaches the
best fit by another, highly correlated variable (say total 5% selection criterion in subsequent iterations.
value). In other words, it is likely that some responses
that would genuinely belong to one bin in figure s6b In the analysis shown in figure s7, we keep separate
“spilled over” to another bin because of noise. So here the 19 variables defined in figure s1. However, it could
too it could be argued that we spuriously added be argued that in some cases different variables really
complexity to the picture by including “unreasonable” represent the same class of response. For example, the
variables in the analysis. And again, one could distinction between value A offered and value B
certainly further complicate the picture by adding offered is somewhat arbitrary, because one particular
more variables, to little or no advantage. juice may be labeled “A” in a given session and “B” in
another session. Furthermore, if a selection procedure
In conclusion, an ideal account of the dataset would on identified only one of the two variables, the result
the one hand make use of the information of figure would be difficult to interpret. Thus it is probably
s6b, namely that some variables (say chosen value) do more correct to combine the variables value A offered
consistently better than others (say total value) in and value B offered using the “collapsed” variable
fitting the population of neuronal responses. On the value A|B offered (see Supplementary Methods).
other hand, an ideal account would also use the Likewise, we can use the other collapsed variables
information embedded in figure s6a, namely that one value A|B chosen. This leaves us with 17 variables.
variable might provide a satisfactory (albeit We re-analyze our dataset using collapsed variables.
suboptimal) explanation for many responses. But how The stepwise procedure selects the three variables
can we identify a small subset of variables to explain a value A|B offered, chosen value, and A|B chosen,
large number of responses? This problem closely confirming our previous result.
resembles that of variable selection encountered in
multi-linear regressions in the presence of multi- In summary, the stepwise method applied to our
collinearity. Methods commonly used for multi-linear dataset selects three variables: chosen value, value A|B
regressions6,7 can be adapted to our case. offered, and A|B chosen. These three variables explain

b
The stepwise selection method is used with the following caveat.
Variable selection: Stepwise method The “marginal explanatory power” of one particular selected
variable X is the number of responses that are explained by X and
One simple way to handle our problem is to select that are not explained by any other selected variable. The 5%
variables one at the time. First, we select the variable selection criterion sets a threshold on the marginal explanatory
that provides the highest number of best fits (the power of newly selected variables. However, the marginal
explanatory power of a selected variable X generally drops over
darkest bin in figure s6b). In our case, this variable is iterations, because variables selected after X may explain some of
A|B chosen. We explore the entire dataset and we the responses previously explained only by X. Eventually, it is
remove all the responses that can be explained by this possible that a selected variable X only explains a small percentage
variable. Then we iterate the procedure by selecting a (less than 5%) of otherwise unexplained responses. For a correct
procedure, we check at each iteration that all of the selected
second variable, then a third variable, etc. We continue
variables actually meet the 5% selection criterion, and we exclude
the procedure as long as any newly selected variable previously selected variables that fail to meet the criterion.
S.7
1085 responses, corresponding to 79% of all responses chosen value, value A|B offered, and A|B chosen, are
significantly modulated by offer type (1379), and to indeed the best possible subset of three variables.
88% of responses explained by all 17 variables (1227). Third, these three variables are also included in the
In other words, in limiting ourselves to three variables, best subset of four variables and in the best subset of
we “lost” only 12% of responses. Having thus five variables. This is not necessarily expected,
identified the variables encoded in OFC, we classify because in general the best subset of d+1 variables
responses based on the R2 (i.e., we assign responses might not include all or any of the variables included
explained by multiple selected variables to the variable the best subset of d variables. The fact that the three
with highest R2). Figure s8 (top) summarizes the final variables that make up the best subset for d = 3 are
result of this classification. Note that, to provide a also included in the best subset for d = 4 and for d = 5
more intuitive interpretation of the variables, in the is a sign of robustness of the result.
main text we renamed variable value A|B offered as
offer value and variable A|B chosen as taste. The In conclusion, the variable-selection analysis using the
histograms in figure s8 (bottom) illustrate the best-subset method confirms the results obtained with
distribution of R2 obtained as a result of the final the stepwise selection method. In both cases, the three
classification. In general, selected variables capture the selected variables are chosen value, value A|B offered,
variability of individual responses remarkably well and A|B chosen.(c, d)
(mean R2 = 0.63).
Variable selection: Over-fitting and post hoc analysis
In the following sections, we show that alternative
procedures for variable selection yield essentially the Although it guarantees optimality, the best-subset
same results. procedure does not provide a measure of reliability of
the result. For example, in our case, the method indi-
Variable selection: Best-subset method cates that, among the subsets of three variables, chosen
value, value A|B offered, and A|B chosen explain the
One potential limitation of the stepwise method for highest number of responses, but we do not know how
variable selection is that the results obtained may be well these three variables do compared to the possible
“path-dependent,” and the method is not guaranteed to alternatives. In other words, we might have a problem
select the best possible subset of variables. This goal of over-fitting. In multi-linear regressions, the standard
can be achieved using the “best-subset” method6,7. way to handle this problem is to repeat measures and
to analyze multiple datasets6,7 (see below). But in
The idea of the best-subset method is to compute for addition to repeating measures, our case also lends
each possible subset of d variables the corresponding itself to an informative post hoc analysis.
number of responses explained; to identify the best
subset of d variables as the subset that explains the In the post hoc analysis, we want to test selected
maximum number of responses; and to repeat the variables against highly correlated but discarded
procedure for d = 1, 2, 3, … If n(d) is the number of variables. For example, the best-subset method selects
responses explained as a function of d, the number d* the variable chosen value. We want to establish
of variables necessary to characterize the population whether selecting this variable is significantly better
can be determined either by an “elbow” in the function than selecting the variable total value, which is highly
n(d), or with a threshold criterion (e.g., imposing that
at least 85% of responses be explained). c
The stepwise selection method and the best-subset method differ
in how different time windows are analyzed. Using the stepwise
Figure s9 shows the results of analyzing our dataset method, responses from different time windows are analyzed
with the best-subset method. In the top panel, the x- separately but in parallel. Variables are selected for the number of
axis represents the number d of variables included in best fits by time window, but variables can then explain responses
in any time window. In contrast, using the best-subset method,
each subset, and the y-axis represents the percentage responses from different time windows are pooled together.
n(d) of responses explained by the best subset. The Keeping time windows separate may or may not be viewed as a
bottom table indicates the variables included in the desirable feature. In any case, it is reassuring that the two methods
best subset for various d. Several points should be provide converging results.
noted. First, although n(d) does not present a clear d
One important advantage of the best-subset method is that results
“elbow,” selecting only three variables seems do not depend on “irrelevant” variables. That is to say, including in
reasonable, as additional variables add limited the analysis variables that turn out to have little explanatory power
explanatory power. Second, the three variables has no effect on the outcome. This ensures that we don’t “add
identified by the stepwise selection method, namely noise” to the procedure by testing “unreasonable” variables.
S.8
correlated with chosen value and is therefore a weighting the contribution of each response to the
presumably “challenging” alternative. That chosen explanatory power of any variable with the
value, and not total value, is part of the best subset corresponding R2. The variable selection analyses
ultimately means that chosen value has a higher using this alternative metrics yields results identical to
marginal explanatory power: disregarding responses the ones obtained with the binary “explained/not-
explained by either value A|B offered or A|B chosen, explained” metrics. Finally, correcting regressions for
the number of responses explained by chosen value unequal variance also provides identical results.(e)
and not by total value is greater than the number of Using collapsed variables, all these procedures
responses explained by total value and not by chosen consistently indicate that the best subset includes value
value. To establish whether this inequality is true in a A|B offered and chosen value, and either A|B chosen or
statistical sense, we can formally proceed as follows. value A|B chosen, with these two latter variables
having statistically indistinguishable marginal
Consider the sub-population of responses explained by explanatory power. Using non-collapsed variables, all
chosen value (variable X) and/or by total value the procedures provide statistically consistent results.
(variable Y), and not explained by any other variable However, using non-collapsed variables, we find that
in the best subset (i.e., value A|B offered and A|B the marginal explanatory power of A|B chosen is
chosen). Of this sub-population, a number nX of significantly higher than that of either value A chosen
responses are explained by X and not by Y; a number or value B chosen.
nY of responses are explained by Y and not by X; and
the other responses are explained by both X and Y. Although the issue between A|B chosen and value A|B
The best-subset method indicates that nX>nY, but it is chosen remains largely unresolved, two arguments
possible that nX is actually quite close to nY. The seem to favor the former variable. First, in the analysis
problem of determining whether the inequality nX>nY of non-collapsed variables, A|B chosen does signi-
is statistically significant is equivalent to asking how ficantly better than either value A chosen or value B
unlikely it is to draw nX heads out of nX+nY tossings chosen taken alone Second (and partly related), A|B
of a fair coin, and can be addressed with a simple chosen, with only one intrinsic degree of freedom (i.e.,
binomial test. For this particular case, we have nX = the relative value of the two juices), is more
58 and nY = 21, from which we infer that the marginal parsimonious than value A|B chosen, which has two
explanatory power of chosen value is significantly intrinsic degrees of freedom (i.e., the relative value,
higher than that of total value (p<10-5). and which one of the two juices is coded for). In
summary, although these arguments are largely
We repeat this procedure for all the pairs of variables heuristic, it seems preferable to summarize our data in
(X,Y) for which X is a variable in the best subset and terms of the variable A|B chosen.(f)
Y is a variable highly correlated to X (i.e.,
ρ ( X ,Y ) > 0.8 ; see figure s5). Additionally, we test e
The least-squares method, used here for all regressions, is based
chosen value against chosen number. The results are on two assumptions: gaussianity (i.e., data must come from
presented in figure s10. In these pairwise comparisons, gaussian distributions) and homoscedasticity (i.e., the gaussians
all the variables included in the best subset do must all have the same variance). Cortical single-trial spike counts
significantly better than the challenging alternatives are known to violate both these assumptions [ref 9]. Responses
analyzed here (i.e., averages across trials) approximately satisfy
(maximal p<10-5). The only exception is that the gaussianity (for the central limit theorem). However, responses
variable A|B chosen fails this test against the variable generally do not satisfy homoscedasticity. In cases of unequal
value A|B chosen (p = 0.12). As discussed below, this variance (heteroscedasticity), the least squares method can be
degree of ambiguity remains largely unresolved in our corrected by weighting the residual associated to each data point
analysis. For all other aspects, the post hoc analysis mi with a term proportional to 1/σi, where σi is the standard
deviation of the distribution from which mi is drawn [ref 8]. In our
confirms our previous conclusions. case, mi is the response measured for a particular trial type and we
can estimate σi with the standard error. Notably, 1/σi is
proportional to the square root of the number of trials. This
Variable selection: Other procedures and conclusions suggests that, in our case, it might be preferable not to correct for
unequal variance, because trial types close to the indifference
We employed a number of alternative procedures to point, which are in many respects the most informative, are also
select variables. Essentially, they all confirm the those for which we have fewer trials (because monkeys “divide”
results described in previous sections. For example, in their choices between the two juice types). For this reason, we
the spirit of repeating measures, we find that data from generally use uncorrected linear regressions in all our analyses.
f
the two monkeys analyzed separately yield statistically Consistent with the results of a recent study of gustatory
indistinguishable results. Another variant consists in responses in this brain area (Pritchard et al., 2005), in our final
classification, we observe A|B chosen (i.e., taste) responses in
S.9
In conclusion, having tested a large number of hypo- variables, we find that in 757/817 (93%) cases qua-
theses with a variety of variable selection procedures, dratic terms fail to provide a statistically appreciable
we identified value A|B offered, chosen value, and A|B gain. Again, we interpret this result as a justification
chosen as the three variables encoded by OFC respon- for the assumption of linear value functions.(g)
ses. These three variables describe our dataset well,
and significantly better than challenging alternatives. In summary, the analysis of second order encoding
provides post facto support for the two main
assumptions underlying the variable selection analysis,
Second order encoding
namely the one response-one variable assumption and
The variable selection analysis presented in previous the assumption of linear value functions. This result
sections rests on two key assumptions: the assumption concludes our “neuro-econometric” analysis.
that OFC responses encode each only one variable and
the assumption that value functions are linear. We now Relationship between slope ratio and relative value
put these two working hypotheses under scrutiny.
During the experiments, we used a large number of
For second order encoding, we test variables selected juice pairs. In each session we measured the relative
at the first order (that is, we test whether responses value n* from the behavioral pattern of choice,
encode mixtures of selected variables). We also test through a sigmoid fit. Naturally, we measured
the two variables value A chosen and value B chosen different relative values for different juice pairs. For
that were excluded last in the variable selection example, monkeys generally had a mild preference for
analysis, as well as the variable chosen number. grape juice over fruit punch (typically, n*<2), but a
Finally, for responses encoding at the first order value strong preference for grape juice over peppermint tea
A offered, value B offered, and chosen value, we test (typically, n*≥3). In addition, the relative value of any
the corresponding quadratic term (value A offered)2, given juice pair could vary from session to session and
(value B offered)2, and (chosen value)2. We extend this from day to day. For example, the relative value of ½
analysis to the population of 1085 responses classified apple juice over peppermint tea varied over many days
at the first order (figure s8) and we pool together in the range n*∈(1.5,3). If U-shaped responses indeed
responses from different time windows. The results are encode the chosen value, we should expect them to
presented in figure s11. reflect this variability. The following analysis confirms
this prediction.
In the two tables, rows indicate first order variables
and columns indicate second order variables. The To avoid any bias, we want to identify U-shaped
rightmost column indicates responses that do not responses independently of the specific variable that
encode any second-order variable. The top table they might encode. We proceed using a bi-linear
(figure s11a) reports the number of responses. The regression:
bottom table (figure s11b) reports the same data as
percentages (normalized by the number of responses fr = a 0 + a A ( # A) + a B ( # B) (1)
explained by each first-order variable). In general, we where fr is the firing rate of the neuron, and (#A) and
observe that the vast majority of responses do not (#B) are, respectively, the number of drops of juice A
encode second order variables, independently of the and juice B chosen by the monkey. (Note that in any
variable encoded at the first order. Over the entire given trial either (#A) = 0 or (#B) = 0.) We then define
population, 837/1085 (77%) responses do not encode a response to be “U-shaped” if the regression slopes aA
second order variables. If we exclude from this and aB are both significantly different from zero
analysis the quadratic value terms, we find that (p<0.01). (For this analysis, we do not pre-screen
890/1085 (82%) responses do not encode second order responses with the ANOVA.)
variables. We interpret this result as a substantial
justification for the one response-one variable
assumption. Similarly, for most value-encoding
responses, adding a quadratic term does not improve g
Of course, the presence and nature of quadratic value terms is a
the regression significantly. Repeating the analysis consequential issue in economic theory, where convexity of the
with only quadratic terms as potential second order value function plays a fundamental role. The present results
indicate that the assumption of linearity is adequate for our dataset
and at the relatively coarse level of our analysis. However, the
present results do not exclude the possibility that a consistent
168/931 (18%) of recorded neurons, most prevalently at the time
departure from linearity may emerge upon more refined
of juice delivery (i.e., in pre-juice and post-juice time windows).
examination, or in experiments that span a wider range of values.
S.10
As illustrated in figure s12 for one particular response, ± s.e.m.). With respect to the intercept, averaging
the regression slopes aA and aB are in general different across juice pairs we obtain <b0> = -0.13 ± 0.15. These
from each other. Since both aA ≠ 0 and aB ≠ 0, Eq.1 results are consistent with the predicted identity k* =
can be re-written as follows: n*.
fr = a 0 + m (k * ( # A) + ( # B) ) (2) In summary, the neuronal “U” shapes recorded in OFC
closely match the behavioral choice pattern on a juice-
with m = aB and k* = aA/aB. The hypothesis that U- by-juice and session-by-session basis, a phenomenon
shaped responses encode the chosen value leads to a
naturally captured by the concept of economic value.
simple prediction regarding the slope ratio k*, which
should be (statistically) equal to the relative value of
the two juices. Critically, k* should co-vary with n*. Ingredient-based hypothesis

At least for the response in figure s12, the relationship One concern is whether U-shaped responses, which
k* ≈ n* holds true. Indeed, from the bi-linear vary with the quantity of both juices A and B chosen
regression we obtain k* = 3.0 (±1.4) (±s.d.), while by the monkey, may simply encode the quantity of one
from the behavioral choice pattern we obtain n* = 3.2. particular ingredient, or combination of ingredients,
Applying the bi-linear regression criterion to all 931 present in both juices. By “ingredient,” we mean any
neurons, we identify 254 U-shaped responses. Figure compound contained in the juice, for example a
s13 illustrates the relationship between k* and n* compound that would elicit a taste response (e.g.,
observed for this population. The scatter plot in figure water, sugar, citric acid, etc.)10-13. The strongest
s13a includes data from all sessions, pooling together argument against the ingredient-based hypothesis
all pairs of juices. In the plot, the x-axis represents the follows from the relationship k* ≈ n* found in the
behaviorally measured relative value n*, the y-axis previous section. To appreciate this point, let us refer
represents the neuronal slope ratio k*, and both axes to the cartoon shown in figure s14.
are plotted in log scale. Each dot represents one
response. Defining “A” as the preferred juice is To summarize the results of the previous section, we
equivalent to imposing n* > 1. In principle, the slope showed that for any two juices A and B, in the scatter
ratio k* could assume any possible value. However, plot of k* versus n*, data lie on a diagonal line (figure
nearly always k* > 1. Moreover, we observe a signifi- s14, gray line) and cannot be described by a horizontal
line. We can now examine different variants of the
cant correlation between k* and n* (p<10-12).
ingredient-based hypothesis.
Most importantly, the relationship k* ≈ n* can be
The first variant is the hypothesis that U-shaped
observed when the analysis is restricted to responses
responses all encode the quantity of one particular
recorded with individual pairs of juices, as shown in
ingredient. If this is true, given the ingredient and two
figure s13b for one particular pair of juices (A = ½
juices A and B, the two regression slopes aA and aB
apple, B = peppermint tea). During the experiments,
We used a total of 25 different juice pairs. The number should be proportional to the concentrations ρA and ρB
of U-shaped responses recorded with any given juice at which the ingredient is present in the juices.
Therefore, the slope ratio k* should be equal to the
pair varied between 1 and 40. Restricting our analysis
to the 7 pairs of juices for which we have >10 respon- concentration ratio ρA/ρB, independently of the relative
ses, we test whether the relationship k*≈ n* holds true value n* recorded in the session. Consider for example
the ingredient water. Because for any amount of juice
using the regression function:
the quantity of water is equal to the juice volume, if U-
k * = b 0 + b1 n * (3) shaped responses all encode the quantity of water,
neuronal data should lie on the horizontal line k* ≈ 1
For the pair of juices shown in figure s13b, the (figure s14, blue line), contrary to what we observe. In
regression indicates b0 = 0.08 and b1 = 1.18, with 95% other words, the sole fact that neuronal data
confidence intervals b0∈(-0.21, 0.37) and b1∈(0.77, overwhelmingly lie in the quadrant {k*>1, n*>1} rules
1.59). Congruent results are obtained for all 7 pairs of out the possibility that U-shaped responses all encode
juices. Figure s13c illustrates in particular for different the quantity of water consumed by the monkey. (This
juice pairs (y-axis), the value of the regression slope b1 is an alternative way to rule out the variable chosen
(x-axis), together with the 95% confidence intervals. number.) Consider now another ingredient, for
Notably, the values of b1 are distributed around the red example sugar, which we may assume to be present in
dashed lined corresponding to b1 = 1. Averaging across given juices A and B at a certain concentration ratio
the 7 juice pairs, we obtain <b1> = 1.05 ± 0.15 (mean
S.11
ρA/ρB. If U-shaped responses all encode the quantity of described by Rolls. To appreciate this point, consider
sugar chosen by the monkey, we should observe again the response shown in figure s12. According to
neuronal data lying on the horizontal line k* ≈ ρA/ρB, the ingredient-based hypothesis, the response is U-
independently of the relative value n* recorded in the shaped because the activity of this neuron encodes the
session. However, our observations contradict this quantity of some ingredient X present in both juices A
prediction. More generally, no matter what ingredient and B. (Here we refer to the first variant of the
one may want to test, the same argument can always hypothesis, but the argument is valid more generally.)
be made. If OFC responses all encode that particular In the context of the ingredient-based hypothesis, the
ingredient, given two juices A and B, neuronal data phenomenon described by Rolls can be described in
should always lie on a horizontal line corresponding to terms of reduced responsiveness, or neuronal de-
the concentration ratio, contrary to our observations. In sensitization. For example, under de-sensitized
conclusion, the simple hypothesis that U-shaped conditions, the response of the neuron in figure s12 to
responses all encode one single ingredient can be any given quantity of ingredient X may be reduced by
rejected. half. Imagine now to record from this same neuron
under conditions of reduced responsiveness. Since the
A second variant of the ingredient-based hypothesis is activity of the neuron encodes the quantity of X
that U-shaped responses all encode a particular linear consumed by the monkey, and since X is present in
combination of multiple ingredients, such as water, both juices, reduced responsiveness will affect both
sugar, etc. However, any linear combination of trials in which the monkey chooses juice A and trials
horizontal lines in figure s14 is itself a horizontal line, in which the monkey chooses juice B. For example, if
such as the brown dashed line in the figure. Therefore, the neuronal responsiveness is halved, we will observe
if U-shaped responses all encode a linear combination half-sized responses when the monkey chooses A, and
of ingredients, neuronal data should lie on some half-sized responses when the monkey chooses B. As a
horizontal line, contrary to our observations. Hence, consequence, in figure s12, both slopes aA and aB will
this variant of the ingredient-based hypothesis can be be halved. But if both slopes are halved, their ratio
also rejected. does not change. More generally, if both slopes are
scaled by the same factor, their ratio does not change.
A third variant is the hypothesis that different U- As quantified in Eq.2, this means that any change in
shaped responses encode different ingredients. For responsiveness to the encoded ingredient affects the
example, the linear relationship between k* and n* scaling parameter m, which multiplies both (#A) and
could be perhaps explained if U-shaped responses with (#B), but does not affect the ratio parameter k*. In
large k* encode, say, sugar (red line), while U-shaped summary, even assuming changes in responsiveness of
responses with small k* encode another ingredient, say the kind described by Rolls, the ingredient-based
ingredient X (green line). However, this hypothesis hypothesis predicts that k* should be constant and
can only be true assuming that we happened to record independent of n*, contrary to our observations. Thus
from sugar-encoding neurons on days in which n* was the ingredient-based hypothesis cannot be “salvaged”
large, and that we happened to record from X- by appealing to changes in neuronal responsiveness a
encoding neurons on days in which n* was small—a la Rolls.
seemingly impossible coincidence. Thus this variant
can also be rejected. To conclude, our analysis demonstrates that U-shaped
responses do not encode the quantity of any particular
The fourth and last variant of the ingredient-based ingredient or combination of ingredients, but rather the
hypothesis is that different U-shaped responses encode value the monkey assigns to the juice it chooses to
different linear combinations of ingredients. This consume.
variant combines the second and third variants, and
can be rejected for the same reasons.
Conclusions
Studies of gustatory responses in various areas of the
orbitofrontal cortex, particularly those of Rolls and We showed that OFC responses do not depend on the
colleagues, found neurons encoding the taste of one visuomotor contingencies of the task. Assuming that
particular juice, whose responses decreased following OFC responses encode each only one variable and that
selective satiation of that juice12,14. We emphasize that value functions are linear, we showed that OFC
the relationship between k* and n* observed in our responses are best described as encoding value A|B
data cannot be accounted for by the ingredient-based offered, chosen value, and A|B chosen. In the main
hypothesis taken in combination with the phenomenon text, we refer to these variables respectively as offer
S.12
value, chosen value, and taste. The explanatory power cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol
of these three variables is high (mean R2 = 0.63) and 346, 366-402 (1994).
significantly higher than that of challenging
alternatives. Conversely, we showed that in the large 5. Carmichael, S. T. & Price, J. L. Connectional
majority of cases, it is sufficient to assume that OFC networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal
responses encode each only one variable, if that cortex of macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 371,
variable is one of the three characteristic of this area. 179-207 (1996).
Indeed, taking into consideration the possibility that
OFC responses encode a mixture of variables 6. Dunn, O. J. & Clark, V. Applied statistics: analysis
generally does not provide a significantly better of variance and regression (Wiley, New York,
account. We also showed that for the large majority of 1987).
OFC responses recorded in our experiment it is
adequate to assume a linear value function. In a 7. Glantz, S. A. & Slinker, B. K. Primer of applied
separate analysis of U-shaped neuronal responses we regression & analysis of variance (McGraw-Hill,
showed a statistical identity between the slope ratio Medical Pub. Division, New York, 2001).
and the behaviorally measured relative value. For any
given pair of juices, the two quantities co-vary on a 8. Neter, J., Wasserman, W. & Kutner, M. H. Applied
session-by-session basis, which rules out any linear statistical models: regression, analysis of
ingredient-based hypothesis. Finally, we showed that variance, and experimental designs (Irwin,
the timing by which OFC neurons encode offer value, Homewood, IL, 1990).
chosen value and taste corresponds well to the mental
operations that monkeys presumably undertake during 9. Rieke, F., Warland, D., de Ruyter van Steveninck,
economic choice. Specifically, neurons encoding the R. & Bialek, W. Spikes: exploring the neural code
offer value—an operation necessary to make a (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1997).
choice—are the most prevalent shortly after the offer
is presented to the monkey. Neurons encoding the 10.Scott, T. R. & Plata-Salaman, C. R. Taste in the
chosen value are frequently observed during the delay, monkey cortex. Physiol Behav 67, 489-511 (1999).
when the monkey has presumably internally made a
choice, but before the choice is actually revealed. 11.Brand, J. G. in Tasting and smelling (eds.
Finally, neurons encoding the taste of the chosen juice Beauchamp, G. K. & Bartoshuk, L.) 1-24
are most prevalent immediately before and after juice (Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 1997).
delivery. Taken together, these results suggest that
neurons in OFC provide a substrate for value 12.Pritchard, T. C. et al. Gustatory neural responses in
assignment during economic choice. the medial orbitofrontal cortex of the old world
monkey. J Neurosci 25, 6047-56 (2005).

13.Zigmond, M. J., Bloom, F. E., Landis, S. C.,


Roberts, J. L. & Squire, L. R. Fundamental
Supplementary References neuroscience (Academic Press, San Diego, CA,
1999).
1. Judge, S. J., Richmond, B. J. & Chu, F. C.
Implantation of magnetic search coils for 14.Rolls, E. T., Sienkiewicz, Z. J. & Yaxley, S.
measurement of eye position: an improved method. Hunger modulates the responses to gustatory
Vision Res 20, 535-8 (1980). stimuli of single neurons in the caudolateral
orbitofrontal cortex of the macaque monkey. Eur J
2. Padoa-Schioppa, C., Jandolo, L. & Visalberghi, E. Neurosci 1, 53-60 (1989).
Multi-stage mental process for economic choice in
capuchins. Cognition 99, B1-B13 (2006).

3. Chiavaras, M. M. & Petrides, M. Orbitofrontal


sulci of the human and macaque monkey brain. J
Comp Neurol 422, 35-54 (2000).

4. Carmichael, S. T. & Price, J. L. Architectonic


subdivision of the orbital and medial prefrontal
S.13
Figure s1

Name Used
in Main Text Collapsed Variable Name Definition

total value chosen value + other value


chosen value ................................................................... chosen value Value of the chosen juice
other value Value of the non-chosen juice
(chosen–other) value chosen value – other value
(other/chosen) value (other value) / (chosen value)
total number max number + min number
max number Maximal offered number
chosen number Chosen number
min number Minimal offered number
other number Non-chosen number
(max–min) number max number – min number
(chosen–other) number chosen number – other number
(min/max) number (min number) / (max number)
(other/chosen) number (other number) / (chosen number)
............ value A offered Value of juice A offered
offer value .............. value A|B offered .... { ............ value B offered Value of juice B offered
taste .................................................................................... A|B chosen Binary: 1 if A chosen, 0 if B chosen
............ value A chosen Value of juice A chosen
value A|B chosen .... { ............ value B chosen Value of juice B chosen

Figure s1. Tested variables. We tested variables related to value, variables related to number, and variables related to
one of the two juices. Note that having assumed a linear value function (see Supplementary Methods), the value of one
of the two juices offered or chosen is proportional to the number of drops of juice. The two collapsed variables are
indicated in the bottom left of the table. In the main text, we re-labeled the variable value A|B offered = offer value, and
the variable A|B chosen = taste.
Figure s2

position move offer


juice A direction type
pre-offer 0 0 1
post-offer 18 14 284
late delay 4 6 207
pre-go 4 8 133
RT 5 5 93
pre-juice 3 14 329
post-juice 5 11 332
total 39 58 1379
at least 1 33 46 505

Figure s2. 3-way ANOVA. We recorded the activity of 931 cells. We analyze each cell in each time window with a 3-
way ANOVA (factors [position of juice A] x [movement direction] x [offer type]). The three columns in the table indicate
the number of cells for which each of the main factors has a significant effect (p<0.001). The bottom row indicates the
number of cells whose activity pass the ANOVA test in at least one time window. The factor [offer type] is significant for
a total of 1379 responses.
Figure s3

offer juice

10
average firing rate (sp/s)

505 task-related
426 other cells
0
-500 0 500 1000 -1000 -500 0 500 1000 1500
time (ms) time (ms)

Figure s3. Average activity profile. Neuronal activity was analyzed in 10ms-large, non-overlapping time bins. For each
cell, we included in this analysis all trials and all trial types. We then averaged the resulting activity profile across cells.
This analysis was done separately for task-related cells (i.e., cells that passed the ANOVA criterion in at least one time
window; blue color) and for other cells (i.e., cells that did not pass the ANOVA criterion in any time window; red color).
100% total value chosen value other value (chosen–other) value
1A = 3.2B 40 40 40 40
75% R 2 = 0.72 R 2 = 0.90 R2 = 0.56
30 30 30 30
50%
20 20 20 20
25%

0% 10 10 10 10
0:1 1:3 1:2 1:1 2:1 3:1 4:1 6:1 3:0
10:1
0 0 0 0
0 5 10 15 0 5 10 0 2 4 6 -5 0 5 10
40
(other/chosen) value total number max number chosen number
40 40 40 40
30 R2 = 0.41 R2 = 0.41 R2 = 0.50
30 30 30 30
20
20 20 20 20

10 10 10 10 10

0 0 0 0 0
0:1 1:3 1:2 1:1 2:1 3:1 4:1 6:110:13:0 0 1 2 0 5 10 0 5 10 0 5 10

offers (#B:#A) min number other number (max–min) number (chosen–other) number
40 40 40 40
R 2 = 0.40 R2 = 0.33
30 30 30 30

20 20 20 20

10 10 10 10

0 0 0 0
0 0.5 1 0 2 4 6 0 5 10 -5 0 5 10

(min/max) number (other/chosen) number value A offered value B offered


40 40 40 40
R2 = 0.32
30 30 30 30

20 20 20 20

10 10 10 10

0 0 0 0
0 0.5 1 0 2 4 6 0 5 10 0 5 10

A|B chosen value A chosen value B chosen


40 40 40

30 30 30

20 20 20

10 10 10

0 0 0
0 1 0 5 10 0 5 10

Figure s4. Linear regressions. The two top left panels show the behavioral choice pattern and one response plotted in
the same coordinates. The response is the same shown in figure 3a (main text), except that here we grouped trials by
trial type (squares for “A” choices; circles for “B” choices). In the other panels, the neuronal response (y-axes) is plotted
against each of the 19 variables (x-axes), and each dot represents one trial type. Values are expressed in units of V(B).
Blue regression lines indicate regression slopes significantly different from zero. For variables that explain the
response, the R2 is indicated on the top left corner in the panel. The highest R2 (chosen value) is marked in red.
Figure s5

e be r er
valu valu
e
b er num ber numb
r ) ) e r m r ) m ) ed ed en n
e he en er er umb ber mber in) nu othe x) nu osen offer offer osen chos chose
a l ue n valu value n–ot /chos umb umb n u m nu m n– a /c h A B Bc h A B
e n e m
al v hose ther chos other otal n ax n hose in n ther max– chos min/ other ue alue ue alue
tot c o ( ( t m c m o ( ( ( ( va l v A | va l v

total value

chosen value

other value

(chosen–other) value

(other/chosen) value

total number

max number

chosen number

min number

other number

(max–min) number

(chosen–other) number

(min/max) number

other/chosen number

value A offered

value B offered

A | B chosen

value A chosen

value B chosen

Scale:
1
0

Figure s5. Correlation matrix. Elements of the correlation matrix vary between 0 and 1 (see Supplementary Methods).
Here the correlation matrix ρ is rendered in gray scale so that ρ=1 is represented in black (diagonal elements) and ρ=0
is represented in white. Small circles and crosses indicate matrix elements for which ρ>0.8 (excluding the diagonal).
We use circles for correlations that include one of the selected variables and crosses otherwise.
Figure s6

a. explained e be r er
valu valu
e
b er num ber numb
d
e )
he r e n
)
er r be r r er
m
nu he r ) num e n) ere ere
d
n ose
n sen
a l ue valu value n–ot /chos umb umbe num umbe numb min) n–ot ax) /chos A off B off chose A ch B cho
n e n e m
al v hose ther chos other otal n ax n hose in n ther max– chos min/ other alue alue | B alue alue
tot c o ( ( t m c m o ( ( ( ( v v A v v
post-offer 130 126 47 79 20 124 130 111 13 21 116 73 4 17 78 112 50 70 94

late delay 68 65 38 47 24 76 73 65 19 26 55 40 4 17 45 63 43 46 50

pre-go 57 52 26 24 9 54 55 48 10 17 45 25 3 7 30 47 24 27 39

RT 38 41 12 24 10 41 43 39 8 8 32 23 2 7 24 34 18 20 25
Scale
pre-juice 110 121 27 76 14 124 133 161 11 30 119 141 5 50 69 108 127 87 148
161 cells
post-juice 130 139 23 81 10 119 126 160 12 23 113 147 1 44 77 100 122 92 147
0 cells

b. best fit v
e
alu valu
e be r
ber ) num ber num d
be r
e er ) n ) r be r r u m er u m n ) re red sen sen
l ue valu alue –oth hose mbe mber num mber umbe in) n –oth ax) n hose offe offe hosen cho cho
a n v en /c u u n u n –m e n m /c A B c A B
al v hose ther chos other otal n ax n hose in n ther max chos min/ other alue alue | B alue alue
tot c o ( ( t m c m o ( ( ( ( v v A v v
post-offer 20 47 10 17 2 6 21 16 4 5 14 3 1 3 20 19 16 18 10

late delay 21 17 7 14 6 3 9 9 3 5 7 7 2 1 13 16 10 19 10

pre-go 15 15 4 4 2 4 6 7 1 5 6 1 1 0 9 11 8 10 5

RT 7 12 3 8 2 3 6 5 1 2 4 3 0 2 6 6 5 6 2
Scale
pre-juice 18 33 3 7 2 2 9 39 1 4 16 20 0 9 23 10 51 26 23
52 cells
post-juice 19 43 2 10 2 6 12 32 0 2 13 23 1 6 24 8 53 23 26
0 cells

Figure s6. Qualitative analysis. Top. Numbers in the top panel represent for each variable (x-axis) and for each time
window (y-axis) the number of responses for which the linear regression provided a non-zero slope (i.e., the number of
responses explained by the variable). The color table underneath reports the same numbers in gray scale. Bottom.
Numbers in the bottom panel represent for each variable and for each time window the number of responses for which
the corresponding variable provided the best fit (highest R2). Again, the color table represents the same numbers in
gray scale.
Figure s7

ue ue ber ber
val ) val r mber ) nummber num d d n
e r)
e en er r e
b r er nu e nu en er ere n ose sen
r ) e
alue valuvalue n–oth/chos umb umbe numumbenumb min) n–oth ax) chos A off B off choseA ch B cho
n n m /
al v se er ose er al n x n se n n er x– ose n/ er ue ue B ue ue
tot cho oth (ch (oth tot ma cho mi oth (ma (ch (mi (oth val val A | val val

% responses explained
100

90

80

70

60

50

40

30

20

10

0
0 2 4
number of variables

Figure s7. Stepwise selection method. See Supplementary Results.


Figure s8

position move offer offer chosen taste other unclas-


juice A direction type value value class sified
0 0 1 pre-offer 0 0 0 1 0
18 14 284 post-offer 113 87 25 27 32
4 6 207 late delay 72 46 26 33 30
4 8 133 pre-go 46 37 16 15 19
5 5 93 RT 29 30 11 13 10
3 14 329 pre-juice 95 79 93 29 33
5 11 332 post-juice 90 93 97 24 28
39 58 1379 total 445 372 268 142 152
33 46 505 at least 1

Scale

113 cells

value A offered 0 cells


50

40
# responses

30

20 chosen value A|B c hosen


50 50
10
40 40
0
0 0 .5 1 30 30

20 20
value B offered
50
10 10
40
# responses

0 0
0 0 .5 1 0 0 .5 1
30
2 2
20 R R
10

0
0 0 .5 1
2
R

Figure s8. Population summary. Top. The figure illustrates the final result of the classification. The three columns on
the left repeat teh results of the s-way ANOVA (figure s2). The factor [offer type] is significant for a total of 1379
responses. The five columns on the right summarize the results of the variable selection analysis. Responses that
cannot be explained by any of the 19 variables appear in the rightmost column (unclassified). Variables value A|B
offered, chosen value and A|B chosen collectively explain 1085 responses (79% of the total, 88% of responses
explained by the 19 variables). Responses explained by one of the 19 variables but not explained by any of the
selected variables appear on the second column on the right (other class). The remaining three columns indicate the
number of responses classified as value A|B offered, chosen value, and A|B chosen, respectively. The color table
represents the same number in gray scale. The prevalence of different response classes varies over time windows:
value A|B offered is most prevalent shortly after the offer, chosen value is prevalent throughout the trial, and A|B
chosen is most prevalent late in the trial, before and after juice delivery. This time course can be also observed at much
higher resolution (figure 4, main text). Bottom. How well are responses accounted for in the final classification? The
histograms show for the four response classes the number of responses (y-axis) with the corresponding R2 (x-axis).
The responses included in the four histograms are 159, 286, 372 and 268, respectively. The mean (avg) and median
(med) of the four distributions are (avg=0.61, med=0.60), (avg=0.63, med=0.61), (avg=0.64, med=0.64) and (avg=0.64,
med=0.63), respectively.
Figure s9

% responses 100
explained
80

60

40

20

0
0 1 2 3 4 5 6

number of variables

d=1 d=2 d=3 d=4 d=5

value A|B offered total value chosen value chosen value chosen value
selected variables

value A|B chosen value A|B offered value A|B offered value A|B offered

A|B chosen A|B chosen A|B chosen


chosen number other value
(chosen–other)
number

Figure s9. Best-subset method. See Supplementary Results.


Figure s10

variable X variable Y nX nY p
chosen value total value 58 21 <10-5
chosen value (chosen–other) value 91 34 <10-7
chosen value chosen number 53 20 <10-5
value A|B offered total number 99 14 <10-10
value A|B offered max number 93 19 <10-10
value A|B offered (max–min) number 102 15 <10-10
value A|B offered value A|B chosen 100 19 <10-10
A|B chosen value A|B chosen 26 19 0.12

Figure s10. Post hoc analysis. The best subset of three variables includes chosen value, value A|B offered, and A|B
chosen. We tested these three variables separately against all other variables highly correlated with them (ρ>0.8). The
relevant pairs of variables are marked by small white circles in the correlation matrix in figure s5. The collapsed variable
value A|B offered is tested here against variables highly correlated either with value A offered or with value B offered. In
addition to the comparisons dictated by the ρ>0.8 criterion, we tested the variable chosen value against the alternative
variable chosen number. In the table, the two left columns indicate the tested variable and the alternative variable, the
next two columns indicate the marginal explanatory power of the two variables, and the right column indicates the result
of a binomial test. In essence, all tests indicate that selected variables have significantly higher explanatory power than
the challenging alternatives, except that the variable A|B chosen does not reach significance level against the
alternative value A|B chosen (bottom row).
Figure s11

a. # responses 2 2
n n r 2
ere
d d
ere alue n ose ose mbe fered fered lue
of f of f v e c h c h nu of o f va
os
lue A lue B osen B ch lue A lue B osen lue A lue B osen ne
va va ch A | va va ch va va ch no
value A offered - 2 1 2 7 1 0 19 - - 127

value B offered 7 - 12 5 4 12 24 - 19 - 203

chosen value 12 15 - 13 4 3 4 - - 16 305

A|B chosen 12 2 22 - 10 8 12 - - - 202

b. % responses 2 2
d d en en er d d 2
f fere f fere alue e n h os h os u mb f fere f fere alue
A o e B o en v chos e A c e B c en
n A o e B o en v
lue lu os |B lu lu os lue lu os ne
v a v a c h A v a v a c h v a va ch no
value A offered - 1 1 1 4 1 0 12 - - 81

value B offered 2 - 4 2 1 4 8 - 7 - 72

chosen value 3 4 - 4 1 1 1 - - 4 83

A|B chosen 5 1 8 - 4 3 5 - - - 77

Figure s11. Second order encoding. See Supplementary Results.


Figure s12

A = grape, B = 1/3 cranberry


100%
n* = 3.2
75%

50%
40
25%

0% 35
0:1 1:3 1:2 1:1 2:1 3:1 4:1 6:1 3:0
10:1

30
40 aA aB
25
30

20
20

10 15 k* = aA/ aB = 3.0 (± 1.4)

0 10
0:1 1:3 1:2 1:1 2:1 3:1 4:1 6:110:13:0
3 2 1 2 3 4 6 10
offers (#B:#A) #A chosen #B chosen →

Figure s12. One example of U-shaped response (same shown in figures 3a and s4). In the left panels, the behavioral
choice pattern and the neuronal response are plotted in the usual “ordinal” x-axis coordinates. From the behavioral
choice pattern, we obtain the relative value n*=3.2. In the right panel, the neuronal response is plotted in “cardinal” x-
axis coordinates, separately for trials in which the monkey chose juice A and juice B. The two regression slopes are
plotted in red. From the bi-linear regression, we obtain a slope ratio k* = 3.0 (±1.4) (±95% confidence interval).
Figure s13

a all juice pairs b 1/2 apple : peppermint


8

4 4

slope ratio (k*)


2
2
slope ratio (k*)

1
k* = 0.08 + 1.18 n*
1

1/2 1 2 4
relative value (n*)

1/4

1/8 c regression slope b1 for different juice pairs


1 2 4 8
relative value (n*) regression: k* = b + b n*
0 1
mean b = 1.05 (±0.15)
1

grape : peppermint

grape : 1/3 cranberry

1/2 apple : peppermint

lemon k.a. : 1/2 apple

grape : 1/2 apple

fruit punch : 1/2 apple

1/2 apple : .60 g/l salt

-1 0 1 2 3
b1

Figure s13. Relationship between slope ratio k* and relative value n*. See Supplementary Results.
Figure s14

sugar

X
slope ratio (k*)

water
1

1
relative value (n*)

Figure s14. Predicted relationship between k* and n*. If U-shaped responses encode chosen value, the values of k*
should lie on a diagonal (gray line). If they encode the quantity of some juice ingredient (e.g., water, sugar, or some
other ingredient X or Y), values of k* should lie on some horizontal line corresponding to the concentration ratio (blue,
red, green, and yellow lines). Similarly, if U-shaped responses encode the linear combination of multiple ingredient,
values of k* should lie on some horizontal line (e.g., brown dotted line).

You might also like