You are on page 1of 6

Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 25 (2016) 72e77

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Parkinsonism and Related Disorders


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/parkreldis

Postural sensory correlates of freezing of gait in Parkinson's disease


Young Eun Huh a, Seonhong Hwang b, Keehoon Kim b, Won-Ho Chung c,
Jinyoung Youn d, e, Jin Whan Cho d, e, *
a
Department of Neurology, CHA Bundang Medical Center, CHA University, Seongnam, South Korea
b
Center for Robotics Research, Korea Institute of Science and Technology, Seoul, South Korea
c
Department of Otolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery, Sungkyunkwan University School of Medicine, Samsung Medical Center, Seoul, South Korea
d
Department of Neurology, Sungkyunkwan University School of Medicine, Samsung Medical Center, Seoul, South Korea
e
Neuroscience Center, Samsung Medical Center, Seoul, South Korea

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Introduction: To elucidate the unique patterns of postural sensory deficits contributing to freezing of gait
Received 7 December 2015 (FOG) in patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) and to identify postural sensory modalities that correlate
Received in revised form with FOG severity.
27 January 2016
Methods: Twenty-five PD patients with FOG, 22 PD patients without FOG, and 26 age-matched controls
Accepted 1 February 2016
were evaluated using a sensory organization test and clinical measures including the Unified Parkinson's
Disease Rating Scale motor score, Montreal Cognitive Assessment, Frontal Assessment Battery, Activities-
Keywords:
specific Balance Confidence, Beck Anxiety Inventory, Beck Depression Inventory, and Berg Balance Scale.
Parkinson's disease
Freezing of gait
Multivariable logistic regression analysis was performed for posturographic parameters and possible
Postural sensory deficits confounders to determine postural sensory contributors to FOG. We also correlated FOG severity,
Vestibular impairment measured using a New Freezing of Gait Questionnaire, with posturographic parameters.
Results: PD patients with FOG showed worse postural sensory processing compared with those without
FOG. In particular, the inability to use the vestibular information (odds ratio [OR] 1.447; 95% confidential
interval [CI]: 1.120, 1.869) and poor control over the perturbed somatosensory inputs (OR 2.904; 95% CI:
1.028, 8.202) significantly contributed to FOG. Among PD patients with FOG, FOG severity was correlated
with higher reliance on visual information (r ¼ 0.432, p ¼ 0.039).
Conclusions: Postural sensory deficits involving specific sensory modalities are strongly associated with
FOG. Quantitative measurement of postural sensory deficits in PD patients with FOG may provide a
better understanding of pathomechanisms of FOG and increase the efficacy of sensory cueing strategies
for alleviating FOG, by more accurately identifying suitable patients for rehabilitative therapies.
© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction with FOG, reflecting its complex pathomechanism [1e3].


PD patients with FOG also exhibit deficits in sensory processing
Freezing of gait (FOG) is characterized by an episodic inability to [4e6]. During normal locomotion, sensory signals from visual,
generate effective stepping despite intention to walk [1]. It is a vestibular, and somatosensory systems are integrated via central
devastating condition in patients with Parkinson's disease (PD), as neural networks and provide real-time information used as refer-
it causes falls and diminishes mobility and quality of life [1]. Several ences for cognitive, emotional, and automatic locomotor processes
factors, including disease severity, cognitive dysfunction, attention [7]. Accordingly, the inability to integrate postural sensory inputs
deficits, emotional state, and postural instability, are associated may contribute to the generation of FOG [7]. Indeed, FOG can be
provoked under sensory conflicting situations in which balance is
challenged [1] and also alleviated by various sensory cues [1,8,9].
* Corresponding author. Department of Neurology, Sungkyunkwan University However, little is known about how postural sensory deficits are
School of Medicine, Neuroscience Center, Samsung Medical Center, 81 Irwon-Ro implicated to pathomechanisms of FOG.
Gangnam-gu, Seoul, 135-710, South Korea. We aimed to characterize postural sensory deficits in PD pa-
E-mail addresses: nana1880@hanmail.net (Y.E. Huh), sh@kist.re.kr (S. Hwang),
tients with FOG and to determine specific patterns of postural
khk@kist.re.kr (K. Kim), wonho.chung@samsung.com (W.-H. Chung), genian.
youn@samsung.com (J. Youn), jinwhan.cho@samsung.com (J.W. Cho). sensory deficits that contribute to FOG. Quantitative

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.02.004
1353-8020/© 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Y.E. Huh et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 25 (2016) 72e77 73

posturographic measures were compared between PD patients center of gravity sway and analyzed using an arithmetic algorithm
with and without FOG using a sensory organization test (SOT). We provided by the EquiTest® system. The force plate and visual sur-
also identified postural sensory modalities associated with the round were fixed or programmed to revolve in reference to sub-
severity of FOG. ject's sways in an anterior-posterior direction. By manipulating
fixed/sway-referenced and eye open/closed conditions, visual
2. Methods and/or somatosensory inputs were distorted or eliminated. The SOT
protocol consisted of six conditions (Fig. 1A). Static balance mea-
2.1. Subjects sures were obtained from condition 1 in which all sensory sources
were available. In conditions 2 and 3, visual inputs were absent and
We recruited 47 patients with idiopathic PD and 26 age- distorted, respectively. In the last three conditions, somatosensory
matched healthy controls through the Movement Disorders Clinic inputs were distorted, while visual inputs were available (condition
of Samsung Medical Center. A diagnosis of idiopathic PD was made 4), absent (condition 5), or inaccurate (condition 6).
by a specialist experienced in movement disorders (J.W.C) based on All participants were outfitted with a safety harness and stood
UK Parkinson's Disease Society Brain Bank criteria. All patients barefoot at a standardized stance width based on their body height.
were taking levodopa in combination with dopamine agonist and They were instructed to remain as still as possible in an upright
experienced levodopa-related motor fluctuations. Subjects were posture during each condition, which was composed of three
excluded if they could not safely stand unaided, or had a history of repeated 20-s trials. A fall was noted when subjects swayed beyond
falls in the last 12 months, dementia, visual disturbances, clinical their limit of stability, requiring a corrective step or assistance.
signs of disorders affecting proprioception, musculoskeletal prob-
lems, orthostatic hypotension, or neurosurgical interventions. 2.3. Balance measurements
Subjects were also excluded if they had a history or clinical signs of
vestibular diseases, including the presence of spontaneous or head- To assess postural stability, an equilibrium score was obtained
shaking nystagmus or corrective saccades during head impulse from each SOT trial, by comparing the maximum angle of center of
testing. Motor disability was assessed using the Unified Parkinson's gravity sway in an anterior-posterior direction with the theoretical
Disease Rating Scale motor score (UPDRS-III). For clinical balance limit of stability which was assumed to be 12.5 (Fig. 1B) [15].
measurement, we used the Berg Balance Scale (BBS), a 56-point Equilibrium scores ranged from 0% to 100%, with higher scores
scale in which lower scores indicate worse balance [10]. Global indicating better ability to maintain balance and 0% indicating a fall.
cognitive and executive functions were tested using Montreal Averaged equilibrium scores of three trials (ESs) from each SOT
Cognitive Assessment (MOCA) and Frontal Assessment Battery condition were used to assess postural stability.
(FAB), respectively. The New Freezing of Gait Questionnaire Sensory ratios (SRs) were calculated to measure the contribu-
(NFOGQ) was used to evaluate the presence and severity of FOG tion of each sensory input to postural balance [15]. The ability to use
[11]. The Activities-specific Balance Confidence (ABC) scale somatosensory information (SR-SOM), visual information (SR-VIS),
(0e100%) was used to estimate fear of falling (FOF) by measuring and vestibular information (SR-VEST) were estimated as ratios
confidence in maintaining balance while performing various between the ES of condition 2 (ES2) and ES of condition 1 (ES1),
ambulatory activities [10,12]. Lower scores indicate greater FOF. All between the ES of condition 4 (ES4) and ES1, and between the ES of
subjects completed the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) and Beck condition 5 (ES5) and ES1, respectively. Lower SRs indicated a
Anxiety Inventory (BAI). Clinical and neurological examinations poorer ability to use relevant sensory information to maintain
and posturography were performed during the morning off- balance. Visual preference (SR-VISPREF) was defined as a ratio of
medication, at least 12 h after withholding anti-parkinsonian the sum of the ES of condition 3 (ES3) and the ES of condition 6
medications. (ES6) to the sum of ES2 and ES5. Lower SR-VISPREFs indicated
Patients were assigned to PD patients with FOG (PD-FOG) if they higher dependency on visual information, even if inaccurate. The
experienced FOG at least once during the past month based on ability to manage distorted somatosensory inputs (SR-MANSOM)
NFOGQ item 1, and one or more FOG episodes were provoked and was calculated by dividing the sum of ESs from sway-referenced
detected by two experienced movement specialists (Y.E.H. and plate conditions (ES4 þ ES5 þ ES6) by that from fixed plate con-
J.W.C.) during experiments. FOG was elicited using a turning-in- ditions (ES1 þ ES2 þ ES3) [16,17].
place task in which patients were asked to make tight and rapid
360 turns to the left or right for 2 min, immediately before pos- 2.4. Statistical analysis
turography assessment [13,14]. All PD-FOG showed worse freezing
during the off-medication state, and no PD patients without FOG To assess overall differences between groups in demographic,
(PD-noFOG) reported either self-assessed or experimentally pro- clinical, and posturographic variables, KruskaleWallis or Man-
voked FOG episodes. PD-FOG and PD-noFOG were matched for age neWhitney U tests, where appropriate, were performed for
and off Hoehn and Yahr stage, which ranged from 2 to 3. continuous data, as normal distributions could not be assumed for
Written informed consent was obtained from each subject and most variables. Significant main effects of group were further
the study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board examined by post hoc analysis with Tukey's tests using ranked data.
of Samsung Medical Center. c2-tests were used to analyze categorical data. To identify postural
sensory modalities contributing to FOG, we conducted a multivar-
2.2. Posturography iable multinomial logistic regression analysis for all SR values with
adjustment for clinical variables that differed between groups with
Postural sensory deficits were assessed using the SOT protocol p < 0.2. Variance inflation factors (VIFs) were estimated for each
of a computerized dynamic posturography system (EquiTest®; variable entering the multivariable model. Variables with a VIF  10
NeuroCom International, Clackamas, OR, USA) [15], which was were considered to have a high level of multi-collinearity and
equipped with a force plate enclosed by a visual surround. During omitted from the model. VIFs of remaining variables were recal-
the SOT, subjects stood on the force plate from which the position of culated to ensure an independent association with FOG. To inves-
their center of pressure was recorded at a sampling rate of 100 Hz. tigate correlations between the severity of FOG and each postural
Digitized center of pressure data were automatically converted into sensory modality, Spearman partial correlations were performed
74 Y.E. Huh et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 25 (2016) 72e77

Fig. 1. Schematic diagram of the sensory organization test. (A) Six conditions of the sensory organization test. Symbols (eye, inner ear, and foot) denote the visual, vestibular, and
somatosensory inputs, respectively. (B) Equation for calculating the equilibrium score (ES). q, the maximum angle of center of gravity sway in an anterior-posterior direction. Images
courtesy of Natus Medical Incorporated.

between NFOGQ scores and SR values, controlling for clinical var- p ¼ 0.980; BDI: p ¼ 0.985).
iables that significantly correlated with NFOGQ scores or SR values
as determined by Spearman correlations. 3.2. Posturographic data

3. Results Compared with controls, PD-FOG showed lower ESs for most
SOT conditions, except condition 1 (ES2: p ¼ 0.014; ES3: p ¼ 0.011;
3.1. Clinical characteristics ES4: p < 0.001; ES5: p < 0.001; ES6: p < 0.001) (Fig. 2A and
Supplemental Table 1). Compared with PD-noFOG, PD-FOG
Demographic and clinical characteristics of subjects are shown exhibited lower ESs for sway-referenced plate conditions, including
in Table 1. Subject groups were similar in terms of age, gender, years ES4 (p ¼ 0.003), ES5 (p < 0.001), and ES6 (p < 0.001). There were no
of education, and MOCA and FAB scores. PD-FOG and PD-noFOG significant differences in ESs for any SOT condition between PD-
showed equivalent disease duration, Hoehn and Yahr stage, fre- noFOG and controls.
quency of motor subtype, and levodopa equivalent daily dose. PD- Sensory analysis showed that PD-FOG had more difficulty using
FOG had lower BBS and ABC scores than PD-noFOG (BBS: p ¼ 0.032; visual (SR-VIS: p ¼ 0.002 vs. PD-noFOG, p < 0.001 vs. controls) and
ABC: p ¼ 0.002) and controls (BBS: p < 0.001; ABC: p < 0.001). PD- vestibular (SR-VEST: p < 0.001 vs. PD-noFOG, p < 0.001 vs. controls)
noFOG showed lower BBS (p < 0.001) and ABC (p < 0.001) scores information, and managing distorted somatosensory inputs (SR-
than controls. PD-FOG and PD-noFOG had higher UPDRS-III, BAI, MANSOM: p < 0.001 vs. PD-noFOG, p < 0.001 vs. controls) to ensure
and BDI scores than controls, with no significant differences be- postural balance compared with PD-noFOG and controls (Fig. 2B
tween the two patient groups (UPDRS-III: p ¼ 0.780; BAI: and Supplemental Table 1). No SR values differed significantly

Table 1
Demographic and clinical characteristics of subjects.

PD-FOG (n ¼ 25) PD-noFOG (n ¼ 22) Control (n ¼ 26) pa

Age (years) 64.0 (58.5e69.5) 64.5 (61.0e67.8) 64.0 (62.0e66.0) 0.890


Gender (M/F) 13/12 13/9 13/13 0.664
Education (years) 12.0 (12.0e16.0) 12.0 (9.0e16.0) 12.0 (12.0e16.0) 0.567
Disease duration (years) 10.0 (8.0e12.0) 9.0 (7.8e10.3) e 0.211
Hoehn and Yahr stage 2.5 (2.5e3.0) 2.5 (2.0e3.0) e 0.203
UPDRS-III 35.0 (33.0e38.3) 36.0 (31.5e37.3) 0 (0e2.0) <0.001b
PD motor subtypes (PIGD/TD) 15/10 11/11 e 0.564
NFOGQ 12.0 (10.5e18.5) 0 e e
BBS 47.0 (45.0e52.0) 50.0 (48.8e51.3) 56.0 (54.3e56.0) <0.001b,c
MOCA 28.0 (26.0e29.0) 26.5 (25.8e28.0) 28.0 (26.3e29.0) 0.416
FAB 16.0 (14.0e17.0) 16.0 (15.0e17.0) 16.0 (16.0e17.0) 0.342
ABC 75.6 (63.4e85.6) 89.1 (79.1e96.3) 98.4 (97.5e99.4) <0.001b,c
BAI 7.0 (3.0e14.0) 5.0 (3.0e12.3) 1.0 (0e2.0) <0.001b
BDI 8.0 (5.5e15.5) 10.0 (3.5e14.5) 2.0 (0e4.8) <0.001b
LEDD (mg) 768.5 (577.0e901.9) 655.0 (392.3e807.8) e 0.176
Height (m) 1.6 (1.5e1.7) 1.6 (1.5e1.7) 1.6 (1.5e1.7) 0.659
BMI (kg/m2) 23.5 (21.4e26.0) 23.3 (21.1e25.8) 23.7 (21.6e26.4) 0.744

Data are shown as median (25the75th quartiles) unless otherwise noted.


PD-FOG, Parkinson's disease patients with freezing of gait; PD-noFOG, Parkinson's disease patients without freezing of gait; UPDRS-III, Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale
motor score; PD, Parkinson's disease; PIGD, postural instability and gait disturbance; TD, tremor dominant; NFOGQ, New Freezing of Gait Questionnaire; BBS, Berg Balance
Scale; MOCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; FAB, Frontal Assessment Battery; ABC, Activities-specific Balance Confidence; BAI, Beck Anxiety Inventory; BDI, Beck Depression
Inventory; LEDD, Levodopa equivalent daily dose; BMI, Body mass index.
a
Based on KruskaleWallis, ManneWhitney U, or c2-tests, as appropriate.
b
Post hoc analysis with Tukey's tests using ranked data: PD-FOG or PD-noFOG vs control.
c
Post hoc analysis with Tukey's tests using ranked data: PD-FOG vs PD-noFOG.
Y.E. Huh et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 25 (2016) 72e77 75

Fig. 2. Comparison of posturographic data between subject groups. Boxplots of (A) the equilibrium scores and (B) sensory ratios for PD-FOG, PD-noFOG, and control individuals. The
lower and upper limits of the boxes delineate the 25th and 75th quartile values, respectively. The lines inside the boxes represent the median, and the whiskers show the 5th and
95th percentiles. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01. PD-FOG, Parkinson's disease patients with freezing of gait; PD-noFOG, Parkinson's disease patients without freezing of gait; C1-6, conditions
1e6; SR-SOM, ability to use somatosensory information; SR-VIS, ability to use visual information; SR-VEST, ability to use vestibular information; SR-VISPREF, dependency on visual
information; SR-MANSOM, ability to manage distorted somatosensory inputs.

between PD-noFOG and controls. ABC (r ¼ 0.435, p ¼ 0.030) and FAB (r ¼ 0.451, p ¼ 0.024), while SR-
The multivariable model included all SR values as well as MANSOM was correlated with BBS (r ¼ 0.450, p ¼ 0.024). NFOGQ
UPDRS-III, BBS, ABC, BAI, and BDI scores (Table 2). Variables with a scores were also correlated with ABC (r ¼ 0.637, p ¼ 0.001), FAB
high risk of collinearity (SR-MANSOM: VIF ¼ 26.558 and SR-VEST: (r ¼ 0.471, p ¼ 0.017), and BBS (r ¼ 0.488, p ¼ 0.014) scores. SR-
VIF ¼ 12.846) were consecutively excluded. The remaining nine VISPREF remained correlated with NFOGQ (r ¼ 0.432, p ¼ 0.039)
variables in each regression model had VIFs < 5, indicating a low after controlling for ABC and FAB scores, while SR-MANSOM did not
risk of collinearity. The multivariable model including SR-VEST after controlling for BBS score (r ¼ 0.374, p ¼ 0.072).
identified SR-VEST as an independent risk factor for FOG among
PD patients. In the multivariable model including SR-MANSOM, SR- 4. Discussion
MANSOM was an independent risk factor for FOG and SR-VISPREF
tended to have an influence on FOG. Our study demonstrated that impaired processing of postural
sensory inputs was strongly related to the presence and severity of
3.3. Correlation analysis FOG in PD. Specific patterns of postural sensory deficits involving
recruitment of vestibular information and control over distorted
Spearman correlations revealed that NFOGQ scores correlated somatosensory inputs substantially contributed to FOG. Moreover,
with SR values for visual dependency (SR-VISPREF: r ¼ 0.660, correlation analysis revealed that PD-FOG with higher reliance on
p < 0.001) and the ability to control distorted somatosensory inputs visual information exhibited more profound freezing behavior. In
(SR-MANSOM: r ¼ 0.511, p ¼ 0.009) among PD-FOG addition, the severity of FOG was correlated with several clinical
(Supplemental Table 2). Also, SR-VISPREF was correlated with measures assessing FOF, global executive function, and clinical
balance performance.
The process of integrating sensory information to ensure
Table 2 postural control has been widely studied in PD patients using
Multivariable analysis of postural sensory contributors to FOG in PD patients.
quantitative posturography. These studies have mainly assessed the
SRs p ORa 95% CI relationship between postural sensory deficits and disease severity
Multivariable model including SR-VEST [16,18] or fall risk [10] and the effects of therapeutic interventions
SR-SOM 0.581 1.092 0.799e1.493 on postural control [17]. However, few studies systematically
SR-VIS 0.073 1.122 0.990e1.272 assessed postural sensory processing in PD patients with FOG [19],
SR-VEST 0.005 1.447 1.120e1.869 despite growing evidence from behavioral [4] and neuroimaging
SR-VISPREF 0.171 1.075 0.969e1.191
Multivariable model including SR-MANSOM
works [5,6] suggesting their potential relationship. Furthermore,
SR-SOM 0.504 1.116 0.809e1.538 although FOG and postural imbalance emerge from a disrupted
SR-VIS 0.138 0.775 0.553e1.085 interplay between motor, cognitive, and affective factors [1,20],
SR-VISPREF 0.056 0.695 0.478e1.009 these factors have been rarely considered in previous posturo-
SR-MANSOM 0.044 2.904 1.028e8.202
graphic studies. In addition, defining the unique patterns of
FOG, freezing of gait; PD, Parkinson's disease; SR, sensory ratio; OR, Odds ratio; CI, postural sensory loss in PD patients with FOG can promote the
confidence interval; SR-VEST, ability to use vestibular information; SR-SOM, ability efficacy of sensory cueing strategies for ameliorating FOG by more
to use somatosensory information; SR-VIS, ability to use visual information; SR-
VISPREF, dependency on visual information; SR-MANSOM, ability to manage dis-
accurately identifying suitable patients for these rehabilitative
torted somatosensory inputs. therapies. Our study gives some answers to these issues by delin-
a
Adjusted for UPDRS-III, BBS, ABC, BAI, and BDI scores. eating independent contributions of postural sensory deficits to
76 Y.E. Huh et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 25 (2016) 72e77

FOG in PD while also considering motor, cognitive, and affective dopaminergically-deficient striatal regions by complementary yet
factors. competing motor-cognitive-limbic inputs (Supplemental
We found that the inability to integrate vestibular information reference). Our PD-FOG showed lower ABC scores, reflecting
for postural balance was an important contributor to FOG. As none greater FOF. Moreover, PD-FOG with greater FOF showed more
of our PD patients showed sensory deficits involving peripheral severe FOG and higher reliance on visual information. These find-
afferents, this finding may stem from a failure in the central pro- ings are consistent with previous studies showing that FOF is
cessing of vestibular feedback, consistent with previous studies strongly associated with poorer gait and balance in PD [12], and is
[16e18]. Several studies report abnormal central processing of also a main characteristic of several balance disorders, such as vi-
vestibular signals in PD. Altered vestibulocollic responses, which sual vertigo, that are often related to over-reliance on visual stimuli
represent neural circuits mediating postural balance, have been [30]. In addition, we found that greater deterioration in executive
documented in PD patients with moderate to severe motor function was associated with more severe FOG and higher visual
disability [21]. A previous posturographic study shows that PD dependency among PD-FOG. FOG is frequently related to executive
patients who experience falls exhibit impaired balance during the dysfunction, especially conflict resolution failure [3]. Accordingly,
SOT when postural control relies on vestibular information [22]. A poorer executive function can further delay selection of internal
recent research reports improved balance control in PD patients locomotor representation, upon encountering environmental
after applying galvanic stimulation activating the vestibular system changes, consequently compelling PD-FOG to rely more on
[23]. However, the relationship between vestibular dysfunction and externally-driven visual stimuli [8] and eliciting more severe FOG.
FOG is largely unknown in PD. Normally, vestibular feedbacks Given this complex interaction between FOG and balance, cogni-
become more important when initiating walking or turning [24], tive, and emotional factors, it is critical to incorporate these factors
when FOG is usually provoked in PD patients [1]. Direct projections into studies investigating FOG in PD [1].
from vestibular nuclei to pedunculopontine nuclei, which are Although impaired postural sensory processing is reported in
involved in the generation of FOG [1,13], have been confirmed in PD patients with moderate to severe motor disability [16,18],
primates [25]. Recently, single cell recordings of pedunculopontine postural sensory processing was generally preserved in our PD-
nuclei in macaque monkeys reveal that vestibular stimuli enhance noFOG with similar disability. Instead, our PD-FOG showed
the activity of these neurons [26]. Together with these previous similar patterns of postural sensory impairment to those in previ-
findings, our results indicate that impaired processing of vestibular ous posturographic studies [16,18]. Presumably, FOG may be, partly,
information to ensure balance plays an essential role in generating responsible for the impaired sensory processing observed in PD
FOG in PD. patients in previous studies [16,18].
In our PD-FOG, postural sensory processing was compromised A couple limitations of our study should be noted. First, because
exclusively during distorted somatosensory conditions. This is in FOG is difficult to elicit in a laboratory setting, we could not exclude
line with a previous study demonstrating that somatosensory il- the possibility that individuals classified as PD-noFOG might
lusions induced by tendon vibration impair force-matching re- experience FOG during daily activities. However, group classifica-
sponses during isometric contraction in PD patients with FOG [27], tion was based on both the NFOGQ and a turning-in-place task,
suggesting deficits in processing distorted somatosensation. Our which reliably discriminate between PD patients with and without
results are further supported by the finding that a voluntary step FOG [11,14]. Thus, it is unlikely that individuals who had freezing
paradigm conjoined with somatosensory perturbation provokes episodes would be included in PD-noFOG. Second, given the cross-
freezing-like knee trembling in PD patients with FOG [2], possibly sectional design of this study, it remains uncertain whether
due to decoupling between anticipatory postural adjustments and postural sensory deficits observed in our patients represent a pri-
step motor programs. Therefore, postural sensory deficits involving mary pathology or a maladaptive change to compensate for pri-
ineffective control over erroneous somatosensory information mary damage. Longitudinal monitoring of postural sensory deficits
could contribute to FOG, possibly by disrupting postural prepara- is needed to determine the nature of their relationship with FOG.
tion process preceding steps.
Our correlation analysis revealed a relationship between visual 5. Conclusion
dependency and the severity of FOG among PD-FOG. PD patients
are known to rely heavily on visual information for locomotor Our data demonstrate that specific patterns of postural sensory
performance [8,28]. For instance, PD patients shows abnormally deficits, including the inability to use vestibular inputs and poor
increased postural sway in response to moving visual scene, control over perturbed somatosensory feedbacks, contribute to
reflecting elevated visual dependency [28]. Similarly, our PD-FOG FOG in PD patients. Moreover, the severity of FOG is associated with
swayed more during sway-referenced visual conditions, condi- increased reliance on visual inputs. Quantitative assessment of
tions 3 and 6, and SR-VISPREF tended to predict the presence of postural sensory deficits in PD patients with FOG may increase our
FOG. This result might be interpreted as a compensatory increment understanding of pathomechanisms of FOG and help identify pa-
of visual dependency to overcome more severe FOG, as visual cues tients who are suitable for employing sensory cueing strategies to
facilitate locomotion in PD by shunting a deficient internal trigger alleviate FOG. Future prospective studies investigating postural
of basal ganglia-supplementary motor area circuits [29]. Alterna- sensory deficits in PD patients might lead to more quantitative
tively, increased visual dependency could induce more severe measures for identifying patients at a high risk of FOG and to the
freezing behavior by entrapping PD-FOG to visual inputs, even if development of rehabilitative therapies customized for PD patients
unreliable, subsequently interrupting sensory processing [8]. Pro- with specific sensory loss.
spective trials are needed to confirm a causal relationship between
visual dependency and FOG. Authors roles
We found that several clinical parameters, including ABC, BBS,
and FAB scores, related to FOG and posturographic measures [1]. Young Eun Huh: Design of the study; acquisition of data;
Given that postural sensory signals are utilized to generate refer- analysis and interpretation of data; drafting the article and revising
ences for cognitive and limbic/affective processes during locomo- it critically for important intellectual content; final approval of the
tion, our data provide support for a recent model of FOG, which version to be submitted. Seonhong Hwang: Analysis of data,
proposes that FOG results from the paroxysmal overload of revising the article critically for important intellectual content;
Y.E. Huh et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 25 (2016) 72e77 77

final approval of the version to be submitted. Keehoon Kim: and posturographic correlates of falling in Parkinson's disease, Mov. Disord.
28 (2013) 1250e1256.
Analysis of data, revising the article critically for important intel-
[11] A. Nieuwboer, L. Rochester, T. Herman, W. Vandenberghe, G.E. Emil,
lectual content; final approval of the version to be submitted. Won- T. Thomaes, N. Giladi, Reliability of the new freezing of gait questionnaire:
Ho Chung: Conception and design of the study; acquisition of data; agreement between patients with Parkinson's disease and their carers, Gait
revising the article critically for important intellectual content; Posture 30 (2009) 459e463.
[12] M.K. Mak, M.Y. Pang, Fear of falling is independently associated with recurrent
final approval of the version to be submitted. Jinyoung Youn: falls in patients with Parkinson's disease: a 1-year prospective study, J. Neurol.
Conception and design of the study; acquisition of data; revising 256 (2009) 1689e1695.
the article critically for important intellectual content; final [13] B.W. Fling, R.G. Cohen, M. Mancini, J.G. Nutt, D.A. Fair, F.B. Horak, Asymmetric
pedunculopontine network connectivity in parkinsonian patients with
approval of the version to be submitted. Jin Whan Cho: Conception freezing of gait, Brain 136 (2013) 2405e2418.
and design of the study; acquisition of data; analysis and inter- [14] A.H. Snijders, C.A. Haaxma, Y.J. Hagen, M. Munneke, B.R. Bloem, Freezer or
pretation of data; revising the article critically for important in- non-freezer: clinical assessment of freezing of gait, Park. Relat. Disord. 18
(2012) 149e154.
tellectual content; final approval of the version to be submitted. [15] L.M. Nashner, J.F. Peters, Dynamic posturography in the diagnosis and man-
agement of dizziness and balance disorders, Neurol. Clin. 8 (1990) 331e349.
Conflict of interest [16] S. Colnat-Coulbois, G.C. Gauchard, L. Maillard, G. Barroche, H. Vespignani,
J. Auque, P.P. Perrin, Management of postural sensory conflict and dynamic
balance control in late-stage Parkinson's disease, Neuroscience 193 (2011)
None. 363e369.
[17] S. Colnat-Coulbois, G.C. Gauchard, L. Maillard, G. Barroche, H. Vespignani,
J. Auque, P.P. Perrin, Bilateral subthalamic nucleus stimulation improves
Study funding
balance control in Parkinson's disease, J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76
(2005) 780e787.
None. [18] A. Frenklach, S. Louie, M.M. Koop, H. Bronte-Stewart, Excessive postural sway
and the risk of falls at different stages of Parkinson's disease, Mov. Disord. 24
(2009) 377e385.
Appendix A. Supplementary data [19] G. Vervoort, E. Nackaerts, F. Mohammadi, E. Heremans, S. Verschueren,
A. Nieuwboer, S. Vercruysse, Which aspects of postural control differentiate
Supplementary data related to this article can be found at http:// between patients with Parkinson's disease with and without freezing of gait?
Park. Dis. 2013 (2013) 971480.
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.parkreldis.2016.02.004. [20] B. Schoneburg, M. Mancini, F. Horak, J.G. Nutt, Framework for understanding
balance dysfunction in Parkinson's disease, Mov. Disord. 28 (2013)
References 1474e1482.
[21] M. Potter-Nerger, M.M. Reich, J.G. Colebatch, G. Deuschl, J. Volkmann, Dif-
ferential effect of dopa and subthalamic stimulation on vestibular activity in
[1] A. Nieuwboer, N. Giladi, Characterizing freezing of gait in Parkinson's disease:
Parkinson's disease, Mov. Disord. 27 (2012) 1268e1275.
models of an episodic phenomenon, Mov. Disord. 28 (2013) 1509e1519.
[22] M. Rossi-Izquierdo, D. Basta, J.P. Rubio-Rodriguez, S. Santos-Perez, A. Ernst,
[2] J.V. Jacobs, J.G. Nutt, P. Carlson-Kuhta, M. Stephens, F.B. Horak, Knee trembling
A. Sesar-Ignacio, M. Alberte-Woodward, M. Guijarro-Del Amo, A. Estany-
during freezing of gait represents multiple anticipatory postural adjustments,
Gestal, E. San Roman-Rodriguez, A. Faraldo-Garcia, A. Zubizarreta-Gutierrez,
Exp. Neurol. 215 (2009) 334e341.
A. Soto-Varela, Is posturography able to identify fallers in patients with Par-
[3] J. Vandenbossche, N. Deroost, E. Soetens, P. Zeischka, J. Spildooren,
kinson's disease? Gait Posture 40 (2014) 53e57.
S. Vercruysse, A. Nieuwboer, E. Kerckhofs, Conflict and freezing of gait in
[23] G. Samoudi, M. Jivegard, A.P. Mulavara, F. Bergquist, Effects of stochastic
Parkinson's disease: support for a response control deficit, Neuroscience 206
vestibular galvanic stimulation and ldopa on balance and motor symptoms in
(2012) 144e154.
patients with Parkinson's disease, Brain Stimul. 8 (2015) 474e480.
[4] J. Nantel, J.C. McDonald, S. Tan, H. Bronte-Stewart, Deficits in visuospatial
[24] P.M. Kennedy, E.K. Cressman, A.N. Carlsen, R. Chua, Assessing vestibular
processing contribute to quantitative measures of freezing of gait in Parkin-
contributions during changes in gait trajectory, Neuroreport 16 (2005)
son's disease, Neuroscience 221 (2012) 151e156.
1097e1100.
[5] V.S. Kostic, F. Agosta, M. Pievani, E. Stefanova, M. Jecmenica-Lukic, A. Scarale,
[25] L.N. Hazrati, A. Parent, Projection from the deep cerebellar nuclei to the
V. Spica, M. Filippi, Pattern of brain tissue loss associated with freezing of gait
pedunculopontine nucleus in the squirrel monkey, Brain Res. 585 (1992)
in Parkinson disease, Neurology 78 (2012) 409e416.
267e271.
[6] T. Herman, K. Rosenberg-Katz, Y. Jacob, N. Giladi, J.M. Hausdorff, Gray matter
[26] B.R. Aravamuthan, D.E. Angelaki, Vestibular responses in the macaque
atrophy and freezing of gait in Parkinson's disease: is the evidence black-on-
pedunculopontine nucleus and central mesencephalic reticular formation,
white? Mov. Disord. 29 (2014) 134e139.
Neuroscience 223 (2012) 183e199.
[7] K. Takakusaki, Neurophysiology of gait: from the spinal cord to the frontal
[27] T. Tan, Q.J. Almeida, F. Rahimi, Proprioceptive deficits in Parkinson's disease
lobe, Mov. Disord. 28 (2013) 1483e1491.
patients with freezing of gait, Neuroscience 192 (2011) 746e752.
[8] J.P. Azulay, S. Mesure, O. Blin, Influence of visual cues on gait in Parkinson's
[28] A.M. Bronstein, J.D. Hood, M.A. Gresty, C. Panagi, Visual control of balance in
disease: contribution to attention or sensory dependence? J. Neurol. Sci. 248
cerebellar and parkinsonian syndromes, Brain 113 (Pt 3) (1990) 767e779.
(2006) 192e195.
[29] J.P. Azulay, S. Mesure, B. Amblard, O. Blin, I. Sangla, J. Pouget, Visual control of
[9] A. Nieuwboer, G. Kwakkel, L. Rochester, D. Jones, E. van Wegen, A.M. Willems,
locomotion in Parkinson's disease, Brain 122 (Pt 1) (1999) 111e120.
F. Chavret, V. Hetherington, K. Baker, I. Lim, Cueing training in the home
[30] A.M. Bronstein, Visual vertigo syndrome: clinical and posturography findings,
improves gait-related mobility in Parkinson's disease: the RESCUE trial,
J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 59 (1995) 472e476.
J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 78 (2007) 134e140.
[10] L. Johnson, I. James, J. Rodrigues, R. Stell, G. Thickbroom, F. Mastaglia, Clinical

You might also like