You are on page 1of 47

Journal Pre-proof

Moral Motivation and the Basal Forebrain

Roland Zahn, Ricardo de Oliveira-Souza, Jorge Moll

PII: S0149-7634(19)30077-6
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.10.022
Reference: NBR 3587

To appear in: Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

Received Date: 28 January 2019


Revised Date: 24 October 2019
Accepted Date: 28 October 2019

Please cite this article as: Zahn R, de Oliveira-Souza R, Moll J, Moral Motivation and the
Basal Forebrain, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews (2019),
doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.10.022

This is a PDF file of an article that has undergone enhancements after acceptance, such as
the addition of a cover page and metadata, and formatting for readability, but it is not yet the
definitive version of record. This version will undergo additional copyediting, typesetting and
review before it is published in its final form, but we are providing this version to give early
visibility of the article. Please note that, during the production process, errors may be
discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal
pertain.

© 2019 Published by Elsevier.


1

Moral Motivation and the Basal Forebrain

Roland Zahn a,b , Ricardo de Oliveira-Souza a,c *, Jorge Moll a

a
Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience Unit, D’Or Institute for Research

& Education (IDOR), Rio de Janeiro, Brazil.


b
Centre for Affective Disorders, Department of Psychological Medicine,

Division of Academic Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and

of
Neuroscience, King’s College London, United Kingdom.

ro
c
The Federal University of the State of Rio de Janeiro, Rio de Janeiro,

Brazil.
-p
re
* Corresponding author
lP

Dr Jorge Moll, MD PhD

Cognitive and Behavioral Neuroscience Unit


na

D’Or Institute for Research & Education (IDOR)

Rua Diniz Cordeiro, 30

22281-100 Rio de Janeiro, Brazil


ur

e-mail: jorge.moll@idor.org.
Jo

Highlights
 Moral motivations drive humans to serve the needs of others and

society.
2

 The basal forebrain is important for enabling moral motivation.

 This includes the septo-hypothalamic region, implicated in kinship

bonding.

 And also includes the connected subgenual frontal cortex.

 Implications for prosocial behaviour and mental health are discussed.

Abstract

of
Moral motivations drive humans to sacrifice selfish needs to serve the needs

ro
of others and internalized sociocultural norms. Over the past two decades,

several brain regions have been associated with different aspects of moral
-p
cognition and behaviour. Only more recently, however, investigations have
re
highlighted the importance of the basal forebrain for moral motivation. This

includes the septo-hypothalamic region, implicated in kinship bonding


lP

across mammal species, and the closely connected subgenual frontal cortex.

Understanding the neuroanatomy of moral motivation and its impairments


na

will be fundamental for future research aiming to promote prosocial

behaviour and mental health.


ur

Keywords:
Jo

moral emotions; Brodmann’s area 25; social cognition; septal area;

attachment; hypothalamus; subgenual cingulate cortex; anterior cingulate

cortex
3

1. Introduction

In a time of complex and rapidly changing social identities, there is an

urgent need to understand the roots of the sheer altruism of voluntary acts,

as well as the ordinary manifestations of moral behaviour in everyday life.

Significant advances have been made in identifying the key neural systems

responsible for moral cognition and behaviour in humans in general since

of
our review over a decade ago(Moll et al., 2005). Despite the rapidly

ro
expanding field of moral cognitive neuroscience(Fumagalli and Priori,

2012), only recently new research has started to unveil the brain systems
-p
underpinning moral motivations (being motivated to e.g. “help an orphan

child”), that are important for driving moral behaviour (e.g. “donating to a
re
charity that benefits orphan children”, Box 1). In keeping with our view of
lP

moral cognition and emotion as inextricably linked psychologically and

anatomically(Moll et al., 2005), we will argue below that moral motivations

require both cognitive components which specify the goal of one’s action
na

(e.g. “alleviating a child’s hunger”) as well as basic emotional elements (e.g.

“a caring/attached emotional state”) which provide the motivational force


ur

allowing us to overcome competing selfish motivations (e.g. “buying


Jo

something for oneself”). This is in keeping with psychological models of

motivation that postulate closely connected, but distinct roles of (1) goal

representations and (2) the magnitude (i.e. force) of their subjective

emotional value(Wigfield, 1994).

Moral motivations promote both costly helping behaviours and moral

punishment. These motivations are fundamental for driving moral


4

behaviour in the absence of external punishment and reward contingencies,

such as norm enforcement, reputation and reciprocity concerns(Gintis et

al., 2008; Nowak and Sigmund, 2005). Impaired moral motivations are

typically illustrated by the behaviour of psychopathic individuals, who can

play the moral rules when self-interest is at stake but fail to do so otherwise

(Cleckley, 1976) , a similar pattern of behaviour is observed in healthy

people with psychopathic traits (Lockwood et al., 2017).

Over the last decade, novel evidence on the neural basis of moral

of
motivations has emerged, and as we will show in this review, has

consistently implicated a set of basal forebrain structures – namely the

ro
septo-hypothalamic and subgenual frontal areas. These structures are closely

-p
connected anatomically (Box 2). In this review, we will integrate this recent

evidence for the first time to elucidate the role of these regions in moral
re
motivation, for which we only had scarce evidence in our review over a

decade ago(Moll et al., 2008). We will discuss the contribution of the


lP

subcortical septo-hypothalamic area to kinship bonding across species (Moll

et al., 2012; Preston, 2013; Stack et al., 2002) and the role of the subgenual
na

area in more complex aspects of moral motivation such as social agency,

and the value of social outcomes (Moll et al., 2008; Moll et al., 2006).
ur

Crucially, these brain regions are functionally and anatomically interlocked

with those responsible for general reinforcement and social cognition (Moll
Jo

et al., 2005; Preston, 2013). Moreover, recent studies addressing moral

emotions and motivation bear direct relevance for a better understanding of

neuropsychiatric disorders, with potential clinical applications (Decety et al.,

2013; Green et al., 2012; Harrison et al., 2012).


5

After defining moral motivation, we will review the evidence for

impaired moral behaviour as observed in patient lesion studies. This is

because, in contrast to functional neuroimaging in healthy individuals,

lesion studies are able to identify brain regions that are necessary for a

specific function(Price et al., 1999). We will then review functional

neuroimaging studies in healthy individuals with direct relevance for the

understanding of the role of the basal forebrain in moral motivations.

Finally, we will discuss implications for the fields of neuropsychiatry and

of
psychological wellbeing research.

ro
2. What are moral motivations?
-p
Morality can be operationally defined as the sets of customs and values

that are embraced by a sociocultural group to guide social behaviour(Moll et


re
al., 2005). This definition, however, does not specify the motivations that
lP

drive these actions(Moll et al., 2008). Only when considering the

motivations behind an action, are we able to delineate the cognitive and

emotional components that are specific for moral behaviour as compared


na

with social behaviour in general. Thus, moral motivation can be more

sharply defined as being driven by the needs of other people, including kin,
ur

or society(Zahn et al., 2011b). This definition allows for individual and


Jo

cultural variability but excludes other adaptive types of social motivations

(e.g., abiding by social norms to avoid reputation loss, helping others to

obtain social recognition or to uphold any form of self-regarding

preferences). We will use the term “prosocial” behaviour as an umbrella

term to describe socially adaptive behaviours when their true motivation

cannot be determined.
6

The term “moral motivation” was given prominence by eighteenth

century philosophers of the Scottish Enlightenment (see Box 1, (Zahn et al.,

2011b)). They highlighted the central importance of moral sentiments for

providing moral motivation. Modern psychologists speak of “moral

emotions” rather than sentiments and generally agree on the importance of

guilt and compassion/sympathy (Eisenberg, 2000; Tangney et al., 2007).

Stressing the subjective and complex nature of moral sentiments which

include cognitive ingredients such as causal attributions, we will refer to

of
them as feelings. We may also use the term emotions synonymously with

feelings or sentiments.

ro
As mentioned above, we conceive of moral motivation as consisting of

-p
both the motivational force and the associated goal. This definition

highlights two key components of moral motivation: the motivational force


re
driving behaviour and the social knowledge about the needs of others and

about sociocultural norms(Moll et al., 2008; Zahn et al., 2007), which


lP

defines the goals entailed in moral motivations.


na

3. The neural architecture of moral motivations

In this section, we will first review studies of patients with brain lesions
ur

and impaired moral behaviour before summarising evidence from fMRI in

healthy volunteers and in psychiatric conditions. We will focus on the


Jo

involvement of basal forebrain and the partly overlapping ventromedial

frontal cortex (FC) subregions, in particular on the septal, hypothalamic,

and subgenual regions, which have been more consistently implicated in

studies of moral decisions and feelings. We emphasize, however, that it is

beyond the scope of this review to systematically report all the brain regions
7

implicated in studies relevant to moral motivation and kinship bonding, or

to appraise all the studies reporting activations in the basal forebrain.

Among others, two isocortical brain regions that have been implicated in

abstract conceptual and sequential/prospective aspects of moral goals – the

superior anterior temporal cortex (ATL) and the medial frontopolar cortex,

respectively – will also be briefly discussed due to converging evidence from

lesion and fMRI studies for their importance in moral motivations.

of
3.1. Brain lesions and impaired moral behaviour

By demonstrating which brain regions are necessary for moral and

ro
prosocial behaviour, lesion studies provide important insights, even if they

-p
relate to less confined anatomical territories such as the ventral frontal areas

which contain but are not limited to the subgenual region. The 19 th century
re
physician Leonore Welt provided the first review of patients who exhibited

changes in moral behaviour as a result of a focal brain injury(Welt,


lP

1888),(Zahn et al., 2011a). In her article, she presented the case of a furrier

who underwent a drastic change in character after a head injury with


na

predominant damage to the right frontal lobe. Based on the location of the

injury found at autopsy in her case as well as in 12 others that she collected
ur

from the literature with similar changes in moral character, Welt concluded

that damage to the right medial orbital region was the common
Jo

denominator in all cases(Zahn et al., 2015). In the 1980s, Eslinger &

Damasio stimulated new interest in the neuroanatomy of impaired prosocial

and moral conduct by describing EVR, a patient with a ventromedial FC

injury resulting from a tumour resection(Eslinger and Damasio, 1985). A

few years later, a neurodegenerative disease originally described by Pick in


8

the late 19th century became recognised as a form of early-onset dementia

(frontotemporal dementia, FTD) that could reliably be diagnosed before

death based on its clinical features alone(Snowden et al., 2001). Patients

with FTD consistently displayed impaired prosocial behaviour(Bozeat et al.,

2000), which was independently associated with atrophy of right ATL and

ventromedial FC, including the subgenual region(Liu et al., 2004). Atrophy

of the subgenual region was later shown to be associated with loss of

empathy(Rankin et al., 2006), affiliative feelings and interpersonal

of
warmth(Sollberger et al., 2009). More recent studies have provided evidence

that empathic concern is primarily impaired in FTD(Baez et al., 2014) and

ro
associated with posterior medial orbitofrontal atrophy (Baez et al., 2016)

-p
close to the subgenual cortex. Loss of guilt was also reported by caregivers

(Koenigs et al., 2007), and associated with decreased prosocial decisions in


re
economic games(Krajbich et al., 2009) in patients with ventromedial FC

lesions that included subgenual sectors of the orbitofrontal cortex.


lP

Converging evidence from fMRI and patients with FTD linked the

ventromedial FC to the anticipation of negative consequences of social


na

behaviour(Grossman et al., 2010). Less evidence is available on the

behavioural effects of the subcortical parts of the basal forebrain in


ur

humans, but hypothalamic lesions were associated with aggressive

behaviour(Haugh and Markesbery, 1983; Weissenberger et al., 2001),


Jo

although endocrine disturbances may have played a role.

Further studies have begun to probe moral motivations experimentally

in FTD whilst controlling for social conceptual knowledge and vice-

versa(Zahn et al., 2009b),(Moll et al., 2011). These studies indicate that loss

of simple interpersonal moral motivations and loss of abstract conceptual


9

social knowledge (e.g., what it means to act “tactfully”) are partly

dissociable (Box 3). Septal damage in FTD was associated with diminished

guilt and pity, but not embarrassment in an experimental task, whilst

frontopolar damage was associated with impaired embarrassment in

addition to guilt and pity(Moll et al., 2011). This showed that septal damage

was associated with impairments of those moral feelings that entail

empathic concern for other people, whilst frontopolar cortical damage was

associated with prosocial feelings more generally, including embarrassment

of
which is primarily related to upholding one’s social reputation rather than

concern for others(Eisenberg, 2000). The experimental task controlled for

ro
other negative emotions such as anger and disgust, whose impairment was

-p
associated with amygdala and dorsomedial FC damage(Moll et al., 2011),

although it is outside of the scope of this review to consider studies of


re
disgust and anger. These selective patterns occurred despite comparable

degrees of social knowledge being required to understand the stimuli used


lP

in the task. In contrast to these associations of different moral feelings with

different frontal-subcortical lesion patterns, another study showed that


na

FTD patients with right ATL damage displayed selective impairments of

abstract conceptual social relative to non-social conceptual knowledge(Zahn


ur

et al., 2009b). These lesion studies confirmed earlier fMRI evidence of

partly dissociable representations of abstract conceptual social knowledge in


Jo

the right superior ATL(Zahn et al., 2007) and different moral feelings in

frontal-subcortical regions(Zahn et al., 2009c), which can independently

contribute to impaired prosocial behaviour(Krajbich et al., 2009; Liu et al.,

2004). These results are further consistent with the hypothesis that frontal

and subcortical basal forebrain regions are necessary for core aspects of
10

moral motivations, whereas ATL regions are relevant for representing

conceptual aspects of abstract moral values which can serve as goals of

complex societal value-based moral motivations (Box 3). This interpretation

is based on the hypothesis that moral motivations in direct interpersonal

interactions such as those driven by the perception of a sad facial

expression of someone in need(Moll et al., 2005) for example, do not

necessarily require more complex moral goal representations such as

abstract conceptual goals (ATL).

of
Taken together, lesions in the hypothalamus, the septal region and

ventromedial parts of the frontal cortex, especially those including the

ro
subgenual cortex (BA25) and the more anterior subgenual cingulate and

-p
subgenual orbitofrontal areas, led to changes in moral behaviour which

would be compatible with their proposed critical role for moral motivation.
re
Lesions to other cortical brain regions which were shown to represent goals

of socio-moral behaviour, such as the long-term consequences (frontopolar


lP

cortex, (Wood and Grafman, 2003; Zahn et al., 2017)) and conceptual

quality of social behaviour (right superior ATL, (Zahn et al., 2009b)) led to
na

changes in moral behaviour as well (Zahn et al., 2017) in keeping with the

notion that motivations rely both on the motivational force as well as the
ur

goal representation (for further lesion evidence on the role of isocortical

frontal regions in moral motivation, see Box 4).


Jo

In contrast, lesions to the occipital and parietal cortices, posterior

inferior temporal, as well as posterior left dorsolateral, posterior

dorsomedial frontal and dorsal anterior cingulate cortices are usually not

clearly associated with observable inappropriate social behaviour(Moll et al.,

2005). This does not preclude an important role of these regions in socio-
11

emotional functions more generally which is beyond the scope of this

review. For example, the dorsal anterior cingulate region is activated for

empathic simulation (Lockwood, 2016), such as sharing others’ pain(Lamm

et al., 2011), with evidence for gyral versus sulcal functional

subdivisions(Apps et al., 2016). Patients with anterior cerebral artery stroke

regularly including the more posterior portions of the dorsal anterior

cingulate develop the syndrome of “akinetic mutism” with a general lack of

spontaneous activity and speech(Nagaratnam et al., 2004), but little

of
evidence of selective social impairments. It is also important to note that

most ventromedial frontal subregions are necessary for prosocial behaviour,

ro
such as strategic cooperation, more generally (Melloni et al., 2016) rather

than selective for moral motivation. -p


Schadenfreude and envy were outside of the scope of this review as they are
re
usually considered as social rather than moral feelings (Jankowski and Takahashi,

2014). A recent study in patients with neurodegenerative disorders, however,


lP

specifically examined these emotions in moral situations (Santamaria-Garcia et al.,

2017). Interestingly, Schadenfreude in moral situations was higher in patients with


na

frontopolar and angular gyrus atrophy and envy in moral situations was more

pronounced in patients with precuneus atrophy. Whilst interesting, it is difficult to


ur

ascertain which cognitive components of these experiences led to the described

associations.
Jo

3.2. Moral vs. self-interested choices

It is well established that patients showing impaired prosocial

behaviour are often able to serve their own immediate self-interests

(Eslinger and Damasio, 1985). This indicates the existence of at least partly
12

separable neural correlates of moral and selfish motivations. The studies

discussed so far, however, have not made this direct comparison, which is

necessary to draw conclusions about selectivity.

Studies of human cooperation in economic games have paved the way

for investigating the neural basis of complex altruistic behaviour (Figure 2).

Using an economic game format, a charity donation paradigm was employed

to determine areas of selective activation for altruistic decisions, compared

with decisions associated with pure monetary gain for oneself(Moll et al.,

of
2006). The septal and subgenual cortex and the anterior medial

orbitofrontal/frontopolar cortex showed selectivity for altruistic decisions,

ro
the latter area being more active during costly choices. Moreover, septal

-p
activations were also detected when pairs of participants relied on

unconditional trust in their decisions to cooperate, in contrast to ventral


re
tegmental area activity during strategic cooperation (i.e., expectancy of

mutual gains from cooperation)(Krueger et al., 2007a). The engagement of


lP

the septal area in donation behaviour has also been confirmed in more

recent studies(Harbaugh et al., 2007; Hsu et al., 2008). Importantly, septal


na

activity was observed when healthy subjects observed sad faces when taking

a compassionate perspective(Kim et al., 2009), and septal responses


ur

specifically predicted daily helping in another fMRI study(Morelli et al.,

2014).
Jo

Another neuroeconomic fMRI study demonstrated that activity in septal

and anterior subgenual orbitofrontal areas tracked the levels of reported

guilt associated with dishonouring trust(Chang et al., 2011). The anterior

subgenual orbitofrontal region was also shown to respond to voluntary

contributions in a public goods game more strongly when identical


13

outcomes were interpreted as emerging from a generous intent than when

they were not (Cooper et al., 2010). Furthermore, activation in the

subgenual orbitofrontal region was shown for upholding a principle of

social equality in costly decisions(Zaki and Mitchell, 2011).

The selectivity of subgenual areas for altruistic compared with selfish

decisions was further corroborated in that individuals with higher empathic

concern displayed stronger subgenual cingulate activation when deciding to

sacrifice their money to prevent others from receiving an electric

of
shock(FeldmanHall et al., 2012) (FeldmanHall et al., 2015). Moreover,

anterior subgenual cingulate activity tracked the amounts donated in a

ro
charitable donation task(Hare et al., 2010) and correlated with individual

-p
differences in aversion to third-party harm(Wiech et al., 2013). A meta-

analysis comparing selfish and vicarious reward fMRI studies found,


re
however, that right anterior subgenual cingulate activations (BA24) were

both found consistently in selfish and vicarious reward studies vs. low level
lP

baseline conditions(Morelli et al., 2015). This meta-analysis did not identify

more posterior subgenual, septal or hypothalamic activations consistently


na

for vicarious rewards as one might have expected on the basis of our review

of the literature. This is not surprising, however, given that quantitative


ur

imaging meta-analyses rely on peak coordinates reported to be significant in

the cited studies and this will bias the results against basal forebrain areas
Jo

which are only reliably detected using optimized fMRI parameters to

improve the otherwise high rate of signal dropout due to the vicinity of the

skull and sinuses(Wastling and Barker, 2015) and by including specific a

priori regions of interest to improve the statistical power of detecting

activations in small brain regions. This could explain why a more recent
14

meta-analysis, which modelled signal dropout, was able to detect selective

activations in the subgenual cortex (BA25) for altruistic vs. selfish decisions

(Cutler and Campbell-Meiklejohn, 2019). Another difference between both

meta-analyses is that Morelli et al. included vicarious rewards in a non-

moral context, whereas Cutler and Campbell-Meiklejohn specifically

investigated differences between moral and selfish motivations.

More recent studies have investigated moral motivation-related brain

responses using computational models. Prosocial prediction errors were

of
specifically encoded within the posterior subgenual cortex and adjacent

septal area employing reinforcement learning models(Lockwood et al.,

ro
2016). In contrast, non-overlapping responses in the ventral striatum

-p
tracked both selfish and prosocial reward prediction errors(Lockwood et al.,

2016). Interestingly, individuals prone to empathise had a higher learning


re
rate in the prosocial condition and higher subgenual activity for prosocial

vs. self-reward prediction errors. Another computational modelling study


lP

measured care and distress levels to narratives of human suffering (Ashar et

al., 2017), which predicted subsequent charitable donation. Empathic care


na

was preferentially associated with septal activity.

Whilst these computational modelling-based studies are in agreement


ur

with the proposed role of basal forebrain areas in moral motivation, a

recent study failed to show selective brain activations for altruistic choices
Jo

to lessen others’ pain by sacrificing monetary reward vs. selfish choices

(Crockett et al., 2017). Instead a large cluster of ventromedial frontal

activation, strongest in the pregenual area, was common to selfish and

altruistic choices; the cluster spread into the subgenual frontal area. This

lack of selective activation, however, may well be due to using cluster-based


15

inference at the whole brain level, thus decreasing the likelihood of

detecting activations in small basal forebrain and subgenual subregions.

Another recent study failing to support the selective importance of

ventromedial frontal areas for altruistic choices was conducted in Vietnam

Veterans with penetrating head injuries(Moll et al., 2018). The study,

however, had limited value in assessing these regions, because the number

of veterans with such lesions was very low. Further limitations of this study

were that MRI scans could not be carried out due to metal injuries, and so

of
reliance on CT scans limited the precision of lesion mapping. In addition,

time post-lesion of around 40 years was exceptionally long, which meant

ro
that compensation as well as ageing-related and neurodegenerative changes

-p
were likely to have added variability to CT-detectable lesion-function

associations.
re
lP
na
ur
Jo
16

3.3. Imaging the experience of moral feelings

The investigation of the neural correlates of subjective experiences of

moral feelings is important for our understanding of moral motivation. This

is because, the previous section showed that while neuroeconomic

paradigms can be used to infer neural correlates of moral motivations as

long as appropriate control conditions are used, they usually fail to reveal

the qualities of underlying emotional experience. For example, one may

decide to sacrifice money to benefit a social cause based on either guilt

of
avoidance or on the positive feelings derived from such action (e.g. “warm

glow”; (Andreoni, 1990)).

ro
In this section, we review research on moral feelings with a focus on

-p
guilt and pity/compassion, given their consistent associations with altruistic

behaviour. We conceive of moral feelings as the subjective experience of


re
moral motivations. Experimentally, however, fMRI studies of moral feelings

often suffer from not being able to directly show which of the activated
lP

brain regions are related to motivating moral behaviour and which regions

are correlates of the experience without being relevant for motivating


na

behaviour. This is why fMRI studies of moral feelings can only be

interpreted in conjunction with the evidence provided in the previous


ur

sections.

The anticipation of guilt is important in preventing us from immoral


Jo

actions and to motivate reparative action(Eisenberg, 2000; Tangney et al.,

2007). Empathic concern—a core component of empathy—is closely related

to pity, compassion and sympathy, which promote altruistic helping(Weng

et al., 2015). Such feelings extend beyond perceiving, sharing or simulating

other's emotions, requiring an extra step of feeling for the other


17

person(Decety et al., 2012). Frontopolar activations emerge as most

reproducible for both guilt(Moll et al., 2007; Zahn et al., 2009c),(Basile et

al., 2011; Kedia et al., 2008; Morey et al., 2012; Seara-Cardoso et al., 2016;

Takahashi et al., 2004) and compassion(Immordino-Yang et al., 2009),(Moll

et al., 2007),(Fehse et al., 2015; Kedia et al., 2008) compared against equally

unpleasant and complex emotions, such as indignation towards others. In

addition, guilt was reproducibly associated with activations of the subgenual

cingulate cortex (extending posteriorly to the adjacent septal area and the

of
more anterior pregenual cingulate area in several studies) when compared

with other complex negative emotions(Zahn et al., 2009a),(Green et al.,

ro
2012; Zahn et al., 2009c),(Basile et al., 2011; Morey et al., 2012). Septal

-p
and/or subgenual cingulate activations for guilt were found in several

studies, however, only when modelling individual differences in guilt


re
proneness and empathic concern(Zahn et al., 2009a),(Green et al., 2012;

Zahn et al., 2009c). Interestingly, a study on counterintuitive utilitarian


lP

judgments found that reduced anterior subgenual cingulate cortex activity

undergirded an increased inclination to accept harmful actions towards a


na

third party, suggesting reduced empathic concern instead of increased

explicit moral deliberation(Wiech et al., 2013).


ur

Despite these reproducible associations of subgenual cingulate and

septal activations with individual differences in guilt-proneness and


Jo

empathic concern, two recent systematic reviews of fMRI studies probing

guilt(Gifuni et al., 2016) (Bastin et al., 2016) have failed to detect these

regions. Because these reviews did not base their conclusions on studies

controlling for individual differences in the experience of guilt-evoking

stimuli, and on those studies using optimised fMRI sequences for ventral
18

frontal regions, it is not surprising that subgenual cingulate/septal

activations were not given prominence and this will be important in future

systematic reviews.

3.4. Social attachment and moral motivation

Being able to feel attached to others is a basic ingredient for moral

feelings such as compassion and guilt (Moll and Schulkin, 2009; Moll et al.,

2011) (Preston, 2013). Specific brain structures have been critically

of
implicated in social attachment mechanisms in non-human animals,

including pair bonding and bonding between mother and offspring(Insel

ro
and Young, 2001; Stack et al., 2002; Swain et al., 2012). These brain

-p
structures include the preoptic-anterior hypothalamic area, septal nuclei and

associated basal forebrain regions (Stack et al., 2002). These areas host
re
abundant receptors for neuropeptides (oxytocin, vasopressin), monoamines

(dopamine) and opioids that have been shown to influence attachment-


lP

related behaviours (Depue and Morrone-Strupinsky, 2005; Insel and Young,

2001; Stack et al., 2002). Septal damage in rodents disrupts maternal


na

caregiving (Febo et al., 2005).

Human fMRI investigations of maternal and romantic love reported


ur

remarkably overlapping activations in these regions(Aron et al., 2005;

Bartels and Zeki, 2004; Swain et al., 2007), and oxytocin receptor
Jo

polymorphisms and prosocial temperament were shown to be associated

with individual differences in hypothalamic volume and function(Tost et al.,

2010). Until recently, an open question was whether brain activation

associated with attachment-related experiences could be dissociated from

that associated with general positive or negative emotions. A recent study


19

employed passive presentation of social narratives involving kin (i.e.,

associated with affiliative states) or not involving kinship(Moll et al., 2012).

The results confirmed the prediction that the septo-hypothalamic region

was selectively activated for affiliative emotional states, while controlling

for positive or negative emotional valence and for the context of agency.

Interestingly, subgenual cingulate cortex activation was only detected when

modelling individual differences in how strongly participants perceived their

own families as a distinctive social group in affiliative social

of
scenarios(Rusch et al., 2014). A recent study confirmed the role of

subgenual frontal areas in distinguishing between in- and outgroups by

ro
showing that the subgenual cortex was selectively activated for efforts

-p
benefitting anonymous fellow fans of one’s soccer club compared with

playing to benefit non-fans(Bortolini et al., 2017).


re
Basal forebrain regions were also found to be activated by witnessing the

delivery of rewards to similar others (“vicarious rewards”(Mobbs et al.,


lP

2009)). In this study, watching another player, that one could identify with,

receive rewards led to activation of the ventral striatum and adjoining


na

septo-hypothalamic area. Interestingly, when correlating with the perceived

degree of similarity of shared values (a more complex construct), higher


ur

activity was observed in the subgenual frontal cortex.


Jo

3.5. Summary of evidence on the basal forebrain’s role

Taken together, this body of evidence suggests a specific neural basis

for moral motivations and associated feelings, which drive altruistic choices

towards other individuals or groups including kin. The emerging picture

indicates that the septo-hypothalamic and subgenual frontal areas may play
20

distinct but interlocking roles in moral motivation.

 The septo-hypothalamic region, given its anatomical

organisation as well as the evidence discussed above, appears to

be more relevant for representing basic components of moral

motivation such as interpersonal affiliation and social bonding.

 In contrast, the subgenual frontal areas are likely to represent

more dynamic and complex aspects of moral motivation, such as

social agency, and the value of social outcomes, as well as

of
perceived group belongingness (see also Figure 1).

ro
4. Moral motivation in psychiatry: emerging applications
-p
The application of insights on the neural architecture of moral

motivation to the study of psychiatric disorders is still in its infancy(Zahn et


re
al., 2015). These insights are likely to become increasingly important in

understanding individual differences in vulnerability to specific psychiatric


lP

disorders. Here, our focus is on psychopathy and major depressive disorder

(MDD) as two examples of conditions associated with moral motivation


na

disturbances.

Psychopathy has classically been described as a disorder of moral


ur

motivation, being specifically associated with an inability to experience guilt

and pity, with a spared ability to understand and manipulate others for
Jo

selfish purposes(Cleckley, 1976). Callousness in patients with

psychopathy(Hare, 2003) correlates with diminished grey matter volume in

the frontopolar and subgenual orbitofrontal areas(de Oliveira-Souza et al.,

2008). Another study revealed that psychopaths show diminished subgenual

cingulate activation to moral violation-depicting pictures(Harenski and


21

Hamann, 2006) and during empathy for pain(Decety et al., 2013). An

association of neurodevelopmental changes in the septum pellucidum with

antisocial behaviour and psychopathy was reported(Raine et al., 2010), in

keeping with a purported role of the septal nuclei for moral behaviour.

Psychopathy has also been reproducibly associated with abnormal fMRI

activation (Harenski and Hamann, 2006; Kiehl et al., 2004) and structure of

the right ATL(Gregory S and et al., 2012; Muller et al., 2008) and

frontopolar cortex(Gregory S and et al., 2012), as well as the right uncinate

of
fasciculus(Craig et al., 2009) connecting anterior temporal and subgenual

areas. This suggests, that psychopathy is not only associated with basal

ro
forebrain dysfunction, but also with isocortical representations of more

complex moral goals. -p


MDD is associated with overgeneralised forms of empathy-based
re
interpersonal guilt(O'Connor et al., 2002) and self-contempt/disgust(Green

et al., 2013), which remain detectable on remission of symptoms(Green et


lP

al., 2013). This indicates that self-blaming emotional biases are a trait

vulnerability factor for MDD as postulated by cognitive models(Abramson


na

et al., 1978). The importance of subgenual FC dysfunction in MDD has

been well established(Price and Drevets, 2010; Ressler and Mayberg, 2007).
ur

Accordingly, overgeneralization of guilt (i.e. feeling guilty for everything

and thus hating oneself as a result) correlated with functional connectivity


Jo

changes between a subgenual cingulate region and the right superior ATL

whilst patients with remitted MDD experienced self-blame in the

scanner(Green et al., 2012), and predicted risk of subsequent

recurrence(Lythe et al., 2015). Another study of moral motivations in MDD

employed a charity donation paradigm in remitted patients during fMRI to


22

show that subgenual cingulate activation was higher whilst sacrificing

money to donate compared with control participants(Pulcu et al., 2014),

despite no overall increase in donations made during the experiment.

Further studies of the neural correlates of moral feelings and altruistic

behaviour in psychopathy and MDD are needed to confirm and extend

these findings.

Promoting prosocial behaviours is a topic of wide interest and concern.

There is some initial evidence pointing to ways on how to boost moral

of
motivation and its neural underpinnings using psychological and

neurotechnological approaches. For example, a recent controlled study

ro
using mindfulness meditation demonstrated that activity in the septal area

-p
was selectively associated with increased cooperation in the Ultimatum

game. Increased cooperation was also associated with increased functional


re
connectivity between the septal area and insula (Kirk et al., 2016).

Technological advances in real-time fMRI-based neurofeedback also


lP

promise exciting opportunities for testing neuroscience-informed models

and for translating them into novel treatments(Kim and Birbaumer, 2014;
na

Sulzer et al., 2013). fMRI neurofeedback provides individuals access to

information about their current brain activity that is usually outside of their
ur

awareness. A recent study has demonstrated that healthy participants can

voluntarily modulate neural correlates of complex moral feelings by


Jo

increasing activation of septo-hypothalamic and frontopolar areas whilst

experiencing affection(Moll et al., 2014). The possibility of enhancing

human capacities has become a hot topic of debate within the scientific,

educational and social policy communities during the last two

decades(Hyman, 2011).
23

5. Future perspectives and conclusion

Research on moral cognitive neuroscience during the last decade has

led to a striking advance in our understanding of human social behaviour,

bringing to the neurobiological arena a topic that belonged exclusively to

the humanities and the psychological sciences. We have discussed evidence

indicating that the subgenual frontal and septo-hypothalamic area are

currently the best candidates for hosting subregions with selective

of
involvement in moral vs. selfish motivations. Future studies are needed to

address the question of a plausible anterior-posterior gradient in complexity

ro
within the septal and subgenual frontal continuum analogous to a similar

-p
proposal for hedonic reward value representations in the medial

orbitofrontal cortex (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2004). It also remains to be


re
investigated whether the anterior subgenual cingulate regions code for

causal social agency due to their closer connection with pregenual anterior
lP

cingulate representations shown to correlate with subjective feelings of

motor agency (Marchesotti et al., 2017) and emerging evidence for


na

functional subdivisions between pregenual and subgenual areas in social

learning(Lockwood and Wittmann, 2018). A primate pharmacological


ur

inactivation study further supports functional subdivisions between the

subgenual cortex (BA25) and more anterior subgenual cingulate areas


Jo

(BA32) as measured by autonomic nervous system responses to aversive

stimuli (Wallis et al., 2017). Inactivation of BA25 reduced autonomic

nervous system correlates of aversive stimuli, whereas BA32 inactivation led

to overgeneralised responses. This would be compatible with more complex

and contextualised information being represented in anterior subgenual


24

cingulate areas which guard against overgeneralisation. Future studies are

needed to probe whether subregions within the subgenual cortex (BA25)

represent attachment-related values of social outcomes analogous to the

proposed role of the medial orbitofrontal cortex in representing hedonic

reward value (Kringelbach and Rolls, 2004). An exciting future of applying

novel neuroscience insights to an improved pathophysiological

understanding and treatment of psychiatric disorders and societal

functioning lies ahead.

of
Acknowledgements

ro
RZ was partly funded by the UK MRC (G09023042) and US Brain & Behavior

-p
Research Foundation (24715). ROS and JM were funded by IDOR. The authors

are thankful for discussions with Ivanei Bramati, Fernanda Tovar-Moll, Leonardo
re
Fontenelle, Yotam Heineberg and João Ascenso.
lP
na
ur
Jo
25

References
Abramson, L.Y., Seligman, M.E.P., Teasdale, J.D., 1978. LEARNED
HELPLESSNESS IN HUMANS - CRITIQUE AND REFORMULATION. J
Abn. Psychol. 87, 49-74.
Allison, T., Puce, A., McCarthy, G., 2000. Social perception from visual cues: role of
the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4, 267-278.
Andreoni, J., 1990. Impure altruism and donations to public good: a theory of warm
glow giving. The Economic Journal 100, 464-477.
Apps, Matthew A.J., Rushworth, Matthew F.S., Chang, Steve W.C., 2016. The Anterior
Cingulate Gyrus and Social Cognition: Tracking the Motivation of Others.
Neuron 90, 692-707.
Aron, A., Fisher, H., Mashek, D.J., Strong, G., Li, H., Brown, L.L., 2005. Reward,
Motivation, and Emotion Systems Associated With Early-Stage Intense

of
Romantic Love. J Neurophysiol 94, 327-337.
Ashar, Y.K., Andrews-Hanna, J.R., Dimidjian, S., Wager, T.D., 2017. Empathic Care
and Distress: Predictive Brain Markers and Dissociable Brain Systems. Neuron

ro
94, 1263-1273 e1264.
Bado, P., Engel, A., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I.E., Paiva, F.F., Basilio, R., Sato,
J.R., Tovar-Moll, F., Moll, J., 2014. Functional dissociation of ventral frontal
-p
and dorsomedial default mode network components during resting state and
emotional autobiographical recall. Hum Brain Mapp 35, 3302-3313.
re
Baez, S., Manes, F., Huepe, D., Torralva, T., Fiorentino, N., Richter, F., Huepe-Artigas,
D., Ferrari, J., Montañes, P., Reyes, P., Matallana, D., Vigliecca, N.S., Decety,
J., Ibanez, A., 2014. Primary empathy deficits in frontotemporal dementia.
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience 6.
lP

Baez, S., Morales, J.P., Slachevsky, A., Torralva, T., Matus, C., Manes, F., Ibanez, A.,
2016. Orbitofrontal and limbic signatures of empathic concern and intentional
harm in the behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Cortex 75, 20-32.
na

Bartels, A., Zeki, S., 2004. The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love.
Neuroimage 21, 1155-1166.
Basile, B., Mancini, F., Macaluso, E., Caltagirone, C., Frackowiak, R.S.J., Bozzali, M.,
2011. Deontological and Altruistic Guilt: Evidence for Distinct Neurobiological
ur

Substrates. Hum Brain Mapp 32, 229-239.


Bastin, C., Harrison, B.J., Davey, C.G., Moll, J., Whittle, S., 2016. Feelings of shame,
embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: A systematic review.
Jo

Neurosci Biobehav Rev 71, 455-471.


Bishop, J.D., 1996. Moral motivation and the development of Francis Hutcheson's
philosophy. Journal of the History of Ideas 57, 277-295.
Bortolini, T., Bado, P., Hoefle, S., Engel, A., Zahn, R., de Oliveira Souza, R., Dreher,
J.C., Moll, J., 2017. Neural bases of ingroup altruistic motivation in soccer fans.
Sci Rep 7, 16122.
Bozeat, S., Gregory, C.A., Ralph, M.A., Hodges, J.R., 2000. Which neuropsychiatric
and behavioural features distinguish frontal and temporal variants of
26

frontotemporal dementia from Alzheimer's disease? J Neurol Neurosurg


Psychiatry 69, 178-186.
Casebeer, W.D., 2003. Moral cognition and its neural constituents. Nat Rev Neurosci 4,
840-846.
Chang, L.J., Smith, A., Dufwenberg, M., Sanfey, A.G., 2011. Triangulating the Neural,
Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion. Neuron 70, 560-572.
Cleckley, H.M., 1976. The mask of sanity, 5th ed. Mosby, St. Louis, MO.
Cooper, J.C., Kreps, T.A., Wiebe, T., Pirkl, T., Knutson, B., 2010. When Giving Is
Good: Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Activation for Others' Intentions. Neuron
67, 511-521.
Coricelli, G., Critchley, H.D., Joffily, M., O'Doherty, J.P., Sirigu, A., Dolan, R.J., 2005.
Regret and its avoidance: a neuroimaging study of choice behavior. Nature
Neuroscience 8, 1255-1262.

of
Craig, M.C., Catani, M., Deeley, Q., Latham, R., Daly, E., Kanaan, R., Picchioni, M.,
McGuire, P.K., Fahy, T., Murphy, D.G., 2009. Altered connections on the road
to psychopathy. Mol Psychiatry 14, 946-953, 907.

ro
Crockett, M.J., Siegel, J.Z., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Dayan, P., Dolan, R.J., 2017. Moral
transgressions corrupt neural representations of value. Nature Neuroscience 20,
879-+.
-p
Cutler, J., Campbell-Meiklejohn, D., 2019. A comparative fMRI meta-analysis of
altruistic and strategic decisions to give. NeuroImage 184, 227-241.
re
de Oliveira-Souza, R., Hare, R.D., Bramati, I.E., Garrido, G.J., Azevedo Ignácio, F.,
Tovar-Moll, F., Moll, J., 2008. Psychopathy as a disorder of the moral brain:
Fronto-temporo-limbic grey matter reductions demonstrated by voxel-based
lP

morphometry. NeuroImage 40, 1202-1213.


Decety, J., Grezes, J., 2006. The power of simulation: Imagining one's own and other's
behavior. Brain Res 1079, 4-14.
Decety, J., Norman, G.J., Berntson, G.G., Cacioppo, J.T., 2012. A neurobehavioral
na

evolutionary perspective on the mechanisms underlying empathy. Prog


Neurobiol 98, 38-48.
Decety, J., Skelly, L.R., Kiehl, K.A., 2013. Brain response to empathy-eliciting
scenarios involving pain in incarcerated individuals with psychopathy. JAMA
ur

psychiatry 70, 638-645.


Delgado, M.R., Frank, R.H., Phelps, E.A., 2005. Perceptions of moral character
Jo

modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Nat Neurosci 8,
1611-1618.
Depue, R.A., Morrone-Strupinsky, J.V., 2005. A neurobehavioral model of affiliative
bonding: implications for conceptualizing a human trait of affiliation. The
Behavioral and brain sciences 28, 313-350; discussion 350-395.
Eisenberg, N., 2000. Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annu Rev Psychol
51, 665-697.
Eslinger, P.J., Damasio, A.R., 1985. Severe Disturbance of Higher Cognition after
Bilateral Frontal-Lobe Ablation - Patient EVR. Neurology 35, 1731-1741.
27

Febo, M., Numan, M., Ferris, C.F., 2005. Functional magnetic resonance imaging
shows oxytocin activates brain regions associated with mother-pup bonding
during suckling. Journal of Neuroscience 25, 11637-11644.
Fehse, K., Silveira, S., Elvers, K., Blautzik, J., 2015. Compassion, guilt and innocence:
An fMRI study of responses to victims who are responsible for their fate. Social
Neuroscience 10, 243-252.
FeldmanHall, O., Dalgleish, T., Evans, D., Mobbs, D., 2015. Empathic concern drives
costly altruism. Neuroimage 105, 347-356.
FeldmanHall, O., Dalgleish, T., Thompson, R., Evans, D., Schweizer, S., Mobbs, D.,
2012. Differential neural circuitry and self-interest in real vs hypothetical moral
decisions. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience 7, 743-751.
Fumagalli, M., Priori, A., 2012. Functional and clinical neuroanatomy of morality.
Brain 135, 2006-2021.

of
Gifuni, A.J., Kendal, A., Jollant, F., 2016. Neural mapping of guilt: a quantitative meta-
analysis of functional imaging studies. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 1-15.
Gintis, H., Henrich, J., Bowles, S., Boyd, R., Fehr, E., 2008. Strong reciprocity and the

ro
roots of human morality. Social Justice Research 21, 241-253.
Green, S., Moll, J., Deakin, J.F., Hulleman, J., Zahn, R., 2013. Proneness to Decreased
Negative Emotions in Major Depressive Disorder when Blaming Others rather
than Oneself. Psychopathology 46, 34-44.
-p
Green, S., Ralph, M.A.L., Moll, J., Deakin, J.F.W., Zahn, R., 2012. Guilt-Selective
Functional Disconnection of Anterior Temporal and Subgenual Cortices in
re
Major Depressive Disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69, 1014-1021.
Gregory S, f.D.S.A., et al., 2012. The antisocial brain: Psychopathy matters: a structural
lP

mri investigation of antisocial male violent offenders. Archives of General


Psychiatry 69, 962-972.
Grossman, M., Eslinger, P.J., Troiani, V., Anderson, C., Avants, B., Gee, J.C.,
McMillan, C., Massimo, L., Khan, A., Antani, S., 2010. The role of ventral
na

medial prefrontal cortex in social decisions: converging evidence from fMRI and
frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neuropsychologia 48, 3505-3512.
Harbaugh, W.T., Mayr, U., Burghart, D.R., 2007. Neural responses to taxation and
voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science 316, 1622-
ur

1625.
Hare, R.D., 2003. The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised, 2nd ed. Multi-Health
Systems, Toronto.
Jo

Hare, T.A., Camerer, C.F., Knoepfle, D.T., Rangel, A., 2010. Value Computations in
Ventral Medial Prefrontal Cortex during Charitable Decision Making
Incorporate Input from Regions Involved in Social Cognition. Journal of
Neuroscience 30, 583-590.
Hare, T.A., Camerer, C.F., Rangel, A., 2009. Self-Control in Decision-Making Involves
Modulation of the vmPFC Valuation System. Science 324, 646-648.
Harenski, C.L., Hamann, S., 2006. Neural correlates of regulating negative emotions
related to moral violations. Neuroimage 30, 313-324.
28

Harris, S., 2011. The moral landscape: How science can determine human values. .
Simon and Schuster.
Harrison, B.J., Pujol, J., Soriano-Mas, C., Hernandez-Ribas, R., Lopez-Sola, M., Ortiz,
H., Alonso, P., Deus, J., Menchon, J.M., Real, E., Segalas, C., Contreras-
Rodriguez, O., Blanco-Hinojo, L., Cardoner, N., 2012. Neural correlates of
moral sensitivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69,
741-749.
Haugh, R.M., Markesbery, W.R., 1983. Hypothalamic astrocytoma. Syndrome of
hyperphagia, obesity, and disturbances of behavior and endocrine and autonomic
function. Arch Neurol 40, 560-563.
Hayashi, T., Ko, J.H., Strafella, A.P., Dagher, A., 2013. Dorsolateral prefrontal and
orbitofrontal cortex interactions during self-control of cigarette craving.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America 110, 4422-4427.

of
Hsu, M., Anen, C., Quartz, S.R., 2008. The right and the good: distributive justice and
neural encoding of equity and efficiency. Science 320, 1092-1095.

ro
Hyman, S.E., 2011. Cognitive enhancement: promises and perils. Neuron 69, 595-598.
Immordino-Yang, M.H., McColl, A., Damasio, H., Damasio, A., 2009. Neural
correlates of admiration and compassion. Proceedings of the National Academy
-p
of Sciences of the United States of America 106, 8021-8026.
Insel, T.R., Young, L.J., 2001. The neurobiology of attachment. Nat Rev Neurosci 2,
129-136.
re
Jankowski, K.F., Takahashi, H., 2014. Cognitive neuroscience of social emotions and
implications for psychopathology: Examining embarrassment, guilt, envy, and
schadenfreude. Psychiat Clin Neuros 68, 319-336.
lP

Kahane, G., 2012. On the Wrong Track: Process and Content in Moral Psychology.
Mind Lang 27, 519-545.
Kant, I., 1786. Grundlegung zur Metaphysik der Sitten, 2nd ed. Johann Friedrich
na

Hartknoch, Riga.
Karim, A.A., Schneider, M., Lotze, M., Veit, R., Sauseng, P., Braun, C., Birbaumer, N.,
2010. The Truth about Lying: Inhibition of the Anterior Prefrontal Cortex
Improves Deceptive Behavior. Cerebral Cortex 20, 205-213.
ur

Kedia, G., Berthoz, S., Wessa, M., Hilton, D., Martinot, J.L., 2008. An agent harms a
victim: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study on specific moral
emotions. J Cogn Neurosci 20, 1788-1798.
Jo

Kiehl, K.A., Smith, A.M., Mendrek, A., Forster, B.B., Hare, R.D., Liddle, P.F., 2004.
Temporal lobe abnormalities in semantic processing by criminal psychopaths as
revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Psychiatry research 130,
297-312.
Kim, J.-W., Kim, S.-E., Kim, J.-J., Jeong, B., Park, C.-H., Son, A.R., Song, J.E., Ki,
S.W., 2009. Compassionate attitude towards others’ suffering activates the
mesolimbic neural system. Neuropsychologia 47, 2073-2081.
Kim, S., Birbaumer, N., 2014. Real-time functional MRI neurofeedback: a tool for
psychiatry. Curr. Opin. Psychiatr. 27, 332-336.
29

Kirk, U., Gu, X., Sharp, C., Hula, A., Fonagy, P., Montague, P.R., 2016. Mindfulness
training increases cooperative decision making in economic exchanges:
Evidence from fMRI. NeuroImage 138, 274-283.
Klingler, J., Gloor, P., 1960. The connections of the amygdala and of the anterior
temporal cortex in the human brain. Journal of Comparative Neurology 115,
333-369.
Knoch, D., Schneider, F., Schunk, D., Hohmann, M., Fehr, E., 2009. Disrupting the
prefrontal cortex diminishes the human ability to build a good reputation.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
America 106, 20895-20899.
Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., Damasio,
A., 2007. Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral
judgements. Nature 446, 908-911.
Krajbich, I., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Denburg, N.L., Camerer, C.F., 2009. Economic

of
Games Quantify Diminished Sense of Guilt in Patients with Damage to the
Prefrontal Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 29, 2188-2192.

ro
Kringelbach, M.L., Rolls, E.T., 2004. The functional neuroanatomy of the human
orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Prog
Neurobiol 72, 341-372.
-p
Krueger, F., McCabe, K., Moll, J., Kriegeskorte, N., Zahn, R., Strenziok, M., Heinecke,
A., Grafman, J., 2007a. Neural correlates of trust. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
104, 20084-20089.
re
Krueger, F., Moll, J., Zahn, R., Heinecke, A., Grafman, J., 2007b. Event frequency
modulates the processing of daily life activities in human medial prefrontal
cortex. Cereb Cortex 17, 2346-2353.
lP

Lamb, R.B., 1974. Adam Smith's system: sympathy not self-interest. Journal of the
History of Ideas 35, 671-682.
Lambon Ralph, M.A., Patterson, K., 2008. Generalization and differentiation in
semantic memory: insights from semantic dementia. Annals of the New York
na

Academy of Sciences 1124, 61-76.


Lamm, C., Decety, J., Singer, T., 2011. Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct
neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain.
ur

NeuroImage 54, 2492-2502.


Liu, W., Miller, B.L., Kramer, J.H., Rankin, K., Wyss-Coray, C., Gearhart, R.,
Phengrasamy, L., Weiner, M., Rosen, H.J., 2004. Behavioral disorders in the
Jo

frontal and temporal variants of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 62, 742-


748.
Lockwood, P.L., 2016. The anatomy of empathy: Vicarious experience and disorders of
social cognition. Behavioural brain research 311, 255-266.
Lockwood, P.L., Apps, M.A.J., Valton, V., Viding, E., Roiser, J.P., 2016.
Neurocomputational mechanisms of prosocial learning and links to empathy.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, 9763-9768.
30

Lockwood, P.L., Hamonet, M., Zhang, S.H., Ratnavel, A., Salmony, F.U., Husain, M.,
Apps, M.A.J., 2017. Prosocial apathy for helping others when effort is required.
Nat Hum Behav 1.
Lockwood, P.L., Wittmann, M.K., 2018. Ventral anterior cingulate cortex and social
decision-making. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 92, 187-191.
Loup, F., Tribollet, E., Duboisdauphin, M., Dreifuss, J.J., 1991. Localization of High-
Affinity Binding-Sites for Oxytocin and Vasopressin in the Human Brain - an
Autoradiographic Study. Brain Res 555, 220-232.
Lythe, K.E., Moll, J., Gethin, J.A., Workman, C.I., Green, S., Lambon Ralph, M.A.,
Deakin, J.F., Zahn, R., 2015. Self-blame-Selective Hyperconnectivity Between
Anterior Temporal and Subgenual Cortices and Prediction of Recurrent
Depressive Episodes. JAMA psychiatry 72, 1119-1126.
Marchesotti, S., Martuzzi, R., Schurger, A., Blefari, M.L., del Millán, J.R., Bleuler, H.,
Blanke, O., 2017. Cortical and subcortical mechanisms of brain-machine

of
interfaces. Hum Brain Mapp 38, 2971-2989.
Melloni, M., Billeke, P., Baez, S., Hesse, E., de la Fuente, L., Forno, G., Birba, A.,

ro
García-Cordero, I., Serrano, C., Plastino, A., Slachevsky, A., Huepe, D.,
Sigman, M., Manes, F., García, A.M., Sedeño, L., Ibáñez, A., 2016. Your
perspective and my benefit: multiple lesion models of self-other integration
strategies during social bargaining. Brain 139, 1-19.
-p
Mobbs, D., Yu, R.J., Meyer, M., Passamonti, L., Seymour, B., Calder, A.J., Schweizer,
S., Frith, C.D., Dalgleish, T., 2009. A Key Role for Similarity in Vicarious
re
Reward. Science 324, 900-900.
Moll, J., Bado, P., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I.E., Lima, D.O., Paiva, F.F., Sato,
J.R., Tovar-Moll, F., Zahn, R., 2012. A Neural Signature of Affiliative Emotion
lP

in the Human Septohypothalamic Area. J.Neurosci. 32, 12499-12505.


Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Basilio, R., Bramati, I.E., Gordon, B., Rodriguez-Nieto,
G., Zahn, R., Krueger, F., Grafman, J., 2018. Altruistic decisions following
penetrating traumatic brain injury. Brain 141, 1558-1569.
na

Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Garrido, G.J., Bramati, I.E., Caparelli-Daquer, E.M.A.,
Paiva, M.M.F., Zahn, R., Grafman, J., 2007. The self as a moral agent: linking
the neural bases of social agency and moral sensitivity. Social Neuroscience 2,
336-352.
ur

Moll, J., De Oliveira-Souza, R., Zahn, R., 2008. The Neural Basis of Moral Cognition:
Sentiments, Concepts, and Values. Ann.NYAcad.Sci. 1124, 161-180.
Jo

Moll, J., Krueger, F., Zahn, R., Pardini, M., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Grafman, J., 2006.
Human fronto-mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable donation.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103, 15623-15628.
Moll, J., Schulkin, J., 2009. Social attachment and aversion in human moral cognition.
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 33, 456-465.
Moll, J., Weingartner, J.H., Bado, P., Basilio, R., Sato, J.R., Melo, B.R., Bramati, I.E.,
de Oliveira-Souza, R., Zahn, R., 2014. Voluntary enhancement of neural
signatures of affiliative emotion using FMRI neurofeedback. PLoS One 9,
e97343.
31

Moll, J., Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I.E., Krueger, F., Tura, B.,
Cavanagh, A.L., Grafman, J., 2011. Impairment of prosocial sentiments is
associated with frontopolar and septal damage in frontotemporal dementia.
Neuroimage 54, 1735-1742.
Moll, J., Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Krueger, F., Grafman, J., 2005. Opinion: the
neural basis of human moral cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 6, 799-809.
Morelli, S.A., Rameson, L.T., Lieberman, M.D., 2014. The neural components of
empathy: predicting daily prosocial behavior. Social cognitive and affective
neuroscience 9, 39-47.
Morelli, S.A., Sacchet, M.D., Zaki, J., 2015. Common and distinct neural correlates of
personal and vicarious reward: A quantitative meta-analysis. NeuroImage 112,
244-253.
Morey, R.A., McCarthy, G., Selgrade, E.S., Seth, S., Nasser, J.D., LaBar, K.S., 2012.
Neural systems for guilt from actions affecting self versus others. NeuroImage

of
60, 683-692.
Muller, J.L., Ganssbauer, S., Sommer, M., Dohnel, K., Weber, T., Schmidt-Wilcke, T.,

ro
Hajak, G., 2008. Gray matter changes in right superior temporal gyrus in
criminal psychopaths. Evidence from voxel-based morphometry. Psychiatry
research 163, 213-222.

stroke. J Clin Neurosci 11, 25-30.


-p
Nagaratnam, N., Nagaratnam, K., Ng, K., Diu, P., 2004. Akinetic mutism following

Nauta, W.H., Domesick, V., 1981/1993. Ramifications of the Limbic System,


re
Neuroanatomy. Birkhäuser Boston, pp. 329-352.
Nowak, M.A., Sigmund, K., 2005. Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature 437, 1291-
1298.
lP

O'Connor, L.E., Berry, J.W., Weiss, J., Gilbert, P., 2002. Guilt, fear, submission, and
empathy in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 71, 19-27.
Patterson, K., Nestor, P.J., Rogers, T.T., 2007. Where do you know what you know?
na

The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nat. Rev.


Neurosci. 8, 976-987.
Preston, S.D., 2013. The Origins of Altruism in Offspring Care. Psychol Bull 139,
1305-1341.
ur

Prevost, C., Pessiglione, M., Metereau, E., Clery-Melin, M.L., Dreher, J.C., 2010.
Separate Valuation Subsystems for Delay and Effort Decision Costs. Journal of
Neuroscience 30, 14080-14090.
Jo

Price, C.J., Mummery, C.J., Moore, C.J., Frakowiak, R.S., Friston, K.J., 1999.
Delineating necessary and sufficient neural systems with functional imaging
studies of neuropsychological patients. J Cogn Neurosci 11, 371-382.
Price, J.L., Drevets, W.C., 2010. Neurocircuitry of mood disorders.
Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 192-216.
Pulcu, E., Zahn, R., Moll, J., Trotter, P.D., Thomas, E.J., Juhasz, G., Deakin, J.F.,
Anderson, I.M., Sahakian, B.J., Elliott, R., 2014. Enhanced subgenual cingulate
response to altruistic decisions in remitted major depressive disorder.
NeuroImage. Clinical 4, 701-710.
32

Raine, A., Lee, L., Yang, Y., Colletti, P., 2010. Neurodevelopmental marker for limbic
maldevelopment in antisocial personality disorder and psychopathy. Br J
Psychiatry 197, 186-192.
Rankin, K.P., Gorno-Tempini, M.L., Allison, S.C., Stanley, C.M., Glenn, S., Weiner,
M.W., Miller, B.L., 2006. Structural anatomy of empathy in neurodegenerative
disease. Brain 129, 2945-2956.
Ressler, K.J., Mayberg, H.S., 2007. Targeting abnormal neural circuits in mood and
anxiety disorders: from the laboratory to the clinic. Nature Neuroscience 10,
1116-1124.
Ribas, G.C., 2007. Neuroanatomical basis of behavior: history and recent contributions.
Rev Bras Psiquiatr 29, 63-71.
Rohan, M.J., 2000. A rose by any name? The values construct. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 4,
255-277.

of
Ross, L.A., Olson, I.R., 2010. Social cognition and the anterior temporal lobes.
NeuroImage 49, 3452-3462.
Ruff, C.C., Ugazio, G., Fehr, E., 2013. Changing Social Norm Compliance with

ro
Noninvasive Brain Stimulation. Science 342, 482-484.
Rusch, N., Bado, P., Zahn, R., Bramati, I.E., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, J., 2014. You
and your kin: Neural signatures of family-based group perception in the
subgenual cortex. Soc Neurosci 9, 326-331.
-p
Santamaria-Garcia, H., Baez, S., Reyes, P., Santamaria-Garcia, J.A., Santacruz-
Escudero, J.M., Matallana, D., Arevalo, A., Sigman, M., Garcia, A.M., Ibanez,
re
A., 2017. A lesion model of envy and Schadenfreude: legal, deservingness and
moral dimensions as revealed by neurodegeneration. Brain 140, 3357-3377.
lP

Saper, C.B., 2011. Diffuse Cortical Projection Systems: Anatomical Organization and
Role in Cortical Function, Comprehensive Physiology. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Schwartz, S.H., Bilsky, W., 1987. Toward a Universal Psychological Structure of
Human-Values. J Pers Soc Psychol 53, 550-562.
na

Seara-Cardoso, A., Sebastian, C.L., McCrory, E., Foulkes, L., Buon, M., Roiser, J.P.,
Viding, E., 2016. Anticipation of guilt for everyday moral transgressions: The
role of the anterior insula and the influence of interpersonal psychopathic traits.
Scientific Reports 6.
ur

Shima, K., Isoda, M., Mushiake, H., Tanji, J., 2007. Categorization of behavioural
sequences in the prefrontal cortex. Nature 445, 315-318.
Jo

Simmons, W.K., Reddish, M., Bellgowan, P.S.F., Martin, A., 2009. The Selectivity and
Functional Connectivity of the Anterior Temporal Lobes. Cereb. Cortex,
bhp149.
Skipper, L.M., Ross, L.A., Olson, I.R., 2011. Sensory and semantic category
subdivisions within the anterior temporal lobes. Neuropsychologia 49, 3419-
3429.
Smith, D.V., Hayden, B.Y., Truong, T.K., Song, A.W., Platt, M.L., Huettel, S.A., 2010.
Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex.
Journal of Neuroscience 30, 2490-2495.
33

Snowden, J.S., Bathgate, D., Varma, A., Blackshaw, A., Gibbons, Z.C., Neary, D.,
2001. Distinct behavioural profiles in frontotemporal dementia and semantic
dementia. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 70, 323-332.
Sollberger, M., Stanley, C.M., Wilson, S.M., Gyurak, A., Beckman, V., Growdon, M.,
Jang, J., Weiner, M.W., Miller, B.L., Rankin, K.P., 2009. Neural basis of
interpersonal traits in neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia 47, 2812-
2827.
Stack, E.C., Balakrishnan, R., Numan, M.J., Numan, M., 2002. A functional
neuroanatomical investigation of the role of the medial preoptic area in neural
circuits regulating maternal behavior. Behavioural brain research 131, 17-36.
Stellar, E., 1954/1994. The physiology of motivation. Psychol Rev 101, 301-311.
Sulzer, J., Haller, S., Scharnowski, F., Weiskopf, N., Birbaumer, N., Blefari, M.L.,
Bruehl, A.B., Cohen, L.G., deCharms, R.C., Gassert, R., Goebel, R., Herwig, U.,
LaConte, S., Linden, D., Luft, A., Seifritz, E., Sitaram, R., 2013. Real-time

of
fMRI neurofeedback: Progress and challenges. Neuroimage 76, 386-399.
Swain, J.E., Konrath, S., Brown, S.L., Finegood, E.D., Akce, L.B., Dayton, C.J., Ho,

ro
S.S., 2012. Parenting and Beyond: Common Neurocircuits Underlying Parental
and Altruistic Caregiving. Parent.-Sci. Pract. 12, 115-123.
Swain, J.E., Lorberbaum, J.P., Kose, S., Strathearn, L., 2007. Brain basis of early
-p
parent-infant interactions: psychology, physiology, and in vivo functional
neuroimaging studies. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied
disciplines 48, 262-287.
re
Takahashi, H., Yahata, N., Koeda, M., Matsuda, T., Asai, K., Okubo, Y., 2004. Brain
activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: an
fMRI study. Neuroimage 23, 967-974.
lP

Tangney, J.P., Stuewig, J., Mashek, D.J., 2007. Moral emotions and moral behavior.
Annual Review of Psychology 58, 345-372.
Tassy, S., Oullier, O., Duclos, Y., Coulon, O., Mancini, J., Deruelle, C., Attarian, S.,
Felician, O., Wicker, B., 2012. Disrupting the right prefrontal cortex alters moral
na

judgement. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience 7, 282-288.


Tost, H., Kolachana, B., Hakimi, S., Lemaitre, H., Verchinski, B.A., Mattay, V.S.,
Weinberger, D.R., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., 2010. A common allele in the
ur

oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) impacts prosocial temperament and human


hypothalamic-limbic structure and function. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107, 13936-13941.
Jo

Wallis, C.U., Cardinal, R.N., Alexander, L., Roberts, A.C., Clarke, H.F., 2017.
Opposing roles of primate areas 25 and 32 and their putative rodent homologs in
the regulation of negative emotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 114, E4075-
E4084.
Walter, H., 2001. Neurophilosophy of Free Will: From Libertarian Illusions to a
Concept of Natural Autonomy MIT Press, Boston.
Wastling, S.J., Barker, G.J., 2015. Designing hyperbolic secant excitation pulses to
reduce signal dropout in gradient-echo echo-planar imaging. Magnetic resonance
in medicine 74, 661-672.
34

Weissenberger, A.A., Dell, M.L., Liow, K., Theodore, W., Frattali, C.M., Hernandez,
D., Zametkin, A.J., 2001. Aggression and psychiatric comorbidity in children
with hypothalamic hamartomas and their unaffected siblings. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 40, 696-703.
Welt, L., 1888. Über Charakterveränderungen des Menschen. Dtsch Arch Klin Med 42,
339-390.
Weng, H.Y., Fox, A.S., Hessenthaler, H.C., Stodola, D.E., Davidson, R.J., 2015. The
Role of Compassion in Altruistic Helping and Punishment Behavior. PLOS
ONE 10, e0143794.
Wiech, K., Kahane, G., Shackel, N., Farias, M., Savulescu, J., Tracey, I., 2013. Cold or
calculating? Reduced activity in the subgenual cingulate cortex reflects
decreased emotional aversion to harming in counterintuitive utilitarian judgment.
Cognition 126, 364-372.
Wigfield, A., 1994. Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation: A

of
developmental perspective. Educational Psychology Review 6, 49-78.
Wood, J.N., Grafman, J., 2003. Human prefrontal cortex: Processing and

ro
representational perspectives. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 4, 139-147.
Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I., Garrido, G., Moll, J., 2009a. Subgenual
cingulate activity reflects individual differences in empathic concern.
Neurosci.Lett. 457, 107-110. -p
Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, J., 2011a. The Neuroanatomical Basis of Moral
Cognition and Emotion, From DNA to Social Cognition. John Wiley & Sons,
re
Inc., pp. 123-138.
Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, J., 2011b. The neuroscience of moral cognition
and emotion, The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience. Oxford University
lP

Press, pp. 477-490.


Zahn, R., De Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, J., 2015. The Neural Foundation of Morality, in:
Wright, J.D. (Ed.), International Encyclopedia of Social and Behavioral
Sciences, 2nd ed. Elsevier, pp. 606-618.
na

Zahn, R., Green, S., Beaumont, H., Burns, A., Moll, J., Caine, D., Gerhard, A.,
Hoffman, P., Shaw, B., Grafman, J., Lambon Ralph, M.A., 2017.
Frontotemporal lobar degeneration and social behaviour: dissociation between
ur

the knowledge of its consequences and its conceptual meaning. Cortex.


Zahn, R., Moll, J., Iyengar, V., Huey, E.D., Tierney, M., Krueger, F., Grafman, J.,
2009b. Social conceptual impairments in frontotemporal lobar degeneration with
Jo

right anterior temporal hypometabolism. Brain 132, 604-616.


Zahn, R., Moll, J., Krueger, F., Huey, E.D., Garrido, G., Grafman, J., 2007. Social
concepts are represented in the superior anterior temporal cortex. Proc Natl Acad
Sci USA 104, 6430-6435.
Zahn, R., Moll, J., Paiva, M., Garrido, G., Krueger, F., Huey, E.D., Grafman, J., 2009c.
The Neural Basis of Human Social Values: Evidence from Functional MRI.
Cereb Cortex 19, 276-283.
35

Zaki, J., Mitchell, J.P., 2011. Equitable decision making is associated with neural
markers of intrinsic value. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of
the United States of America 108, 19761-19766.
Zhu, L.S., Jenkins, A.C., Set, E., Scabini, D., Knight, R.T., Chiu, P.H., King-Casas, B.,
Hsu, M., 2014. Damage to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affects tradeoffs
between honesty and self-interest. Nature Neuroscience 17, 1319-1321.

Boxes

Box 1| The history of the idea of moral motivations

Following the Aristotelian tradition, the perspective of virtue ethics highlighted the

of
importance of attaining virtues by training oneself to act according to their

prescriptions(Casebeer, 2003). In the 18th century, philosophers of the Scottish

ro
Enlightenment – Francis Hutcheson, his successor Adam Smith, and David

Hume(Zahn et al., 2011b) – highlighted the central importance of moral sentiments


-p
for moral behaviour. “Sympathy”, defined as “man's capacity for fellow feeling with

others”, was considered the primary moral sentiment by Adam Smith(Lamb, 1974).
re
Hutcheson postulated that “benevolence” motivates virtuous actions and thereby
lP

provides “moral motivations”(Bishop, 1996). Modern psychologists speak of

“moral emotions” rather than “moral sentiments” with some variation as to which

emotions are considered moral(Eisenberg, 2000; Tangney et al., 2007). The German
na

philosopher Immanuel Kant, who was a contemporary of Hume, distinguished the

ability to judge what is morally right and wrong (“principium diiudicationis”) from
ur

the motivation (“principium motivationis”) to act accordingly(Kant, 1786) , He

opposed the view that moral actions could be defined by the experienced moral
Jo

sentiments, which were thought to originate from the external senses. He claimed

instead that true moral actions are motivated directly by respect (“Achtung”) for

the moral law, which is self-generated and an act of free will(Kant, 1786). Kant

further pointed out that respect for the moral law is the impression of a moral

value that opposes our selfishness. Moral actions are those guided by a principle
36

that the individual wants to become a general law. Thus, moral motivations as

defined by the opposing schools of moral philosophy are either the respect for

moral rules (Kant) or moral sentiments (Hutcheson and Smith). As previously

pointed out(Zahn et al., 2011a), there is an often overlooked agreement across

schools in that those motivations that enable us to overcome self-interest are key to

understanding moral behaviour. Neuroscience and psychology can produce

evidence and theories about the structure and dynamics of subjective experiences

and behavioural expressions of moral motivations, such as “respect for moral

principles” or “feelings of guilt”, and their neural underpinnings. Beyond the realm

of
of pure empirical science, however, lies the contemporary philosophical debate

among supporters of versions of deontology, sentimentalism, consequentialism and

ro
virtue theory and their implications for free will, moral realism and moral

-p
philosophy in general (Casebeer, 2003),(Harris, 2011; Kahane, 2012; Walter, 2001).
re
lP
na
ur
Jo
37

Box 2 | Anatomy of the basal forebrain

The basal forebrain (BF) is a heterogeneous collection of structures and pathways

lying underneath the intercommissural plane(Klingler and Gloor, 1960). We here

briefly review and summarise the complex anatomy of this region. The medial BF

comprises the more anteriorly located subgenual cingulate cortex (subgenual parts

of areas 24, 32 and, 33), the posterior subgenual cortex (area 25), Broca’s

paraolfactory area, preoptic area, and ventromedial hypothalamus. These areas

contain the highest concentrations of receptors for the “affiliative neuropeptides”

oxytocin and vasopressin in the human brain(Loup et al., 1991) and comprise a core

of
node of the human “default mode” network(Bado et al., 2014). Laterally, the BF

encompasses the nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum, the amygdala and the

ro
basal nucleus of Meynert(Nauta and Domesick, 1981/1993). The medial and lateral

-p
BF structures are heavily interconnected by the medial forebrain bundle, Broca’s

diagonal band (ventral amygdalofugal pathway), stria terminalis (dorsal


re
amygdalofugal pathway), and stria medullaris(Ribas, 2007). Together with the

median-paramedian structures of the upper brainstem tegmentum, they provide


lP

massive direct extrathalamic projections to the entire cerebral cortex(Saper, 2011).

The orbitofrontal, subgenual, and paraolfactory areas, anterior insula and temporal
na

pole (TP) are given anatomic unity by the uncinate fascicle, representing endpoints

of massive ascending extrathalamic and thalamic projections.

Figure Box 2. Parasagittal and coronal sections to illustrate the BF structures


ur

related to the visceroendocrine representation of the internal milieu and motivational

states. (A) Subgenual: structures underneath the genu (“knee”) of the corpus
Jo

callosum at this level are represented by the posterior subgenual cortex (area 25),

originally referred to as “subgenual area” by Brodmann, and the more anterior

subgenual cingulate cortex (subgenual parts of areas 24, 32, and 33). The subgenual

cingulate cortex is bordered ventrally by the subgenual sectors of the orbitofrontal

cortex ( [OFC], area 11) including the subgenual sector of the paracingulate cortex
38

which some atlases consider to be part of area 32 (B) Precommissural: the medial

septal nuclei, which make up the surface of the ventromedial wall of the cerebral

hemisphere at this level, are represented by the paraterminal and paraolfactory gyri

(Broca’s paraolfactory area). Note that area 25 has also been described as extending

more posteriorly(Price and Drevets, 2010) into the region here identified as

paraolfactory area. (C) Commissural: the anterior commissure [AC] pierces the basal

ganglia creating a subcommissural sector (nucleus accumbens [NAcc] and ventral

pallidum [VP]), which is laterally coextensive with the basal nucleus of Meynert and

the ansa peduncularis [AP]; the AP connects the amygdala [Amyg] with the

of
ventromedial hypothalamus (not shown).

ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo
39

of
ro
-p
re
lP
na
ur

Box 3 | The neural basis of socio-moral values


Jo

Abstract social concepts such as “honour” and “honesty” are used to describe

social and moral values which operate at a higher level of abstraction than simple

social attitudes towards a person or specific action(Rohan, 2000). Social

psychologists have defined social values as trans-situational goals which motivate

and guide our behaviour(Schwartz and Bilsky, 1987). FMRI studies have shown that
40

the ATLs, especially within their right superior sectors(Skipper et al., 2011),(Zahn

et al., 2007), are selectively more activated when considering social concepts than

they are for non-social concepts(Ross and Olson, 2010; Zahn et al., 2015; Zahn et

al., 2007),(Simmons et al., 2009),(Skipper et al., 2011). Furthermore, right superior

ATL activation increased with the richness of detail with which social concepts

describe social behaviour(Zahn et al., 2007) in keeping with the role of the ATLs in

representing differentiated conceptual knowledge(Lambon Ralph and Patterson,

2008),(Patterson et al., 2007). Activation in the right superior ATL was reproduced

when making emotional rather than semantic judgments about social values(Zahn et

of
al., 2009c). Interestingly, activation in this region was independent of the emotional

(valence: positive vs. negative(Zahn et al., 2007),(Zahn et al., 2011a; Zahn et al.,

ro
2009c), moral sentiment: guilt vs. pride vs. gratitude vs. indignation(Zahn et al.,

-p
2009c)) and agency (self vs. other) context, supporting the hypothesis that the ATL

represents the context-independent (i.e. trans-situational) meaning of social values


re
and thus goals entailed in moral motivations, whereas fronto-subcortical regions

represent cognitive and emotional ingredients specific for particular moral


lP

sentiments(Moll et al., 2005).


na
ur
Jo
41

Box 4 | Lesions of frontal lobe subregions and moral motivation

Altruistic giving is a prototypic behaviour involving moral motivation. Recent

studies using non-invasive “virtual lesion” methods that employ brain stimulation

techniques (transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) and transcranial magnetic

stimulation (TMS)) and lesion mapping have provided important hints on the brain

regions that are necessary for moral behaviours and choices(Tassy et al., 2012). TMS

or tDCS studies using two-person economic games show that the right dorsolateral

prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) is necessary for making choices that maximise one’s

reputation(Knoch et al., 2009) and for social norm compliance under punishment

of
threats(Ruff et al., 2013), findings that are consistent with the suggested role of this

region in reducing subjective values associated with the pursuit of immediate self-

ro
interest(Hare et al., 2009). Decisions to uphold social norms in these experiments

-p
do not necessarily rely on moral motivation, because these decisions could also be

driven by self-regarding motivations, such as avoidance of punishment, preserving


re
one’s social reputation and anger resulting from one’s self-interest being

challenged. By contrast, interference with medial frontopolar cortex function using


lP

tDCS reduced guilt and increased deceitful behaviour, facilitating lying in mock

crime interrogations(Karim et al., 2010). A recent study(Zhu et al., 2014) showed


na

that damage to the (mostly left) DLPFC, but not to the orbitofrontal cortex,

impaired honesty concerns. The economic decision task enabled teasing apart

honesty concerns from altruistic preferences per se. Whereas all groups were equally
ur

altruistic in sharing money with anonymous participants in a dictator-type task,

DLPFC lesion patients were less concerned about honesty and thus indulged more
Jo

in sending “false messages”. These results highlight the roles of the DLPFC and

medial orbitofrontal cortex in prosocial behaviour―i.e., abiding to the value of

honesty, and are corroborated by their functional interplay in self-control(Hare et

al., 2009; Hayashi et al., 2013) and value computations(Prevost et al., 2010). In

patients with FTD, reduced resting glucose metabolism in the septal region and
42

frontopolar cortex predicted impairments of guilt and compassion on a moral

sentiment task. Impairment of “other-critical” feelings (anger/indignation and

disgust), in contrast, was associated with abnormal amygdala and dorsomedial

prefrontal (DMPFC) metabolism(Moll et al., 2011). Taken together, these studies

point to the importance of frontopolar and ventromedial frontal regions (subgenual

cingulate and adjacent medial orbitofrontal cortex) in motivating positive prosocial

behaviours, and indicate that the DLPFC and lateral orbitofrontal cortices are key

for responses that enforce honesty, self-reputation concerns, self-control and third-

party punishment(Coricelli et al., 2005; Delgado et al., 2005). The frontopolar

of
contribution to moral motivation is most likely related to its representation of long-

term consequences of social behaviour (Wood and Grafman, 2003; Zahn et al.,

ro
2017) and thus its importance for representing goals of moral motivations.

-p
re
lP
na
ur
Jo
43

Figure legends

Figure 1

The neural architecture of human moral motivation

Panel a| Brain regions underpinning moral cognition and general motivation.

Neural structures important to moral motivation include the septal/preoptic

hypothalamic area (Septal/hypoth) and subgenual frontal cortex (SGC). The

frontopolar and anterior superior temporal cortices enable more complex moral

goals. These are closely integrated with reward and behavioural reinforcement

of
systems, including the ventral tegmental area (VTA), ventral striatum (VStr), medial

forebrain bundle (MFB), raphe nuclei and the amygdalae (Amyg). These regions are

ro
further integrated with the anterior and posterior medial (ant and post mOFC) and

lateral (lat OFC) sectors of the orbitofrontal and dorsolateral (DLPFC) prefrontal
-p
cortices, pregenual anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), superior anterior temporal

cortex (Sup ATL) and posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS) and temporo-
re
parietal junction (TPJ).

b| The event-feature-emotion complex (EFEC) model(Moll et al., 2005) postulates


lP

that moral cognition, emotion and motivation emerge from network integration via

temporal binding. Neural components having core roles for moral motivation: (1)
na

basal forebrain, especially the septal/hypothalamic area, implicated in basic (i.e.

context-independent) affiliative states(Moll et al., 2012; Moll et al., 2005; Stellar,


ur

1954/1994), and subgenual frontal cortex [SGC], involved in more complex

context-dependent aspects such as social agency and responsibility attribution(Moll


Jo

et al., 2008),(Rusch et al., 2014); (2) anterior prefrontal / frontopolar cortex [FPC],

which stores event and action sequences(Krueger et al., 2007b; Shima et al., 2007;

Wood and Grafman, 2003; Zahn et al., 2017) enabling long-term goal

representations; (3) anterior temporal cortex (especially the right Sup ATL),

representing abstract social conceptual knowledge(Zahn et al., 2007), e.g.

“generous” or “rude” qualities of behaviour(Zahn et al., 2009b)); and (4) the


44

pSTS/TPJ, encoding perceptual social features such as emotional face and body

posture(Allison et al., 2000) and mental models of socially-relevant

perspectives(Decety and Grezes, 2006).

of
ro
Figure 2
-p
re
Functional imaging the basal forebrain in moral motivation

Recent fMRI studies have implicated basal forebrain regions in social decision-
lP

making, altruistic choices and prosocial emotions. a| Septo-hypothalamic activity

(arrow) during fMRI neurofeedback-training of affectionate/tender feelings(Moll et


na

al., 2014). b| Cooperation increased septo-hypothalamic activity (circled), as trust

developed in a repeated interaction economic task(Krueger et al., 2007a). c| Septal

area fMRI responses evoked when seeing empathy-eliciting pictures depicting


ur

other’s pain, anxiety or happiness. Activation predicted self-reported scores of real-

life helping behaviours, as illustrated by the scatterplot (pain vs. neutral


Jo

contrast)(Morelli et al., 2014). d| Subgenual cingulate cortex fMRI responses to

kinship-associated narratives tracked individual differences in “family entitativity”,

i.e., perceiving own family as a cohesive social group(Rusch et al., 2014). e|

Individuals higher in empathic concern showed increased subgenual cingulate

responses to guilt/compassion-evoking scenarios(Zahn et al., 2009a). f| Functional


45

MRI responses of the subgenual cingulate cortex / septal area during charitable

donations (altruistic decisions) (Moll et al., 2006) . g|Subgenual cortex fMRI

activation selective for prosocial vs. selfish prediction errors which was higher in

empathic individuals(Lockwood et al., 2016). h| Increased insula, SMA, and right

DLPFC activity (yellow) when matching monetary expectations of the partner in a

trust game. Activity in subgenual cortex (not shown) and in septal area / nucleus

accumbens (NAcc, in blue), when participants returned less than expected,

generating feelings of guilt(Chang et al., 2011). i| Scatterplot shows the relationship

between counterfactual guilt sensitivity and septal area / NAcc responses(Chang et

of
al., 2011).

ro
Panel a|, adapted from (Moll et al., 2014), panel b| adapted from (Krueger et al.,

-p
2007a), panel c| adapted from (Morelli et al., 2014), panel d| adapted from (Rusch

et al., 2014), panel e| adapted from (Zahn et al., 2009a), panel (f) adapted from
re
(Moll et al., 2006) , all with permission. Panel g| reproduced from (Smith et al.,

2010), with permission. Panels (h) and (i) adapted from (Chang et al., 2011), with
lP

permission.
na
ur
Jo
Jo
ur
na
lP
re
-p
ro
of
46

You might also like