You are on page 1of 15

Sports Mad.

1996 Apr; 21 (4); 277·291


REVIEW ARTICLE o112·1642/96/WJMJ277/ S07 .fiJ/O
© Adis International limited. All rights reserved .

The Effects of Acute and Chronic


Exercise on Sleep
A Meta-Analytic Review
Karla A. Kubitz,l Daniel M. Landers,2 Steven J. Petruzzello3 and Myungwoo Han 4
1 Department of Kinesiology, Kansas State University, Manhattan, Kansas, USA
2 Exercise and Sport Research Institute, Department of Exercise Science and Physical Education,
Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona, USA
3 Department of Kinesiology, University of Illinois, Urbana, Illinois, USA
4 Korea Sport Science Institute, Seoul, Korea

Contents
Summary ........ . 277
1. Review Procedures. . . . 280
1.1 Problem Formation . 280
1.2 Selection of Studies . 281
1.3 Coding the Studies . 281
1.3.1 Study Characteristics 281
1.3.2 Individual Descriptors . 281
1.3.3 Exercise Characteristics 282
1.4 Data Analysis Procedures . . . 282
2. Results and Discussion . . . . . . . . 282
2.1 Studies Without Data Available . 282
2.2 Studies With Data Available . 283
2.2.1 Acute Exercise .. 283
2.2.2 Chronic Exercise. 285
3. General Discussion . . . . . . 285
3.1 Summary of Findings . . 285
3.2 Suggested Underlying Mechanisms 286
4. Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 287

Summary Studies attempting to ascertain the effects of acute and chronic exercise on
measures of sleep have yielded conflicting results and interpretations. Methodo-
logical differences among studies may explain this lack of consensus; however,
small sample sizes and subsequently low statistical power may also have contrib-
uted. In an attempt to resolve these issues, this review used meta-analytical tech-
niques to: (a) re-examine the effects of exercise on sleep; and (b) examine possible
moderators of these effects. Studies meeting the selection criteria were included
in the analysis. Analyses of moderating factors were performed for stage 4 sleep
and rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. The results indicated that acute and
chronic exercise increased slow wave sleep (SWS) and total sleep time but de-
creased sleep onset latency and REM sleep. Moderating variables influencing the
278 Kubitz et al.

magnitude and direction of these effects were related to characteristics of the


individual (e.g. sex, age, fitness level) and the exercise (e.g. time of day exercise
was completed, type of exercise, exercise duration). Mechanisms which have
been suggested to explain the relationship between exercise and sleep are dis-
cussed and directions for further research are provided.

The effects of exercise, which is a waking external impulses to act during sleep' .II] Thus, ex-
behaviour, have most often been related to other ercise would be likely to influence sleep only to the
waking behaviours (e.g. stress reactivity, depres- extent that it alters physiological and/or psycholog-
sion, self-esteem). Less attention has been given to ical excitation during sleep. Operationally, sleep
examining the effects of exercise on non waking researchers have predicted that physiological
behaviours. However, individuals cycle between measures such as heart rate, temperature, galvanic
activity (i.e. waking) and inactivity (i.e. sleeping) skin response and/or respiration would be higher
and it is often believed that since sleeping and wak- on nights following exercise than on nights follow-
ing constitute alternate phases of the same cycle, ing no exercise (i.e. acute effect).
changes in one portion of the cycle significantly Researchers interested in the effects of phys-
affect the other. ical activity on sleep have examined a number of
The position, or 'theory' , that emphasises the operational measures of sleep (e.g. behavioural,
reciprocal nature of sleep in relation to wakefulness physiological and psychological measures). Psycho-
has been called 'complemental'!ll or 'compen- physiological recordings (i.e. the EEG, electro-
satory' .12] According to this position, 'those sys- oculogram and electromyogram [EMG]) have
tems which are most fatigued during wakefulness been compared between nights with and without
automatically slow down the most during sleep and exercise. Furthermore, these recordings have also
thus achieve the most rest' .[1] Fatiguing daytime been used to quantify other indices of sleep, includ-
activity (e.g. exercise) would probably result in a ing length of sleep (total sleep time), time taken to
compensatory increase in the need for and depth of fall asleep (sleep onset latency), the quietness or
night-time sleep,!2] thereby facilitating recupera- restfulness of sleep (movement time) and the per-
tive,!I] restorative!3] and/or energy conservation!4] centages of time spent in particular types of sleep
processes. Operationally, sleep researchers have [i.e. rapid eye movement (REM) or SWS] . Heart
predicted that measures such as sleep duration, rate, temperature, biochemical and retrospective
self-reports of sleep quality and the amount of high self-report measures have also been employed in
amplitude, slow wave electroencephalographic studies examining the effects of physical activity
(EEG) activity, [i.e. slow wave sleep (SWS)] would on sleep.
be higher in physically fit individuals than in those In the exercise and sleep literature, the most
who are unfit (i.e. chronic effect) and higher on common measure is the sleep EEG and researchers
nights following exercise than on nights following make inferences concerning sleep based on various
no exercise (i.e. acute effect). characteristics of the scalp-recorded EEG (e.g.
An alternative to the compensatory position is waveform morphology, amplitude and/or fre-
the 'continuity' position which maintains that sleep quency). For example, in terms of frequency, the
is determined by activity levels during the sleep typical spontaneous EEG is composed of a mixture
period. As such, sleep is seen as a resting state of ~ (12 to 30Hz), a. (8 to 12Hz), e (4 to 8Hz) and
where physiological and psychological excitation 8 (0 to 4Hz) waves. The waking EEG has a greater
gradually tapers off from waking activity. Its ben- proportion of faster frequency (a. and ~) activity
efits (i.e. in terms of facilitating the resting state) than the sleep EEG. Furthermore, standard scoring
'stem mainly from the scarcity of both internal and procedures!5] classify sleep into 5 distinct stages

© Adis International limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med. 1996 Apr; 21 (4)
Effects of Exercise on Sleep 279

based on characteristic waveform morphology, increased SWS but he also reported additional
amplitude and frequency criteria, previous EEG effects of exercise on sleep, including increased
activity, ocular motions and EMG activity. These subjective tiredness, increased sleep duration, in-
include stages 1,2,3,4 and REM. More specific- creased growth hormone release and decreased
ally, the Manual of Standardised Terminology, sleep onset latency.
Techniques and Scoring Systemfor Sleep Stages in In 1988, 2 additional reviews of the literature,
Human Subjects[51 describes the following criteria: covering approximately 50 studies, were publish-
• 'Stage I - A relatively low voltage, mixed fre- edJ2, IO I Shapiro and Driver[lOI basically recon-
quency EEG without rapid eye movements firmed the findings of Shapiro's earlier review;
(REMs). Trinder et alPI found the literature less consistent,
• Stage 2 - 12 to 14Hz sleep spindles and K com- particularly with regard to the acute effects of ex-
plexes on a background of relatively low volt- ercise. Trinder and his colleagues reported that
age, mixed frequency EEG. only 3 of the 21 papers considered in the earlier
• Stage 3 - Moderate amounts of high amplitude, reviews showed that acute exercise significantly
slow wave activity. increased SWS. Similarly, only 2 of 14 studies and
• Stage 4 - Large amounts of high amplitude, 4 of 19 showed that acute exercise significantly
slow wave activity. increased sleep duration or decreased the amount
• Stage REM - A relatively low voltage, mixed of REM activity, respectively. Trinder et aU21 also
frequency EEG in conjunction with episodic found very little support for the compensatory hy-
REMs and low amplitude EMG.' pothesis in either more recent acute exercise stud-
Baekeland and Lasky[61 were among the first to ies (i.e. those published between 1982 and 1988)
examine the effects of strenuous exercise on sub- or in chronic exercise studies. However, they did
sequent sleep. They concluded that exercise re- conclude in favour of a chronic effect of endurance
sulted in an increased amount of 8 sleep (0 to 4Hz) training (i.e. aerobic training increased SWS and
in the post-exercise sleep period. Although sub- total sleep time).
sequent investigators have demonstrated similar One of the potential strengths of the review by
effects, others have suggested that the reported Trinder et alPI was its objectivity. Their conclu-
effects are not consistent or occur only in certain sions were based on statistical (i.e. probability
circumstances. Hence, the 5 narrative reviews that value) outcomes. Significant (p < 0.05) and non-
have examined this literature have had little suc- significant (p > 0.05) effects of exercise on various
cess in reaching a consensus regarding the effects measures of sleep were tallied. This 'counting
of exercise on sleep. method' is ideal when the statistical power is high
In 1981, 3 reviews of the literature (e.g. covering in the studies being counted. However, it may be
approximately 30 studies) examining the effects problematic when statistical power is compro-
of physical activity on sleep were published.[7-91 mised, either by small sample sizes and/or by mod-
Horne[71 and TorsvaW81 concluded that, although the est treatment effects. In these conditions, the type
exercise/sleep literature was plagued by methodo- II error rate is often compromised.
logical problems (i.e. varying exercise stimuli, To circumvent the problems associated with the
within-individual variability in the sleep EEG, 'counting method', we contrasted the effect sizes
weak experimental designs, varying EEG scoring (ESs) from studies comparing an exercise treat-
criteria and insufficient recording nights), exercise ment or a fit group!condition with a comparison
appeared to reliably increase SWS (i.e. stages 3 group/condition using meta-analytic procedures.
and 4) in physically fit individuals. Shapiro,[91 Iike- Meta-analysis, as defined by Glass,[111 is a method-
wise, concluded in favour of the compensatory po- ology for combining the results of independent in-
sition. Not only did he find that exercise reliably vestigations with the purpose of integrating their

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med. 1996 Apr; 21 (4)
280 Kubitz et al.

findings. It is a systematic, replicable, statistical pro- rative summary of study conclusions were exam-
cedure that was designed to overcome many of the ined when ES information was not available.
problems inherent in traditional narrative reviews.
Meta-analysis has 2 advantages over other types 1.1 Problem Formation
of reviews. The first is that it quantifies the magni-
tude of treatment effects. Moreover, because an ES A pilot meta-analysis was completed!13] which
is a standardised estimate of treatment effects, the provided a basis for refining research questions and
relative efficacy of various treatments can be com- for developing the coding form used in the present
pared across meta-analyses. The second advantage review. In particular, the pilot meta-analysis: (a)
is that meta-analysis facilitates examination of po- identified 21 different measures of sleep (e.g.
tential moderators of treatment effects. It does so SWS, REM sleep, sleep onset latency, sleep effi-
by coding individual studies according to potential ciency) common to this literature (see table I); and
moderating factors (e.g. study quality) and statisti- (b) suggested that acute (i.e. exercise vs nonexerc-
cally examining the influence of these factors on ise) and chronic (e.g. athlete vs non-athlete, fit vs
treatment outcomes. Because of these 2 advan- unfit, trained vs untrained) effects be examined
tages, meta-analysis is ideal for increasing statisti- separately.
cal power, resolving conflicts among studies with
Fairly typical operational definitions for some
contradictory results and addressing research ques-
of the sleep measures identified included: 'sleep
tions (e.g. those related to potential moderators of
onset latency as the time from lights out to the be-
treatment effects) not posed by the authors of indi- ginning of the first period of 15 minutes of contin-
vidual studies. [12]
uous stage 2 sleep; sleep period time as the time
Thus, a meta-analysis of the exercise/sleep lit- from sleep onset to final awakening; total sleep
erature will not only provide insight into the con- time as sleep period time minus interim wakeful-
flicting conclusions reached by previous reviewers, ness and REM latency as the time from sleep onset
but also will quantify the magnitude of exercise/
sleep effects and address questions regarding po-
tential moderators of these effects. Quantifying the Table I. Types of sleep measure
magnitude of exercise/sleep effects will allow com- Stage 1
parisons of the effects of exercise with those of Stage 2
other health-related interventions. Meta-analytical Stage 3
data exist regarding the effects of a variety of med- Stage 4
Slow wave sleep (SWS)
ical and behavioural interventions. Examining po-
Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep
tential moderating factors will assist in providing Movement time
answers to questions raised in previous reviews, Sleep onset latency
including the effects of various study (e.g. study Total sleep time
validity), individual (age, sex and fitness) and ex- Self report
ercise characteristics (time of day, exercise type Heart rate
and exercise duration) on sleep outcomes. Biochemical
Temperature
REM latency
1. Review Procedures Awake time
lime in bed
An attempt was made in this review to include Sleep period time
all the published and unpublished English-language Stage 1 + movement
studies that could be located. Although the review SWS latency
was primarily based on meta-analytical informa- Sleep efficiency
tion derived from ESs, mean differences or a nar- Sleep cycle length

© Adis Intemational Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med. 1996 Apr; 21 (4)
Effects of Exercise on Sleep 281

to the onset of the first REM sleep period longer Table II. Moderator variables for effect size data
than a 3-minute duration.'[ 141 Study characteristics
Also typical was sleep efficiency, as the propor- Validity (high, medium or low)
tion of time spent asleep out of the total recording Individual descriptors
period. Self-report measures included a number of Sex (male, female or mixed)
questionnaires reflecting sleepiness/alertness or Fitness (mIlkg/min)
perceived sleep quality (e.g. Stanford Sleepiness Age(y)
Scale). Biochemical measures included catechola- Body mass index

mine, cortisol and human growth hormone. Exercise characteristics


Exercise type (aerobic, nonaerobic, mixed or not reported)
1.2 Selection of Studies Exercise situation (lab or field)
Duration (min)
Studies were located through manual and com- Intensity (%V02max)
puterised searches (ERIC, PsychLit, Dissertation Time of day exercise completed (h)
Abbreviations: V02max = maximum oxygen uptake.
Abstracts International and Medline) of research
journals, conference proceedings and dissertation
abstracts. The key terms used in these searches
on suggestions in the previous literature about po-
were 'sleep', 'sleep deprivation', 'exercise', 'physi-
tential moderating factors. These characteristics
cal activity', 'physical fitness' and 'fitness'. The
were divided into 3 areas: study characteristics, in-
more recent publications were identified and their
dividual descriptors, and exercise characteristics.
reference lists were examined for further studies.
The factors examined within each of these catego-
In addition, a bibliography of studies (n = 1400)
ries are listed in table II.
examining psychological consequences of exer-
cise was obtained from Dr Michael Sachs (personal
' .3. 1 Study Characteristics
communication) and compared with the studies
Using the procedure outlined by Glass et al.,[15 1
already located.
studies were coded as having high, medium or low
The majority of the studies located (n = 73)
internal validity. High internal validity ratings
came from either the fields of psychophysiology or
were given to studies that randomly assigned indi-
exercise science and included published articles
viduals to conditions and had mortality rates ofless
(n =64), abstracts (n =4) and unpublished disser-
than 15%, medium ratings to studies with higher
tations/theses/reports (n = 5). The articles were read
or nonequivalent mortality and low ratings to stud-
and if clarification was needed, a letter requesting
ies with other design flaws (i.e. the use of intact
the necessary information was sent to the authors.
convenience samples). Furthermore, high ratings
There were 3 criteria for inclusion of studies in
were assigned to studies with repeated measures
the meta-analysis. First, each study had to have at
designs that counterbalanced exposure to exercise
least one outcome measure of sleep related to ex-
and nonexercise conditions, and low ratings were
ercise or fitness. Secondly, studies had to have
assigned to studies that did not counterbalance ex-
studied sleep in humans. Thirdly, studies published
posure to exercise and nonexercise conditions.
in English must have been available prior to De-
Studies were rated on internal validity by 2 inde-
cember, 1992. Thus, this review included cross-
pendent raters. Initial agreement was 73% and dif-
sectional, longitudinal, published and unpublished
ferences were resolved by clarifying points of dis-
studies.
agreement in order to reach 100% agreement.
1.3 Coding the Studies
'.3.2 Individual Descriptors
The studies/ESs were coded on several charac- Individual characteristics coded included sex
teristics which reflected a priori decisions based (male, female or mixed), age (in years), body mass

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med. 1996 Apr; 21 (4)
282 Kubitz et al.

index (i.e. kg/m2) and fitness level (in mllkg/min). The ESs were examined using the following
Sex was coded as a categorical variable - the remain- procedures. Stouffer meta-analytic Z-tests l181 were
ing characteristics were coded as continuous vari- used to determine whether the mean ESs were sig-
ables. nificantly different from zero. A mean ES that is
significantly different from zero is comparable to
1.3.3 Exercise Characteristics
rejecting the null hypothesis in an experimental
Exercise characteristics coded included exer-
study. H-statistics l191 were used to determine whether
cise duration (in minutes), exercise intensity (as a
the mean ESs for each of the sleep measures were
percentage of maximum oxygen uptake [V0 2max ])
homogeneous. For sleep measures with an H-
time of day when exercise was completed (0 to
statistic indicating ES heterogeneity, examination
2400 hours), exercise type (aerobic, nonaerobic or
of possible moderating effects was carried out us-
mixed) and exercise situation (alone, in laboratory,
ing a variety of parametric techniques [i.e. one-way
in race). The first three were coded as continuous
analysis of variance tests (with Tukey post hoc tests
variables, the remaining two as categorical vari-
where indicated), t-tests or Pearson correlations].
ables.
Finally, either (02 or r2 was calculated as indices of
the percentage of variance accounted for by the ob-
1.4 Data Analysis Procedures served statistics.
The results of the studies were handled in 3
ways. First, if the study did not present means and 2. Results and Discussion
standard deviations/errors, the findings were dis- Of the 73 studies located, 64 contained informa-
cussed in a narrative review. Secondly, if the study tion that could be used in this review. Effect sizes
presented means, but not standard deviations/ were calculated for 38 of these studies, mean dif-
errors, mean difference scores were calculated. ferences were derived for 12 others and findings
Thirdly, if the study presented both means and were summarised for 14 studies in which means
standard deviations/errors, ESs were calculated. and standard deviations were unavailable. Nine
The narrative and mean difference information, al- studies were excluded from this review either be-
though less conclusive, was compared with the ES cause the data were also reported in another study
data, thereby minimising elimination of studies already included in this review,[1 .20-22J or because
from the meta-analysis. However, once these com- we were unable to obtain the studies,123- 25 1 or be-
parisons were made, follow-up analyses of moder- cause, upon closer inspection, the study did not
ating variables were done only for studies provid- meet the inclusion criteria.126.271
ing ES information.
The ES was calculated using the following for- 2.1 Studies Without Data Available
mula: 1161
Many of the studies without means and standard
. _ Mtreatment - Mcomparison deviations available involved epidemiological or
Eff
eet sIze - SD qualitative research. The epidemiological stud-
pooled
ies128-301 were designed to examine lifestyle choices
Thus, a positive ES reflects a higher level of the either promoting or disturbing sleep, and supported
sleep measure for the treatment group mean, in a view of exercise facilitating sleep. For example,
comparison with the control group mean. A pooled Vuori et aI.l 301 found that 1 of every 3 people sur-
standard deviation was used in this calculation veyed felt that exercise had a positive impact on
since it provides a more precise estimate of the pop- sleep quality. Similarly, Hasan et al.l 281 found that
ulation variance'! 171 Additionally, the ESs were 20% of the population surveyed felt that they slept
corrected for positive bias, using the formula sug- more deeply and soundly after exercise (i.e. day-
gested by Hedges.!171 time rather than evening exercise).

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med. 1996 Apr; 21 (4)
Effects of Exercise on Sleep 283

Table III. Difference scores (n =456) by sleep measure type


No. of studies No. of difference scores Mean difference score Standard error
Stage 1 5 25 -<>.25 1.0
Stage 2 6 28 2.16 1.6
Stage 3 7 33 3.00 1.4
Stage 4 7 33 1.32 1.0
SWS 9 42 3.00 1.4
REM sleep 9 41 -2.25 1.1
Movement time 2 9 0.13 0.1
Sleep onset latency 5 13 -1.72 1.9
Total sleep time 6 19 2.77 4.2
Self report 3 14 0.61 0.2
Heart rate 2 8 5.09 0.9
Biochemical 0
Temperature 1 6 0.02 0.0
REM latency 3 10 10.96 7.2
Awake time 5 20 -<>.19 2.9
Time in bed 3 9 13.00 4.2
Sleep period time 4 12 8.49 4.2
Stage 1 + movement 2 9 2.61 1.9
SWS latency 4 9 1.53 1.9
Sleep efficiency 3 5.97 2.2
Sleep cycle length 16 -5.78 2.0
Abbreviations: REM =rapid eye movement; SWS =slow wave sleep.

The qualitative studies[31,32] were designed to 2.2 Studies With Data Available
examine self-reported effects of regular exercise
From the 38 studies with means and standard
and found that fit individuals reported sleeping
deviations/errors available, 1124 ESs were calcu-
better than unfit individuals. Furthermore, 6 pilot
lated. The majority (n = 32) of the studies provided
studies available only as abstracts had no data avail-
acute ESs. Somewhat fewer (n = 12) provided
able. Five of these were also consistent with the chronic ESs (i.e. including 6 studies contributing
meta-analytical results (i .e. they showed exercise/ both acute and chronic ESs). From the 1124 ESs,
fitness effects);[33-37] one was not.[38] 22 that were more than 3 standard deviations from
Finally,3 studies[39-41] which also failed to report the grand mean were eliminated from the analyses
means and standard deviations addressed slightly and 19 of these came from the same study.l42] The
different exercise-related questions. Roussel and aforementioned study may have been particularly
Buguet[40J found that heart rates were increased likely to yield outlying ESs because of its small
about 10% during sleep after exercise. Iguchi et sample size (n = 2), highly trained individuals and
al.[39] found that the method of quantifying SWS long duration (e.g. 2-,4- and 6-hour) exercise ses-
influenced the likelihood of exercise-induced SWS sions. The 12 studies without standard devia-
increases. SWS increases were found only when tions[43-54] yielded 456 mean difference scores (see
the EEG power was examined. Ryback and Lewis[41] table III).
found that individuals confined to bed increased 2.2. 1 Acute Exercise
SWS, regardless of whether or not they were al- From the 32 studies examining the effects of
lowed to exercise during bedrest periods. acute exercise, 828 ESs were calculated. The mean

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med. 1996 Apr; 21 (4)
284 Kubitz et al.

Table IV. Effect sizes by sleep measure type. In order to minimise the likelihood of spurious findings, ESs were not tested for significance
(Stouffer meta-analytical Z-test) or heterogeneity (Hedges H-statistic) unless n > 10
Acute exercise Chronic exercise
no. of no. of ESs mean ES SE no. of no. of ESs mean ES SE
studies studies
Stage 1 14 52 -{l.08 0.09 3 6 0.06 0.15
Stage 2 17 60 -{l.07 0.08 4 10 0.17 0.09
Stage 3 14 48 0.34" 0.13 4 10 0.34 0.10
Stage 4 13 47 0.75.. t 0.22 4 10 0.23 0.19
SWS 29 125 0.26" 0.08 9 33 0.43.. t 0.10
REM sleep 21 110 -{l.14't 0.08 5 30 -{l.57*' 0.12
Movement time 6 39 0.03 0.06 2 5 0.72 0.25
Sleep onset latency 14 36 -{l.20" 0.06 7 18 -{l.45" 0.09
Total sleep time 15 33 0.31" 0.08 6 15 0.94" 0.40
Self report 7 42 -{l.16 0.10 5 14 0.12 0.31
Heart rate 4 0.81 0.16 0
Biochemical 4 15 0.22 0.17 2 6 0.45 0.27
Temperature 0 0
REM latency 15 34 0.29" 0.10 6 12 0.06 0.13
Awake time 12 42 0.02 0.08 6 19 -{l.40" 0.17
Time in bed 6 9 -{l.07 0.14 4 9 0.18 0.12
Sleep period time 3 6 0.31 0.21 3 6 0.29 0.18
Stage 1 + movement 2 4 0.18 0.14 1 2 0.07 0.03
SWS latency 1 2 0.05 0.46 2 5 -{).12 0.21
Sleep efficiency 3 7 0.42 0.21 3 7 0.12 0.10
Sleep cycle length 2 -{l.10 0.10 0
Abbreviations and symbols: ESs = effect sizes; REM = rapid eye movement; SE = standard error; SWS = slow wave sleep; , indicates
significant Z-test, p < 0.05; " indicates significant Z-test, p < 0.01 ; t indicates significant H-statistic, p < 0.05.

ESs were significantly different from zero (p < examined for these sleep measures. Individual
0.05) for stage 3, stage 4, SWS, REM sleep, sleep characteristics found to significantly influence ES
onset latency, total sleep time and REM latency included sex and fitness levels. The mean ES for
(see table IV). Positive ESs were observed for stage REM sleep was larger for women than for men,
3, stage 4, SWS, total sleep time and REM latency. F(2, 107) = 3.10, p < 0.05,0)2 = 0.06. The mean ES
Negative ESs were observed for REM sleep and for women was -0.37 (ESs n = 12), while for men
sleep onset latency. The largest effect was for stage it was -0.23 CESs n = 67). With regard to fitness ,
4 (ES = 0.75), while the smallest was for REM the mean ESs for both REM sleep, r(38) =-0.55,
sleep (ES = -0.14). These ESs were associated with p < 0.000 1, r2 = 0.30 and stage 4 sleep, r(22) = -0.57,
mean difference scores of 2 to 5 minutes for stage p < 0.003, r2 = 0.32, were larger for less fit than for
3, stage 4, SWS, REM sleep, and sleep onset la- more fit individuals. Additionally, examination of
tency; and of 10 minutes for total sleep time and the correlations among the various moderating fac-
REM latency. Thus, exercise individuals not only tors showed no significant relationship between fit-
fell asleep faster, but also slept somewhat longer ness and sex, r(281) =0.007, p > 0.05. There was,
and deeper than nonexercise individuals. however, a significant relationship between fitness
Although most of the mean ESs for acute exer- and age, r(205) = -0.40, P < 0.0001. Less fit indi-
cise were homogeneous, the H-statistics were sig- viduals tended to be older than those which were
nificant for stage 4 sleep, X2(78 degrees of freedom) more fit.
= 69.4, p < 0.05 and REM sleep, X2(l40) = 136.7, Exercise characteristics found to significantly
p < 0.05 (see table IV). Moderating variables were influence ES included exercise type, exercise situ-

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med. 1996 Apr; 21 (4)
Effects of Exercise on Sleep 285

ation, exercise duration and time of day. The mean 3. General Discussion
ES for REM sleep was negative (consistent with
overall findings) for aerobic exercise and positive 3.1 Summary of Findings
(inconsistent with overall findings) for anaerobic
exercise, F(3,106) = 13.3, P < 0.0001,0)2 = 0.27. In contrast to the conclusions of Trinder et al.,[2]
The mean ES for aerobic exercise was -0.28 (ESs the meta-analysis revealed significant acute, as
n = 83), while for anaerobic exercise it was 1.12 well as chronic, effects of exercise on sleep. The
(ESs n = 10). The mean ES for REM sleep was acute effects of exercise included increases in stage
3, stage 4, slow wave sleep, total sleep time and
larger when exercise took place in a laboratory set-
REM sleep latency, and decreases in sleep onset
ting than it was when exercise took place in a field
latency and REM sleep. The chronic effects of ex-
setting, F(2,99) = 4.32, P < 0.02, 0)2 = 0.08. The
ercise included increases in slow wave sleep and
mean ES for laboratory exercise was -0.45 (ESs
total sleep time, as well as decreases in sleep onset
n = 48), while for field exercise it was -0.10 (ESs
latency, REM sleep and awake time. Both acute
n = 58). Moreover, stage 4 ESs were larger when
and chronic exercise had similar effects with re-
exercise: (a) was longer in duration, r(34) = 0.61,
gard to slow wave sleep, total sleep time, REM
p < 0.0001, r2 = 0.37; and (b) was completed earlier sleep and sleep onset latency. Moreover, the mean
in the day, r(65) = -0.37, p < 0.001, r2 = 0.14. ESs were for the most part small to moderate in
2.2.2 Chronic Exercise magnitude. Mean difference scores and narrative
From the 12 studies examining the effects of review conclusions, where available, were direction-
chronic exercise, 274 ESs were calculated. The ally consistent with the mean ESs (i.e. those signif-
mean ESs were significantly different from zero icantly different from zero).
(p < 0.05) for SWS, REM sleep, sleep onset latency, The conflicting findings are probably not due to
total sleep time and awake time (see table IV). Pos- the impact of new data because 79% of the studies
itive ESs were observed for SWS and total sleep included in the meta-analysis were also included in
time. Negative ESs were observed for REM sleep, the review by Trinder et alP] It is more likely that
they are due to inadequate statistical power in
sleep onset latency and awake time. The largest
individual exercise/sleep studies. Given the mag-
effect was for total sleep time (ES = 0.94), while
nitude of the typical exercise/sleep effect (i.e.
the smallest was for awake time (ES = -0.40).
small to moderate ESs), relatively large samples
Thus, fit individuals not only fell asleep faster, but
would be necessary for adequate statistical power.
also slept somewhat deeper and longer (with less
For example, exercise/sleep effects with an ES of
awake time) than unfit individuals.
approximately 0.30 (i.e. total sleep time and REM
The mean ESs for chronic exercise were mostly latency) would need 83 individuals to achieve sta-
homogeneous. As table IV indicates, the H-statis- tistical significance. Unfortunately, sample sizes in
tics were significant only for REM sleep, X2(88) = the exercise/sleep literature average only 9 ± 3 in-
60.9, P < 0.01. Consequently, moderating variables dividuals. Given the size of the average exercise/
were examined for this sleep measure. The only sleep effect and the sample sizes typically used to
characteristics found to significantly influence ES examine these effects, it would appear that statis-
were sex and age. The mean ES was larger for tical power has probably been routinely compro-
women than for men, F(l,28) = 6.25, P < 0.02, mised. The conclusions reached by Trinder et al.
0)2 = 0.18. The mean ES for women was -1.29 (ESs seem largely to reflect this weakness.
n = 4), while for men it was -0.46 (ESs n = 26). In light of the seemingly modest ESs observed,
With regard to age, the ESs were larger for older the practical effects of exercise on sleep remain to
than for younger individuals, r(26) = 0.50, P < 0.004, be seen. With regard to potential health effects,
r2 = 0.25. exercise/sleep ESs are the same magnitude as those

© Adis International limited. All rights reserved . Sports Med. 1996 Apr; 21 (4)
286 Kubitz et al.

from a variety of medical interventions. For exam- sleep when: (a) the individuals are female, low fit,
ple, the mean ESs for medical interventions deemed or older; and (b) the exercise is longer duration,
effective for mortality reduction range from 0.08 aerobic exercise that is completed earlier in the day.
to 0.47, while those for medical interventions It seems important to keep in mind that the link
deemed effective for other health threatening prob- between fitness and the effects of acute exercise
lems range from 0.24 to 0.80.[55] Additionally, the (i.e. that higher fitness was associated with smaller
effects of exercise on sleep are fairly similar to effects) is consistent with what would be expected
those of exercise on depression, ES = 0.78,f56] stress given the 'law of initial values' common to psycho-
reactivity, ES = 0.48[57] and anxiety, ESs = 0.24 to physiological research. Fit individuals have al-
0.56.l58 ] Thus, exercise/fitness effects on sleep ready achieved the sleep pattern associated with
compare favourably with those of other health- chronic exercise (i.e. falling asleep faster, sleeping
related interventions. Moreover, the observed ESs deeper and sleeping longer). Therefore, they may
may be somewhat conservative since exercise/ simply have less 'room for improvement' .
sleep studies have generally examined individuals Furthermore, the analysis of moderating vari-
without sleep difficulties. ables allows several suggestions regarding future
Theoretically, exercise/sleep effects appear to research. First, experimental design should pro-
be generally consistent with the compensatory ceed with an eye towards obtaining satisfactory
position on sleep. Sleep after exercise was longer, statistical power. The meta-analytic ESs, which
deeper and more readily achieved. This was true have quantified the magnitude of the expected
for both short and long term exercise involvement. exercise/sleep effects, should facilitate optimal
experimental design. Secondly, it is important to
In fact, where both ESs were significant (SWS,
consider the influence of individual characteristics.
REM sleep, total sleep time and sleep onset la-
For example, even though the individual's sex and
tency) the chronic effects were larger than acute
age do not have very far reaching effects, they do
effects, suggesting that long term involvement
influence certain sleep measures (e.g. REM sleep),
maximises the beneficial effects of exercise. This
as does the cardiorespiratory fitness level. Thirdly,
has also been shown to be true for the effects of
it is also important to appropriately quantify and
exercise on anxiety and depression.l 56.58 ]
administer the exercise stimulus. Exercise duration
With a few exceptions, the concerns expressed
appears to be particularly important, as does the
by previous reviewers[7,8,IO] about methodological
time of day when exercise is completed.
influences on exercise/sleep outcomes were not
substantiated by the examination of moderating
3.2 Suggested Underlying Mechanisms
variables. Where moderating effects were signifi-
cant, they accounted for small to moderate (6 to Previous reviews[7,59] have hypothesised that
37%) amounts of the variance in ES. Among those exercise-induced peripheral and/or brain heating
accounting for at least 25% of the variance in ESs underlies effects on sleep. The evidence for the hy-
were the individual's fitness and age, as well as the pothesis is based on studies in which manipulations
type and duration of exercise. The effects of the of the thermogenic effects of exercise have differ-
individual's sex, the exercise situation and the time entially influenced sleep,[14,60] as well as on studies
of day were more limited, accounting for no more in which passive warming (e.g. warm baths) has
than 10 to 20% of the variance in ESs. ESs were produced exercise-like sleep effects.[61,62] Unfortu-
not influenced by either the internal validity of the nately, only 1 study[l] in the meta-analysis actually
study, the individual's body mass index, or the in- examined the effects of exercise on temperature
tensity of exercise. during sleep and it lacked the data necessary for ES
In general, the moderating variable analyses calculation. However, the mean difference score,
suggest that exercise has the biggest impact on Mdiff =0.02°F, suggested that there were no exer-

© Adis International limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med. 1996 Apr; 21 (4)
Effects of Exercise on Sleep 287

cise-related increases in temperature during sleep. calculated were positive, suggesting that exercise-
These data are consistent with other work demon- induced changes in heart rate and/or biochemical
strating that elevations in body temperature fol- activity may also be worthy of further attention.
lowing exercise typically return to normal within
90 minutes.l 63l Thus, it appears unlikely that tem- 4. Conclusions
perature during sleep would still be elevated, espe-
cially for morning or afternoon exercise, and if A strength of the meta-analytic review process
body heating underlies exercise/sleep effects, the is the fact that it synthesises the literature (all that
relationship is unlikely to be a direct one. can be located) on a topic. Thus, in the present
Others have suggested that certain fitness ef- review, exercise/sleep effects were combined across
fects (i.e. increases in lean body mass and maximal studies using, for example, different designs, types
oxygen consumption) underlie effects on sleep. of individuals and exercise types. Nonetheless,
The evidence for lean body mass is based on stud- exercise/sleep effects emerged in a manner consis-
ies[64- 66 l showing significant, albeit contradictory, tent with expectations derived from most of the
previous reviews. In terms of magnitude, the ef-
relationships between lean body mass and sleep.
fects of exercise on sleep are fairly similar to the
The evidence for cardiorespiratory fitness is mostly
other known psychophysiological effects of exer-
based on studies[67- 70 l showing significant cross-
cise.l56-581 Moreover, meta-analytical procedures
sectional differences in the sleep of fit and unfit
also facilitate a very useful teasing apart of metho-
individuals. However, the cross-sectional stud-
dological moderating factors. Certain study, indi-
ies[70,71 1 that failed to replicate these differences
vidual and exercise characteristics were found to
and the mixed results from longitudinal stud-
moderate exercise/sleep effects. Those with the
ies[69.72-74] leave the overall findings inconclusive.
strongest effects were the individual's age and fit-
Although the meta-analysis did not show a signif-
ness level, as well as the type and duration of ex-
icant relationship between the individual's body ercise. The characteristics of these studies are out-
mass index and ES, there was a significant relation- lined in table V.
ship between the individual's V'02max and ES. The Questions that remain to be addressed in future
relationship was an inverse one, in that as fitness research were also highlighted. Counting the num-
increased, REM sleep and stage 4 sleep ESs de- ber of studies and/or ESs per sleep measure reveals
creased. Thus, the meta-analysis provided some a paucity of evidence regarding the effects of ex-
support for the fitness effects hypothesis. ercise on temperature, heart rate, sleep efficiency,
Nonetheless, the question remains regarding the self-reported sleep quality, biochemical measures
mechanism by which exercise (and ultimately fit- and movement during sleep. Counting the number
ness) influences sleep. As mentioned above, the of studies and/or ESs for each of the moderating
effects of exercise appear unlikely to be directly variables shows a need for attention to exercise/
related to exercise-induced temperature increases sleep effects in women, with anaerobic exercise,
that linger on into and thereby influence sleep. How- with morning/evening exercise and in competitive
ever this does not necessarily mean that exercise- situations. Also needed are additional training
induced temperature increases are not part of the studies, particularly studies in which exercise stim-
mechanism by which exercise influences sleep. As uli and the corresponding fitness effects (i.e.
Trinder et al. [2] suggested, exercise-related temper- V'02max, body composition changes) are carefully
ature increases may indirectly influence sleep by monitored. Moreover, there is still a need to under-
altering circadian temperature rhythms. Moreover, stand the mechanisms underlying exercise/sleep
although there were not enough ESs available to effects. Hypotheses regarding temperature re-
examine the effects of exercise on heart rate and ceived little direct support, mostly due to the pau-
biochemical activity during sleep, the mean ESs city of research available. It appears that the effects

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med. 1996 Apr; 21 (4)
@ Table V. Characteristics of studies used in the meta·analysis N
00
» Exercise stimulus No, of individuals Age (y)
00
Q. Reference No, of ES type Sex V02max Exercise type Time of day
"'" ESs (mVkglmin) when exercise (minancl/or [mean/s or range]
~ completed (h) % of maximum)

~
0"
Browman & Tepasl75]
Adamson et alP 61
30
7
A
A
M
M
NR
NR
Aerobic
Mixed
NR
1400
45 (5 workl5 rest)
NR
9
12
18,9
19-31
::J
Q. Ziret alF7J 12 A M NR Mixed 1600 120; 360 10 17-23
r BaeklandI7. ) 14
104 A NR NR NR NR NR NR
3
Bunnell et a1.l79) 18 A M, F 48 Aerobic 1630 28 8 21-30
~ Bunnell et al. l' O] 38 A M,F 47 Aerobic 1700 151 (50 @50%,60%,70%) 9 21-30
~ Browman[81 ) 18 A M, F NR Nonaerobic NR 80 7 19·22
cO" Baekeland & Lasky(6) 16 A M NR NR NR NR 10 NR
-::r
or Shapiro et al.l42] 21 A M NR Aerobic 1400 120; 180; 240; 360 @ 50%; 160 @ 75% 2 24-26
Buguet et a1. 182] 4 A M 63 Aerobic 1700 35% 6 20,1
m
Torsvall et a1.l83]
~
Q.
66 A M NR Aerobic 1900 NR 6 30·35
Home & Moor"'4) 72 A F 55 Aerobic 1730 80@75% 6 20-23
Walker et alF' ] 134 A,C M NR Aerobic 1600 14;36 20 18-22
Paxton et al.l841 27 A M,F 29,32 Aerobic 1800 45 @ 50%; 60 @ 75% 24 21 ,7
A M 59 Aerobic 1800 60, 360(?%) 11 19,5
Kupfer et a1.l85] 42 A M NR Aerobic NR NR 10 24,8
Paxton et a1.l69) 6 A,C M 38,53 Aerobic NR NR 17 18-22
Griffin & Trinde~67] 4 A,C M, F 14, 22 Aerobic 1800 NR 8 23,0
Trinder et al.l86] 42 C M 68 Aerobic 40 22,3
46 Nonaerobic 22,5
55 Mixed 21 .1
Paxton et al.l64 ] 32 C M 32,47 Mixed 34 20.0
Home & StafflSO) 65 A M, F 62 Aerobic 1800 160 @ 40%; 80@ 80% 8 25.4
Matsumoto et a1. 187] 14 A M NR Aerobic 1700 140@ 50% 6 20-24
Walsh et a1.l88] 18 C F NR 11 20·33
Shapiro et a1F4) 12 C NR 51 , 55 8 17-21
Shapiro/89) 4 A NR NR Aerobic NR NR 6 NR
Montgomery et al.l68] 120 A,C M NR Aerobic NR 90 41 22,41
Montgomery et al.l90] 9 A M,F 51,59 Mixed NR NR 14 23.0
M 57 Aerobic NR NR 22 22,41
NR 46 Nonaerobic NR NR 10 23,0
Shapiro et a1.l9 ' ) 32 A M 43 Aerobic NR 15; 240 4 18-24
Meintjes et a1F3 ] 18 C F 32, 35 Mixed 8 20·28
Matsumoto et al.l92) 46 A M NR Aerobic 140@50%(over9h) 6 20-24
Jennings(93) 20 A, C F 26, 30 Aerobic NR 20@75% 9 27-43
Moses et al.l94) 11 A M NR Aerobic 600 (over 40h) 9 18·22
~ Driver et a1.l72) 28 A,C F 32, 45 Aerobic 1900 60@70% 8 18·28
a- Bevie~95) 68 A M, F 35 Aerobic 1600 6O@60% 14 57-72
'5::
~ Montgomery et al. (96) 4 A NR 59 Aerobic 1800 90; 171 8 40,8
Weydahl197J 3 C F NR 14 16-18
~» Buchegger et aI. 19.) 24 A M, F NR Nonaerobic 1500 NR 16 21-24 :><:
::::
sr.
Bunnell et al.l99) 8 A M, F 40,55 Aerobic 1700 149 @ 50-70% 9 21-30
~ Buchegger & Meier-KolIlHlOl 6 A M, F NR Nonaerobic 1900 NR 8 21-25
N
~
'" Abbreviations and symbol: A =acute; C =chronic; ESs =effect sizes; F =female; M =male; NR =not reported; ? =unknown,
~ ~
Effects of Exercise on Sleep 289

of exercise on circadian temperature rhythms may 5. Rechtschaffen A, Kales A. A manual of standardized terminol-
ogy, techniques and scoring system for sleep stages of human
be a viable hypothesis, as is the influence of exer- subjects. Washington (DC): Government Printing Office,
cise on cardiorespiratory fitness. Additional hypoth- 1968
eses suggested by the meta-analysis include the ef- 6. Baekeland F, Lasky, R. Exercise and sleep patterns in college
athletes. Percept Mot Skills 1966; 23: 1203-7
fects of exercise on heart rate and/or biochemical 7. Home JA. The effects of exercise upon sleep: a critical review.
activity during sleep. Bioi Psychol 1981; 12: 241-90
8. Torsvall L. Sleep after exercise: a literature review. J Sports
Additionally, recent reviews of the exercise/ Med 1981 ; 21: 218-25
sleep literature complement our findings . Driver 9. Shapiro CM. Sleep and the athlete. Br J Sports Med 1981 ; 15:
and Taylor[lOl] completed a narrative review cov- 51-5
10. Shapiro eM, Driver HS. Exercise and sleep - a review. NATO
ering 12 studies included in our paper, as well as 3 Colloquium, New York : Plenum Press, 1988
studies published since 1992. They focused on sev- II. Glass GY. Integrating findings : the meta-analysis of research.
eral exercise-related questions, including the gen- Rev Res Educ 1978; 5: 351-79
12. Sacks HS, Berrier K, Reitman 0 , et al. Meta-analysis of ran-
eral effects of sleep, the disrupting effects of in- domized controlled trials. N Eng J Med 1987; 316: 450-5
tense exercise of sleep, the effects of overtraining 13. Kubitz KA, Landers OM, Salazar W, et al. A meta-analytic
of sleep, and effects of disrupted sleep of exercise review of the effects of exercise on total sleep time. Psycho-
physiology 1989; 26: S40
performance. Driver and Taylor's conclusions 14. Horne JA, Moore Vl Sleep EEG effects of exercise with and
were consistent with our findings. They reported a without additional body cooling. Electroenceph Clin Neuro-
beneficial effect of exercise on sleep that was mod- physiol 1985; 60: 33-8
15. Glass GV, McGaw B, Smith ML. Meta-analysis in social re-
erated by factors related to the intensity and dura- search. Beverly Hills (CA): Sage Publications, 1981
tion of the exercise, as well as by factors related to 16. Thomas JR, French KE. The use of meta-analysis in exercise
the time of day that the exercise was completed. and sport: a tutorial. Res Q 1986; 57: 196-204
17. Hedges LY. Fitting categorical models to effect sizes from a
O'Connor and Youngstedt[102] completed a combi- series of experiments. J Educ Stat 1981; 7: 119-37
nation narrative/meta-analytic review of28 studies 18. Rosenthal R. Combining results of independent studies. Psy-
included in our paper, as well as 4 studies pub- chol Bull 1978; 85: 185-93
19. Hedges LV, Olkin J. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. New
lished since 1992. Although only a brief summary York: Academic Press, 1985
of the meta-analysis was presented in the paper, 20. Hauri P. The influence of evening activity on the onset of sleep.
their results were generally consistent with our Psychophysiology 1969; 5: 426-30
21. Home JA, Porter JM. Time of day effects with standardized
findings. O'Connor and Youngstedt reported a sig- exercise on subsequent sleep. Electroenceph Clin Neuro-
nificant increase in total sleep time (mean ES =0.31), physiol 1976; 40: 178-84
stage 2 sleep (mean ES = 0.19), SWS (mean ES = 22. Porter JM, Home JA. Exercise and sleep behaviour: a question-
naire approach. In: Koella WP, editor. Sleep 1980. Basel:
0.25), and REM latency (mean ES = 0.48), as well Karger, 1981; 406-7
as a decrease in REM sleep (mean ES = -0.48). 23. Maloletnev VI, Telia ZA. The influence of exercise on the night
sleep in man. Hum Animal Physiol1975; 77: 449-52
Thus, the exercise/sleep literature seems to con-
24. Revis CJ. Sleep differences in aerobically fit and unfit older
tinue to demonstrate small to moderate effects of adults [dissertation]. Mississippi: University of Southern
exercise on sleep moderated by various methodo- Mississippi , 1989
25. Shapiro CM. Effect of exercise on sleep. S Afr Sports Med
logical factors. 1979; 6: 9-12
26. Davies B, Shapiro CM, Daggett A, et al. Physiological changes
and sleep responses during and following a world record con-
References tinuous walking record. Sr J Sports Med 1984; 18: 173-80
I. Hauri P. Effects of evening activities on early night sleep. Psycho- 27. Zloty RB, Burdick JA, Adamson JD. Sleep of distance runners.
physiology 1968; 4: 267-77 Act Nerv Super 1973; 15: 217-21
2. Trinder J, Montgomery I, Paxton SJ. The effect of exercise on 28. Hasan J, Urponen H, Vuori I, et al. Exercise habits and sleep in
sleep: the negative view. Acta Physiol Scand 1988; 133 Suppl. a middle-aged Finnish popUlation. Acta Physiol Scand 1988;
574: 14-20 133: 33-5
3. Home JA. Human sleep and tissue restitution: some qualifica- 29. Shapiro CM, Bachmayer D. Epidemiological aspects of sleep
tions and doubts. Clin Sci 1983; 65: 569-78 in general public and hospital outpatient samples. Acta Phys-
4. Berger RJ. Slow wave sleep, shallow torpor and hibernation: iol Scand 1988; 133: 41-3
homologous states of diminished metabolism and body tem- 30. Vuori I, Urponen H, Hasan J, et al. Epidemiology of exercise
perature. Bioi Psychol 1984; 19: 305-26 effects on sleep. Acta Physiol Scand 1988; 133: 3-7

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med. 1996 Apr; 21 (4)
290 Kubitz et al.

31. Gary V, Gutherie G. The effect of jogging on physical fitness 56. North TC, McCullagh P, Tran Zv. Effect of exercise on depres-
and self concept in hospitalized alcoholics. J Stud Alcohol sion. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 1990; 18: 379-415
1972; 33: 1073-8 57. Crews OJ, Landers OM. A meta-analytic review of aerobic fit-
32. Porter JM, Horne JA. Exercise and sleep behaviour: a question- ness and reactivity to psychosocial stressors. Med Sci Sports
naire approach. Ergonomics 1981 ; 24: 511-21 Exerc 1987; 19: 114-20
33. Nakazawa Y, Hasuzawa H, Ohkawa T, et al. Slow wave sleep, 58. Petruzzello SJ, Landers OM, Hatfield BD, et al. Ameta-analysis
behavioural pattern and physical constitution. Sleep Res 1978; on the anxiety-reducing effects of acute and chronic exercise.
7:202 Sports Med 1991; II : 143-82
34. Shapiro CM, Bartel PR, Griesel RD. Effect offatigue on electro- 59. Horne JA. Why we sleep: the functions of sleep in humans and
cerebral excitability. Bioi Psychol 1980; I\: 282 other mammals. New York: Oxford University Press, 1988
35. Shapiro CM, Bortz R. Sleep after exercise in a hot environment. 60. Horne JA, Staff LHE. Exercise and sleep: body heating effects.
Sleep Res 1980; 9: 149 Sleep 1983; 6: 36-46
36. Shapiro CM, Bortz R, Mitchell 0, et al. Slow wave sleep: a 61. Horne JA, Reid AJ. Night-time sleep EEG changes following
recovery period after exercise. Science 1981; 214: 1253-4 body heating in a warm bath. Electroenceph Clin Neuro-
37. Torsvall L. EEG, catecholamines, and subjective ratings of physiol 1985; 60: 154-7
sleep after different degrees of daytime exercise: a pilot study 62. Horne JA , Shackell BS. Slow wave sleep elevations after body
with two fit subjects [report no. 146]. Reports from the Lab- heating: proximity to sleep and effects of aspirin. Sleep 1987;
oratory for Clinical Stress Research; 1981 Feb: 1-12 10: 383-92
38. Browman CP, Cartwright RD. Exercise and sleep. Sleep Res 63. Hori M, Mayzumi M, Tanaka N, et al. Oxygen intake of men
1980; 9: 144 and women during exercise and recovery in a hot environment
39. Iguchi Y, Kobayashi T, Yamamoto T. Slow wave sleep due to and a comfortable environment. In: Folinsbee LJ, Wagner JA,
daytime activities and individual differences. Jpn J Psychiatry Borgia JF, et aI., editors. Environmental stress. New York:
Neurol 1988; 42: 165-6 Academic Press, 1978: 39-52
40. Roussel B, Buguet A. Changes in human heart rate during sleep 64. Paxton SJ, Trinder J, Shapiro C, et al. Effect of physical fitness
following daily physical exercise. Eur J Appl Physiol 1982; and body composition on sleep and sleep related hormone
49: 409-16 concentrations. Sleep 1984; 7: 339-46
41. Ryback RS, Lewis OF. Effects of prolonged bedrest on EEG 65. Paxton SJ, Trinder J, Montgomery T, et al. Body composition
sleep patterns in young, healthy volunteers. Electroenceph and human sleep. Aust J Psychol 1984; 36: 181-9
Clin Neurophysiol 1971; 31: 395-9
66. Shapiro CM, Catterall J, Warren P, et al. Lean body mass and
42. Shapiro CM, Griesel RD, Bartel PR, et al. Sleep patterns after non-rapid eye movement sleep. BMJ 1987 ; 294: 22
graded exercise. J Appl Physiol 1975; 39: 187-90
67. Griffin SJ, Trinder J. Physical fitness, exercise and human sleep.
43. Horne JA, Porter JM. Exercise and human sleep. Nature 1975;
Psychophysiology 1978; 15: 447-50
256: 573-5
68. Montgomery I, Trinder J, Fraser G, et al. Aerobic fitness and
44. Bonnet MH. Sleep performance and mood after the energy ex-
exercise: effects on the sleep of younger and older adults. Aust
penditure equivalent of 40 hours of sleep deprivation. Psycho-
J Psycho I 1987: 39: 259-71
physiology 1980; 17: 56-63
45. Boland BD, Dewsbury DA. Characteristics of sleep following 69. Paxton SJ, Trinder J, Montgomery I. Does aerobic fitness affect
sleep? Psychophysiology 1983; 20: 320-4
sexual activity and wheel running in male rats. Physiol Behav
1971; 6: 145-9 70. Trinder J, Bruck 0, Paxton S, et al. Physical fitness, exercise,
46. Harma MI, I1marinen J, Knauth P, et al. Physical training inter- age and human sleep. Aust J Psychol 1982; 34: 131-8
vention in female shift workers: I. The effects of intervention 71. Walker JM, Floyd TC, Fein G, et al. Effects of exercise on sleep.
of fitness fatigue, sleep, and psychosomatic symptoms. Ergo- J Appl Physiol 1978; 44: 945-51
nomics 1988; 3 \: 39-50 72. Driver HS, Meintjes AF, Rogers GG, et al. Submaximal exercise
47. Trinder J, Stevenson J, Paxton SJ, et al. Physical fitness, exer- effects on sleep patterns in young women before and after an
cise and REM sleep cycle length. Psychophysiology 1982; 19: aerobic training program. Acta Physiol Scand 1988; 133: 8-13
89-93 73. Meintjes AF, Driver HS, Shapiro CM. Improved physical fitness
48. Hobson JA. Sleep after exercise. Science 1968; 162: 1503-5 failed to alter the EEG patterns of sleep in young women. Eur
49. Trinder J, Bruck 0, Paxton SJ, et al. Physical fitness, exercise, J Appl Physiol 1989; 59: 123-7
age and human sleep. Aust J Psychol 1982; 34: 131-8 74. Shapiro CM, Warren PM, Trinder J, et al. Fitness facilitates
50. Montgomery I, Trinder J, Paxton S. Energy expenditure and sleep. Eur J Appl Physiol 1984; 53: 1-4
total sleep time : effect of physical exercise. Sleep 1982; 5: 75. Browman CP, Tepas DI. The effects of presleep activity on all-
159-68 night sleep. Psychophysiology 1976; 13: 536-40
51. Hauri P. Effects of evening activities on subsequent sleep and 76. Adamson L, Hunter WM, Ogunremi 00, et al. Growth hormone
dreams [dissertation]. Chicago: University of Chicago, 1966 increase during sleep after daytime exercise. J Endocrin 1974;
52. Desjardins J, Healey T, Broughton R. Early evening exercise 62: 473-8
and sleep. Sleep Res 1974; 3: 31 77. Zir LM, Smith RA, Parker DC. Human growth hormone release
53. Stevenson JS, Topp R. Effects of moderate and low intensity in sleep: effect of daytime exercise. J Clin Endocrinol 1971;
long-term exercise by older adults. Res Nurs Health 1990; 13: 32: 662-5
209-18 78. Baekeland F. Exercise deprivation. Sleep and psychological re-
54. Shapiro CM, Vershoor GJ. Sleep patterns after a marathon. S actions. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1970; 22: 365-9
Afr J Sci 1979; 75: 415-6 79. Bunnell 0, Bevier WC, Horvath SM. Nocturnal sleep, cardio-
55. Lipsey MW, Wilson DB. The efficacy of psychological, edu- vascular function, and adrenal activity following maximum
cational, and behavioral treatment. Am Psychol 1993; 48: capacity exercise. Electroenceph Clin Neurophysiol1983; 56:
1181-209 186-9

© Adis International limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med. 1996 Apr; 21 (4)
Effects of Exercise on Sleep 291

80. Bunnell D, Bevier WC, Horvath SM. Effects of exhaustive 93. Jennings SE. The effect of a 9-week conditioning program on
exercise on the sleep of men and women . Psychophysiology slow wave sleep and mood states of physically inactive
1983; 20: 50-8 women [dissertation). University Park (PA): Pennsylvania
81. Browman CPo Sleep following sustained exercise. Psychophys- State University, 1981
iology 1980; 17: 577-80 94. Moses J, Lubin A, Naitoh P, et a!. Exercise and sleep loss:
82. Buguet A, Roussel B, Angus R, et al. Human sleep and adrenal effects on recovery sleep. Psychophysiology 1977; 14: 414-6
individual reactions to exercise. Electroenceph Clin Neuro- 95. Bevier We. Sleep of older adults and the effects of exercise
physiol 1980; 49: 515-23 [dissertation). Santa Barbara (CA): University of California,
83. Torsvall L, Akerstedt T, Lindbeck G. Effects on sleep stages and 1985
EEG power density of different degrees of exercise in fit sub- 96. Montgomery I, Trinder J, Paxton S, et a!. Sleep disruption fol-
jects. Electroenceph Clin Neurophysiol1984; 57: 347-53 lowing a marathon. J Sports Med 1985; 25: 69-74
84. Paxton SJ, Montgomery I, Trinder J, et al. Sleep after exercise 97. Weydahl A. Sleep-quality among girls with different involve-
of variable intensity in fit and unfit subjects. Aust J Psychol ment in competitive sports during fall in the Arctic Circle.
1982; 34: 289-96 Percept Mot Skills 1991; 73: 883-92
85. Kupfer DJ, Sewitch DE, Epstein LH, et al. Exercise and sub-
98. Buchegger R, Fritsch R, Meier-Koll A, et a!. Does trampolining
sequent sleep in male runners: failure to support the slow wave
and anaerobic physical fitness affect sleep? Percept Mot
sleep-mood hypothesis. Neuropsychobiolgy 1985; 14: 5-12
Skills 1991; 73: 243-52
86. Trinder J, Paxton SJ, Montgomery I, et al. Endurance as op-
posed to power training: their effect on sleep. Psychophysi- 99. Bunnell DE, Bevier WC, Horvath SM. Effects of exhaustive
ology 1985; 22: 668-73 submaximal exercise on cardiovascular function during sleep.
87. Matsumoto K, Saito Y, Abe M, et al. The effects of daytime J Appl Physiol 1985; 58: 1909-13
exercise on night sleep. J Human Ergo11984; 13: 31-6 100. Buchegger J, Meier-Koll A. Motor learning and ultradian sleep
88. Walsh BT, Puig-Antich J, Goetz R, et al. Sleep and growth hor- cycle: an electroencephalographic study of trampoliners. Per-
mone secretion in women athletes. Electroenceph Clin Neuro- cept Mot Skills 1988; 67: 635-45
physiol 1984; 57: 528-31 101. Driver HS, Taylor SR. Sleep disturbances and exercise. Sports
89. Shapiro CM. Sleepiness of the long-distance runner. J Physiol Med 1996; 21: 1-6
1978; 276: 50-1 102. O'Connor PJ, Youngstedt MA. Influence of exercise on human
90. Montgomery I, Trinder J, Paxton S, et al. Physical exercise and sleep. In: Holloszy JO, editor. Exercise and sport sciences
sleep: the effect of age and sex of the subjects and type of reviews. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins, 1995: 105-34
exercise. Acta Physiol Scand 1988; 133: 36-40
91. Shapiro CM, Bortz R, Mitchell D, et a!. Effect of exercise in a
hot environment on human sleep patterns. S Afr J Sci 1985;
81: 624-7
92. Matsumoto K, Saito Y, Furmi K, et a!. Effect of night exercise Correspondence and reprints: Dr Karla A. Kubitz, Depart-
during 27 -hr total sleep deprivation on the subsequent sleep. ment of Kinesiology, Kansas State University, Manhattan,
Percept Mot Skills 1985; 60: 915-24 KS 66506, USA.

© Adis International Limited. All rights reserved. Sports Med. 1996 Apr; 21 (4)

You might also like