You are on page 1of 14

Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Brain and Cognition


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c

Differential effects of differing intensities of acute exercise on speed and accuracy


of cognition: A meta-analytical investigation
Terry McMorris ⇑, Beverley J. Hale
Chichester Centre of Applied Sport and Exercise Sciences, University of Chichester, College Lane, Chichester, West Sussex PO19 6PE, United Kingdom

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The primary purpose of this study was to examine, using meta-analytical techniques, the differential
Accepted 3 September 2012 effects of differing intensities of acute exercise on speed and accuracy of cognition. Overall, exercise dem-
Available online 11 October 2012 onstrated a small, significant mean effect size (g = 0.14, p < 0.01) on cognition. Examination of the com-
parison between speed and accuracy dependent variables showed that speed accounted for most of the
Keywords: effect. For speed, moderate intensity exercise demonstrated a significantly larger mean effect size than
Central executive those for low and high intensities. For speed of processing during moderate intensity exercise, central
Catecholamines
executive tasks showed a larger effect size than recall and alertness/attention tasks; and mean effect size
Brain derived neurotrophic factor
Prefrontal cortex
for counterbalanced or randomized studies was significantly greater than for studies in which a pre-exer-
Response time cise followed by during or post-exercise protocol was used. There was no significant difference between
Arousal mean effect sizes when testing took place post-exercise compared to during exercise for speed but accu-
racy studies demonstrated a significantly larger mean effect size post-exercise. It was concluded that
increased arousal during moderate intensity exercise resulted in faster speed of processing. The very lim-
ited effect on accuracy may be due to the failure to choose tests which are complex enough to measure
exercise-induced changes in accuracy of performance.
Ó 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction II errors could not be dismissed. To some extent, the problems of


small sample sizes can be overcome by the use of meta-analytical
Early studies examining the effect of exercise on cognition were techniques, which allow the investigation of hypotheses based on
atheoretical (e.g., Gutin & Di Gennaro, 1968; McAdam & Wang, effect sizes rather than probabilities. Hence the use of meta-
1967). The first to offer a theoretical rationale was Davey (1973), analytical techniques in the present study. However, a recent
who reasoned that exercise was a stressor which would induce meta-analysis (McMorris, Sproule, Turner, & Hale, 2011) raised
changes in arousal levels. He, therefore, drew on Yerkes and an issue which may affect the present study. Those authors exam-
Dodson’s (1908) arousal–performance interaction theory to ined the effect of acute, moderate intensity exercise on working
hypothesize an inverted-U effect of acute exercise on cognitive memory tasks and found that speed and accuracy of processing
performance, with moderate intensity exercise inducing signifi- were differentially affected. Speed showed a large effect size which
cantly better performance than low and high intensities. While demonstrated facilitation, while accuracy demonstrated a small
meta-analyses (Etnier et al., 1997; Lambourne & Tomporowski, effect size which signified a detrimental effect.
2010) have shown small but significant mean effects sizes for an An obvious possibility, if speed of cognition improved while
acute exercise effect on cognition, narrative reviewers (Brisswalter, accuracy deteriorated, is that a speed-accuracy trade-off occurred.
Collardeau, & Arcelin, 2002; McMorris & Graydon, 2000; Tompo- McMorris et al. (2011) ruled this out after examining studies in
rowski, 2003; Tomporowski & Ellis, 1986) have failed to find evi- which P3 latency and response time were both measured. In nor-
dence for an inverted-U effect. The conclusions reached by the mal circumstances, the individual classifies and evaluates the stim-
reviewers were based on probability data. However, McMorris uli, and then chooses the response, as well as preparing the
(2009b) urged caution with regard to dismissing the inverted-U movement. If an individual decides to increase speed at the
effect, as he pointed out that, in most studies, sample sizes were expense of accuracy, the response and motor preparation are pre-
very small and effect sizes suggested that the possibility of Type determined regardless of stimulus classification and evaluation.
Response time involves speed of stimulus classification and evalu-
ation, response selection and motor preparation (Kutas, McCarthy,
⇑ Corresponding author. Address: 63 Four Winds Court, Hartlepool TS26 0LP, & Donchin, 1977), while P3 latency only measures speed of stimu-
United Kingdom. lus classification and evaluation. Therefore, if decreases in P3
E-mail address: t.mcmorris@chi.ac.uk (T. McMorris).

0278-2626/$ - see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2012.09.001
T. McMorris, B.J. Hale / Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351 339

latency are mirrored by decreases in response time, this would heart rate reserve, percent maximum aerobic power, percent ven-
suggest that these changes are due to exercise-induced increases tilatory threshold and percent lactate threshold power, the exer-
in arousal (Yanagisawa et al., 2010). However, if there are changes cise physiology and exercise endocrinology literatures were
in response time but not P3, it would appear that response selec- examined to ascertain whether or not the intensity would be with-
tion and motor preparation had taken place before stimulus classi- in the 40–79% W _ MAX limits. It should be noted that there is an ele-
fication and evaluation. In other words, speed is favored over ment of subjectivity using this method. Measures <40% W _ MAX were
accuracy. McMorris et al. (2011) found that none of the research _
deemed to be light exercise and P80% W MAX heavy.
they examined showed changes in response time but not P3. More-
over, results for tasks which do not measure speed plus accuracy, 1.1. Moderator variables
e.g. random number generation, and reading span and operation
span, showed similar results for accuracy as did those tasks in Moderator variables were determined following examination of
which a speed-accuracy trade off is possible, e.g. flanker task and the theoretical and empirical literature. The first moderator
Stroop color test. Furthermore, several studies have demonstrated variable to be examined was timing of testing (during versus
significantly faster speed of processing with no detrimental effect post-exercise). Given that the half-life of catecholamines in the
on accuracy of processing (e.g., Arcelin, Brisswalter, & Delignières, periphery is only 3 min, some authors have argued that effects
1997; Davranche & McMorris, 2009; Delignières, Brisswalter, & would only be seen during exercise or for a very short period of time
Legros, 1994; Joyce, Graydon, McMorris, & Davranche, 2009; following exercise (Dietrich, 2003; McMorris & Graydon, 2000).
McMorris & Graydon, 1996a, 1996b, 1997). Lambourne and Tomporowski (2010), however, showed higher
Based on Cooper’s (1973) assertion that acute exercise induces effect sizes for post-exercise testing than for testing during exer-
increases in brain concentrations of the catecholamines neuro- cise. It should be noted that they did not carry out any statistical
transmitters, dopamine and norepinephrine, McMorris et al. comparisons between during and post-exercise effect sizes. More-
(2011) claimed that the increased neurotransmitter concentrations over, Joyce et al. (2009) showed improved cognitive performance
would have a positive effect on speed of processing but possibly on a Stop-signal task as much as 50 min following cessation of exer-
induce neural noise in areas of the brain involved in accuracy of cise. Therefore, we decided to examine the hypothesis that testing
processing working memory tasks. Although they proffered this during exercise would differ from testing following exercise.
explanation with regard to moderate intensity exercise and work- The second moderator variable to be examined was task com-
ing memory tasks only, observation of the literature covered for plexity (central executive tasks versus recall and attention/alert-
the present study suggested that a similar effect may be occurring ness tasks). Several authors (Dietrich, 2003; Luft, Takase, &
when analysis is extended to all intensities of exercise and all types Darby, 2009; McMorris & Graydon, 2000) have claimed that com-
of cognitive task. Therefore, an initial examination of whether plex tasks are more likely to be affected by exercise than more sim-
exercise would have differential effects on speed and accuracy of ple tasks. The argument that complex tasks will be more
processing was undertaken. It was decided that if, as hypothesized, susceptible to both positive and negative affects of acute exercise
there were a significant difference between effect sizes when speed is based on the interaction between central executive tasks, brain
was the dependent variable compared to when accuracy was the function and stress. Central executive tasks require more in the
dependent variable, further statistical analyses would be under- way of prefrontal cortex activation than other tasks (Leh, Petrides,
taken for the two variables separately. & Strafella, 2010) and the prefrontal cortex is more sensitive to
Based on the theories that moderate intensity exercise induces stress than other areas of the brain (Arnsten, 2009; Ramos &
increases in arousal (Davey, 1973) and brain neurotransmitter con- Arnsten, 2007). Exercise is commonly used as a stressor in animal
centrations (Cooper, 1973), it was hypothesized that, for speed, ef- and human studies (e.g., Can et al., 2012; Kirschbaum, Scherer, &
fect sizes as a result of acute, moderate intensity exercise would Strasburger, 1994; Tian, Nie, Tong, & Baker, 2012), therefore these
demonstrate significantly larger effect sizes than those induced authors hypothesized that central executive tasks would be more
by low and heavy intensity exercise. Given that the results of susceptible to the effects of exercise than would the more simple
McMorris et al.’s (2011) meta-analysis question the beneficial recall and attention/alertness tasks.
effects of moderate intensity exercise on accuracy data, no The possibility of a ceiling effect on some more simple tasks,
directional hypothesis was proposed. However, both cognitive- such as simple reaction time or even choice reaction time, cannot
energetical (e.g., Kahneman, 1973; Sanders, 1983) and neuroen- be ruled out, while working memory tasks have been shown to
docrinological theories (Cooper, 1973; McMorris, 2009a) would be significantly affected by acute, moderate intensity exercise
suggest a hypothesis that different intensities of exercise would re- (McMorris et al., 2011). Baddeley (1986) divided working memory
sult in significantly different effect sizes. If the claims of McMorris into the central executive and two short-term memory compo-
et al. (2011) are correct, it is most likely that low intensity exercise nents. While McMorris et al. (2011) examined all working memory
would induce the largest effect size. In a previous meta-analysis, tasks, in this study we decided to examine central executive tasks
Etnier et al. (1997) tested for an inverted-U effect between short only because these tasks are thought to be the most susceptible to
duration (2–15 min), moderate intensity; longer duration effects of stress (Barbas, 2000; Ramos & Arnsten, 2007). We com-
(>15 min), moderate intensity; light exercise; and ‘‘difficult’’ exer- pared these to the other tasks used, recall or short-term memory,
cise (p. 262), which presumably means heavy, but showed no sup- visual search, reaction time, soccer decision making, simple arith-
port for the hypothesis. Exact details of what constituted each of metic and coincidence anticipation. Such tasks require limited pre-
the categories are not provided. frontal cortex activity and are more concerned with attention and
In the present study, we used Borer’s (2003) classification of alertness (Dietrich & Audiffren, 2011; Rektor et al., 2003). We
low, moderate and heavy exercise, and the intensities used in the hypothesized that central executive tasks would demonstrate a
acute exercise-cognition research to determine the exercise inten- larger mean effect size than the recall and attention/alertness
sities. Moderate intensity exercise was determined as being tasks.
between 40% and 79% maximum power output (W _ MAX ) or equiva- The third moderator variable to be examined was a protocol is-
lent. If W_ MAX values were not presented but percent volume of sue, the use of counterbalancing/randomization of testing (coun-
maximum oxygen uptake (VO2MAX) or percent maximum heart terbalanced/random testing versus pre-exercise testing followed
rate were given, the conversion formulae of Arts and Kuipers by during or post-exercise testing). Research methods texts all rec-
(1994) were applied. For other indicators of intensity, e.g. percent ommend counterbalancing or randomization but many studies in
340 T. McMorris, B.J. Hale / Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351

this field have tested using the same order (pre-exercise followed dependent variable type, speed versus accuracy, was significant,
by during or post-exercise) throughout the experiments. The pos- all further analyses would be undertaken separately for speed
sibility of a learning or habituation effect is obvious. Some authors and accuracy.
(e.g., McMorris & Graydon, 1996a, 1996b, 1997) carried out preli- The first analyses, with speed and accuracy data separate, were
minary, reliability studies on their cognitive tests to show that comparisons between effect sizes for low, moderate and heavy
pre-/post-exercise testing would not affect results, while others exercise, regardless of whether they came from studies that exam-
(e.g. Brisswalter, Arcelin, Audiffren, & Delignières, 1997; Collar- ined one, two or three intensities. There were 10 effect sizes for
deau, Brisswalter, & Audiffren, 2001; Delignières et al., 1994) have low intensity, 39 for moderate and 17 for heavy exercise, for speed:
used tests in which there is no feedback, presumably believing that and 12, 28, and 16 for low, moderate and heavy, respectively, for
this removes the necessity to counterbalance or randomize. As a accuracy. Following this, three moderator variables were exam-
large number of studies have used the same order, we felt that it ined. The moderator variables identified were timing of testing
was necessary to compare mean effect sizes for studies using coun- (during versus post-exercise); task complexity (central executive
terbalanced/randomized designs compared to those using pre- task versus recall and attention/alertness tasks); and counterbal-
exercise followed by during or/post-exercise testing and we ancing of testing (counterbalanced/random testing versus pre-
hypothesized that the latter would show larger mean effect sizes. exercise testing followed by during or post-exercise testing).
Other possible moderator variables were considered, especially
fitness level which might interact with neuroendocrine responses
2.1. Data analyses
to exercise and cognitive perceptions of distress, but there were
not enough studies measuring these variables and meeting the cri-
Where means and SDs were available, effect sizes were calcu-
teria for inclusion to warrant examination by meta-analysis.
lated using Cohen’s d formula for matched pairs (see Borenstein
et al., 2009, pp. 28–30). Where means and SDs were only provided
graphically, an initial attempt was made to estimate their values
2. Materials and methods
but inter-and intra-experimenter reliability were unsatisfactory,
therefore these data were not included. Where means and SDs
A literature search using the computer data bases PsycArticles, p
were not available, d was calculated from t (d = 2t/ (df)). As accu-
PsycINFO, Pubmed, SportsDISCUS and Web of Knowledge was
racy data were sometimes reported with accuracy as the depen-
undertaken. Key words used in the searches were combinations
dent variable and sometimes with error as the dependent
of ‘‘acute’’, ‘‘exercise’’, ‘‘cognition’’, ‘‘cognitive function’’, ‘‘cognitive
variable, all effect sizes were transposed to show changes in accu-
performance’’, ‘‘reaction time’’, ‘‘response time’’, ‘‘accuracy’’, ‘‘error’’,
racy, i.e. positive effect sizes indicate better accuracy. Similarly,
‘‘working memory’’, ‘‘short-term memory’’, ‘‘long-term memory’’,
where speed of responding was the dependent variable, effect sizes
‘‘memory’’, ‘‘recall’’, ‘‘executive function’’, ‘‘central executive’’, ‘‘odd-
were multiplied by 1 so that positive effect sizes demonstrated
ball’’, ‘‘visual search’’, ‘‘attention’’, ‘‘anticipation’’, ‘‘coincidence antici-
an improvement in performance. Each individual d was then trans-
pation’’, ‘‘decision making’’, ‘‘flanker task’’, ‘‘Simon task’’, ‘‘Sternberg
formed to the bias-corrected standardized mean difference,
test’’, ‘‘Wisconsin card sorting task’’, ‘‘Tower of London’’ and ‘‘Tower
Hedges’ g, by applying the correction factor J (J = 1  (3/4df  1))
of Hanoi’’. In addition, reference lists from empirical reports and re-
and this was used to calculate a mean effect size using the ran-
views were examined. Studies were included if they were per-
dom-effects model. Orwin’s (1983) Fail-safe N was calculated
formed on healthy individuals; the exercise intervention required
when the mean effect size g was P0.02. Results of the Q test for
the activation of large muscle groups; repeated measures, with-
homogeneity were calculated and reported as was s2, which is a
in-subject design, was used; data were provided showing the
measure of absolute variance whereas Q is a measure of relative
intensity of the exercise with reference to a maximum or thresh-
variance (Borenstein et al., 2009, p. 121). Where sub-group analy-
old; exercise intensity was based on objective measures; the range
_ MAX or equiv- ses were undertaken with two groups, effect sizes for each group
of measures for a specific intensity was not P10% W
were compared using a Z-test on the differences with a random-ef-
alent; and the dependent variables were objective. Studies includ-
fects model, with separate estimates of s2 for each sub-group
ing pharmacological treatments were not included.
(Borenstein et al., 2009, pp. 149–186). Where there were three
Most studies provided more than one effect size and the depen-
sub-groups, an omnibus measure of Q based on analysis of variance
dent variables were either speed of response or accuracy/errors. In
was calculated with a random-effects model, with separate esti-
order to limit bias, one effect size per study was calculated from
mates of s2 for each subgroup (see Borenstein et al., 2009, pp.
those studies in which either speed or accuracy were the only
169–170), and post hoc Z-tests on the differences, with the Bonfer-
dependent variable and where only one exercise intensity was
roni correction factor. The proportion of variance explained by the
undertaken. Some studies examined both dependent variables
moderator variable, R2, was calculated (Borenstein et al., 2009, pp.
and we wished to test the hypothesis that effect sizes, with speed
179–183).
as the dependent variable, would be significantly different from
those where accuracy was the dependent variable. Similarly, some
studies measured more than one exercise intensity and we wished 3. Theory
to compare these. Therefore, it was decided to calculate separate
effect sizes for speed and accuracy at each intensity. This unfortu- The first to proffer a theoretical underpinning for an acute exer-
nately biases the importance of these studies (see Borenstein, cise-cognition interaction was Davey (1973), who hypothesized an
Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009, pp. 226–227). Therefore, fol- inverted-U effect as he saw exercise as a stressor, which would in-
lowing an initial analysis, a sub-group comparison was undertaken duce increases in arousal as intensity rose. In line with Yerkes and
in which effect sizes from studies which only measured either Dodson’s (1908) theory, he claimed that moderate intensity exer-
speed or accuracy at one exercise intensity (one effect size in our cise would induce optimal cognitive performance, while low and
initial analysis) were compared to the effect sizes from studies high intensities would have negative effects. Other researchers
which measured both speed and accuracy, and/or at more than (e.g., Arcelin et al., 1997; Delignières et al., 1994) drew on later
one exercise intensity (more than one effect size in the initial anal- arousal–performance interaction theories such as those of
ysis). Following this a sub-group analysis between speed and accu- Kahneman (1973) and Sanders (1983). These theorists also hypoth-
racy effect sizes was undertaken. If the comparison between esized an inverted-U effect, although they claimed that it was
T. McMorris, B.J. Hale / Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351 341

possible for performance at low levels of arousal to be as good as at of norepinephrine only. The authors concluded that the CSF in-
moderate levels if cognitive effort were able to allocate sufficient crease in norepinephrine concentrations was probably due to
resources to the task. During high levels of arousal, however, neu- activity in the locus coeruleus. This may have been the result of va-
ral noise would make it impossible for effort to allocate sufficient gal/nucleus tractus solitarii pathway activation by circulating cat-
resources to the task. echolamines. The locus coeruleus has extensive connections to
More recently, Dietrich (Dietrich, 2003; Dietrich & Audiffren, most other parts of the brain (Miyashita & Williams, 2006).
2011) put forward the reticular activation hypofrontality theory. Increases in brain concentrations of catecholamines during and
According to this theory, during moderate intensity exercise acti- following moderate intensity exercise should theoretically facili-
vation of the reticular system will increase alertness and arousal tate cognition. During and following low intensity exercise perfor-
leading to improved performance of well-learned and habitual mance will be inhibited due to limited activation in the relevant
tasks. However, heavy exercise requires greater activation of the brain areas. During and following heavy exercise, the much larger
premotor cortex and supplementary motor area, therefore, if exer- increases in catecholamines will lead to neural noise, which will
cise intensity is to be maintained, these areas will be activated at inhibit performance (Arnsten & Goldman-Rakic, 1985). It should
the expense of the prefrontal cortex, hence poorer cognitive perfor- be noted, however, that high concentrations of brain catechola-
mance of tasks requiring prefrontal cortex activation. McMorris mines have been shown to enhance memory consolidation by
(2009a) agreed with Dietrich except that he claimed that, during the hippocampus in rats (Hopkins & Johnston, 1988). In humans,
heavy exercise, it will be the limbic system that is activated at this may only apply to emotionally charged information (Payne
the expense of the prefrontal cortex due to increased anxiety. et al., 2007; Qin, Hermans, van Marle, & Fernández, 2012). Further
Closely linked to these theories is the catecholamines hypothe- complicating matters is the fact that exercise P80% VO2MAX in-
sis, first proposed by Cooper (1973), which states that during, and duces increases in plasma concentrations of the stress hormones,
even immediately before, exercise the hypothalamus and brain- adrenocorticotropin hormone (ACTH) and cortisol (De Vries, Ber-
stem initiate action of the sympathoadrenal system. This results nards, De Rooij, & Koppeschaar, 2000; Hill et al., 2008; McMorris,
in the release of catecholamines at the postganglionic cells of those Davranche, Jones, Hall, & Minter, 2009). Rodent studies have
neurons that require activating or inhibiting. As exercise increases shown evidence of acute exercise-induced increases in brain con-
in intensity from low to moderate there is also release of epineph- centrations of corticosterone (Droste et al., 2008) and increased
rine and, to a lesser extent norepinephrine, into the blood from the corticotropin releasing factor messenger ribonucleic acid (mRNA)
adrenal medulla. Peripherally, catecholamines play important roles expression in the brain (Hand et al., 2002; Jiang et al., 2004: Yan-
in the organism’s ability to undertake exercise, while centrally agita et al., 2007) following acute exercise. Corticotropin releasing
dopamine and norepinephrine activate the primary motor cortex, factor, ACTH and cortisol interact with norepinephrine and dopa-
premotor cortex and supplementary motor area during exercise. mine in the brain, resulting in increased catecholamines neuro-
It has long been thought that there is an interaction between transmitter synthesis and release, hence increasing the potential
the release of catecholamines into the periphery and increases in for neural noise.
brain concentrations. Despite the presence of the blood–brain bar- Although theoretically moderate intensity exercise appears to
rier, there is a direct effect of peripheral epinephrine and norepi- be beneficial, there is a problem with regard to moderate intensity
nephrine on synthesis and release of norepinephrine in the brain. exercise. The intensity at which increases in brain catecholamines
Peripheral epinephrine and norepinephrine activate b-adrenore- concentrations begin is a contentious issue. Chmura, Nazar, and
ceptors on vagal afferent nerves, which runs from the abdomen Kaciuba-Uscilko (1994) argued that the point at which exercise
through the chest, neck and head, and terminates in the nucleus would induce an improvement in cognition would be at, or shortly
tractus solitarii within the blood–brain barrier. Noradrenergic cells after, peripheral plasma concentrations of norepinephrine and epi-
in the nucleus tractus solitarii project into the locus coeruleus and nephrine began to rise exponentially. Green, Hughson, Orr, and
stimulate norepinephrine synthesis and release to other parts of Ranney (1983) termed the points at which plasma concentrations
the brain (Miyashita & Williams, 2006). There is also feedback to of norepinephrine and epinephrine show significant increases,
the hypothalamus via the thalamus, reticular activation system the norepinephrine threshold and the epinephrine threshold. Im-
and limbic system, concerning stress on the cardiorespiratory sys- proved cognition has been shown at, or just after, these thresholds
tem, pain and glycogen depletion. This triggers a response by the were reached (Chmura et al., 1994; McMorris et al., 1999) but find-
hypothalamus, which results in increased release of catechola- ings are not unequivocal (McMorris et al., 2003, 2000). It is gener-
mines peripherally and possibly centrally. Release centrally may ally thought that, although there are huge inter-individual
well also be due to the individual perceiving the situation as being variations (Urhausen, Weiler, Coen, & Kindermann, 1994), intensity
unpredictable and/or one in which he/she is not in control (Mason, needs to be 75+% VO2MAX (Podolin, Munger, & Mazzeo, 1991),
1975a, 1975b), which has been shown to induce the release of cat- which according to Arts and Kuipers (1994) equates to 65+%
echolamines in plasma in humans (Sothmann, Hart, Horn, & Gus- W_ MAX , before the thresholds are reached. This intensity is, of
tafson, 1991) and in the brain in animals (Wann, Audet, & course, much higher than the lower end of the moderate intensity
Anisman, 2010). criteria chosen for this study. Our choice was based on the range
Animal studies provide some evidence for exercise-induced in- classified by Borer (2003) to be of intermediate intensity (50–80%
creases in brain concentrations of dopamine and norepinephrine, VO2MAX) and the fact that several authors have used intensities be-
the norepinephrine metabolite 3-methoxy 4-hydroxyphenylglycol, low the thresholds and shown significant effects on cognition
and the dopamine metabolites 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid and (Delignières et al., 1994; Fontana, Mazzardo, Mokgothu, Furtado,
4-hydroxy 3-methoxyphenylacetic acid, also known as homovanil- & Gallagher, 2009; Joyce et al., 2009).
lic acid (see Meeusen and De Meirleir (1995) and Meeusen, Piacen- The success of exercise below the norepinephrine and epineph-
tini and De Meirleir (2001) for reviews). Increased activity of rine thresholds to induce improved cognition may be because
tyrosine hydroxylase, the rate limiter for catecholamines synthesis, peripheral measures are not good indicators of central changes. In-
has also been shown during exercise in rodents (Hattori, Naoi, & creased psychological stress due to perception of the task as being
Nishino, 1994; Morgan, Yamamoto, & Freed, 1984). In humans, unpredictable and/or one in which the person does not feel in con-
Dalsgaard et al. (2004), examining norepinephrine and epinephrine trol, or anxiety about the exercise, especially if it is to continue for
concentrations in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) by lumbar puncture, any length of time, would result in increased central concentrations
demonstrated exercise-induced increases in CSF concentrations of dopamine and norepinephrine (Mason, 1975a, 1975b; Mason
342 T. McMorris, B.J. Hale / Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351

et al., 1973). The duration of the exercise is another issue when (Ferris, Williams, & Shen, 2007; Goekint et al., 2008; Tang, Chu, Hui,
intensity is below the catecholamines thresholds. Even though par- Helmeste, & Law, 2008; Winter et al., 2007). However, non-signifi-
ticipants may begin exercising below the threshold, if it continues cant effects were shown by other authors (Laske et al., 2010; Ströhle
long enough they may find that they have passed the threshold. et al., 2010). As BDNF crosses the blood–brain barrier (Sartorius
Chmura, Kristztofiak, Ziemba, Nazar, and Kaciuba-Uścilko (1998) et al., 2009) peripheral concentrations are indicative of brain con-
found that participants exercising at 75% of their lactate threshold centrations. In all cases the increase was transient and soon returned
(the norepinephrine and epinephrine thresholds correlate highly to baseline levels, but as it is the downstream CaMKII, MAPK and
with the lactate threshold (Podolin et al., 1991)) demonstrated a CREB activity that is the key issue, the transient nature of the BDNF
significant increase in lactate concentrations in the blood and sig- concentrations is not an issue. Indeed, exercise-induced increases in
nificant increases in plasma epinephrine and norepinephrine con- BDNF concentrations in the brain have interested researchers exam-
centrations after 20 min. Winter et al. (2007), using an exercise ining the use of exercise as an aid for cognition in individuals suffer-
regimen designed to have participants work for 40 min at an exer- ing from Parkinson’s disease (Ahlskog, 2011) and multiple sclerosis
cise intensity that would not elicit blood lactate concentrations of (Cardinal, Wilson, Giesser, Drain, & Sicotte, 2008; Gold et al., 2003).
>2 mmol l1, also demonstrated significant increases in plasma epi- The delayed actions of BDNF may mean that the positive effects
nephrine and norepinephrine concentrations. Therefore, exercise of exercise continue following cessation of activity. This is one of
may begin below the threshold but eventually exceed it. the moderator variables to be examined. There are also sound rea-
While the theories outlined above may provide logical ratio- sons for expecting task complexity to prove to be a significant
nales for an acute exercise-cognition interaction they would sug- moderator. The complex tasks in this study were those which have
gest that both speed and accuracy of processing would be been described as being central executive tasks (Baddeley, 1986;
affected in the same way. As we saw in Section 1, McMorris et al. Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, & Howerter, 2000). These
(2011) found that moderate intensity exercise had a positive effect tasks activate mainly the prefrontal cortex but also draw on infor-
on speed of processing but a negative one on accuracy of working mation recalled from other parts of the brain (see Barbas (2000)
memory tasks. Having ruled out a speed-accuracy trade-off (see and Leh et al. (2010) for reviews). Central executive tasks appear
Section 1), the authors claimed that increased neurotransmitter to be more readily affected by stress than other types of task
concentrations would have a positive effect on speed of processing (Arnsten, 2009; Ramos & Arnsten, 2007). There is evidence that
but possibly induce neural noise in areas of the brain involved in when stress levels are high, the prefrontal cortex is taken off-line
accuracy of processing working memory tasks. This is based on (Arnsten, 2009, 2011).
Yerkes and Dodson’s (1908) theory. Yerkes and Dodson argued that Miyake et al. (2000) described the central executive processes
complex tasks require lower levels of arousal for optimal perfor- as involving several functions including shifting between tasks or
mance than do more simple tasks. In fact, they claimed that very mental sets; inhibition of prepotent responses; updating and mon-
simple tasks may need high levels arousal for performance to be itoring working memory representations, which involves the re-
optimal. This argument assumes that changes in speed and accu- moval of redundant information and replacing it with new,
racy are caused by the activity of different parts of the brain. relevant information; planning; and the coordination of multiple
McMorris et al. (2009) claimed that this may be the case in tasks tasks. Leh et al. (2010) provided other examples, e.g. abstract
like the flanker and Simon tasks where the task is cognitive but thinking, cognitive flexibility and selecting relevant sensory infor-
the response is motoric, relying on premotor cortex and basal gan- mation. The tasks delegated to the central executive category in
glia activation, while accuracy is dependent on anterior cingulate this study demonstrated at least one of the processes outlined by
cortex and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity (Ochsner, Miyake et al. and/or Leh et al. Inhibition of prepotent responses
Hughes, Robertson, Cooper, & Gabrieli, 2009; Wittfoth, Kuster- is central to the go/nogo, flanker, Stroop, random number genera-
mann, Fahle, & Herrmann, 2008). McMorris et al. (2009) showed tion, oddball, stop-signal and Simon tasks. Random number gener-
that exercise-induced increases in plasma norepinephrine concen- ation also requires updating and monitoring of working memory
trations were related to different effects on speed and accuracy in a processes, as does the paced auditory serial addition task. The
flanker task. Sternberg task requires memory encoding, and updating and mon-
Another possible reason for different results for speed and accu- itoring of working memory processes. The switch visual attention
racy of processing is that tasks requiring recall and/or learning may task used by Pesce and colleagues (Cereatti, Casella, Manganelli,
be affected less by catecholamines and more by an interaction be- & Pesce, 2009; Pesce, Capranica, Tessitore, & Figura, 2002, 2003;
tween acute exercise and brain concentrations of the protein, brain Pesce, Casella, & Capranica, 2004; Pesce, Cereatti, Casella, Caldari,
derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), as they depend on neuroplas- & Capranica, 2007a; Pesce, Tessitore, Casella, Pirritano, & Capra-
ticity. Acute exercise induces increased concentrations of BDNF. nica, 2007b) requires the shifting of mental sets. The Wisconsin
The binding of BDNF to the tropomyosin-related kinase-B receptor card sorting task involves the shifting of mental sets, and updating
initiates a number of intracellular signaling cascades, including cal- and monitoring of working memory representations. The Tower of
cium/calmodulin kinase II (CaMKII) and mitogen-activated protein Hanoi test requires planning and cognitive flexibility.
kinase (MAPK), resulting in the phosphorylation of cyclic adeno- Moreover, positron emission tomography (PET) and functional
sinemonophosphate (cAMP)-response element binding protein magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have shown that areas
(CREB) (Binder & Scharfman, 2004). While CaMKII, MAPK and CREB of the brain known to be involved in central executive activity (see
play major roles in the individual’s adaptation to exercise, these Barbas (2000), Leh et al. (2010), and van den Wildenberg et al.
signaling pathways are also essential for neurogenesis and neuro- (2010) for reviews) are activated by random number generation
plasticity, and hence learning and memory. Their effect as a result (Jahanshahi, Dirnberger, Fuller, & Frith, 2000), oddball (Huettel &
of acute exercise is unlikely to be immediate because the cascades McCarthy, 2004), Wisconsin card sorting task (Henaff, Bayle,
initiated by BDNF are downstream and therefore take time. So the Krolak-Salmon, & Fonlupt, 2010), flanker task (Ochsner et al.,
effect of BDNF is most likely to take place post-exercise. Interest- 2009), Simon task (Wittfoth et al., 2008), Sternberg test (Marvel
ingly, Lambourne and Tomporowski (2010) found that recall tasks & Desmond, 2010), Stroop color test (Leung, Skudlarski, Gatenby,
showed significantly larger effect sizes than processing tasks fol- Peterson, & Gore, 2000), paced auditory serial addition task
lowing exercise but not during exercise. (Cardinal et al., 2008), Tower of Hanoi (Fincham, Carter, van Veen,
Several studies have shown that bouts of acute exercise have in- Stenger, & Anderson, 2002), go/nogo (Goya-Maldonado et al., 2010)
duced significant increases in serum or plasma BDNF concentrations and stop-signal (Li Huang, Constable, & Sinha, 2006) tasks. We
T. McMorris, B.J. Hale / Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351 343

could not find any direct evidence of parieto-occipatal activation 4.1. Initial analyses
for the task used by Coles and Tomporowski (2008), however
Galati, Pelle, Berthoz, and Commiteri (2010) showed evidence of In the overall analysis, including both the speed and accuracy
such activity using a similar task. While similar tasks to that of dependent variables, effect sizes were heterogeneous
Pesce and colleagues (Cereatti et al., 2009; Pesce et al., 2002, Q(122) = 472.93 (p < 0.001), s2 = 0.29. The mean effect size was sig-
2003, 2004, 2007a, 2007b) have shown activation of the parietal nificant, g = 0.14 (Z = 2.80, p < 0.01), variance 0.003, SE = 0.05, and
lobes and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Hedden & Gabrieli, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.04–0.24. The fail-safe N was not cal-
2010). Although similarity does not equate to being synonymous, culated as g < 0.20. Forty-two effect sizes were negative, there was
it would not appear unreasonable to conclude that these tasks one zero and 81 positive. Sub-group analyses comparing effect
are most likely to activate areas of the brain that have been shown sizes where one effect size from a single study were calculated
to be activated during performance of central executive tasks. with those studies where there was more than one effect size
The recall and alertness/attention tasks are generally thought of found no significant between group differences. Sub-group analy-
as being more simple. Although the tasks involved with reaction ses for speed and accuracy showed a significant difference between
time (simple and choice reaction time) are primarily controlled the two variables (Meandiff = 0.26, SE = 0.10, Zdiff = 2.60, p = 0.01,
by the prefrontal cortex, less neural activity is demonstrated than R2 = 0.98). Mean effect size for accuracy was non-significant
during the performance of complex tasks (Bares, Rektor, Kanovsky, (g = 0.04, SE = 0.06), while speed demonstrated a significant mean
& Streitova, 2003) and the basal ganglia also play an important role effect size (g = 0.30, Z = 3.75, p < 0.001, variance = 0.01, SE = 0.08,
(Rektor et al., 2003). Recall/short-term memory tasks require hold- CI = 0.14–0.46). Effect sizes were heterogeneous (Q(65) = 301.37,
ing information in the visuospatial sketchpad (visual information) p < 0.001, s2 = 0.37 and the fail-safe N was 1220.
or the phonological loop (verbal information) (Baddeley, 1986). Sub-group comparison of mean effect sizes for low, moderate
fMRI research has shown that the phonological loop involves the and high intensity exercise demonstrated no significant differences
lateral frontal and inferior parietal lobes of the left hemisphere, for accuracy, with all exercise intensities demonstrating non-sig-
while the visuospatial loop seems to be situated in the parieto- nificant mean effect sizes ranging from g = 0.14 to g = 0.09. For
occipatal region of both hemispheres, although it is more active speed, low and high intensity exercise showed non-significant
in the right hemisphere (Barbas, 2000; Leh et al., 2010). Visual mean effect sizes barely above zero but moderate intensity exer-
search and coincidence anticipation depend on the primary visual cise demonstrated a significant mean effect size (g = 0.50,
cortex and the superior colliculus (Anderson & Rees, 2011). Verbal Z = 4.82, p < 0.001, variance 0.01, SE = 0.10, CI 0.30–0.70). Effect
fluency is heavily dependent on Broca’s area (Caplan, Alpert, sizes were heterogeneous (Q(38) = 150.19, p < 0.001, s2 = 0.30).
Waters, & Olivieri, 2000), while simple arithmetic is controlled There was a significant effect of intensity (Qbet = 9.08, p < 0.02,
by the parietal lobe (Hamid, Yusoff, Mukari, & Mohamad, 2011). R2 = 0.98). Post hoc Zdiff tests with the Bonferroni correction factor
There is no evidence as to brain areas concerned in the soccer deci- found no significant differences (p > 0.017) between high and low
sion making tests (McMorris & Graydon, 1996a, 1996b, 1997; intensity effect sizes, but mean effect sizes for moderate intensity
McMorris et al., 1999). McMorris (2009b) reported that the soccer were significantly different to those for low (Meandiff = 0.50,
players, who were his participants, simply recalled what they had SE = 0.22, Zdiff = 2.28, p < 0.017) and high intensities
been taught by their coaches and provided an automatic response. (Meandiff = 0.50, SE = 0.20, Zdiff = 2.51, p < 0.017).
So they are probably of little difference to the visual search tasks.
Given that the prefrontal cortex plays a lesser role in such tasks, 4.1.1. Moderator variables
one would expect that the effect of moderate intensity exercise For accuracy, only the timing of testing (during versus post-
will be less for these studies than for the central executive tasks. exercise) showed any significant effect. Testing post-exercise dem-
onstrated a significant mean effect size (g = 0.17, Z = 1.70, p < 0.05,
variance 0.01, SE = 0.10, CI 0.03 to 0.37), while the mean effect
size for during exercise was non-significant (g = 0.08, SE = 0.08).
4. Results There was a significant difference between the means
(Meandiff = 0.25, SE = 0.13, Zdiff = 1.98, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.97).
The literature reviewed yielded 53 articles which met the crite- For speed, examination of the moderator variables was under-
ria for inclusion, eight of these included more than one study. Five taken with results for speed of processing for moderate intensity
of these articles reported two experiments using the same partici- exercise only, as this was the only condition to elicit significant ef-
pants in each experiment, therefore only one effect size was calcu- fect sizes. The moderator variable, the timing of testing (during
lated per article. In the other three articles, two experiments were versus post-exercise), failed to show a significant effect (p > 0.05).
reported but which used different participants, and different exer- Testing during exercise demonstrated a significant mean effect size
cise intensities and/or cognitive tasks. The experiments were trea- (g = 0.48, Z = 3.72, p < 0.001, variance 0.02, SE = 0.13, CI 0.23–0.73),
ted as separate studies. Córdova, Silva, Moraes, Simo }es, and as did post-exercise testing (g = 0.53, Z = 3.37, p < 0.001, variance
Nóbrega (2009) reported one experiment but the design was such 0.02, SE = 0.16, CI 0.22–0.84).
that there were three different groups each with different partici- Examination of the task complexity (central executive task ver-
pants and each exercising at different intensities. These were trea- sus recall and attention/alertness tasks) moderator variable dem-
ted as three separate studies. There were 25 studies where effect onstrated a significant effect (Meandiff = 0.46, SE = 0.17,
sizes were calculated for both the speed and accuracy dependent Zdiff = 2.28, p < 0.01, R2 = 0.96). Central executive tasks showed a
variables. For speed (k = 66), there were 31 studies in which effect significant mean effect size (g = 0.77, Z = 4.70, p < 0.001, variance
sizes were calculated for one intensity, 13 where two intensities 0.03, SE = 0.16, CI 0.45–1.09). The other tasks, which were made
were included and three where there were three intensities. For up of recall and attention/alertness type tasks, also showed a sig-
accuracy (k = 57), there were 20 studies in which effect sizes were nificant mean effect size (g = 0.31, Z = 2.64, p = 0.01, variance
calculated for one intensity, eight where two intensities were in- 0.01, SE = 0.12, and 95% CI 0.08–0.54).
cluded and seven where there were three intensities. In total, there The effects of counterbalancing of testing (counterbalanced/
were 124 effect sizes and 2270 participants. The types of tasks and random testing versus pre-exercise testing followed by post-exer-
exercise intensities used in the speed and accuracy studies can be cise testing) were significant (Meandiff = 0.63, SE = 0.20, Zdiff = 3.47,
seen in Tables 1 and 2, respectively. p < 0.001, R2 = 0.92). The mean effect size for counterbalanced/
344 T. McMorris, B.J. Hale / Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351

Table 1
Participants’ demographics, exercise intensities, cognitive tests and time of testing, for speed of processing studies.

Authors N M/F Age years Exercise intensity Design Test Test time Effect size (g)
(Intensity)
Arcelin et al. (1997) 22 MF 23.78 (4.81) 60% MAP Pre/post CRT During 0.59 (Mod)
Bard and Fleury (1978) 16 M N/R 100% Fatigue CB Visual Post 0.20 (Heavy)
search CA
Brisswalter et al. (1995) 10 M N/R 50% VO2MAX 6 min Pre/post SRT During 1.03 (Mod)
Brisswalter et al. (1997) 20 M 23.50 (1.65) 20%, 40%, 60%, 80% MAP Pre/post SRT During 1.93 (Low)
1.53 (Mod)
1.21 (Heavy)
Cereatti et al. (2009) 24 M 15.75 (1.6) 60% HRRES CB SVA During 2.35 (Mod)
Coco, Di Corrado, Calogero, Perciavalle, and 17 M 20.8 (2.21) Fatigue Pre/post ACT Post 0.91 (Heavy)
Maci (2009)
Collardeau et al. (2001) 11 N/R 26.5 (4.8) VT 40 and 60 min Pre/post CRT During and 0.47 (Mod)
post
Córdova et al. (2009) Test 1 12 F 65.9 (3.6) 60% TLA Pre/post SRT TOH Post 0.11 (Low)
Test 2 12 F 63.8 (4.9) 90% TLA 0.49 (Mod)
Test 3 12 F 63.1 (8.7) TLA 20 min 0.01 (Mod)
Davranche, Burle, Audiffren, and 12 MF 27 (4) 50% MAP CB CRT During 0.56 (Mod)
Hasbroucq (2005)
Davranche and McMorris (2009a) 12 M F 32 (9.0) VT CB Simon During 0.39 (Mod)
Davranche, Hall, and McMorris (2009a) 14 M F 30 (8) 50% MAP 20 min CB Flanker task During 0.40 (Mod)
Davranche, Audiffren, and Denjean (2006) 11 M F 23.9 (3.9) 90% VT 17 min CB CRT During 1.05 (Mod)
Delignières et al. (1994) 20 M F 23.3 (5.50) 20, 40, 60, 80% VO2MAX Pre/post CRT During 0.01 (Low)
0.22 (Mod)
0.02 (Heavy)
Féry, Ferry, Vom Hofe, and Rieu (1997) 13 30% VO2MAX, Fatigue 0.04 (Low)
0.05 (Heavy)
Fontana et al. (2009) 32 M 21.13 (1.62) 40%, 60%, 80% VO2MAX Pre/post SDM Post 0.26 (Low)
0.44 (Mod)
Guizani et al. (2006) 12 N/R 19.10 (2.99) 20%, 40%, 60%, 80% Pre/post SRT CRT During 0.31 (Low)
VO2MAX6 min
0.66 (Mod)
0.36 (Heavy)
Heckler and Croce (1992) 18 F 38.2 (6.1) 70% HRMAX 20 min CB Arithmetic Pos 0.60 (Mod)
Hogervorst, Riedel, Jeukendrup, and Jolies 15 M 24.9 (7.9) 75% WMAX Pre/post SRT CRT SCT Post 0.28 (Mod)
(1996)
Joyce et al. (2009) 10 MF 23 (2) 40% MAP 30 min CB Stop-signal During and 0.35 (Mod)
post
Kamijo et al. (2004a) 12 M 22–23 Fatigue Random Nogo/go Post 0.07 (Heavy)
Kamijo et al. (2004b) 12 M 22–23 Fatigue CB SRT Post 0.07 (Heavy)
Kamijo et al. (2009) 24 M 21.8 (0.6)/65.5 30%, 50% VO2MAX 20 min CB Flanker task Post 0.13 (Low)
(1.5)
0.77 (Mod)
Kashihara and Nakahara (2005) 6 M 23.5 (0.6) TLA 10 min CB CRT Post 0.89 (Mod)
McMorris and Graydon (1996a) 20 M 21.10 (1.65) 70%, 100% WMAX Pre/post SDM During 0.57 (Mod)
0.80 (Heavy)
McMorris and Graydon (1996b) Exp 1 10 M N/R 70%, 100% WMAX Pre/post SDM During 0.39 (Mod)
0.82 (Heavy)
McMorris and Graydon (1996b) Exp 2 20 M N/R 70%, 100% WMAX Pre/post SDM During 0.18 (Mod)
0.43 (Heavy)
McMorris and Graydon (1997) Exp 1 12 M 20.80 (1.34) 70%, 100% WMAX Pre/post Visual During 0.05 (Mod)
search
0.64 (Heavy)
McMorris and Graydon (1997) Exp 2 12 M 20.80 (1.78) 70%, 100% WMAX Pre/post SDM During 0.28 (Mod)
0.38 (Heavy)
McMorris, Swain, Lauder, Smith, and Kelly 12 M 21.04 (2.12) TLA CB SDM Post 0.21 (Mod)
(2006)
McMorris et al. (2009) 24 M 24.32 (7.1) 50%, 80% WMAX CB Flanker task During 0.78 (Mod)
1.30 (Heavy)
McMorris and Keen (1994) 12 MF 18–22 70%, 100% WMAX Pre/post SRT During 0.20 (Mod)
0.70 (Heavy)
McMorris et al. (1999) 9 M 22.0 (2.4) TE, WMAX Pre/post SDM During 0.47 (Mod)
0.70 (Heavy)
McMorris et al. (2003) 9 M 22.2 (2.10) 70%, 100% WMAX Pre/post NC-CRT During 0.16 (Mod)
0.34 (Heavy)
McMorris et al. (2000) 12 M 20.00 (2.00) TE, WMAX CB SDM Post 0.14 (Mod)
0.21 (Heavy)
O’Leary et al. (2011) 36 MF 21.8 (1.6) 60% HRMAX 20 min CB Flanker task Post 0.05 (Low)
Ozyemisci-Taskiran et al. (2008) 11 N/R N/R 70% HRMAX 20 min Pre/post ERT Post 0.53 (Mod)
Pesce and Audiffren (2011) 100 MF 16–24 and 65– 60% HRRES 20–24 min CB CRT During 1.13 (Mod)
74
Pesce et al. (2002) 16 MF 19–40 60% VO2MAX CB SVA During 1.30 (Mod)
Pesce et al. (2007b) 48 M 16–19 60% HRRES CB SVA During 1.15 (Mod)
Pesce et al. (2004) 42 MF 22.20 (4.25) 60% HRRES CB SVA During 0.89 (Mod)
Pesce et al. (2003) 16 MF 19–40 60% VO2MAX CB SVA During 1.17 (Mod)
T. McMorris, B.J. Hale / Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351 345

Table 1 (continued)

Authors N M/F Age years Exercise intensity Design Test Test time Effect size (g)
(Intensity)
Pesce et al. (2007a) 25 MF 66.25 (4.65) 60% HRRES CB SVA During 0.15 (Mod)
12 min
Pontifex and Hillman (2007) 41 MF 20.2 (1.6) 60% VO2MAX 6.5 min CB Flanker task During 0.01 (Mod)
Stroth et al. (2009) 33 MF 14.2 (0.5) 60% HRMAX 20 min Random Flanker task Post 0.96 (Low)
Yanagisawa et al. (2010) 20 MF 21.5 (4.8) 50% VO2MAX CB SCT Post 2.14 (Mod)

Note: Test included switch costs but these were not included in meta-analyses: CB = counterbalanced: Random = randomized: Mod = moderate: VO2MAX = maximum volume
of oxygen uptake: MAP = maximum aerobic power: WMAX = maximum power output: M = male: F = female: N/R = not recorded: VT = ventilatory threshold: TLA = lactate
threshold: TE = epinephrine threshold: HRRES = heart rate reserve: HRMAX = maximum heart rate: Exp = experiment: CRT = choice reaction time: SRT = simple reaction time:
SVA = switch visual attention: TOH = Tower of Hanoi: ACT = attention and concentration task: NC-CRT = non-compatible choice reaction time: SDM = soccer decision making:
SCT = Stroop color test: ERT = electromyographical reaction time.

Table 2
Participants’ demographics, exercise intensities, cognitive tests and time of testing for accuracy studies.

Authors N M/F Age years Exercise intensity Design Test Test time Effect size (g)
(Intensity)
Ando, Kokubu, Yamada, and Kimura 12 M 25.3 (3.1) 40%, 60%, 80% VO2MAX CB Flanker task During 0.09 (Low)
(2011)
0.07 (Mod)
0.40 (Heavy)
Arcelin et al. (1997) 22 MF 23.78 (4.810 60% MAP Pre/post CRT During 0.10 (Mod)
Audiffren, Tomporowski, and 16 MF 21.13 (0.87) 90% VT 35 min CB RNG During and 0.47 (Mod)
Zagrodnik (2009) post
Bard and Fleury (1978) 16 M N/R 100% Fatigue CB Visual Post 0.17 (Heavy)
search CA
Brisswalter et al. (1997) 20 M 23.50 (1.65) 20%, 40%, 60%, 80% MAP Pre/post SRT During 0.78 (Low)
0.70 (Mod)
0.84 (Heavy)
Coco et al. (2009) 17 M 20.8 (2.21) Fatigue Pre/post ACT Post 0.71 (Heavy)
Coles and Tomporowski (2008) 18 MF 22.2 (1.6) 60% VO2MAX 30 min Pre/post visual STM Post 0.11 (Mod)
Collardeau et al. (2001) 11 N/R 26.5 (4.8) VT 40 and 60 min Pre/post CRT During and 0.12 (Mod)
post
Córdova et al. (2009) Test 1 12 F 65.9 (3.6) 60% TLA Pre/post VF Post 1.07 (Low)
Test 2 12 F 63.8 (4.9) 90% TLA 0.97 (Mod)
Test 3 12 F 63.1 (8.7) TLA 20 min 0.58 (Mod)
Davranche et al. (2006) 11 MF 23.9 (3.9) 90% VT 17 min CB CRT During 0.84 (Mod)
Del Giorno, Hall, O’Leary, Bixby, and 30 MF 20.2 (1.1) 75% VT and VT 25 min Pre/post WCST During and 0.01 (Low)
Miller (2010) post
0.28 (Mod)
Delignières et al. (1994) 20 MF 23.3 (5.50) 20%, 40%, 60%, 80% Pre/post CRT During 0.10 (Low)
VO2MAX
0.25 (Mod)
0.21 (Heavy)
Fontana et al. (2009) 32 M 21.13 (1.62) 40%, 60%, 80% VO2MAX Pre/post SDM Post 0.04 (Low)
0.22 (Mod)
0.40 (Heavy)
Griffin et al. (2011) 30 M 22 (2.0) 100% VO2MAX Pre/post SCT Post 0.79 (Heavy)
Guizani et al. (2006) 12 N/R 19.10 (2.99) 20%, 40%, 60%, 80% Pre/post SRT CRT During 0.10 (Low)
VO2MAX 6 min
0.03 (Mod)
0.07 (Heavy)
Heckler and Croce (1992) 18 F 38.2 (6.1) 70% HRMAX 20 min CB Arithmetic Post 0.09 (Mod)
Hillman et al. (2009) 20 MF 9.6 (0.7) 65% HRMAX 20 min Pre/post Flanker task Post 1.34 (Mod)
Isaacs and Pohlman (1991) 12 M 24.40 (4.00) 25%, 40%, 75%, 100% Random CA During 0.07 (Low)
VO2MAX
0.30 (Mod)
1.13 (Heavy)
Joyce et al. (2009) 10 MF 23 (2) 40% MAP 30 min CB Stop-signal During and 0.02 (Mod)
post
Kamijo et al. (2009) 24 M 21.8 (0.6)/65.5 30%, 50% VO2MAX 20 min CB Flanker task Post 0.59 (Low)
(1.5)
0.03 (Mod)
Kashihara and Nakahara (2005) 6 M 23.5 (0.6) TLA 10 min CB CRT Post 0.97 (Mod)
Lambourne, Audiffren, and 19 MF 21.37 (0.92) 90% VT 40 min CB PASAT During and 0.15 (Mod)
Tomporowski (2010) post
McMorris and Graydon (1996a) 20 M 21.10 (1.65) 70, 100% WMAX Pre/post SDM During 0.07 (Mod)
0.00 (Heavy)
McMorris and Graydon (1996b) Exp 1 10 M 20.60 (1.51) 70, 100% WMAX Pre/post SDM During 0.02 (Mod)
0.27 (Heavy)
McMorris and Graydon (1996b) Exp 2 20 M N/R 70, 100% WMAX Pre/post SDM During 0.71 (Mod)
0.22 (Heavy)

(continued on next page)


346 T. McMorris, B.J. Hale / Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351

Table 2 (continued)

Authors N M/F Age years Exercise intensity Design Test Test time Effect size (g)
(Intensity)
McMorris and Graydon (1997) Exp 2 12 M 20.80 (1.78) 70, 100% WMAX Pre/post SDM During 0.69 (Mod)
0.75 (Heavy)
McMorris et al. (2009) 24 M 24.32 (7.1) 50%, 80% WMAX CB Flanker task During 0.45 (Mod)
1.32 (Heavy)
McMorris et al. (1999) 9 M 22.0 (2.4) TE, WMAX Pre/post SDM During 0.36 (Mod)
0.10 (Heavy)
O’Leary et al. (2011) 36 M F 21.8 (1.6) 60% HRMAX 20 min CB Flanker task Post 0.22 (Low)
Pontifex and Hillman (2007) 41 M F 20.2 (1.6) 60% VO2MAX 6.5 min CB Flanker task During 0.82 (Mod)
Stroth et al. (2009) 33 M F 14.2 (0.5) 60% HRMAX 20 min Random Flanker task Post 0.11(Low)
Themanson and Hillman (2006) 28 M F 20.35 (2.05) 83% HRMAX 30 min CB Flanker task Post 0.15 (Mod)
Travlos and Marisi (1995) 20 M 23 (3) 40%, 50, 60% VO2MAX CB plus RNG During 0.06 (Low)
10 min control
0.08 (Mod)
0.18 (Heavy)

Note: CB = counterbalanced: Random = randomized: VO2MAX = maximum volume of oxygen uptake: MAP = maximum aerobic power: WMAX = maximum power output:
M = male: F = female: N/R = not recorded: Mod = moderate: VT = ventilatory threshold: TLA = lactate threshold: TE = epinephrine threshold: HRMAX = maximum heart rate:
RNG = random number generation: Exp = experiment: WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Task: PASAT = paced auditory serial addition test: CRT = choice reaction time:
SRT = simple reaction time: VF = verbal fluency: STM = short-term memory: SDM = soccer decision making: SCT = Stroop color test: CA = coincidence anticipation.

randomized studies was g = 0.80 (Z = 5.96, p < 0.001, variance 0.02, racy, it was hypothesized that different intensities of exercise
SE = 0.13, CI 0.54–1.06). For studies using a pre-/during or post- would result in significantly different mean effect sizes. This was
exercise order, the mean effect size (g = 0.17, SE = 0.12) was small not supported by the data. All three intensities showed results
and non-significant (p > 0.05). close to zero. These result were different to those of McMorris
et al. (2011) for the effect of moderate intensity exercise on work-
5. Discussion ing memory tasks. The results for accuracy and the inverted-U
effect are similar to those of Etnier et al. (1997), who found no
The results for the initial, overall meta-analysis show a positive significant differences in effect sizes between short duration (2–
and significant, small effect size. Although significant, the effect 15 min), moderate intensity; longer duration (>15 min), moderate
size is well below the criteria set by Cohen (1988) as signifying a intensity; light exercise; and heavy exercise. However, these
small effect size. This result could be said to be statistically signif- authors combined speed and accuracy data and used different
icant but practically insignificant. This is similar to Lambourne and intensity categories.
Tomporowski’s (2010) findings for results during exercise, but The findings outlined above show that speed and accuracy of
slightly smaller than those post-exercise. Etnier et al. (1997) also processing appear to have been affected differently by acute exer-
found a small mean effect size. However, examination of the re- cise, with accuracy being unaffected while speed demonstrated an
sults for the analyses comparing speed and accuracy of processing, inverted-U effect. The results for speed of processing can be ex-
and those examining the inverted-U effect paint a different picture plained by Davey’s (1973) exercise-arousal-cognition interaction
and show both statistical and practical significance. The hypothesis theory. Davey perceived low intensity exercise as being synony-
that speed of processing would show a larger mean effect size than mous to low arousal, moderate intensity as equalling moderate
accuracy was borne out by the analysis, with speed demonstrating arousal and heavy exercise as inducing high levels of arousal.
a moderate, positive mean effect size, while accuracy showed a Therefore, based on Yerkes and Dodson’s (1908) arousal–
non-significant, mean effect size approaching zero. Lambourne performance theory, an inverted-U effect would be expected. The
and Tomporowski (2010) found that, for results during exercise, ef- results can also explained, to some extent, by Dietrich’s reticular
fect sizes for tasks that measured processing speed actually dem- activation hypofrontality theory (Dietrich, 2003; Dietrich & Audif-
onstrated a negative effect. However, Lambourne and fren, 2011). According to Dietrich, during moderate intensity exer-
Tomporowski did not include dependent variables such as inspec- cise, activation of the reticular system will increase arousal leading
tion time in visual search tasks or speed of recall in memory tasks to improved performance of well-learned and habitual tasks. How-
as representing speed of processing. In the present study such ever, heavy exercise requires greater activation of the premotor
tasks were considered to measure speed of processing. That accu- cortex and supplementary motor area, therefore, if exercise
racy results showed no significant effect was a little surprising and intensity is to be maintained, these areas will be activated at the
is in contrast with the findings of McMorris et al. (2011), who expense of the prefrontal cortex, hence poorer cognitive perfor-
found a moderate effect size demonstrating a detrimental effect mance of tasks requiring prefrontal cortex activation. The catechol-
on performance. This was, however, only examining working amines hypothesis (Cooper, 1973; McMorris, Collard, Corbett,
memory tasks and only for moderate intensity exercise. To fully Dicks, & Swain, 2008) also provides a rational explanation for the
understand our results we need also to examine the results testing speed data. Increases in brain concentrations of the neurotransmit-
the hypotheses concerning the inverted-U effect. ters dopamine and norepinephrine, during and following moderate
For speed of processing, it was hypothesized that effect sizes as intensity exercise, should theoretically facilitate cognition. During
a result of acute, moderate intensity exercise would be signifi- and following low intensity exercise speed will be slower due to
cantly higher than those induced by low and high intensity exer- less activation in the relevant brain areas. During and following
cise. This was strongly supported by the data with moderate heavy exercise, the much larger increases in catecholamines will
intensity exercise showing a positive, moderate mean effect size, lead to neural noise, which will inhibit performance.
while low and heavy exercise demonstrated effect sizes close to It is easy to see that, during moderate intensity exercise, in-
zero. These results are similar to those found by McMorris et al. creased arousal, catecholamines neurotransmitters availability in
(2011), who demonstrated a large mean effect size for the effect the brain or increased reticular formation activation would facili-
of moderate intensity exercise on working memory tasks. For accu- tate speed of processing, but it is difficult to see why accuracy
T. McMorris, B.J. Hale / Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351 347

would not be affected. McMorris et al. (2011) argued that for work- than testing post-exercise. The arguments for this have been based
ing memory tasks, even moderate intensity exercise may induce on the half-life of catecholamines peripherally, which is 3 min.
neural noise that would be detrimental to the performance of these This fails to take into account that the key issue is central concen-
complex tasks. However, we found no significant difference be- trations of catecholamines not peripheral. Eisenhofer, Kopin, and
tween effect sizes for accuracy data for central executive tasks Goldstein (2004) reported the half-life of brain catecholamines as
compared to recall and attention/alertness tasks. Moreover, if being in the range of 8–12 h, while Kadzierski, Aguila-Mansilla,
moderate intensity exercise induces neural noise that negatively Kozlowski, and Porter (1994) stated that the half-life of tyrosine
affects accuracy of performance, one would have expected that hydroxylase mRNA was 14 ± 1 h.
low intensity exercise would induce optimal performance, in line Another factor that has not been taken into account is the pos-
with Yerkes and Dodson’s (1908) assertion that complex tasks re- sible effect of exercise-induced increases in BDNF, which were out-
quire lower levels of arousal. Observation of the results for accu- lined in the previous section. The BDNF-induced activity of CaMKII,
racy in the individual studies suggest a much more mundane MAPK and CREB occur downstream from the increased synthesis
reason for accuracy measures showing no significant effects. In and release of BDNF and are therefore most likely to be seen
tasks such as the flanker task, Simon task, soccer decision making post-exercise. This activity is necessary for neuroplasticity to take
tasks and reaction time, there were very few errors made. This place and may account for the fact that, for accuracy, testing
strongly suggests a ceiling effect. This is probably due to the way post-exercise resulted in a larger mean effect size than did testing
these tests have been designed. These tests measure performance during exercise. However, one should be very cautious when inter-
through speed of processing, which is seen as being indicative of preting these data. The mean effect size was very small and it is
ease of processing. The accuracy measures are only there to ensure probably fair to say that these results merely supply a prima facie
that the participants remain honest and continue to process the case for an acute exercise effect and further research is required.
problem fully. Basically it is to safeguard against a speed-accuracy
trade-off. It could be argued that if we wish to really test the effect 5.2. Task complexity (central executive tasks versus recall and
of acute exercise on accuracy of cognition, we need to use different attention/alertness tasks)
cognitive measures. This is similar to claims made by Etnier and
Chang (2009), who felt that the tasks were not complex enough. There was no significant difference, for accuracy, between the
The central executive tasks examined in this analysis, processes mean effect size for central executive tasks compared to that for re-
like inhibition (Stroop color test, random number generation, oper- call and attention/alertness tasks. Even though the overall mean ef-
ation span, reading span, oddball, Simon tasks), updating and mon- fect size for accuracy was around zero, we expected central
itoring of working memory processes (random number generation, executive tasks to show a small but significant mean effect size
paced auditory serial addition task), shifting of mental sets rather than the non-significant g = 0.05, which was found. The
(Wisconsin card sorting task, switch visual attention tests), while present results may differ from those of McMorris et al. (2011)
demanding may not be complex enough to actually test accuracy due to the fact that the central executive is only one part, albeit
of cognition. They are not as complex as planning, abstract think- the main part, of working memory but is more likely to be due
ing, cognitive flexibility or selecting relevant sensory information. to differences in the criteria for inclusion in the two meta-analyses.
Research needs to be carried out on these more complex processes. In the present study, protocols which used heart rate without ref-
Evidence also exists to show that learning can be positively af- erence to a criterion point, e.g. maximum heart rate, were not in-
fected by acute exercise (Winter et al., 2007), while Lambourne cluded but they were in the McMorris et al. (2011) study.
and Tomporowski (2010) showed larger effect sizes, though still Perhaps more importantly, where performance was measured dur-
small, for memory tasks compared to speed of processing tasks, ing and post-exercise within the same study, two effect sizes were
when testing took place post-exercise. The effect of acute exercise calculated by McMorris et al. (2011). This increases the importance
on learning and memory may be due to exercise-induced increases of those studies in the analyses.
in brain concentrations of BDNF and the resulting CaMKII, MAPK For speed of processing, the mean effect size for central execu-
and CREB intracellular signaling cascades, which are essential for tive tasks was significantly larger than for recall and attention/
neuroplasticity. Recent research (Griffin et al., 2011) demonstrated alertness tasks. The variance explained was very high. The recall
improved memory, which coincided with increased serum BDNF and attention/alertness tasks showed a small, significant mean ef-
concentrations, following exercise to exhaustion. There were no fect size. The central executive tasks, however, demonstrated a
significant effects on a Stroop test, which we might expect large effect size. It was not as large as that found by McMorris
following our results. Winter et al. (2007) found that changes from et al. (2011) for working memory tasks. The difference in results
pre-exercise BDNF serum concentrations demonstrated a small between the two studies may be due to the facts outlined above
but significant correlation with changes in performance on a regarding accuracy data but with an added factor. In the present
language task. However, Ferris et al. (2007) found no significant study only one effect size was calculated for those studies where
correlations between BDNF concentrations and performance on two experiments were carried out but by the same participants
the Stroop test. and using similar tests in both experiments (Pesce et al., 2002,
2003, 2004, 2007a, 2007b), whereas McMorris et al. (2011) calcu-
5.1. Timing of testing (during versus post-exercise) lated separate effect sizes for reach experiment. All except one of
these studies show large effect sizes and the calculation of two ef-
There were no significant differences in mean effect sizes for fect sizes per study would greatly exaggerate the mean effect size.
speed, when testing took place during or post-exercise. Both condi- These results were as expected based on the claims that central
tions showed significant, moderate mean effect sizes. For accuracy, executive tasks are more susceptible to changes in arousal (Arnsten,
testing post-exercise demonstrated a very small mean effect size, 2009; Ramos & Arnsten, 2007) and catecholamines concentrations
one below that which Cohen (1988) classed as being small. It (Chrousos, 2009) than the more simple tasks. It should be noted,
was, however, significant and differed significantly to the mean ef- however, that few authors have actually examined prefrontal cortex
fect size for testing during exercise, which was almost zero. This activity during or following exercise, while undertaking testing on
moderator variable was included because several authors (e.g., central executive tasks. Event related potential research (e.g.,
Dietrich, 2003; McMorris & Graydon, 1996a, 1996b, 1997) have Hillman et al., 2009; Kamijo, Nishihira, Higashiura, & Kuroiwa,
claimed that testing during exercise would result in greater effects 2007; Nakamura, Nishimoto, Akamatu, Takahashi, & Maruyama,
348 T. McMorris, B.J. Hale / Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351

1999; Pontifex & Hillman, 2007) has shown changes in P3 and N2 6. Conclusions
amplitudes during performance of a flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen,
1974) following moderate intensity exercise, but not unequivocally. Overall acute exercise demonstrated a small but significant
N2 amplitude reflects activity in the anterior cingulate cortex, which mean effect on cognition but examination of the comparison
plays an important role in inhibition of prepotent responses (Barbas, between speed and accuracy dependent variables showed that
2000). P3 amplitude is described by Kok (2001) as being a measure of it was the former that accounted for 98% of the variance. For
processing during working memory performance. Yanagisawa et al. speed of processing, an inverted-U effect was shown, with mod-
(2010), using functional near infra-red spectroscopy, found a rela- erate intensity exercise inducing significantly improved perfor-
tionship between increased dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activation mance, with a moderate effect size. Examination of the
and response time on a Stroop color test following exercise. Thus, moderator variables found that central executive tasks showed
there is some support for the claim that central executive tasks are a larger effect size than recall and alertness/attention tasks. Sim-
more readily affected by acute exercise is due to the role of the pre- ilarly, counterbalancing or randomizing cognitive testing induced
frontal cortex in such tasks. larger effect sizes than using a pre-exercise followed by during
or post-exercise testing protocol. However, timing of testing
5.3. Counterbalancing of testing (counterbalanced/random testing (post-exercise compared to during exercise) had no significant
versus pre-exercise testing followed by during or post-exercise testing) effect. For accuracy, the only significant mean effect size found
was that when testing took place post-exercise compared to dur-
There was no significant difference between counterbalanced/ ing exercise but this was very small and could only be said to
random testing and pre-exercise testing followed by during or demonstrate a trend. It was concluded that increased arousal
post-exercise testing, for accuracy. However, for speed of process- during moderate intensity exercise, probably due to increased
ing, mean effect sizes in counterbalanced/random testing studies brain concentrations of the neurotransmitters dopamine and
were significantly larger than in pre-exercise testing followed by norepinephrine resulted in faster speed of processing. The almost
during or post-exercise testing studies. The former demonstrated non-existent effect on accuracy data may well be due to the fail-
a large, significant mean effect size and the latter a small and ure to choose tests which are complex enough to measure exer-
non-significant mean effect size. Moreover, the variance explained cise-induced changes in performance.
was very high. This was the opposite to what had been
hypothesized.
Research textbooks recommend counterbalancing or randomi- References
zation in order to control for learning/habituation effects. The use
of pre-exercise testing followed by testing during or post-exercise ⁄Denotes study included in meta-analyses.
opens up the possibility, some might say probability, of a learning/
habituation effect. This would naturally result in larger effect sizes Ahlskog, J. E. (2011). Does vigorous exercise have a neuroprotective effect in
Parkinson disease? Neurology, 77, 288–294.
from the pre- followed by during or post-exercise studies. That the Anderson, E. J., & Rees, G. (2011). Neural correlates of spatial orienting in the human
opposite occurred is very surprising. Certainly in some of the stud- superior colliculus. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106, 2273–2284.
ies examined in the meta-analysis, authors with pre-/during exer- ⁄Ando, S., Kokubu, M., Yamada, Y., & Kimura, M. (2011). Does cerebral oxygenation
affect cognitive function during exercise? European Journal of Applied Physiology,
cise testing designs used cognitive tests that had been shown to be 111, 1973–1982.
reliable in test/re-test conditions (McMorris & Graydon, 1996a, ⁄Arcelin, R., Brisswalter, J., & Delignières, D. (1997). Effect of physical exercise
1996b, 1997; McMorris et al., 1999). Others used tests in which duration on decisional performance. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 32,
123–140.
feedback is not available (Arcelin et al., 1997; Brisswalter, Durand, Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex
Delignieres, & Legros, 1995; Brisswalter et al., 1997; Delignières structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10, 410–422.
et al., 1994; Hillman et al., 2009; Ozyemisci-Taskiran, Gunendi, Arnsten, A. F. T. (2011). Catecholamine influences on dorsolateral prefrontal cortical
networks. Biological Psychiatry, 69, e89–e99.
Bolukbasi, & Beyazova, 2008), which would at least limit any learn- Arnsten, A. F. T., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1985). Alpha 2-adrenergic mechanisms in
ing/habituation effect. From our results it would look as though prefrontal cortex associated with cognitive decline in aged nonhuman primates.
these methods have been successful. Science, 230, 1273–1276.
Arts, F. J. P., & Kuipers, H. (1994). The relation between power output, oxygen
This does not explain, however, why mean effect sizes, where
uptake and heart rate in male athletes. International Journal of Sports Medicine,
counterbalancing or randomization were used, were the larger. 15, 228–231.
This could be due to the nature of the pre- followed by during or ⁄Audiffren, M., Tomporowski, P. D., & Zagrodnik, J. A. (2009). Acute aerobic exercise
post-exercise designs. In such studies participants undertook the and information processing: Modulation of executive control in a random
number generation task. Acta Psychologica, 132, 85–95.
pre-exercise test just before they began exercising. At this stage Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. New York: Oxford University Press.
they knew that they were very soon going to undertake exercise. Barbas, H. (2000). Connections underlying the synthesis of cognition, memory, and
Moreover, in many of these studies exercise was to be incremental emotion in primate prefrontal cortices. Brain Research Bulletin, 52, 319–330.
⁄Bard, C., & Fleury, M. (1978). Influence of imposed metabolic fatigue on visual
to exhaustion or to an intensity P80% VO2MAX (Brisswalter et al., capacity components. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 1283–1287.
1995, 1997; Delignières et al., 1994; McMorris & Graydon, 1996a, Bares, M., Rektor, I., Kanovsky, P., & Streitova, H. (2003). Cortical and subcortical
1996b, 1997; McMorris et al., 1999). In such cases it is common distribution of middle and long latency auditory and visual evoked potentials in
a cognitive (CNV) paradigm. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 2447–2460.
to see pre-exercise increases in plasma catecholamines concentra- Binder, D. K., & Scharfman, H. E. (2004). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Growth
tions as a result of anticipation of stress (Mason et al., 1973). This is Factors, 22, 123–131.
in line with Mason’s (1975a, 1975b) notion of psychologically-in- Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction
to meta-analysis. Chichester: John Wiley and Sons.
duced increases in stress. For example, McMorris et al. (2009) Borer, K. T. (2003). Exercise endocrinology. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
showed that pre-exercise plasma concentrations of catecholamines ⁄Brisswalter, J., Arcelin, R., Audiffren, M., & Delignières, D. (1997). Influence of
demonstrated a significant linear increase from rest to 50% maxi- physical exercise on simple reaction time: Effect of physical fitness. Perceptual
and Motor Skills, 85, 1019–1027.
mum aerobic power (MAP) and onto 80% MAP despite the fact that,
Brisswalter, J. B., Collardeau, M., & Arcelin, R. (2002). Effects of acute physical
in all cases, the participants were resting. Thus in the supposed exercise characteristics on cognitive performance. Sports Medicine (Auckland, N.
‘‘control’’, pre-exercise condition the participant may actually be Z.), 32, 555–566.
performing above their real resting, control capacity. As a result ⁄Brisswalter, J. B., Durand, M., Delignieres, D., & Legros, P. (1995). Optimal and non-
optimal demand in a dual task of pedaling and simple reaction time: Effects on
the true effect on cognitive performance between rest and moder- energy expenditure and cognitive performance. Journal of Human Movement
ate intensity exercise would not be seen. Studies, 29, 15–34.
T. McMorris, B.J. Hale / Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351 349

Can, A., Dao, D. T., Arad, M., Terrillion, C. E., Piantadosi, S. C., & Gould, T. D. (2012). Fincham, J. M., Carter, C. S., van Veen, V., Stenger, V. A., & Anderson, J. R. (2002).
The mouse forced swim test. Journal of Visualized Experiments, e3638. Neural mechanisms of planning: A computational analysis using event-related
doi:10.3791/3638. fMRI. Proceeding of the National Academic of Sciences of the United States, 99,
Cardinal, K. S., Wilson, S. M., Giesser, B. S., Drain, A. E., & Sicotte, N. L. (2008). A 3346–3351.
longitudinal fMRI study of the paced auditory serial addition task. Multiple ⁄Fontana, F. E., Mazzardo, O., Mokgothu, C., Furtado, O., & Gallagher, J. D. (2009).
Sclerosis, 14, 465–471. Influence of exercise intensity on the decision-making performance of
Caplan, D., Alpert, N., Waters, G., & Olivieri, A. (2000). Activation of Broca’s area by experienced and inexperienced soccer players. Journal of Sport and Exercise
syntactic processing under conditions of concurrent articulation. Human Brain Psychology, 31, 135–151.
Mapping, 9, 65–71. Galati, G., Pelle, G., Berthoz, A., & Commiteri, G. (2010). Multiple reference frames
⁄Cereatti, L., Casella, R., Manganelli, M., & Pesce, C. (2009). Visual attention in used by the human brain for spatial perception and memory. Experimental Brain
adolescents: Facilitating effects of sport expertise and acute physical exercise. Research, 206, 109–120.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 136–145. Goekint, M., Heyman, E., Roelands, B., Njemini, R., Bautmans, I., Mets, T., et al.
Chmura, J., Kristztofiak, H., Ziemba, A. W., Nazar, K., & Kaciuba-Uścilko, H. (1998). (2008). No influence of noradrenaline manipulation on acute exercise-induced
Psychomotor performance during prolonged exercise above and below the increase of brain derived neurotrophic factor. Medicine and Science in Sports and
blood lactate threshold. European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Exercise, 40, 1990–1998.
Physiology, 77, 77–80. Gold, S. M., Schulz, K. H., Hartmann, S., Mladek, M., Lang, U. E., Hellweg, R., et al.
Chmura, J., Nazar, H., & Kaciuba-Uscilko, H. (1994). Choice reaction time during (2003). Basal serum levels and reactivity of nerve growth factor and brain-
graded exercise in relation to blood lactate and plasma catecholamine derived neurotrophic factor to standardized acute exercise in multiple sclerosis
thresholds. International Journal of Sports Medicine, 15, 172–176. and controls. Journal of Neuroimmunology, 138, 99–105.
Chrousos, G. P. (2009). Stress and disorders of the stress system. Nature Reviews Goya-Maldonado, R., Walther, S., Simon, J., Stippich, C., Weisbrod, M., & Kaiser, S.
Endocrinology, 5, 374–381. (2010). Motor impulsivity and ventolateral prefrontal cortex. Psychiatry
⁄Coco, M., Di Corrado, D., Calogero, R. A., Perciavalle, V., & Maci, T. (2009). Research, 183, 89–91.
Attentional processes and blood lactate levels. Brain Research, 1302, 205–211. Green, H. J., Hughson, R. L., Orr, G. W., & Ranney, D. A. (1983). Anaerobic threshold,
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). blood lactate, and muscle metabolites in progressive exercise. Journal of Applied
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Physiology, 54, 1032–1038.
⁄Coles, K., & Tomporowski, P. D. (2008). Effects of acute exercise on executive ⁄Griffin, É. W., Mullally, S., Foley, C., Warmington, S. A., O’Mara, S. M., & Kelly,
processing, short-term memory and long-term memory. Journal of Sports Á. M. (2011). Aerobic exercise improves hippocampal function and increases
Sciences, 26, 333–344. BDNF in the serum of young adult males. Physiology and Behavior, 104,
⁄Collardeau, M., Brisswalter, J., & Audiffren, M. (2001). Effects of a prolonged run on 934–941.
simple reaction time of well trained runners. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 93, ⁄Guizani, S. M., Bouzaouach, I., Tenenbaum, G., Ben Kheder, A., Feki, Y., & Bouaziz, M.
679–689. (2006). Simple and choice reaction times under varying levels of physical load
Cooper, C. J. (1973). Anatomical and physiological mechanisms of arousal with in high skilled fencers. The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 46,
specific reference to the effects of exercise. Ergonomics, 16, 601–609. 344–351.
⁄Córdova, C., Silva, V. C., Moraes, C. F., Simo }es, H. G., & Nóbrega, O. T. (2009). Acute Gutin, B., & Di Gennaro, J. (1968). Effect of one-minute step-ups on performance of
exercise performed close to the anaerobic threshold improves cognitive simple addition. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 39, 81–85.
performance in elderly females. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Hamid, A. I. A., Yusoff, A. N., Mukari, S. Z-M., & Mohamad, M. (2011). Brain
Research, 42, 458–464. activation during addition and subtraction tasks in-noise and in-quiet. The
Dalsgaard, M. K., Ott, P., Dela, F., Juul, A., Pedersen, B. K., Warburg, J., et al. (2004). Malaysian Journal of Medical Science, 18, 3–15.
The CSF and arterial to internal jugular venous hormonal differences during Hand, G. A., Hewitt, C. B., Fulk, L. J., Stock, H. S., Carson, J. A., Davis, J. M.,
exercise in humans. Experimental Physiology, 89, 271–277. et al. (2002). Differential release of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH)
Davey, C. P. (1973). Physical exertion and mental performance. Ergonomics, 16, in the amygdala during different types of stressors. Brain Research, 949,
595–599. 122–130.
⁄Davranche, K., Audiffren, M., & Denjean, A. (2006). A distributional analysis of Hattori, S., Naoi, M., & Nishino, H. (1994). Striatal dopamine turnover during
physical exercise on a choice reaction time task. Journal of Sports Sciences, 24, treadmill running in the rat – relation to the speed of running. Brain Research
323–329. Bulletin, 35, 41–49.
⁄Davranche, K., Burle, B., Audiffren, M., & Hasbroucq, T. (2005). Information ⁄Heckler, B., & Croce, R. (1992). Effects of time of posttest after two durations of
processing during physical exercise: A chronometric and electromyographic exercise on speed and accuracy of addition and subtraction by fit and less-fit
study. Experimental Brain Research, 165, 532–540. women. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 1059–1065.
⁄Davranche, K., Hall, B., & McMorris, T. (2009). Effect of acute exercise on cognitive Hedden, T., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2010). Shared and selective neural correlates of
control required during an Eriksen flanker task. Journal of Sport and Exercise inhibition, facilitation, and shifting processes during executive control.
Psychology, 31, 628–639. Neuroimage, 51, 421–431.
⁄Davranche, K., & McMorris, T. (2009). Specific effects of acute moderate exercise on Henaff, M. A., Bayle, D., Krolak-Salmon, P., & Fonlupt, P. (2010). Cortical dynamics of
cognitive control. Brain and Cognition, 69, 565–570. a self driven choice: A MEG study during a card sorting task. Clinical
⁄Del Giorno, J. M., Hall, E. E., O’Leary, K. C., Bixby, W. R., & Miller, P. C. (2010). Neurophysiology, 121, 508–515.
Cognitive function during acute exercise: A test of the transient hypofrontality Hill, E. E., Zack, E., Battaglini, C., Viru, M., Viru, A., & Hackney, A. C. (2008). Exercise
theory. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 32, 312–323. and circulating cortisol levels: The intensity threshold effect. Journal of
⁄Delignières, D., Brisswalter, J., & Legros, P. (1994). Influence of physical exercise on Endocrinological Investigation, 31, 587–591.
choice reaction time in sports experts: The mediating role of resources ⁄Hillman, C. H., Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Castelli, D. M., Hall, E. E., & Kramer, A. F.
allocation. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 27, 173–188. (2009). The effect of acute treadmill walking on cognitive control and academic
De Vries, W. R., Bernards, N. T. M., De Rooij, M. H., & Koppeschaar, H. P. F. (2000). achievement in preadolescent children. Neuroscience, 159, 1044–1054.
Dynamic exercise discloses different time-related responses in stress hormones. ⁄Hogervorst, E., Riedel, W. J., Jeukendrup, A., & Jolies, J. (1996). Cognitive
Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 866–872. performance after strenuous physical exercise. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 83,
Dietrich, A. (2003). Functional neuroanatomy of altered states of consciousness: The 479–488.
transient hypofrontality hypothesis. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 231–256. Hopkins, W. F., & Johnston, D. (1988). Noradrenergic enhancement of long-term
Dietrich, A., & Audiffren, M. (2011). The reticular-activating hypofrontality (RAH) potentiation at mossy fiber synapses in the hippocampus. Journal of
model of acute exercise. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1305–1325. Neurophysiology, 59, 667–687.
Droste, S. K., de Groote, L., Atkinson, H. C., Lightman, S. L., Reul, J. M., & Linthorst, A. Huettel, S. A., & McCarthy, G. (2004). What is odd in the oddball task? Prefrontal
C. (2008). Corticosterone levels in the brain show a distinct ultradian rhythm cortex is activated by dynamic changes in response strategy. Neuropsychologia,
but a delayed response to forced swim stress. Endocrinology, 149, 3244–3253. 42, 379–386.
Eisenhofer, G., Kopin, I. J., & Goldstein, D. S. (2004). Catecholamine metabolism: A ⁄Isaacs, L. D., & Pohlman, R. L. (1991). Effects of exercise intensity on an
contemporary view with implications for physiology and medicine. accompanying timing task. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 20, 123–131.
Pharmacological Reviews, 56, 331–348. Jahanshahi, M., Dirnberger, G., Fuller, R., & Frith, C. D. (2000). The role of the
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in random number generation: A study with
of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149. positron emission tomography. Neuroimage, 12, 13–25.
Etnier, J. L., & Chang, Y.-K. (2009). The effect of physical activity on executive Jiang, Q. Y., Kawashima, H., Iwasaki, Y., Uchida, K., Sugimoto, K., & Itoi, K. (2004).
function: A brief commentary on definitions, measurement issues, and the Differential effects of forced swim-stress on the corticotropin-releasing
current state of the literature. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 31, hormone and vasopressin gene transcription in the paravocellular division of
469–483. the paraventricular nucleus of rat hypothalamus. Neuroscience Letters, 358,
Etnier, J. L., Salazar, W., Landers, D. M., Petruzzello, S. J., Han, M., & Nowell, P. (1997). 201–204.
The influence of physical fitness and exercise upon cognitive functioning: A ⁄Joyce, J., Graydon, J., McMorris, T., & Davranche, K. (2009). The time course effect of
meta-analysis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 19, 249–277. moderate intensity exercise on response execution and response inhibition.
Ferris, L. T., Williams, J. S., & Shen, C. (2007). The effect of acute exercise on serum Brain and Cognition, 71, 14–19.
brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and cognitive function. Medicine and Kadzierski, W., Aguila-Mansilla, N., Kozlowski, G. P., & Porter, J. C. (1994).
Science in Sports and Exercise, 39, 728–734. Expression of tyrosine hydroxylase gene in cultured hypothalamic cells: Roles
⁄Féry, Y.-A., Ferry, A., Vom Hofe, A., & Rieu, M. (1997). Effect of physical exhaustion of protein kinase A and C. Journal of Neurochemistry, 62, 431–437.
on cognitive functioning. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84, 291–298. Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
350 T. McMorris, B.J. Hale / Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351

⁄Kamijo, K., Hayashi, Y., Sakai, T., Yahiro, T., Tanaka, K., & Nishihira, Y. (2009). Acute ⁄McMorris, T., Sproule, J., Draper, S., & Child, R. (2000). Performance of a
effects of aerobic exercise on cognitive function in older adults. Journal of psychomotor skill following rest, exercise at the plasma epinephrine
Gerontology B – Psychology, 64, 356–363. threshold and maximal intensity exercise. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91,
⁄Kamijo, K., Nishihira, Y., Hatta, A., Kaneda, K., Wasaka, T., Kida, T., et al. 553–562.
(2004a). Differential influences of exercise intensity on information McMorris, T., Sproule, J., Turner, A., & Hale, B. J. (2011). Acute, intermediate intensity
processing in the central nervous system. European Journal of Applied exercise, and speed and accuracy in working memory tasks: A meta-analytical
Physiology, 92, 305–311. comparison of effects. Physiology and Behavior, 102, 421–428.
⁄Kamijo, K., Nishihira, Y., Hatta, A., Kaneda, T., Kida, T., Higashiura, T., et al. (2004b). ⁄McMorris, T., Swain, J., Lauder, M., Smith, N., & Kelly, J. (2006). Warm-up prior to
Changes in arousal level by differential exercise intensity. Clinical undertaking a dynamic psychomotor task: Does it aid performance? Journal
Neurophysiology, 115, 2693–2698. Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 46, 328–334.
Kamijo, K., Nishihira, Y., Higashiura, T., & Kuroiwa, K. (2007). The interactive effect ⁄McMorris, T., Tallon, M., Williams, C., Sproule, J., Potter, J., Swain, J., et al. (2003).
of exercise intensity and task difficulty on human cognitive processing. Incremental exercise, plasma concentrations of catecholamines, reaction time,
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 65, 114–121. and motor time during performance of a noncompatible choice response time
⁄Kashihara, K., & Nakahara, Y. (2005). Short-term effect of physical exercise at task. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 97, 590–604.
lactate threshold on choice reaction time. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 100, Meeusen, R., & De Meirleir, K. (1995). Exercise and brain neurotransmission. Sports
275–291. Medicine (Auckland, N. Z.), 20, 160–188.
Kirschbaum, C., Scherer, G., & Strasburger, C. J. (1994). Pituitary and adrenal Meeusen, R., Piacentini, M. F., & De Meirleir, K. (2001). Brain microdialysis in
hormone responses to pharmacological, physical, and psychological exercise research. Sports Medicine (Auckland, N. Z.), 31, 965–983.
stimulation in habitual smokers and nonsmokers. Clinical Investigator, 72, Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The
804–810. unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex
Kok, A. (2001). On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. ‘‘frontal lobe’’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100.
Psychophysiology, 38, 557–577. Miyashita, T., & Williams, C. L. (2006). Epinephrine administration increases neural
Kutas, M., McCarthy, G., & Donchin, E. (1977). Augmenting mental chronometry: impulses propagated along the vagus nerve: Role of peripheral beta-adrenergic
The P300 is a measure of stimulus evaluation. Science, 197, 792–795. receptors. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 85, 116–124.
Lambourne, K., & Tomporowski, P. D. (2010). The effect of acute exercise on Morgan, M. E., Yamamoto, B. K., & Freed, C. R. (1984). Unilateral activation of
cognitive task performance: A meta-regression analysis. Brain Research, 1341, caudate tyrosien hydroxylase during voluntary circling behavior. Journal of
12–24. Neurochemistry, 43, 737–741.
⁄Lambourne, K., Audiffren, M., & Tomporowski, P. D. (2010). Effects of acute exercise Nakamura, Y., Nishimoto, K., Akamatu, M., Takahashi, M., & Maruyama, A. (1999).
on sensory and executive processing tasks. Medicine and Science in Sports and The effect of jogging on P300 event related potentials. Electromyography Clinical
Exercise, 42, 1396–1402. Neurophysiology, 39, 71–74.
Laske, C., Banachbach, S., Stransky, E., Bosch, S., Straten, G., Machann, J., et al. (2010). Ochsner, K. N., Hughes, B., Robertson, E. R., Cooper, J. C., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2009).
Exercise-induced normalization of decreased BDNF serum concentration in Neural systems supporting the control of affective and cognitive conflicts.
elderly women with remitted major depression. International Journal of Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1842–1855.
Neuropsychopharmacology, 13, 595–602. ⁄O’Leary, K. C., Pontifex, M. B., Scudder, M. R., Brown, M. L., & Hillman, C. H. (2011).
Leh, S. E., Petrides, M., & Strafella, A. P. (2010). The neural circuitry of executive The effects of single bouts of aerobic exercise, exergaming, and videogame play
functions in healthy subjects and Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychopharmacol, on cognitive control. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122, 1518–1525.
35, 70–85. Orwin, R. G. (1983). A fail-safe N for effect size in meta-analysis. Journal of
Leung, H. C., Skudlarski, P., Gatenby, J. C., Peterson, B. S., & Gore, J. C. (2000). An Educational Statistics, 8, 157–159.
event-related functional MRI study of the Stroop color word interference task. ⁄Ozyemisci-Taskiran, O., Gunendi, Z., Bolukbasi, N., & Beyazova, M. (2008). The
Cerebral Cortex, 10, 552–560. effect of a single session submaximal aerobic exercise on premotor fraction
Li, C. S., Huang, C., Constable, R. T., & Sinha, R. (2006). Imaging response inhibition in of reaction time: An electromyographic study. Clinical Biomechanics, 23,
a stop-signal task: Neural correlates independent of signal monitoring and post- 231–235.
response processing. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 186–192. Payne, J. D., Jackson, E. D., Hoscheidt, S., Ryan, L., Jacobs, W. J., & Nadel, N. (2007).
Luft, C. D., Takase, E., & Darby, D. (2009). Heart rate variability and cognitive Stress administered prior to encoding impairs neutral but enhances emotional
function: Effects of physical effort. Biological Psychology, 82, 164–168. long-term episodic memories. Learning and Memory, 14, 861–868.
Marvel, C. L., & Desmond, J. E. (2010). The contributions of cerebro-cerebellar ⁄Pesce, C., & Audiffren, M. (2011). Does acute exercise switch off switch costs? A
circuitry to executive verbal working memory. Cortex, 46, 880–895. study with younger and older athletes. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,
Mason, J. W. (1975a). A historical view of the stress field. Part I. Journal of Human 33, 609–626.
Stress, 1, 6–12. ⁄Pesce, C., Capranica, L., Tessitore, A., & Figura, F. (2002). Orienting and focusing of
Mason, J. W. (1975b). A historical view of the stress field. Part II. Journal of Human visual attention under submaximal physical load. Journal of Human Movement
Stress, 1, 22–36. Studies, 42, 401–420.
Mason, J. W., Hartley, L. H., Kotchen, T. A., Mougey, E. H., Ricketts, P. T., & Jones, L. G. ⁄Pesce, C., Capranica, L., Tessitore, A., & Figura, F. (2003). Focusing of visual attention
(1973). Plasma cortisol and norepinephrine responses in anticipation of under submaximal physical load. International Journal of Sport and Exercise
muscular exercise. Psychosomatic Medicine, 35, 406–414. Psychology, 1, 275–292.
McAdam, R. E., & Wang, Y. K. (1967). Performance of a simple mental task ⁄Pesce, C., Casella, R., & Capranica, L. (2004). Modulation of visuospatial attention at
following various treatments. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 38, rest and during physical exercise: Gender differences. International Journal of
208–212. Sport Psychology, 35, 328–341.
McMorris, T. (2009a). Exercise and cognitive function: A neuroendocrinological ⁄Pesce, C., Cereatti, L., Casella, R., Caldari, C., & Capranica, L. (2007a). Preservation of
approach. In T. McMorris, P. D. Tomporowski, & M. Audiffren (Eds.), Exercise and visual attention in older expert orienteers at rest and under physical effort.
cognitive function (pp. 41–68). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 29, 78–99.
McMorris, T. (2009b). Exercise and decision making in team games. In T. McMorris, ⁄Pesce, C., Tessitore, A., Casella, R., Pirritano, M., & Capranica, L. (2007b). Focusing of
P. D. Tomporowski, & M. Audiffren (Eds.), Exercise and cognitive function visual attention at rest and during physical exercise in soccer players. Journal of
(pp. 180–192). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell. Sports Sciences, 25, 1259–1270.
McMorris, T., Collard, K., Corbett, J., Dicks, M., & Swain, J. P. (2008). A test of the Podolin, D. A., Munger, P. A., & Mazzeo, R. S. (1991). Plasma-catecholamine and
catecholamines hypothesis for an acute exercise–cognition interaction. lactate response during graded-exercise with varied glycogen conditions.
Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 89, 106–115. Journal of Applied Physiology, 71, 1427–1433.
⁄McMorris, T., Davranche, K., Jones, G., Hall, B., & Minter, C. (2009). Acute ⁄Pontifex, M. B., & Hillman, C. H. (2007). Neuroelectric and behavioral indices of
incremental exercise, performance of a central executive task, and interference control during acute cycling. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118,
sympathoadrenal system and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis activity. 570–580.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 73, 334–340. Qin, S., Hermans, E. J., van Marle, H. J., & Fernández, G. (2012). Understanding low
⁄McMorris, T., & Graydon, J. (1996a). The effect of exercise on the decision-making reliability of memories for neutral information encoded under stress:
performance of experienced and inexperienced soccer players. Research Alterations in memory-related activation in the hippocampus and midbrain.
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 67, 109–114. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 4032–4041.
⁄McMorris, T., & Graydon, J. (1996b). Effect of exercise on soccer decision-making Ramos, B. P., & Arnsten, A. F. (2007). Adrenergic pharmacology and cognition: Focus
tasks of differing complexities. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 30, on the prefrontal cortex. Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 113, 523–536.
177–193. Rektor, I., Kanovsky, P., Bares, M., Brázdil, M., Streitová, H., Klaiblová, H., et al.
⁄McMorris, T., & Graydon, J. (1997). The effect of exercise on cognitive performance (2003). A SEEG study of ERP in motor and premotor cortices and in the basal
in soccer-specific tests. Journal Sport Science, 15, 459–468. ganglia. Clinical Neurophysiology, 114, 463–471.
McMorris, T., & Graydon, J. (2000). The effect of incremental exercise on cognitive Sanders, A. F. (1983). Towards a model of stress and human performance. Acta
performance. International Journal of Sports Psychology, 31, 66–81. Psychologica, 53, 61–97.
⁄McMorris, T., & Keen, P. (1994). Effect of exercise on simple reaction times of Sartorius, A., Hellweg, R., Litzke, J., Dormann, C., Vollmayr, B., Danker-Hopfe, H.,
recreational athletes. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 78, 123–130. et al. (2009). Correlations and discrepancies between serum and brain levels of
⁄McMorris, T., Myers, S., MacGillivary, W. W., Sexsmith, J. R., Fallowfield, J., Graydon, neurotrophins after electroconvulsive treatment in rats. Pharmacopsychiatry, 42,
J., et al. (1999). Exercise, plasma catecholamine concentration and decision- 270–276.
making performance of soccer players on a soccer-specific test. Journal Sport Sothmann, M. S., Hart, B. A., Horn, T. S., & Gustafson, A. B. (1991). Plasma
Sciences, 17, 667–676. catecholamines and performance association during psychological stress:
T. McMorris, B.J. Hale / Brain and Cognition 80 (2012) 338–351 351

Evidence for peripheral noradrenergic involvement with an attention- Urhausen, A., Weiler, B., Coen, B., & Kindermann, W. (1994). Plasma catecholamines
demanding task. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 23, 860–867. during endurance exercise of different intensities as related to the individual
Ströhle, A., Stoy, M., Graetz, B., Scheel, M., Wittmann, A., Gallinat, J., et al. anaerobic threshold. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 69, 16–20.
(2010). Acute exercise ameliorates reduced brain-derived neurotrophic van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Wylie, S. A., Forstmann, B. U., Burle, B., Hasbroucq, T.,
factor in patients with panic disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2010). To head or to heed? Beyond the surface of selective
384–388. action inhibition: A review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 222.
⁄Stroth, S., Kubesch, S., Dieterle, K., Ruchsow, M., Heim, R., & Kiefer, M. (2009). doi.10.3389/fnhum.2010.00222.
Physical fitness, but not acute exercise modulates event-related potential Wann, B. P., Audet, M. C., & Anisman, H. (2010). Impact of acute and chronic stressor
indices for executive control in healthy adolescents. Brain Research, 1269, experiences on heart atrial and brain natriuretic peptides in response to a
114–124. subsequent stressor. Hormones and Behavior, 58, 907–916.
Tang, S. W., Chu, E., Hui, T., Helmeste, D., & Law, C. (2008). Influence of exercise on Winter, B., Breitenstein, C., Mooren, F. C., Voelker, K., Fobker, M., Lechtermann, A.,
serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor concentrations in healthy human et al. (2007). High impact running improves learning. Neurobiology of Learning
subjects. Neuroscience Letters, 431, 62–65. and Memory, 87, 597–609.
⁄Themanson, J. R., & Hillman, C. H. (2006). Cardiorespiratory fitness and acute Wittfoth, M., Kustermann, E., Fahle, M., & Herrmann, M. (2008). The influence of
aerobic exercise effects on neuroelectric and behavioural measures of action response conflict on error processing: Evidence from event-related fMRI. Brain
monitoring. Neuroscience, 141, 757–767. Research, 1194, 118–129.
Tian, Y., Nie, J., Tong, T. K., & Baker, J. S. (2012). The release of ⁄Yanagisawa, H., Dan, I., Tsuzuki, D., Kato, M., Okamoto, M., Kyutoku, Y., et al.
immunosuppressive factor(s) in young males following exercise. Sensors (2010). Acute moderate exercise elicits increased dorsolateral prefrontal
(Basel), 12, 5586–5595. activation and improves cognitive performance with Stroop test. Neuroimage,
Tomporowski, P. D. (2003). Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta 50, 1702–1710.
Psychologica, 112, 297–324. Yanagita, S., Amemiya, S., Suzuki, S., & Kita, I. (2007). Effects of spontaneous and
Tomporowski, P. D., & Ellis, N. R. (1986). Effects of exercise on cognitive processes: A forced running on activation of hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing hormone
review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 338–346. neurons in rats. Life Sciences, 80, 356–363.
⁄Travlos, A. K., & Marisi, D. Q. (1995). Information processing and concentration as a Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to the
function of fitness level and exercise-induced activation to exhaustion. rapidity of habit formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18,
Perceptual and Motor Skills, 80, 15–26. 459–482.

You might also like