You are on page 1of 22

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/16053310

Interparental Conflict and Children of Discord and Divorce

Article  in  Psychological Bulletin · October 1982


DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.92.2.310 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
976 10,280

1 author:

Robert Emery
University of Virginia
168 PUBLICATIONS   8,947 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Child effects View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Robert Emery on 14 September 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychological Bulletin Copyright 1982 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
1982, Vol. 92, No. 2, 310-330 0033-2909/82/9202-0310S00.75

Interparental Conflict
and the Children of Discord and Divorce
Robert E. Emery
University of Virginia

Data on the relation between marital turmoil (i.e., discord and divorce) and
behavior problems in children are reviewed. It is concluded that a relation be-
tween the two domains docs exist. Several parameters of this relation are outlined,
including type of marital turmoil, form of the child's behavioral response, sex
differences, age effects, parental buffering, and effects of parental psychopa-
thology. Conclusions drawn from this review are used to evaluate several broad
etiological hypotheses about the effect of marital turmoil on children, and im-
plications for the treatment of behavior problems in children from these families
are discussed. Finally, interpretative and methodological refinements are sug-
gested for future research.

The idea that marital turmoil 1 is the cause family for at least several months, primarily
of a variety of behavior problems in children as a result of divorce (Cherlin, 1977; Glick
is widely held both in the public and in the & Norton, 1978). The census bureau has
professional domain. During the first 6 reported a 79% increase in the number of
months of 1980, such prominent media as single-parent families between 1970 and
Newsweek and the New York Times Mag- 1980, so the current prevalence is one in five
azine ran cover stories on children of divorce, (U.S. Census Bureau, 1980). Because not
and the National Institute of Mental Health every discordant marriage is dissolved, these
solicited research proposals on the topic. astronomical figures on the number of chil-
Recent interest has been spurred by popular dren of divorce omit a significant and largely
movies and books, which themselves reflect unknown number of children who are ex-
the turmoil found in an increasing number posed to serious marital conflict.
of American families. In fact, the Academy Psychologists have been called on to pro-
Award winner for best motion picture in vide both explanations and treatments for
1980 depicted the problems of marital sep- families who are affected by marital turmoil.
aration and child custody (Kramer vs. Kra- The response has encompassed almost the
mer), and the 1981 winner portrayed family entire range of possibilities. Some psychol-
conflict in a two-parent household (Ordinary ogists have suggested that whenever there
People). is a problem child, there is a problem mar-
It has been estimated that 38% of the first riage (Framo, 1975); others have concluded
marriages of women in their late twenties that a relation between divorce and child
will end in divorce (Glick & Norton, 1978). problems has yet to be demonstrated (Her-
Although divorce is slightly less frequent zog & Sudia, 1973). The relative lack of
among couples who have children under 5 sophisticated research regarding the relation
years of age, an estimated 45% of the chil- between marital turmoil and child problems
dren born in 1977 will live in a one-parent encourages such a diversity of opinion. Nev-

1
I would like to thank K. Daniel O'Leary, Alan O. The term turmoil refers to families characterized
Ross, and Everett Waters, as well as John Darley, as- by discord in marriage, separation, or divorce as a group.
sociate editor, and several anonymous reviewers for their The term marital problems is occasionally used as a
comments on earlier versions of this article. synonym. Interparental conflict is used to denote open
Requests for reprints should be sent to Robert Emery, hostility between married, separated, or divorced par-
Department of Psychology, Gilmer Hall, University of ents. Marital discord refers to problems in intact mar-
Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia 22901. riages only.

310
CHILDREN OF DISCORD AND DIVORCE 311

ertheless, careful examination of the litera- nonclinic ones on some third variable that
ture reveals that a considerable body of in- increased estimates of the association be-
formation is available. tween marital and child problems (O'Leary
& Emery, in press).
Some data suggest that the expectation
Covariation Between Marital bias2 (Rosenthal, 1966) of raters of child
and Child Problems behavior who are not blind to the marital
relationship can also lead to higher estimates
A number of investigators have found a of the relation between marital and child
relation between discord in intact marriages problems. Robinson and Anderson (Note 3)
and the severity or frequency of behavior found that a significant correlation between
problems in children. This rinding remains mothers' checklist ratings of their marriage
consistent across such countries as the United and their children's adjustment was reduced
States (Emery & O'Leary, in press; Olt- to a chance level when social desirability was
manns, Broderick, & O'Leary, 1977; Porter partialed out. Emery (Note 2) found stron-
& O'Leary, 1980), England (Rutter, 1971, ger correlations between mothers' marital
1979), and India (Chawla & Gupt, 1979). ratings and their own judgments of distur-
Similarly, divorce and child problems have bance in their children than between moth-
often been found to be related (Anthony, ers' marital ratings and teachers' child ad-
1974; Hetherington, 1979; McDermott, justment scores. Because mothers and teach-
1968, 1970; Wallerstein & Kelly, 1980; ers rate behavior in different settings,
Westman, Cline, Swift, & Kramer, 1970). however, situation-specific behavior could
Many studies of marital and child problems, explain this result. In fact, one might expect
however, suffer from one or more of a variety child behavior to be most affected in the set-
of methodological flaws. The three most ting where the marital turmoil is present.
common problems are (a) biased sampling— Thus, independent measures of marital dis-
usually from a clinic population; (b) non- cord and child problems in the home setting
independent data—that is, the same judges may be exceedingly difficult, but important,
rate both the marriage and the child, or, in to obtain.
the case of divorce, the judges of child be- Unlike the effects of using clinic samples
havior are aware of the marital status; and and nonindependent ratings, the use of un-
(c) the use of measures lacking in reliability reliable measures would tend to attenuate
and validity. Each of these problems may rather than increase estimates of the mag-
affect estimates of the magnitude of the as- nitude of the association between marital
sociation between marital and child prob- and child problems. Researchers are not
lems. fully satisfied with the reliability and validity
Stronger associations between marital dis- of the available measures of either childhood
cord and child adjustment seem to be found adjustment (Achenbach, 1978) or marital
in clinic rather than in nonclinic samples relations (O'Leary & Turkewitz, 1978). It
(O'Leary & Emery, in press). In a series of is not surprising, then, that many of the re-
studies by the Stony Brook group, all of ports reviewed below used measures of ques-
which used parents' ratings on well-estab-
lished measures of child behavior (Behavior
2
Problem Checklist, Quay & Peterson, Note The expectation—held by parents, teachers, and
1) and marital adjustment (Short Marital mental health professionals—that marital turmoil causes
Adjustment Test, Locke & Wallace, 1959), behavior problems in children not only may create
biased rating data but also may be problematic in that
stronger associations were found in clinic a self-fulfilling prophecy may result. For example, par-
(Emery & O'Leary, in press; Oltmanns et ents who attribute a child's (normal) misbehavior to an
al, 1977; Porter & O'Leary, 1980) than in emotional reaction to marital turmoil may not respond
nonclinic samples (Oltmanns et al., 1977; to that child with their usual discipline. Thus, in their
attempt to understand the child, some parents may set
Emery, Note 2). Significant relations were limits that are inconsistent and confusing and thereby
found in both sets of samples; the clinic sam- accidentally create the problems that they are trying to
ples, however, may have differed from the avoid.
312 ROBERT E. EMERY

tionable adequacy. Unreliability is especially Pathogenic Dimensions of Marital Turmoil


important to consider when comparing cor-
relations between a given measure and sev- A necessary step in further specifying the
eral other measures of different reliabilities. covariation between marital and child prob-
In this case, differences between the correla- lems is to define marital turmoil and child
tions that appear to be substantive may, in behavior problems more precisely. This task,
fact, only reflect error variance. however, is extremely difficult. Neither the
Given the above concerns, one could choose field of abnormal child psychology (Achen-
to attend to only the very small number of bach, 1978) nor the study of marital rela-
reports that contain none of these problems. tions (Gurman & Kniskern, 1978; O'Leary
This option was rejected in the present re- & Turkewitz, 1978) has produced a widely
view for four reasons. First, the topic poses accepted classification system. The present
important questions that must be answered review takes a cautious approach to child
by clinicians on the basis of existing data. adjustment by limiting discussions primarily
It seems worthwhile, therefore, to attend to to two broad and well-accepted dimensions
whatever evidence is available, albeit with of child disturbance: problems of overcontrol
appropriate caution toward methodological (e.g., anxiety, withdrawal) and problems of
limitations. Second, close inspection of the undercontrol (e.g., aggression, conduct dis-
data across studies often reveals a conver- orders; Achenbach & Edelbroch, 1978).
gence of results from studies containing dif- More specific problems, as identified by var-
ferent flaws. It is unlikely, though possible, ious investigators, are occasionally men-
that different flaws would consistently lead tioned. It also should be made clear that this
to similar erroneous conclusions. Third, some review is concerned only with children's so-
of the results that may be attributable to cial behavior that is considered by parents,
methodological differences are of interest in teachers, or mental health professionals to
their own right. For example, the difference be maladaptive. Important factors such as
between clinic and nonclinic samples is an cognitive development and adjustment dur-
intriguing one. Finally, in those few studies ing adult life are not reviewed here.
that contain none of the three problems, A definition of marital turmoil is more
marital and child problems are related. That difficult to construct. A movement toward
is, researchers who used well-established specificity has occurred among divorce re-
measures while studying intact, nonclinic searchers who originally studied single-par-
families have found that teachers' ratings of ent families (grouping the divorced, wid-
children's behavioral adjustment were re- owed, and unmarried together), then focused
lated to independently obtained parental re- on divorce in particular, and now increas-
ports of discord (Block, Block, & Morrison, ingly recognize the importance of studying
1981; Emery, Weintraub, & Neale, in press; the conflict between divorced or divorcing
Rutter, 1971; Emery, Note 2). Similarly, parents (Levitin, 1979). Unfortunately, how
divorce and child adjustment problems have one best defines and measures conflict, in
been found to be related in nonclinic samples intact or in broken families, is a matter of
where child behavior was indexed by such controversy. Three theoretically relevant as-
independent measures as behavioral obser- pects of the definition are the process of con-
vation, teacher ratings, and peer nomina- flict (e.g., hitting, arguing, avoidance), its
tions (Hetherington, 1972; Hetherington, content (e.g., sex, child rearing, money), and
Cox, & Cox, 1978). Thus, although meth- the length of time it lasts.
odology can affect estimates of the magni- The ideal system, therefore, would eval-
tude of the relation between marital and uate families in terms of two discrete cate-
child problems, the relation is nevertheless gories: family structure (divorced, separated,
a real and important one. One can be con- intact) and content of conflict. It would also
fident that the results of investigations that provide magnitude estimates along two con-
used weaker methods are not wholly attrib- tinuua representing the length and the pro-
utable to methodological third variables. cess (from openly hostile to calm) of conflict
CHILDREN OF DISCORD AND DIVORCE 313

within each content category. Except, how- & Thurber, 1962; Nye, 1957; Power, Ash,
ever, for the structural divorced/intact dis- Schoenberg, & Sorey, 1974). Third, as noted
tinction, which is preserved throughout the throughout this review, children's responses
present review, researchers have neither pre- to divorce and discord share many features.
cisely nor uniformly measured marital tur- That is, undercontrolled behavior, sex dif-
moil. This is unfortunate. Particular content, ferences, and buffering effects are commonly
process, and temporal aspects of conflict ap- found among the children of both discord
pear to have more detrimental effects on and divorce. Fourth, children of divorced
children than do other aspects. parents who continue to have conflicts be-
yond the divorce have more problems than
Separation From a Parent Versus do children from conflict-free divorces as
Interparental Conflict shown both by clinical impressions (An-
thony, 1974; Kelly & Wallerstein, 1976) and
A critical question is whether separation by research results (Hetherington, Cox, &
from a parent per se or the interparental Cox, 1976; Jacobson, 1978; Westman et al,
conflict that is concomitant with divorce is 1970). Finally, one longitudinal investiga-
more strongly related to child behavior prob- tion found that many of the problems evident
lems. This distinction is particularly relevant in children from broken homes were present
because it bears on such issues as whether well before the children were separated from
parents should stay together for the chil- a parent (Lambert, Essen, & Head, 1977).
dren's sake. The association between divorce In sum, in studies of clinic and nonclinic
and behavior problems in children has been groups that have used both independent and
interpreted as evidence that separation per nonindependent raters, interparental conflict
se has a direct and substantial negative effect has been associated with behavior problems
on the child, regardless of the circumstances in children whether that conflict occurred in
surrounding the separation (Bowlby, 1973). intact marriages, before a divorce, or after
This interpretation has met with popular a divorce. Thus, considerable evidence sup-
support; thus, beliefs about the negative ef- ports Hetherington's observation that chil-
fects of a broken home are widely held. dren from broken or intact homes charac-
Recent reviews suggest that this causal terized by interparental conflict are at a
hypothesis has failed to consider adequately greater risk than are children from broken
the effects of interparental conflict (Rutter, or intact homes that are relatively harmo-
1971, 1979). Current evidence suggests that nious (Hetherington, Cox, & Cox, 1979).
interparental conflict, not separation, may
be the principal explanation for the associ- Separation and Life-Change Responses
ation found between divorce and continuing
childhood problems. Five different research Although such data argue against attrib-
approaches support this conclusion. First, uting the association between divorce and
several investigators have compared children child behavior disorders solely to separation
from homes broken by divorce or separation from a parent, there is no suggestion that
with those from homes broken by death. separation has no effect on the child. An
More behavior problems were found in "acute distress syndrome" is commonly found
homes broken by divorce, suggesting that in children on separation from a parent
something other than separation per se is (Bowlby, 1973; Rutter, 1979). This reaction
having a significant effect on these children follows a three-stage process of acute upset
(Douglas, Ross, Hammond, & Mulligan, followed by apathy or depression, and, fi-
1966; Gibson, 1969; Gregory, 1965). Sec- nally, loss of interest in the parents (Bowlby,
ond, other researchers have found that chil- 1973) or adaptation to the new situation
dren from broken but conflict-free homes (Rutter, 1972).
were less likely to have problems than were Awareness of the acute distress syndrome
children from conflictual, unbroken homes is important to the understanding of children
(Gibson, 1969; McCord, JM McCord, W,, from homes where a parent is leaving or has
314 ROBERT E. EMERY

recently left. Children's separation responses seem to be important determinants of the


in reaction to divorce need to be docu- effect of that conflict on the child. Conflict
mented, as do the effects of variations in the that is openly hostile exposes the child to
treatment of the children of divorce that may more, presumably pathogenic, parental in-
affect separation responses (e.g., visitation teractions, as does conflict that lasts for a
patterns, different custody arrangements). long period of time. The few studies that
Still, there are two further reasons why the have investigated these variables are in
effects of separation per se appear to be less agreement with this conjecture. Rutter et al.
important than the results of the concomi- (1974), using interview ratings, found a
tant conflict. First, as suggested by the stronger relation between child problems
three-stage model and according to the and unhappy marriages characterized by
impressions of clinicians and researchers, the quarrelsomeness than between child prob-
separation effects appear to be time-lim- lems and unhappy marriages characterized
ited (Anthony, 1974; Hetherington, 1979), by apathy. Similarly, Porter and O'Leary
whereas conflict responses may be more en- (1980) found that a self-report measure of
during (Hetherington et al., 1978). Second, open marital conflict was a superior predic-
even those behavioral changes found follow- tor of problems in children when compared
ing conflict-free divorce cannot be wholly with a general index of marital satisfaction.
attributed to separation per se (Rutter, Finally, in a study of the effects of divorce,
1979). Significant life changes occur con- Hetherington (Note 4) found that encap-
comitantly with divorce and further compli- sulated conflict was more weakly associated
cate inferences about the effects of separa- with child behavior problems than was open
tion per se (Hetherington, 1979; Kurdek, hostility. It should be noted, however, that
1981). These include sometimes dramatic although these results are high in face va-
changes in financial status (Herzog & Sudia, lidity, the first two studies were flawed in
1973) and altered discipline practices (Heth- that data were obtained from a single source
erington et al., 1976). (i.e., parents). Further documentation of the
In sum, it must be recognized that paren- effects of open conflict would therefore be
tal conflict does not terminate with the mar- useful.
riage, and it may, in fact, increase after di- Evidence also supports the presumption
vorce. Thus, much of the association be- of the increasingly detrimental effects of
tween divorce and ongoing child behavior continuing conflict. Rutter (1980) studied
problems may be explained by a frequently children who were separated from their
overlooked third variable, namely, interpar- homes at an early age as a result of family
ental conflict. As indicated later, the "con- discord or distress. He found that the chil-
flict responses" found among the children of dren who later resided in harmonious homes
divorce have features in common with the were at a decreased risk for emotional dis-
responses of children from discordant, intact turbance when compared with their earlier
marriages. The two areas of investigation, status. On the other hand, those children
however, are treated separately insofar as who stayed in homes characterized by con-
children can have at least three sets of re- flict continued to show problem behavior.
actions to divorce: conflict responses, sepa- Studies of divorce are consistent with this
ration responses, and life-change responses finding. When there is postdivorce turbu-
associated with the new single-parent family. lence between parents, children have more
Although the latter two types of responses problems and more frequently come to the
are important, children's conflict responses attention of professionals (Hetherington et
in both broken and intact families are the al., 1976; Kelly & Wallerstein, 1976; West-
present concern. man et al., 1970). Because most children of
divorce are exposed to some unknown amount
Amount and Type of Conflict of predivorce conflict, these data suggest that
the termination of conflict, as opposed to its
Both the amount and type of interparental continuation or exacerbation, is to the chil-
conflict to which the child is exposed would dren's benefit.
CHILDREN OF DISCORD AND DIVORCE 315

The effects of both the amount and type linquency and found a substantial covaria-
of conflict merit more detailed investigation tion (e.g., McCord & McCord, 1959). Sub-
because all marriages have some periods of sequent investigations, which have included
conflict. Further study of these two variables measures of other child behavior, have pro-
could provide valuable information on how vided mixed results.
best to handle normal interparental dis- In studies of divorce, Tuckman and Regan
agreements (in intact and in broken mar- (1966) found that clinic children from homes
riages) and on when professional help should broken by divorce or separation were apt to
be sought. have conduct problems, whereas children
To summarize, the following can be con- from intact families or homes broken by
cluded about the dimensions of marital rela- death had more anxiety-related problems.
tions relevant to children's adjustment: The McDermott (1968, 1970) reported that con-
distinction between divorced and intact fam- duct problems were most prevalent in his
ilies is important because separation and divorce sample, but he also found signs of
life-change responses are adaptations that depression in these children. Whereas the
are unique to the children of divorce. On the above two studies were based on noninde-
other hand, children of both divorce and pendent, clinical impressions, a study by
discord are likely to show conflict responses. Hetherington et al. (1978) showed that non-
In an assessment of the effects of interpar- clinic children of divorce were more depen-
ental conflict, distinctions along hostile-calm dent, disobedient, aggressive, whining, de-
and temporal continuua and across various manding, and unaffectionate than were chil-
content areas appear to be important. Un- dren from intact marriages as indexed by
fortunately, such distinctions have not been reliable observational measures.
made consistently in past research. In fact, In studies of intact marriages, McCord et
the reader should be alerted that, unless oth- al. (1962) found that discord was related to
erwise noted, the studies reviewed below feminine-aggressive behavior, antisocial be-
used summary measures of conflict or dis- havior, and sex anxiety but not to abnormal
cord. These consist either of global judg- fears in a nonclinic sample. In a clinic group,
ments provided by a parent or an interviewer Pemberton and Benady (1973) found that
or of rating scales that sum ratings of various marital discord was related to childhood
content and process aspects of marital rela- aggression and encopresis but not to enu-
tions. resis. Porter and O'Leary (1980) found mar-
ital discord to be related both to problems
Additional Parameters of the Association of conduct and to anxiety in clinic children,
Between Marital and Child Problems but two similar investigations found signif-
icant relations only for conduct problems
A review of the research literature reveals
additional variables that more precisely (Emery & O'Leary, 1982; Oltmanns et al.,
specify the relation between marital turmoil 1977). Also in direct conflict, Rutter (1971;
and child behavior problems. These variables Wolkind & Rutter, 1973) found significant
relations for conduct problems but not for
are (a) form of the child's behavioral re-
sponse, (b) sex effects, (c) age effects, (d) neurotic problems in nonclinic samples,
effects of having a good relationship with one whereas another British general population
parent, and (e) effects of parental psycho- report found significant relations for both
pathology. conduct and neurotic problems (Whitehead,
1979). Finally, in a study that used exem-
Form of the Child Problem plary methodology, Block et al. (1981) found
that a measure of parental disagreement
It has been suggested that discord in intact about child rearing was more strongly re-
marriages is related to children's disorders lated to teachers' ratings of undercontrol of
of undercontrol but not to their disorders of impulses than to their ratings of overcontrol
overcontrol (Rutter, 1971). Most of the early of impulses in nonclinic children who were
investigations of marital turmoil and prob- evaluated 1, 2, and 4 years after the marital
lem children focused on relations with de- index was completed.
316 ROBERT E. EMERY

Although these studies present a some- discord and their ratings of behavior prob-
what conflicting pattern of results, every in- lems at home for boys, whereas no signifi-
vestigation, whether of questionable or sound cant relations were found for girls. Similarly,
methodology, found marital turmoil to be Gassner and Murray (1969) reported find-
related to some form of undercontrolled be- ing a greater difference in the degree of ob-
havior. The results for overcontrol were in- served parental conflict between groups of
consistent. The fact that undercontrol is typ- clinic and nonclinic boys than between sim-
ically rated more reliably than overcontrol ilar groups of girls.
(Cantwell, Russell, Mattison, & Will, 1979; A factor that affects the sex difference
Quay & Peterson, Note 1; Emery, Note 2) more than the setting may be the type of
may partially account for this difference. sample used. In nonclinic samples of intact
Still, it seems safe to conclude that inter- marriages, problems in both boys and girls
parental conflict—in broken and in intact have been found to be related to discord
families—appears to be related more strongly (Block et al., 1981; Whitehead, 1979; Emery,
to children's problems of undercontrol than Note 2), whereas in clinic samples, relations
of overcontrol. have been found only for boys (Emery &
O'Leary, 1982; Porter & O'Leary, 1980).
When it is noted that Rutter's (1971) non-
Different Effects on Boys and Girls
clinic sample was preselected to contain a
Considerable recent evidence suggests that high proportion of disturbed children and
marital turmoil has a greater effect on boys children of parents with individual psycho-
than on girls from both divorced (Cadoret pathology—thus making it comparable in
& Cain, 1980; Hess & Camara, 1979; Heth- many ways to a clinic group—this study, too,
erington et al, 1978; McDermott, 1968; is consistent with the clinic/nonclinic sex
Wallerstein & Kelly, 1980) and intact, dis- differences.
cordant marriages (Block et al., 1981; Emery How is this pattern of results to be ex-
& O'Leary, 1982; Porter & O'Leary, 1980; plained? An examination of how each sex
Rutter, 1971; Wolkind & Rutter, 1973). may respond to marital turmoil may hold an
Nevertheless, a careful examination of the answer. Two nonclinic studies of intact mar-
literature suggests that one must consider a riages (Block et al., 1981; Whitehead, 1979)
number of factors when interpreting this and one of divorce (Hess & Camara, 1979)
finding. found marital turmoil to be related directly
On the basis of data obtained in interviews to measures of undercontrol only for boys,
with nonclinic families, Rutter (1971) found whereas those weaker associations that were
that discord in intact marriages was asso- found for girls were with overcontrolled be-
ciated with school problems in boys but not havior. (In fact, one study [Block et al.,
in girls. Whitehead (1979) questioned this 1981] found that discord was inversely re-
result. Citing Becker's (1964) work docu- lated to undercontrol in girls.) Because chil-
menting that parental punitiveness was re- dren are more likely to be referred to clinics
lated only to boys' behavior in school but to because undercontrolled behavior is more
both boys' and girls' behavior at home, readily identified as a problem than is over-
Whitehead argued that the relation with controlled behavior (Ross, 1980), it may be
marital turmoil was obscured because girls the case that boys respond to interparental
inhibit aggression in school. She presented conflict in a more pervasive, maladaptive
data from a nonclinic sample showing sig- manner that is more likely to lead to a clinic
nificant associations between mothers' re- referral. Thus, it may be that there is a sex
ports of the marriage and both boys' and difference in response to marital discord, but
girls' problems at home. The results reported the difference is in how and how much the
by other investigators (Emery & O'Leary, sexes respond, not whether they do.
1982; Porter & O'Leary, 1980), however, A further complication exists in inter-
are contrary to Whitehead's. In samples of preting sex differences among the children
clinic children, significant relations were of divorce. These differences appear to be a
found between parents' ratings of marital result of at least a two-fold process. First,
CHILDREN OF DISCORD AND DIVORCE 317

as in studies of conflict in intact families, Although such speculations are common,


differences betwen the sexes in terms of con- surprisingly little evidence is available on
flict responses are expected. In addition, differential age effects. Two investigations
there may be a second effect related to being of discord in intact marriages that controlled
in the custody of the opposite-sex parent. for age found no major effect (Porter &
Santrock and Warshak (1979), in a study O'Leary, 1980; Rutter, Graham, Chadwick,
that used observational and self-report mea- & Yule, 1976). These studies, however, ob-
sures of a nonclinic sample, found that both tained nonindependent data and were lim-
boys and girls of divorce had more behavior ited to children 6 years of age or older.
problems when the opposite-sex parent had Wallerstein and Kelly (1974, 1975; Kelly
custody than when the same-sex parent had & Wallerstein, 1975, 1976), in contrast, sug-
custody. Because divorced mothers currently gested that divorce has unique effects on
have child custody over 90% of the time children of different ages, from 2 years to
(Glick, 1979), boys would be expected to adolescence. Their results were based solely
have more postdivorce adjustment problems on clinical impressions and small samples,
than would girls. Thus, it appears that boys however, and therefore cannot be viewed as
whose parents divorce and whose mothers reliable. Further, although these investiga-
have custody have a dual risk for developing tors reported subtly different effects at dif-
behavior problems. ferent ages, all age groups were found to be
In sum, it can be concluded that marital affected substantially by divorce.
turmoil, in several studies of adequate meth- Results from studies examining the effects
odology, is more strongly related to boys' of a child's age at the time of divorce have
than to girls' obviously maladaptive behav- been inconsistent. Findings that suggest
ior. One needs to consider, however, setting more detrimental effects of separations oc-
effects, sampling, form of the response, and curring before the age of five or six (Heth-
custody arrangements in interpreting sex erington, 1972), after this age (Gibson,
differences. Further, it is possible that the 1969; McCord et al, 1962), and no effect
effects on girls may be delayed and, as was of age at the time of separation (Power et
found in one study, appear prominently in al., 1974), have all been reported. Thus, it
subsequent relations with the opposite sex has not been demonstrated that a child's age
(Hetherington, 1972). Girls are likely to be is an important determinant of the effects
just as troubled by marital turmoil as boys of marital turmoil. Nevertheless, age contin-
are, but they may demonstrate their feelings ues to be of interest and merits careful con-
in a manner that is more appropriate to their sideration in future investigations.
sex role, namely, by becoming anxious, with-
drawn, or perhaps very well behaved.
Good Parent-Child Relationships
Child's Age Some recent empirical investigations have
supported the clinical impression that the
Various arguments suggest that either parent-child relationship deteriorates as a
older or younger children should be more result of marital turmoil (Hess & Camara,
affected by marital turmoil. For example, it 1979; Hetherington et al., 1979; Rutter,
could be argued that younger children are 1971). None of these investigators, however,
more susceptible because of their greater suggest that deterioration is inevitable, and,
dependence on their parents. Children as in fact, it is argued that a1 good relationship
young as 1 year old have been shown to re- with one parent can "buffer" the child from
spond to interparental conflict with upset some of the negative effects of marital tur-
and anger (Cummings, Zahn-Waxier, & moil (Hetherington et al., 1979).
Radke-Yarrow, 1981). On the other hand, In a study of intact, nonclinic families,
older children are more sensitive to emotion Rutter (1971) reported that a good relation-
and may feel pressure to become involved ship with at least one parent significantly
in interparental conflict, thus making them- reduced the likelihood that boys would be
selves more vulnerable to its effects. judged antisocial by their teachers. It is
318 ROBERT E. EMERY

worth noting, however, that boys from dis- & McNeil, 1968). Further, disordered in-
cordant marriages still had more problems dividuals are more likely to have discordant
than did boys from happy marriages, despite marriages and to divorce (Molholm & Din-
the good parent-child relationship. Further,itz, 1972); thus, it is possible that marital
a good parent-child relationship was strictly
turmoil may partially explain the increased
defined as consisting of both positive warmth
problems among the children of these indi-
and lack of negative feelings as determined viduals.
by parental interview. Data supporting this possibility come from
Similarly, only particularly good parent-Rutter (1971). He found that discord in in-
child relationships produced buffering ef- tact marriages was related to antisocial be-
fects in a study of children of divorce (Heth-
havior in the children both of normal parents
erington et al., 1979). Children's adjust- and of parents with a personality disorder.
ment, as measured by multiple methods, was On the other hand, when the marriage was
better only when, based on several parentingharmonious, there was no increased antiso-
indices, the parent-child relationship was cial behavior associated with parental per-
classified as very good. Relationships judged
sonality disturbance. There was, however, a
to be moderate had no buffering effect. Fur-trend toward an even greater risk for anti-
ther, the very good relationship had to be social problems when both discord and per-
with the mother. Positive father-child rela-sonality disturbance were present.
tions did not mitigate the detrimental effects Emery et al. (in press) reported similar
of marital turmoil. findings among the children of parents with
In another nonclinic study of divorce, other diagnoses. In intact marriages discord
Hess and Camara (1979) also found that explained most of the association between
measures of the quality of the parent-child parents' affective disorder (unipolar depres-
relationship were important predictors of sion or bipolar disorder) and children's dis-
child adjustment as indexed by multiple in- turbed school behavior. When the effect of
dependent measures. In fact, statistical anal-
marital discord was taken into account, little
ysis revealed that parent-child relations pre-
association was found between these disor-
dicted a greater proportion of the variance ders and children's behavior. But when the
in child adjustment than did interparental diagnosis was parental schizophrenia, mar-
conflict. As with Rutter's (1971) study, how-
ital discord did not explain the children's
ever, conflict was related to children's ad-problems in school. In fact, little association
justment even when there was a good par- was found between discord and child behav-
ent-child relationship. Finally, in contrastior problems in the schizophrenic group, per-
to Hetherington's findings, good relation- haps because the children of schizophrenics
ships with either parent appeared to have a were already disturbed for other reasons.
buffering effect. Another possibility is that, although the
In sum, there is emerging evidence based global satisfaction ratings were similar, mar-
on adequate methodology that a particularly ital discord may be qualitatively different in
warm relationship with at least one parent schizophrenic families. Conflict may be re-
can mitigate, but not eliminate, the effectsduced because one partner is acknowledged
of marital turmoil on children. Without as sick.
doubt, the quality of parent-child relation- These two reports—both based on non-
ships deserves further attention in studies of
clinic samples, independent raters, and re-
both discord and divorce. liable measures—suggest that, except in the
case of schizophrenia, concomitant marital
Individual Parental Psychopathology turmoil may explain a good part of the in-
creased problems among the children of be-
It is well documented that children from haviorally disordered parents. Researchers
families where a parent has a psychological need to examine more closely the dual, non-
disturbance are at an increased risk for a independent effects of interparental conflict
variety of behavioral problems (Mednick and parental psychopathology on children.
CHILDREN OF DISCORD AND DIVORCE 319

Mechanisms By Which Marital Turmoil ment figure is lost for any reason, anxiety
Affects Children results.
The specificity of this hypothesis makes
Research on marital and child problems it easily testable, but theoretical assumptions
has not generally been guided by broad eti- in relation to marital turmoil may have been
ological rationales. Rather, mini-theories overstated. The strongest prediction from
that offer only a few predictions or make no the attachment hypothesis is that divorce—
allowance for critical evaluation are more because it involves separation from a par-
common. Because of such theoretical defi- ent—has a direct, negative effect on the
ciencies, this review evaluates several hy- child. Although separation responses have
potheses about how marital turmoil may clearly been demonstrated, evidence re-
produce childhood disorders: (a) disruption viewed earlier suggests that concomitant in-
of attachment bonds, (b) modeling, (c) al- terparental conflict, rather than separation
tered discipline practices, and (d) other mod- per se, is the more salient factor in creating
els, including stress, taking on the symptom, behavior problems in the children of divorce.
and child effects. Further, the hypothesis predicts that mater-
Each hypothesis is evaluated in terms of nal deprivation is the most damaging be-
the parameters of the relation between mar- cause the mother is most commonly the pri-
ital and child problems. Where appropriate, mary attachment figure. Divorce usually re-
theoretical assumptions are weighed against sults in paternal deprivation, thus the
the conclusions that (a) concomitant con- hypothesized effects on these children are
flict, not separation per se, appears to be re- unclear.
sponsible for many of divorce's serious, long- Other parameters discussed earlier are
term pathogenic effects; (b) openly hostile also in conflict with the attachment hypoth-
and continued conflict has a great effect; (c) esis. Separations before the age of five are
the most prominent behavior disorder is a predicted to be the most damaging (Bowlby,
problem of undercontrol; (d) boys demon- 1973), but no consistent effects of age at the
strate a greater observable response than do time of divorce were found. Problems dem-
girls; (e) age has not been shown to alter the onstrated in the animal literature as result-
child's reaction; (f) a good relationship with ing from maternal deprivation are frequently
at least one parent can partially buffer the related to fears and poor social development
negative effects; and (g) increased interpar- (Rutter, 1972), yet marital turmoil was
ental conflict may explain many of the nega- found to be most strongly associated with
tive effects found among children of parents conduct disorders. Finally, sex differences
with individual psychopathology. Other di- are not explicitly predicted by the theory.
rect evidence is discussed where it is avail- Variations on the attachment hypothesis
able and appropriate. (e.g., Goldstein, Freud, & Solnit, 1973) have
suggested that loss of love (emotional de-
Attachment and Separation privation) may be the central cause of prob-
lems. In support of this hypothesis, some
One of the few detailed hypotheses about studies comparing clinic and nonclinic fam-
the effect of marital turmoil on children is ilies found that parents of clinic children
the maternal deprivation hypothesis (Bowlby, were more negative with their children (Bug-
1969). According to this account, the loss ental, Love, & Kaswan, 1972; Lobitz &
of a figure to whom one is attached (usually Johnson, 1975a; Love & Kaswan, 1974).
the mother) produces an instinctive fear re- Further, review of the buffering effect sug-
sponse. The survival value of the fear re- gested that a good parent-child relationship
sponse accounts for its instinctive nature. may lessen the detrimental effect of marital
Fear keeps the infant proximal to the care- turmoil.
taker, thus increasing the chances for sur- Nevertheless, it is unclear whether loss of
vival. On the basis of this rationale, Bowlby love accounts for these findings or whether
(1973) concluded that whenever an attach- some other aspect of the parent-child rela-
320 ROBERT E. EMERY

tionship produces the effect. Some direct Modeling


evidence supports the latter alternative. Us-
ing a measure of felt acceptance rated by Modeling is a second major etiological
the child (Schaefer, 1965), Emery and mechanism by which marital turmoil may
O'Leary (1982), in a clinic sample, found affect children. Schwarz (1979) suggested
that children's perceptions of being accepted that interparental conflict may interfere with
by their parents were not significantly re- imitation of the same-sex parent or may lead
lated to parental ratings of satisfaction with to rejection of both parents as models. Either
their intact marriages. Further, although the process would disrupt normal socialization
children were generally aware of marital in that appropriate parental behavior might
discord, their ratings of felt acceptance did not be imitated, and other, more deviant,
not correlate significantly with their own rat- models might be found. Other modeling hy-
ings of parental discord. Thus, marital dis- potheses are also applicable. For instance,
cord had little direct effect on the children's parents in an unhappy marriage may exhibit
feelings of being loved, perhaps because par- more hostile and aggressive behavior than
ents were particularly concerned about this do happily married couples, and this behav-
possibility and explicitly guarded against it, ior could be imitated by their children. Un-
In sum, although it is unlikely that de- happy couples show more conflict than do
privation from a parent's love is the principal happy couples (Jacobson & Martin, 1976;
explanation for the association between mar- Patterson, Weiss, & Hops, 1976), and much
ital turmoil and ongoing behavior problems of this behavior might occur in the presence
in children, the attachment hypothesis has of their children. The increase in negativity
made a valuable contribution in identifying toward children that corresponds with mar-
the acute distress syndrome. Researchers ital turmoil (Johnson & Lobitz, 1974) could
would do well to attempt to identify more be interpreted as evidence in support of this
fully separation responses in the children of latter hypothesis.
divorce. This could lead to distinguishing Considerable research on the parameters
children's reactions to the difficult, but ap- of marital turmoil is consistent with mod-
parently temporary, 1-2-year separation pe- eling hypotheses. Interparental conflict, as
riod identified by divorce researchers (Heth- opposed to separation from a parent, would
erington, 1979) from their ongoing behavior be expected to be related more strongly to
problems that appear to have a different child behavior problems. The stronger rela-
cause. Such information would be of great tion between interparental conflict and con-
value to clinicians and to parents who could duct problems is also congruent with a mod-
be educated as to what to expect from chil- eling hypothesis. Disagreements and argu-
dren as a natural course of adaptation during ments are often features of marital turmoil,
a trying period in family life. and aggression is readily imitated by chil-
Before we leave the topic of attachment dren (Bandura, 1973). Because the specifics
and separation, two additional points should of the interparental conflict and children's
be made. First, although loss of love is prob- conduct problems may be quite different,
ably not the primary mediating variable be- however, research that details the corre-
tween marital and child problems, it does spondence between how parents express hos-
account for important variance in explaining tility and how children respond to witnessing
behavior problems of childhood (e.g., Ar- it is needed.
mentrout, 1971; Emery & O'Leary, 1982). Modeling explanations of the sex differ-
The attachment hypothesis therefore merits ence in response to marital turmoil are per-
further consideration. Second, the specificity haps most interesting. The modeling litera-
of the hypothesis has stimulated interest, ture suggests that boys are, in general, more
offered testable predictions, and generated likely to imitate aggressive behavior than are
research that states clear objectives. The girls (Flanders, 1968). The observed sex dif-
area would benefit from similarly precise ference in response to marital turmoil may
hypotheses. simply be a subset of this general sex dif-
CHILDREN OF DISCORD AND DIVORCE 321

ference. An alternative modeling explana- of child rearing as warm-hostile, restric-


tion is that fathers in an unhappy marriage tive-permissive, and calm-anxious (Becker,
are more aggressive and uncooperative and 1964). In addition, reinforcement theories
that boys imitate fathers more than girls do. such as coercion (Patterson & Reid, 1970)
There is evidence that children are more and the positive reinforcer trap (Wahler,
likely to imitate a same-sex model (Bandura, 1976) have been offered. An attempt to ad-
1969; Margolin & Patterson, 1975). The dress each dimension in detail would be cum-
possibility that husbands in disturbed mar- bersome; therefore, the following discussion
riages act more aggressively, whereas wives treats discipline practices as a group.
react with more anxiety and withdrawal, is This general approach prohibits a detailed
readily translated into testable research. evaluation in terms of the parameters of the
Modeling hypotheses may also explain the relation between child and marital problems.
sex differences in children's separation and Still, there are congruencies. Discipline is a
life-change responses to different custody frequent topic of argument, and disagree-
arrangements. Because mothers have cus- ment about discipline in front of a child ob-
tody in most divorces (Glick, 1979), divorce viously produces more inconsistent discipline
may have a more noticeable effect on boys than when the disagreement is kept private
because the male model is missing. Santrock (Hetherington et al., 1976; Rutter, 1972).
and Warshak's (1979) finding of better ad- Problems of conduct and aggression have
justment when the same-sex parent has cus- frequently been related to inconsistent dis-
tody is congruent with the hypothesis, as is cipline (Becker, 1964; Becker et al., 1962;
evidence suggesting that contact with adults McCord, McCord, & Howard, 1961; Pat-
other than the custodial parent is beneficial terson, 1977); thus, an increased frequency
to the child's development (Jacobson, 1978). of undercontrolled behavior might be ex-
Its apparent fit with data on the various pected. But insofar as these are not the only
parameters of marital and child problems is aspects of child behavior that are affected
a strength of the modeling hypothesis. Nev- by inconsistent discipline, more specific dis-
ertheless, modeling explanations, unlike at- cipline predictions need to be formulated.
tachment theory, typically have been offered For example, a coercion hypothesis might
post hoc. Further, several variations of the predict that parents in marital turmoil be-
hypothesis offer viable etiological explana- come more likely to give in to a child's coer-
tions. These variations deserve further spec- cive demands.
ification and testing. As with modeling theory, discipline ex-
planations of sex differences are of great in-
Discipline Practices terest. Several accounts are plausible. For
example, evidence suggests that parents are
A third mechanism by which marital tur- more involved with disciplining the same-sex
moil may affect children is through an al- child than the opposite-sex child (Baumrind,
teration in discipline practices. Clinic and 1971). If marital turmoil causes fathers to
nonclinic parents have been differentiated on alter their discipline more radically than
a variety of measures of discipline (Becker, mothers do, a greater effect would be seen
Peterson, Luria, Shoemaker, & Hellmer, on sons. Researchers have previously been
1962); it is possible that marital turmoil warned not to ignore the father's effect on
leads to a change in the use of important the family (Becker, Peterson, Hellmer,
discipline techniques, to the detriment of the Shoemaker, & Quay, 1959).
child, or it may lead to increased inconsis- The father's role is also highlighted if one
tency in discipline either between parents or hypothesizes that sons receive more incon-
in the practices of a single parent. sistent discipline during times of marital tur-
Potential discipline hypotheses are nu- moil than do girls. Research shows that sons
merous because a wide range of discipline are disciplined about equally by both par-
constructs have been offered. These con- ents, whereas daughters are disciplined more
structs include such traditional dimensions by their mothers (Margolin & Patterson,
322 ROBERT E. EMERY

1975). If parents disagree about discipline, consistent discipline is a mediating factor


boys should be affected more than girls be- between marital turmoil and child behavior
cause boys are disciplined more often by problems. As such, the investigation provides
both parents. For example, fathers may a model for future research.
counteract maternal discipline of their sons In sum, discipline is an important influ-
but not object to the mothers' rules for their ence on children and one that is altered con-
daughters. In short, sons more frequently comitantly with marital problems. Inconsis-
than daughters may become the focus of in- tent discipline, in particular, demands con-
terparental conflict about discipline. tinued attention, according to the results of
Direct evidence on the relation between two well-constructed investigations.
marital turmoil and discipline practices is
rare. One sound empirical study (Hether- Other Hypotheses
ington et al., 1976) documented changes in
postdivorce discipline. When compared with Three additional hypotheses—stress, tak-
parents from intact marriages, divorced par- ing on the symptom, and child-effects—
ents were found to make fewer maturity de- make important contributions to under-
mands, have poorer communication, be less standing how marital turmoil may affect
affectionate, and be more inconsistent with children. Because the data on these hy-
their children. Effects of the discipline potheses are more limited than that on the
changes were that children of divorce, espe- previous hypotheses, their unique implica-
cially boys, were less compliant with paren- tions are presented below with less critical
tal commands than were children of intact evaluation of each hypothesis as a whole.
marriages. This latter finding is particularly Stress. Marital problems can be concep-
important to the discipline hypothesis be- tualized simply as a stressor to which chil-
cause it was also found that boys were ex- dren, as well as adults, are differentially ex-
posed to more ineffective discipline than posed. Marital disruption has been noted to
were girls (Hetherington et al., 1978). have pathogenic effects on adults (Bloom,
Changes in discipline practices were Asher, & White, 1978), and it may similarly
greatest 1 year after the divorce. By the sec- pose problems for children in that coping
ond year, differences between intact and di- with life's difficulties takes a psychological
vorced parents were less noticeable (Heth- toll.
erington et al., 1976). It is not known, how- A stress hypothesis is of interest primarily
ever, whether the changes in discipline began in explaining the differential sex effects of
before or after the divorce. This is a very marital turmoil. The simplest stress hypoth-
important distinction for questions of etiol- esis would suggest that exposure to the
ogy and treatment. Still, the study demon- stressor is different for each sex. That is, girls
strated that discipline was disrupted by mar- may be shielded from interparental conflict
ital turmoil, although when the change oc- more than are boys, Clinical impressions
curred and what type of change most affected that this is indeed the case have been offered
the child are important questions that have (Wallerstein & Kelly, 1980). Research,
been left unanswered. however, has shown that parents report
Supporting the possibility that discipline fighting equally in front of children of both
may change before a divorce, a study of in- sexes (Porter & O'Leary, 1980) and that
tact marriages (Block et al., 1981) found boys and girls report similar awareness of
that an index of parental disagreement about discord between their parents (Emery &
child rearing was related both to subsequent O'Leary, 1982). Further, if differential ex-
marital dissolution and to future undercon- posure accounted for the sex effects, mea-
trol in boys' and overcontrol in girls' school sures of open conflict or of children's aware-
behavior. Inconsistency in discipline was ness of conflict should relate at least to over-
thus shown to be both an aspect of marital controlled problems in girls, but this has not
turmoil and a precursor of problems in chil- been the case (Emery & O'Leary, 1982;
dren. Such prospective findings provide Porter & O'Leary, 1980).
strong support for the hypothesis that in- As an alternative, the sexes may be ex-
CHILDREN OF DISCORD AND DIVORCE 323

posed equally to interparental conflict, but symptom of interparental conflict in the sex-
girls may be less susceptible to the stressor. role-appropriate manner of becoming ag-
One such explanation is that girls are less gressive, noncompliant, and so on. This very
susceptible than boys to a variety of physical noticeable disruptiveness may indeed dis-
stressors; therefore, they may also have a tract a boy's parents from their own prob-
greater resistance to psychological stress lems. Undercontrolled behavior is more ob-
(Erne, 1979; Rutter, 1970). vious than overcontrolled behavior and might
A second such explanation of the sex dif- facilitate the distraction.
ference is suggested by a recent investiga- But why wouldn't girls serve the same
tion. Male and female college students were function? Perhaps they do, but girls' behav-
found to be equally accurate in recognizing ioral distractions might have a very different
marital conflict, but the two sexes made dif- topography. Rather than being disruptive,
ferent interpretations of the conflict. Specif- girls may become anxious, withdrawn, or
ically, when judging marital interactions even very well behaved. Girls may actually
that were seen as equally discordant by both refocus parental attention by their extreme
sexes, college women rated the couple as prosocial behavior. For girls, prosocial and
more "right for each other" (Epstein, Fin- overcontrolled behavior is more sex-role ap-
negan, & Bythell, 1979). This suggests that propriate than is undercontrolled behavior,
sex-role socialization may explain the dif- therefore being nice or worried may serve
ferent effect on boys and girls. Although the distraction function better. This hypoth-
both sexes are equally aware of the discord, esis would explain the paradox in the Block
parental conflict may be interpreted differ- et al. (1981) data that showed that, for girls,
ently by girls, who may view the same parental disagreement was related to over-
stressor as being less stressful. control in the expected direction but was in-
A diathesis-stress model offers a final versely related to undercontrol. At the least,
stress explanation of the sex difference. a taking-on-the-symptom hypothesis sug-
Base-rate data on childhood disorders in the gests that prosocial or competent behavior
United States suggest that boys are predis- be included as a dependent measure in stud-
posed toward undercontrol, whereas girls are ies of marital turmoil.
predisposed toward overcontrol (Ross, 1980). Child-effects, Instead of the assumption
Given this diathesis and a marital turmoil that marital turmoil causes problems in chil-
stressor, each sex may be at risk for devel- dren, the reverse argument could be true;
oping a disorder. Because undercontrol is namely, that a deviant child places strain on
more easily recognized and less tolerated a marriage. Bell and Harper's (1977) cogent
than overcontrol, however, the finding that arguments concerning such child-effects need
boys are more affected than girls by marital to be considered. Indeed, it has been found
turmoil would be expected. that children can decrease marital satisfac-
Taking on the symptom. The hypothesis tion as indexed by such findings as (a) the
that one member of a family becomes the decline of marital satisfaction after the birth
focus of larger family conflicts also merits of the first child, (b) the negative effect on
consideration. Minuchin and his colleagues a marriage as a result of rearing a handi-
(Minuchin, 1974; Minuchin et al., 1975) capped child, and (c) survey data wherein
suggested that children serve the function of parents report that their children are an
distracting their parents from their own con- added stress on their marriage (Lerner &
flicts by developing a problem themselves Spanier, 1978). It is likely that a conduct
and thus redirecting parental concerns. Chil- problem child would similarly strain a mar-
dren's defusion of interparental conflict by riage.
"taking on the symptom" is, therefore, a Despite the consideration of a child-ef-
fifth etiological hypothesis. fects alternative, the causal sequence from
This hypothesis offers yet another expla- parent to child is probably a more important
nation of the sex differences and also might pathway. One group of researchers (Olt-
explain some data on the form of the child's manns et al., 1977) found that marital sat-
behavioral response. Boys may take on the isfaction did not increase concomitantly with
324 ROBERT E. EMERY

parent-rated improvement in problem chil- problems appears to be clear: Efforts should


dren's behavior, a result contrary to a child- be made to minimize children's involvement
effects prediction. Further, from a strict in interparental conflict. Although this may
child-effects position, it becomes difficult to seem to be only common sense, there are
explain why boys disrupt a marriage more therapists who argue that one must increase
or why open conflict is the principal result family-wide conflict following a "civilized"
of a child's problem behavior. Still, as others divorce as a means of allowing the members
have noted (Lerner & Spanier, 1978; Mar- to work through their feelings (e.g., Futter-
golin, in press), the best explanation of the man, 1980).
relation between the two areas of difficulty At the level of prevention, four suggestions
is reciprocal influence. Marital and child can be offered. First, parents should work
problems are best viewed as interactive: To toward the difficult goal of keeping their
an extent, each causes and exacerbates the children out of their angry disagreements,
other. lest the children learn that differences are
resolved by yelling, fighting, or hitting. Sec-
ond, parents should attempt always to agree
Summary in front of the child about at least one im-
It is unlikely that any single hypothesis portant topic: discipline. Third, parents
fully explains the relation between marital should make a special effort to maintain
and child problems, yet each may prove to their individual relationship with each child,
have merit. Parents involved in conflict with as this may partially buffer the child from
each other are probably poorer models, are the interparental conflict. Finally, more par-
more inconsistent in their discipline, and ents need to be aware that conflicts between
place more stress on their children. Some them can have negative effects on their chil-
children probably serve to distract attention dren. Parents need to be sensitive to how
away from parental conflict, whereas others their children react to marital turmoil and
may aggravate the conflict. Attachment to be prepared to seek outside help if these
bonds are certainly disrupted by separation reactions are prolonged.
and may also be affected by conflict. These At the treatment level, families in which
(and perhaps other) processes are likely to marital and child problems coexist present
operate collectively in affecting the children particularly difficult problems. This is docu-
of marital turmoil, although, in any given mented both by clinical impressions (Kent
instance, one influence may predominate. & O'Leary, 1976; Patterson, Cobb, & Ray,
When the hypotheses are compared, mod- 1973) and by research that suggests that
eling and discipline practices would seem to treatment is less effective (Strain, Young,
hold particular merit in light of the available & Horowitz, 1979; Clark & Baker, Note 5).
data. Still, because most of the data have Because of this difficulty therapists need to
preceded specific predictions, even these two be open to new approaches and roles. For
etiological accounts cannot be given strong example, the suggestion that individually
support. This general lack of hypothesis test- oriented interventions may be less successful
ing is characteristic of the literature on mar- than treatment that takes place on the fam-
ital and child problems and should not con- ily level (e.g., Minuchen, 1974) deserves
tinue. It is hoped that the above delineation consideration. Behavioral family therapy
of various potential etiological mechanisms was shown in one study (Oltmanns et al.,
will encourage future investigators to make 1977) to be effective despite the presence of
specific predictions based on theoretical ra- marital discord. As an alternative, recent
tionales. evidence suggests that the marriage rather
than the entire family may be the more ap-
Implications for Treatment propriate initial level for treatment (Mar-
golin & Christensen, Note 6). Families with
Although questions of etiology are far both marital and child problems were found
from being answered, at least one implica- to improve more on self-report and obser-
tion of the research on marital and child vational measures of the family, the mar-
CHILDREN OF DISCORD AND DIVORCE 325

riage, and the child when randomly assigned should also know that, unfortunately, psy-
to behavioral marital therapy as opposed to chologists do not yet have all the answers to
family treatment (Margolin & Christensen, the many questions on how marital turmoil
Note 6). Although these data are prelimi- affects children.
nary, they point out the importance of com-
parisons of innovative treatments. Future Trends
Child and family therapists must also be
willing to adopt other, nontraditional ap- If answers to some of the questions are to
proaches. Parents who are divorced or who be forthcoming, much more sound research
come from discordant marriages often do not will have to be done. Given below are some
receive the spousal support that can main- interpretative and methodological consider-
tain positive efforts at child rearing (Wahler, ations that will need to be addressed in ad-
1980). Therapists, therefore, should consider dition to specific proposals for future re-
group approaches to parent training when search.
family and couple options are impossible.
Therapists also need to take a more active Methodological Considerations
role in legal decisions involving marital and
child problems. For example, Haynes (1978) Because marital and child problems can
suggested a new role for mental health be studied only in a correlational design and
professionals as divorce mediators. Rather because alternative interpretations are theo-
than resolving decisions such as child cus- retically relevant to the topic, reverse cau-
tody and visitation via the adversarial legal sality and third variable interpretations need
process, it is possible that more of these de- to be considered. Reverse causality was dis-
cisions can be reached with the help of an cussed earlier as the etiological model of
impartial mediator. The latter process would child-effects; only third variables are consid-
seem to offer opportunities for reducing con- ered now.
flict, whereas the former procedure is likely A variety of strategies are available for
to exacerbate it. investigating the effects of third variables.
Finally, therapists need to keep in touch The important point is to recognize those
with the emerging empirical literature and variables that are potential mediators. In the
distinguish it from opinion. For example, marital-child area, variables related to mar-
contrary to popular belief, the literature re- ital turmoil such as social class and parental
viewed here suggests that divorce may ulti- psychopathology can be controlled for in
mately result in a less detrimental environ- various research designs. Indeed, it has been
ment for a child's emotional development, shown that these two variables do not fully
at least in homes where conflict is great and account for the observed relation (Rutter,
where divorce will lead to a diminution of 1971; Rutter et al., 1974), although the fi-
that conflict. Such an interpretation might nancial decline experienced by one-parent
not only benefit children from intact but con- families clearly produces important life-
tinually conflicted families, but it might also change responses in the children (Colletta,
relieve divorced parents' guilt over the ir- 1979; Herzog & Sudia, 1973). Other vari-
reversible damage they may believe they ables such as postdivorce custody arrange-
have caused their children. Further, the fo- ments, visitation patterns, and social support
cus on interparental conflict underscores networks also need to be measured and con-
both parents' continued responsibility to im- trolled as relevant third variables.
prove relations with the former spouse for One third variable mentioned earlier is
the children's sake. Nevertheless, despite the worth discussing again: response bias in non-
fact that divorce may sometimes be the least independent reports of marital and child
detrimental alternative, evidence does sug- problems. Recent investigations suggest that
gest that divorcing parents should be pre- parents' reports of behavior problems in
pared for three unique reactions from their their children can sometimes reflect more
children: conflict responses, separation re- about the parent than about the child. In one
sponses, and life-change responses. Parents study (Griest, Wells, & Forehand, 1979),
326 ROBERT E. EMERY

maternal ratings of child adjustment were populations, usually children referred for
more strongly related to maternal depression therapy. Although a relation between mar-
than to the child's observed behavior. It was ital and child problems is found in nonclinic
argued that the depression had colored the samples (Block et al., 1981; Gibson, 1969;
mothers' perceptions. Rutter et al., 1976), stronger associations
It is possible that a similar bias might ex- seem to be found within clinic groups
plain some of the association found between (O'Leary & Emery, in press; Oltmanns et
marital turmoil and child behavior problems. al., 1977). The study of nonclinic families
One way to avoid this problem is to use in- would not only provide valuable epidemio-
dependent raters or observational measures. logical data but would also control for two
Because parents have unique access to many important confounds. First, people who seek
aspects of their children's behavior, however, treatment are self-selected and represent an
it is important to determine whether marital ill-defined subgroup of the general popula-
turmoil does alter parents' perceptions of tion. Second, the fact of being the subject
their children. The comparison of parental of both research and treatment may have
report with observational measures (Griest problematic reactive effects. Because it has
et al., 1979), the assessment of rating biases been demonstrated that it is easier to fake
such as social desirability (Robinson & An- bad (i.e., make the family look more dis-
derson, Note 3), and the measurement of turbed than it is) than to fake good (i.e.,
cognitive set toward evaluating children's make the family look healthier than it is;
behavior (Messe, Stollak, Larson, & Mi- Lobitz & Johnson, 1975b), the possibility
chaels, 1979) are three methods that can be that clinic samples might wish to make their
used in obtaining data on this issue. family and children look more disturbed in
In addition to third-variable considera- order to justify treatment is of most concern.
tions, methodological improvements are
needed. The taxonomy of child and marital Research Directions
problems and the instruments available to
measure their dimensions need to be devel- Although suggestions for research have
oped further. In the assessment of children, been made throughout the text, four specific
more reliable measures of social competence research proposals deserve special attention.
and of overcontrol are especially needed. First, a large, cross-sectional, multi-pre-
With regard to marital problems, instru- dictor epidemiological study on a represen-
ments are needed that will assess the process tative U.S. sample is needed. The degree of
and the length of conflict independently covariation between types of marital turmoil
along different continuua, within discrete and child behavior problems has not yet been
categories of conflict content. While the sys- documented in sufficient detail. Marital tur-
tems of classification and measurement of moil is related to increased behavioral dis-
child and marital problems are being re- turbance in children, but it cannot yet be
fined, research should use at least one ac- clearly specified how great the risk is. The
cepted measure of each problem. Measures value of epidemiological data would be
of adequate reliability are available on the greatly augmented by including moderator
self-report of child adjustment (Achenbach variables such as the type, length, and con-
& Edelbrock, 1978) and marital relations tent of interparental conflict; the age and sex
(Cromwell, Olson, & Fournier, 1976) as well of children; and the presence of psycholog-
as on the observation of child behavior (Pat- ical problems in the parents, as well as by
terson, Ray, Shaw, & Cobb, Note 7) and including demographic information. Find-
marital interaction (Olson & Strauss, 1972; ings from such a cross-sectional analysis
Hops, Wills, Patterson, & Weiss, Note 8). would yield valuable data on which children
The definition of the population of interest in what circumstances are at greatest risk
is a final methodological consideration that because of a given type of marital problem.
needs to be addressed. Most of the investi- Second, more descriptive studies using
gations reviewed above have studied special detailed and reliable self-report and obser-
CHILDREN OF DISCORD AND DIVORCE 327

vational measures are needed. Comparisons and treatment directed at lowering this risk
between (a) intact and broken families char- have not been of high priority. Given the
acterized by interparental conflict and (b) importance and the frequency of the prob-
other families in terms of variables such as lems, this should not be allowed to continue.
open conflict, child-rearing practices, affec-
tion, treatment of children of different sexes, Reference Notes
and child behavior are needed. Data from 1. Quay, H. C., & Peterson, D. R. Manual for the
investigations of this nature could provide Behavior Problem Checklist. Unpublished manu-
tests of etiological accounts and offer de- script, 1979. (Available from D. R. Peterson, 39
scriptions that would help to develop a tax- North Fifth Street, Highland Park, New Jersey
onomy of marital and family relations. 08904.)
2. Emery, R. E. Marital discord and child behavior
Third, and perhaps most important, pro- problems in a nonclinic sample. Unpublished manu-
spective investigations are badly needed. script, University of Virginia, 1981.
Longitudinal data can provide controls for 3. Robinson, E, A., & Anderson, L. L. Family ad-
reverse causality as well as yield descriptive justment, parental attitudes, and social desirability.
information on the course of children's re- Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psy-
chological Association, Los Angeles, August 1981.
sponses to marital discord and divorce. Such 4. Hetherington, E. M. Personal communication, March
critical issues as (a) the distinction of sep- 22, 1981.
aration, life-change, and conflict responses; 5. Clark, D. B., & Baker, B. L. Training parents of
(b) the timing of changes in parent-child developmentally disabled children: Prediction of
follow-up outcome. 1. Paper presented at the meet-
interactions resulting from changes in par- ing of the American Psychological Association, New
ent-parent interactions; (c) the effect of con- York, August 1979.
flict of various types and lengths; and (d) the 6. Margolin, G., & Christensen, A. The treatment of
child's role in aggravating interparental con- families with marital and child problems. Paper
flict can be properly addressed only through presented at the meeting of the Association for the
Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Toronto, No-
prospective investigation. The added expense vember 1981.
involved in gathering longitudinal data is 7. Patterson, G. R., Ray, R. S., Shaw, D. A., & Cobb,
clearly offset by its quality and importance. J. A. Manual for coding of family interactions. Un-
Finally, studies are needed on how martial published manuscript, University of Oregon and
Oregon Research Institute, 1969.
turmoil affects the outcome of traditional 8. Hops, H., Wills, T. A., Patterson, G. R., & Weiss,
treatments for children as well as on the ef- R. L. Marital interaction coding system. Unpub-
fectiveness of new treatments. Alternative lished manuscript, University of Oregon and Oregon
treatments for study include (a) family ther- Research Institute, 1972.
apy > (b) conjoint therapy for parents (from
both intact and broken marriages) preceding References
treatment of the child, (c) teaching child Achenbach, T. W. Psychopathology of childhood: Re-
management to single parents in groups, and search problems and issues. Journal of Consulting
(d) divorce mediation as an alternative to and Clinical Psychology, 1978, 46, 759-776.
the legal, adversarial resolution of custody Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbroch, C. S. The classifica-
and visitation decisions. tion of child psychopathology: A review and analysis
of empirical efforts. Psychological Bulletin, 1978, 55,
Both marital and child problems are com- 1275-1301.
mon in the United States today, Careful in- Anthony, E. J. Children at risk from divorce: A review.
vestigation of the relation between the prob- In E. J. Anthony and C. Koupernik (Eds.), The child
lems could have broad implications for the in his family. III. New York: Wiley, 1974.
prevention and treatment of behavior dis- Armentrout, J. A. Parent child-rearing attitudes and
preadolescents' problem behaviors. Journal of Con-
orders in children and could allow profes- sulting and Clinical Psychology, 1971, 35, 278-285.
sionals to better meet the needs of both par- Bandura, A. Principles of behavior modification. New
ents and children. At present, the many chil- York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1969.
dren from homes where interparental conflict Bandura, A. Aggression: A social learning approach.
Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1973.
is a common occurrence are a high-risk pop- Baumrind, D. Current patterns of parental authority.
ulation. As demonstrated by the relatively Developmental Psychology Monographs, 1971, 4(1,
meager psychological literature, research Pt. 2).
328 ROBERT E. EMERY

Becker, W. C. Consequences of different kinds of pa- of marital discord on the school behavior of children
rental discipline. In M. L. Hoffman & L. W. Hoffman of schizophrenic, affectively disordered, and normal
(Eds.), Review of child development. New York: parents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, in
Russell Sage Foundation, 1964. press.
Becker, W. C., Peterson, D. R., Hellmer, L. A., Shoe- Epstein, N., Finnegan, D., & Bythell, D. Irrational be-
maker, D. J., & Quay, H. C. Factors in parental liefs and perceptions of marital conflict. Journal of
behavior and personality as related to problem be- Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1979, 67, 608-
havior in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical 609.
Psychology, 1959, 23, 107-118. Flanders, J. P. A review of research on imitative be-
Becker, W. C., Peterson, D. R., Luria, Z., Shoemaker, havior. Psychological Bulletin, 1968, 69, 316-337.
D. J., & Hellmer, L. A. Relations of factors derived Framo, J. L. Personal reflections of a therapist. Journal
from parent interview rating to behaviour problems of Marriage and Family Counseling, 1975, /, 15-28.
of five-year-olds. Child Development, 1962, 33, 509- Futterman, E. A. After the "civilized" divorce. Journal
535. of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 1980,
Bell, R. Q., & Harper, L. V. Child effects on adults. 19, 525-530.
Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 1977. Gassner, S., & Murray, E. J. Dominance and conflict
Block, J. H., Block, J., & Morrison, A. Parental agree- in the interactions between parents of normal and
ment-disagreement on child-rearing orientations and neurotic children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
gender-related personality correlates in children. Child 1969, 74, 33-41.
Development, 1981, 52, 965-974. Gibson, H. B. Early delinquency in relation to broken
Bloom, B. L., Asher, S. J., & White, S. W. Marital homes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
disruption as a stressor: A review and analysis. Psy- and Allied Disciplines, 1969, 10, 195-204.
chological Bulletin, 1978, 85, 867-894. Glick, P. C. Children of divorced parents in demo-
Bowlby, J. Attachment and loss: I. Attachment. New graphic perspective. Journal of Social Issues, 1979,
York: Basic Books, 1969. 55, 170-182.
Bowlby, J. Attachment and loss: II. Separation. New Glick, P. C., & Norton, A. J. Marrying, divorcing, and
York: Basic Books, 1973. living together in the U.S. today. Population Bulletin,
Bugental, D. E., Love, L. R., & Kaswan, J. W. Vid- 1978, 32, 3-38.
eotaped family interaction: Differences reflecting Goldstein, J., Freud, A., & Solnit, A. J. Beyond the best
presence and type of child disturbance. Journal of interests of the child. New York: Free Press, 1973.
Abnormal Psychology, 1912,79, 285-290. Gregory, I. Anterospective data following childhood loss
Cadoret, R. J., & Cain, C. Sex differences in predictors of a parent. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1965,
of antisocial behavior in adoptees. Archives of Gen- 13. 110-120.
eral Psychiatry, 1980, 37, 1171-1175. Griest, D., Wells, K. C., & Forehand, R. Examination
Cantwell, D. P., Russell, A. T., Mattison, R., & Will, of predictors of maternal perceptions of maladjust-
L. A comparison of DSM II and DSM III in the ment in clinic-referred children. Journal of Abnormal
diagnosis of childhood psychiatric disorders. Archives Psychology, 1979, 88, 277-281.
of General Psychiatry, 1979, 36, 1208-1228. Gurman, A. S., & Kniskern, D. P. Research on marital
Chawla, P. L., & Gupt, K. A comparative study of and family therapy: Progress, perspective, and pros-
parents of emotionally disturbed and normal children. pect. In S. L. Garfield & A. E. Bergin (Eds.), Hand-
British Journal of Psychiatry, 1979, 134, 406-411. book of psychotherapy and behavior change. New
Cherlin, A. The effect of children on marital dissolution. York: Wiley, 1978.
Demography, 1977, 14, 265-272. Haynes, J. M. Divorce mediator: A new role. Social
Colletta, N. D. The impact of divorce: Father absence Work, 1978, 23, 5-9.
or poverty? Journal of Divorce, 1979, 3, 27-38. Herzog, E., & Sudia, C. E. Children in fatherless fam-
Cromwell, R. E., Olson, D. H. L., & Fournier, D. G. ilies. In B. M. Caldwell & H. N. Riccuti (Eds.), Re-
Diagnosis and evaluation in marital and family coun- view of child development research (Vol. 3). Chicago:
seling. In D. H. L. Olson (Ed.), Treating relation- University of Chicago Press, 1973.
ships. Lake Mills, Iowa: Graphic, 1976. Hess, R. D., & Camara, K. A. Post-divorce family rela-
Cummings, E. M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, tionships as mediating factors in the consequences of
M. Young children's responses to expressions of anger divorce for children. Journal of Social Issues, 1979,
and affection by others in the family. Child Devel- 35, 79-96.
opment, 1981, 52, 1274-1282. Hetherington, E. M. Effects of father absence on per-
Douglas, J. W. B., Ross, T. M., Hammond, W. A., & sonality development in adolescent daughters. Devel-
Mulligan, D. G. Delinquency and social class. British opmental Psychology, 1972, 7, 313-326.
Journal of Criminology, 1966, 6, 294-302. Hetherington, E. M. Divorce: A child's perspective.
Erne, R. F. Sex differences in childhood psychopathol- American Psychologist, 1979, 34, 851-858.
ogy: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 1979, 86, 574- Hetherington, E. M., Cox, M., & Cox, R. Divorced
596. fathers. Family Coordinator, 1976, 25, 417-428.
Emery, R. E., & O'Leary, K. D. Children's perceptions Hetherington, E. M., Cox, M., & Cox, R. The aftermath
of marital discord and behavior problems of boys and of divorce. In J. H. Stevens & M. Mathews (Eds.),
girls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1982, Mother/Child Father/Child Relationships. Washing-
10, 11-24. ton, D.C.: National Association for the Education of
Emery, R. E., Weintraub, S., & Neale, J. M. Effects Young Children, 1978.
CHILDREN OF DISCORD AND DIVORCE 329

Hetherington, E. M, Cox, M., & Cox, R. Family in- evaluation of the Cambridge-Somerville Youth Study.
teraction and the social emotional and cognitive de- New York: Columbia University Press, 1959.
velopment of children following divorce. In V. Vaughn McCord, W., McCord, J., & Howard, A, Familial cor-
& T. Brazelton (Eds.), The family: Setting priorities. relates of aggression in non-delinquent male children.
New York: Science and Medicine, 1979. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1961,
Jacobson, D. S. The impact of marital separation/di- 62, 79-83.
vorce on children: II. Interparental hostility and child McDermott, J. F. Parental divorce in early childhood.
adjustment. Journal of Divorce, 1978, 2, 3-20. American Journal of Psychiatry, 1968, 124, 1424-
Jacobson, N. S., & Martin, B. Behavioral marriage 1432.
therapy: Current status. Psychological Bulletin, 1976, McDermott, J. F. Divorce and its psychiatric sequelae
83, 540-556. in children. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1970, 23,
Johnson, S. M., & Lobitz, G. K. The personal and mar- 421-427.
ital adjustment of parents as related to observed child Mednick, S. A., & McNeil, T. F. Current methodology
deviance and parenting behaviors. Journal of Abnor- in research on the etiology of schizophrenia: Serious
mal Child Psychology, 1974, 2, 193-207. difficulties which suggest the use of the high-risk
Kelly, J. B., & Wallerstein, J. S. The effects of parental group method. Psychological Bulletin, 1968, 70, 681-
divorce: 1. The experience of the child in early latency, 693.
2. The experience of the child in late latency. Amer- Messe, L. A., Stollak, G. E., Larson, R. W., & Michaels,
ican Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 1975, 45, 253-255. G. Y. Interpersonal consequences of person percep-
Kelly, J. B., & Wallerstein, J. S. The effects of parental tion processes in two social contexts. Journal of Per-
divorce: Experiences of the child in early latency. sonality and Social Psychology, 1979, 37, 369-379.
American Journal of'Orthopsychiatry, 197'6, 46, 20- Minuchin, S. Families and family therapy, Cambridge,
32. Mass.: Harvard University Press, 1974.
Kent, R. N., & O'Leary, K. D. A controlled evaluation Minuchin, S. et at. A conceptual model of psychoso-
of behavior modification with conduct problem chil- matic illness in children: Family organization and
dren. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, family therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1975,
1976, 44, 586-596. 32, 1031-1038.
Kurdek, L. A. An integrative perspective on children's Molholm, L. H., & Dinitz, S. Female mental patients
divorce adjustment. American Psychologist, 1981, and their normal controls. Archives of General Psy-
36, 856-866. chiatry, 1972, 27, 606-610.
Lambert, L., Essen, J., & Head, J. Variations in be- Nye, F. I. Child adjustment in broken and unhappy
havior ratings of children who have been in care. Jour- unbroken homes. Marriage and Family Living, 1957,
nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied 19. 356-361.
Disciplines, 1977, 18. 335-346. O'Leary, K. D., & Emery, R. E. Marital discord and
Lerner, R. M., & Spanier, G. B. Child influences on child behavior problems. In M. D. Levine & P. Satz
marital and family interaction: A life-span perspec- (Eds.), Developmental variation and dysfunction.
tive. New York: Academic Press, 1978. New York: Academic Press, in press.
Levitin, T. E. Children of divorce. Journal of Social O'Leary, K. D., & Turkewitz, H. Methodological errors
Issues, 1979, 35, 1-25. in marital and child treatment research. Journal of
Lobitz, G. K., & Johnson, S. M. Normal versus deviant Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1978, 46, 747-
children: A multimethod comparison. Journal of Ab- 758.
normal Child Psychology, 1975, 3, 353-374. (a) Olson, D. H., & Strauss, M. A. A diagnostic tool for
Lobitz, W. C., & Johnson, S. M. Parental manipulation marital and family therapy. Family Coordinator,
of the behavior of normal and deviant children. Child 1972, 21, 251-258.
Development, 1975, 46, 719-726. (b) Oltmanns, T. F., Broderick, J. E., & O'Leary, K. D.
Locke, H. J., & Wallace, K. M. Short marital-adjust- Marital adjustment and the efficacy of behavior ther-
ment and prediction tests: Their reliability and valid- apy with children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
ity. Marriage and Family Living, 1959, 21, 251-255. Psychology, 1977, 45, 724-729.
Patterson, G. R. Accelerating stimuli for two classes of
Love, L. R., & Kaswan, J. W. Troubled children: Their coercive behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psy-
families, schools, and treatments. New York: Wiley, chology, 1977, 5, 335-350.
1974. Patterson, G. R., Cobb, J. A., & Ray, R. S. A social
Margolin, G. The reciprocal relationship between mar- engineering technology for retraining the families of
ital and child problems. In J. P. Vincent (Ed.), Ad- aggressive boys. In H. E. Adams & I. P. Unikel
vances in Family Intervention, Assessment, and The- (Eds.), Issues and trends in behavior therapy. Spring-
ory. Greenwich, Conn.: JAI Press, in press. field, 111.: Charles C Thomas, 1973.
Margolin, G., & Patterson, G. R. Differential conse- Patterson, G. R., & Reid, J. B. Reciprocity and coer-
quences provided by mothers and fathers for their cion: Two facets of social systems. In C. Neuringer
sons and daughters. Developmental Psychology, 1975, & J. Michaels (Eds.), Behavior modification in clin-
/ / , 537-538. ical psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts,
McCord, J., McCord, W., & Thurber, E. Some effects 1970.
of paternal absence on male children. Journal of Patterson, G. R., Weiss, R. L., & Hops, H. Training
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1962,64, 361-369. in marital skills: Some problems and concepts. In H.
McCord, W., & McCord, J. Origins of crime: A new Leitenberg (Ed.), Handbook of behavior modifica-
330 ROBERT E. EMERY

lion and behavior therapy. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Schaefer, E. S. Children's reports of parental behavior:
Prentice-Hall, 1976. An inventory. Child Development, 1965,36, 413-421.
Pemberton, D. A., & Benady, D. R. Consciously re- Schwarz, J. C. Childhood origins of psychopathology.
jected children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1973, American Psychologist, 1979, 34, 879-885.
123. 575-578. Strain, P. S., Young, C. C., & Horowitz, J. An ex-
Porter, B., & O'Leary, K. D. Marital discord and child- amination of child and family demographic variables
hood behavior problems. Journal of Abnormal Child related to treatment outcomes during oppositional
Psychology, 1980, 80, 287-295. child training. Behavior Modification, 1979, 3, 61-
Power, M. J., Ash, P. M., Schoenberg, E., & Sorey, 76.
E. C. Delinquency and the family. British Journal Tuckman, J., & Regan, R. A. Intactness of home and
of Social Work, 1974, 4, 17-38. behavioural problems in children. Journal of Child
Rosenthal, R. Experimenter bias in behavioral re- Psychology and Psychiatry, 1966, 7, 225-233.
search. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1966. U.S. Census Bureau. Statistical abstract and the United
Ross, A. O. Psychological disorders of children. New States. Washington, D.C.: Author, 1980.
York: McGraw-Hill, 1980. Wahler, R. G. Deviant child behavior within the family:
Rutter, M. Sex differences in response to family stress. Developmental speculations and behavior change
In E. J. Anthony & C. Koupernik (Eds.), The child strategies. In H. Leitenberg (Ed.), Handbook of be-
in his family. New York: Wiley, 1970. havior modification and behavior therapy. Engle-
Rutter, M. Parent-child separation: Psychological ef- wood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1976.
fects on the children. Journal of Child Psychology Wahler, R. G. The insular mother: Her problems in
and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 1971,12, 233- parent-child treatment. Journal of Applied Behavior
260. Analysis, 1980, 13, 207-219.
Rutter, M. The quality of mothering: Maternal depri- Wallerstein, J. S., & Kelly, J. B. The effects of parental
vation reassessed. Harmondsworth, Middlesex, En- divorce: The adolescent experience. In E. J. Anthony
gland: Penguin, 1972. & C. Koupernik (Eds.), The child in his family: Chil-
Rutter, M. Maternal deprivation, 1972-1978: New dren at psychiatric risk. New York: Wiley, 1974.
findings, new concepts, new approaches. Child De- Wallerstein, J. S., & Kelly, J. B. The effects of parental
velopment, 1979, 50, 283-305. divorce: Experiences of the preschool child. Journal
Rutter, M. Protective factors in children's responses to of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 1975,
stress and disadvantage. In M. W. Kent & J. E. Rolf 14, 600-616.
(Eds.), Primary prevention of psychopathology: 111. Wallerstein, J. S., & Kelly, J. B. Surviving the breakup.
Promoting social competence and coping in children. New York: Basic Books, 1980.
Hanover, N.H.: University Press of New England, Westman, J. D., Cline, D. W., Swift, W. J., & Kramer,
1980. D. A. The role of child psychiatry in divorce. Archives
Rutter, M., Graham, P., Chadwick, O. F. D., & Yule, of General Psychiatry, 1970, 23. 416-420.
W. Adolescent turmoil: Fact or fiction? Journal of Whitehead, L. Sex differences in children's responses
Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci- to family stress: A re-evaluation. Journal of Child
plines, 1976, 17, 35-56. Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines,
Rutter, M. et al. Attainment and adjustment in two 1979, 20, 247-254.
geographical areas: III. Some factors accounting for Wolkind, S., & Rutter, M. Children who have been
area differences. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1974, "in care"—An epidemiological study. Journal of
125, 520-533. Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disci-
Santrock, J. W., & Warshak, R. A. Father custody and plines, 1973, 14. 97-105.
social development in boys and girls. Social Issues,
1979, 35, 112-125. Received May 26, 1981 •

View publication stats

You might also like