You are on page 1of 13

Decision Support Systems 61 (2014) 34–46

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Decision Support Systems


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/dss

Consumer decision-making across modern and traditional channels:


E-commerce, m-commerce, in-store☆,☆☆
Moutusy Maity a,⁎, Mayukh Dass b,1
a
Marketing Area, Indian Institute of Management Lucknow, 1309 Faculty Block, Plot B1, Sector 62, Noida, UP 201307, India
b
Marketing, Rawls College of Business, Texas Tech University, MS 2101, Lubbock, TX, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study investigates the effect of media richness on consumer decision-making and channel choice, and
Received 7 April 2013 grounds the investigation in media richness theory, task-media fit hypotheses and cognitive cost (behavioral de-
Received in revised form 20 December 2013 cision theory). Findings from three experiments provide evidence that consumers prefer channels with medium
Accepted 15 January 2014
(e.g., e-commerce) and high (e.g., in-store) media richness for carrying out complex decision-making tasks. Find-
Available online 24 January 2014
ings reveal that consumers are likely to undertake simple decision-making tasks on channels that incorporate
Keywords:
low (e.g., m-commerce) levels of media richness. Findings also demonstrate that product type moderates the ef-
E-commerce fect of media richness on perceived channel-task fit, post-purchase evaluation, and channel choice. These insights
M-commerce should prove helpful to managers in managing content across different channels.
Channel choice © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Consumer decision-making
Media richness
Cognitive cost
Product type

1. Introduction In this paper, we address this limitation in the literature, and


examine the role of media richness of channels on channel choice
With technological advancements, firms are increasingly reaching and decision-making tasks [12]. Media richness, as originally de-
out to their customers through a variety of channels such as electronic scribed by media richness theory (MRT) [15,16], is a set of objective
commerce, mobile commerce, and brick and mortar establishments characteristics such as feedback and communication capability,
[34]. The decision support systems literature has a strong tradition in language variety, and personal focus, which determine a channel's
studying the relationship between channels and consumer decision ability to communicate richness of information [35]. The more a me-
making [11,17,27,29,59]. These studies have mostly focused on how dium incorporates these elements, the richer it is [3,8,12,31,33].
customers evaluate, utilize [29], and adopt channels [59] and how the Media richness of channels has been represented in two ways in ex-
purchase environment (e.g., factors such as risk [27], trust and uncer- tant literature: a) different forms of technology usage in the same
tainty [7,53]) plays a fundamental role during the adoption process. channel impart different degrees of media richness to the channel
Although these studies advance our understanding of how consumers [32]; and b) different channels represent different degrees of media
often undertake decision-making tasks on separate channels [39], limited richness [12]. The latter method of representing media richness is
research exists on investigating how different channel characteristics widely accepted [32], and thus is used in this paper to investigate
affect decision-making and channel choice [46], across a variety of the effects of media richness of retail channels on consumer
channels. decision-making and channel choice.
Originally developed in the context of organization communica-
tion channels, media richness has subsequently been applied to
retail channels. Prior literature considering media richness as a channel
☆ This work was undertaken as part of the first author's dissertation at the University of
characteristic has mostly focused on examining its effect on message
Georgia, Athens, GA, USA.
☆☆ The authors wish to thank the New Media Institute at the University of Georgia, processing in interactive media [14], creation of telepresence [32],
Athens, GA, for providing infrastructure-related support for conducting the research understanding consumers' behavioral intentions [31], and efficacy of in-
reported in this paper. The study design, data collection, analyses and interpretation formation acquisition [12], among other things. Although these studies
of data, the writing of the report and the decision to submit the article for publication advanced our understanding of media richness in retail channels, we
are undertaken entirely by the submitting authors.
⁎ Corresponding author.
still have limited knowledge on how media richness affects channel
E-mail addresses: mmaity@iiml.ac.in (M. Maity), mayukh.dass@ttu.edu (M. Dass). choice and purchase tasks. In particular, this study raises and investigates
1
Fax: +1 806 742 2199. the following questions: How does media richness affect consumer

0167-9236/$ – see front matter © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dss.2014.01.008
M. Maity, M. Dass / Decision Support Systems 61 (2014) 34–46 35

decision-making and channel choice? How does media richness affect setting suggests that consumers access a website through a computer
perceived media richness-task fit? And, how does media richness impact terminal, whereas an m-commerce setting suggests that a mobile de-
post purchase evaluation? With increasing technological advances that vice is used as a channel. This study examines the effects of channel
enable channels to incorporate new capabilities (e.g., embed audio/ characteristics on consumer decision making across three channels:
video, pay through mobile device) and with firms interested in engaging in-store, e-commerce and m-commerce that represent three levels of
with the customer through multiple channels, it is imperative to conduct media richness.
a formal study to investigate the effects of media richness on consumer Decision-making inside a store involves face-to-face interactions,
decision-making. and has feedback and communication capabilities due to the avail-
This investigation focuses on three levels of media richness ability of a channel representative (in-store salespeople). These fea-
(i.e., high, medium, low) and undertakes three experimental studies tures afford a personal focus and a wide variety of language support.
that examine the effects of media richness on information search, and Hence, the in-store channel is high in media richness. Decision-
post purchase evaluation (e.g., satisfaction). The research examines per- making through e-commerce involves accessing websites through
ceived media richness-task fit, and evaluates the appropriate levels of computer terminals [13]. Most websites use text and images (in
media richness for different types of decision-making tasks. Product exclusion of audio and multi-media) to provide consumers with
type and task complexity are potential moderators. Findings provide the required information [48], and is the scope of e-commerce in
evidence that consumers prefer channels with medium media richness this study. These websites do not have the spontaneity in communi-
level over high richness for carrying out complex decision-making cation, feedback, and personal focus capabilities of the in-store chan-
tasks. Findings reveal that consumers are likely to undertake simple nel. Such websites may have wider language support than that
decision-making tasks on channels that incorporate low levels of available in-store, but are typically limited to a database of non-
media richness. Findings also demonstrate that product type moderates customized vocabulary. Hence, the media richness of e-commerce
the effect of media richness on perceived channel-task fit, post- is medium. Finally, m-commerce refers to the pairing of mobile
purchase evaluation, and channel choice. devices with commercial transactions, providing consumers with
The contributions of this paper are manifold. First, this paper in- the ability to carry out transactions through wireless Internet-
vestigates the effect of media richness on consumer decision- enabled devices [13]. In addition to all the features that are available on
making and channel choice, a crucial under-researched area, and e-commerce, m-commerce also offers portability [1], and ubiquity [1].
provides evidence that media richness is a driver of channel choice. The other major difference between e-commerce and m-commerce is
Second, from theoretical and managerial perspectives, this paper the interface (large versus small screen) of the mobile device. The setting
provides insights on media richness continuum and perceived of m-commerce imposes limitations, notably those of attention
media richness-task fit, which are essential for retail channel design constraints and small-screen access devices [9]. These differences limit
and content management. Finally, this research opens up many new the extent of communication, feedback and personal focus capabilities
avenues for future research. The paper proceeds as follows: the next that are possible on m-commerce. For example, in e-commerce, a con-
section discusses media richness theory, delineates the effect of sumer may simultaneously browse a product listing on a webpage, and
media richness on decision-making tasks in terms of cognitive cost watch a video about the product on the same computer screen. However,
and behavioral decision theory, and presents the hypotheses. The in m-commerce, such simultaneity is difficult to attain due to channel-
section that follows, presents three studies and the findings. The specific limitations. Moreover, this investigation includes internet-
paper ends with theoretical and managerial implications of the find- enabled mobile devices that support text and images. Therefore, com-
ings, and proposals for future research. pared to in-store and e-commerce, m-commerce has the lowest media
richness.
2. Literature review and hypotheses It is important to note that the goal of this research is to examine
how channel characteristics defined by media richness impact
2.1. Media richness consumer decision making. The selection of the three channels (i.e.,
m-commerce, e-commerce and in-store) and their settings are
McGrath and Hollingshead [35] classify media along a continuum used to manipulate three levels of media richness (i.e. low, medium
of “increasing potential richness of information” ([50], p. 297). The and high). Different settings of these channels may yield different
four types of media that the researchers identify along this richness richness levels.
continuum are: text, audio, video and face-to-face communications.
MRT is usually used in the context of media choice. Face-to-face is
the richest medium as it allows mutual feedback and simultaneously 2.2. Task-media fit
conveys a variety of cues (e.g., tonal, facial, emotional). A text-based
interaction (e.g. texting through mobile devices or browsing infor- McGrath and Hollingshead [35] suggest task-media fit hypothe-
mation through text-only cell phone browsers) is less “rich” than ses as an extension to media richness theory, and present a matrix
audio, video or face-to-face interaction (e.g. in-store interactions or (Fig. 1a), which classifies patterns of differential fit along the two di-
communications using Apple iPhone's Facetime feature). Media mensions of communication media and task type (see [50], for expla-
that are highest and lowest in media richness anchor the two ends nation). Communication media is aligned along the “increasing
of the continuum. Researchers extensively use MRT in the context potential richness of information” continuum ([50], p. 297). Task
of dyadic intra-organizational communication situations in information type includes choice (e.g., choosing) and negotiating (e.g., generat-
systems literature [50]. ing ideas) tasks. The best fits for the choice tasks between media
Extending MRT to decision making in marketing channels, media and task type appear to be in media that offer medium richness.
richness is a characteristic that imparts to a marketing channel the Task-media fit hypothesizes that media at the two ends of the con-
ability to communicate information to consumers, and help them un- tinuum (i.e., media with the highest and lowest media richness)
dertake decision-making on that channel. The degree of media richness are ineffective for carrying out the communication tasks, as they
may not only vary across channels, but also within a specific channel. cause distraction (too rich) or are incapable of transmitting the nec-
For example, a mobile channel with audio/video capabilities is richer essary information (too lean) [50]. In our study, the choice task that
than a mobile channel with text-only capabilities. Also, an in-store set- consumers are required to undertake is similar to “choosing”. Our
ting suggests a face-to-face interaction with a sales associate, as well as study advances this literature and investigates task-media fit in the
the possibility of physical inspection of goods. Similarly, an e-commerce context of choice tasks.
36 M. Maity, M. Dass / Decision Support Systems 61 (2014) 34–46

Source: McGrath and Hollingshead 1994


Fig. 1. Media richness-task fit.

2.3. Channel choice decision-making task [51]. The cognitive cost model theorizes that con-
sumers display limited cognitive abilities and thus, their actions during
Gupta et al. point out that it is “critically important” ([26] pp. 31) for decision-making tasks depend on the associated cognitive costs [41].
both practitioners and academic researchers to understand the fac- Payne [41] proposes a cost-benefit framework of cognition, which is
tors that drive consumers to choose one channel over another. Previ- subsequently supported by other researchers [4]. As per this framework,
ous research shows that a multiple-channel retail strategy enhances consumers continue to search for additional information only until the
the performance of the portfolio of services offered to the customer, benefit from additional information is equal to or greater than the
thus leading to high customer satisfaction and ultimately customer cognitive cost of search.
loyalty [55]. Therefore, a greater understanding of consumer deci- Theories explaining consumer decision-making, such as behavioral
sion making and channel choice is necessary for making multichan- decision theory, cognitive cost model, and cognitive cost-benefit frame-
nel strategies more efficient [39]. Extant literature has investigated work suggest that consumers might not be equally at ease when under-
the effects of various drivers of channel choice and preferences, in- taking the same decision-making task across channels that incorporate
cluding consumer characteristics [52], consumer perception of different degrees of media richness. A decrease in media richness leads
price, quality, value and risk of channels [49], interactivity of the to an increase in cognitive costs [50]. For example, technological
channel [22], different purchase tasks [54], and customer satisfaction advances in e-commerce and m-commerce may reduce the physical
[37]. efforts of going to a store to make a choice, but these two channels
While these studies focus on how consumer and product character- can incorporate different capabilities that can result in different degrees
istics impact channel choice, limited research exists on how channel of media richness, and may impose different levels of cognitive cost
characteristics impact consumer decision-making and channel choice. on the consumer when undertaking decision-making. Hence,
A notable exception is Michaelidou, Arnott and Dibb [36] where the au- consumer decision-making is more cognitively “costly” in m-commerce
thors present a framework for examining the effects of channel involve- (e.g., wifi-enabled phones with browsing capabilities that support only
ment, channel perceived risk, channel loyalty, channel similarity and text and images) than in an in-store setting (e.g. store setting where
channel hedonism on channel choice, albeit without empirically testing the consumer can interact with a sales associate and physically inspect
it. Our study addresses this gap in the literature by examining the effect the product).
of media richness (a pertinent and important channel characteristic) on
channel choice and consumer decision-making. 2.5. Effect of media richness on information search

2.4. Behavioral decision theory and cognitive cost model The cognitive cost-benefit framework and the cognitive cost model
predict a positive linear relationship between media richness and infor-
Behavioral decision theory [51] and the cognitive cost model [44] mation search. The amount of information search that a consumer un-
play important roles in understanding consumer decision-making. The dertakes decreases as cognitive cost increases over benefit. In other
cognitive cost perspective in behavioral decision theory indicates that words, consumers search less as media richness decreases (from high
consumers undertake cost-benefit tradeoffs when going through a to medium to low) because cognitive cost increases concomitantly
M. Maity, M. Dass / Decision Support Systems 61 (2014) 34–46 37

(while associated benefits remain constant). Therefore, according to the dependent variables for the high and medium media richness condi-
cost-benefit framework, information search is the highest in the high tions as the competing theories contradict each other and there is no
media richness condition and the lowest in the low media richness reason to believe that one theory will overshadow the other. Therefore,
condition. the following hypothesis is presented:
On the other hand, task-media fit hypotheses predict that the
richness of the channel can adversely affect the outcome of a choice H2. Perceived media richness-task fit, satisfaction and channel choice
task, especially when media richness is too high or too low. The task- intention (for information search and future purchase) are different
fit hypothesis predicts that as media richness increases too much, it across the three levels of media richness conditions, where the fit, satis-
distracts the consumer and leads to ineffective task completion. As faction and channel choice intention are the lowest in the low media
media richness decreases too much, channels become less capable of richness condition.
transmitting sufficient information for effective completion of the task.
In other words, the amount of information search that a consumer un- 2.7. Moderators: product type, task complexity
dertakes increases as media richness decreases from high to medium,
but as media richness further decreases from medium to low, the Next, this study investigates whether effects of media richness on
amount of information search decreases as well. Therefore, according channel choice and consumer decision-making vary across product
to task-media fit, information search is the highest in the medium type and task complexity (i.e. moderating effect). Extant literature
media richness condition and low in the high media richness and low classifies products in diverse ways, including business vs. consumer
media richness conditions. products, tangible vs. intangible goods, credence vs. experience vs.
The cognitive cost-benefit framework (the cognitive cost model) search goods, high cost vs. low cost products, and convenience goods
and the task-media fit hypotheses make competing predictions about vs. shopping goods vs. specialty goods [28], among others. In particular,
the direction of information search for the high and medium media researchers frequently use search and experience to describe a product
richness conditions. Therefore, we remain agnostic in formulating our or service [30]. As per Euromonitor [20], the top product categories that
hypotheses about information search in the high and medium richness consumers buy on the Internet are books and airline tickets, both of
conditions. However, the two sets of theories predict that information which are search products. Food is an experience product [38].
search will be low in the low media richness condition. Therefore, we Most people are comfortable buying travel and food delivery via
hypothesize: e-commerce and m-commerce [19]. In this investigation, product
type is either search or experience (airline tickets or food menu from
H1. Amount of information search is different across the three levels of
restaurants), across the three levels of media richness, resulting in six
media richness conditions, where the amount of search is the lowest in
media richness-product type conditions. The distinction between
the low media richness condition.
search and experience products is whether consumers can assess the
quality of the product before or after experiencing it [38]. Since con-
2.6. Effect of media richness on perceived fit, post purchase evaluation, sumers assess an experience product by consuming the product, the
channel choice risk (i.e., the uncertainty) associated with choosing an experience prod-
uct is higher than that associated with choosing a search product. Ac-
This study next investigates whether consumers perceive a fit cording to risk theory [18], consumers are expected to undertake
between a decision-making task and specific levels of media richness. more information search in the context of increased perceived risk.
More specifically, this study examines two questions: a) do consumers We argue that consumers will prefer to undertake decision-making
perceive all levels of media richness as fit for undertaking all types of for experience products in a medium that offers high richness, as it al-
decision-making tasks? or, b) do consumers perceive only specific levels lows the consumer to collect different types of information (i.e., text-
of media richness as fit for certain types of decision-making tasks based as well as those made possible by the objective characteristics
(specifically, whether product type and task complexity act as of the media). Similarly, consumers are expected to prefer medium
moderators)? richness condition for undertaking a decision-making task for search
The concept of fit has been widely used in extant literature. Using products due to the lower associated risk with the decision. Based on
task-technology fit theory, Goodhue [24] advances the perspective of this literature and the ones discussed while presenting the previous hy-
fit as the interaction of technology, task, and individual characteristics. potheses, it is expected that for both product types consumers will indi-
This research defines media richness-task fit as the match that cate low fit, satisfaction and channel choice intention in the low media
consumers perceive between a type of decision-making task and the richness condition. Therefore:
level of media richness, and extends the concept of fit into the context
of channel choice and consumer decision-making in a multichannel H3. Product type moderates the effect of media richness on information
environment. Following Goodhue's [24] suggestion, this study employs search, media richness-task fit, satisfaction, and channel choice inten-
user evaluation to measure the construct of perceived fit. tion such that the effects are different across the six levels of media
Media richness theory postulates that rich media allow people to richness-product type conditions: a high media richness condition is
interpret and understand difficult and complex communication issues, preferred for undertaking a decision-making task on an experience
while less rich media enable the handling of simple communication product, a medium media richness condition is preferred for undertak-
situations [35]. Media richness theory predicts a positive relationship ing a decision-making task on a search product, a low media richness
between media richness and the complexity of the choice task undertak- condition is not preferred for undertaking a decision-making task on
en (originally, “message equivocality” communicated [16], pp. 359). either search or experience products.
However, task-media fit tempers this postulation (i.e., media that are
too high or too low in richness are judged as ineffective for carrying Similarly, extant literature often defines task [60] in terms of simple
out choice tasks), and predicts an inverted-U shaped curve. Therefore, vs. complex categorization. Simple tasks require processing fewer cues,
applying task-media fit hypotheses to consumer decision-making i.e., pieces of data, while complex tasks require processing of higher
tasks (in relation to the first question), media richness is expected to number of cues [41]. Information search literature also suggests that
affect perceived fit, post purchase experiences and channel choice in as task difficulty increases, consumers' cognitive effort increases [43],
the future such that consumers find specific levels of media richness and consumers search less for complex tasks and more for simple
more suitable for carrying out certain decision-making tasks. As in the tasks, indicating a possible moderating effect for task complexity.
case of H1, no predictions can be made about the direction of the Therefore, this study defines task complexity in terms of number of
38 M. Maity, M. Dass / Decision Support Systems 61 (2014) 34–46

alternatives/brands, and this factor three levels are: simple (few alterna- ticket task. Information was on round-trip flight options. The brand
tives), moderate (more than a few alternatives) and complex (many names of airlines were changed to names of airlines that operated
alternatives), thus generating nine media richness-task complexity mainly in parts of the world other than the United States. Similarly,
conditions. the output generated by the website www.delivery.com for specific
It is expected that consumers perceive a higher risk in completing a restaurants, provided the format for the stimuli for the food menu
complex task and will prefer the high media richness condition for carry- task. The names of the restaurants were changed for the final stimuli.
ing out this task. A possible reason is that a high media richness condition For the in-store environment, brochures presented each brand on a sep-
helps in collecting more information (as mentioned in the preceding sec- arate page. Separate screens presented information on separate brands
tion), and reduces risk in the choice process. Similarly, a simple task has a on websites for the e-commerce and m-commerce channels.
lower associated risk, and it is expected that consumers will prefer to use
a channel with medium media richness for completing a simple 3.1.3. Pretest
decision-making task. Therefore, we hypothesize that: Thirty-nine participants took part in a pretest that tested for per-
ceived media richness, product type and task complexity. Each par-
H4. Task complexity moderates the effect of media richness on infor- ticipant undertook a decision-making task across one of the three
mation search, media richness-task fit, satisfaction and channel choice levels of media richness (high, medium, low), one of the two levels
intention, such that the effects are different across the nine levels of of product type (search, experience), and one of the three levels of
media richness-task complexity conditions: a high media richness con- task complexity (high, moderate, low). Upon completion of the
dition is preferred for undertaking a complex decision-making task, a task, participants answered questions on perceived media richness,
medium media richness condition is preferred for undertaking a moder- perceived product type, perceived task complexity, and additional
ate decision-making task, a low media richness condition is preferred questions on perceived cost. A three-way ANOVA of media richness,
for undertaking a simple decision-making task. product type and task complexity on perceived media richness
revealed that participants rated richness across the three media as
Three experiments test the hypotheses presented in this sufficiently different (MHigh = 5.4; MMedium = 4.7; MLow = 3.1;
section. Study 1 is a between subjects experiment. Studies 2 and 3 F(2,37) = 14.1, p b 0.05; where, 1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly
(mixed design experiments) are designed to provide more convincing agree). A separate three-way ANOVA on the amount of information
evidence for decision-making at individual levels, and also repli- search showed a significant effect of all three factors (Media richness:
cate the findings of Study 1. In particular, Study 2 revisits the com- MHigh = 3.7; MMedium = 6.6; MLow = 4.4; F(2,37) = 5.9, p b 0.05)
parison between channels that incorporate medium and low (product type: M Airline Ticket = 5.7; M Food = 4.0; F(1,38) = 5.5,
richness (e-commerce and m-commerce) and, Study 3 compares p b 0.05) (task complexity: MFew = 2.4; MModerate = 5.8; MMany =
medium and high richness (e-commerce and in-store) channels. 6.3; F(2,37) = 19.7, p b 0.05), demonstrating the successful manipula-
tion of all the factors.
3. Study 1
3.1.4. Independent variables
Study 1 manipulates media richness (high, medium, low) Each participant began the assigned task either by meeting a re-
through three channels: in-store, e-commerce and m-commerce. search associate, or by logging onto the Internet (through an internet-
Product type is operationalized by search and experience products. enabled laptop computer or through a web-enabled mobile device).
Task complexity (few, moderate, many) is operationalized by the All subjects were asked to imagine themselves in a situation where
number of brands that consumers can access for making a choice. they had to choose either an airline ticket or a food menu for a friend.
Participants with prior experience of using hand-held mobile Existing validated scales measured the independent variables (see
devices and familiar with e-commerce for at least two years partici- Table 1): perceived media richness [50] and perceived task complexity
pate in the experiment. Stimuli, including information presentation [25]. A two-item measure asking whether the participants can assess
format, layout, fonts, colors and images, are the same for all tasks the product before using it measures perceived product type. The two
across all levels of media richness. statements were: “I can collect information about the product before
purchasing it” and “I can determine if the product is ‘good’ or ‘bad’ im-
3.1. Study 1: Method mediately before using the product” (as per the definition of search and
experience attributes [23]). An averaged index was formed for each of
3.1.1. Design and participants the constructs of perceived media richness (Cronbach's α = 0.85), per-
Study 1 was a 3 (media richness: high, medium, low) × 2 (product ceived product type (Cronbach's α = 0.88) and task complexity
type: search, experience) × 3 (task complexity: few, moderate, many) (Cronbach's α = 0.89). All statements were measured on a seven-
between subjects design. To test the hypotheses, participants were point scale (1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree).
randomly assigned to one of the eighteen cells where each participant
carried out a decision-making task. Responses were solicited for volun- 3.1.5. Manipulation checks
tary participation from undergraduate students registered as business As intended, a 3 × 2 × 3 ANOVA of media richness × product type ×
majors at a major south-eastern university in the United States. Partici- task complexity on perceived media richness showed a main effect of
pants earned extra credit for taking part in the study. Further, partici- media richness: participants considered in-store (i.e., high media rich-
pants were entered in five raffle drawings of US$ 30.00 each. In all, ness condition) the most rich, and m-commerce (i.e., low media rich-
162 participants completed the study. There were 54 participants in ness condition) the least rich (MHigh = 5.1; MMedium = 4.8; MLow =
each channel. All participants were under 30 years of age (18–27). 3.5; F(2,144) = 28.9, p b 0.05; where, 1 = strongly disagree; 7 =
The mean age was 20.7 years. Females constituted 53.1% of the sample, strongly agree). A post-hoc Tukey's HSD test reveals that participants
35.2% had a family income of over a hundred thousand US$, and 85.2% do not perceive significant difference between high and medium levels
were Caucasian Americans. of media richness. A reason for this finding may be that participants
assigned to each channel responded to the scale items (see Table 1)
3.1.2. Stimuli with particular reference to that channel only. We also measure partic-
Participants undertook decision-making tasks specifically designed ipants' responses of their perception of the comparative differences in
for this study. The output generated by the website www.orbitz.com decision-making on the three channels, separately. This measure
(for specific dates) provided the format for the stimuli for the airline provides an additional method for measuring perceived (relative)
M. Maity, M. Dass / Decision Support Systems 61 (2014) 34–46 39

Table 1
Scales used for the independent variables, dependent variables and covariate (studies 1, 2, 3).b

Construct Measure Item Reliability (Cronbach's α) studies 1, 2, 3 Scales based


on/adapted
S1 S2 S2 S3 S3
E-com M-com E-com In-store

Perceived media MR1 While shopping at online stores, the e-commerce environment helps 0.85 0.86 0.84 0.85 0.89 [50]
richnessa me in making a good decision.
MR2 While shopping at online stores, when I do not understand a
piece of information, the e-commerce environment makes it more
difficult for me to come to a decision. (RC)
MR3 While shopping at online stores, the conditions provided by an
e-commerce environment slow down the decision-making process. (RC)
MR4 While shopping at online stores, the e-commerce environment makes
it easy for me to come to a decision.
MR5 While shopping in an e-commerce environment, I can easily
understand things.
MR6 An e-commerce environment helps me reach a decision quickly.
Pcvd diff across m-com PD1 “The decision-making experience on a mobile device is very similar to N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A
and e-com the experience in the e-commerce environment.”
Pcvd diff across m-com PD2 “The decision-making experience on a mobile device is very similar to N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A
and in-store the experience in the in-store environment.”
Pcvd diff across in-store PD3 “The decision-making experience in the in-store setting is very similar N/A N/A N/A N/A N/A
and e-com to the experience in the e-commerce environment.”
Perceived product PT1 “I can collect information about the product before purchasing it.” 0.88 0.91 0.87 0.88 0.86
Type PT2 “I can determine if the product is ‘good’ or ‘bad’ immediately before using
the product.”
Perceived media PF1 “This task is best carried out in an e-commerce environment.” 0.97 0.97 0.98 0.94 0.96 [25]
Richness-task fita PF2 “An e-commerce environment is the most conducive for carrying out this task.”
Satisfactiona S1 “If I could do it over again, I would not have made the decision using an 0.8 0.82 0.8 0.82 0.84 [57]
e-commerce environment.”
S2 “I feel bad about using an e-commerce environment.”
S3 “I'm sure it was the right thing for me to use an e-commerce environment.”
Channel choicea C1 “I intend searching for information and making a purchase decision in an 0.94 0.94 0.95 0.84 0.92 [42]
e-commerce environment.”
C2 “I shall recommend others to search for information and make a
purchase decision in an e-commerce environment.”
Perceived cost PC1 “I felt it took a lot of effort to search.” 0.91 0.92 0.84 0.93 0.84 [47]
PC2 “I felt it took a lot of time to search.”
a
In the items included in the questionnaires, e-commerce appears in questionnaires administered to participants assigned to the e-commerce channel, while m-commerce and in-store
appear in the questionnaires administered to participants in the m-commerce and in-store channels respectively.
b
All scales are measured on a 7-point Likert scale, where 1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree.

media richness. The participant responses (MM–E = 4.1; MM–I = 2.9; tasks, all participants completed questionnaires that were administered
MI–E = 3.0; where, 1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree) reveal in the paper-and-pen format. In the end, all participants responded to
that participants perceive decision-making across in-store and the following question: “Now tell your friend about you decision-
e-commerce as different (also see Studies 2 and 3). A separate making experiences. What will you say?”.
three-way ANOVA on perceived product type was found to have a
main effect of product type (MAirlineTickets = 6.3; MFood = 5.7; 3.1.7. Dependent Variables
F(1,144) = 3.9, p b 0.05). Task complexity (i.e., perceived complexity) The dependent variable, i.e. amount of information searched, was
was not significant (MFew = 2.9; MModerate = 4.7; MMany = 4.7; operationalized by counting the number of brands examined by the
F(2,144) = 1.2, n.s.). participants for each task [5]. Perceived fit [25], satisfaction [57],
channel choice intention [42] and the covariate perceived cost [47]
3.1.6. Procedure were measured with items adapted from validated scales. The
The research associate posed as a travel agent or a concierge for the Cronbach's alpha for each of the scales were (Table 1): 0.97, 0.80,
high media richness condition. Participants walked into the office and 0.94 and 0.91.
interacted with the live representative in order to complete a task. Par-
ticipants, in this condition, were free to examine a brochure (which was 3.2. Study 1: qualitative results
identical in terms of the content that participants in the e-commerce
and m-commerce environments examined) and interact with the asso- Critical Incident Technique (CIT) is selected for identifying underly-
ciate who posed as the travel agent. In the medium media richness con- ing critical factors that affect consumer decision-making across the
dition, participants logged on to specific websites via a wifi-enabled three channels (by analyzing the qualitative responses from the partic-
laptop computer, while participants logged on to the same websites ipants). CIT has been used in several studies, and is effective when the
through wifi-enabled mobile devices for the low media richness condi- incidents reported are fairly recent and the observers are motivated to
tion. Participants accessed the same wireless network for undertaking make detailed observations and evaluations at the time of the incident
the decision-making tasks on e-commerce and m-commerce. In an ex- [10]. Data analysis using CIT has been executed in two different ways
perimental set-up, it was ensured that all participants had to reach the in extant literature: a) to identify distinct ‘critical incidents,’ where
same venue for the experiment. For each of the tasks, the price informa- each respondent's experience is treated as a critical incident [21]; and
tion was included. For the airlines task, participants chose an airline b) to identify distinct ‘critical factors,’ where each respondent's experi-
ticket for a friend. For the restaurant task, participants chose the food ence is considered as an accumulation of a number of critical factors
that they would order for a group of friends. After completing the [2005]. This study adopts the latter approach and identifies the critical
40 M. Maity, M. Dass / Decision Support Systems 61 (2014) 34–46

factors that shape positive and negative consumer experiences in each F(2, 143) = 16.3, p b 0.05), and channel choice (M High = 4.9;
of the three channels. MMedium = 5.3; MLow = 2.4; F(2, 143) = 58.9, p b 0.05) (Table 3a).
The 162 responses written by the participations after completing the These findings reveal that perceived fit, satisfaction, and channel
tasks were content analyzed, as CIT proposes [6], with the purpose of choice are the highest in the medium richness condition. All the indi-
identifying categories of critical factors that lead to outcomes of interest. cators are the lowest in the low richness condition. These findings
For the purposes of this study, critical factors were defined as those fac- provide support for H2, and demonstrate that consumer behavior
tors that contribute toward consumer experiences after some decision- follows the predictions made by task-media fit hypotheses.
making tasks (information search and choice) were undertaken in each
of the three channels (in-store, e-commerce, and m-commerce). An
3.3.3. Moderating effect of product type and task complexity
analysis of the responses yields critical factors. These critical factors
The interaction effect between media richness and product type is sig-
were either positive or negative in nature.
nificant for information search in the choice task (MHigh*AirlineTickets = 5.2;
The first phase of analysis of the responses required the inductive
MHigh*FoodMenu = 4.8; MMedium*AirlineTickets = 6.4; MMedium*FoodMenu = 5.5;
identification of major categories of critical factors that lead to positive
MLow*AirlineTickets = 4.8; MLow*FoodMenu = 2.3; F(2, 143) = 3.6, p b 0.05).
and negative consumer experiences. Two judges with considerable ex-
The interaction effect between media richness and product type is also
pertise in consumer behavior grouped the responses into separate cate-
significant for perceived media richness-task fit (MHigh*AirlineTickets = 3.4;
gories, identifying factors that led to positive and negative experience in
MHigh*FoodMenu = 4.7; MMedium*AirlineTickets = 5.7; MMedium*FoodMenu =
each of the three channels. None of the judges had any knowledge of the
4.2; MLow*AirlineTickets = 2.4; MLow*FoodMenu = 2.2; F(2, 143) = 15.2,
objectives of the study. Three categories of factors emerged: channel-
p b 0.05), satisfaction (MHigh*AirlineTickets = 4.4; MHigh*FoodMenu = 5.0;
related (convenience, ease), user-related and product-related (along
MMedium*AirlineTickets = 5.8; MMedium*FoodMenu = 4.9; MMedium*AirlineTickets =
with various sub-categories). Inter-rater agreement was 82.7%. In the
3.8; MMedium*FoodMenu = 3.6; F(2, 143) = 3.3, p b 0.05), and channel choice
next phase, the judges met to compare the classifications, and the dis-
(MHigh*AirlineTickets = 4.7; MHigh*FoodMenu = 5.1; MMedium*AirlineTickets = 5.5;
agreements were resolved (see Table 2a).
MMedium*FoodMenu = 5.0; MLow*AirlineTickets = 2.8; MLow*FoodMenu = 2.0;
As per the classification provided by the independent judges, analy-
F(2, 143) = 3.2, p b 0.05) (Table 3b, Figs. 2a and b).
sis is undertaken in order to identify the level of “importance” that par-
Though Information search for both product types is high in the me-
ticipants associate with the identified critical factors. To that end, the
dium richness condition, and medium in high richness condition, in the
number of times a critical factor is mentioned by a participant is tabulat-
low richness condition, consumers display a sharp decrease in informa-
ed and then converted to an associated percentage (see Table 2a). These
tion search for the experience product compared to the search product.
percentages are then taken as the “weights” that consumers attach to
A possible reason for this finding is that consumers may prefer under-
their experiences (i.e., positive or negative) to each of the sub-
taking information search in the medium richness condition. Therefore,
categories that are identified. Those factors that are “common” across
irrespective of what the product type is, consumers prefer searching for
the three channels and those that are “unique” to a specific channel
information in this level of media richness.
are also identified (Table 2a). The associated weights for the “common”
However, perceived media richness-task fit, satisfaction, and chan-
and “unique” channel factors are calculated (as reported in Table 2b),
nel choice intention for experience products, are high in high richness
and then the cumulative weights are assigned to each channel. These
condition and medium in medium richness condition, while for search
weights are used in a mediation analysis that is reported in the next
products the outcomes are high in the medium richness condition and
section.
medium in the high richness condition. These results reveal a moderat-
ing effect of product type and sharpen the findings presented in
3.3. Study 1: quantitative results
Table 3a.
Findings demonstrate that participants prefer undertaking tasks
3.3.1. Effect of media richness on information search
on search products in the medium richness condition. For tasks on
Three-way ANCOVAs with cost of search as covariate and the three
experience products, participants indicate a higher fit, satisfaction,
independent factors, on the amount of information search as dependent
and channel choice intention in the high media richness condition,
variable, suggest that cost of search is significant (F(1,143) = 15.0,
thus demonstrating their preference for undertaking this type of
p b 0.05). Information search across the levels of media richness is
task in the high richness condition. As per our predictions, participants
such that it is the highest for the medium richness condition
in the low media richness condition do not perceive media richness-
(MHigh = 4.9; MMedium = 5.9; MLow = 3.6; F(2, 143) = 13.8, p b 0.05)
task fit for either task (Table 3b). These findings support the task-
(Table 3a), which demonstrates the main effect of media richness,
media fit hypotheses, which predict that certain levels of media rich-
providing support for H1, and for task-media fit hypothesis. Hence,
ness are fit for undertaking certain types of tasks. These findings provide
participants in the high (i.e., in-store) and low (i.e., m-commerce)
support for H3 for perceived fit, post purchase evaluation, and channel
media richness conditions consider less number of brands than par-
choice. Interaction effect between media richness and task complexity
ticipants in the medium (i.e., e-commerce) media richness condi-
is not significant for any of the variables. Hence, no support is obtained
tion. Participants consider more number of brands in the high
for H4 (see Table 3c). A possible reason for the non-significant finding
media richness condition compared to that in the low media richness
for task complexity is that perceived cost plays a reduced role in infor-
condition. (Post-hoc tests (Bonferroni) confirm the differences
mation search in the online environment [45].
across channels. Results can be obtained from the authors upon
request.)
3.3.4. Mediation analysis
3.3.2. Effect of media richness on perceived fit, satisfaction, channel choice This study does not explicitly manipulate media richness, but uses
Separate three-way ANCOVAs with cost of search as covariate and three channels that differ, a priori, in terms of media richness, among
perceived media richness-task fit, and satisfaction as dependent vari- other things. This observation raises the issue of whether some aspect
ables, suggest that cost of search is significant for media richness-task of channel type, other than media richness, might be responsible for
fit (F(1,143) = 30.3, p b 0.05), and satisfaction (F(1,143) = 16.9, the estimated differences in the dependent variables across treatments.
p b 0.05). Note, as already mentioned, Table 2a identifies all the characteristics
A main effect of media richness is noted on perceived media richness- that participants associate with each of the three channels, along with
task fit (MHigh = 4.0; MMedium = 4.9; MLow = 2.3; F(2, 143) = 51.9, the “weights” associated with the “unique” and “common” channel
p b 0.05), satisfaction (MHigh = 4.7; MMedium = 5.4; M Low = 3.7; characteristics (Table 2b).
M. Maity, M. Dass / Decision Support Systems 61 (2014) 34–46 41

Table 2a
Critical incident analysis.

Critical factors Low richness Low richness Medium richness Medium richness High richness High richness Common/unique
positive negative positive negative positive negative factors

Channel-related
Layout 1.21 0.80 6.35 1.59 α
Navigability 3.22 5.55 α
Convenience 5.62 3.97 α
Save time 0.40 1.61 3.17 1.31 5.88 γ
Usability/device features 2.41 12.05 Unique (m-com)
Mobility 2.41 0.40 Unique (m-com)
Atmosphere 1.95 0.65 Unique (in-store)

User-related
Decision making 10.04 24.10 25.39 7.93 20.27 15.69 γ
Amount of information 12.85 5.56 3.16 1.31 9.15 γ
(overwhelming)
Physical comfort (exhausting) 3.61 0.66 β
Emotional comfort (irritated) 3.21 Unique (m-com)
Compare with e-commerce 10.04 6.54 β
Privacy 0.79 Unique (e-com)

Product-related
Price 3.61 13.5 11.76 γ
Other product attributes 1.61 15.1 15.03 γ
Product selection 0.80 7.94 9.80 γ
Total 31.33 68.67 87.32 12.68 61.43 38.57

The figures in the table are the percentages (of each critical factor) for that channel. Therefore, the percentage of positive and negative critical factors for each channel adds up to a 100
(87.32 + 12.68; 31.33 + 68.67; 61.43 + 38.57). The blank cells indicate no responses were recorded for these cells. Interesting results are in bold.
E-commerce participants: (54 respondents: 110 positive critical factors; 16 negative critical factors).
M-commerce participants: (54 respondents: 78 positive critical factors; 171 negative critical factors).
In-store participants: (54 respondents: 104 positive critical factors; 49 negative critical factors).
α = Factors common to m-commerce and e-commerce.
β = Factors common to m-commerce and in-store.
γ = Factors common across all three channels.

In order to establish that media richness is an essential characteristic perceived task-media fit, satisfaction, and channel choice. Consumers
that contributes to the estimated differences between channel types, indicate that they will choose the channel with medium richness for un-
two different mediation analyses [2] are undertaken: a) mediation anal- dertaking decision-making tasks on search products, and the channel
ysis without controlling for any other channel characteristic, and b) me- with high richness for undertaking decision-making tasks on experi-
diation analysis by controlling for the “unique” channel characteristics. ence products. Overall, we find support for task-media fit hypotheses.
In the first analysis, the weights are not considered (since the mediation Finally, a mediation analysis confirmed that the results are not con-
analysis does not control for any factors). In the second analysis, the founded by other channel characteristics, thus enhancing the reliability
cumulative weights associated with the “unique” channel characteristics of our findings.
are assigned to each of the corresponding level of media richness, and
this variable is included in each of the three regression equations that 4. Studies 2 and 3
test mediation analysis. The results of the two analyses demonstrate
that media richness mediate the influence of channel type on all depen- Studies 2 and 3 examine whether the findings of the hypotheses
dent variables, except information search (Figs. 3a and b). Specifically, it hold at an individual level (i.e., e-commerce and m-commerce; e-
should be noted that even after controlling for the unique characteristics commerce and in-store). Stimuli, procedures, participation and reward
for each channel, media richness mediates the relationship between the norms for these two studies are the same as in Study 1. These two stud-
channel type and media richness-task fit, satisfaction and channel choice. ies do not test H3 and H4, since product type is a nested factor, and sam-
ple size is low for testing these hypotheses.
3.4. Study 1: discussion
4.1. Studies 2 and 3: method
Study 1 reveals that media richness does impact consumer decision-
making and channel choice, where consumers at different levels of 4.1.1. Design and participants
media richness respond differently to the same tasks. Information Study 2 was a 2 (media richness: medium, low) × 3 (task complexity:
search is medium in high media richness condition, high in medium few, moderate, many) mixed design experiment, where media richness
media richness condition, and low in low media richness condition. was the within subjects factor, and task complexity was the between
This study also demonstrates the moderating effect of product type on groups factor. In Study 3 media richness was medium and high, while

Table 2b
Weights used in mediation analysis.

Critical factors Low rich Low rich Medium rich Medium rich High rich High rich Low rich Medium rich High rich
positive negative positive negative positive negative cumula cumula cumula

Common features 16.46 38.56 70.66 11.09 59.48 30.72 −22.1 59.57 28.76
Unique features 14.87 30.11 16.66 1.59 1.95 7.85 −15.24 15.07 −5.9
Total 31.33 68.67 87.32 12.68 61.43 38.57 −37.34 74.64 22.86
a
Cumulative weights used in the mediation analysis for each of the three media richness conditions
42 M. Maity, M. Dass / Decision Support Systems 61 (2014) 34–46

the other factor and the study design were similar to Study 2. To test the media richness condition. Satisfaction (MMedium = 5.5; MHigh =
hypotheses in the two studies, participants were randomly assigned to 4.3; F(1,28) = 28.6, p b 0.05) concurs with the findings of media
one of the three cells of the between groups factor. Participants under- richness-task fit. The direct effect of media richness on channel
took a task in the medium media richness condition (either search or choice is not significant. Study 3 provides partial support for H2,
experience product). They returned after a month, and upon returning and to task-media fit hypotheses.
undertook a task in the low/high media richness condition (Study 2 or
Study 3) on the other product type. Thirty-one participants completed 4.3. Studies 2 and 3: discussion
Study 2 (few = 9; moderate = 10; many = 12). Thirty-one participants
completed Study 3 (few = 11; moderate = 12; many = 8). Participant Studies 2 and 3 broadly agree with the findings of Study 1. Con-
demographics in these two studies were similar to that of participants sumers undertaking the same decision-making task at medium and
in Study 1. low levels of media richness search for a greater amount of information
in the medium richness condition; those in the medium and high rich-
4.1.2. Manipulation checks ness conditions also undertake the greatest amount of search at the me-
In Study 2, a 2 × 3 repeated measures ANOVA of media richness × task dium level of richness. Perceived media richness-task fit, satisfaction,
complexity on perceived media richness (αMedium =0.86; αLow = 0.84) and channel choice are the lowest in the low media richness condition,
showed a direct effect of media richness: participants perceived the me- and highest in the medium richness condition.
dium media richness condition as more rich than the low media richness
condition (MMedium = 5.3; MLow = 3.8; F(1,28) = 45.5, p b 0.05) (1 = 5. General discussion
strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree). The repeated measures ANOVA
on task complexity (αMedium = 0.86; αLow = 0.94) was not significant. A retailer's channel mix often includes traditional (e.g., the in-store
In Study 3, a 2 × 3 repeated measures ANOVA of media richness x task environment) and new and evolving channels. Prior research on retail
complexity on perceived media richness (αMedium = 0.85; αHigh = channels does little to address the impact of channel characteristics on
0.89) indicated a direct effect of media richness: participants considered channel choice and decision making. The media richness construct
the medium richness condition less rich than the high richness condition (a pertinent channel characteristic), lends itself particularly well to
(MMedium = 4.5; MHigh = 4.9; F(1,28) = 8.3, p b 0.05). The repeated situations where users interact with retail channels that include tradi-
measures ANOVA on task complexity (αMedium = 0.91; αHigh = 0.82) tional and new channels. This study focuses on three retail channels
was not significant. with three levels of media richness (low, medium and high) and
shows that media richness is a key channel characteristic that affects
4.1.3. Dependent variables consumer behavior. In particular, H1, and H2 find support across all
The two studies measured perceived fit, satisfaction, channel choice three studies. In Study 1, product type moderates the effect of media
intention and perceived cost with the same scales as in Study 1. richness on perceived fit, satisfaction, and channel choice, which
Cronbach's alpha for each of the scales in the two studies are above provide broad support for H3. These findings resonate with the findings
0.8, and are reported in Table 1. obtained by Huang et al. [30], where the authors find that there are
important differences in the browsing and purchase behavior of con-
4.2. Studies 2 and 3: results sumers between search and experience goods. However, the study
does not find support for the moderating effect of product type on infor-
4.2.1. Effect of media richness on information search mation search, indicating that consumers prefer to undertake informa-
In Study 2, separate 2 × 3 repeated measures ANOVAs of media rich- tion search in the medium richness condition irrespective of the
ness × task complexity on the amount of information search reveals a product type. No support is obtained for the moderating effect of task
main effect of media richness (MMedium = 6.5; MLow = 4.5; F(1,28) complexity (H4). This research tests competing hypotheses made by
= 18.8, p b 0.05). Participants in the medium richness condition con- task-media fit hypotheses and cognitive cost theory. The results of the
sider more number of brands than those in the low richness condition. research find support for task-media fit hypotheses, which explain the
In Study 3, separate 2 × 3 repeated measures ANOVAs of media rich- differences in consumer decision-making across the three levels of
ness × task complexity on the amount of information search, sug- media richness.
gests a marginally significant main effect of media richness The results of the research bear out our suppositions that cognitive
(MMedium = 6.7; MHigh = 5.4; F(1,28) = 3.3, p b 0.10). Participants in cost contributes to the differences in consumer decision-making across
the medium richness condition search for a greater number of brands the three levels of media richness. It explains why results from Study 1
than those in the high richness condition. Results from Study 2 agree show that in the context of channel users' decision-making and channel
with that of Study 1, while the findings from Study 3 partially agree choice, channels with high media richness (e.g., in-store) are preferred
with the findings of Study 1, providing partial support for H1, and to to channels with low media richness (e.g., m-commerce), and that users
task-media fit hypotheses. prefer channels with medium richness (e-commerce) over channels
that have higher/lower media richness. Study 2 provides validation
4.2.2. Effect of media richness on perceived fit, satisfaction, and channel to Study 1 and shows that e-commerce dominates m-commerce, as
choice expected. Study 3 replicates Study 1 and finds similar support, i.e.
In Study 2, separate 2 × 3 repeated measures ANOVAs of media e-commerce dominates in-store.
richness × task complexity on perceived media richness-task fit, sat- A few points of concern, often raised in the context of research that
isfaction, and channel choice intention reveal significant effect of requires consumer interaction with channels through devices, are ad-
media richness. Media richness-task fit (MMedium = 5.5; MLow = 2.3; dressed. First, it should be noted that, specifically for m-commerce
F(1,28) = 43.1, p b 0.05), satisfaction (MMedium = 5.9; MLow = 3.6; and e-commerce, the devices are integral to the channels as per the
F(1,28) = 51.2, p b 0.05), and channel choice intention (MMedium = definitions adopted in this study [13]. Hence, the devices through
5.4; MLow = 2.6; F(1,28) = 101.5, p b 0.05) demonstrate that partici- which consumers undertake decision-making, contribute to user expe-
pants indicate a higher score in the medium richness condition rience at that particular level of media richness. Consumer attitudes are
than in the low richness condition. This study provides support for uncovered through their engagement with these levels of media
H2. In Study 3, media richness-task fit (MMedium = 5.2; MHigh = richness. Second, there is little possibility of “novelty” of the channels
3.2; F(1,28) = 66.2, p b 0.05) indicates that participants perceive a impacting the study outcomes. Only participants who have at least
higher fit in the medium media richness condition than in the high two years of prior experience with mobile devices are included in the
M. Maity, M. Dass / Decision Support Systems 61 (2014) 34–46 43

Table 3a
Effect of media richness on information search, perceived media richness-task fit, post-purchase evaluation and channel choice.

Decision-making task Amount of information searche (brands) Perceived media richness-task fitf (fit) Post-purchase evaluationf (satisfaction) Channel choice intentionf

Media richness
High (in-store) Medium 4.9a Medium 4.0b Medium 4.7c Medium 4.9d
Medium (e-commerce) High 5.9 High 4.9 High 5.4 High 5.3
Low (m-commerce) Low 3.6 Low 2.3 Low 3.7 Low 2.4
a, b, c, d
Results in the table are from Study 1. Results from Studies 2 and 3 agree with the findings of Study 1.
a
Amount of information search: F(2, 143) = 13.8, p b 0.05;
b
Perceived media richness-task fit: F(2, 143) = 51.9, p b 0.05;
c
Satisfaction: F(2, 143) = 16.3, p b 0.05;
d
Channel choice: F(2, 143) = 58.9, p b 0.05.
e
Actual number of brands is measured in the choice task.
f
Fit, Satisfaction and channel choice: scales are measured on a 7-point Likert scale, where 1 = strongly disagree; 7 = strongly agree.

study. Hence, consumer responses reflect their attitude toward the level highlights that consumers prefer undertaking simple tasks on m-
of media richness rather than toward the “novelty” of any device. Third, commerce. Fourth, product type moderates the effect of media richness
as already mentioned, this research finds support for task-media fit hy- on fit, satisfaction, and channel choice. Fifth, this study also establishes
pothesis, which is well-established in information systems literature. that retail channels can be arrayed along a media richness continuum.
Fourth, our research seems to bear out what is observed in actual con-
sumer behavior. We find that consumers prefer to undertake information 5.2. Managerial contributions
search on medium richness conditions, as is commonly observed — most
consumers prefer undertaking product information search online, even if This study offers several implications for managers. First, from a
the purchase is through another channel. managerial perspective, support for the hypotheses suggests that
media richness is a fundamental channel characteristic and should be
considered while planning multi-channel strategies. A manager needs
5.1. Theoretical contributions to decide on how to manage channels that offer different levels of
media richness so as to provide the best possible offering to the cus-
Overall, this research contributes to our understanding of user- tomers for particular decision-making tasks. For example, if a manager
channel characteristic interaction across retail channels in the following wants a consumer to engage with a channel for more information, the
ways. From a theoretical perspective, first, building on media-richness best channel would most likely be a channel that incorporates medium
theory, this research contributes to the decision support systems litera- level of richness (e.g. e-commerce), and not a channel that is low in rich-
ture on channels and consumer behavior and examines how media ness (e.g. text and image based m-commerce). Likewise, for experience
richness of retail channels may influence channel users' decision- products, managers may want to engage with consumers through a
making, as well as channel choice. Second, we conduct our study in a channel that is high in richness (e.g., face-to-face). Similarly, managers
multichannel environment where we compare decision making in retail will want consumers to undertake simple tasks on mobile devices,
channels that incorporate advanced technology as well as those that do which echo Chiu et al. [11], who find that content providers should pro-
not incorporate such technologies, thus extending the multi-channel lit- vide targeted content to users on their mobile devices.
erature in decision support systems [7]. Third, evidence for the media Second, the findings demonstrate that managers need to note that
richness-task fit hypotheses implies that specific decision-making m-commerce is not the same as e-commerce. In the participants' final
tasks are suited to specific levels of media richness. In particular, it evaluation, m-commerce is unable to help consumers achieve the
implies that consumers will choose to undertake complex decision- same things as e-commerce does, with the same ease. Managers need
making tasks in channels that have medium or high levels of media rich- to keep this finding in perspective, especially as mobile phone compa-
ness, and not on channels that offer low media richness. This finding nies and service providers strive to make more features available on
their phones that compare with the features and capabilities available
on laptops and personal computers. Managers need to evaluate the fol-
Table 3b
lowing questions: How comfortable are consumers carrying out the
Moderating effect of product type on information search, perceived media richness-task
fit, post-purchase evaluation and channel choice. same tasks on their personal computers and mobile phones? How
often do consumers use the personal computer and the mobile phone
Product type media richness Search product Experience
for carrying out similar tasks?
(airline ticket) product (food menu)
Third, managers need to “manage” the contents of services made
High (in-store) 5.2a, e 4.8
available to consumers on channels that are low in media richness.
3.4b, f 4.7
4.4c, f 5.0
This research suggests that it is desirable to limit the amount of informa-
4.7d, f 5.1 tion made available to consumers on such channels. Findings suggest
Medium (e-commerce) 6.4 5.5 that consumers may undergo negative experiences when there is an in-
5.7 4.2 crease in cognitive cost in low media richness environments. For exam-
5.8 4.9
ple, advertisers and content providers need to tailor not only the
5.5 5.0
Low (m-commerce) 4.8 2.3 content but also the task that they expect consumers to complete in
2.4 2.2 channels that have low levels of media richness, thus providing custom-
3.8 3.6 er satisfaction with the channel. Managers, therefore, need to have an
2.8 2.0 understanding of the level of media richness that is offered by each
a
Amount of information search: F(2, 143) = 3.6, p b 0.05. channel, and match the channel's capabilities with what they expect
b
Perceived media richness-task fit: F(2, 143) = 15.2, p b 0.05. consumers to achieve at that level of media richness. This exercise will
c
Satisfaction: F(2, 143) = 3.3, p b 0.05.
d result in high perceived media richness-task fit and post purchase
Channel Choice: F(2, 143) = 3.2, p b 0.05.
e
Actual number of brands is measured in the choice task. evaluations.
f
All scales are measured on a 7-point Likert scale, where 1 = strongly disagree; Fourth, firms interested in sending advertising materials to mobile
7 = strongly agree. devices need to limit the amount of information transmitted, and
44 M. Maity, M. Dass / Decision Support Systems 61 (2014) 34–46

a b

Search Product Experience Product

Figs. 2. a and b: Interaction effect.

make the information relevant [58]. Information disseminated via mo- which might not be generalizable to other product categories. Fourth,
bile devices needs to be relatively simple as consumers find scrolling this research measures media richness with items that gauge the
(on m-commerce) extremely “stressful,” as one participant put it. Not consumer's ability to undertake decision-making on a particular medi-
only the layout of the advertising material, but also the content needs um, and not with items that measure the objective characteristics of
to be simple so as to keep cognitive load low. Finally, managers need the medium (e.g., cue multiplicity, and feedback [56]). Future research
to ensure that consumers do not experience negative experiences due may use the latter group of items to measure media richness.
to the unique factors of the m-commerce channel (usability, mobility, Finally, this research opens up avenues for studies that delve into
emotional comfort). user-channel characteristic interactions in the context of retail channel
choice. Future research can explicitly manipulate media richness within
5.3. Limitations, suggestions for future research and conclusion a channel type to study the effects of media richness on different aspects
of consumer behavior. Further research needs to investigate audio,
There are several limitations in the study that need to be acknowl- video and face-to-face elements that can be incorporated to define spe-
edged. First, the study participants are guided to visit sites and make de- cific levels of media richness that might affect channel choice. In addi-
cisions. Although efforts are made to replicate actual shopping tion, user interactions with other channels need to be examined in
environments, the findings might be different when consumers shop order to find their position along the media richness continuum for re-
in more natural settings. For example, since this study consists of a se- tail channels. Researchers might also be interested in testing other types
ries of experiments, it keeps the information made available to partici- of choice tasks that vary the amount of information across the different
pants the same, across all channels for specific levels of task channels, as well as measure the choice accuracy of tasks. Research also
complexity. In the future, it is worthwhile to examine actual user behav- needs to study the moderating effect of other types of products (includ-
ior through a field experiment setting to validate our findings. Second, ing credence, durable and frequently purchased products, among
the sample consists of college students based in the United States who others), on media richness.
are savvy with respect to Internet use. Future research can potentially Recent studies focusing on media choice have identified the impor-
test the media richness theory across other settings including additional tance of task situations such as urgency, confidentiality, accountability,
product categories and other consumer groups. Third, this study in- social interaction, and information integrity in the decision making pro-
cludes only search and experience products in the investigation, cess [40]. Therefore, future studies should investigate the interplay

Table 3c
Summarizing the hypotheses.

Hypotheses Study 1 Study 2 Study 3

H1 Amount of information search is different across the three levels of media Supported Supported Partially s
richness conditions, where the amount of search is the lowest in the low upported
media richness condition.
H2 Perceived media richness-task fit, satisfaction and channel choice intention Supported Supported Supported (for all DVs,
(for information search and future purchase) are different across the three except channel choice)
levels of media richness conditions, where the fit, satisfaction and channel
choice intention are the lowest in the low media richness condition.
H3 Product type moderates the effect of media richness on information search, Supported (for all DVs, Not tested Not tested
media richness-task fit, satisfaction, and channel choice intention such that the except information search)
effects are different across the six levels of media richness-product type
conditions: a high media richness condition is preferred for undertaking a
decision-making task on an experience product, a medium media richness
condition is preferred for undertaking a decision-making task on a search
product, a low media richness condition is not preferred for undertaking a
decision-making task on either search or experience products.
H4 Task complexity moderates the effect of media richness on information search, Not supported Not tested Not tested
media richness-task fit, satisfaction and channel choice intention, such that the
effects are different across the nine levels of media richness-task complexity
conditions: a high media richness condition is preferred for undertaking a
complex decision-making task, a medium media richness condition is
preferred for undertaking a moderate decision-making task, a low media
richness condition is preferred for undertaking a simple decision-making task.
M. Maity, M. Dass / Decision Support Systems 61 (2014) 34–46 45

a) Mediation Analysis
channel-Fit= .483; (t=6.727; p < .05); (F (2,160) = 46.172; p < .05) ***
channel-Sat= .543; (t=7.280; p < .05); (F (2,160) = 37.037; p < .05) ***
channel = .478; (t=6.876; p < .05); (F (1,161) = 47.276; p < .05) * channel-Loy = .532; (t=8.447; p < .05); (F (2,160) = 84.004; p < .05) ***
Media Richness

channel-Fit= .433; (t=6.074; p < .05); (F (1,161) = 36.890; p < .05) **


channel-Sat= .301; (t=3.988; p < .05); (F (1,161) = 15.902; p < .05) **
channel-Loy = .544; (t=8.194; p < .05); (F (1,161) = 67.134; p < .05) **

Channel Fit, Satisfaction,


Loyalty
channel-Fit= .202; t=2.817; p < .05 ***
channel-Sat= .041; t=0.556; p > .05 n.s. ***
channel-Loy = .290; t=4.603; p < .05 ***

Sobel Testchannel-Fit: z = 3.069; p < .05; Goodman Test channel-Fit: z = 3.110; p < .05
Sobel Testchannel-Sat: z = 3.378; p < .05; Goodman Test channel-Sat: z = 3.412; p < .05
Sobel Testchannel-Loy: z = 3.380; p < .05; Goodman Test channel-Loy: z = 3.413; p < .05

* Equation 1: Regressing Mediator (Media Richness) on IV (Channel)


** Equation 2: Regressing DV (Fit, Satisfaction, and Loyalty) on IV (Channel)
***Equation 3: Regressing DV (Fit, Satisfaction, and Loyalty) on Mediator (Media Richness) and IV (Channel)

b) Mediation Analysis (Controlling for the Unique Channel Characteristics)


channel-Fit= .384; (t=6.213; p < .05); (F (3,159) = 64.927; p < .05) ***
channel-Sat= .469; (t=6.670; p < .05); (F (3,159) = 38.036; p < .05) ***
= .428; (t=5.902; p < .05); (F (2,160) = 27.695; p < .05) *
channel-Loy = .458; (t=8.003; p < .05); (F (3,159) = 84.462; p < .05) ***
channel

Media Richness

channel-Fit= .273; (t=4.405; p < .05); (F (2,160) = 63.156; p < .05) **


channel-Sat= .172; (t=2.403; p < .05); (F (2,160) = 27.336; p < .05) **
channel-Loy = .416; (t=6.803; p < .05); (F (2,160) = 67.790; p < .05) **

Channel Fit, Satisfaction,


Loyalty
channel-Fit= .111; t=1.802; p > .05 n.s. ***
channel-Sat= -.026; t=-0.372; p > .05 n.s. ***
channel-Loy = .222; t=3.889; p < .05 ***

Sobel Testchannel-Fit: z = 2.731; p < .05; Goodman Test channel-Fit: z = 2.776; p < .05
Sobel Testchannel-Sat: z = 2.967; p < .05; Goodman Test channel-Sat: z = 3.003; p < .05
Sobel Testchannel-Loy: z = 2.998; p < .05; Goodman Test channel-Loy: z = 3.034; p < .05

* Equation 1: Regressing Mediator (Media Richness) on IV (Channel) and Unique Characteristics


** Equation 2: Regressing DV (Fit, Satisfaction, and Loyalty) on IV (Channel) and Unique Characteristics
***Equation 3: Regressing DV (Fit, Satisfaction, and Loyalty) on Mediator (Media Richness), IV (Channel) and
Unique Characteristics

Fig. 3. a: Mediation analysis. b: Mediation analysis. (controlling for the unique channel characteristics).

between media richness of channels and these task situations on con- [3] R. Barkhi, V.S. Jacob, L. Pipino, H. Pirkul, A study of the effect of communication chan-
nel and authority on group decision processes and outcomes, Decision Support Sys-
sumers' media-task fit. As new features are incorporated into tems 23 (1998) 205–226.
established channels, it is becoming imperative to understand how [4] J.R. Bettman, E.J. Johnson, J.W. Payne, A componential analysis of cognitive effort in
channel characteristics impact consumer decision making. The types choice, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 45 (1) (1990) 111–138.
[5] G. Biehal, D. Chakravarti, Information–presentation format and learning goals as de-
of information that can be effectively provided to consumers at different terminants of consumers' memory retrieval and choice processes, Journal of Con-
levels of media richness need to be examined. We hope that this paper sumer Research 8 (March) (1982) 431–441.
will encourage future studies to investigate the importance of media [6] M.J. Bitner, B.H. Booms, M.S. Tetreault, The service encounter: diagnosing favorable
and unfavorable incidents, Journal of Marketing 54 (January 1990) 71–84.
richness in decision making across retail channel mix. [7] G. Bock, J. Lee, H. Kuan, J. Kim, The progression of online trust in the multi-channel
retailer context and the role of product uncertainty, Decision Support Systems 53
(2012) 97–107.
References [8] K. Chen, D.C. Yen, S. Hung, A.H. Huang, An exploratory study of the selection of com-
munication media: the relationship between flow and communication outcomes,
[1] S. Balasubramanian, R.A. Peterson, S.L. Jarvenpaa, Exploring the implications of Decision Support Systems 45 (2008) 822–832.
m-commerce for markets and marketing, Journal of the Academy of Marketing [9] T. Chen, Y. Yesilada, S. Harper, What input errors do you experience? Typing and
Science 30 (4) (2002) 348–366. pointing errors of mobile Web users, International Journal of Human Computer
[2] R.M. Baron, D.A. Kenny, The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psy- Studies 68 (3) (2010) 138–157.
chological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations, Journal of [10] T.L. Childers, C.L. Carr, J. Peck, S. Carson, Hedonic and utilitarian motivations for on-
Personality and Social Psychology 51 (6) (1986) 1173–1182. line retail shopping behavior, Journal of Retailing 77 (2001) 511–535.
46 M. Maity, M. Dass / Decision Support Systems 61 (2014) 34–46

[11] P.H. Chiu, G.Y.M. Kao, C.C. Lo, Personalized blog content recommender system for [44] G.N. Punj, R. Moore, Smart versus knowledgeable online recommendation agents,
mobile phone users, International Journal of Human Computer Studies 68 (8) Journal of Interactive Marketing 21 (4) (2007) 46–60.
(2010) 496–507. [45] S. Rose, P. Samouel, Internal psychological versus external market-driven determi-
[12] V. Choudhury, E. Karahanna, The relative advantage of electronic channels: a multi- nants of the amount of consumer information search amongst online shoppers,
dimensional view, MIS Quarterly 32 (1) (2008) 179–200. Journal of Marketing Management 25 (1/2) (2009) 171–190.
[13] I. Clarke III, Emerging value propositions for m-commerce, Journal of Business [46] A. Sa Vinhas, S. Chatterjee, S. Dutta, A. Fein, J. Lajos, S. Neslin, L. Scheer, W. Ross, Q.
Strategies 25 (2) (2008) 41–57. Wang, Channel design, coordination, and performance: future research directions,
[14] E. Coupey, E. Sandgathe, Rethinking research on communications media: information Marketing Letters 21 (2010) 223–237.
modality and message structuring, Advances in Consumer Research 27 (2000) [47] N. Srinivasan, B.T. Ratchford, An empirical test of a model of external search for
224–229. automobiles, Journal of Consumer Research 18 (2) (1991) 233–242.
[15] R.L. Daft, R.H. Lengel, Information richness: a new approach to managerial behavior [48] S. Stableford, W. Mettger, Plain language: a strategic response to the health literacy
and organization design, Research in Organizational Behavior 6 (1984) 191–233. challenge, Journal of Public Health Policy 28 (2007) 71–93.
[16] R.L. Daft, R.H. Lengel, L.K. Trevino, Message equivocality, media selection, and man- [49] J. Strebel, T. Erdem, J. Swait, Consumer search in high technology markets: exploring
ager performance: implications for information systems, MIS Quarterly 11 (3) the use of traditional information channels, Journal of Consumer Psychology 14
(1987) 355–366. (1/2) (2004) 96–104.
[17] S. Devaraj, M. Fan, R. Kohli, Examination of online channel preference: using the struc- [50] K.S. Suh, Impact of communication medium on task performance and satisfaction:
ture–conduct–outcome framework, Decision Support Systems 42 (2006) 1089–1103. an examination of media-richness theory, Information and Management 35
[18] G.R. Dowling, R. Staelin, A model of perceived risk and intended risk-handling activ- (1999) 295–312.
ity, Journal of Consumer Research 21 (June 1994) 119–134. [51] P. Todd, I. Benbasat, The use of information in decision making: an experimental in-
[19] Emarketer, Is mobile the next frontier in online shopping? http://www.emarketer. vestigation of the impact of computer-based decision aids, MIS Quarterly 16 (3)
com/blog/index.php/mobile-frontier-online-shopping/, 2010. (Retrieved January 17). (1992) 373–393.
[20] Euromonitor Database, 2010. (Retrieved April 5). [52] A.C.B. Tse, F. Yim, Factors affecting the choice of channels: online vs. conventional,
[21] J.C. Flanagan, The critical incident technique, Psychological Bulletin 51 (July 1954) Journal of International Consumer Marketing 14 (2/3) (2002) 137–152.
327–357. [53] J.E. Urbany, P.R. Dickson, W.L. Wilkie, Buyer uncertainty and information search,
[22] B. Florenthal, S. Aviv, Four-mode channel interactivity concept and channel prefer- Journal of Consumer Research 16 (2) (1989) 208–214.
ences, Journal of Services Marketing 24 (1) (2010) 29–41. [54] P.C. Verhoef, S.A. Neslin, B. Vroomen, Multi-channel customer management: under-
[23] G.T. Ford, D.B. Smith, J.L. Swasy, Consumer skepticism of advertising claims: testing standing the research shopper phenomenon, International Journal of Research in
hypotheses from economics of information, Journal of Consumer Research 16 (4) Marketing 24 (2) (2007) 129–148.
(1990) 433–441. [55] D.W. Wallace, J.L. Giese, J.L. Johnson, Customer retailer loyalty in the context of
[24] D.L. Goodhue, Understanding user evaluations of information systems, Management multiple channel strategies, Journal of Retailing 80 (4) (2004) 249–263.
Science 4 (12) (1995) 1827–1844. [56] J. Webster, L.K. Trevino, Rational and social theories as complementary explanations
[25] D.L. Goodhue, Development and measurement validity of a task-technology fit in- of communication media choices: two policy-capturing studies, Academy Manage-
strument for user evaluations of information systems, Decision Sciences 29 (1) ment Journal 38 (1995) 1544–1572.
(1998) 105–138. [57] R.A. Westbrook, R.L. Oliver, The dimensionality of consumption emotion pat-
[26] A. Gupta, B.C. Su, Z. Walter, An empirical study of consumer switching from tradi- terns and consumer satisfaction, Journal of Consumer Research 18 (1) (1991)
tional to electronic channels: a purchase-decision process perspective, International 84–92.
Journal of Electronic Commerce 8 (3) (2004) 131–161. [58] D.J. Xu, S. Liao, Q. Li, Combining empirical approach and modeling techniques for
[27] A. Gupta, B.C. Su, Z. Walter, Risk profile and consumer shopping behavior in personalized mobile advertising application, Decision Support Systems 44 (3)
electronic and traditional channels, Decision Support Systems 38 (2004) 347–367. (February 2008) 710–724.
[28] K. Hassanein, M. Head, The impact of infusing social presence in the Web interface: [59] S. Yang, Y. Lu, P.Y.K. Chau, Why do consumers adopt online channel? An empirical
an investigation across product types, International Journal of Electronic Commerce investigation of two channel extension mechanisms, Decision Support Systems 54
10 (2) (2006) 31–55. (2013) 858–869.
[29] H. Hoehle, E. Scornavacca, S. Huff, Three decades of research on consumer adoption [60] I. Zigurs, B.K. Buckland, A theory of task/technology fit and group support systems
and utilization of electronic banking channels: a literature analysis, Decision effectiveness, MIS Quarterly 22 (3) (1998) 313–334.
Support Systems 54 (2012) 122–132.
[30] P. Huang, N.H. Lurie, S. Mitra, Searching for experience on the Web: an empirical exam-
Moutusy Maity (mmaity@iiml.ac.in) is an associate profes-
ination of for search and Experience goods, Journal of Marketing 73 (March 2009) 55–69.
sor of marketing at the Indian Institute of Management,
[31] J. Järveläinen, Online purchase intentions: an empirical testing of a multiple-theory
Lucknow, India. She holds a Ph.D. in Business Administration
model, Journal of Organizational Computing and Electronic Commerce 17 (1)
from the University of Georgia. Her research focuses on is-
(2007) 53–74.
sues regarding adoption of technology.
[32] L.R. Klein, Creating virtual product experiences, Journal of Interactive Marketing 17
(1) (2003) 41–55.
[33] Y. Lee, K.A. Kozar, K.R. Larsen, Avatar e-mail versus traditional e-mail: perceptual differ-
ence and media selection difference, Decision Support Systems 46 (2009) 451–467.
[34] T.P. Liang, C.P. Wei, Introduction to the special issue: mobile commerce applications,
International Journal of Electronic Commerce 8 (3) (2004) 7–17.
[35] J.E. McGrath, A.B. Hollingshead, Groups Interacting with Technology, Sage Publica-
tions, Newbury Park, CA, 1994.
[36] N. Michaelidou, D.C. Arnott, S. Dibb, Characteristics of marketing channels: a theo-
retical framework, Marketing Review 5 (1) (2005) 45–57.
[37] M. Montoya-Weiss, G.B. Voss, D. Grewal, Determinants of on-line channel use and
overall satisfaction with a relational multi-channel service provider, Journal of the
Mayukh Dass (mayukh.dass@ttu.edu) is an associate profes-
Academy of Marketing Science 31 (4) (2003) 448–465.
sor of marketing at the Rawls College of Business, Texas Tech
[38] P. Nelson, Information and consumer behavior, Journal of Political Economy 78 (2)
University. He holds a Ph.D. in Business Administration, an
(1970) 311–329.
M.S. in Statistics and an M.S. in Artificial Intelligence from
[39] S.A. Neslin, D. Grewal, R. Leghorn, S.S. Venkatesh, M.L. Teerling, J.S. Thomas, P.C.
the University of Georgia. His research focuses on statistical
Verhoef, Challenges and opportunities in multichannel customer management,
and analytical methods with applications to valuation issues
Journal of Service Research 9 (2) (2006) 95–112.
in dynamic economies and brand management. He is a mem-
[40] P. Palvia, P. Pinjani, S. Cannoy, T. Jacks, Contextual constraints in media choice: be-
ber of the INFORMS.
yond information richness, Decision Support Systems 51 (2011) 657–670.
[41] J.W. Payne, Contingent decision making, Psychological Bulletin 92 (1982) 382–402.
[42] M.P. Pritchard, M.E. Havitz, D.R. Howard, Analyzing the commitment-loyalty link in ser-
vice contexts, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 27 (3) (1999) 333–348.
[43] G.N. Punj, R. Staelin, A model of consumer information search behavior for new
automobiles, Journal of Consumer Research 9 (March 1983) 366–380.

You might also like