You are on page 1of 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/228196854

Sources of Work-Family Conflict: A Sino-U.S. Comparison of the Effects of


Work and Family Demands

Article  in  The Academy of Management Journal · August 2007


DOI: 10.2307/1556390

CITATIONS READS

319 2,100

3 authors:

Nini Yang Chao C. Chen


San Francisco State University Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey
1 PUBLICATION   319 CITATIONS    85 PUBLICATIONS   6,339 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Jaepil Choi
Sungkyunkwan University
19 PUBLICATIONS   2,171 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Communitarianism View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Nini Yang on 10 March 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


* Academy of Management Journal
2000, Vol. 43, No. 1, 113-123.

SOURCES OF WORK-FAMILY CONFLICT: A SINO-U.S. COMPARISON OF


THE EFFECTS OF WORK AND FAMILY DEMANDS
NINI YANG
Clayton College & State University of Georgia

CHAO C. CHEN
JAEPIL CHOI
Rutgers University

YIMIN ZOU
Nanjing University

Given differences in values about work and family time, we hypothesize that Ameri-
cans will experience greater family demand, which will have greater impact on
work-family conflict, whereas the Chinese will experience greater work demand,
which will have the greater impact on work..family conflict. The results of a survey of
working men and women in the two countries generally supported the hypotheses;
however, work demand did not differ significantly between the two countries and did
not have a greater effect than family demand on work-family conflict in China.

Research on wrork-family conflict has been con- domains rather than to the domains themselves.
ducted primarily in Western industrialized na- Second, mere differences in values, social relation-
tions, most notably the United States, but economic ships, and requirements between work and family
and business globalization has made work-family lives do not automatically constitute conflict.
issues increasingly important in developing coun- Third, the major concern is those role conflicts that
tries. And because v^rork and family issues are in- cause problems of role participation.
tricately related to cultural beliefs, values, and Greenhaus and Beutell (1985) identified three
norms (Lobel, 1991; Schein, 1984), multinational major types of work-family conflicts. The first is
companies need to be aware of cultural influences time-based. Time spent on role performance in one
on their operations and to develop culturally ap- domain often precludes time spent in the other
propriate strategies to deal with work-family con- domain. Time expended on role performance may
flict and its effects. deplete energy or generate strain. The second work-
In this study, we sought to help fill a critical void family conflict, strain-based conflict, arises when
in work-family conflict research by exploring cross- strain in one role affects one's performance in an-
national differences in the sources of such conflict.
other role. The last type is behavior-based conflict,
We surveyed employees of American and Chinese
which refers to incompatibility between the behav-
enterprises to compare the extent to which differ-
ent sources affected work-family conflict. ior patterns that are desirable in the two domains.
In this cross-national study, we focused on time-
based conflict, recognizing that the concept also
included time-induced strain (Greenhaus & Beu-
WORK-FAMILY CONFLICT AND ITS SOURCES
tell, 1985). The conceptual specificity of time-
Work-Family Conflict based conflict allowed us to better measure and
validate the construct in different social, economic,
Greenhaus and Beutell defined work-family con-
flict as follows: "[It is] a form of inter-role conflict and cultural systems. Drawing on recent distinc-
in which the role pressures from the work and tions between work-to-family conflict and family-
family domains are mutually noncompatible in to-work conflict (e.g., Frone, Russell, & Cooper,
some respect. That is, participation in the work 1992; Greenhaus & Parasuraman, 1994; Gutek,
(family) role is made more difficult by virtue of Searle, & Klepa, 1991), we included both directions
participation in the family (work) role" (1985: 77). in the conception of work-family conflict but did
This definition delimits the scope of work-family not decompose them into different constructs since
conflict in a number of ways. First, the term "work- we were concerned with the sources rather than
family" refers to roles within the work and family directionality of conflict.
113
114 Academy of Management Journal February

Work and Family Demands these two mechanisms, a focal person will be more
Different types of pressure can be sources of responsive to role demands from the domain with
work-family conflict. One type exists in an individ- higher priority.
ual's objective environment; expectations or pres- The work cited above highlights potential effects
sures "sent" to the focal person by members of his of culture on work-family conflict. A Sino-U.S.
or her role set are examples (Kahn, Wolfe, Quinn, comparison provides an excellent opportunity to
Snoek, & Rosenthal, 1964). The other type exists in examine such cultural effects. Earlier cross-cultural
the individual's subjective psychological environ- research has shown striking differences between
ment. The objective pressures have to be perceived the two countries on various dimensions, including
by the focal person to affect his or her role perfor- individualism-collectivism, goal priorities, and
mance. Demands may also truly originate from long- and short-term orientations (e.g., Chen, 1995;
within the person; these are known as "own forces" Earley, 1989,1993; Hofstede, 1991; Schwartz, 1994;
(Kahn et al., 1964: 17). Shenkar & Ronen, 1987). Implications of these cul-
tural differences for work-family conflict, however,
As the subjective environment reflects both the have not been investigated.
objective environment and the values and expecta-
tions resulting from a long process of socialization, Despite popular images of Americans as career-
we defined demand in terms of role performers' oriented and of the Chinese as family-oriented,
perception and feeling of pressure. Following Hofstede (1980) found that Western individualist
Frone, Russell, and Cooper (1997) and keeping our societies valued family and personal time more
focus on time-based work-family conflict, we con- strongly than Eastern coUectivist societies. Consis-
ceptualized work-family demands as time-based tently, Shenkar and Ronen (1987) showed that
role pressures (Kahn et al., 1964). Work demand mainland Chinese managers assigned low impor-
refers primarily to pressures arising from excessive tance to family and personal time, just as their
workloads and typical workplace time pressures counterparts in Hong Kong, Taiwan, and Singapore
such as rush jobs and deadlines. Family demand did. Schein (1984) also proposed that Eastern soci-
refers primarily to time pressures associated with eties gave greater priority to work than do Western
tasks like housekeeping and child care. Family de- societies.
mand is often related to family characteristics such When there is a conflict of interests, individual-
as the number of dependents, family size, and fam- ists tend to put self-interests above collective inter-
ily composition (e.g., Frone et al., 1992; Near, Rice, ests, and collectivists tend to do the opposite
& Hunt, 1980). (Chen, Meindl, & Hunt, 1997; Hofstede, 1980; Par-
sons, 1951; Triandis, 1995). Next, we examine how
individualism-collectivism affects relationships
A CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE ON WORK-
within families and between families and work or-
FAMILY CONFLICT ganizations.

Work and family pressures reflect social expec-


tations and self-expectations and are most suscep- Individualism and Work-Family Priority in the
tible to values, beliefs, and role-related self-concep- United States
tions internalized through socialization (Greenhaus In the United States, according to Schein (1984),
& Beutell, 1985; Gutek et al., 1991; Kahn et al., an individual's career connotes personal ambition
1964; Lobel, 1991; Parasuraman, Purohit, God- and achievement. If the main purpose of work is to
shalk, & Beutell, 1996). The cited authors have further the personal career, a good family person
argued that role-related self-conceptions not only should not allow work to interfere with family.
moderate the relationship between demand and Allowing work to interfere is likely to cause dissat-
conflict but also have a direct impact on demand isfaction in other family members that may lead to
within a domain. That is, demand is greater from serious consequences, like separation, divorce, and
the domain with the higher priority. disowning. There has been evidence that as work
Three rationales are proposed to support this pri- interferes with family, family members, instead of
ority-demand link. First, from the perspective of providing more social and emotional support to the
self-sender expectation (Kahn et al., 1964), a person distressed worker, withdraw such support (Adams,
becomes more ego-involved, investing more time King, & King, 1996; Jackson, Zedeck, & Summers,
and energy in the high-priority domain. Second, 1985).
the domain on which the role set and the society at In dealing with other, nonfamily, collectivities,
large put higher priority exerts greater role pressure individualists by definition place priority on indi-
through social expectations and norms. Under vidual and family interests (Hofstede, 1980, 1991).
2000 Yang, Chen, Choi, and Zou 115

When work and family conflict, Americans are ex- ing extra work responsibilities and assignments
pected to side with family. Colin Powell explained that may disrupt family life temporarily but are
to his, supporters that he would not run for presi- expected to bring future benefits.
dent of the United States hecause running for and The cultural norm of collectivism in China also
assuming that office would involve too much per- legitimizes giving priority to work by emphasizing
sonal and family sacrifice. Regardless of whether or reciprocity between family and other, larger collec-
not family concerns were his reason for not run- tivities (Bond & Hwang, 1986: 216; Redding, 1993:
ning, Powell's citing that reason illustrates the 61). Even after decades of economic reform, Chi-
greater legitimacy and primacy of family relative to nese industrial enterprises continue to be the pri-
other collectivities in America. mary providers of social welfare benefits, and em-
The family value has heen enhanced hy a more ployees still have strong organizational or work
general emphasis on quality of life in American group identities (Child, 1994; Tung, 1991). The eco-
society; this emphasis may he largely a result of nomic reform in general and Deng's slogan "to be
high industrialization and living standards (Ingle- rich is glorious" in particular have legitimized
hart, 1990). Bellah, Madsen, Sullivan, Swidler, and wealth-seeking hard work as benefiting not only
Tipton (1985) labeled these relationship values the family, but also the community and the nation.
"expressive individualism," suggesting that more The Chinese work priority argument has found em-
Americans sought self-fulfillment through expres- pirical support in research by Lai (1995) and Lin
sive than through "utilitarian" individualism, such and Lai (1995). These authors found that work role
as advancement in organizations and careers. Har- stressors had greater impact than did family role
ris and Associates' (1981) national survey found stressors in Shanghai and Tianjing.
that 62 percent of working adults considered family Finally, we also recognize that a number of social
to he important when they decided about work and family factors in China may help reduce family
schedules, joh-related travel, and joh relocation. demand. The decades-long, rigorously enforced
For reasons including the ahove, research has one-child-per-family policy has significantly re-
shown that family events tend to have a stronger duced Chinese family size and hence child care
effect than work events on psychological well- and housework. Chinese parents often help adult
heing in North America and Western Europe (Lin & and married children with household chores and
Lai, 1995). Systematic research hy Frone and col- child rearing. Childcare is more available in the
leagues (Frone et al., 1992; Frone, Russell, & Coo- workplace or in the local community in China than
per, 1995; Frone, Russell, & Barnes, 1996; Frone, in the United States. Chinese women have worked
Yardley, & Markel, 1997) has consistently shown on a large scale since the founding of the PRC in
that family stressors had a greater impact on psy- 1949. The Chinese therefore may be more experi-
chological and physical health than did work stres- enced in dealing with families' dual careers than
sors in the United States. the Americans.

Collectivism and Work-Family Priority in China


Summary and Hypotheses
In contrast to the American individualist notion
of career, in the Chinese societies of Hong Kong, In summary, because of national differences in
Taiwan, and the People's Repuhlic of China (PRC), orientations to self and family, overwork is likely to
work is seen as for the welfare of the family (Red- be perceived as sacrificing family for one's own
ding, 1993; Redding & Wong, 1986). Redding (1993: career in the United States but as sacrificing self for
39) explained how the Chinese strive to bring the family in China. Furthermore, American cul-
honor and prosperity to their families through their tural norms put family before work, but Chinese
work. According to this family-based work ethic, norms put work before family. Finally, the above
extra work after official hours or on weekends is a contrasts may be enhanced by expressive individ-
self-sacrifice made for the benefit of the family ualism in the United States and the economic re-
rather than a sacrifice of the family for the selfish form in China. Comhined, these ideas lead to the
pursuit of one's own career. A study of 23 countries following hypotheses:
(Chinese Culture Connection, 1987) showed China
ranking 1st on values such as perseverance, thrift, Hypothesis la. Family demand will be greater
saving, and willingness to subordinate oneself for a in the United States than in China.
purpose, whereas the United States ranked 17th
(Hofstede, 1991). Such self-sacrificial, long-term Hypothesis lb. Work demand will be greater in
orientations legitimize and even encourage assum- China than in the United States.
116 Academy of Management Journal February

Hypothesis 2a. The effect of family demand on fied time as the most consistent basis of work-
work-family conflict will be greater than that of family conflict cited by the groups. Indeed, all six
work demand in the United States. groups agreed that most Chinese employees expe-
rienced work-family conflict; five groups cited time
Hypothesis 2b. The effect of work demand on
conflict, and the one group that did not explicitly
work-family conflict will be greater than that of
cite time cited being too busy, which implied time
family demand in China.
conflict. Ofthe five groups that specified time, four
Hypothesis 3a. The effect of family demand on listed it first. Other types of conflict identified by
work-family conflict will be greater in the no more than two groups included energy, spiritual
United States than in China. or psychological, role shifting, and low income.
The findings of the qualitative data showed that
Hypothesis 3b. The effect of work demand on work-family conflict was experienced by this sam-
work-family conflict will be greater in China ple of Chinese employees and that time was a crit-
than in the United States. ical issue in the work and family lives of these people.
Because the major constructs were all percep- The second sample consisted of 117 Chinese em-
tional and had been developed in the United States, ployees working in Beijing. About half were attend-
w^e conducted a construct validation study in China ing an evening training program offered by the Bei-
before we tested the hypotheses. The validation jing Municipal Bureau of Light Industry; the rest of
study employed two samples. In the first, we ex- the respondents were spread evenly among a state-
amined the relevance of time-based work-family owned smeltery, a state-owned publishing house,
conflict in China through qualitative data and in and a collectively owned printing factory. The av-
the second, we examined the discriminant validity erage age was 32 years; 65 percent were women,
of measures of work demand, family demand, and and 62 percent were married.
work-family conflict through quantitative data.
Measures
STUDY 1: VALIDATING THE CONSTRUCTS Measures of work demand, family demand, and
IN CHINA work-family conflict were administered to the
Samples
members of the second sample.
Four items assessing time-related family pres-
The first sample consisted of 41 Chinese part- sures were taken from Yang's (1993) family de-
time master's of business administration students mand scale. Respondents were queried on feelings
attending a class in organizational behavior at a about shortness of time and lack of energy resulting
Chinese university. The average age of class mem- from family role pressures. The response scale
bers was 29 years; 14 were women, and 19 were ranged from 1 (almost none/never] to 5 (very much/
married. About half of the class's members were always). To measure work demand, we used three
managers; one-quarter were administrators; and the items on time-based work role pressures from Spec-
rest were engineers or other professionals. For the tor's (1975) Organizational Frustration Scale; the
study, we divided the students into six groups with response scale ranged from 1 (completely disagree)
similar mixes of age, marriage, sex, and job posi- to 5 (completely agree). The work-family conflict
tion. Each group selected one person to take notes measure consisted of four items on the extent to
to be handed in at the end of the discussion. which work and family competed for a person's
Both the questions and the discussions were in time and energy. Two items did not specify direc-
Chinese. Discussions on two questions are of inter- tion; one represented work-to-family conflict; and
est here: Are there any work-family contradictions one represented family-to-work conflict. The re-
or conflicts salient to most Chinese enterprise em- sponse scale ranged from 1 (not at all/never) to 5
ployees? If there are, in what ways are they mani- (a lot/very often). All the measures were translated
fested? The discussion notes were collected and into Chinese through a back-translation method
translated into English to be analyzed by two or- (Brislin, 1970).
ganizational behavior professors unrelated to this
research. The professors were asked to categorize
the work-family conflicts and identify the conflict Confirmatory Factor Analysis
identified most consistently across the six groups. Confirmatory factor analysis using LISREL 8
Of 17 nominated conflicts, 2 provoked some dis- (Joreskog & Sorbom, 1993) was conducted on the
agreement, so the interrater agreement on the con- three constructs. Initially all items ofthe three con-
flict categories was 94 percent. Both raters identi- structs were entered for analysis. On the basis of
2000 Yang, Chen, Choi, and Zou 117

modification indexes, we dropped poorly loaded three-factor model was more appropriate for the
items. The final confirmatory factor analysis in- - data.
cluded four items for family demand, two items for
work demand, and three items for work-family con-
flict (for specific items, see Table 1). The reliability STUDY 2: HYPOTHESIS TESTING
of those three factors was satisfactory [a = .78, .76,
and .76, respectively). The overall goodness-of-fit Sample and Procedures
statistics indicated that the data fit the three-factor The United States data were collected from a
model well (;^^24 = 43.93, goodness-of-fit index large multidivisional manufacturing company in
[GFI] = .92, incremental fit index [IFI] = .94, root- the northeastern United States that produced pre-
mean-square error of approximation [RMSEA] = cision tools and traded with Ghina. Survey ques-
.08). All items loaded significantly on their under- tionnaires were distributed to employees at all lev-
lying common factors, providing evidence of the els through the company's divisional offices of
measures' acceptable convergent validity (BoUen, human resources. Questionnaires were completed
1989). voluntarily and returned in sealed envelopes ad-
We additionally assessed the discriminant valid- dressed to the first author. A total of 129 completed
ity of the three constructs by conducting chi-square surveys were returned (for a response rate of 31
difference tests. For each pair of factor correlations, percent). Because the 21 managers who returned
we compared the chi-square value of the uncon- the survey used a work demand scale targeted for
strained model with the value of the constrained managerial jobs, we excluded them from our cur-
model (in which the correlation of two factors is rent cross-national analysis.
fixed at 1.0; see Anderson and Gerbing [1988]). All The Chinese data were collected from 181 re-
between-model chi-square differences were highly spondents. They were employees of a state-owned
significant, indicating the presence of discriminant company making dial plates (27%) or of a joint
validity. Furthermore, we created a latent variable venture making printed circuit boards (29%) or
and forced all nine items onto it. The goodness-of- were students in a business training program (44%)
fit statistics of this single-factor model were all offered by the Beijing Municipal Bureau of Light
below the acceptable level, suggesting that the Industry. At each site, a Chinese business professor

TABLE 1
Results of Full Gonfirinatory Factor Analyses, Study 2"'
Factors and Items China United States

Family demand
How much time do you spend on home/family-related activities such as taking care of .61 .64
children or other, cooking, laundry, house cleaning, yard work, etc.?
How often do family duties and responsibilities make you feel tired out? .78 .79
How often do you feel short of time for these home/family-related activities? .76 .78
How difficult is it for you to do everything you should as a family member? .45 .78

Work demand
I often feel that I am being run ragged. .43 .69
I am given entirely too much work to do. .88 .83
Work-family conflict
How much conflict do you feel there is between the demands of your job and your family life? .63 .94
How much does your job situation interfere with your family life? .65 .73
How much does your family situation interfere with your job? .41 .67

Goodness-of-fit indexes
100.53 (54) 77.90 (54)
p< .00 .02
GFI .90 .91
IFI .90 .96
RMSEA .08 .06

" All of the factor loadings in this table are from an unstandardized solution. The respective null models from which the relevant
statistics were calculated are 508.19, 91 df, for the Ghinese sample and 581.92, 91 df, for the U.S. sample.
*" All of the factor loadings in this table are significant at or below p = .001. Because the five control variables (sex, age, marriage, number
of dependents, and unpaid working hours) were measured with single items, their factor loadings were fixed at 1.00.
118 Academy of Management Journal February

introduced the study and collected completed sur- loadings were significant at the .001 level. Further,
veys. For the same reason noted for the American the interfactor correlations between the three latent
sample, we excluded 59 Chinese managers from the variables in either group were fairly low (mean r =
cross-national analysis. The final sample for this .26), except for that between family demand and
study consisted of 108 American and 122 Chinese work-family conflict in the U.S. sample (r = .86). A
employees. reexamination of the item content of family de-
Efforts were made to survey samples of Chinese mand and work-family conflict showed a clear con-
and American manufacturing employees with sim- ceptual distinction between the two factors. Given
ilar gender composition because previous research the conceptual distinction and the goodness-of-fit
has suggested that work-family relationships vary index of the U.S. three-factor model, we treated
with gender and occupation (Gutek et al., 1991; family demand and work-family conflict as inde-
Lobel, 1991; Near et al., 1980). T-test results pendent constructs despite their high factor corre-
showed no differences between the groups in gen- lation in the U.S. sample.
der composition and marital status. However, the Measurement equivalence. Table 2 presents re-
Chinese respondents were, on the average, seven sults of a two-group analysis in which we tested for
years younger than the Americans, and there were cross-national equivalence in the factor structures
15 percent more dual-career couples among the of family demand, work demand, and work-family
married or quasi-married families. conflict. We first formulated a baseline model
(model 1 in Table 2) in which factor loadings were
allowed to vary for the nations. The goodness-of-fit
Measures and Analyses
indexes of this model showed that the number of
In all of the analyses, we used four family de- factors was the same for both nations (GFI = .91,
mand items, two work demand items, and three IFI = .93, RMSEA = .07). However, when invari-
work-family conflict items that emerged from the ance constraints were put on the factor loadings,
validation study. Control variables included sex the new model (model 2) was significantly different
(man = 1, woman = 2), age in years, marital status from the baseline model (A^^ = 42.48, Ad/ = 9),
(married/partner = 1, single = 0), number of de- indicating the corresponding factor loadings were
pendents (number of people being cared for, such not equivalent for the two nations. We took the
as children or elders), and unpaid hours of over- effect of this nonequivalence into account in testing
time. Paid working hours w^ere excluded because it the hypotheses.
was unclear whether the Chinese trainees counted
class time as working hours.
Results
We used LISREL analyses both to examine the
measurement validity and equivalence and to test Table 3 provides descriptive statistics and corre-
the hypotheses. The input was a 14 X 14 covari- lation coefficients for the variables in each nation.
ance matrix for each nation. Using a two-step ap- As expected, family and work demands were sig-
proach (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988), we first as- nificantly related to work-family conflict in both
sessed the measurement model for each nation groups. Also in both groups, family demand was
using LISREL single-group confirmatory factor positively correlated with the number of depen-
analyses. We then explored measurement equiva- dents. Although work demand was significantly
lence between the two nations through two-group correlated with unpaid working hours in the
analyses, which consisted of a series of compari- United States, the relationship was not significant
sons of nested models (BoUen, 1989). Following a in China. Interestingly, the correlation between
similar approach, we first examined structural re- family demand and age was positive in China but
lationships between exogenous and endogenous la- negative in the United States.
tent variables in each sample and then tested inter- To test Hypotheses la and lb, we estimated and
action effects between nation and family-work compared the group means of the latent variables of
demand through two-group analyses of structural family and work demands. Since measurement
similarities and differences (Schumacker & Lomax, equivalence tests showed inequivalence in the fac-
1996; Singh, 1995). tor loadings for the two nations, we estimated
Convergent and discriminant validity of the group means without imposing equivalence con-
measures. Our hypothesized measurement model straints on the factor loadings. When the Chinese
fit moderately well with both the Chinese and U.S. means were set to zero, the United States scored
data, as indicated by the results on the goodness- significantly higher for family demand (x = .92,
of-fit indexes presented in Table 1. All items s.e. = .13, p < .001), but the two countries showed
loaded on the hypothesized factors, and the factor no significant difference in work demand x = - . 1 3 ,
2000 Yang, Chen, Choi, and Zou 119

TABLE 2
Results of the Two-Group LISREL Analyses, Study 2
Model df Ad/ GFI

Measurement equivalence test


1. Equal number of factors 178.43 108 .91
2. Invariance constraints on factor loadings 220.91 117 .89
Model 2-model 1 42.48 9

Structural equivalence test


A. No invariance constraints on all structural parameters 298.55 115 .92
B. Invariance constraints on parameters of control variables - work-family conflict 303.80 120 .91
Model B-model A 5.25 5
C. Invariance constraints on parameters of primary variables • work-family conflict 343.15 122 .89
Model C-model B 39.35 2
D. Invariance constraints on parameter of family demand —» \ )rk-family conflict 341.90 121 .89
Model D-model B 38.10 1
E. Invariance constraints on parameter of work demand —» w( k-family conflict 312.97 121 .90
Model E-model B 9.17 1

s.e. = .15, n.s.). Additionally, we conducted the and work demands in China {t = 0.35, n.s.). Hy-
comparisons with the equivalence constraints im- pothesis 2a was therefore supported, hut Hypothe-
posed and found the same pattern of differences sis 2h was not.
(U.S. family demand, x = .96, s.e. = .13, p < .001; To test the interactive effects of nation and fam-
U.S. work demand, x = .03, s.e. = .16, n.s.}. Hy- ily-work demands on work-family conflict, we con-
pothesis la was therefore supported, hut Hypothe- ducted structural equivalence tests, using a series
sis lh was not. of comparisons of nested models (see Tahle 2;
We used structural models to examine the effects Bollen, 1989; Schumacker & Lomax, 1996; Singh,
of family and work demands on work-family con- 1995). We first assessed all structural parameters
flict in each nation. As can be seen in Table 4, (from all main and control variables to work-family
although hoth family and work demands had sig- conflict), initially allowing variance in the factor
nificant effects on work-family conflict in China, loadings of family demand, work demand, and
only family demand was a significant predictor in work-family conflict across the nations (model A,
the United States. T-tests for standardized path co- Tahle 2). Model B, which included cross-national
efficients within each nation showed that in the invariance constraints on the parameters from con-
United States, family demand had a stronger im- trol variahles to work-family conflict, was not sig-
pact on work-family conflict than did work de- nificantly different from model A (A^^ = 5.25,
mand [t = 3.75, p < .001). However, there was no /\df = 5). This ohservation suggested that nation
significant difference in the coefficients for family did not interact with the control variahles on work-

TABLE 3
Descriptive Statistics and Correlations, Study 2°
Variable Mean S.d. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 s.d. Mean

1. Family demand 2.58 0.77 (.78, 83) .33** .73** .12 -.35** .19 .26* .12 0.89 3.53
2. Work demand 2.98 1.09 .28** (.76, .73) .33** -.11 -.18 .11 -.01 .31** 1.04 2.83
3. Work-family conflict 2.30 0.68 .33** .42** (.76, .83) .13 -.26* .20 .21* .15 0.99 2.53
4. Sex 1.64 0.48 .08 -.14 -.23* .11 .10 -.10 -.15 0.50 1.52
5. Age 29.02 7.44 .57** .07 .15 -.12 .08 .01 .11 9.57 36.61
6. Marriage 0.55 0.50 .50** .12 .04 -.01 .63** .17 .18 0.47 0.68
7. Number of dependents 1.45 1.59 .38** .24* .14 .05 .36** .44** .21* 3.05 2.64
8. Unpaid working hours 1.83 3.77 .06 .14 .15 .07 .16 .10 .27* 3.24 1.10

" Means and standard deviations on the left are for the Chinese sample, and those on the right are for the U.S. sample. Correlations above
the diagonal are for the U.S. sample and those below the diagonal are for the Chinese sample. Parentheses contain reliability coefficients
for the Chinese and U.S. samples, respectively.
* p < .05
** p < .01
120 Academy of Management Journal February

TABLE 4 We found that American employees experienced


Maximum Likelihood Parameter Estimates for greater family demand than did Chinese employ-
the Major Paths, Study 2^ ees. Furthermore, family demand had greater im-
pact on work-family conflict in the United States
United than in China, whereas work demand had greater
Path Description China States
impact on work-family conflict in China than in the
Family demand —> work-family conflict .57* .85*** United States. We predicted the above differences
Work demand —> work-family conflict .45* .03 mainly on the basis of the relative values placed on
Sex —> work-family conflict -.58* .09 family and work time in the two countries. We
Age -^ work-family conflict -.02 .00 contended that in China sacrificing family time for
Marriage —> work-family conflict -.38 .11
work is viewed as self-sacrifice for the benefit of the
Number of dependents —> work-family conflict .02 -.01
Unpaid working hours —> work-family conflict .04 .02 family or as a short-term cost incurred to gain long-
term benefits, but that in the United States sacrific-
" All of the parameter estimates are unstandardized coeffi- ing family time for work is often perceived as a
cients. failure to care for significant others in one's life. We
* p < .05 attributed such differences partly to cultural differ-
** p < .01
*** p < .001
ences in individualism-collectivism and partly to
different levels of industrial development and ma-
terial affluence in the two nations.
family conflict. All subsequent models that im- We did not find full support for the work priority
posed cross-national invariance constraints on pa- argument for the Chinese because, although work
rameters involving family and work demands demand was greater than family demand, the effect
either in combination (model C) or separately of the former on work-family conflict was not sig-
(models D and E) showed significant differences nificantly greater than that of the latter. One possi-
from model B. In other words, nationality did in- ble reason for the finding is that investigating time-
teract with family demand and work demand to based conflict is too limited. Future, expanded
affect work-family conflict. conflict research might investigate how personal
The nested model comparisons were conducted relations in the family or the workplace affect
again but with cross-national invariance con- strain-based conflict between the domains.
straints imposed on the factor loadings. The pattern In study 2, demographic information mainly pro-
of effects we found in the unconstrained models vided control variables. Yet some of these variables
held. It suggests that the moderating effects of na- had contrasting effects in the two countries. Men
tion were not due to the variance in factor loadings. and women did not differ much in the U.S. sample,
In summary, the analyses provided support for Hy- but the Chinese men in our study reported more
potheses 3a and 3b: family demand had a stronger work-family conflict than the Chinese women. The
effect on work-family conflict in the United States correlation between sex and work demand, though
than in China, but work demand had a stronger not statistically significant, suggested a similar pat-
effect on work-family conflict in China than in the tern for those two variables. Future research could
United States. explore w^hether the Chinese economic reform af-
fects men and women differently regarding work
and family roles. Another noteworthy factor is age.
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION
The correlation coefficients showed that older
We reported two studies in this article. In the workers were more likely to be married, to have
first study, we found that work-family conflict, a more dependents, and to experience greater family
well-known social and psychological phenomenon demand in China, but older Americans experi-
in the United States, was also experienced by work- enced less family demand and less work-family
ing men and women in Chinese organizations, es- conflict. It could be that most of the Chinese in this
pecially in terms of time for family and work obli- study, who were on the average younger than the
gations. There w^as also preliminary evidence that Americans, were at a time of life when increasing
time-based work demand, family demand, and age led to more family responsibilities. We, how-
work-family conflict were distinct constructs for ever, suspect that, after marriage, family demand
Chinese employees. The second study compared on Chinese people starts to increase and that even
U.S. and Chinese employees in regard to the levels after retirement, there may be pressure to help
of work and family demands they experience and younger generations if one enjoys good health.
the effects of these demands on work-family con- Such may not be the case with Americans, whose
flict. primary family obligations are to spouses and non-
2000 Yang, Chen, Choi, and Zou 121

adult children. In any case, the effects of demo- family conflict than family-to-work conflict, sug-
graphic characteristics may, as Tsui and Farh gesting that work demand is the major source of
(1997) pointed out, differ in different cultures. stress. On the other hand, the statistical effect of
This research has some limitations. First, al- family-to-work conflict on stress has been shown to
though the results of our second, cross-national be consistently greater than that of work-to-family
study are consistent with cultural expectations, we conflict, suggesting that family demand is the major
did not measure cultural elements to statistically source of stress. In any case, more direct research is
test their effects. Second, more work needs to he needed to explain these seemingly contradictory
done to improve the validity of the measures of findings.
work demand, family demand, and work-family The results of this study also have important
conflict used for cross-cultural research, with spe- implications for designing family- and work-
cial attention paid to content and metric equiva- oriented organizational interventions. Before em-
lence. Third, compared with single-country stud- barking on an intervention, companies should first
ies, this cross-national study appears simplistic in identify the sources of work-family conflict rele-
terms of the numher of variahles explored and the vant to their employees. Where resources are lim-
refinement of each of those variables (e.g., Frone et ited, companies can start with the domain that
al., 1997; Kossek & Ozeki, 1998; Parasuraman et al., poses the most prohlems.
1996). Chinese managers or American managers work-
Despite its limitations, as one of the first studies ing in China should note that, despite the positive
to compare sources of work-family conflict cross- social norm making work the first priority in that
nationally, this study has some important impli- country, this work ethic may not sustain employees
cations for future research. The first is that re- if work pressure continuously results in work-fam-
searchers should resist stereotyping in developing ily conflict. Efforts toward creating a more halanced
hypotheses ahout cross-national differences. Our work environment may hetter serve the reformist
hypotheses were hased on a careful Sino-U.S. com- emphasis on accountability, competitiveness, and
parative analysis of the fundamental relations productivity. For American managers or Chinese
within families and hetween families and work- managers working in America, the message is that
places. Had we taken the stereotypical view of work motivation and productivity could very well
Americans as heing career-oriented and of the Chi- be affected by how employers respond to the family
nese as heing family-oriented, we would not have concerns of their employees. Some companies treat
posed the research questions or formulated the hy- family-friendly programs as part of public rela-
potheses that we did. tions, but only those that effectively adapt work
Second, identifying specific cultural constructs systems to the changing needs of American families
targeted at the special issues under study is impor- can obtain willing cooperation from their otherwise
tant for hoth theory development and theory test- career-oriented employees.
ing. In theorizing about the effects of individualism-
collectivism, we used specific cultural constructs
such as the meaning of work, the importance of
family time, and the contrast hetween expressive REFERENCES
and utilitarian values. For research that measures Adams, G. A., King, L. A., & King, D. W. 1996. Relation-
cultural values, specific constructs may be more ship of job and family involvement, family social
useful because they hetter represent the cognitive support, and work-family conflict with job and life
and affective dilemma experienced by employees satisfaction. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81:
(Morris & Leung, 2000). 411-420.
Third, the findings of this study should inspire Anderson, J. C, & Gerbing, D. W. 1988. Structural equa-
more research on the sources of work-family con- tion modeling in practice: A review and recom-
flict in the United States. There has heen surpris- mended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin,
ingly little work directly assessing the relative pres- 103: 411-423.
sures from work and family and their relative Bellah, R. N., Madsen, R., Sullivan, W. M., Swidler, A.,
effects on work-family conflict, and yet there seems & Tipton, S. M. 1985. Habits of the heart: Individ-
to he a general assumption that work demand is ualism and commitment in American life. Los An-
the primary source of conflict for Americans (e.g., geles: University of Galifornia Press.
Parasuraman et al., 1996: 280). Research that has Bollen, K. A. 1989. Structural equations with latent
indirectly addressed these questions has led to con- variables. New York: Wiley.
tradictory interpretations. On the one hand, Amer- Bond, M. H., & Hwang, K. 1986. The social psychology of
icans have consistently reported higher work-to- Ghinese people. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The psychol-
122 Academy of Management Journal February

ogy of the Chinese people: 213-266. New York: Current research issues: 213-229. London: Ghap-
Oxford University Press. man.
Brislin, R. 1970. Back-translation for cross-cultural re- Gutek, B. A., Searle, S., & Klepa, L. 1991. Rational versus
search. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1: gender role explanations for work-family conflict.
185-216. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76: 560-568.
Ghen, G. G. 1995. New trends in rewards allocation pref- Harris, L., & Associates Inc. 1981. Families at work:
erences: A Sino-United States comparison. Acad- Strengths and strains. Minneapolis: General Mills.
emy of Management Journal, 38: 408-428.
Hofstede, G. 1980. Culture's consequences: Interna-
Ghen, G. G., Meindl, J. R., & Hunt, R. G. 1997. Testing the tional differences in work related values. Beverly
effects of horizontal and vertical collectivism: A Hill, GA: Sage.
study of rewards allocation preferences in Ghina.
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28: 44-70. Hofstede, G. 1991. Cultures and organizations: Software
of the mind. Gambridge, England: McGraw-Hill.
Ghild, J. 1994. Management in China during the age of
reform. New York: Gambridge University Press. Inglehart, R. 1990. Culture shift in advanced industrial
Ghinese Gulture Gonnection. 1987. Ghinese values and society. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
the search for culture-free dimensions of culture. Jackson, S. E., Zedeck, S., & Summers, E. 1985. Family
Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 18: 143-164. life disruptions: Effects of job-induced structural and
Eeirley, P. G. 1989. Social loafing and collectivism: A emotion interference. Academy of Management
comparison of the United States and the People's Journal, 28: 574-586.
Republic of Ghina. Administrative Science Quar- Joreskog, K. G., & Sorbom, D. 1993. LISBEL 8 user's
terly, 34: 565-581. reference guide. Ghicago: Scientific Software Inter-
Earley, P. G. 1993. East meets West meets Mideast: Fur- national.
ther explorations of collectivistic and individualistic Kahn, R. L., Wolfe, D., Quinn, R., Snoek, J. D., &
work groups. Academy of Management Journal, 36: Rosenthal, R. A. 1964. Organizational stress: Stud-
271-288. ies in role conflict and ambiguity. New York:
Frone, M. R., Russell,-M., & Gooper, M. L. 1992. Ante- Wiley.
cedents and outcomes of work-family conflict: Test-
Kossek, E. E., & Ozeki, G. 1998. Work-family conflict,
ing a model of the work-family interface. Journal of
policies, and the job-life satisfaction relationship: A
Applied Psychology, 77: 55—78.
review and directions for organizational behavior-
Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Gooper, M. L. 1995. Relation- human resources research. Journal of Applied Psy-
ship of work and family stressors to psychological chology, 83: 139-149.
distress: The independent moderating influence
of social support, mastery, active coping, and self- Lai, G. 1995. Work and family roles and psychological
focused attention. In R. Grandall & P. L. Perrewe well-being in urban Ghina. Journal of Health and
(Eds.), Occupational stress: A handbook: 129-150. Social Behavior, 36: 11-37.
Washington, DG: Taylor & Francis. Lin, N., & Lai, G. 1995. Urban stress in Ghina. Social
Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Barnes, G. M. 1996. Work- Science and Medicine, 41: 1131-1145.
family conflict, gender, and health-related outcomes: Lobel, S. A. 1991. Allocation of investment in work and
A study of employed parents in two community family roles: Alternative theories and implications
samples. Journal of Occupational Health Psychol- for research. Academy of Management Beview, 16:
ogy, 1: 57-69. 507-521.
Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Gooper, M. L. 1997. Relation
Morris, M. W., & Leung, K. 2000. Justice for all? Progress
of work-family conflict to health outcomes: A four-
in research on cultural variation in the psychology of
year longitudinal study of employed parents. Jour-
nal of Occupational and Organizational Psychol- distributive and procedural justice. Applied Psy-
ogy, 70: 325-335. chology: An International Beview: In press.

Frone, M. R., Yardley, J. K., & Markel, K. S. 1997. Devel- Near, J. P., Rice, R. W., & Hunt, R. G. 1980. The relation-
oping and testing an integrative model of the work- ship between work and nonwork domains: A review
family interface. Journal of Vocational Behavior, of empirical research. Academy of Management
50: 145-167. Beview, 5: 415-429.
Greenhaus, J. H., & Beutell, N. J. 1985. Sources of conflict Parasuraman, S., Purohit, Y. S., Godshalk, V. M., & Beu-
between work and family roles. Academy of Man- tell, N. J. 1996. Work and family variables, entrepre-
agement Review, 10: 76-88. neurial career success, and psychological weW-
Greenhaus, J. H., & Parasuraman, S. 1994. Work-family heing. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 48: 275-300.
conflict, social support, and well-being. In M. J. Da- / Parsons, T. 1951. The social system. New York: Free
vidson & R. J. Burke (Eds.), Women in management: Press.
2000 Yang, Chen, Choi, and Zou 123

Redding, S. G. 1993. The spirit of Chinese capitahsm. Tung, R. 1991. Motivation in Ghinese industrial enter-
New York: de Gruyter. prises. In R. M. Steers & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Motiva-
Redding, G., & Wong, G. Y. Y. 1986. The psychology of tion and work behavior (5th ed.): 342-351. New
Chinese organizational behaviour. In M. H. Bond York: McGraw-Hill.
(Ed.), The psychology of the Chinese people: 267- Yang, N. 1993. Work-family conflict and supervisor sup-
295. New York: Oxford University Press. port. Eastern Academy of Management 1993 Pro-
Schein, E. H. 1984. Gulture as an environmental context ceedings: 174-177.
for careers. lournal of Occupational Behavior, 5:
71-81. Nini Yang is an associate professor of organization and
Schumacker, R. E., & Lomax, R. G. 1996. A beginner's human resource management at Glayton GoUege & State
guide to structural equation modeling. Mahwah, University of Georgia. She received her Ph.D. in organi-
NJ: Erlbaum. zational behavior and human resources from the State
University of New York at Buffalo. Her current studies
Schwartz, S. H. 1994. Beyond individualism/collectiv- focus on globalization and its impact on the U.S. market,
ism: New cultural dimensions of value. In U. Kim, the cultural relativity of employee motivation, work-fam-
H. C. Triandis, G. Kaptcibasi, S. G. Ghoi, & G. Yoon ily interfaces in culturally distinct societies, and the im-
(Eds.), Individualism and collectivism: Theory, pact of socioeconomic changes on life stress and career.
method, and applications: 85-122. Thousand Oaks,
GA: Sage. Ghao G. Ghen is an associate professor of organization
management at Rutgers University. He received his Ph.D.
Shenkar, O., & Ronen, S. 1987. Structure and importance in organizational behavior and human resources from the
of work goals among managers in the People's Re- State University of New York at Buffalo. His current
public of Ghina. Academy of Management lournal, research focuses on cross-cultural issues in distributive
30: 564-576. and procedural justice.
Singh, ). 1995. Measurement issues in cross-national re-
search, lournal of International Business Studies, Jaepil Ghoi is a doctoral candidate in organizational be-
26: 597-619. havior at Rutgers University. He received his M.B.A.
from Seoul National University. His current research fo-
Spector, P. E. 1975. Relationships of organizational frus- cuses on individualism-collectivism, organizational jus-
tration with reported behavioral reactions of em- tice, leadership, and diversity in organizations.
ployees, lournal of Applied Psychology, 60: 635-
637. Yimin Zou is an associate professor of management at
Nanjing University. He received his M.S. in quantitative
Triandis, H. G. 1995. Individualism and collectivism. economics from Nanjing University. His current research
Boulder, GO: Westview. interests include the management of township enter-
Tsui, A., & Farh, ). L. L. 1997. Where guanxi matters. prises in Ghina and international comparative manage-
Work and Occupations, 24: 56-79. ment.
View publication stats

You might also like