You are on page 1of 16

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 52:8 (2011), pp 819–833 doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02399.

Practitioner Review: Clinical applications of


attachment theory and research for infants and
young children
Charles H. Zeanah,1 Lisa J. Berlin,2 and Neil W. Boris1
1
Tulane University, New Orleans, LA, USA; 2Duke University, Durham, NC, USA

Study of attachment in the 1970s and 1980s focused with a specific figure and to do so in certain situa-
on operationalizing and validating many of the tenets tions, notably when frightened, tired or ill’ (Bowlby
of attachment theory articulated in Bowlby’s land- 1969/1982, p. 371). In contemporary use, attach-
mark trilogy, Attachment and Loss (Bowlby, 1982, ment refers to the infant’s or young child’s emotional
1973, 1980), robustly underscoring the central role connection to an adult caregiver – an attachment
of child to parent attachment in the child’s develop- figure – as inferred from the child’s tendency to turn
ment and mental health. Attachment theory and its selectively to that adult to increase proximity when
implications have long interested clinicians, though needing comfort, support, nurturance or protection.
determining how best to translate complex theoreti- Importantly, attachment behaviors are distinguished
cal constructs and research methods into the clinical from affiliative behavior or social engagement with
arena has been challenging. Nevertheless, well- others because they involve seeking proximity when
defined landmarks in early childhood attachment experiencing distress. According to Bowlby, the
are clinically useful, and the emergence of interven- attachment behavioral system operates in tandem
tions drawn from systematic research is promising. with the exploratory behavioral system, such that,
The purpose of this paper is to summarize salient when one is highly activated, the other is deactivated.
issues from attachment theory and research and In other words, if a child feels secure in the presence
discuss how these issues inform clinical work with of an attachment figure, the child’s motivation to
infants and young children. venture out and explore intensifies (see Figure 1). If
We recognize that there is a range of clinical set- the child becomes frightened or stressed, however,
tings in which child–parent attachment will be the child’s motivation to explore diminishes, and the
important. Likewise, among practitioners serving motivation to seek proximity intensifies.
young children and their families, there is a broad
range of familiarity with and expertise in attachment Development of attachment
principles and attachment-based treatment. We Human infants are born without being attached to
assert that all clinical services for young children any particular caregivers. Attachments develop and
and their families will be enhanced by providers’ emerge during the first few years of life in conjunc-
understanding of attachment theory and research. tion with predictable major biobehavioral shifts that
We further assert that in some clinical contexts occur at 2–3 months of age, 7–9 months of age,
understanding child–parent attachment is essential. 18–20 months of age and, less dramatically, at
We begin by reviewing developmental research on 12 months of age (Table 1). These shifts occur when
attachment to describe how attachments develop, qualitatively new behaviors and capacities appear
how individual differences in selective attachments for the first time. Between the shifts, it is possible to
manifest, and the characteristics of clinical disorders describe the emergence of infant attachment behav-
of attachment. Next, we turn to assessment of iors. These attachment benchmarks provide a refer-
attachment in clinical settings. Then, we describe ence for clinicians’ expectations for children of
selected specialized clinical contexts in which different ages.
assessing attachments are uniquely important. The developmental milestones in Table 1 are tied
Finally, we describe four interventions for young to cognitive rather than chronological ages, and they
children and their families, all of which are closely also may be affected to varying degrees by other
derived from attachment theory, supported by rig- conditions or circumstances affecting the child.
orous evaluations, and designed to support directly Interestingly, aberrant environmental conditions
the developing child–parent relationship. seem to impair the development of attachment more
than physical or neurological child abnormalities do.
Basics of attachment theory and research For example, in children being raised in institutions,
Defining attachment the main effect appears to be that attachments are
Bowlby defined attachment in young children as ‘a incompletely formed or even absent (Dobrova-Krol
strong disposition to seek proximity to and contact et al., in press; Zeanah et al., 2005). In contrast, a
meta-analysis drawing on assessments of attach-
Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared. ment in over 1,600 infants and their mothers found

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Published by Blackwell Publishing, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main St, Malden, MA 02148, USA
820 Charles H. Zeanah, Lisa J. Berlin, and Neil W. Boris

CIRCLE OF SECURITY
PARENT ATTENDING TO THE TODDLER‛S NEEDS

I need
you to

• Watch over me
Support My • Help me
Exploration • Enjoy with me
• Delight in me

I need
you to

• Protect me Welcome My
• Comfort me Coming To You
• Delight in me
• Organize my feelings

Figure 1 Circle of Security: parent attending to the toddler’s needs

Table 1 Development of attachment in infancy and early childhood

Biobehavioral shifts
and attachment
benchmarks during
periods between shifts Behavioral characteristics

First 2 months Infant has limited ability to discriminate among different caregivers; recognize mothers’ smell and
sound but no preference expressed.
2–3 month shift Emergence of social interaction, with increased eye to eye contact, social smiling and responsive cooing.
2–7 months Able to discriminate among different caregivers but no strong preferences expressed; comfortable with
many familiar and unfamiliar adults and intensely motivated to engage them.
7–9 month shift Emergence of selective attachment as evidenced by onset of stranger wariness (initial reticence) and
separation protest (distress in anticipation of separation from attachment figures).
9–18 months Hierarchy of attachment figures evident. Infant balances the need to explore and the need to seek
proximity; these become even more evident with independent ambulation emerging at approximately
12 months. Secure base behaviors (moving away from the caregiver to explore) and safe haven
behaviors (returning to the caregiver for comfort and support) both evident.
18–20 month shift Emergence of symbolic representation, including pretend play and language.
20–36 months Goal-corrected partnership in which the child becomes increasingly aware of conflicting goals with
others and for the need to negotiate, compromise and delay gratification.
36+ months Secure base and safe haven behaviors continue but behavioral manifestations become less evident
because of the child’s increased verbal skills. Internal representations of attachment more accessible
to observers through narrative doll play.

that intrinsic infants’ challenges, including deafness Once selective attachments have begun to emerge
and Down syndrome, played less of a role in the in the latter part of the first year of life, clinicians can
developing quality of the child’s attachment to the focus on the young child’s use of the attachment
mother than did mothers’ problems, including figure as a secure base from which to explore (illus-
affective disorders (van IJzendoorn, Goldberg, Kroo- trated by the top of the circle in Figure 1), and as a
nenberg, & Frenkel, 1992). Further, although chil- safe haven to whom to return in times of distress
dren with autism display aberrant behaviors and are (illustrated by the bottom of the circle in Figure 1).
at increased risk for disorganized attachments, it is Individual differences in the young child’s balance
clear that they form selective attachments (Rutgers between attachment behaviors and exploratory
et al., 2004). Though it is clear that infants’ devel- behaviors may vary with different caregivers, as
opmental abnormalities may lead to some aberrant these behaviors are believed to be emergent proper-
attachment behaviors, they do not appear to signif- ties of an infant’s interaction with particular adults.
icantly impede the actual formation of attachment, A laboratory paradigm, known as the Strange Situ-
as occurs in rearing environments characterized by ation Procedure (SSP), was developed to examine
extreme social privation. individual differences in the young child’s balance of

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Practitioner Review: Clinical applications of attachment theory and research for infants and young children 821

attachment and exploration while interacting with described more fully below. Using a slightly modified
an attachment figure and an unknown adult (Ains- SSP, researchers also have identified analogous
worth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978). classifications preschool children (Cassidy & Marvin,
1992 [see Table 2]).
Classifications of attachment in infants and young
Disorganized and other atypical patterns of attach-
children
ment. Among the attachment classifications in
Strange Situation Procedure and classifications of infancy, disorganized attachment (Main & Solomon,
attachment. Designed to assess the child’s bal- 1990) is particularly relevant to psychopathology
ancing of proximity-seeking and exploration, the SSP and clinical intervention. Disorganized, disoriented,
is the ‘gold standard’ for systematically identifying and frightened behaviors in infants during the SSP
patterns of infant–parent attachment. The SSP may appear as a temporary breakdown in an orga-
involves a series of interactions between a 12- to nized attachment strategy or as a complete lack of
20-month-old infant, a caregiver, and a female strategy for obtaining proximity to increase feelings
‘stranger.’ Two brief infant–caregiver separations are of security. As some children reach preschool age,
included as moderate stressors designed to activate disorganization is transformed into controlling/
the child’s need for caregiver support. Differences in punitive or solicitous/caregiving behaviors directed
how infants organize their attachment and explor- towards the parent, though in some high-risk pre-
atory behaviors, especially during reunion episodes, school samples, disorganized behaviors remain evi-
can be reliably classified as secure, avoidant, or dent (Smyke, Zeanah, Fox, Nelson, & Guthrie, 2010).
ambivalent/resistant (Ainsworth et al., 1978 [see In low-risk samples, disorganized attachments have
Table 2]). A fourth classification, disorganized, is a prevalence of about 15%, whereas in populations

Table 2 Classifications of attachment in infants, toddlers, and preschoolers according to the Strange Situation Procedure

Estimated
prevalence
Infant and toddler in low risk
classifications Reunion behavior samples

Secure Direct expression of distress elicited by separation, active comfort-seeking, 65%


resolution of distress, resumption of exploration.
Avoidant Minimal response to separation from the caregiver, though quality of exploration 20%
may diminish, ignoring or actively avoiding the caregiver on reunion.
Resistant Intense distress induced by separation, attempts to obtain comfort are limited, 10%
awkward or interrupted, little or incomplete resolution of distress on reunion, and
resistance of caregiver attempts to soothe.
Disorganized Anomalous reactions to caregiver that may include mixtures of rapid, incoherent 15%
sequences of proximity-seeking, avoidance or resistance, or fearful of the parent,
or other behaviors indicating failure to use caregiver as an attachment figure (e.g.,
trying to get out of the door, preferring the stranger to the caregiver or showing
intensely fearful responses in presence of caregiver).
Unclassifiable Minimal social engagement with caregiver or stranger, little to no evidence of Rare
proximity-seeking, avoidance or resistance directed to caregiver, minimal
emotional response to separation or reunion.
Preschool classifications
Secure Reconnection with parent through gaze, verbal interaction, greeting; often Unknown
invitation to joint activity.
Avoidant Avoidance conveyed through child’s orientation away from parent, distance, Unknown
neutral affect, proximity for ‘business’ purposes (e.g., fixing toys).
Dependent Ambivalence about contact. Helplessness, whiney, petulant or forced Unknown
over-brightness; also coyness or hesitant, seemingly shy behavior.
Disorganized Similar to infant D pattern if not covered by Insecure/Other behaviors (see below). Unknown
Controlling Developmentally inappropriate attempts by the child to control the behavior of the Unknown
parent, through punitive or solicitous, caregiving behavior.
Insecure/other Varies with subtypes (below): Rare
– A/C combination of avoidant and dependent patterns
– disengaged child mirrors parent’s lack of responsiveness
– inhibited/fearful child fears the parent: compulsively compliant behavior
– affectively dysregulated anxiety manifested as escalating ‘silly,’ ‘goofy,’ ‘hyper’ behavior.

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
822 Charles H. Zeanah, Lisa J. Berlin, and Neil W. Boris

of risk, the rates are much higher. For example, in a and SSP classifications of attachment define quali-
clinically referred sample of preschoolers the rate tatively different patterns. In more extreme rearing
was 32% (Greenberg, Speltz, DeKlyen, & Endriga, conditions, as noted above, such as social neglect or
1991), but in maltreated preschoolers the rate was institutional care, attachment may be seriously
55% (Cicchetti & Barnett, 1991). compromised or even absent. Attachment disorders
Increasing numbers and severity of risk factors describe a constellation of aberrant attachment
within parent or child appear to increase probability behaviors and other behavioral anomalies that are
of disorganized attachment (Cyr, Euser, Bakermans- defined as resulting from social neglect and depri-
Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2010; van IJzendo- vation. Obviously, no disorder of attachment can
orn et al., 1999). Infants classified as disorganized exist before a child forms selective attachments, so a
have been shown to be at increased risk for later developmental age of 9–10 months ought to be
externalizing behavior problems, post-traumatic required to make the diagnosis.
stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms, and dissociation Two clinical patterns of attachment disorders have
(Fearon, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn, been most commonly described: an emotionally
Lapsley, & Roisman, 2010). withdrawn/inhibited type and an indiscriminately
social/disinhibited type (Zeanah & Smyke, 2009). In
Clinical significance of SSP patterns of attach- the emotionally withdrawn/inhibited type, the child
ment. Classifications of attachment in infants, tod- exhibits limited or absent initiation or response
dlers, and preschoolers have proven enormously to social interactions with caregivers and aberrant
valuable for understanding early child–parent rela- social behaviors. In particular, when distressed, the
tionships. There is consistent evidence from high-risk child fails to seek or respond consistently to comfort
samples of a connection between insecure attach- from caregivers and exhibits emotion dysregulation.
ment during infancy and later internalizing and In the indiscriminately social/disinhibited type, the
externalizing problems (DeKlyen & Greenberg, 2008). child exhibits lack of social reticence with unfamiliar
Although links between insecure attachment and adults, failure to check back with caregivers in
child psychopathology have been observed less con- unfamiliar settings and a willingness to ‘go off’ with
sistently in low-risk families, in the NICHD Study of strangers. As the child becomes older, he/she
Early Child Care, a longitudinal multi-site study of exhibits intrusive and overly familiar behavior with
over 1,000 US low-risk children, avoidant and disor- strangers, including asking overly personal ques-
ganized attachment at 15 months predicted lower tions, violating personal space, or initiating physical
maternal ratings of social competence and higher contact without hesitation.
teacher ratings of internalizing and externalizing In DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association,
behavior problems from preschool through first 2000), the two patterns are considered subtypes of
grade, and infant attachment security appeared to reactive attachment disorder, that is, variations on a
serve a strong protective function, even when mater- unitary construct of disordered attachment. In con-
nal parenting quality declined over time (NICHD Early trast, in ICD-10 (World Health Organization, 1992),
Child Care Research Network, 2006). For longer-term the emotionally withdrawn/inhibited type is desig-
outcomes, associations with early attachment appear nated reactive attachment disorder, whereas the
increasingly less likely to be direct and more likely to indiscriminately social/disinhibited type is defined
operate through other relationships and social cog- as disinhibited attachment disorder. Recent reviews
nitions (Carlson, Sroufe, & Egeland, 2004; Gross- are consistent in concluding that the evidence favors
man, Grossman, & Waters, 2005). two distinct disorders because of differences in
Attachment patterns, however, should not be phenomenology, correlates, and response to inter-
confused with clinical diagnoses. Thus, there is no vention (Rutter, Kreppner, & Sonuga-Barke, 2009;
role for the SSP in clinical settings as a diagnostic Zeanah & Gleason, 2010; Zeanah & Smyke, 2009).
measure. From recent studies, it seems clear that signs of
To the extent that clinicians can understand what attachment disorders are rare to non-existent in low-
the SSP elicits, however, and how attachment secu- risk samples, still rare in higher-risk samples, but
rity and insecurity are identified, practice with readily identifiable in maltreated and institutional-
infants and young children will be enhanced. Thus, ized samples (Zeanah & Smyke, 2009). Recent
as will be described below, the major underlying research has demonstrated that these disorders
principles and observational practices of the SSP can often remit when caregiving conditions improve,
be applied to clinical work with infants and young though with variability for each type: the emotionally
children in ways that should significantly enhance withdrawn/inhibited type is more responsive to
the therapeutic work. intervention whereas the indiscriminately social/
disinhibited type is more resistant and may persist
in some children for years.
Disorders of attachment
The relationship between attachment disorders
In species-typical rearing conditions, virtually all and classifications of attachment is not straightfor-
children develop attachments to their caregivers, ward (Rutter, Kreppner, & Sonuga-Barke, 2009).

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Practitioner Review: Clinical applications of attachment theory and research for infants and young children 823

There is some tendency for more atypical classifica- (Miron, Lewis, & Zeanah, 2009), but procedures that
tions of attachment to be present when children have involve active elicitation of the child’s attachment
attachment disorders (Boris et al., 2004; Gleason behaviors are most useful. Reunion following a brief
et al., 2011; O’Connor et al., 2003; Zeanah et al., separation, whether as part of the SSP or some other
2005), but some children with high levels of indis- method, is often considered the most reliable elicitor
criminate behavior also demonstrate secure attach- of attachment behaviors. Separation activates the
ment (Gleason et al., 2011; O’Connor et al., 2003; child’s need for comfort, and during the reunion, the
Zeanah et al., 2005). caregiver must read and respond to the child at a
moment of intense emotional arousal. The clinician’s
task is carefully to analyze how the dyad resolves
Assessing attachment in clinical settings distress following separation or positively reconnects
Assessment of attachment involves a paradigmatic even in the absence of distress. Note that this does
shift in the clinical frame. Following the long tradi- not mean determining the SSP classification but
tion of medicine, psychiatric disorders are concep- instead noting how attachment behaviors are
tualized as existing within an individual, even if the organized within a particular relationship.
disorder has significant interpersonal manifesta- Especially in the context of a reunion, but also
tions (e.g., conduct disorder). The quality of the during naturalistic observations, there are several
caregiver–child attachment, on the other hand, aspects of child–parent interaction that merit close
requires shifting from considering clinical problems consideration. First, careful attention to presence or
as existing within an individual child to conceiving of absence of child behaviors that reflect proximity-
clinical problems (and strengths) existing between seeking, avoidance, resistance or disorganization in
caregiver and child. One major implication of this response to distress may be useful in highlighting
shift is recognizing that the same child may be dif- strengths and weaknesses in the parent–child rela-
ferentially attached to different caregivers. This shift tionship. Especially important is observing the
in the clinical frame from the individual to the dyad balance between the child’s exploring and proximity-
has implications for both assessment and treatment. seeking and the child’s use of the caregiver to
Two clinical approaches, derived from research, are regulate distress and other negative emotions (see
fundamental to adequate assessment of attachment Figure 1). Behaviors that are clinically salient
in young children: (a) a structured interaction be- include the child’s showing affection to the caregiver,
tween caregiver and child designed to examine the seeking closeness – especially when needing comfort,
way that the young child uses the caregiver to balance relying on the caregiver for help, and cooperating
between the need to explore and the need to seek with the caregiver (Boris, Aoki, & Zeanah, 1999).
physical closeness (see Figure 1), and (b) a narrative Familiarity with disorganized and other atypical
interview with the caregiver. As discussed below, attachment behaviors is of particular clinical value.
these approaches may be implemented through for- Disorganized attachment behaviors have been
mal and validated measures or less systematically described in some detail (Main & Solomon, 1990),
with attention to naturalistically observed behavior but training from videotapes is a useful adjunct to
and narrative qualities in describing the child. Such written descriptions. In preschoolers, controlling
approaches are likely to be significantly more useful behaviors – whether evident as solicitous caregiving
when they are videotaped so that clinicians and cli- or as being bossy and punitive – should be noted.
nicians and parents can review them later. Though The ‘insecure/other’ behaviors described in Table 3
not available in all clinical settings, videotaping is are also important indicators of disturbed attach-
increasingly central to work with young children and ment relationships.
their families (Miron, Lewis, & Zeanah, 2009). Co-occurrence of clinical problems of a variety of
Video review with parents is valuable because it types with disorganized attachment behaviors may
augments and facilitates reflective functioning, that increase urgency about intervening to change the
is, parents’ ability to notice and reflect upon their child–parent relationship. Here, efforts to increase
own and their infants’ behavior. Increasingly, parental behaviors associated with child security,
reflective functioning is recognized as a component such as sensitive caregiving (De Wolff & van IJzen-
of the parent’s ability to function as a secure base doorn, 1997) and reflective functioning (Slade,
and safe haven for the child (Slade, Grienenberger, Grienenberger, Bernbach, Levy, & Locker, 2005),
Bernbach, Levy, & Locker, 2005). As a result, may be important clinical efforts. Problematic
reviewing videotaped interactions with parents also parental behaviors such as frightening or frightened
is a central component of most manualized attach- behaviors, which have been linked both to trauma
ment-based interventions, reviewed in detail later. and to disorganized attachment (Schuengel et al.,
1999), are important to identify. These behaviors
could in turn be targeted through specific attach-
Assessing caregiver–child interaction
ment-based interventions (described in more detail
There are various interactional assessments that below) and/or through related modalities already
may be used to elicit attachment-relevant behaviors practiced successfully by the clinician.

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
824 Charles H. Zeanah, Lisa J. Berlin, and Neil W. Boris

Table 3 Atypical parenting behaviors associated with disorganized attachment (adapted from Benoit, 2000, and based on the
Atypical Maternal Behavior Instrument for Assessment and Classification Scale) (Lyons-Ruth & Jacovitz, 2008)

Atypical parenting behaviors Examples

Withdrawal Creating physical distance – directing the child away with toys; not greeting on reunion
Fearful behaviors Frightened/hesitant/uncertain – haunted or high-pitched voice, sudden mood change,
dissociation/’going through the motions’
Role confusion Pleading with the child, threatening to cry, hushed tones, speaking as if the child were an
adult partner
Affective communication errors Contradictory signals – inviting approach, then directing child away; using a positive voice,
but teasing or putting the child down; laughing at child’s distress and/or expressing distress
in response to the child’s positive affect
Intrusiveness/Negativity Mocking, teasing, derogating; withholding a toy; pushing the child away; hushing the child

Given considerable evidence that attachment (e.g., the parent laughs at the child’s upset, or acts
classifications can differ with different caregivers upset when the child expresses joy).
(Fearon et al., 2010), relationship-specific informa- Of course, careful observation of the interaction
tion about how attachment behaviors are organized between caregiver and child in isolation reveals little
may impact how interventions are designed. to the clinician about what underlies the caregiver’s
Throughout observation, clinicians should expect difficulty in meeting his or her child’s needs. Bowlby
the child’s attachment behaviors to be organized (1969/1982) posited, and subsequent research has
towards the attachment figure (i.e., parent or care- confirmed (van IJzendoorn, 1995), that each care-
giver) and to contrast sharply with the child’s giver’s interactive behavior with the child is influ-
behavior with an unfamiliar adult (usually the cli- enced largely by their own ‘internal working models’
nician). Any direction of the child’s attachment of attachment, forged in part by their own early
behaviors directly towards the unfamiliar adult family experiences.
indicates a significant disturbance. Similarly, par-
ent–child attachment relationships are asymmetri-
Assessing the caregiver’s narrative
cal, meaning that it is the parent’s job to provide
comfort, support, nurturance and protection to the The Adult Attachment Interview (AAI) developed by
child but not the other way around. Indications of Main and colleagues heralded ‘a move to the level of
abdication of the parental role are clinically signifi- representation’ in attachment research (Main, Kap-
cant, as are indications that the child is the one lan, & Cassidy, 1985). Main and colleagues argued
structuring the interaction with the parent and/or that the AAI assessed internal representations of
treating the parent in a caregiving manner. attachment, indicating different ‘states of mind’ with
Observations should be supplemented by detailed respect to attachment. This includes patterns of
interview questions about typical behaviors that are emotion regulation, attachment information pro-
clinically salient to attachment. In the latter part of cessing and the degree to which attachment is val-
the first year, these would involve stranger wariness ued (Steele & Steele, 2008).
and separation protest, and then in the second year The major advance provided by the AAI was to
would include behaviors describing the child’s bal- analyze patterns of narrative discourse about early
ance between, and comfort with, exploratory behav- childhood relationship experiences. Instead of
ior and proximity-seeking. Use of the caregiver to merely accepting reports of childhood relationship
regulate distress and other negative emotions, experiences at face value, this approach evaluates
seeking comfort and closeness, particularly when the reports in terms of well-anchored qualitative
distressed, showing affection to the caregiver, relying features of the narrative. Some adults, classified as
on the caregiver for help, and cooperating with the ‘autonomous,’ relate organized, emotionally inte-
caregiver are all important (Boris et al., 1999). grated narratives about their experiences and in
On the caregiver side, there are also behaviors which they clearly value attachment relationships.
especially important to note. Because of the link with Those classified as ‘dismissing’ minimize the impor-
disorganized attachment, frightening or frightened tance of attachment experiences often while idealiz-
behavior by the caregiver are especially notable ing their childhood experiences, yet at the same time
(Hesse, 2008). In addition, other behaviors reflecting failing to provide credible memories to support their
emotionally disrupted communications have been descriptions. Narratives in which the adult provides
described by Lyons-Ruth and colleagues (Lyons- incoherent, affect-laden stories without an overall,
Ruth, Bureau, Riley, & Atlas-Corbett, 2009) and are integrated perspective are classified as ‘preoccupied.’
linked to disorganized attachment ([Table 3] see also Finally, narratives in which the adult has lapses in
Benoit, 2000). These behaviors include overt and coherence when discussing traumatic experiences or
covert hostility, withdrawal, and mismatched affect losses are classified as ‘unresolved.’ A rarer group,

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Practitioner Review: Clinical applications of attachment theory and research for infants and young children 825

lacking sufficient indicators of the preceding, is cations (Zeanah et al., 1994), even when adminis-
‘cannot classify’ (see Hesse, 2008, for a comprehen- tered before the child is born (Benoit et al., 1997).
sive review). A meta-analysis of more than 800 dyads These and other narrative interviews have been
found significant concordance (70%), as predicted, used in both evaluation and treatment planning.
between classification of infant–parent attachment Narrative interviews take approximately one hour to
patterns from the SSP and classification of parental conduct and, like the interactive piece of the assess-
state of mind regarding attachment from the AAI (van ment, are best video recorded for later review. In some
IJzendoorn, 1995). clinical settings, interviews about caregivers’ child-
As with the SSP, actual attachment classifications hood experiences may be desirable and illuminating.
from the AAI are less important in clinical settings In others, such as settings in which the child is the
than the behaviors – in this case, narrative qualities clinical focus, parents may find questioning about
– underlying them, which are often quite usefully their own childhood objectionable, and interviews
noted. Steele and Steele (2008) detailed a number of about the child may be better tolerated. The Circle of
ways in which the AAI may be useful in clinical set- Security Interview (Powell, Cooper, Hoffman, & Mar-
tings. Among others, they noted the AAI may be vin, 2009) is a hybrid that includes probes about both
useful in helping to set the agenda for psychother- the parent’s childhood relationships and relation-
apy, uncovering traumas or losses, delineating ships with the child. Narrative features of interviews
defensive processes, assessing reflective functioning, about the caregiver’s relationship with the child that
and identifying positive relationship models from are clinically salient are presented in Table 4.
which to build. Specific uses of the interview will vary Importantly, it is not necessary to obtain training
with the setting and the specific clinical issues being to reliability in these measures to find them clinically
addressed. valuable, especially since an overall classification is
Following the AAI, other narrative interviews were not necessarily in and of itself illuminating. Fur-
developed. For example, rather than focus on the thermore, in clinical use, deviation from strict
caregiver’s past experiences with caregiving rela- research administration protocols is often indicated
tionships, interviews like the Working Model of the in order to pursue more details about important
Child Interview (WMCI; Zeanah et al., 1994) and the issues. Nevertheless, detailed understanding of
Parent Development Interview (Slade et al., 1999) narrative features and how they relate to emotion
focus on the caregiver’s perceptions of the child and regulation in parent and child means that training in
their relationship with their child. The WMCI also administration and interpretation of a particular
has been shown to be concordant with SSP classifi- interview will enhance its usefulness for the clini-

Table 4 Clinically relevant features of narrative attachment interviews

Narrative feature Examples of areas of clinical concern

Attributions about the child • Instances of derogation – direct expression of anger about the child’s needs – are noted.
• The narrative is marked by distortion, such that the child’s needs are characterized as hurtful,
frightening, or overwhelming.
• The caregiver is generically positive about the (e.g., ‘loving’ or ‘caring’) and yet the story of the
relationship is characterized by extreme disengagement such that the caregiver struggles to find
language to talk about the child infant’s needs and seems disinterested.
• The caregiver talks about the child as a friend, peer or confidant.
• The caregiver talks about the child in an impersonal way – without showing understanding/
appreciation of the child as a unique individual.
The tone of the interview • Indifference.
• Moments of significant anger/hostility or thematic anger.
• Notable guilt, shame, or disappointment.
The capacity to reflect on • Little capacity to imagine what the child needs or feels.
the child’s experience • Salient developmental issues like regulating sleep or crying or toilet training are described as
(i.e., reflective functioning, burdensome, overwhelming or not described at all.
empathy and insight) • Challenges with regulation are blamed, at least in part, on the infant though significant guilt may
be expressed.
Memories about the child • Limited.
• Lack detail, positive emotion and richness.
• Contradictory such that, for example, specific memories of the child described as loving focus
instead on the child being manipulative or difficult.
Psychological defenses/ • Moments of confusion or even dissociation.
trauma • Previous experiences of loss or trauma are talked about even if not clearly related to the interview
question.
• Increasing irritation with the interviewer.
• Questions are challenged or criticized or interview process is demeaned.

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
826 Charles H. Zeanah, Lisa J. Berlin, and Neil W. Boris

cian. Finally, for less experienced clinicians the Barriers to the formation of attachments and
structure afforded by these interviews may be valu- particularly secure attachments may come from
able if it does not become overly rigid. parent or child or from the unique fit between the
There is, however, a significant cost in time, both two. Stovall-McClough and Dozier (2004) reported
in conducting interactive assessments and narrative that children placed prior to 10 months of age
interviews, and in being trained to interpret the data readily developed secure attachments to their foster
provided by such measures. Still, these measures parents, but those placed after 10 months were more
may be clinically valuable whether used alone or as likely to exhibit insecure behavior when distressed.
part of attachment-based interventions (Steele & Importantly, they noted that when children avoided
Steele, 2008; Zeanah & Benoit, 1995). their foster parents, the foster parents responded
with withdrawal, and when children exhibited
resistance in the form of difficulty settling once dis-
Selected clinical contexts tressed, foster parents responded with impatience
Although we suggest that attachment ought to be a and irritability. These cycles can be identified soon
prominent component of assessment of young chil- after placement and should be the target of clinical
dren in any clinical setting, there are certain situa- intervention.
tions in which attachment assessment should have a Clearly, clinical intervention should begin with
central role. We next turn to a discussion of appli- helping foster parents understand children’s
cations of attachment theory and research in four attachment and exploratory needs, but also with
specific clinical contexts. teaching foster parents that traumatized children
often mislead caregivers about what they need.
Caregivers, thus, must override their own problem-
Maltreatment and foster care
atic reactions to challenging behaviors (Dozier et al.,
Attachment disturbances are inherent in foster care 2005). In fact, foster parents’ own state of mind with
for several reasons. First, young children are dis- respect to attachment has been identified as the
proportionately likely to be classified as disorganized strongest predictor of whether foster children will
in relation to their maltreating parents, with rates of become securely attached to them (Dozier et al.,
disorganization found to be as high as 90% (Cicchetti 2001).
et al., 2006). Second, as noted, maltreatment, and Another critical component is foster parents’
specifically neglect, are necessary but not sufficient commitment to the children in their care. Young
as etiologic conditions for attachment disorders children have an urgent need for regular and sub-
(Zeanah & Smyke, 2009). Third, maltreated children stantial contact with their attachment figures, and it
who are removed from their primary caregivers and is rarely possible for biological parents to serve this
placed with foster parents they often have never seen function even with frequent visitation. Dozier (2007)
before must form attachments to entirely new care- found that greater commitment by foster parents to
givers. Finally, these children must attempt to the young child in their care was associated with
resolve and/or repair attachments to their biological fewer placement disruptions. Parental commitment
parents even as they develop new attachments to also is salient in work with post-institutionalized,
foster parents. adopted children, to which we turn next.
An initial question for clinicians evaluating young
children in foster care is whether they are attached to
Post-institutionalized and inter-country adopted
anyone. For example, one study found that about a
children
third of 1- to 4-year-old children removed from their
parents and placed in foster care had limited or no Attachment disturbances are among the most
attachments three months after their removal prominent problems noted in post-institutionalized
(Zeanah et al., 2004). Dozier and colleagues (Stovall- children and among those who are adopted into
McClough & Dozier, 2004) had foster parents com- another country (Zeanah, 2000). In addition to
plete structured daily diaries to describe the nascent reduced security and increased prevalence of
attachment behaviors of the young children in their atypical classifications (e.g., insecure/other) in these
care. They noted that attachments began to organize children, high levels of indiscriminant behavior are
as secure, avoidant or resistant patterns within days quite common. Even after children have established
to weeks of placement. Thus, attention to develop- attachments in their adoptive families, some con-
ment and quality of attachment to foster parents tinue to exhibit high levels of indiscriminate behavior
should be monitored from the outset. Important (Rutter et al., 2007). In studies to date, children who
questions for the clinician include: Does the child are placed in families following early institutional
consistently turn to preferred attachment figures for rearing develop more secure and less atypical
comfort, support, nurturance and protection? Does patterns of attachment following enhanced caregiv-
the child directly express negative emotions? Is the ing. For example, the Bucharest Early Intervention
child convincingly comforted when seeking it from Project (BEIP) included careful assessment of
preferred attachment figures? attachment of young children being raised in insti-

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Practitioner Review: Clinical applications of attachment theory and research for infants and young children 827

tutions (Zeanah et al., 2005). Following this assess- a full commitment to the children. Also, parents’ own
ment, children were randomly assigned to foster care attachment histories (and interpretations of their
or to ‘care as usual.’ In this study, the intervention histories) are likely to be important for informing
was an attachment-based model of child-centered their parenting behaviors, suggesting that clinicians
foster care, and a team of Romanian social workers ought to help them increase reflective capacity and
trained and supported foster parents in managing understanding of both their children’s behaviors and
the complex challenges of caring for post-institu- their own responses to them. Increasing parents’
tionalized infants and toddlers (Smyke et al., 2009). reflective capacities is a central feature of interven-
Paralleling Dozier’s emphasis on commitment tion efforts, including those with children experi-
(Lindheim & Dozier, 2007), these foster parents were encing parental divorce.
urged by their Romanian social workers to commit
fully to the children in their care and to love them as
Divorce and child custody
their own. Children were placed between the ages of
6 and 30 months, and through 54 months of age, There is widespread consensus about the relevance
only two placements disrupted (Smyke et al., 2009). of attachment theory, research, and assessments to
By 42 months of age, secure attachments were sig- child custody decision-making required when par-
nificantly increased and atypical (disorganized and ents divorce, including legal custody, physical cus-
insecure other) attachments were significantly tody, daily and overnight visitation, and relocation
decreased in children in foster care compared to (see Byrne, O’Connor, Marvin, & Whelan, 2005, for a
children who experienced more prolonged care in comprehensive review).
institutions (Smyke et al., 2010). Furthermore, Although it is hard to determine the proportion of
recent data from this group indicate that, for girls these decisions that require a court hearing and/or
(but not boys), security of attachment fully mediated other forms of dispute resolution (e.g., mediation), it
the effect of the intervention on internalizing disor- is likely that those that do are more complex and
ders (McLaughlin et al., under review). Timing of contentious, and can include accusations by one
removal from institutional rearing also matters, as a parent concerning the other’s suitability to parent
recent meta-analysis indicated that for children in their child. Exactly how best to apply attachment
adoptive homes for an average of 26 months, those theory and research to custody decisions is not well
who were adopted before 12 months of age were determined, however.
significantly more likely to be securely attached than Recent thoughtful and comprehensive reviews
those adopted after (Van den Dries, Juffer, van have recommended that principles of attachment
IJzendoorn, & Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2009). theory and well-validated measures of attachment
What is less clear from adoption studies and the security can help to inform custody evaluations
BEIP is which parental behaviors and characteristics (Byrne et al., 2005; Calloway & Erard, 2009). At the
most strongly promote secure attachments in chil- same time, standard attachment measures such as
dren with histories of institutional rearing, although the SSP have not yet been widely used nor evaluated
this is a current area of investigation. Until more in the context of divorce and custody litigation. The
data are available, clinicians ought to be guided by resources required to use standard attachment
the findings derived from studies of maltreated assessments may be prohibitive in the context of
children. For example, Dozier et al. (2001) showed divorce and custody litigation. Moreover, even if
that infants who had experienced serious neglect affordable and well implemented, it is not clear how
could form secure attachments to their new care- assessments of attachment security should be
givers, but this was far more likely if their mothers brought to bear on custody decisions (Garber, 2009).
had organized (i.e., secure) responses during the That is, best interest of the child determinations
AAI. Similarly, a study of 4- to 7-year-old children should be informed by more factors than attachment
who had experienced multiple foster placements and alone, unless serious disturbances of attachment
who had had disorganized attachments to their bio with either parent are evident. Ideally, young chil-
parent became securely attached in their new dren can enjoy the benefits of regular contact and
placements if they were placed with an adoptive sustained relationships with both parents and other
parent who was classified as secure according to the family members through the inevitable stressors
AAI (Steele et al., 2008). accompanying divorce.
These findings from maltreated children (Dozier et Sometimes, because of conflicts or logistics, this
al., 2002; Steele et al., 2008) and what evidence will not be possible, however. Specific issues of
there is from studies of post-institutionalized chil- issues of separation and loss are especially impor-
dren (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., in press) con- tant in the context of divorce. When parents do not
verge on several key clinical issues. Parents adopting co-reside and/or rarely share time together as a
post-institutionalized children must be encouraged family, young children who have been attached to
to learn what children need regardless of the off- both parents may have chronic feelings of loss and
putting signals the children may transmit. Clinicians longing to be with both attachment figures at once.
should also identify impediments to parents’ making The question is how best to manage these situations.

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
828 Charles H. Zeanah, Lisa J. Berlin, and Neil W. Boris

Once attachment to a parent develops, usually at a helping families recover from exposure to partner
cognitive age of 7 to 9 months, any visitation with the violence, establishing a safe and secure living situ-
non-custodial parent most likely will involve sepa- ation is the most important initial consideration. The
ration from the primary attachment figure. Sustain- clinician should work to make the physical envi-
ing attachments to different adults living apart may ronment as safe as possible: re-establishing routines
not be developmentally possible for young children, and rituals for the child, restoring interpersonal
and separations such as overnight visitations for connections, and highlighting the fact that protec-
children younger than 2 or 3 years may not only tion of the child’s needs are part of this effort. Chil-
harm the primary attachment relationship, they can dren who are constantly afraid about the wellbeing of
also make establishing a healthy attachment to the a traumatized caregiver can become overly con-
non-custodial parent more difficult (see Solomon & cerned about the caregiver’s emotional wellbeing;
George, 1999). For this reason, in children younger this means that emphasizing protection includes
than 2 or 3 years old, extended visits with the non- restoring or establishing caregiver limit-setting and
custodial parent may best be accomplished if the developing a treatment plan to address caregiver–
custodial parent is present, assuming that parents child role reversal when it is apparent. Partner vio-
can be together without conflict. For preschool chil- lence is a potent risk factor for disorganized attach-
dren, who have a greater capacity to sustain ment and/or role-reversed relationship disturbances
attachments over time and distance, sustaining (Macfie et al., 2008). Attachment-based interven-
attachments to both parents may be possible. tions are often warranted when exposure to partner
Child comfort may be increased if parents can violence is prolonged (Lieberman et al., 2006).
maintain the child’s routines for sleeping/waking,
feeding, and activities in both settings. Child comfort
will also be increased by both parents allowing the Attachment-based interventions
child to use the same transitional object (e.g., A number of early childhood interventions are com-
favorite stuffed animal) when with both of them, and patible with or derived from Bowlby’s work (see
by their following similar rules for the use of the Berlin, Zeanah, & Lieberman, 2008, for a compre-
child’s bottle and/or pacifier. Similarly, although hensive review). We consider four examples of
often a source of parental conflict, both parents’ use interventions that derived in whole or in part from
of the same or similar parenting strategies with attachment theory and research. All of these thera-
respect to emotional soothing and limit-setting will pies have an evidence base supporting them, as well
provide a more consistent and coherent child-rearing as specific training protocols or processes through
environment, as a whole. For older toddlers and which clinicians may train to fidelity in one or more
preschool-aged children, consistency in parents’ of these approaches. Even if not training to research
narratives about why they are apart should also be reliability, clinicians will likely enhance their prac-
helpful. tices by becoming familiar with the detailed nuances
High levels of parental conflict are likely to in- of implementing these interventions. It is important
crease the probability of attachment disturbances in to note that these interventions are not employed
young children, perhaps through reduced sensitivity only in the case of attachment disorders, but rather
in parents who are preoccupied by their own con- to support the development of child–parent attach-
cerns. When high levels of conflict, including vio- ment in high-risk conditions and/or to address
lence, are evident, the threats to attachment concerning child–parent interactions and relation-
relationships are even greater. ship disturbances.

Families with intimate partner violence Child–Parent Psychotherapy (CPP)


Partner violence is considered both a reflection of The oldest dyadic psychotherapy that shared the
and a risk for disorganized attachment (Lyons-Ruth relational focus of attachment theory was Fraiberg’s
& Jacobvitz, 2008). As Kobak and Madsen (2008) Infant Parent Psychotherapy (Fraiberg, 1980), since
note, partner violence is likely to undermine the refined and extended to serve infants and preschool
child’s confidence in the parent’s availability and is a children by Lieberman and colleagues (Lieberman &
context in which the child is likely to be frightened of Van Horn, 2005). Now called Child–Parent Psycho-
and for the parent. Therefore, it is not surprising that therapy (CPP), this manualized dyadic intervention
within a group of impoverished young mothers, has been used primarily with impoverished and
partner violence was significantly associated with traumatized families with children younger than 5.
disorganized infant attachment (Zeanah et al., CPP sessions include both parent and child together
1999). and take place either in the home or in an office
Lieberman and colleagues have noted that the playroom. Sessions are unstructured, with the
effects of partner violence on young children and themes largely determined by the parent and by the
their relationships may be lasting (Lieberman, unfolding interactions between the parent and child.
Ghosh Ippen, & Van Horn, 2006). For clinicians CPP emphasizes enhancing emotional communica-

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Practitioner Review: Clinical applications of attachment theory and research for infants and young children 829

tion between parent and child, sometimes by et al., 2002). Originally created as a time-limited
exploring links between the parents’ early childhood group psychotherapy using video feedback, the
experiences and their current feelings, perceptions, intervention is easily adapted for individual therapy.
and behaviors towards their children, as well as a More recently, parenting DVDs based on the model
focus on the parent’s current stressful life circum- have been created; these combine education about
stances and culturally derived values. This emphasis attachment with an opportunity for caregivers to
on emotional communication, and defensive pro- reflect on their child’s needs and the challenges each
cesses that threaten to distort it, highlights the link faces in meeting those needs (Cooper, Hoffman, &
between attachment theory and CPP. Powell, 2009). For the video-therapy approach, an
Five randomized clinical trials support the efficacy observational procedure including the standard SSP
of CPP. These include infants of stressed, immigrant and the Circle of Security Interview are completed at
families (Lieberman, Weston, & Pawl, 1991), infants baseline for treatment planning (Powell et al., 2009).
and preschoolers from maltreating families (Cicch- Toward the end of treatment, a second SSP is con-
etti, Rogosch, & Toth, 2006; Toth, Maughan, Manly, ducted for additional review. In this treatment, the
Spagnola, & Cicchetti, 2002), toddlers of depressed SSP is used to not to classify attachment patterns,
mothers (Cicchetti, Toth, & Rogosch, 1999; Toth, but rather to illustrate the parent’s emotional pres-
Rogosch, Manly, & Cicchetti, 2006), and preschool- ence and the child’s exploratory and comfort-seeking
ers exposed to partner violence (Lieberman et al., behavior (see Figure 1).
2006). In each, a central component of the therapy is The intervention emphasizes the sophisticated
establishing open expression of the child’s experi- capabilities of young children and draws caregivers’
ence of distress and a need for the parent to respond attention to the meaning of subtle behaviors, using
effectively, much as is seen in secure attachment video review liberally to highlight positive moments
relationships. of parent–child interaction in order to engage the
caregiver. This approach builds upon the work of
McDonough (McDonough, 2000; Rusconi-Serpa
Video-based Intervention to Promote Positive
et al., 2009) and Shaver and colleagues (Mikulincer
Parenting (VIPP)
& Shaver, 2007), both of whom demonstrated that
Video-based Intervention to Promote Positive Par- images of positive interactions effectively engage
enting (VIPP) is a brief, home-based attachment difficult-to-reach parents and build unity in groups.
intervention delivered in four home visits to parents Another key element of the COS approach is edu-
of infants less than 1 year old, typically in lower-risk cation of caregivers about attachment and its
families (Juffer, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van development. Once again, review of video material is
IJzendoorn, 2007). VIPP was drawn explicitly from central. The Circle (Figure 1) is provided as a visual
attachment research and attempts to promote guide for parents to learn about young children’s
maternal sensitivity through interveners’ presenta- attachment needs and necessary caregiver behav-
tion of written materials and review of in-home, iors, including supportive presence (‘Hands’), sup-
videotaped infant–parent interactions. An expanded port for exploration (‘top of the Circle’), and support
version, VIPP-R, provides an additional three-hour for closeness (‘bottom of the Circle’). The intervention
home visit that focuses on the parent’s childhood aims to help parents better understand their child’s
attachment experiences. attachment needs and to recognize when their own
Findings are generally supportive of positive pro- reactions impede an appropriate response.
gram effects on sensitive and nurturing parenting There are preliminary data on the efficacy of the
behaviors, attachment disorganization, and exter- Circle of Security intervention. A pre–post study of
nalizing behavior problems during preschool (Juffer, impoverished parents showed significant changes in
Bakermans-Kranenburg, & van IJzendoorn, 2005; the proportion of securely attached toddlers. A study
Klein Velderman, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Juffer, & of a perinatal Circle of Security intervention, pro-
van IJzendoorn, 2006a; Klein Velderman et al., vided to pregnant, nonviolent offenders with a his-
2006b). VIPP-SD, a version of VIPP emphasizing tory of substance abuse, demonstrated rates of
sensitive disciplinary practices for toddlers with infant attachment security (70%) and disorganiza-
early signs of externalizing problems, has shown tion (20%) comparable to those in low-risk samples
positive effects on mothers’ disciplinary attitudes (Cassidy et al., 2010). Although no randomized,
and behaviors compared to control mothers (Mes- controlled trial has been conducted, COS is tightly
man et al, 2007; van Zeijl et al., 2006). linked to attachment theory and research and, in its
use of video feedback and promotion of caregiver
reflection, leverages emerging best practices.
The Circle of Security (COS)
The Circle of Security was developed as an inter-
Attachment and Biobehavioral Catch-up (ABC)
vention to enhance attachment relationships
between infants and young children and their care- Dozier and her colleagues developed a 10-session
givers, primarily through work with parents (Marvin intervention aimed at reducing barriers to the

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
830 Charles H. Zeanah, Lisa J. Berlin, and Neil W. Boris

development of secure attachment relationships ple, have effectively rewritten the criteria for attach-
between foster parents and the young children in ment disorders and created a series of loosely related
their care (Dozier et al., 2005). They note four major interventions which are coercive and dangerous (see
areas of challenge facing caregivers who foster young Chaffin et al., 2006, for a review and recommenda-
children: (a) young children in foster care may reject tions). Treatments which force children to submit to
care that is offered to them, (b) caregivers’ own being held or to sustain eye contact, promote
histories may interfere with their providing nurtur- regression to achieve ‘reattachment,’ or encourage
ing care, (c) young children in foster care may need children to ‘vent anger’ while being restrained are not
special help with self-regulation, and (d) young only contraindicated, but such treatments have led
maltreated children may be especially sensitive to to injury and even death (Mercer, Sarner, & Rosa,
frightening behavior in caregivers. 2003).
These premises were used to develop the ABC
program’s four treatment modules: (a) parental
nurturance, (b) following the child’s lead, (c) ‘over- Conclusions
riding’ one’s own history and/or non-nurturing Developmental research has established parent–
impulses, particularly as regards off-putting behav- child attachment as a central aspect of social and
ior and negative emotional reactions in children, and emotional development. Established landmarks
(d) avoiding frightening behavior with the child. detailing the emergence of attachment in children
Carefully selected videotaped examples drawn from during the first few years of life describe an expected
interactions between the foster parent and foster developmental trajectory against which an individ-
child are used to discuss and develop these themes. ual infant may be compared. In addition, there are
In addition, live interactions between parent and clearly defined phenotypes associated with extreme
child are included as part of the therapeutic focus. disturbances, including attachment disorders. In
A randomized controlled trial of the ABC inter- most clinical settings, young children’s attachment
vention with foster parents compared it to a psy- should be assessed routinely, either formally or
choeducational intervention of comparable intensity informally. Moreover, there are many clinical set-
and frequency in 60 children between 3 months and tings in which a thorough knowledge about attach-
3 years of age and their foster parents (Dozier et al., ment and its manifestations ought to be a central
2006b). Initial results demonstrated normalization focus, such as children who are maltreated (espe-
in diurnal cortisol levels in 93 foster children cially those in foster care), adopted children (espe-
between the ages of 3 months and 3 years (Dozier cially post-institutionalized), children of divorce, and
et al., 2006a; Dozier, Peloso, Lewis, Laurenceau, & children exposed to parental violence. Information
Levine, 2008). A subset of 46 children whose parents about the development of attachment in early
had received the ABC intervention showed signifi- childhood is widely available, though it is our
cantly less avoidance than children of parents in the impression that many training programs which lack
psychoeducational intervention (Dozier et al., 2009). infant mental health expertise do not adequately
The ABC program has been adapted and is being cover this material or its applications. For this rea-
evaluated with birth parents whose young children son we have emphasized throughout that thorough
have been maltreated but not removed from them. understanding of attachment and its developmental
The approach is also now being used with inter- course is essential in all clinical settings serving
country adopted children. That nearly identical young children and their families. More specialized
interventions are applied to foster parents, mal- attachment expertise will likely enhance clinical
treating parents, and adoptive parents speaks to the work with young children and their families.
fundamental attachment needs in young children. The relational paradigm that attachment research
has informed now dominates the interdisciplinary
field of infant mental health. In working with young
Contraindicated interventions claiming attachment
children and caregivers, clinicians have been pro-
derivation
vided methods for assessing both external,
Despite the recent development of well-validated observable interactive behaviors between caregiver
interventions to improve child–parent attachment, and child, and internal, subjective experiences of
there are other clinical interventions which have caregivers about their child. Understanding the
been developed with the goal of treating children who internal and external components of the relationship
have a history of disruptions in early attachment allows clinicians to shift the clinical frame from an
who also have developed oppositional or aggressive individual child to a child developing in the context
behavior. Although early traumatic experiences of caregiving relationships. Though available, these
coupled with disruptions in attachment can be methods require some training to master and may
associated with aggressive behavior in affected chil- require additional time and resources to implement
dren (Heller et al., 2006), proponents of the various meaningfully. Our view is that these additional
treatments designed to promote ‘reattachment,’ investments are amply rewarded by the richness of
through coercive holding and rebirthing, for exam- the data they provide.

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Practitioner Review: Clinical applications of attachment theory and research for infants and young children 831

Finally, there is a growing evidence base about Byrne, J.G., O’Connor, T.G., Marvin, R.S., & Whelan, W.F.
effective attachment derived/compatible early (2005). Practitioner review: The contribution of attachment
theory to child custody assessments. Journal of Child
childhood interventions that have been shown to be
Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 115–127.
effective. They share an emphasis on enhancing Calloway, G., & Erard, R.E. (2009). Introduction to the special
caregivers’ appreciation of the complexity of the issue on attachment and child custody. Journal of Child
emotional development of young children, the power Custody, 6, 1–7.
caregivers have to affect their children, and the kinds Carlson, E.A., Sroufe, L.A., & Egeland, B.E. (2004). The
construction of experience: A longitudinal of representation
of factors within and around the caregiver that may
and behavior. Child Development, 75, 66–83.
interfere with providing what young children need. Cassidy, J., Ziv, Y., Stupica, B., Sherman, L.J., Butler, H.,
For clinicians who work regularly with young chil- Karfgin, A., Cooper, G., Hoffman, K.T. & Powell, B. (2010).
dren and their families, these approaches also Enhancing attachment security in the infants of women in a
demand much but offer more to those willing to jail-diversion program. Attachment and Human Develop-
ment, 12, 333–353.
invest in their mastery and implementation.
Cassidy, J., Marvin, R.S., & The MacArthur Working Group on
Attachment. (1992). Attachment organization in preschool
children: Coding guidelines (4th edn). Unpublished manu-
Acknowledgements script, University of Virginia.
Charles H. Zeanah’s work on this paper was supported Chaffin, M., Hanson, R., Saunders, B.E., Nichols, T., Barnett,
in part by NIMH R01 MH091363-01 (Charles Nelson, D., Zeanah, C., et al. (2006). Report of the APSAC task force
PI); Lisa Berlin’s work on this paper was supported by on attachment therapy, reactive attachment disorder, and
NIMH K01MH70378 and by NIDA P30DA023026/the attachment problems. Child Maltreatment, 11, 76–89.
Duke University Transdisciplinary Prevention Research Cicchetti, D., & Barnett, D. (1991). Attachment organization in
maltreated preschoolers. Development and Psychopathol-
Center.
ogy, 3, 397–411.
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F.A., & Toth, S.L. (2006). Fostering
secure attachment in infants in maltreating families through
Correspondence to preventive interventions. Development and Psychopathology,
Charles H. Zeanah; Email: czeanah@tulane.edu 18, 623–649.
Cicchetti, D., Toth, S.L., & Rogosch, F.A. (1999). The efficacy of
toddler–parent psychotherapy to increase attachment secu-
References rity in offspring of depressed mothers. Attachment and
Ainsworth, M.D., Blehar, M.C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Human Development, 1, 34–66.
Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Cooper, G., Hoffman, K.T., & Powell, B. (2009). Circle of
Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Security: COS-P facilitator DVD manual 5.0. Unpublished
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and sta- manuscript, Marycliff Institute, Spokane, WA.
tistical manual (4th edn, text rev.). Arlington, VA: Author. Cyr, C., Euser, E.M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & van
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., Steele, H., Zeanah, C.H., Ijzendoorn, M.H. (2010). Attachment security and disorgani-
Muhamedrahimov, R.J., Vorria, P., Dobrova-Krol, N.A., zation in maltreating and high-risk families: A series of meta-
Steele, M., van IJzendoorn, M.H., Juffer, F., & Gunnar, analyses. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 87–108.
M.R. (in press). Attachment and emotional development in DeKlyen, M., & Greenberg, M.T. (2008). Attachment and
institutional care: Characteristics and catch-up. In R.B. psychopathology in childhood. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver
McCall, M.H. van IJzendoorn, F. Juffer, V.K. Groza, & C.J. (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clin-
Groark (Eds.), Children without permanent parental care: ical applications (2nd edn, pp. 637–665). New York: Guilford
Research, practice, and policy. Monographs of the Society for Press.
Research in Child Development. De Wolff, M., & van IJzendoorn, M.J. (1997). Sensitivity and
Benoit, D. (2000). Atypical caregiver behaviours and disorga- attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of
nized infant attachment. Newsletter of the Infant Mental infant attachment. Child Development, 68, 571–591.
Health Promotion Project (IMP), 29, 1–3. Dobrova-Krol, N.A., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., Van IJzen-
Benoit, D., Parker, K., & Zeanah, C.H. (1997). Mothers’ doorn, M.H., & Juffer, F. (in press). Effects of perinatal HIV
representations of their infants assessed prenatally: Stabil- infection and early institutional rearing on preschoolers’
ity and association with infants’ attachment classifications. attachment and indiscriminate friendliness. Journal of Child
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 307–313. Psychology and Psychiatry.
Berlin, L.J., Zeanah, C.H., & Lieberman, A.F. (2008). Preven- Dozier, M., Higley, E., Albus, K.E., & Nutter, A. (2002).
tion and intervention programs for supporting early attach- Intervening with foster infants’ caregivers: Targeting three
ment security. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook critical needs. Infant Mental Health Journal, 23, 541–554.
of attachment (2nd edn, pp. 745–761). New York: Guilford Dozier, M., & Lindheim, O. (2006). This is my child: Differences
Press. among foster parents in commitment to their young chil-
Boris, N.W., Aoki, Y., & Zeanah, C.H. (1999). The development dren. Child Maltreatment, 11, 338–345.
of infant–parent attachment: Considerations for assess- Dozier, M., Lindheim, O., & Ackerman, J. (2005). Attachment
ment. Infants and Young Children, 11, 1–10. and biobehavioral catch-up. In L. Berlin, Y. Ziv, L. Amaya-
Boris, N.W., Hinshaw-Fuselier, S.S., Smyke, A.T., Scheeringa, Jackson, & M.T. Greenberg (Eds.), Enhancing early attach-
M., Heller, S.S., & Zeanah, C.H. (2004). Comparing criteria ments (pp. 178–194). New York: Guilford Press.
for attachment disorders: Establishing reliability and valid- Dozier, M., Lindeheim, O., Lewis, E., Bick, J., Bernard, K., &
ity in high-risk samples. Journal of the American Academy of Peloso, E. (2009). Effects of a foster parent training program
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43, 568–577. on young children’s attachment behaviors: Preliminary
Bowlby, J. (1973). Separation. New York: Basic Books. evidence from a randomized clinical trial. Child and Adoles-
Bowlby, J. (1980). Loss. New York: Basic Books. cent Social Work, 26, 321–332.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment (2nd edn). New York: Basic Dozier, M., Manni, M., Gordon, M.K., Peloso, E., Gunnar, M.R.,
Books. Stovall-McClough, K.C., Eldreth, D., & Levine, S. (2006a).

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
832 Charles H. Zeanah, Lisa J. Berlin, and Neil W. Boris

Foster children’s diurnal production of cortisol: An explor- Lieberman, A.F., Ghosh Ippen, C., & Van Horn, P. (2006).
atory study. Child Maltreatment, 11, 189–197. Child–Parent Psychotherapy: 6-month follow-up of a ran-
Dozier, M., Peloso, E., Lewis, E., Laurenceau, J., & Levine, S. domized controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of
(2008). Effects of an attachment-based intervention on the Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 913–918.
cortisol production of infants and toddlers in foster care. Lieberman, A.F., & Van Horn, P. (2005). Don’t hit my mommy: A
Development and Psychopathology, 20, 845–859. manual for Child–Parent Psychotherapy with young wit-
Dozier, M., Peloso, E., Lindheim, O., Gordon, M.K., Manni, M., nesses of family violence. Washington: Zero to Three Press.
Sepulveda, S., & Ackerman, J. (2006b). Developing evi- Lieberman, A.F., Weston, D.R., & Pawl, J.H. (1991). Preventive
dence-based interventions for foster children: An example of intervention and outcome with anxiously attached dyads.
a randomized clinical trial with infants and toddlers. Journal Child Development, 62, 199–209.
of Social Issues, 62, 767–785. Lindheim, O., & Dozier, M. (2007). Caregiver commitment to
Dozier, M., Stovall, K.C., Albus, K.E., & Bates, B. (2001). foster children: The role of child behavior. Child Abuse and
Attachment for infants in foster care: The role of caregiver Neglect, 31, 361–374.
state of mind. Child Development, 72, 1467–1477. Lyons-Ruth, K., Bureau, J.F., Riley, C.D., & Atlas-Corbett, A.F.
Fearon, R.P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., Van IJzendoorn, (2009). Socially indiscriminate attachment behavior in the
M.H., Lapsley, A., & Roisman, G.I. (2010). The significance Strange Situation: Convergent and discriminant validity in
of insecure attachment and disorganization in the develop- relation to caregiving risk, later behavior problems, and
ment of children’s externalizing behavior: A meta-analytic attachment insecurity. Development and Psychopathology,
study. Child Development, 81, 435–456. 21, 355–372.
Fraiberg, S. (1980). Clinical studies in infant mental health: The Lyons-Ruth, K., & Jacobvitz, D. (2008). Attachment disorga-
first year of life. New York: Basic Books. nization: Genetic factors, parenting contexts, and develop-
Garber, B.D. (2009). Attachment methodology in custody mental transformation from infancy to adulthood. In J.
evaluation: Four hurdles standing between developmental Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment (2nd
theory and forensic application. Journal of Child Custody, 6, edn, pp. 666–697). New York: Guilford Press.
38–61. Macfie, J., Fitzpatrick, K., Rivas, E., & Cox, M. (2008).
Gleason, M.M., Fox, N.A., Drury, S., Smyke, A.T., Egger, H.L., Independent influences upon mother–toddler role reversal:
Nelson, C.A., Gregas, M.G., & Zeanah, C.H. (2011). The valid- Infant–mother attachment disorganization and role reversal
ity of evidence-derived criteria for reactive attachment disor- in mother’s childhood. Attachment and Human Development,
der: Indiscriminately social/disinhibited and emotionally 10, 29–39.
withdrawn/inhibited types. Journal of the American Academy Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security of
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 50, 216–231. attachment in infancy, childhood and adulthood: A move
Greenberg, M.T., Speltz, M.L., DeKlyen, M., & Endriga, M.C. to the level of representation. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters
(1991). Attachment security in preschoolers with and with- (Eds.), Growing points of attachment theory and research.
out externalizing problems: A replication. Development and Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
Psychopathology, 3, 413–430. ment, 50 (1–2, Serial No. 209), 66–104.
Grossman, K.E., Grossman, K., & Waters, E. (Eds.). (2005). Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1990). Procedures for identifying
Attachment from infancy to adulthood. New York: Guilford infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth
Press. Strange Situation. In M.T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E.M.
Heller, S.S., Boris, N.W., Hinshaw-Fuselier, S.S., Page, T., Cummings (Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory,
Koren-Karie, N., & Miron, D. (2006). Reactive attachment research and intervention (pp. 121–160). Chicago: University
disorder in maltreated twins: Follow-up from 18 months to of Chicago Press.
8 years. Attachment and Human Development, 8, 63–86. Marvin, R.S., Cooper, G., Hoffman, K., & Powell, B. (2002). The
Hesse, E. (2008). The Adult Attachment Interview: Protocol, Circle of Security project: Attachment-based intervention
method of analysis, and empirical studies. In J. Cassidy & with caregiver–preschool child dyads. Attachment and Hu-
P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, man Development, 4, 107–124.
research and clinical applications (2nd edn, pp. 552–598). McDonough, S. (2000). Interaction guidance: An approach for
New York: Guilford Press. difficult-to-engage families. In C.H. Zeanah, (Ed.), Handbook
Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & van IJzendoorn, of infant mental health (2nd edn, pp. 485–493). New York:
M.H. (2005). The importance of parenting in the develop- Guilford Press.
ment of disorganized attachment: Evidence from a preven- Mercer, J., Sarner, L., & Rosa, L. (2003). Attachment therapy
tive intervention study in adoptive families. Journal of Child on trial: The torture and death of Candace Newmaker.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 263–274. Westport, CT: Praeger.
Juffer, F., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & van IJzendoorn, Mesman, J., Stolk, M.N., van Zeijl, J., Alink, L.R.A., Juffer, F.,
M.H. (2007). Promoting positive parenting: An attachment- Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., et al. (2007). Extending the
based intervention. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. video-feedback intervention to sensitive discipline: The early
Klein Velderman, M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., Juffer, F., prevention of anti-social behavior. In F. Juffer, M.J. Baker-
& van IJzendoorn, M.H. (2006a). Effects of attachment- mans-Kranenburg, & M.H. van Ijzendoorn (Eds.), Promoting
based interventions on maternal sensitivity and infant positive parenting: An attachment-based intervention (pp.
attachment: Differential susceptibility of highly reactive 171–192). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
infants. Journal of Family Psychology, 20, 266–274. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P.R. (2007). Attachment in adult-
Klein Velderman, M., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., Juffer, F., hood: Structure, dynamics and change. New York: Guilford
van IJzendoorn, M.H., Mangelsdorf, S.C., & Zevalkink, J. Press.
(2006b). Preventing preschool externalizing behavior prob- Miron, D., Lewis, M., & Zeanah, C.H. (2009). Clinical use
lems through video-feedback intervention in infancy. Infant of observational procedures in early childhood relationship
Mental Health Journal, 27, 466–493. assessment. In C.H. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of infant
Kobak, R., & Madsen, S. (2008). Disruptions in attachment mental health (pp. 252–265). New York: Guilford Press.
bonds: Implications for theory, research and clinical inter- NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2006). Infant–
vention. In J. Cassidy & P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of mother attachment: Risk and protection in relation to
attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (2nd changing maternal caregiving quality over time. Develop-
edn, pp. 23–47). New York: Guilford Press. mental Psychology, 42, 38–58.

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Practitioner Review: Clinical applications of attachment theory and research for infants and young children 833

O’Connor, T.G., Marvin, R.S., Rutter, M., Olrick, J., Britner, Toth, S.L., Rogosch, F.A., Manly, J.T., & Cicchetti, D. (2006).
P.A., & the ERA Study Team. (2003). Child–parent attach- The efficacy of toddler–parent psychotherapy to reorganize
ment following early institutional deprivation. Development attachment in the young offspring of mothers with major
and Psychopathology, 15, 19–38. depressive disorder: A randomized preventive trial. Journal
Powell, B., Cooper, G., Hoffman, K., & Marvin, R.S. (2009). The of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 1006–1016.
Circle of Security. In C.H. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of infant Van den Dries, L., Juffer, F., Van IJzendoorn, M.H., &
mental health (3rd edn, pp. 450–467). New York: Guilford Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J. (2009). Fostering security? A
Press. meta-analysis of attachment in adopted children. Children
Rusconi-Serpa, C., Sancho Rossignol, A., & McDonough, S. and Youth Services Review, 31, 410–421.
(2009). Video feedback in parent–infant treatments. Child and van IJzendoorn, M.H. (1995). Adult attachment respresenta-
Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 18, 735–751. tions, parental responsiveness, and infant attachment: A
Rutgers, A.H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., van IJzendoorn, meta-analysis on the predictive validity of the Adult Attach-
M.H., & Van Berckelaer-Onnes, I.A. (2004). Autism and ment Interview. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 387–403.
attachment: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Child Psy- van IJzendoorn, M.H., Goldberg, S., Kroonenberg, P.M., &
chology and Psychiatry, 45, 1123–1134. Frenkel, O.J. (1992). The relative effects of maternal and
Rutter, M., Colvert, E., Kreppner, J., Beckett, C., Castle, J., child problems on the quality of attachment: A meta-
Groothues, C., et al. (2007). Early adolescent outcomes for analysis of attachment in clinical samples. Child Develop-
institutionally-deprived and non-deprived adoptees. I: Dis- ment, 63, 840–858.
inhibited attachment. Journal of Child Psychology and van IJzendoorn, M.H., Schuengel, C., & Bakermans-Kranen-
Psychiatry, 48, 17–30. burg, M.J. (1999). Disorganized attachment in early child-
Rutter, M., Kreppner, J., & Sonuga-Barke, E. (2009). Emanuel hood: Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and
Miller Lecture: Attachment insecurity, disinhibited attach- sequelae. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 225–249.
ment, and attachment disorders: Where do research findings Van Zeijl, J., Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M.H., Bakermans-
leave the concepts? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi- Kranenburg, M.J., Juffer, F., Stolk, M.N., et al. (2006).
atry, 50, 529–543. Attachment-based intervention for enhancing sensitive dis-
Schuengel, C., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M.J., & van IJzendo- cipline in mothers of 1-to-3-year-old children at risk for
orn, M.H. (1999). Frightening maternal behavior linking externalizing behavior problems: A randomized controlled
unresolved loss and disorganized infant attachment. Journal trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 994–
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 54–63. 1005.
Slade, A., Belsky, J., Aber, J.L., & Phelps, J.L. (1999). Mothers’ World Health Organization. (1992). The IDC-10 classification of
representations of their relationships with their toddlers: mental and behavioral disorders: Clinical descriptions and
Links to adult attachment and observed mothering. Devel- diagnostic guidelines (10th edn). Geneva, Switzerland: World
opmental Psychology, 35, 611–619. Health Organization.
Slade, A., Grienenberger, J., Bernbach, E., Levy, D., & Locker, Zeanah, C.H. (2000). Disturbances of attachment in young
A. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning, attachment, and children adopted from institutions. Journal of Developmental
the transmission gap: A preliminary study. Attachment and and Behavioral Pediatrics, 21, 230–236.
Human Development, 7, 283–298. Zeanah, C.H., & Benoit, D. (1995). Clinical applications of a
Smyke, A.T., Zeanah, C.H., Fox, N.A., & Nelson, C.A. (2009). A parent perception interview. In K. Minde (Ed.), Infant
new model of foster care for young children: The Bucharest psychiatry: Child Psychiatric Clinics of North America (pp.
Early Intervention Project. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric 539–554). Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders.
Clinics of North America, 18, 721–734. Zeanah, C.H., Benoit, D., Hirshberg, L., Barton, M.L., & Regan,
Smyke, A.T., Zeanah, C.H., Fox, N.A., Nelson, C.A., & Guthrie, C. (1994). Mothers’ representations of their infants are
D. (2010). Placement in foster care enhances attachment concordant with infant attachment classifications. Develop-
among young children in institutions. Child Development, mental Issues in Psychiatry and Psychology, 1, 9–18.
81, 212–223. Zeanah, C.H., Danis, B., Hirshberg, L., Benoit, D., Miller, D., &
Solomon, J., & George, C. (1999). The development of attach- Heller, S.S. (1999). Disorganized attachment associated with
ment in separated and divorced families: The effects of partner violence: A research note. Infant Mental Health
overnight visitation, parent and couple variables. Attach- Journal, 20, 77–86.
ment and Human Development, 1, 2–33. Zeanah, C.H., & Gleason, M.M. (2010). Reactive attachment
Steele, M., Hodges, J., Kaniuk, J., Steele, H., Hillman, S., & disorders: A review for DSM-V. Retrieved December 29,
Asquith, K. (2008). Forecasting outcomes in previously 2010 from http://stage.dsm5.org/Proposed%20Revision%20
maltreated children: The use of the AAI in a longitudinal Attachments/APA%20DSM-5%20Reactive%20Attachment%
adoption study. In H. Steele & M. Steele (Eds.), Clinical 20Disorder%20Review.pdf.
applications of the Adult Attachment Interview (pp. 427–451). Zeanah, C.H., Scheeringa, M.S., Boris, N.W., Heller, S.S.,
New York: Guilford Press. Smyke, A.T., & Trapani, J. (2004). Reactive attachment
Steele, H., & Steele, M. (Eds.). (2008). Ten clinical uses of the disorder in maltreated toddlers. Child Abuse and Neglect,
Adult Attachment Interview. In H. Steele & M. Steele (Eds.), 28, 877–888.
Clinical applications of the Adult Attachment Interview (pp. Zeanah, C.H., & Smyke, A.T. (2009). Disorders of attachment.
3–30). New York: Guilford Press. In C.H. Zeanah (Ed.), Handbook of infant mental health (3rd
Stovall-McClough, K.C., & Dozier, M. (2004). Forming attach- edn, pp. 421–434). New York: Guilford Press.
ments in foster care: Infant attachment behaviors during the Zeanah, C.H., Smyke, A.T., Koga, S., Carlson, E., & the BEIP
first 2 months of placement. Development and Psychopa- Core Group. (2005). Attachment in institutionalized and
thology, 16, 253–271. community children in Romania. Child Development, 76,
Toth, S.L., Maughan, A., Manly, J.T., Spagnola, M., & Cicch- 1015–1028.
etti, D. (2002). The relative efficacy of two interventions in
altering maltreated preschool children’s representational Accepted for publication: 12 January 2011
models: Implications for attachment theory. Development Published online: 9 May 2011
and Psychopathology, 14, 877–908.

 2011 The Authors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry  2011 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.
Copyright of Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may
not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like