You are on page 1of 8

Learning and Individual Differences 29 (2014) 98–105

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Learning and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/lindif

Systemizing and special interests: Characterizing the continuum


from neurotypical to autism spectrum disorder
Catherine L. Caldwell-Harris ⁎, Chloe J. Jordan
Department of Psychology, Boston University, 64 Cummington St., Boston, MA 02215, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Special interests have been studied in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) but not in adults. Using an
Received 8 September 2012 online survey, it was found that individuals with ASD reported more intense interests in systemizable domains,
Received in revised form 25 July 2013 relative to neurotypical adults. Self-reported systemizing preference was correlated with intensity of interest in
Accepted 11 October 2013
systemizable domains both for those with ASD and for neurotypical young adults. Few gender differences were
found in the neurotypical group in the expected categories of machines, technology and vehicles, where gender
Keywords:
Autism spectrum disorder
differences have been found in children. Gender differences in these categories did appear for the ASD group. We
Asperger syndrome propose a strength-based model of special interests, with the hobbies of neurotypical forming a continuum with
Special interests the special interests of ASD.
Adults © 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
Systemizing

1. Introduction 2006) and the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (Baron-Cohen,
Wheelwright, Hill, Raste, & Plumb, 2001).
Among individuals with Asperger syndrome, repetitive behaviors Past research has focused primarily on the special interests of
often occur in the form of intense interests which may be considered children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), who develop these
unusual or atypical in their content (e.g., World War II airplanes; see interests as early as 1–4 years of age (Attwood, 2003; Bashe & Kirby,
Asperger, 1991; Attwood, 2003; Winter-Messiers, 2007). These are 2001; Moore & Goodson, 2003), although some interests cannot be
sometimes called restricted or circumscribed interests (e.g., Klin, reliably scored on certain assessments until age 4 (Moore & Goodson,
Danovitch, Merz, & Volkmar, 2007). Following Winter-Messiers (2007), 2003). Approximately 75–90% of individuals with mild to moderate
we use the term “special interests” because it promotes a strength- autism or Asperger syndrome develop one or more special interests
based approach and it is also the term most frequently used by (Bashe & Kirby, 2001; Klin et al., 2007). Other individuals on the autism
individuals on the Internet discussion forums from which we recruited spectrum, including those with PDD-NOS (Sturm, Fernell, & Gillberg,
survey participants in the current study. We describe and defend 2004) and Rett syndrome (Mazzocco et al., 1998), also exhibit intense
the view that special interests are not primarily repetitive behaviors, interests.
but reflect information processing styles and cognitive strengths Special interests are often manifested in efforts to collect objects or
(Baron-Cohen, 2002). The interests and hobbies of neurotypical information relevant to the interest area, which can require extensive
individuals (i.e., those without ASD, Attwood, 1998; Winter-Messiers, amounts of time (Bashe & Kirby, 2001). Parents often find special
2007) frequently reflect individuals' cognitive-personality styles, and interests the most difficult to accommodate of autistic behaviors due
we thus propose that the special interests of those with Asperger to their intensity (Mercier, Mottron, & Belleville, 2000). Unlike other
syndrome or on the autism spectrum lie on a continuum with behaviors, special interests do not lessen with age (Fecteau, Mottron,
neurotypical hobbies. This leads to two primary hypotheses: (1) that Berthiaume, & Burack, 2003) and the number of interests may increase
the content of special interests exists on a continuum between as the individual approaches adulthood (Bashe & Kirby, 2001).
neurotypical individuals and individuals with autism spectrum disorder Among children with ASD, special interests often reflect exceptional
(ASD), and (2) that special interests reflect information processing styles, abilities, such as systemizing and heightened attention to detail.
such that the interests of individuals with ASD are correlated with Systemizing is the drive to explore, analyze, and construct systems
systemizing ability, and interests of neurotypicals with mentalizing (Baron-Cohen, Richler, Bisarya, Gurunathan, & Wheelwright, 2003).
ability, as measured by the Systemizing Quotient (Wheelwright et al., Domains that are amenable to systemizing are rule-based and
predictable, facilitating detection of input-operation–output relationships
⁎ Corresponding author. (Baron-Cohen, Ashwin, Ashwin, Tavassoli, & Chakrabarti, 2009). The
E-mail address: charris@bu.edu (C.L. Caldwell-Harris). special interests of children with ASD frequently occur in systemizable

1041-6080/$ – see front matter © 2013 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.10.005
C.L. Caldwell-Harris, C.J. Jordan / Learning and Individual Differences 29 (2014) 98–105 99

domains, such as in mechanical, collectible, natural, and numerical Gender differences in interests also exist in typically developing
systems (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 1999). children. In the study of DeLoache et al. (2007), half of the intense
Neurotypical (NT) individuals also develop special interests. While interests that parents reported for boys were for vehicles, trains, and
up to 90% of individuals with ASD develop special interests (Bashe & machines, and another 27% were for balls, dinosaurs, and tools. Girls'
Kirby, 2001; Klin et al., 2007), about 30% of NT children develop an intense interests were clothes/dressing up, babies, and tea sets. These
“extremely intense interest” (DeLoache, Simcock, & Macari, 2007). gender differences in interests were observed in the youngest children
Special interests in NT children may appear as early as 1–2 years of studied (approximately 1 year old). Since knowledge of gender stereo-
age. Like children with ASD, the special interests of NT children reflect types, implicit or explicit, does not occur until 18 months or later,
their cognitive strengths. NT children are often preoccupied by interests DeLoache et al. speculated that biological factors, such as fetal
in people, imaginative play, and the social environment more broadly testosterone, play a role.
(Attwood, 2003; DeLoache et al., 2007). These interests are consistent Gender differences in special interests have not previously been
with strong mentalizing ability (i.e., the capacity to understand and studied in adult persons with ASD. Our secondary hypothesis is that
attribute mental states to others, Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, we will see continuity between childhood and adulthood. We thus
Raste, & Plumb, 2001; Frith, Morton, & Leslie, 1991). While individuals expect that both ASD and neurotypical males will have interests in
with ASD often have higher systemizing ability than NT individuals more systemizable domains than will ASD and neurotypical females.
(Baron-Cohen et al., 2003), NT individuals generally have higher Understanding gender differences in special interests during adulthood
mentalizing abilities (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, et al., 2001; is important, as it can provide a means to adapt skill development in the
Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Skinner, Martin, & Clubley, 2001). workplace and higher educational settings to individual cognitive styles.
Furthermore, NT females tend to have higher mentalizing ability than
NT males (Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, et al., 2001; Baron-Cohen, 2. Materials and methods
Wheelwright, Skinner, et al., 2001), while NT males generally have
higher systemizing ability than NT females (Baron-Cohen et al., 2003). 2.1. Participants
Little is currently known about special interests in adulthood.
Understanding adults' special interests is important for illuminating the Participants with ASD were recruited from WrongPlanet.net, a large
positive role that interests play in adult life, such as rewarding careers online discussion forum created for individuals with ASD, and their
(Attwood, 2003; Grandin & Duffy, 2008; Jackson, 2002). No studies yet families, friends, and supporters (Plank & Yellow Sneaker Media,
compare the interests of individuals with ASD to those of NT individuals. 2004). Among 70 individuals who self-reported themselves as having
The current study examines the content of special interests held by an ASD, 67 identified themselves as having Asperger syndrome, while
adults with ASD and NT adults, with the expectation that individuals only 2 reported autism spectrum disorder and 1 reported PDD-NOS.
with ASD will hold more intense interests in domains previously Slightly more than half (58.6%) of individuals with ASD reported being
identified as systemizable, such as machines and technology, sorting, diagnosed by a clinician (see Table 1 for summary of other demographic
categorizing, and organizing, factual and numerical information, and diagnostic variables). The most common disorders reported by both
collecting, and the sciences. We also expect that NT individuals will groups were anxiety disorders (including panic attacks, social phobia,
hold more intense interests in mentalizing domains, such as in people generalized anxiety), followed by depression. Less commonly reported
and sports and games (as discussion of sports fosters social affiliation, was ADHD, followed by OCD.
see Mueller, Agamanolis, & Picard, 2003). The current study will also In the ASD group, 42.9% of participants were female, while 50.4% of
assess participants' systemizing and mentalizing abilities, with the goal the NT group was female. Although ASD is typically more prevalent in
of identifying whether interest categories previously identified as males, this gender ratio is similar to that reported in past studies of
systemizable correlate with high systemizing ability, and likewise ASD special interests on Internet discussion forums (Jordan & Caldwell-
whether interest categories expected to rely on mentalizing correlate Harris, 2012).
with high mentalizing ability. By identifying cognitive abilities that Neurotypical participants included participants who were forum
contribute to special interests, we hope to develop a strength-based users from the WrongPlanet.net website (n = 68) who identified
continuum model of special interests, where the interests of individuals themselves as neurotypical (i.e., not having an ASD) and students
with ASD and NT individuals coexist and vary depending on systemizing from Boston University who participated for course credits (n = 51).
and mentalizing ability (see Fig. 3 and Discussion section). A strength- Recruitment from a university allowed us to approximately match the
based model of special interests can inform the development of age and education level of the ASD and NT groups.
educational and therapeutic programs that capitalize on individual To determine whether participants reporting themselves as NT who
abilities and use special interests as a medium for learning other were recruited from the WrongPlanet.net forum should be considered
important skills. neurotypical, we compared their Systemizing Quotient, Autism
We also hope to better understand gender differences in special Quotient, and Eyes Test scores (see Materials) to those earned by NT
interests. Winter-Messiers (2007) reported that ASD girls were more participants recruited from Boston University. WrongPlanet NT
likely than boys to have more neurotypical interests, such as a individuals did score slightly higher on the autism quotient than Boston
10 year-old girl being interested in horses. Attwood (2003) also noted University NT participants (WrongPlanet NT M = 22.9, SD = 11.1, BU
ASD girls' intense interests in dolls, animals and fiction, but remarked M = 17.1, SD = 7; t(79) = −3.1, p b .005) and thus some of them may
on some aberrant qualities of these interests. Girls didn't use their be considered part of the broader autism phenotype (Constantino &
dolls to play with others, but would self-play with a large doll collection. Todd, 2003). However, there were no differences between the two NT
An interest in animals could become consuming; girls would want to groups on the Systemizing Quotient or Eyes Test. Furthermore,
act like animals, or to sleep in a stable. Interests in fiction included WrongPlanet NT individuals had lower scores than ASD individuals on
collecting and wanting to contact the author. The focus of the writing the Autism Quotient (WrongPlanet NT M = 22.9, SD = 11.1, ASD M =
interest was not related to school success or an anticipated career. 36.4, SD = 7.3; t(78) = −7.2, p b .001) and the Systemizing Quotient
ASD is well known to be less frequent in girls, which has (WrongPlanet NT M = 35.1, SD = 14.7, ASD M = 43.8, SD = 16.5;
been hypothesized to reflect protective aspects of girls' socialization t(97) = −2.8, p b .05), and higher scores on the Eyes Test (WrongPlanet
(e.g., Constantino & Todd, 2003) or lower fetal testosterone (Auyeung NT M = 24, SD = 5.4, ASD M = 18.4, SD = 9; t(88) = 3.9, p b .001).
et al., 2006). It would be useful to know if systemizing and/or a Although the WrongPlanet NT participants were substantially
person's autism traits predicted their type of interests independently neurotypical, their scores were slightly elevated compared to the Boston
of gender. University NT students and the NT norms of 16.4 reported by Baron-
100 C.L. Caldwell-Harris, C.J. Jordan / Learning and Individual Differences 29 (2014) 98–105

Table 1
Summary of participants.

Age M Male % Ethnicity Education % Clinician % Receiving % With other If have other disorder,
(SD, range) (% non-Caucasian) (% some college) diagnosed ASDa therapy for ASD (non-ASD) disorder % clinician diagnosedb

Autism spectrum disorder 29.2 (12.5, 16–42) 57.1% 4.2% 74.2% 58.6% 67.1% 68.6% 87.5%
(N = 70)
Neuro-typical 23.8 (10.7, 15–59) 49.6% 23.4% 80.6% – – 22.7% 88.9%
(N = 119)

Note. ASD = autism spectrum disorder.


a
The remaining percentage were self-diagnosed (11.4%) or did not respond (30%).
b
The remaining percentage were self-diagnosed (6.3% of ASD group, 0% of NT group) or did not respond (6.3% of ASD group, 1.1% of NT group).

Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, et al. (2001), Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, 3. Results


Skinner, et al. (2001). Our overall NT sample thus did have a slightly
higher than average number of autistic traits. 3.1. Preliminary analyses Systemizing Quotient and Eyes Test

2.2. Materials 3.1.1. Internal consistency of scales


The internal consistency of the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ) was
An online survey was administered using SurveyMonkey online high, with a Cronbach alpha of 0.94. Our modified version of the
survey software. The Cambridge University Obsessions Questionnaire Systemizing Quotient-Revised (SQ-R) also showed high consistency,
(Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 1999) was modified by adding the with a Cronbach alpha coefficient of 0.89.
categories of nature, history and culture, and psychological disorders,
with the existing category of plants being included under the broader 3.1.2. Effects of age and diagnosis type
nature category (see Table 2). The Systemizing Quotient, AQ, and Eyes Test were examined for
If a participant indicated an interest in a given category, they were effects of age and whether individuals reported a disorder other than
asked to provide a brief description of their particular interest, and to ASD. Age did not correlate significantly with scores on any of the three
rate the intensity of their interest in from 1 to 3, 1 indicating a casual measures, nor were there differences in scores between individuals
interest, 2 a moderate interest, and 3 an intense interest. Participants with a disorder other than ASD in either group. In the ASD group, no
who did not express an interest in a given category were assigned an differences in scores on these three scales were obtained for individuals
interest intensity of 0. diagnosed by a clinician and those who were self-diagnosed, or between
To measure how strongly autistic traits were expressed, the individuals who had received therapy and those who had not.
survey included the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ; Baron-Cohen,
Wheelwright, Skinner, et al., 2001). The Reading the Mind in the Eyes 3.1.3. Correlations between scales
Test Revised (Eyes Test, Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, et al., 2001) Consistent with prior work, the Systemizing Quotient correlated
provided a measure of mentalizing ability, and a modified version of positively with the AQ (r = 0.49, p b .001), and negatively with the
the Systemizing Quotient-Revised (SQ, Wheelwright et al., 2006) as- Eyes Test (r = −.19, p b .05). The AQ also correlated negatively with
sessed systemizing ability. the Eyes Test (r = −.49, p b .001).

Table 2
Special interest categories.

Special interest category Description

Machines & technology Interests in computers, radios, television, clocks, etc., or expresses interest in how things work. Does not include interests in
watching or listening to movies, television, music, etc.
Belief systems Interests in religions or mythologies, political systems, philosophies, alternative beliefs (such as conspiracy theories), etc.
Numerical information Interests in timetables, calculators or calculations, calendars, prime numbers, reading or creating charts or tables of information, etc.
Sports & games Interests in football, tennis, walking, biking, tennis, playing cards, chess, board games, puzzles, video games, etc.
Item attachment Interests focusing on a particular item or type of object, as well as interests in certain words or phrases.
Sensory Interests in touching or feeling certain things, or mentions fascinations with texture, specific sounds, lighting, colors, smells, etc.
Crafts Interests in model making, knitting, sewing, carpentry, etc.
Factual information Interests in reading or memorizing lists, reading encyclopedias, dictionaries, newspapers, etc.
Creative arts Interests in movies, television shows, art work, painting, playing an instrument, music, writing and reading fiction, creating media
(e.g., online films), performing arts, etc.
Psychological disorders Expresses interest in any psychological disorder.
History & culture Interests in existing languages, particular countries or civilizations, time periods or eras in history, etc.
Information & mechanical systems Interests in plumbing, light switches or electrical wiring, maps, city planning, subway maps and/or schedules, businesses and organizations,
etc., or an interest in creating systems (such as languages or maps).
Sorting, categorizing, & organizing Interests in making lists, lining objects up, arranging objects in certain orders or categories, planning, or obsession with neatness or organization.
Food & drink Interests in consuming or creating a particular food or drink, cooking, baking, etc.
Vehicles Interests in trains, airplanes, buses, boats, cars, etc.
People Interests in a particular person, in types or groups of people, or in interacting with people, including participation in online communities.
Collecting Interests in acquiring collections of particular items, e.g., bottles, keys, caps, stamps, rocks, etc.
Nature Interests in plants, interacting with nature (e.g., hiking, gardening, exploring, etc.), and natural phenomena
(e.g., volcanoes, tsunamis, lightning, etc.).
Animals Interests in pets, wild or farm animals, insects, fish, birds, etc., also includes animal-related activities (e.g., bird watching), but does not include
mythical creatures.
Sciences Interests in astronomy, chemistry, biology, physics, engineering, mathematics, logic, economics, psychology, meteorology,
specific diseases or conditions, etc.
Other Interests that did not fit within a specific category.
C.L. Caldwell-Harris, C.J. Jordan / Learning and Individual Differences 29 (2014) 98–105 101

3.1.4. Gender and group differences in the scales Table 4


For each of the three measures, a two-way between-groups analysis Intercorrelations for the intensity of interest in special interest categories and scores on the
Systemizing Quotient and Eyes Test.
of variance was conducted to examine main effects of gender and
group as well as potential interaction effects. Not surprisingly, ASD Special interest category Systemizing Quotient Eyes Test
participants had significantly higher scores than NT participants on the ASD Neurotypical Combined ASD
Systemizing Quotient (ASD M = 44.8, SD = 16.8, NT M = 32.4, SD = only only AD and NT only
11.9, F(1, 108) = 14.1, p b .001, η2p = .12) and on the AQ (ASD M = 36.4, Machines & technology .30# .42⁎⁎ .40⁎⁎⁎ −.08
SD = 7.3, NT M = 20.0, SD = 9.7, t(153) = −11.2, p b .001, η2 = .45). Vehicles .47# −.21# .30⁎⁎ 0
Also as hypothesized, NT participants had higher Eyes Test scores than Sorting, categorizing, & organizing .13 .11 .17# −.02
ASD participants (ASD M = 18.4, SD = 9, NT M = 24.9, SD = 5, Item attachment .02 .27# .25⁎⁎ −.10
Sensory .08 .17 .19# −.11
t(71.8) = 4.8, p b .001, η2 = .14). The only gender differences occurred Factual information .36⁎⁎ .35⁎⁎ .41⁎⁎⁎ −.05
for the Systemizing Quotient, with no interaction of gender and Collecting .29# .17 .32⁎⁎⁎ −.01
diagnosis. Males had higher systemizing scores than females (male Sciences .27# .05 .18# .20
M = 42.6, SD = 15.5, female M = 33.6, SD = 13.8, F(1, 108) = 4.8, Food & drink .08 .14 −.01 .08
Average interest intensity .38⁎⁎ .38⁎⁎ .37⁎⁎ .23#
p b .05, η2p = .04).
Note. For the Systemizing Quotient, higher scores are indicative of higher systemizing
3.2. Special interests analyses ability, while for the Eyes Test, higher scores are indicative of higher mentalizing ability.
Participants indicated the intensity of their interests on a scale from 0 to 3, 0
corresponding to no interest, 1 a casual interest, 2 a moderate interest, and 3 an intense
Our primary hypotheses were that individuals with ASD would have interest.
interests of different contents than NT individuals, and that interests ⁎⁎⁎ p b .001.
would vary with systemizing and mentalizing abilities for both groups. ⁎⁎ p b .005.
#
p b .05.
As shown in Table 3, individuals with ASD had more intense interests
in item attachment, collecting, factual information, and sorting,
categorizing, and organizing, as well as in sciences, sensory, and 3.3. Special interests, group and gender
machines and technology. NT individuals had more intense interests in
food and drinks. Individuals with ASD also identified interests in more Our secondary hypothesis was that ASD and NT males would have
categories overall than NT individuals (ASD M = 10.9, SD = 4.9, NT interests in more systemizable domains than ASD and neurotypical
M = 9, SD = 5.2, t(187) = −2.4, p b .05, η2 = .03), and had more intense females. We analyzed how gender interests interact with diagnosis and
interests on average (ASD M = 1.3, SD = 0.7, NT M = 1, SD = 0.6, type of special interest by conducting 2 × 2 analyses of variance on each
t(187) = −2.8, p b .01, η2 = .04). interest category. Four categories revealed significant 2 × 2 interactions.
Combining over both ASD and neurotypical individuals, scores on The resulting patterns are complex and can be best appreciated in
the Systemizing Quotient correlated positively with intensity of interest graphic forms. Fig. 1 plots the significant gender X diagnostic group
in machines and technology, sorting, categorizing, and organizing, item interactions for machines and technology, η2 = .04, and vehicles, η2 =
attachment, sensory, and factual information, as well as vehicles, .05, both ps b .05. These are plotted together because they show the
collecting items, and sciences (see Table 4). Scores on the Eyes Test same pattern of no gender differences for neurotypicals, but males having
correlated positively with food and drink, but negatively with item stronger interests than females in the ASD group, confirmed with post-
attachment, factual information, and collecting. The Systemizing hoc Scheffe tests. Fig. 2 plots the significant interactions for sorting,
Quotient also correlated positively with the total number of interest categorizing, and organizing, η2 = .06, and collecting interest category,
categories (M = 9.7, SD = 5.1, r = .37, p b .001) and with the average η2 =.05, both psb.05. We plotted these special interests together because
intensity of all interests (M = 1.1, SD = 0.7, r = .41, p b .001). Many the Scheffe post-hoc tests revealed a pattern of greater interests by
correlations remained significant even when correlating within the females than males among neurotypicals for both interest categories,
ASD or neurotypical group, although correlations were lower because but either no gender differences in the ASD group (Sorting, Organizing)
the SQ range was smaller when only one group was analyzed. Finding or males having somewhat greater interest than females (but smaller
robust correlations between systemizing scores and intensity of differences than seen in the ASD group in Fig. 1).
interests among neurotypicals is consistent with the concept of a Since the lack of gender differences among neurotypicals was
continuum between neurotypicals' hobbies and ASD individuals' special unexpected, we scrutinized individuals' comments accompanying
interests. their intensity ratings to determine whether engagement with these
objects occurred because objects facilitate systemizing.
Table 3
Intensity of special interests of individuals with autism spectrum disorder and 3.3.1. Machines and technology
neurotypical individuals.
Some comments did indicate that the object afforded systemizing, as
Special interest category Autism spectrum Neurotypical t(187) η2 in examples (1)–(3):
disorder M (SD)
M (SD)
(1) “I like to take things apart when I get the opportunity to observe
Item attachment 1.5 (1.3) 0.7 (1.2) −4.1⁎⁎⁎ .08 how it functions firsthand.” Female NT
Collecting 1.1 (1.2) 0.6 (0.9) −3.3⁎⁎⁎ .06 (2) “Building computers from scratch and this hobby/interest has
Factual information 1.8 (1.3) 1.1 (1.2) −3.5⁎⁎⁎ .06
been around since I was 8 years old and its still around.” Male
Sorting, categorizing, & organizing 1.7 (1.1) 1.2 (1.2) −2.7⁎⁎ .04
Sciences 1.6 (1.3) 1.3 (1.3) −2.6⁎⁎ .04 ASD
Sensory fixation 1.7 (1.1) 1.3 (1.3) −2.4# .04 (3) “Computers, mostly. Have repaired and built several. Also a Linux
Machines & technology 1.6 (1.4) 1.2 (1.2) −2.3# .03 user.” Female NT
Food & drink 0.3 (0.5) 0.5 (0.5) 3.6# .06
However, comments such as those in (4)–(6) emphasized machines
Note. Participants indicated the intensity of their interests on a scale from 0 to 3, 0
corresponding to no interest, 1 a casual interest, 2 a moderate interest, and 3 an intense as merely conduits to information, not as being mechanistically
interest. interesting.
⁎⁎⁎ p b .001.
⁎⁎ p b .01. (4) “Tv since I was 4, I can watch tv for hours at a time. I am now
#
p b .05. addicted to the Internet.” Female NT
102 C.L. Caldwell-Harris, C.J. Jordan / Learning and Individual Differences 29 (2014) 98–105

Fig. 1. Respondents' intensity of interests (0–3 scale) is plotted for two categories that both show a similar interaction between gender and group (ASD vs. neurotypical). Bars show that
ASD males have stronger interests than females in the interest category of machines and technology, and also the interest category of vehicles. Neurotypical males and females did not
differ.

(5) “I am in college so obviously my laptop, ipod, and other interests, both averaged around 0.5, which is low compared to other
electronics are basics in my life!” Female NT interest categories. Lack of gender differences may thus have occurred
(6) “I like using my Iphone and Mac for virtually everything.” Male NT because of floor effects. But a larger issue has become apparent,
concerning these interests and developmental stage. Vehicles (like
The proportion of males and females who gave examples of each machines) afford systemizing because they have many parts that can
type were similar, although the comments that emphasized systemizing be analyzed. These parts can be studied to determine how they work
were fewer in number in both genders. as independent components (doors and windows open and close,
wheels turn), and what these parts contribute to the functioning of
3.3.2. Vehicles the whole object. Vehicles also afford systemizing because sets of
ASD males reported the highest intensity of interests in vehicles, but vehicles can be compared and contrasted, thus yielding opportunities
the average intensity was still only just above 1, indicating a casual for developing mechanistic theories around attributes such as the
interest, while ASD females were almost 0 (on average no interest). function and speed of different categories of vehicles. But note that
While NT males and females reported having similar intensity of these are reasons why children who like systemizing will be attracted

Fig. 2. Respondents' intensity of interests (0–3 scale) is plotted for two categories, in order to depict the interaction between gender and diagnostic group (ASD vs. neurotypical). Bars show
that ASD males have stronger interests than females in the interest category of collecting, but were only slightly (and nonsignificantly) more interested in sorting/collecting. For both of
these categories, among neurotypical respondents, females expressed more intense interests than males.
C.L. Caldwell-Harris, C.J. Jordan / Learning and Individual Differences 29 (2014) 98–105 103

to vehicles. Adults who like systemizing will turn to domains that are least partly an artifact of using The Cambridge University Obsessions
more challenging for their cognitive abilities (computer programming, Questionnaire (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 1999; see Table 2). Before
the sciences). widespread computer and Internet use, being interested in machines
and technology typically occurred because machines and technology
3.3.3. Sorting, categorizing and organizing afforded systemizing (taking apart machines; computer programming).
The comments of female neurotypicals were similar to ASD males But the neurotypical young adults we surveyed reported being
and females. These three groups also had roughly similar reported interested in machines and technology primarily for information access,
intensities (see Fig. 2). Examples appear in (7)–(9). By contrast, NT socializing, and entertainment. No gender differences were suggested
males expressed almost no interests in this category. by the few respondents who did report having systemizing interests
in machines and technology. Similarly, lack of gender differences in
(7) “I like to write things down in my yearly organizer, if I have any interest in vehicles, and possibly also in sorting, organizing, categorizing
plans. Only when cleaning my room, I sometimes like to arrange and collecting may have occurred because these interests do not afford
objects in a certain order.” ASD female challenging systemizing opportunities for adults. Future work on
(8) “Since I was a child, I have loved organizing and planning. gender differences in systemizing can start anew by asking what
Sometimes I spend a whole day to clean my room. Especially, systemizing activities are common for the target age group in the
my closet has to be in some order. Ex. colors.” NT female Internet and personal computer era.
(9) “I *love* list-making; lining objects up; symmetry; organizing
music; filing; data entry.” ASD male
4.2. What causes special interests?
The comments overall suggest that sorting, categorizing and
organizing inhabit the shallow level of systemizing. They are not used The prevailing explanations for special interests are (1) that they
to analyze or predict a system, but to impose control and order into function to reduce anxiety (Attwood, 2003), or (2) reduce over-
one's life. There was a trend among the comments for all females to arousal caused by sensory stimuli (Hutt, Hutt, Lee, & Ounsted, 1965).
sort, categorize and organize as an end towards a goal of having an These explanations need to be reconsidered given Turner's (1997)
organized life, while ASD males reported enjoying sorting, categorizing observation that individuals with ASD engaged in repetitive behaviors
and organizing because it enabled systemizing (i.e., understanding) and special interests when alone and un-stimulated. Turner also found
systems that were not set up by oneself. that repetitive behaviors were least likely to occur during social
interactions when anxiety is maximal.
3.3.4. Collecting We propose that people's unique cognitive abilities, and especially
Neurotypical respondents reported quite low intensities, with only systemizing and mentalizing, prompt the development of special
ASD males reporting on average having a moderate interest in interests in particular domains (as illustrated in Fig. 3; see also Baron-
collecting. Types of collections were similar for neurotypical males and Cohen & Wheelwright, 1999). In this view, ASD special interests exist
females and ASD females, with rocks being the most common item on a continuum with NT interests. ASD special interests that draw on
collected, then shot glasses, sea shells, and coins, as in examples strong systemizing abilities may be shaped in early childhood by the
(10)–(12). low salience of social stimuli (Carruthers, 1996; Sasson, Turner-Brown,
Holtzclaw, Lam, & Bodfish, 2008). Sasson et al. (2008) found that
(10) “Rocks and minerals, turtles (not real ones), stamps, coins, children with ASD showed decreased gaze time at social objects such
books, …spool knitters, small looms, antique plates…” NT Female as faces, and increased gaze time at objects that are frequently the
(11) “Dolls, buttons, fairies, egyptian, chinese, flying tigers” ASD subject of ASD special interests (“high-autism-interest” stimuli), such
Female as trains, vehicles, and electronics.
(12) “Collect boxes and perfume oils.” ASD Female The concept of a continuum of special interests is consistent with
social scientists' conception that the domains of human knowledge
Statements were more eccentric for ASD males, as in (13)–(14).
range along dimensions of compact/diffuse and rule-governed vs. open-
(13) “When I was 4, I used to collect the cushions that go on the gaps ended (Simonton, 2009; Toulmin, 1972). We will summarize these
between eye pieces of swimming goggles. At various times in dimensions with the term granularity. Fields of knowledge (interests)
childhood I obsessed over certain items, but this no longer with small granularity are predictable and rule-based, such as the
happens.” ASD Male sciences. The opposing end of the continuum is comprised of large
(14) “I delete nothing. Not files, not letters, not notes, not music, not granularity interests, which, like diffuse domains and “soft” disciplines,
books, not computers, not electronics, nothing. I keep it all…” involve few rules and are less definable. Fields of knowledge with large
ASD Male granularity require the ability to understand broadly defined domains,
which involve few rules. The social environment is the earliest large
This category thus resembles the vehicles interest in that floor granularity domain to which a child is exposed, and mentalizing abilities
effects in interest intensity means reduced opportunity to observe develop so that the individual can navigate the social world. High
gender effects. The higher interest levels reported among ASD males mentalizing ability may thus represent a cognitive style that is most
are consistent with this group having the highest systemizing attracted to large granularity interests.
propensity but also may reflect hoarding, a characteristic of OCD, The granularity concept provides a means of explaining why an
which can be co-morbid with ASD, as in example (14). individual develops interests in particular domains, as the appeal
of phenomena at different granularities would appeal to different
4. Discussion cognitive styles. We propose that each individual has a unique cognitive
“comfort zone,” or preferred granularity, which describes the range of
4.1. Gender differences and special interests granularities most attractive to a person with specific cognitive abilities,
such as systemizing and mentalizing. Existing evidence for a continuum
We found no gender differences in interests among neurotypical includes Nettle's finding (2007) that NT individuals' scores on the
adults, contrary to the robust differences observed in children Systemizing Quotient correlated positively with technological interests,
(DeLoache et al., 2007). While this could reflect lack of power and which are lawful and of small granularity. Billington, Baron-Cohen, and
sampling error due to small sample sizes, we suggest that this is at Wheelwright (2007) found that strong systemizing ability and weak
104 C.L. Caldwell-Harris, C.J. Jordan / Learning and Individual Differences 29 (2014) 98–105

Fig. 3. A proposed continuum of special interests exists ranging the autism-neurotypical continuum. The special interest continuum is defined by systemizing and empathizing or
mentalizing ability. Individuals with autism spectrum disorder, which is also characterized as the extreme male brain, have interests that are highly systemizable and require little
mentalizing ability. Autism spectrum interests are also more intense, specific, and numerous than neurotypical interests. Conversely, neurotypical individuals have interests that may
require high mentalizing ability but less systemizing ability. Neurotypical interests are also less intense, broader, and fewer in number than autism spectrum interests. Female interests
lie on the extreme neurotypical end of the continuum, as neurotypical females tend to have higher empathizing ability than males. Males, who are better systemizers than females but
have lower empathizing ability, would have interests falling on the mid- to end-range of the continuum depending on the strength of their systemizing and empathizing skills.

empathizing ability predicted academic study in the physical sciences processing and result in the development of interests in unusual domains
(e.g., mathematics, physics, engineering, etc). (Baron-Cohen et al., 2009). Mottron, Dawson, Soulières, Hubert, and
In our study, the reported intensity of interests in large granularity Burack (2006) propose that hypersensitivity may result from under-
domains, such as food and drink (which may heavily involve social connectivity or local-overconnectivity between neurons in the brain.
interaction) correlated with mentalizing ability. High systemizing The under-connectivity hypothesis suggests that the ASD brain lacks
correlated not only with intense small granularity interests but also long-range neural connections responsible for high-level cognitive
with having more intense interests overall, as well as a high total number processes (Mottron et al., 2006). The failure to develop integrated
of interests. Individuals with large granularity interests may have fewer connections might lead to hyperspecialization in some brain areas in
interests overall than those with small grain interests, because large the form of local overconnectivity, which could contribute to the
granularity interests are broadly defined and may encompass a wider abnormal sensory processing that causes hypersensitivity.
range of topics in themselves. The extreme large granularity end of the
continuum represents the cognitive comfort zone of individuals with 4.4. Implications for education
high mentalizing ability. NT interests, particularly those of females, are
predicted to occur more frequently on the large granularity end, Understanding special interests is crucial for educators, since
as females tend to have higher mentalizing abilities and develop students find their interests inherently rewarding, and educators can
mentalizing earlier than NT males (Frith & Frith, 2003). The interests of bring them into individualized lesson plans. The continuum model
NT individuals with varying propensities for systemizing and mentalizing suggests that individuals with ASD, and males, on average, are more
may occur in the middle range of the continuum, but would not likely to have small granularity interests, associated with high system-
otherwise be labeled as restricted or obsessive. izing ability. This understanding could be used in the classroom or
work environment, where large granularity topics can be gradually
4.3. Individual variation in cognitive styles introduced and related to preexisting interests. For example, children
or adults may be interested in machines and technology, but could be
What causes individual differences in systemizing and mentalizing required to study history, a more “diffuse” domain, for a school or a
ability? Previous work has implicated several brain regions in the workplace requirement. Initial learning could first be stimulated in the
development and maintenance of mentalizing, including the mirror context of small granularity subtopics, such as a discussion of how
neuron system, the temporo-parietal junction, temporal poles, and technology propelled historical events. Large granularity abstract
medial prefrontal cortex (Frith & Frith, 2003). The functioning of these concepts could then be progressively added to the curriculum. The
areas can vary depending on many genetic and environmental factors. continuum model also suggests that females, on average, tend to have
Fetal testosterone is thought to be one cause of high systemizing large granularity interests, associated with high mentalizing ability.
ability. Fetal testosterone is elevated in boys compared to girls, and This understanding could be used to adapt educational materials for
high fetal testosterone was found to correlate with systemizing ability teaching more compact small granularity topics, such as mathematics.
for both genders (Auyeung et al., 2006). High fetal testosterone also Work by Gredlein and Bjoyklund (2005) suggests that even minor
correlated with boys having a number of special interests, while low educational tailoring can significantly improve individual performance:
fetal testosterone correlated with high quality social relationships in while boys were initially more likely to use tools to retrieve a toy, girls
both boys and girls (Knickmeyer, Baron-Cohen, Raggatt, & Taylor, 2005). performed at the same level once they were given verbal hints by
Another cause of high systemizing ability may be a heightened the experimenter, and generalized their learning to a second tool-use
attention to detail, as details in a system must be tracked should they session. It is interesting to note that verbal hints may have capitalized
contribute to input-operation–output relationships (Baron-Cohen et al., on girls' mentalizing ability.
2009). Attention to detail, in turn, could arise from hypersensitivity The strength-based continuum model of interests can inform career
to various sensory stimuli, which might affect low-level information placement, degree selection, or aptitude testing (Armstrong, 2011;
C.L. Caldwell-Harris, C.J. Jordan / Learning and Individual Differences 29 (2014) 98–105 105

Gardner, 2006; Grigorenko & Stemberg, 1997). Understanding the Frith, U., Morton, J., & Leslie, A.M. (1991). The cognitive basis of a biological disorder:
Autism. Trends in Neurosciences, 14(10), 433–438.
nature and career potential of special interests may provide support Gardner, H. (2006). Five minds for the future. Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press.
and relief for parents who are frustrated with their child's consuming Grandin, T., & Duffy, K. (2008). Developing talents: Careers for individuals with Asperger
interests. More generally, the continuum approach lends further insight syndrome and high-functioning autism. Shawnee Mission, KS: Autism Asperger
Publishing Company.
into individual cognitive differences, and may help us better understand Gredlein, J. M., & Bjoyklund, D. F. (2005). Sex differences in young children's use of tools in
why these abilities develop. Finally, the special interests continuum a problem-solving task. Human Nature, 16(2), 211–232.
provides a more complete understanding of the deep interests that Grigorenko, E. L., & Stemberg, R. J. (1997). Styles of thinking, abilities, and academic
performance. Exceptional Children, 63, 295–312.
motivate people, bring richness to our lives, and often lead to new Hutt, S. J., Hutt, C. C., Lee, D.D., & Ounsted, C. C. (1965). A behavioural and electro-
scientific and technological advances. encephalographic study of autistic children. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 3(3),
181–197.
Jackson, L. (2002). Freaks, geeks and Asperger syndrome: A user guide to adolescence.
References London England: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Jordan, C. J., & Caldwell-Harris, C. L. (2012). Understanding differences in neurotypical and
Armstrong, T. (2011). The power of neurodiversity: Unleashing the advantages of your autism spectrum special interests through internet forums. Intellectual and
differently wired brain. Cambridge, MA: PerseusBooks. Developmental Disabilities, 50(5), 391–402.
Asperger, H. (1991). “Autistic” psychopathy in childhood. In U. Frith (Ed., Translator), Klin, A., Danovitch, J. H., Merz, A.B., & Volkmar, F. R. (2007). Circumscribed interests
Autism and Asperger Syndrome (pp. 37–92). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. in higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: An exploratory
Attwood, T. (1998). Asperger's syndrome: A guide for parents and professionals. London: study. Research & Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 32(2), 89–100.
Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Knickmeyer, R., Baron-Cohen, S., Raggatt, P., & Taylor, K. (2005). Foetal testosterone, social
Attwood, T. (2003). Understanding and managing circumscribed interests. In M. Prior cognition, and restricted interests in children. Journal of Child Psychology and
(Ed.), Learning and behaviour problems in Asperger syndrome (pp. 126–147). New Psychiatry, 45, 1–13.
York: Guilford Press. Mazzocco, M., Pulsifer, M., Fiumara, A., Cocuzza, M., Nigro, E., Incorpora, G., et al. (1998).
Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Chapman, E., Knickmeyer, R., Taylor, K., & Hackett, G. Brief report: Autistic behaviors among children with fragile x or Rett syndrome:
(2006). Foetal testosterone and the child systemizing quotient (SQ-C). European Implications for the classification of pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of
Journal of Endocrinology, 155, 123–130. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 321–328.
Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., Ashwin, C., Tavassoli, T., & Chakrabarti, B. (2009). Talent in Mercier, C., Mottron, L., & Belleville, S. (2000). A psychosocial study on restricted interests
autism: Hyper-systemizing, hyper-attention to detail and sensory hypersensitivity. in high-functioning persons with pervasive developmental disorders. Autism, 4(4),
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 406–425.
364, 1377–1383. Moore, V., & Goodson, S. (2003). How well does early diagnosis of autism stand the test of
Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N., & Wheelwright, S. (2003). The time?: Follow-up study of children assessed for autism at age 2 and development of
systemizing quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger's syndrome or high an early diagnostic service. Autism, 7(1), 47–63.
functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Philosophical Transactions of The Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual
Royal Society, 358(1430), 361–374. functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal
Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (1999). ‘Obsessions’ in children with autism or of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.
asperger syndrome: Content analysis in terms of core domains of cognition. British Mueller, F., Agamanolis, S., & Picard, R. (2003). Exertion interfaces: Sports over a distance
Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 484–490. for social bonding and fun. Proceedings of CHI (pp. 561–568). Fort Lauderdale, FL:
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001a). The ‘reading the ACM Press.
mind in the eyes’ test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Nettle, D. (2007). Empathizing and systemizing: What are they, and what do they
Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and contribute to our understanding of psychological sex differences? British Journal of
Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251. Psychology, 98, 237–255.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001b). The autism Plank, A., & Yellow Sneaker Media, L. L. C. (2004). WrongPlanet.net: The online resource and
spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high functioning autism, community for autism and Asperger's. http://www.wrongplanet.net/forums.html
males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental (Retrieved March 17, 2010).
Disorders, 31, 5–17. Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L., Holtzclaw, T. N., Lam, K. S. L., & Bodfish, J. W. (2008).
Bashe, P. R., & Kirby, B.L. (2001). What Asperger syndrome looks like. In B.L. Kirby, & P. R. Children with autism demonstrate circumscribed attention during passive viewing
Bashe (Eds.), The OASIS guide to Asperger syndrome: Advice, support, insight, and of complex social and nonsocial picture arrays. Autism Research, 1, 31–42.
inspiration. New York: Crown Publishers. Simonton, D. K. K. (2009). Varieties of (scientific) creativity: A hierarchical model of
Billington, J., Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2007). Cognitive style predicts entry domain-specific disposition, development, and achievement. Perspectives on
into physical sciences and humanities: Questionnaire and performance tests of Psychological Science, 4, 441–452.
empathy and systemizing. Learning and Individual Differences, 17, 260–268. Sturm, H., Fernell, E., & Gillberg, C. (2004). Autism spectrum disorders in children with
Carruthers, P. (1996). Autism as mind-blindness: An elaboration and partial defense. In P. normal intellectual levels: Associated impairments and subgroups. Developmental
Carruthers, & P. K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of Theories of Mind (pp. 257–273). Medicine & Child Neurology, 46, 444–447.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Toulmin, S. (1972). Human understanding, volume 1: The collective use and development of
Constantino, J. N., & Todd, R. D. (2003). Autistic traits in the general population: A twin concepts. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 524–530. Turner, M. (1997). Towards an executive dysfunction account of repetitive behavior in
DeLoache, J. S., Simcock, G., & Macari, S. (2007). Planes, trains, automobiles—and tea sets: autism. In J. Russel (Ed.), Autism as an Executive Disorder (pp. 57–100). Oxford: Oxford
Extremely intense interests in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 43, University Press.
1579–1586. Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen, S., Goldenfeld, N., Delaney, J., Fine, D., Smith, R., et al.
Fecteau, S., Mottron, L., Berthiaume, C., & Burack, J. A. (2003). Developmental changes of (2006). Predicting autism spectrum quotient (AQ) from the systemizing quotient-
autistic symptoms. Autism, 7(3), 255–268. revised (SQ-R) and quotient (EQ). Brain Research, 1079, 47–56.
Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Winter-Messiers, M. (2007). From tarantulas to toilet brushes: Understanding the special
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, interest areas of children and youth with Asperger's syndrome. Remedial and Special
358, 459–473. Education, 28, 140–152.

You might also like