You are on page 1of 12

J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:879–890

DOI 10.1007/s10803-010-1112-x

ORIGINAL PAPER

Stress and Personal Resource as Predictors of the Adjustment


of Parents to Autistic Children: A Multivariate Model
Ayelet Siman-Tov • Shlomo Kaniel

Published online: 25 September 2010


Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Abstract The research validates a multivariate model that long-term care not only impacts on parents’ various life
predicts parental adjustment to coping successfully with an domains (e.g. marriage, career) and leads to caregiver
autistic child. The model comprises four elements: parental stress, but also affects service utilization and treatment
stress, parental resources, parental adjustment and the child’s effectiveness for the children and their family as a whole,
autism symptoms. 176 parents of children aged between 6 to which amount to significant societal costs. Thus, from both
16 diagnosed with PDD answered several questionnaires the individual and societal perspectives, identifying and
measuring parental stress, personal resources (sense of understanding factors related to parenting stress are
coherence, locus of control, social support) adjustment important to the treatment of the child, the well-being of
(mental health and marriage quality) and the child’s autism the caregivers and the entire family, and the interests of the
symptoms. Path analysis showed that sense of coherence, community (Mak et al. 2007; Saloviita et al. 2003).
internal locus of control, social support and quality of mar- The suggested multivariate model relies on several
riage increase the ability to cope with the stress of parenting theories such as: the double ABCX model of family stress
an autistic child. Directions for further research are suggested. (Pakenham et al. 2005; Saloviita et al. 2003), the appraisal
theory (Lazarus 2001) and resource-based theories (Anto-
Keywords Stress  Adjustment  Resources  Autism  novsky 1993; Vancouver et al. 2008). It is assumed that
Parents  Coherence these variables have a high probability of predicting
parental adjustment for successful coping with the autistic
child. The suggested model comprises four elements:
Theoretical Background parental stress, parental resources, parental adjustment and
the child’s autism symptoms.
Parenting a child with a developmental disability such as
autism is a complex situation of chronic stress for the Parental Stress
parents (for review see Shapiro and Accardo 2008). Such
Stressors refer to threats, demands, or structural constraints
The research reported here served as part of the first author’s
which, when present, may call into question the operating
dissertation, supervised by the second author, submitted to the School integrity of the organism (Wheaton 1997). There is little
of Education Bar Ilan University Israel, in partial fulfillment of question that raising a child with a developmental disability
requirements for the Ph.D. constitutes a significant stressor for families. Parents of
children with autism reported higher levels of stress than
A. Siman-Tov (&)
Department of Special Education, Kibbutzim College parents of children with Down’s syndrome, developmental
of Education Technology and the Arts, Tel Aviv, Israel disabilities, or other psychiatric difficulties (Eisenhower
e-mail: yosicpa@netvision.net.il et al. 2005; Fisman et al. 2000; Hastings et al. 2005;
Oelofsen and Richardson 2006; Pisula 2007).
S. Kaniel
School of Education, Bar Ilan University, Ramat Gan, Israel The parents’ perception of the stressors are the key
e-mail: kaniels@mail.biu.ac.il element for understanding the notion that the greater the

123
880 J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:879–890

severity of the disability, the more distress reported by coping strategies and the appraisal process vis-à-vis a
parents (Perry et al. 2005). Such perceptions are deter- particular stressor, a sense of coherence underscores a
mined mainly by stressor appraisals (Folkman 1997). global orientation in the perception of life events that
Appraisal has been described as the cognitive interpretation individuals may bring to different situations. It may act as a
one has regarding a potential stressor. The degree of stress general resilience to stress as well as a precursor in setting
and subsequent adjustment depend on the individual’s other cognitive processes in motion that are specific to the
appraisals of both the stressor and his or her coping ability situation at hand (i.e. taking care of a child with autism).
and support resources, and the types of coping strategies he Numerous studies have found SOC to be a potent stress
or she uses to manage the event. When the nature of the resource across various settings and populations. People
stressor and its implications are clear, it is easier to cope with a strong SOC were more likely to perceive their lives
than when the situation is ambiguous. Autism is of unclear as less stressful, to report fewer stressful events and to rate
etiology and children with autism often do not ‘‘look’’ life events as less stressful. SOC was also found to be
disabled. Parents frequently report getting the ‘‘runaround’’ inversely correlated to depression, anxiety and physical
from friends, family and professionals who say their chil- symptoms, psychological health and well-being (Chumbler
dren will grow out of it or imply that poor parenting is an et al. 2004; Lindstrom and Eriksson 2005; Mak et al. 2007;
issue. Ambiguity may also lead to disagreements between Sagy and Antonovsky 2000) and positively correlated with
parents as to the nature and cause of the child’s problem, academic competence and perceived wellbeing (Amirkhan
which may reinforce parental feelings of uncertainty and and Greaves 2003; Johnson 2004). Although ample
increase stress (Bristol et al. 1988). empirical support has been found on the positive effects of
SOC on physical and mental health, very little research has
Resources been conducted on using SOC to understand the experience
of caregivers of children with autism and other develop-
Resources are those skills, objects, personal characteristics, mental disabilities (Al-Yagon and Margalit 2009; Olsson
conditions, or energies that help people cope with stressors and Hawang 2002).
(Hobfoll 2001). Resource-based theories of stress (Anto-
novsky 1993; Vancouver et al. 2008) complement apprai- Locus of Control
sal-based theories, because the latter are only one
component of the many resources. Among the wide range One more resource is locus of control, which determines
of resources, we will focus on three that have been proven how individuals explain the situations they experience
as having the power to predict adjustment in parents and (Rotter 1975). There are two different dimensions in locus
children: sense of coherence, locus of control and social of control: internality and externality. Internals explain life
support. events as a result of their own behavior and actions which
are under their control. As such, internals actively seek
Sense of Coherence (SOC) information and knowledge concerning their situation, are
more likely to assume their efforts will be successful, and
Sense of coherence (SOC) may be construed as a mecha- more likely to take initiative and responsibility.
nism that helps individuals select the best available Externals explain life events as under the control of
resources to cope with various stressors in their lives powerful others such as ‘fate’. Since they locate control
(Antonovsky 1993). SOC is conceptualized as a global outside themselves, externals tend to feel they have less
orientation that expresses the extent to which one feels control over their life, tend to be more stressed and more
confident that (a) the stimuli deriving from one’s internal prone to clinical depression (Konstantareas and Papageor-
and external environments in the course of living are giou 2006; Kormanik and Rocco 2009; Lefcourt 1991).
structured, predictable, and explicable–comprehensibility; Parents with an internal locus of control and a belief that
(b) resources are available to meet the demands posed by they control their lives were characterized by family
these stimuli–manageability; and (c) these demands are cohesion, optimism and less stress. On the other hand,
challenges worthy of investment and engagement–mean- parents of autistic children with an external locus of control
ingfulness (Antonovsky 1993). were characterized by less family cohesion pessimism and
Even though there are three elements, SOC should be depression (for review see Kormanik and Rocco 2009).
regarded as a unitary construct that has been linked to
greater stress-resistance and better health. Conscious of Social Support
difficulties or frustrations, a person with a strong SOC does
not ignore them and is more confident that basic difficulties Social support refers to the individual’s perception that he
can be resolved or dealt with. Unlike focusing on specific or she can be helped or can attain the understanding,

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:879–890 881

cooperation and assistance of close or significant persons. and creative life, and the flexibility to deal with life’s
This help can be instrumental (e.g., a parent’s need for a inevitable challenges.
ride) and/or social and emotional from a close person while Mothers of autistic children reported lower mental
coping with stress. Social and community support also health than mothers of children with other syndromes
emerges as a significant resource for families under stress (Abbeduto et al. 2004; Eisenhower et al. 2005). Other
and as a protective factor that contributes to successful researches found higher levels of depression and anxiety
adjustment of parents of children with developmental dis- among mothers of children with Asperger syndrome
orders (Altiere and Kluge 2009; Armstrong et al. 2005; (Pakenham et al. 2005) and PDD (Fisman et al. 2000).
Bromley et al. 2004; Dunst et al. 1997; Tehee et al. 2009;
White and Hastings 2004). Quality of Marriage

Parental Adjustment Quality of marriage relates to the spouses’ subjective


evaluation of the quality of their relationships in different
Adjustment is characterized by a balance between the domains such as: communication, satisfaction, friendship,
demands of the stressors and the resources. Good adjust- assigning roles, recreation, sharing personal experiences
ment is characterized by a balance between the family and mutual tolerance (Florian and Findler 2001; Lavee and
demands of the stressors and the ability to cope with these Olson 1991). Researches demonstrated how parenting an
demands. Poor adjustment is characterized by imbalance autistic child can lower the quality of marriage and satis-
between family demands of the stressors and the ability to faction (e.g., Bristol et al. 1988; Risdal and Singer 2004;
cope with them (Lavee and Olson 1991). Parental adjust- Schulz et al. 2004; Stoneman and Gavidia-Payn 2006).
ment involves two main variables: (a) mental health, (b) Coping with high levels of stress in domains external to the
quality of marriage. Although no definitive consensus has relationship tends to tax parents’ cognitive resources,
emerged to distinguish the various types of adjustment or leaving them fewer resources to effectively manage nega-
coping activities, there is widespread acknowledgement of tive perceptions of their relationships (Baumeister 2002)
the fact that the concept may refer to a variety of behaviors and navigate the ups and downs with their partners (Neff
designed to handle stressors. In this study we focus on two and Karney 2009). Parents whose global satisfaction fluc-
important variables that emerged as high predictors: (a) tuates more severely report lower levels of commitment
mental health, (b) quality of marriage. and are more likely to be in relationships that ultimately
end in dissolution compared with those whose satisfaction
Mental Health levels remain stable, even if those stable feelings are not
particularly positive. Spouses under stress often tend to
The term ‘mental health’ has no single ‘‘official’’ defini- report more specific problems within the relationship such
tion, since it is used by many disciplines such as anthro- as: withdrawal from their partners, reduced involvement at
pology, education, psychology, religion and sociology. home by engaging in fewer household tasks, showing less
Cultural differences, subjective assessments, and compet- affection to one another and spending less time together
ing professional theories all affect how mental health is (Neff and Karney 2009).
defined. The World Health Organization defines mental
health as ‘‘a state of wellbeing in which the individual Child Autism Symptom Severity
realizes his or her own abilities, can cope with the normal
stresses of life, can work productively and fruitfully, and is According to DSM-IV (APA 2000) the diagnosis of an
able to make a contribution to his or her community’’. For autistic disorder is made on the basis of deficits in three
our purposes, we will characterize mental health by two domains of criteria: (a) reciprocal social behavior; (b)
elements: (a) an absence of a major mental health condition language development; and (c) repetitive/stereotypic
or psychological distress such as anxiety, depression and behaviors. A child’s autism symptoms will be demon-
loss of emotional or behavioral control, (b) psychological strated in the level of the severity of autism based on the
wellbeing (Brown et al. 2003; Diener et al. 2008). Mental above criteria. In a comprehensive study, Constantino et al.
health can be seen as a continuum along which an indi- (2004) found a singular underlying factor of major effect
vidual’s mental health may have many different possible across the three domains of criteria for autistic disorders.
values. Mental wellness is generally viewed as a positive Moreover, differences within the autistic syndrome are not
attribute, such that a person can reach enhanced levels of recognized by an item endorsement profile, but by the
mental health, even if they do not have any diagnosable degree of impairment that existed (mild, moderate or
mental health condition. This definition of mental health severe) across all three DSM-IV criterion domains for
highlights emotional wellbeing, the capacity to live a full autistic disorders.

123
882 J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:879–890

The Main Hypothesis (43.7%). Some 3.4% have only one child and 6.9% have
four or more children. The economic status of most of the
The process of selecting research variables had two stages. research subjects is above average (66.7%), 28.7% reported
First, the integrative theory imposed the choice of the an average economic situation and a minority (4.6%)
broad variables: resources and adjustment to stress. Sec- declared their economic situation was low. About two-
ond, within each broad variable we were free to choose the thirds (67.4%) of the families are members of one of the
variables defined as resources and stress variables. A associations for children with developmental disabilities
review of the literature on resources led us to choose the and about one-third (32.6%) reported that they do not
variables of: sense of coherence, locus of control and social belong to any such association.
support. The sense of coherence is a very common variable The PDD participants were mostly male (86%) and aged
in psychology literature. Coherence—a global worldview between 6 and 16 years (mean = 10.3, SD = 3.1).
on stress situations comprised of comprehensibility, All the children are the parents’ biological offspring.
meaningfulness and manageability—is a very important Regarding the characteristics of the disability, this research
variable for interpreting stress situations. Locus of control did not include children with Rett’s Disorder, Childhood
adds to the world view of coherence the personal percep- Disintegrative Disorder or Asperger’s Disorder. The most
tion in that it examines to what extent a person believes in common diagnosis among the children was PDD: most of
his or her ability to control every day life. Social support the children (81%) were diagnosed as suffering from PDD
expands on the variable of resources and adds the parents’ and the rest (19%) as suffering from PDDNOS (Pervasive
perspective of their immediate environment. Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise Specified). Most of
Selecting the variables of adjustment to stress: mental the children in the research are boys (86%). Over half of
health and the quality of marriage, are in accordance with the children in the research are first-born (56.3%).
the family stress approach and the theories of stress relating Most of the children were treated with integrative ther-
to the individual (Folkman and Lazarus 1985; Lazarus apeutic approach (65.1%) and the rest (34.9%) with a spe-
1993; Lazarus and Folkman 1984; McCubbin and Patterson cific therapeutic approach. Most of them (49.4%) had a
1983; McCubbin and Thompson 1987). These approaches moderate level of functioning, about one-third (31%)
stress the mental health of the individual and the quality of functioned at a low level and the rest (19.5%) at a high level.
the marriage as key variables for parental adjustment. One-third of the children in the sample need supervision for
All of these briefly reviewed variables have never pre- most of the day, 30% need supervision for a reasonable
viously been put together in one multidimensional model. number of hours a day, 24% need supervision 24 h a day
Our main hypothesis is that the resources (sense of and 12.6% only need a few hours of supervision a day.
coherence, locus of control and social support), will be
negatively correlated with parental stress. The more social Measurements
support parents have and the greater their sense of coher-
ence and internal locus of control, the less stress they will There is an arsenal of valid and reliable measurements for
report in parenting their autistic child. The intensity of the the variables suggested in the model. For example, in a
stress will be also negatively correlated with parental review, Lefcourt (1991) described 18 different measures
adjustment (mental health and quality of marriage). designed to assess control beliefs among different groups
and for different goals and situations. Consequently, we
preferred those measurements that were translated and used
Method successfully in Hebrew and were reported in scientific lit-
erature with high satisfactory Cronbach alphas.
Participants
Level of Stress in Raising an Autistic Child
The participants were 176 fathers and mothers (88 married
couples) of children who had been diagnosed with PDD The Questionnaire of Resources and Stress (QRS-F) scale
(Pervasive Developmental Disorders) by clinical psychol- was translated to Hebrew (Raif and Rimmerman 1993) and
ogists or medical doctors. The parents were 22–50 years of adapted for the purposes of this research. The scale consists
age (mean = 35.39, SD = 6.22) and most of them had a of 51 statements describing the negative and positive
college education (70.7%). Based on their education and impact of the autistic child on the family (e.g., I worry what
income, the parents belong to the middle and upper class. might happen to my child in the future when I cannot take
Most of the research subjects (79.3%) live in towns and care of him). The questionnaire used 5-point Likert-like
the rest (20.7%) live in non-urban settlements. The number scales (ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly
of children in the family ranges from 2 (46%) to three agree). The level of stress was determined from an average

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:879–890 883

score of items. The version in the present study obtained agree). The version for the mental health scale in the
high internal consistency (Cronbach alpha = 0.94). present study obtained high internal consistency (Cronbach
alpha = 0.96).
Antonovsky (1993) Sense of Coherence Scale (SOC):
Quality of Marriage
The SOC scale consists of 29 items organized in three
categories: (a) Comprehensibility: ‘‘When you talk to The quality of marriage scale (Olson et al. 1987) consists
people, do you have the feeling that they don’t understand of 95 items on which each parent registered the quality of
you?’’; (b) Manageability: ‘‘When you have a difficult the marriage in 9 dimensions: (a) personality traits, (b)
problem, the choice of a solution is always confusing and communication between the couple, (c) conflict resolution,
hard to find?’’; and (c) Meaningfulness: ‘‘Do you anticipate (d) financial management, (e) spending leisure time, (f)
that your personal life in the future will be totally without sexual contact and affection, (g) parenting and child care,
meaning, or full of meaning and purpose?’’ The question- (h) relationships with extended family members and (i) role
naire used a 7-point Likert scale, from never (1) to always equality. The participants scored their answers on 5-point
(7). A higher score indicates a higher sense of coherence. Likert-type scales (ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to
Criteria validity for the SOC was established by Anto- 5 = strongly agree). The version for the quality of mar-
novsky (1993) by presenting significant correlations riage scale in the present study obtained high internal
between the SOC and measures of stress, health and well- consistency (Cronbach alpha = 0.92).
being, attitudes and behavior. The version in the present
study for SOC obtained high internal consistency (Cron- Child Autism Symptom Severity
bach alpha = 0.90).
Each child’s autism symptoms were assessed by the parents
Locus of Control and by caregivers who have accompanied the child for at
least 2 years. Caregivers included preschool teachers,
The locus of control scale (Rotter 1975) consists of 14 items special education teachers, paramedical professionals,
on which individuals express their explanations about clinical communications therapists, occupational therapists
events in their lives on a 5-point Likert scale, from never (1) and educational psychologists.
to always (5). Factor analysis showed that there are two Each parent and caregiver reported the child autism
factors: internals (e.g. my child’s progress depends on my symptoms on the ABC scale (Autism Behavior Checklist)
efforts) and externals (e.g. No one can change the fate of developed by Krug and Arick (2003). The scale consists of
my child). The version in the present study for both internal 57 items on 5 dimensions: (a) using senses, (b) social
and external locus of control obtained high internal con- connections, (c) using objects and body, (d) language, (e)
sistency respectively (Cronbach alpha = 0.74 and = 0.80). help seeking. The Cronbach alpha for the autism behavior
checklist scale is 0.95.
Social Support In the questionnaire, the measurement scale was expan-
ded from ‘‘correct’’–‘‘incorrect’’ (a dichotomous scale) into a
Social support scale (Dunst et al. 1997) consists of 20 items 5-point scale where 1 denotes ‘‘not at all correct’’ and 5
on which each parent registered the amount of support they denotes ‘‘correct to a very great extent’’. From the ques-
felt they had from family, friends and different institutions tionnaire, one measure was generated according to the
(e.g., How much help did you receive from your friends? average of the subject’s assessments for the 57 items. The
From social workers?) The participants scored their higher the score, the higher the severity of the symptoms.
answers on 5-point Likert-type scales (ranging from
1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree). The version Demographics
for the family support scale in the present study obtained
high internal consistency (Cronbach alpha = 0.86). The following demographic data were elicited from the
parents: Residence, age, education, family income and
Mental Health their children’s age, gender, school, diagnosis of autism
and treatment history.
The mental health scale (Veit and Ware 1983) consists of
38 items on which each parent scored a global feeling of Procedures
distress and physical health (Do you feel lonely?). The
participants registered their answers on 5-point Likert-type Parents were approached through parents’ associations or
scales (ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly community treatment centers. In the first stage, several

123
884 J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:879–890

formal permissions were received to ensure that there were the results reported here include only the path analysis and
no ethical problems. The parents were informed of the not all the coefficients matrix and regression that support
nature and purpose of the study and were invited to par- such analysis. Path coefficients were calculated on the basis
ticipate. In the second stage, a pilot study with 10 parents of within-report Pearson rs converted to r.s. The fit of this
was conducted. All the measurements were tested and model was good, NFI = .99, CFI = .99, RMCEA = .01,
changes were made accordingly. In the final stage, all the X2 = 4.23, p = .52.
participants received the questionnaires in random order Figure 1 shows that the path coefficients of the resources
and answered them at home. (the exogenous variable) of social support (b = -0.24;
p \ .001) and internal locus of control (b = -0.37; p \
.001) and external locus of control (b = 0.25; p \ .001)) to
Results stress are high and significant. However, sense of coherence
is not correlated with stress but with both variables of
An analysis of the results was based on each parent sepa- parental adjustment, mental health (b = 0.21; p \ .001) and
rately since the integrative theory underpinning this marriage quality (b = 0.24; p \ .001). Parental mental
research (resources and stress) is based on the subjective health has no path coefficient to child symptom severity but
perception of the individual regarding his or her resources quality of marriage has a negative path coefficient to child
and stress. (Folkman and Lazarus 1985; Lazarus 1993, symptom severity (b = -0.23; p \ .001). The intensity of
2001; Lazarus and Folkman 1984;). Consequently, each parental stress is positively correlated with child symptom
parent completed a self-reporting questionnaire that mea- severity (b = 0.49; p \ .001).
sures his or her resources or perceptions of stress and Internal locus of control is negatively correlated to
personal coping with caring for the child. Accordingly, we quality of marriage (b = -0.21; p \ .001) and to mental
performed separate SEM path analyses for mothers and health (b = -0.19; p \ .01). Parental stress is negatively
fathers for simultaneous estimation of the relationships correlated to quality of marriage (b = -0.35; p \ .001)
among the variables. Although the number of subjects was and to mental health (b = -0.53; p \ .001).
relatively small (88 subjects), it still complies with the Separate path analysis for fathers (Fig. 2) and mothers
rules of the SEM test (Tabachnick and Fidell 2007). The (Fig. 3) were calculated. The fit of the model for fathers
results of the analysis for fathers and mothers separately was good, (NFI = .96, CFI = .99, RMCEA = .07,
were lower than the results of the model in which the X2 = 7.07, p = .22) as well for the mothers (NFI = .97,
parents were entered as individuals. As such we will CFI = .99, RMCEA = .01, X2 = 4.88, p = .43). It seems
present the path model and path coefficients for the vari- that there are only two minor differences between the two
ables when parents are analyzed as individuals. models. The first difference relates to internal locus of
The path analysis was performed using the structural control and quality of marriage. In the fathers’ model there
equation modeling program within the AMOS 5 procedure is no relationship between internal locus of control and
(Arbuckle 2003). For simplicity and ease of explanation, quality of marriage, while in the mothers’ model there is a

Social
support
Quality of
-.24***
marriage

.24*** -.23***
Sense of -.35***
coherence -.21***

Parental Child autism


.49***
stress symptom severity
Internal -.37***
locus of .21***
control -.53***
-.19**

.25*** Parental mental


External health
locus of
control
*P<.05 **P<.01 ***P<.001

Fig. 1 Path model and path coefficients for fathers and mothers

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:879–890 885

Social
support
Quality of
-.29***
.33*** marriage

Sense of -.24**
-.21*
coherence

Parental Child autism


.51***
stress symptom severity
Internal -.38***
locus of .53***
control -.33***

.25**
Parental mental
External health
locus of
control
*P<.05 **P<.01 ***P<.001

Fig. 2 Path model and path coefficients for the fathers

Social
support
Quality of
-.17*
marriage
.44***
Sense of -.19*
-.31***
coherence -.23**
-.30***
Parental Child autism
.47***
stress symptom severity
Internal -.41***
locus of .34***
control -.48***

.21*
Parental mental
External health
locus of
control
*P<.05 **P<.01 ***P<.001

Fig. 3 Path model and path coefficients for mothers

negative path coefficient (b = -0.23; p \ .01) between symptoms. After presenting the general picture we can now
internal locus of control and quality of marriage. The focus on each variable.
second difference relates to sense of coherence and
parental stress. In the fathers’ model there is no relation- Sense of Coherence (Soc)
ship between sense of coherence and parental stress while
in the mothers’ model there is a negative path coefficient The results show positive pathway coefficients between
(b = -0.30; p \ .001) between sense of coherence and SOC and mental health (0.21) and quality of marriage
parental stress. (0.24) but not a straight path to stress. The connection of
SOC to stress is through mental health (-0.53) and quality
of marriage (-0.35). These results support other researches
Discussion that showed positive correlations between SOC and mental
health, wellbeing and adjustment and a negative correlation
The present data provided support for the multivariate with distress (Antonovsky 1993; Chumbler et al. 2004).
model that predicts relationships among parental resources The three elements of sense of coherence constitute
to parental stress and the severity of their autistic children a general view towards explaining the world as

123
886 J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:879–890

comprehensible, manageable and meaningful. Parents with Stress, Mental Health and Marriage Quality
a high SOC, feel generally confident about being able to
cope with general stressors (Johnson 2004; Sagy and The negative path coefficient among stress and mental
Antonovsky 2000). However, this general view is not health (-0.53) and quality of marriage (-0.35) is sup-
correlated to the specific elements of the stressors in ported also by other researches (Abbeduto et al. 2004;
parenting an autistic child but rather with a general view Eisenhower et al. 2005; Fisman et al. 2000; Pakenham
about mental health and quality of marriage. As asserted by et al. 2005; White and Hastings 2004). While talking with
Antonovsky (1987), SOC lowers stress and promotes the participants after they answered the questionnaires we
health by facilitating the application of various resources could visualize the stressors: giving up vacations and per-
within the individual to cope with stressors. Thus, parents’ sonal dreams, conflicts about the appropriate treatment for
confidence in their parenting role and their acceptance of the child, medical crises, the complex management of
their child’s behaviors and feelings may be more proximal multiple caregivers and a great deal of concern about the
cognitive factors that mediate the relationship between child’s future independent capabilities. These stressors
SOC and perceived stress (Mak et al. 2007). create endless fluctuations between hope and despair, fear
and courage, optimism and pessimism and between help-
lessness and control.
Locus of Control
The capacity to distinguish global judgments of the
relationship from daily experiences of specific relationship
Internal locus of control has a straight pathways negative
events requires cognitive effort. Stress may magnify the
coefficients with parental stress (-0.37), with quality of
importance of these daily experiences for the individuals’
marriage (-0.21) and with mental health (-0.19). External
global judgments of their relationships. In contrast, parents
locus of control has straight pathways positive coefficient
who are less stressed may find it easier to dismiss the
with stress (0.25). These results are compatible with other
significance of these daily experiences, which allows them
research that showed that internal locus of control is one of
to maintain stable levels of global satisfaction even though
the main predictors of stress in parenting children with
their daily experiences fluctuate over time. In this way,
severe problems (Jones and Passey 2005). The parents with
stress may act as a double-edged sword, increasing the
a high internal locus of control believe that their outcomes
parents’ likelihood of experiencing negative relationship
and experiences are at least partially shaped by their own
events while simultaneously hindering their capacity to
actions. Consequently, they actively deal with stressors in
process specific relationship information adaptively.
the hope of overcoming them. A more external locus of
Maintaining a relationship requires that parents success-
control suggests less activity, and a conformity more ready
fully navigate the ups and downs of daily relationship
to capitulate when confronting stressful life events. As
experiences. Greater levels of external stress may deplete
such, the results show a positive correlation between
parents of the resources necessary for positive relationship
external locus of control and stress. Namely, the more
functioning (Baumeister 2002).
external the locus of control, the more stress parents have.
These results demonstrate the utility of locus of control as a
Child Autism Symptom Severity
predictor of how parents cope with stress.
A child’s severity of symptoms is predicted directly only
Social Support by quality of marriage (-0.23) and parental stress (0.49). It
is important to note that the significant correlation between
Social support has only one negative path coefficient to quality of marriage and child symptoms supports many
stress (-0.24). Social support indirectly affects parental findings in the literature. For example, parents to autistic
adjustment and the child’s severity of symptoms through children reported on lower levels of satisfactions from
stress. It seems that parents who perceive that they are marriage than other parents (Higgins et al. 2005). It seems
helped and can attain the understanding, cooperation, that extreme conflicts and very bad quality of marriage may
assistance, and appraisal of close or significant persons feel increase the number of actual stressors that parents expe-
less stress. Social support is a resource provided by others rience. In other words, the experience of a very bad mar-
and may be instrumental, tangible, informational, or emo- riage—unhappy parents–perceive more external events as
tional. Receiving support decreases stress by increasing the negative (e.g., Bodenmann and Shantinath 2004) and this
feeling of control and gives meaning to individuals’ lives reduces their resources to cope with the child’s needs.
by virtue of motivating them to give in return, to feel Although there is no path coefficient between mental
obligated, and to be attached to their ties (Bromley et al. health and the severity of a child’s symptoms, the corre-
2004; Hobfoll 2001). lation is quite satisfactory and significant (-0.23). It seems

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:879–890 887

that the very high path coefficient between stress and the compound from volunteers. The parents who volunteered
severity of child’s symptoms (0.49) is contaminating the for the research are naturally from a high socioeconomic
variance in mental health variable. The Amos technique is level and we couldn’t have a normal distribution on the
like a very big telescope that has problems to detect minor socioeconomic variable. As a result, the external validity of
relationships. the research is limited.
In general, the findings of this study are contrary to
some other findings that report positive changes in parents Control Beliefs and Self Regulation as Main Variables
of children with developmental disorders (e.g., Brown et al. for Further Research
2003; Summers et al. 2005). These positive changes as a
result of stressors are often referred to as ‘‘stress-related It seems that the explanations of the results of this research
growth’’ sometimes called post-traumatic growth (Park and emphasize the concept of control. Sense of coherence does
Fenster 2004). In this study, the processes involved in not directly measure the ‘‘control’’ concept. Moreover,
confronting the stressors did not promote broadened per- even locus of control which has been proven a good pre-
spectives, new coping skills, and the development of per- dictor is also narrow and limited in scope. A strong sense of
sonal and social resources (Armeli et al. 2001; Park and control may facilitate use of active coping and support-
Fenster 2004; Tedeschi and Calhoun 1995). Partial results seeking methods, encourage persistence and promote
that had not entered the path analysis indicate the possi- domain-specific satisfaction and affect (Lent 2004).
bility that the parents’ appraisals of the stress was not Research shows that control could not be a superordinate
positive–a worthy challenge, but as a threat to be variable that organizes self beliefs. Factor structures basi-
overcome. cally confirm that the component beliefs are distinct from
one another, with each typically forming its own factorial
Limitations and Further Research dimension. As with many popular variables, control has
become a multivariate multidimensional construct that is
The main strength of this research is the successful vali- both complex and vague (Shell and Husman 2008).
dation of a multivariate model with an exceptionally large Out of this complexity and vagueness of the concept of
sample (176). Despite this strength, there are several lim- control, we suggest adding three main variables that are
itations to the current study. First, the participants were not strongly relate to it: Self efficacy (Bandura 2006), causal
a representative sample due to the constraints of accepting attributions (Weiner 2006) and self regulation (Kaplan
only those who agreed to participate. Consequently, there 2008). Together with sense of coherence and locus of
is no way to ensure the sample is truly representative and control, these five variables embody the concept of control
thus the reader must be more cautious in generalizing the and will have a stronger predictive validity for stress
results (external validity). Second, the differences between and adjustment. Such validity should be located in a well
fathers (Fig. 2) and mothers (Fig. 3) are minor and we can structured theory that would validate the terms used,
deduce that the relationships among the variables are quite the relationships between them, and the choice of
similar. There are contrary result about the differences measurements.
between fathers and mothers relating coping with raising It is important to mention that these five variables are
handicapped children. Few researches found differences also problematic (Hall et al. 2003; Schmeichel and Bau-
between mothers and fathers (e.g., Bristol et al. 1988; meister 2004; Shell and Husman 2008) and their definitions
Rimmerman et al. 2003). Other researches did not find such and measurements have certain overlapping elements. For
differences (e.g., Altiere and Kluge 2009; Keller and Honig example, careful distinctions should be made between
2004). It is quite difficult to integrate the researches and to locus of control linked with expectancies about the future,
find a clear answer about the differences between fathers and attributional style which is linked with explanations for
and mothers. This is due to the fact the each research is past outcomes. A thorough analysis is needed to formulate
focusing on different variables, different measures and valid and reliable measurements. Moreover, specific
wide range of handicapped participants. Moreover, we attention must be paid so that each of the variables is
couldn’t find a research with similar variables and multi- measured both in the specific domain (raising an autistic
variate model with path analysis method. It seems that child) and the general domain (for review see Kaniel
further research has to be done in a multivariate designs 2010).
regarding the differences between fathers and mothers.
Regarding the socio-economic variable, one can see that Focusing on Processes
most of the research subjects were above average. The
reason for this is that it was not possible to obligate parents Another important recommendation is to focus on pro-
to participate in the study as such the sample was cesses. All of the data reported in this research are

123
888 J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:879–890

correlational and different processes (appraisal, control) are American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical
suggested to explain the path coefficients. It is like ‘‘putting manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Amer-
ican Psychiatric Association.
the players on the court’’ and drawing some inferences Amirkhan, J. H., & Greaves, H. (2003). Sense of coherence and
about the game. The main challenge for further research is stress: The mechanics of a healthy disposition. Psychology &
to identify the processes among the variables. This can be Health, 18(1), 31–62.
done by using a different paradigm of methodology Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unraveling the mystery of health: How
people manage stress and stay well. San-Francisco: Jossey Bass.
because in the usual experimental designs, however Antonovsky, A. (1993). The structure and properties of the sense of
important universal principles are, they sometimes obscure coherence scale. Social Science and Medicine, 36, 725–733.
how real people actually function. By attempting to Arbuckle, J. L. (2003). Amos 5.0 update to the Amos user’s guide.
describe only the average, one runs the risk of describing Chicago, IL: Small Waters Corporation, SPSS Inc.
Armeli, S., C Gunthert, K. C., & Cohen, L, I. I. (2001). Stressor
nobody in particular. Process frameworks emphasize reci- appraisals, coping, and post-event outcomes: The dimensionality
procal relations between variables. As such, they should be and antecedents of stress-related growth. Journal of Social and
validated by combining quantitative and qualitative meth- Clinical Psychology, 20, 366–395.
odologies (stressful incidents in parenting), ‘thinking Armstrong, M. I., Birnie-Lefcovitch, S., & Unger, M. T. (2005).
Pathways between social support, family wellbeing, quality of
aloud’ protocols (Ericsson 2003), narrative and case study parenting, and child resilience: What we know. Journal of Child
design. Namely, studying processes as they unfold natu- and Family Studies, 14, 269–270.
rally in the course of daily life and people’s naive, or Bandura, A. (2006). Guide for constructing self-efficacy scales. In
‘‘lay,’’ theories (Molden and Dweck 2006). Each parent F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Adolescence and education, Vol. 5:
Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 307–337). Greenwich,
should be treated as a system and characterized by a set of CT: Information Age.
interconnected variables that affect each other over the Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Ego depletion and self-control failure: An
course of time (Hollenstein 2007). Thus, in addition to the energy model of the self’s executive function. Self and Identity,
controlled experimental (in vitro) laboratory studies, we 1, 129–136.
Bodenmann, G., & Shantinath, S. D. (2004). The Couples Coping
also saw merit in the naturalistic (in vivo) view (Dunbar Enhancement Training (CCET): A new approach to prevention
and Blanchette 2001). of marital distress based upon stress and coping. Family
Another aspect of combining approaches is to perform a Relations, 53, 477–484.
longitudinal study of parents coping with autism over a Bristol, M. M., Gallagher, J. J., & Schopler, E. (1988). Mothers and
fathers of young developmentally disabled and non-disabled
period of approximately a decade. Researches show that boys: Adaptation and spousal support. Developmental Psychol-
parents of children with autism have changed over time ogy, 24, 441–451.
(Gray 2006). Such methodology will have much more Bromley, J., Hare, D. J., Davison, K., & Emerson, E. (2004). Mothers
power to establish causal linkage between personal and supporting children with autistic spectrum disorders: Social
support, mental health status and satisfaction with services.
external resources, stress experience, parental adjustment Autism, 8, 409–423.
and child symptom severity. These suggested studies may Brown, I., Anand, S., Isaacs, B., Baum, N., & Fung, W. L. (2003).
not only enhance our understanding of the mechanisms Family quality of life: Canadian results from an international
behind effective coping and mental health, but also con- study. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 15,
207–230.
tribute to the enhancement of psycho-social services for Chumbler, N. R., Rittman, M., Van-Puymbroeck, M., Vogel, W. B.,
children and caregivers and the development of effective & Qnin, H. (2004). The sense of coherence, burden, and
service models that could benefit both the families and their depressive symptoms in informal caregivers during the first
communities. month after stroke. International Journal of Geriatric Psychia-
try, 19, 944–953.
Constantino, J. N., Gruber, C. P., Davis, S., Hayes, S., Passanante, N.,
& Przybeck, T. (2004). The factor structure of autistic traits.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 719–726.
Diener, E., Lucas, R., & Kesebir, P. (2008). Benefits of account
References wellbeing for societies and for psychological sciences. Applied
Psychology: An International Review, 37, 37–55.
Abbeduto, L., Seltzer, M. M., Shattuck, P., Krauss, M. W., Dunbar, K., & Blanchette, I. (2001). The in vivo/in vitro approach to
Orsmond, G., & Murphy, M. M. (2004). Psychological well- cognition: The case of analogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
being and coping in mothers of youths with autism, Down 5(8), 334–339.
syndrome, or Fragile X syndrome. American Journal on Mental Dunst, C. J., Trivette, C. M., & Jodry, W. (1997). Influences of social
Retardation, 109, 237–254. support on children with disabilities and their families. In
Altiere, M. J., & Kluge, S. (2009). Family functioning and coping M. J. Guralnick (Ed.), The effectiveness of early intervention
behaviors in parents of children with autism. Journal of Child (pp. 499–522). Baltimore: Brookes.
and Family Studies, 18, 83–92. Eisenhower, A. S., Baker, B. L., & Blacher, J. (2005). Preschool
Al-Yagon, M., & Margalit, M. (2009). Positive and negative affect children with intellectual disability: Syndrome specificity,
among mothers of children with intellectual disabilities. The behavior problems and maternal well-being. Journal of Intellec-
British Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 55, 109–127. tual Disability Research, 49, 657–671.

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:879–890 889

Ericsson, A. K. (2003). Valid and non-reactive verbalization of Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Coping theory and research: Past, present, and
thoughts during performance of tasks. Journal of Consciousness future. Psychosomatic Medicine, 55, 234–247.
Studies, 10(9–10), 1–18. Lazarus, R. S. (2001). Relational meaning and discrete emotions. In
Fisman, S., Wolf, L., Ellison, D., & Freeman, T. (2000). A K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal
longitudinal study of siblings of children with chronic disabil- processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 37–67).
ities. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 45, 369–375. New York: Oxford University Press.
Florian, V., & Findler, L. (2001). Mental health and marital Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress appraisal, and coping.
adaptation among mothers of children with cerebral palsy. New York: Springer.
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 71, 358–367. Lefcourt, H. M. (1991). Locus of control. In J. P. Robinson,
Folkman, S. (1997). Positive psychological states and coping with P. R. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of
severe stress. Social Science and Medicine, 45, 1207–1221. personality and social psychological attitudes (pp. 413–499).
Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1985). If it changes it must be a San Diego: Academic Press.
process: Study of emotion and coping during three stages of a Lent, R. W. (2004). Toward a unifying theoretical and practical
college examination. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- perspective on wellbeing and psychosocial adjustment. Journal
chology, 48, 150–170. of Counseling Psychology, 51, 482–509.
Gray, D. E. (2006). Coping over time: The parents of children with Lindstrom, B., & Eriksson, M. (2005). Salutogenesis. Journal of
autism. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 50, 970–976. Epidemiological Community Health, 59, 440–442.
Hall, S., French, D. P., & Marteau, T. M. (2003). Causal attributions Mak, W. W. S., Ho, A. H. Y., & Law, R. W. (2007). Sense of
following serious unexpected negative events: A systematic coherence, parenting attitudes and stress among mothers of
review. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22(5), children with autism in Hong Kong. Journal of Applied Research
515–536. in Intellectual Disabilities, 20, 157–167.
Hastings, R. P., Kovshoff, H., Ward, N. J., Ward, N. J., Espinosa, F. McCubbin, H. I., & Patterson, J. M. (1983). Family transitions:
D., Brown, T., et al. (2005). Systems analysis of stress and Adaptation to stress. In H. I. McCubbin & C. R. Figley (Eds.),
positive perceptions in mothers and fathers of preschool children Stress and the family. Vol 1: Coping with normative transitions
with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, (pp. 5–25). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
35, 635–644. McCubbin, H. I., & Thompson, A. I. (1987). Family assessment
Higgins, D. J., Bailey, S. R., & Pearce, J. C. (2005). Factors inventories for research and practice. Madison: University of
associated with functioning style and coping strategies of Wisconsin-Madison.
families with a child with an autism spectrum disorder. Autism, Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding ‘‘meaning’’ in
9, 125–137. psychology: A lay theories approach to self-regulation, social
Hobfoll, S. E. (2001). The influence of culture, community, and the perception, and social development. American Psychologist,
nested-self in the stress process: Advancing conservation of 61(3), 192–203.
resources theory. Applied Psychology: An International Review, Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2009). Stress and reactivity to daily
50(3), 337–338. relationship experiences: How stress hinders adaptive processes
Hollenstein, T. (2007). State space grids: Analyzing dynamics across in marriage. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
development. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 97(3), 435–450.
31, 384–396. Oelofsen, N., & Richardson, P. (2006). Sense of coherence and
Johnson, M. (2004). Approaching the salutogenesis of sense of parenting stress in mothers and fathers of preschool children with
coherence: The role of ‘active’ self-esteem and coping. British developmental disability. Journal of Intellectual and develop-
Journal of Health Psychology, 9, 412–432. mental Disability, 31, 1–12.
Jones, J., & Passey, J. (2005). Family adaptation, coping and Olson, D. H., Fournier, D. G., & Druckman, J. M. (1987). Counselors’
resources: Parents of children with developmental disabilities manual for Prepare/Enrich. MN: Prepare/Enrich.
and behavior problems. Journal on Developmental Disabilities, Olsson, M. B., & Hawang, C. P. (2002). Sense of coherence in parents
11, 31–46. of children with different developmental disabilities. Journal of
Kaniel, S. (2010). Domain specific vs domain general: Implications Intellectual Disability Research, 46, 548–559.
for dynamic assessment. Gifted Education International, 26, Pakenham, K. I., Samios, C., & Sofronoff, K. (2005). Adjustment in
93–107. mothers of children with asperger syndrome: An application of
Kaplan, A. (2008). Clarifying meta-cognition, self-regulation, and the double ABCX model of family adjustment. Autism, 9,
self-regulated learning: What’s the purpose? Educational Psy- 191–212.
chology Review, 20(4), 477–484. Park, C. L., & Fenster, J. R. (2004). Stress-related growth: Predictors
Keller, D., & Honig, A. S. (2004). Maternal and paternal stress in of occurrence and correlates with psychological adjustment.
families with school-aged children with disabilities. American Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, 195–215.
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 74, 337–348. Perry, A., Harris, K., & Minnes, P. (2005). Family environments and
Konstantareas, M. M., & Papageorgiou, V. (2006). Effects of family harmony: An exploration across severity, age, and type of
temperament, symptom severity and level of functioning on dd. Journal on Developmental Disabilities, 11, 17–29.
maternal stress in Greek children and youth with ASD. Autism, Pisula, E. (2007). A comparative study of stress profiles in mothers of
10, 593–607. children with autism and those of children with Down’s
Kormanik, M. B., & Rocco, T. S. (2009). Internal versus external syndrome. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabil-
control of reinforcement: A review of the locus of control ities, 20, 274–278.
construct. Human Resource Development Review, 8(4), Raif, R., & Rimmerman, A. (1993). Parental attitudes to out-of-home
463–485. placement of young children with developmental disabilities.
Krug, D. A., & Arick, J. R. (2003). Krug Asperger’s disorder index. International Journal of Rehabilitation Research, 16, 97–105.
Austin, TX: Pro-Ed Inc. Rimmerman, A., Turkel, L., & Crossman, R. (2003). Perception of
Lavee, Y., & Olson, D. H. (1991). Family types and response to child development, child-related stress and dyadic adjustment:
stress. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53, 786–798. Pair analysis of married couples of young children with

123
890 J Autism Dev Disord (2011) 41:879–890

developmental disabilities. Journal of Intellectual and Develop- daily hassles and problem-focused coping. American Journal on
mental Disability, 28, 188–195. Mental Retardation, 111, 1–14.
Risdal, D., & Singer, G. H. S. (2004). Marital adjustment in parents of Summers, J. A., Poston, D. J., Turnbull, A. P., Marquis, J., Hoffman,
children with disabilities: An historical review and meta- L., Mannan, H., et al. (2005). Conceptualizing and measuring
analysis. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe family quality of life. Journal of Intellectual Disability
Disabilities, 29, 95–103. Research, 49, 777–783.
Rotter, J. B. (1975). Some problems and misconceptions related to the Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate
construct of internal versus external control of reinforcement. statistics (5th ed.). London: Pearson/A&B.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 56–67. Tedeschi, R., & Calhoun, L. (1995). Trauma and transformation:
Sagy, S., & Antonovsky, H. (2000). The development of the sense of Growing in the aftermath of suffering. Thousand Oaks, CA:
coherence: A retrospective study of early life experiences in the Sage.
family. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, Tehee, E., Honan, R., & Hevey, D. (2009). Factors contributing to
51, 155–166. stress in parents of individuals with autistic spectrum disorders.
Saloviita, T., Itälinna, M., & Leinonen, E. (2003). Explaining the Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 22,
parental stress of fathers and mothers caring for a child with 34–42.
intellectual disability: A double ABCX model. Journal of Vancouver, J. B., Kristen M. More, K. M., & Yoder, R. J. (2008).
Intellectual Disability Research, 47, 300–312. Self-Efficacy and resource allocation: Support for a non-
Schmeichel, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2004). Self-regulatory monotonic, discontinuous model. Journal of Applied Psychol-
strength. In R. F. Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of ogy, 93(1), 35–47.
self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 84–98). Veit, C. T., & Ware, J. E. (1983). The structure of psychological
New York: Guilford Press. stress and well-being in general populations. Journal of
Schulz, M. S., Cowan, P. A., Pape Cowan, C., & Brennan, R. T. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 730–742.
(2004). Coming home upset: Gender, marital satisfaction, and Weiner, B. (2006). Social motivation, justice, and the moral
the daily spillover of workday experience into couples’ interac- emotions: An attributional approach. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
tions. Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 250–263. Associates, Publishers.
Shapiro, B. K., & Accardo, P. J. (2008). Autism frontiers: Clinical Wheaton, B. (1997). The nature of chronic stress. In B. H. Gottlieb
issues and innovations. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes (Ed.), Coping with chronic stress (pp. 43–73). New York:
Publishing. Plenum.
Shell, D. F., & Husman, J. (2008). Control, motivation, affect, and White, N., & Hastings, R. P. (2004). Social and professional support
strategic self-regulation in the college classroom: A multidi- for parents of adolescents with severe intellectual disabilities.
mensional phenomenon. Journal of Educational Psychology, Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 17,
100(2), 443–459. 181–190.
Stoneman, Z., & Gavidia-Payn, S. (2006). Marital adjustment in
families of young children with disabilities: Associations with

123

You might also like