You are on page 1of 17

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/301244457

The Hub-and-Spoke Hypothesis of Semantic Memory

Chapter · December 2016


DOI: 10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00061-4

CITATIONS READS

8 3,031

2 authors, including:

Karalyn Patterson
University of Cambridge
362 PUBLICATIONS   34,318 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Development of a 'Mini Linguistic State Examination' View project

MEG biomarkers of incipient dementia View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Karalyn Patterson on 29 March 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

AUTHOR QUERY FORM

Book: Hickok-1611153 Please e-mail your responses and any corrections to:
Chapter: CH061 E-mail: NeurobiologyOfLanguage@elsevier.com

Dear Author,
Any queries or remarks that have arisen during the processing of your manuscript are listed below and are highlighted by flags in the proof.
(AU indicates author queries; ED indicates editor queries; and TS/TY indicates typesetter queries.) Please check your proof carefully and
answer all AU queries. Mark all corrections and query answers at the appropriate place in the proof using on-screen annotation in the PDF file.
For a written tutorial on how to annotate PDFs, click http://www.elsevier.com/__data/assets/pdf_file/0016/203560/Annotating-PDFs-
Adobe-Reader-9-X-or-XI.pdf. A video tutorial is also available at http://www.screencast.com/t/9OIDFhihgE9a. Alternatively, you may com-
pile them in a separate list and tick off below to indicate that you have answered the query.
Please return your input as instructed by the project manager.

Uncited references: References that occur in the reference list but are not cited in the text. Please position each reference in the text or delete
it from the reference list.
Wittgenstein (2001)

Missing references: References listed below were noted in the text but are missing from the reference list. Please make the reference list
complete or remove the references from the text.

Location in Article Query / remark

AU:1, Page 7 Please define the following if applicable: VBM, MEG, PET, ITG. ZQBX

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

S E C T I O N K

CONCEPTUAL SEMANTIC
KNOWLEDGE

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

C H A P T E R

c0061 61
The Hub-and-Spoke Hypothesis of Semantic
Memory
Karalyn Patterson1,2 and Matthew A. Lambon Ralph3
1
Neurology Unit, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK; 2MRC Cognition
and Brain Sciences Unit, Cambridge, UK; 3Neuroscience and Aphasia Research Unit, School of Psychological Sciences,
University of Manchester, Manchester, UK

s0010
61.1 INTRODUCTION two parts (Figure 61.1). First, many different modality-
specific cortical regions represent the aspects of con-
p0010 The quantity of, quality of, and speed of access to ceptual knowledge that are modality-specific: the color
semantic knowledge in the human brain is astonishing. of a camel in color regions, its shape in visual-form
Given any fragment of information, such as the word regions, the way it moves in regions that code visual
“camel,” a healthy adult human can almost instantly movement, its name in language-specific cortex, and
generate a vast amount of other related information, so on. Second, all of these regions send information
such as the ways in which camels are and are not like to and receive information from a hub component
other animals, how they look and move, where they of the semantic network, which codes semantic similar-
live, how they interact with people, that it is the name ity structure and represents concepts in a manner
of an American brand of cigarettes, that they are the that abstracts away from the specific features of
subject of a famous story by Rudyard Kipling (How the how they look or sound or move or what they are
Camel Got His Hump), and so forth. Curiously, this kind called. The links between each modality-specific region
of conceptual knowledge—although often discussed by and the hub are called spokes. Because it is the hub
philosophers—did not become a major topic of research part of this story that is (somewhat) novel, other
in cognitive science and neuroscience until relatively semantic-memory researchers—especially those critical
recently. Endel Tulving put it on the psychological map of this hypothesis—occasionally describe our theory
in 1972, when he proposed that semantic memory was as one in which concepts have a completely abstract
a qualitatively different kind of knowledge from the form of representation and reside in the hub. For exam-
episodic memory typically studied by experimental ple, Gainotti (2014) included our work in a short list of
psychologists at that time. Elizabeth Warrington put models “that assume that semantic representations. . .
it on the neuropsychological map in 1975, when she are stored in an abstract and propositional format”
demonstrated that brain disease could selectively (Gainotti, 2014, p. 7). We acknowledge that some earlier
impair semantic memory. Now, approximately 40 years expositions of our incompletely formed view may have
later, semantic memory is a commonplace object of sounded a bit like this (Patterson & Hodges, 2000),
behavioral study, brain research, and theorizing. but it is certainly not an accurate description of our cur-
p0015 Over the past 15 20 years, we and our colleagues rent hub-and-spoke hypothesis about semantic memory.
have been developing a story about the structure and We therefore begin the account of our position with
neural basis of semantic memory that we call the hub- an explanation of the crucial spoke components of
and-spoke hypothesis (Lambon Ralph, 2014; Lambon the theory, called “the importance of the spokes and
Ralph & Patterson, 2008; Lambon Ralph, Sage, Jones, & the regions from which they emanate.” Sections 61.2
Mayberry, 2010; Patterson, Nestor, & Rogers, 2007; and 61.3 argue that, although essential, the spokes
Rogers et al., 2004). The basic plot of this story comes in and their modality-specific sources are insufficient and

Neurobiology of Language. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407794-2.00061-4 3 © 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

4 61. THE HUB-AND-SPOKE HYPOTHESIS OF SEMANTIC MEMORY

Olfaction Praxis

ATL modality-invariant
Sounds Somatosensory

Verbal descriptors Visual features

f0010 FIGURE 61.1 The “hub-and-spoke” model of conceptualization. Under this hypothesis, concepts are formed by the mutual interaction of
modality-specific sources of information (the “spokes”) with a central representational hub (red) that provides additional modality-invariant
representational resource. The spokes are assumed to rely on modality-specific secondary association cortical regions while regions within the
anterior temporal region (and, perhaps, other tertiary association areas) underpin the transmodal hub. Conceptualization follows from the
joint action of the hub and spokes (see text for further details). Various computational implementations of this framework have been described
in detail and demonstrate how concepts can be coded in this framework and how data from SD and patients with unilateral temporal lobe
damage can be mimicked. From Rogers et al., 2004 and Schapiro et al., 2013.

that a transmodal hub is also necessary. Section 61.4 different locations, that also differ in principled ways
addresses reasons for locating the proposed hub in depending on the sensory or motor features of the
the anterior temporal lobe (ATL). Section 61.5 offers stimulus or response; and (iii) data from transient dis-
evidence for the bilateral nature of the ATL hub. ruption of neural function in different regions via tran-
Section 61.6, on the graded hub hypothesis, provides scranial magnetic stimulation in neurologically intact
some more specific suggestions about different roles for participants (Pobric, Jefferies, & Lambon Ralph,
various areas of the ATL. 2010b). To be sure, some specific claims about modal-
ity or category-specific effects are open to debate
(see Chen & Rogers, 2014, for a good discussion). To
s0015 61.2 THE IMPORTANCE OF THE be even more sure, brain function is almost infinitely
SPOKES AND THE REGIONS FROM more subtle and complex than our limited ability to
WHICH THEY EMANATE study it and think about it (Patterson & Plaut, 2009).
This means that any claim for a simple relationship
p0020 It now seems almost incontrovertible that our between a specific category and a specific brain
knowledge about the modality-specific sensory and region (e.g., “the fusiform face area [FFA] in the
motor features of objects and other concepts is repre- right posterior temporal lobe is completely and
sented in or very near the same brain regions that pro- uniquely dedicated to processing faces”) is unlikely to
cess such information when it is encountered in the characterize the true state of things in the brain. We
real world (Barsalou, 2008). This view, usually called already know that areas other than the FFA respond to
either grounded or embodied cognition, is endorsed in faces (Pitcher, Walsh, Yovel, & Duchaine, 2007) and
one form or another by so many researchers that if we that damage to the FFA resulting in prosopagnosia
were to try to mention all of them, the rest of this sec- typically disrupts some abilities beyond face proces-
tion might consist of nothing but a list of references. sing (Behrmann & Plaut, 2012). Nevertheless, there is a
Several different forms of evidence converge to considerable degree of sensory-feature and motor-
support this idea, principally: (i) regionally specific feature specialization in both processing and represen-
patterns of brain activation, observed in healthy parti- tation of different kinds of things that no theory of
cipants, that differ as a function of the modality or cat- semantic representation can ignore. And it is crucial to
egory of the stimulus and/or response; (ii) patterns of emphasize that, according to embodied/grounded the-
cognitive impairment, resulting from brain lesions in ories, this specialization does not just reflect surface

K. CONCEPTUAL SEMANTIC KNOWLEDGE

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

61.3 THE INSUFFICIENCY OF THE SPOKES AND THEIR SOURCES: WHY WE NEED A HUB 5
features of stimuli: words referring to concepts of dif- activation rapidly spreads via the inter-area connec-
ferent types can also produce both brain activations in tions to other modality-specific aspects of your camel
modality-relevant sensory or motor regions (Hauk, knowledge. Straightforward enough, so why reject this
Shtyrov, & Pulvermüller, 2008; Pulvermuller, 2005; hypothesis?
Simmons et al., 2007) (see de Zubicaray, Arciuli, & First, consider semantic dementia (SD), a neurode- p0035
McMahon, 2013 for an alternative interpretation of generative condition in the spectrum of frontotemporal
some results that might seem to support necessary dementia. The characteristics of SD especially germane
activation in motor cortex when participants process to the current discussion—one neuroanatomical and
action words) and distinctive patterns of impairment four behavioral/cognitive—are as follows:
in patients with lesions in or near such sensory/motor
1. All clinical diagnoses of neurological origin are o0010
areas (Boulenger et al., 2008; Pulvermüller et al., 2010).
associated with a degree, larger or smaller, of
That is, a degree of specialization seems to apply to
variability across individual patients in “the three
the concepts or mental representations of objects, not
S’s”: site, size, and/or side of lesion. This fact, of
just objects in the real world.
course, makes it somewhat tricky to be confident
p0025 In a useful review of “embodiment and the neuro-
about the precise neuroanatomical basis of the
science of semantics,” Meteyard, Cuadrado, Bahrami,
deficits. Such variability inevitably applies to SD,
and Vigliocco (2012) placed various theories about
but it is widely agreed among researchers of
conceptual knowledge on an embodiment continuum,
neurodegenerative diseases that SD falls at the
from “strong” to “weak” to “secondary” to “absent.”
lower end of this spectrum of variability, especially
The previous paragraph summarizes our reasons for
in its early-middle phases (Acosta-Cabronero et al.,
rejecting theories that argue for purely abstract seman-
2011; Guo et al., 2013; Mion et al., 2010). To quote
tic representations with no element of embodiment.
from Hodges and Patterson (2007), “Patients with
However, we also reject the most strongly embodied
even early-stage SD show bilateral, though typically
theories for reasons described in the next section.
asymmetrical, atrophy of the ATLs; as the disease
To summarize this first section, the hub-and-spoke
progresses, the degeneration extends either caudally
hypothesis suggests that semantic knowledge is repre-
into the posterior temporal lobes or rostrally into
sented in a widely distributed brain network including
the posterior, inferior frontal lobes, or both.
modality-specific regions of cortex from which connec-
Quantitative MRI studies. . .have refined these
tions (spokes) emanate, sending activation to and receiv-
observations by showing consistent and extreme
ing activation back from another essential component of
atrophy (commonly 50 80% gray matter loss) of
the network: a transmodal semantic hub.
the polar and perirhinal cortices and the anterior
fusiform gyri (Davies, Graham, Xuereb, Williams, &
Hodges, 2004; Du, Schuff, & Kramer, 2007; Gorno-
s0020 61.3 THE INSUFFICIENCY OF THE Tempini, Dronkers, & Rankin, 2004)” (p. 1009). It
SPOKES AND THEIR SOURCES: should be noted that the hippocampus and
WHY WE NEED A HUB entorhinal cortex are also substantially abnormal in
SD (Chan, Fox, & Rossor, 2002; Williams, Nestor, &
p0030 Strongly embodied theories of conceptual knowl- Hodges, 2005). Unlike the case of Alzheimer’s
edge “effectively push semantics out into primary disease (AD), however, the medial temporal
cortical areas and make it completely dependent on atrophy in SD is much more severe in the rostral
sensory and motor systems” (Meteyard et al., 2012, than in the caudal region. Why SD patients, despite
p. 793). In other words, according to such theories, their hippocampal abnormality, do not have the
the brain network for your knowledge of a concept profoundly impaired anterograde amnesia
such as camel consists only of the specific sensory/ characteristic of AD is a fascinating and evolving
motor/linguistic features that you have experienced story (Nestor, Fryer, & Hodges, 2006; Nestor, Fryer,
concerning camels. How do strongly embodied theo- Ikeda, & Hodges, 2003), but this is not especially
ries account for the fact that, on hearing the word germane to this chapter on semantic memory.
“camel,” you can almost instantly generate all that Results of the multimodal imaging study of SD
other information mentioned in the introduction? They patients and healthy controls by Guo et al. (2013),
assume that all of the feature representations in the in agreement with many other investigations of SD,
modality-specific regions of cortex are connected to established that focal atrophy was confined to the
one another. Hearing the word “camel” initially pro- ATL bilaterally. The critical finding of Guo et al.,
duces selective activation of an auditory/linguistic however, is that this focal structural damage was
representation; then, in a pattern-completion fashion, accompanied by reduced physiological integrity

K. CONCEPTUAL SEMANTIC KNOWLEDGE

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

6 61. THE HUB-AND-SPOKE HYPOTHESIS OF SEMANTIC MEMORY

in many or all of the modality-specific regions nonverbal contexts). In object naming, for example,
demonstrated to be connected to the ATL in the instances of a category that are of moderate
control participants. Furthermore, the patients’ familiarity and typicality can no longer be
semantic deficits correlated with both the ATL discriminated correctly, resulting in errors such
atrophy and the reduced connectivity to more as naming a fox as “dog” or a zebra as “horse” or
specific regions. This one highly salient piece of calling them both “animal.” But less familiar and
research thus supports the existence and necessity atypical animals such as a seahorse or a snail even
of both the hub and the spokes. Guo et al. concluded fail to be generalized to the animal category: they
that their evidence favors a model of the semantic usually result in content-less naming responses
network in which “a critical transmodal ATL such as “a little thing” or “I don’t know” or “what
semantic hub [is] positioned to integrate functionally do you do with that?” (Patterson et al., 2007;
relevant and topographically organized links to Woollams et al., 2008). The same is true for object
distributed modality-specific regions” (p. 2,988). and word categorization: typical things are
o0015 2. At least in the prefinal stages of the disease, the overgeneralized and atypical things are
cognitive consequences of SD are largely confined undergeneralized (Mayberry, Sage, & Lambon
to disruption of semantic knowledge per se Ralph, 2011; Snowden, Griffiths, & Neary, 1996).
(Guo et al., 2013; Hodges, Patterson, Oxbury, &
Funnell, 1992; Snowden, Goulding, & Neary, 1989; The first neuroanatomical characteristic indicates p0070
Warrington, 1975) and of abilities in which semantic that the brain dysfunction in SD is typically centered on
memory plays an important interactive role the ATL, with consequent disruption to the integrity of
(Patterson et al., 2006). connections between the ATL and modality-specific
o0020 3. The deterioration of semantic memory in SD is primary and association cortices. The last four neuropsy-
pan-category and transmodal: different kinds of chological characteristics indicate that the cognitive
concepts (e.g., concrete/abstract; living/human- abnormality is relatively restricted to semantic memory
made) are all impaired, and the semantic deficits (and its impact on other tasks) and follows a highly con-
are apparent no matter what format of stimulus sistent pattern. Given this set of well-established obser-
(object, picture, spoken word, written word, vations, it seems difficult to construct an account of SD
environmental sound, taste, touch, etc.) is used to that is compatible with a semantic network consisting
probe for conceptual knowledge and no matter only of modality-specific sensory, motor, and language
what modality of response (pointing, speaking, regions of the brain. To our knowledge, there are no
writing, drawing, etc.) is used to measure reports of significant structural degradation of modality-
performance (Bozeat, Lambon Ralph, Patterson, specific regions in SD. Furthermore, if SD did involve
Garrard, & Hodges, 2000; Rogers et al., 2004). widespread degeneration of modality-specific regions,
o0025 4. The pattern of the cognitive deficits in SD invariably then it should yield cognitive deficits well beyond
reflects an interactive impact of the familiarity or semantic memory, as seen in AD. For example, visuo-
frequency of the stimulus concept (i.e., how often a spatial processing depends substantially on parietal lobe
concept is experienced) and the typicality of the regions; AD patients have parietal hypometabolism/
stimulus in its domain (i.e., how similar each atrophy with disrupted visuospatial ability; SD patients
exemplar is to the category average or “prototype”). have neither (Pengas et al., 2010). Finally, if the transmo-
In many diverse tests and tasks that have dal semantic disorder in SD did arise from widespread
manipulated the frequency and typicality of the abnormalities to modality-specific regions, then why
stimuli, significantly impaired and poorest would the pattern of disorder (the interaction between
performance by SD patients is always observed for familiarity and typicality and the nature of the errors) be
atypical and less familiar stimuli/concepts (Lambon so consistent across all modalities and tasks? It seems
Ralph et al., 2011; Patterson et al., 2006; Rogers much more plausible that the deficits in SD all emerge
et al., 2004; Woollams, Cooper-Pye, Hodges, & from disruption to a part of the semantic network that
Patterson, 2008). codes conceptual space independent of any modality-
o0030 5. The nature of the errors made by SD patients is specific features of concepts in which the structure is
anything but random and, in virtually any task, defined by similarity of instances within domains and
their errors can be described as resulting from strongly influenced by familiar and typical instances
failures of appropriate generalization and (Rogers et al., 2004).
discrimination (see Lambon Ralph & Patterson, SD provides a clear, potentially definitive line of p0075
2008, for an extended discussion of such errors in evidence against the strongest embodied theories of
both receptive and expressive experimental tasks, conceptualization, but it should also be noted that
as well as in everyday life, and in both verbal and some researchers in related fields (such as philosophy,

K. CONCEPTUAL SEMANTIC KNOWLEDGE

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

61.4 WHY SHOULD THE HUB BE CENTERED ON THE ATL? 7


e.g., Wittgenstein 1957, and cognitive science, e.g., Because no one is satisfied with scientific proposals/ p0085
Rosch, 1975; Smith & Medin, 1981) have also argued conclusions based on a single form of evidence, it is
that models based on modality-specific features alone fortunate that there are populations other than SD
might be inadequate for the formation of coherent patients and techniques other than the ones already
concepts and categories. A full exposition of the various mentioned that are compatible with a transmodal
arguments is beyond the scope of this chapter (these component of the semantic network located in the
can be found in Lambon Ralph, 2014; Lambon Ralph, ATL. In this context, it is vital to note that standard
Sage, et al., 2010; Patterson et al., 2007; Rogers & fMRI—the source of most evidence concerning locali-
McClelland, 2004), but the kernel of the issue is that zation of function in the healthy brain—has been a
the relationship between the abundance of modality- rather inadequate source of evidence concerning the
specific information and the plethora of concepts that ATL because of various methodological limitations,
emerge from them is highly complex and nonlinear. including MRI field inhomogeneities, limited field-
Our key working hypothesis, implemented computa- of-view, and the nature of the baseline task against
tionally (Rogers et al., 2004; Schapiro, McClelland, which semantic task performance is compared (Devlin
Welbourne, Rogers, & Lambon Ralph, 2013) and sup- et al., 2000; Visser, Jefferies, & Lambon Ralph, 2010).
ported neuroanatomically (Binney, Parker, & Lambon Functional imaging studies using full field-of-view
Ralph, 2012; Guo et al., 2013), is that concepts represent PET have often reported ATL activations when healthy
the joint action of distributed modality-specific regions participants perform semantic tasks (Sharp, Scott, &
(the “spokes”) with an intermediate transmodal repre- Wise, 2004; Vandenberghe, Price, Wise, Josephs, &
sentational region (the “hub”). The hub provides the Frackowiak, 1996). Furthermore, the relevance of the
key additional computational capacity to draw the ATL to semantic knowledge is supported by recent
multiple and complex sources of information together, results from: (i) distortion-corrected fMRI of this
thereby deriving coherent and generalizable concepts. region (Visser, Jefferies, Embleton, & Lambon Ralph,
2012; Visser & Lambon Ralph, 2011); (ii) multivoxel
pattern analyses of fMRI data (Peelen & Caramazza,
2012); (iii) the application of rTMS to the lateral part
s0025 61.4 WHY SHOULD THE HUB of this region (Lambon Ralph, Pobric, & Jefferies,
BE CENTERED ON THE ATL? 2009; Pobric, Jefferies, & Lambon Ralph, 2010a;
Pobric et al., 2010b; Pobric, Jefferies, & Lambon Ralph,
p0080 The principal argument here is, unsurprisingly, that 2007); and (iv) source-based analyses of MEG signals
the SD patients whose behavior strongly suggests the (Marinkovic et al., 2003).
existence of a semantic hub have atrophy consistently Three kinds of evidence combine to suggest that the p0090
centered on the ATL. It is worth noting that the best ATL might constitute a reasonable location for a trans-
hypothesis about the location of a semantic hub in modal semantic hub. Findings from nonhuman primate
the ATL has changed somewhat since the earliest physiology (Gloor, 1997) and human functional and
AU:1 proposals. Because damage (e.g., as measured by VBM structural connectivity studies (Binney et al., 2012; Guo
analyses on structural MR images in SD) appears to et al., 2013) indicate that a number of primary sensory
be maximal at the temporal pole (Brodmann area 38), and motor areas, along with their related association
that was the originally suggested hub region (Hodges cortices, connect to and merge information in the
et al., 1992; Patterson & Hodges, 2000). These earlier ATL. In addition to the rather factual nature of much
studies did not have sufficient power—because of conceptual information, semantic memory is often “fla-
relatively low patient numbers—to support correlations vored” by aspects of emotion and reward. Brain regions
between lesion location and semantic performance. vital to these aspects of experience, including the amyg-
With improved diagnostic recognition of and further dala and other limbic structures as well as the orbito-
research on SD, however, that limitation has begun to frontal cortex, are also close and well-connected to
recede, and the best evidence points to a region in the ATL. The temporal lobe regions apparently impor-
the ATL posterior to the temporal pole, the anterior tant in semantic memory are immediately adjacent
portion of the fusiform gyrus, which is subjacent to to the anterior parts of the medial temporal lobe mem-
the head and body of the hippocampus (Brodmann ory system critical for rapid learning of new episodic
areas 20 and 36). That is, atrophy and hypometabolism information. Conceptual knowledge, although mostly,
are severe across the entire anterior temporal region and for good reasons, acquired gradually (McClelland,
including both the temporal pole and this slightly more McNaughton, & O’Reilly, 1995), is ultimately based on
posterior and inferior region; however, the abnormality information experienced in episodes.
in the anterior fusiform correlates best with semantic As a final note on this topic, it is potentially inter- p0095
scores (Libon et al., 2013; Mion et al., 2010). esting and important that the center of the ATL

K. CONCEPTUAL SEMANTIC KNOWLEDGE

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

8 61. THE HUB-AND-SPOKE HYPOTHESIS OF SEMANTIC MEMORY

transmodal hub (anterior fusiform/ITG) aligns with Recent studies that have explored semantic function p0105
previously separate literature on neurophysiological in patients with unilateral ATL damage indicate that
evidence from patients who have depth or grid electro- semantic performance is generally much better than in
des surgically implanted as part of investigation and patients with bilateral ATL diseases, especially SD.
treatment for long-term epilepsy. Ventral ATL regions With more sensitive assessments, however, expressive
are often considered to be visually specific, reflecting and receptive semantic deficits can be observed after
the apex of a visual “ventral” stream of processing unilateral ATL lesions (Bi et al., 2011; Lambon Ralph,
(Albright, 2012); however, the same area also appar- Cipolotti, Manes, & Patterson, 2010; Lambon Ralph,
ently plays a role in language processing. In their sem- Ehsan, Baker, & Rogers, 2012). These contemporary
inal studies, Lüders et al. (1986, 1991) demonstrated studies fit with the older comparative neurology litera-
that stimulation of this region gave rise to transient ture that consistently reported chronic multimodal
language impairment (of reading and naming) and, (visual and auditory) “semantic” impairment in
consequently, they coined the term “basal temporal primates and monkeys after bilateral ATL resection,
language area.” Direct measurement of local field but only a milder and transient form of the same
potentials confirmed its role in naming (Nobre, impairment after unilateral resection (Brown &
Allison, & McCarthy, 1994). More recent studies have Schafer, 1888; Klüver & Bucy, 1939). Intriguingly, and
demonstrated that these regions show sensitivity to relevant to the cross-methodology data noted at the
semantic categories, but not low-level variations in end of their seminal paper, Klüver and Bucy
visual characteristics (e.g., orientation and size). highlighted striking similarities between primates
Strikingly, these semantic effects emerge very rapidly following bilateral ATL resection and the patients
(B120 ms) after the onset of the stimulus (Chan et al., previously described by Pick with frontotemporal
2011; Liu, Agam, Madsen, & Kreiman, 2009). Although dementia. The same pattern as that observed in the
further direct comparisons with SD patients and func- primate studies was replicated in a rare human single-
tional neuroimaging are required, these data from the case neurosurgery study (Terzian & Dalle Ore, 1955).
cognitive neurosurgery literature appear to support In an (ultimately unsuccessful) attempt to control the
the hypothesis that this region is the centerpoint of patient’s epilepsy, these investigators performed
a transmodal semantic hub. sequential full-depth ATL resections. As in the previ-
ous primate studies, after initial unilateral removal, the
patient exhibited transient deficits of language, recog-
s0030 61.5 EVIDENCE FOR AND POSSIBLE nition. and memory. However, after bilateral resection,
REASONS FOR A BILATERAL ATL HUB a chronic and severe comprehension deficit emerged,
along with other features of what is now referred to
p0100 So far, our attention has been on the ATL as a as Klüver-Bucy syndrome. Taken together, the full
whole and in the singular. In the next two sections we (although admittedly still rather limited) set of neuro-
consider emerging evidence for subdivisions both psychological and neurosurgery findings can be sum-
within the ATL in each hemisphere, from dorsal to marized as follows: unilateral ATL damage generates
ventral (see Section 61.6) and (in this section) across semantic impairment in the acute stage that often
the two hemispheres. The ATL abnormality in SD diminishes rapidly, leaving patients in the chronic
patients is always bilateral (although often very asym- phase with a mild comprehension deficit. This
metrical, at least until late in the disease), suggesting impairment can be detected with sensitive neuropsy-
that both left and right regions may contribute to chological assessments but is nothing like the level of
conceptual knowledge. Other patient groups with clini- semantic impairment found in SD and other bilateral
cally notable impairment of semantic memory typically ATL diseases.
have bilateral ATL damage [e.g., AD (Hodges & Before considering the explanation for these p0110
Patterson, 1995) and herpes simplex virus encephalitis findings in more detail, a brief aside to avoid poten-
(Lambon Ralph, Lowe, & Rogers, 2007; Noppeney tial confusion is merited. By far the most famous
et al., 2007)]. Likewise, formal meta-analyses of the example of bilateral temporal resection is that of
functional neuroimaging literature indicate that both patient HM. After surgery, he and the other patients
ATLs are implicated in semantic function in neurologi- reported in the seminal case-series study of Scoville
cally intact participants (Binder, Desai, Graves, & and Milner (Scoville & Milner, 1957) presented with
Conant, 2009; Visser et al., 2010). These neuropsycho- profound anterograde amnesia but not with other
logical and neuroimaging results prompt the some impairments of higher cognition, including language
obvious but important questions. First, does semantic and semantic memory. At face value, this might
impairment require bilateral ATL damage? And, if so, appear to contradict the hypothesis that semantic
then what are the roles of the left versus right ATL? representation is reliant on a bilateral ATL system; in

K. CONCEPTUAL SEMANTIC KNOWLEDGE

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

61.5 EVIDENCE FOR AND POSSIBLE REASONS FOR A BILATERAL ATL HUB 9
fact, a closer consideration of the location of the resec- was distributed across both demi-hubs, a substantial
tions adds further weight to our hypothesis that the and considerably greater semantic deficit emerged—
ventrolateral aspects of the ATL are crucial to semantic replicating the difference between patients with unilat-
memory. Because of the fact that Scoville used an eral versus bilateral ATL lesions. Detailed analysis and
orbital approach to the medial temporal regions when mathematical exploration demonstrated that the differ-
performing the bilateral selective hippocampectomy ence between unilateral and bilateral damage in the
(rather than the lateral approach commonly used in model was related to the differential propagation of
most modern neurosurgical procedures), the ventral noisy activation. Damage in any computational model
and lateral aspects of the ATL were preserved in patient not only diminishes the level of activation but also
HM and the other patients in the original case series. adds noise to its processing. When damage is focussed
p0115 The notion that both left and right ATL contribute within one (only partly interconnected) demi-hub, the
to semantic representation is reinforced by recent stud- unaffected system generates strong and relatively
ies applying rTMS to neurologically intact participants. noise-free activations that are able to dominate the
The technique is complementary to neuropsychological response from the model; thus, it performs generally
studies in that one can assess performance before stim- accurately (albeit slightly less efficiently than before).
ulation and then after it during the transient refractory In contrast, bilateral damage across both demi-hubs
period. Its advantage over lesion studies is that the results in weakened and noisy representations, leading
location and timing of the stimulation are under exper- to inaccurate responses.
imental control; however, the behavioral effect of stim- Although it seems clear that both left and right p0125
ulation is (happily!) not only transient but also much ATLs are important to conceptual knowledge, the cur-
smaller than that of neurological damage. Careful, rent literature contains mixed results and conclusions
sensitive assessment methods are thus required. As regarding the nature of the contribution from each
noted, SD patients always have bilateral temporal side. Based on results from a variety of techniques,
abnormalities, making it difficult in SD to separate and some researchers argue for a left right dissociation
establish a selective role, if any, of left versus right in which the left ATL is primarily responsible for ver-
ATL. It is possible, however, to contrast the effect of bal semantic processing, whereas the right is much
rTMS to left versus right ATL, and this has been done more important in processing faces, objects, and non-
for verbal and nonverbal comprehension tasks in a verbal sounds (Gainotti, 2012, 2014; Mesulam et al.,
series of studies (Lambon Ralph, Pobric, & Jafferies, 2013; Snowden, Thompson, & Neary, 2004; Thierry &
2009; Pobric et al., 2010a). In these studies, rTMS to Price, 2006). Other research groups report results
either ATL generated an equivalent effect on both ver- more compatible with either similar contributions from
bal and nonverbal semantic tasks, thus supporting our the two ATL sides or at least only graded dissocia-
working hypothesis that both ATLs contribute to a tions. Furthermore, even results indicating left right
transmodal semantic hub. dissociation are open to a different and more subtle
p0120 A potential explanatory framework for the patient interpretation than a categorical “left 5 verbal, right 5
(SD and unilateral resection) and rTMS results was nonverbal.” There is no doubt that cortical regions
explored in a recent computational model of bilateral posterior to the ATL have significantly different
ATL semantic representation (Schapiro et al., 2013). involvement in the processing of verbal versus nonver-
This was based on a direct extension of the original bal information. Given that the great majority of corti-
Rogers “hub-and-spoke” computational model (Rogers cocortical connections are very local and within rather
et al., 2004). Instead of a single transmodal representa- than across the hemisphere, this means that as infor-
tional hub, however, Schapiro and colleagues split it mation moves from caudal (sensory) regions to rostral
into two separate “demi-hubs” with only partial con- (conceptual) regions, verbal input will activate the left
nectivity between them. The model, which was trained ATL more than the right, and nonverbal input such as
in exactly the same manner as the Rogers single- faces and objects will preferentially activate the right
hub variant, was able to learn all of the training ATL. This account of left/right differences in semantic
patterns despite having a lower number of connec- processing, including two implemented computational
tions overall. When considerable unilateral damage models, has been advanced by Ikeda, Patterson,
was applied to the simulation, a small semantic Graham, Lambon Ralph, and Hodges (2006), Lambon
impairment resulted that could be reduced by allow- Ralph, McClelland, Patterson, Galton, and Hodges
ing some postdamage weight adjustment (to mimic (2001), Mion et al. (2010), and Schapiro et al. (2013).
spontaneous recovery; Keidel, Welbourne, & Lambon In conclusion, the current literature provides only a p0130
Ralph, 2010; Welbourne & Lambon Ralph, 2007; partial answer to the nature of the contribution of left
Welbourne, Woollams, Crisp, & Lambon Ralph, 2011). versus right ATL to semantic processing. There is
In contrast, when the same total amount of damage broad agreement (across methods and patient groups)

K. CONCEPTUAL SEMANTIC KNOWLEDGE

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

10 61. THE HUB-AND-SPOKE HYPOTHESIS OF SEMANTIC MEMORY

that both left and right ATL contribute in some fashion absolute in nature. Although local cytoarchitecture
and that the left ATL is more involved than the right is only one factor in neurocomputation (others—like
in generating names and speech from semantic connectivity—are also crucial), these classic and con-
memory. The issues that are subject to considerable temporary findings might lead us to expect some func-
current research activity center around two alternative tional variations across the ATL, albeit they are
hypotheses: (i) graded connectivity-related differences probably graded in nature.
in the contribution of each ATL to a primarily bilateral Recent (distortion-corrected) neuroimaging studies p0145
neural system or (ii) a more modular distinction in have provided new insights about white-matter con-
which each ATL is relatively specialized for discrete nectivity within the ATL and also the location of
subdivisions of semantic processing. semantically related functional activations. With
regard to white-matter connectivity, Binney et al.
(2012) observed connections both along gyri and
s0035
61.6 THE GRADED HUB HYPOTHESIS between neighboring gyri, providing a neural basis for
blending of information in a rostral and lateral direc-
p0135 In this final section, we consider potential functional tion along the temporal cortex. In addition, there is a
variations within each ATL. As noted, although the more regionally specific pattern of connectivity from
robust and consistent data from SD patients have been the temporal lobe to other areas. The temporopolar
crucial in linking bilateral ATLs to semantic represen- cortex is primarily connected via the uncinate fascicu-
tation, these findings are a less powerful source of lus to orbitofrontal and ventral prefrontal regions, pos-
information regarding the contributions of left versus terior temporal areas to inferior parietal regions, and
right ATL. In a similar vein, although the atrophy in superior temporal areas to inferior prefrontal regions.
SD is generally agreed to be more profound in the ven- Very similar patterns of connectivity have been
tral than the dorsal ATL, most of the ATL is abnormal revealed by a recent analysis of resting-state functional
by the time that SD patients come to clinical attention, connectivity seeded from different parts of the human
with the result that the condition of SD is insufficient temporopolar cortex (Pascual et al., 2013). These long-
to inform us about potential variations in representa- range connections are likely to be germane not only for
tion of conceptual knowledge from dorsal to ventral considering how various forms of modality-specific
regions. There is also the question of lateral versus information interact with a transmodal ATL hub but
medial aspects of the ATL. All of these “unknowns” also for considering how semantic representations
constitute yet another reason for adopting multiple interface with other cognitive mechanisms, such as
methods, because limitations in one approach can executive functions and working memory (Binney
often be compensated by advantages conferred by et al., 2012; Jefferies & Lambon Ralph, 2006).
other techniques. With the benefit of methods and techniques to allow p0150
p0140 Ever since the seminal work of Brodmann (1909), it effective probing of activations across the entire ATL
has been known that there are variations in cyto- (including ventral regions), recent fMRI studies have
architecture that, in turn, imply different types of started to reveal various graded patterns of semantic
neurocomputation. Two major distinctions in the ATL activation, which seem to fit with the graded differences
region noted by Brodmann were a shift from granular in connectivity and cytoarchitecture in this area (Binney
through dysgranular to agranular cortex in moving et al., 2012; Ding et al., 2009). These neuroimaging stud-
from lateral to medial structures around the temporal ies point to the bilateral ventrolateral subregion as the
pole and a significant change in cytoarchitecture from centerpoint of a graded ATL semantic hub. Across stud-
BA22 (approximately equivalent to the superior tem- ies, this region is consistently activated, irrespective of
poral gyrus) to the neighboring areas (pole, BA38; task, input modality, or category of stimuli (Sharp et al.,
middle and inferior temporal gyri, BA21 and BA20, 2004; Vandenberghe et al., 1996; Visser et al., 2012;
respectively). Although noting that there were also Visser & Lambon Ralph, 2011). Moving away from
differences between the other ATL regions (sufficient this neural location, there seem to be gradual shifts in
to give them different labels), Brodmann commented the semantic function dependent on the proximity/
that they were graded rather than absolute differences; connectivity to different primary inputs (Visser et al.,
in fact, it is possible to consider them to be one single 2012; Visser & Lambon Ralph, 2011). Thus, activation is
larger cytoarchitectural region as some of his contem- more sensitive to auditory-related stimuli (verbal and
poraries did. Modern, sophisticated studies (Ding, Van nonverbal) in lateral superior regions and to visual
Hoesen, Cassell, & Poremba, 2009) have increased the inputs in posterior ventral temporal areas. These early
number of cytoarchitectural divisions proposed for the explorations of the ATL region are consistent with
ATL but, echoing Brodmann’s original observations, variants of the hub-and-spoke model that include
these divisions are thought to be graded rather than graded variations of connectivity (Plaut, 2002).

K. CONCEPTUAL SEMANTIC KNOWLEDGE

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

REFERENCES 11
p0155 In summary, recent evidence has made it clear that 19(12), 2767 2796. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/
the ATL does not function as one undifferentiated cercor/bhp055.
Binney, R. J., Parker, G. J. M., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2012).
mass but rather with important graded variations of Convergent connectivity and graded specialization in the rostral
function across the region. Given the pattern of its human temporal lobe as revealed by diffusion-weighted imaging
structural and functional connectivity, these graded probabilistic tractography. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24,
changes in ATL function appear to reflect the varying 1998 2014.
influence of different inputs and outputs on the Boulenger, V., Mechtouff, L., Thobois, S., Broussolle, E.,
Jeannerod, M., & Nazir, T. A. (2008). Word processing in
processing in local ATL subregions. Parkinson’s disease is impaired for action verbs but not con-
crete nouns. Neuropsychologia, 46(2), 743 756.
Bozeat, S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Patterson, K., Garrard, P., &
s0040
61.7 CONCLUDING COMMENT Hodges, J. R. (2000). Non-verbal semantic impairment in semantic
dementia. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1207 1215.
p0160 Semantic memory/conceptual knowledge is central Brodmann, K. (1909). Vergleichende lokalisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde.
Leipzig: Barth.
to much of human life. It may not be involved when Brown, S., & Schafer, E. A. (1888). An investigation into the functions of
you walk into a room to sit down, but recognizing that the occipital and temporal lobes of the monkey’s brain. Philosophical
an unfamiliar chair is something on which to sit Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, 179, 303 327.
requires semantic memory. It may not be involved Chan, A. M., Baker, J. M., Eskandar, E., Schomer, D., Ulbert, I.,
when you move your tongue and lips, but it is the basis Marinkovic, K., et al. (2011). First-Pass selectivity for semantic
categories in human anteroventral temporal lobe. The Journal
of every novel utterance that you make. A huge amount of Neuroscience, 31(49), 18119 18129. Available from: http://
has been learned about this central aspect of human dx.doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3122-11.2011.
function in the relatively short time in which cognitive Chan, D., Fox, N., & Rossor, M. (2002). Differing patterns of temporal
neuroscientists have been addressing it in their experi- atrophy in Alzheimer’s disease and semantic dementia. Neurology,
mental work and theorizing, but there is so far to go 58, 838.
Chen, L., & Rogers, T. T. (2014). Revisiting domain-general accounts
that future models of the organization and neural basis of category specificity in mind and brain. Wiley Interdisciplinary
of semantic memory may look like “objects” that we Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5(3), 327 344. Available from: http://
have never encountered before. It would be a foolhardy dx.doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1283.
researcher who tries to predict which of the field’s Davies, R., Graham, K. S., Xuereb, J. H., Williams, G. B., & Hodges,
current interpretations of data will still be alive one or J. R. (2004). The human perirhinal cortex and semantic memory.
European Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 2441 2446.
several decades from now. Maybe the proposal of a Devlin, J. T., Russell, R. P., Davis, M. H., Price, C. J., Wilson, J.,
semantic hub will be here today and gone tomorrow. Moss, H. E., et al. (2000). Susceptibility-induced loss of
All we can say is that today, we find it difficult to con- signal: Comparing PET and fMRI on a semantic task. NeuroImage,
ceive of a hub-less framework that would account for 11(6; Pt 2), 589 600.
existing data regarding semantic memory. de Zubicaray, G., Arciuli, J., & McMahon, K. (2013). Putting an
“end” to the motor cortex representations of action words. Journal
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25(11), 1957 1974. Available from:
http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00437.
References Ding, S., Van Hoesen, G. W., Cassell, M. D., & Poremba, A. (2009).
Acosta-Cabronero, J., Patterson, K., Fryer, T. D., Hodges, J. R., Parcellation of human temporal polar cortex: A combined analy-
Pengas, G., Williams, G. B., et al. (2011). Atrophy, hypometabo- sis of multiple cytoarchitectonic, chemoarchitectonic, and patho-
lism and white matter abnormalities in semantic dementia tell a logical markers. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 514, 595 623.
coherent story. Brain, 134, 2025 2035. Du, A. T., Schuff, N., Kramer, J. H., Rosen, H. J., Gorno-Tempini,
Albright, T. D. (2012). On the perception of probable things: Neural M. L., Rankin, K., et al. (2007). Different regional patterns of
substrates of associative memory, imagery, and perception. cortical thinning in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal
Neuron, 74(2), 227 245. dementia. Brain, 130, 1159 1166.
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Gainotti, G. (2012). The format of conceptual representations disrupted
Psychology, 59(1), 617 645. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/ in semantic dementia: A position paper. Cortex, 48(5), 521 529.
10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639. Gainotti, G. (2014). Why are the right and left hemisphere conceptual
Behrmann, M., & Plaut, D. C. (2012). Bilateral hemispheric proces- representations different? Behavioural Neurology, 2014, 10. Available
sing of words and faces: Evidence from word impairments from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2014/603134.
in prosopagnosia and face impairments in pure alexia. Cerebral Gloor, P. (1997). The temporal lobe and the limbic system. Oxford:
Cortex, 24, 1102 1118. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/ Oxford University Press.
10.1093/cercor/bhs390. Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Dronkers, N. F., Rankin, K. P., et al. (2004).
Bi, Y., Wei, T., Wu, C., Han, Z., Jiang, T., & Caramazza, A. (2011). Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary progressive
The role of the left anterior temporal lobe in language processing aphasia. Annals of Neurology, 55, 335 346.
revisited: Evidence from an individual with ATL resection. Guo, C. C., Gorno-Tempini, M. L., Gesierich, B., Henry, M., Trujillo, A.,
Cortex, 47(5), 575 587. Shany-Ur, T., et al. (2013). Anterior temporal lobe degeneration
Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., & Conant, L. L. (2009). produces widespread network-driven dysfunction. Brain, 136(10),
Where Is the semantic system? A critical review and meta- 2979 2991. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/
analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, awt222.

K. CONCEPTUAL SEMANTIC KNOWLEDGE

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

12 61. THE HUB-AND-SPOKE HYPOTHESIS OF SEMANTIC MEMORY

Hauk, O., Shtyrov, Y., & Pulvermüller, F. (2008). The time course of Libon, D. J., Rascovsky, K., Powers, J., Irwin, D. J., Boller, A.,
action and action-word comprehension in the human brain as Weinberg, D., et al. (2013). Comparative semantic profiles in seman-
revealed by neurophysiology. Journal of Physiology Paris, 102(1 3), tic dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, 136(8), 2497 2509.
50 58. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt165.
Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (1995). Is semantic memory consis- Liu, H., Agam, Y., Madsen, J. R., & Kreiman, G. (2009). Timing,
tently impaired early in the course of Alzheimer’s disease? timing, timing: Fast decoding of object information from intra-
Neuroanatomical and diagnostic implications. Neuropsychologia, cranial field potentials in human visual cortex. Neuron, 62(2),
33(4), 441 459. 281 290.
Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K. (2007). Semantic dementia: A unique Lüders, H., Lesser, R. P., Hahn, J., Dinner, D. S., Morris, H., Resor, S.,
clinico-pathological syndrome. Lancet Neurology, 6, 1004 1014. et al. (1986). Basal temporal language area demonstrated by elec-
Hodges, J. R., Patterson, K., Oxbury, S., & Funnell, E. (1992). trical stimulation. Neurology, 36(4), 505 510.
Semantic dementia: Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal Lüders, H., Lesser, R. P., Hahn, J., Dinner, D. S., Morris, H. H.,
lobe atrophy. Brain, 115(Pt 6), 1783 1806. Wyllie, E., et al. (1991). Basal temporal language area. Brain, 114
Ikeda, M., Patterson, K., Graham, K. S., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & (Pt 2), 743 754.
Hodges, J. R. (2006). A horse of a different colour: Do patients Marinkovic, K., Dhond, R. P., Dale, A. M., Glessner, M., Carr, V., &
with semantic dementia recognise different versions of the same Halgren, E. (2003). Spatiotemporal dynamics of modality-specific
object as the same? Neuropsychologia, 44(4), 566 575. and supramodal word processing. Neuron, 38, 487 497.
Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2006). Semantic impairment in Mayberry, E. J., Sage, K., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2011). At the
stroke aphasia vs. semantic dementia: A case-series comparison. edge of semantic space: The breakdown of coherent concepts in
Brain, 129, 2132 2147. semantic dementia is constrained by typicality and severity but
Keidel, J. L., Welbourne, S. R., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010). not modality. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(9), 2240 2251.
Solving the paradox of the equipotential and modular brain: A Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21582.
neurocomputational model of stroke vs. slow-growing glioma. McClelland, J. L., McNaughton, B. L., & O’Reilly, R. C. (1995). Why
Neuropsychologia, 48(6), 1716 1724. there are complementary learning-systems in the hippocampus
Klüver, H., & Bucy, P. (1939). Preliminary analysis of functions of and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of con-
the temporal lobes in monkeys. Archives of Neurology and nectionist models of learning and memory. Psychological Review,
Psychiatry, 42(6), 979 1000. 102(3), 419 457.
Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2014). Neurocognitive insights on conceptual Mesulam, M.-M., Wieneke, C., Hurley, R., Rademaker, A.,
knowledge and its breakdown. Philosophical Transactions of the Thompson, C. K., Weintraub, S., et al. (2013). Words and objects
Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1634), 20120392. Available at the tip of the left temporal lobe in primary progressive aphasia.
from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0392. Brain, 136(2), 601 618. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/
Lambon Ralph, M. A., Cipolotti, L., Manes, F., & Patterson, K. (2010). 10.1093/brain/aws336.
Taking both sides: Do unilateral anterior temporal lobe lesions Meteyard, L., Cuadrado, S. R., Bahrami, B., & Vigliocco, G. (2012).
disrupt semantic memory? Brain, 133(11), 3243 3255. Available Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of
from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awq264. semantics. Cortex, 48(7), 788 804.
Lambon Ralph, M. A., Ehsan, S., Baker, G. A., & Rogers, T. T. (2012). Mion, M., Patterson, K., Acosta-Cabronero, J., Pengas, G., Izquierdo-
Semantic memory is impaired in patients with unilateral anterior Garcia, D., Hong, Y. T., et al. (2010). What the left and right
temporal lobe resection for temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain, 135(1), anterior fusiform gyri tell us about semantic memory. Brain, 133
242 258. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/brain/awr325. (11), 3256 3268. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/
Lambon Ralph, M. A., Lowe, C., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Neural basis brain/awq272.
of category-specific semantic deficits for living things: Evidence Nestor, P. J., Fryer, T. D., & Hodges, J. R. (2006). Declarative memory
from semantic dementia, HSVE and a neural network model. impairments in Alzheimer’s disease and semantic dementia.
Brain, 130, 1127 1137. NeuroImage, 30, 1010 1020.
Lambon Ralph, M. A., McClelland, J. L., Patterson, K., Galton, C. J., & Nestor, P. J., Fryer, T. D., Ikeda, M., & Hodges, J. R. (2003).
Hodges, J. R. (2001). No right to speak? The relationship between Retrosplenial cortex—B29/30—hypometabolism in mild cognitive
object naming and semantic impairment: Neuropsychological evi- impairment (prodromal Alzheimer’s disease). The European
dence and a computational model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 2663 2667.
13, 341 356. Nobre, A. C., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (1994). Word recognition in
Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Patterson, K. (2008). Generalisation and the human inferior temporal lobe. Nature, 372(6503), 260 263.
differentiation in semantic memory: Insights from semantic Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/372260a0.
dementia. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1124, 61 76. Noppeney, U., Patterson, K., Tyler, L. K., Moss, H., Stamatakis, E. A.,
Lambon Ralph, M. A., Pobric, G., & Jefferies, E. (2009). Conceptual Bright, P., et al. (2007). Temporal lobe lesions and semantic
knowledge is underpinned by the temporal pole bilaterally: impairment: A comparison of herpes simplex virus encephalitis
Convergent evidence from rTMS. Cerebral Cortex, 19(4), 832 838. and semantic dementia. Brain, 130, 1138 1147.
Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn131. Pascual, B., Masdeu, J. C., Hollenbeck, M., Makris, N., Insausti, R.,
Lambon Ralph, M. A., Sage, K., Green Heredia, C., Berthier, M. L., Ding, S.-L., et al. (2013). Large-scale brain networks of the human
Martı́nez-Cuitiño, M., Torralva, T., et al. (2011). El-La: The impact left temporal pole: A functional connectivity MRI study. Cerebral
of degraded semantic representations on knowledge of gram- Cortex [Epub ahead of print September 2013]. Available from:
matical gender in semantic dementia. Acta Neuropsychologica, 9, http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht260.
115 132. Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2000). Semantic dementia: One
Lambon Ralph, M. A., Sage, K., Jones, R. W., & Mayberry, E. J. window on the structure and organization of semantic memory.
(2010). Coherent concepts are computed in the anterior temporal In L. Cermak (Ed.), Handbook of neuropsychology (Vol. 2, pp. 313 333).
lobes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United Amsterdam: Elsevier.
States of America, 107(6), 2717 2722. Available from: http://dx. Patterson, K., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Jefferies, E., Woollams, A.,
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0907307107. Jones, R. W., Hodges, J. R., et al. (2006). “Presemantic” cognition

K. CONCEPTUAL SEMANTIC KNOWLEDGE

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

REFERENCES 13
in semantic dementia: Six deficits in search of an explanation. Sharp, D. J., Scott, S. K., & Wise, R. J. S. (2004). Retrieving meaning
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(2), 169 183. after temporal lobe infarction: The role of the basal language area.
Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you Annals of Neurology, 56(6), 836 846.
know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge Simmons, W. K., Ramjee, V., Beauchamp, M. S., McRae, K., Martin, A.,
in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(12), 976 987. & Barsalou, L. W. (2007). A common neural substrate for perceiving
Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn2277. and knowing about colour. Neuropsychologia, 45(12), 2802 2810.
Patterson, K., & Plaut, D. (2009). “Shallow draughts intoxicate the Smith, E. E., & Medin, D. L. (1981). Categories and concepts.
brain”: Lessons from cognitive science for cognitive neuropsy- Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
chology. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 39 58. Snowden, J. S., Goulding, P. J., & Neary, D. (1989). Semantic demen-
Peelen, M. V., & Caramazza, A. (2012). Conceptual object repre- tia: A form of circumscribed cerebral atrophy. Behavioural
sentations in human anterior temporal cortex. The Journal of Neurology, 2(3), 167 182.
Neuroscience, 32(45), 15728 15736. Available from: http://dx.doi. Snowden, J. S., Griffiths, H. L., & Neary, D. (1996). Semantic-episodic
org/10.1523/jneurosci.1953-12.2012. memory interactions in semantic dementia: Implications for retro-
Pengas, G., Patterson, K., Arnold, R. J., Bird, C. M., Burgess, N., & grade memory function. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13(8), 1101 1137.
Nestor, P. J. (2010). Lost and found: Bespoke memory testing for Snowden, J. S., Thompson, J. C., & Neary, D. (2004). Knowledge of
Alzheimer’s disease and semantic dementia. Journal of Alzheimer’s famous faces and names in semantic dementia. Brain, 127, 860 872.
Disease, 21(4), 1347 1365. Available from: http://dx.doi.org/ Terzian, H., & Dalle Ore, G. (1955). Syndrome of Kluver-Bucy repro-
10.3233/jad-2010-100654. duced in man by bilateral removal of the temporal lobes.
Pitcher, D., Walsh, V., Yovel, G., & Duchaine, B. (2007). TMS evi- Neurology, 5, 373 380.
dence for the involvement of the right occipital face area in early Thierry, G., & Price, C. J. (2006). Dissociating verbal and nonverbal
face processing. Current Biology, 17(18), 1568 1573. Available conceptual processing in the human brain. Journal of Cognitive
from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.07.063. Neuroscience, 18(6), 1018 1028. Available from: http://dx.doi.
Plaut, D. C. (2002). Graded modality-specific specialization in org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.1018.
semantics: A computational account of optic aphasia. Cognitive Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving, &
Neuropsychology, 19, 603 639. W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organisation of memory. London: Academic
Pobric, G., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010a). Amodal Press.
semantic representations depend on both anterior temporal lobes: Vandenberghe, R., Price, C., Wise, R., Josephs, O., & Frackowiak,
Evidence from repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation. R. S. J. (1996). Functional-anatomy of a common semantic system
Neuropsychologia, 48(5), 1336 1342. for words and pictures. Nature, 383(6597), 254 256.
Pobric, G., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010b). Category- Visser, M., Jefferies, E., Embleton, K. V., & Lambon Ralph, M. A.
specific versus category-general semantic impairment induced (2012). Both the middle temporal gyrus and the ventral anterior
by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Current Biology, 20(10), temporal area are crucial for multimodal semantic processing:
964 968. Distortion-corrected fMRI evidence for a double gradient of infor-
Pobric, G. G., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2007). Anterior mation convergence in the temporal lobes. Journal of Cognitive
temporal lobes mediate semantic representation: Mimicking Neuroscience, 24(8), 1766 1778.
semantic dementia by using rTMS in normal participants. Visser, M., Jefferies, E., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2010). Semantic
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of processing in the anterior temporal lobes: A meta-analysis of
America, 104, 20137 20141. the functional neuroimaging literature. Journal of Cognitive
Pulvermuller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and Neuroscience, 22, 1083 1094.
action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(7), 576 582. Visser, M., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2011). Differential contributions
Pulvermüller, F., Cooper-Pye, E., Dine, C., Hauk, O., Nestor, P. J., & of bilateral ventral anterior temporal lobe and left anterior super-
Patterson, K. (2010). The word processing deficit in semantic demen- ior temporal gyrus to semantic processes. Journal of Cognitive
tia: All categories are equal, but some categories are more equal Neuroscience, 23(10), 3121 3131.
than others. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(9), 2027 2041. Warrington, E. K. (1975). The selective impairment of semantic mem-
Available from: http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21339. ory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27(4), 635 657.
Rogers, T. T., Lambon Ralph, M. A., Garrard, P., Bozeat, S., Welbourne, S. R., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2007). Using PDP models
McClelland, J. L., Hodges, J. R., et al. (2004). The structure to simulate phonological dyslexia: The key role of plasticity-
and deterioration of semantic memory: A neuropsychological related recovery. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1125 1139.
and computational investigation. Psychological Review, 111, Welbourne, S. R., Woollams, A. M., Crisp, J., & Lambon Ralph, M. A.
205 235. (2011). The role of plasticity-related functional reorganization in
Rogers, T. T., & McClelland, J. L. (2004). Semantic cognition: A parallel the explanation of central dyslexias. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 28
distributed processing approach. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. (2), 65 108.
Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive representations of semantic categories. Williams, G. B., Nestor, P. J., & Hodges, J. R. (2005). The neural cor-
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192 233. relates of semantic and behavioural deficits in frontotemporal
Schapiro, A. C., McClelland, J. L., Welbourne, S. R., Rogers, T. T., & dementia. NeuroImage, 24, 1042 1051.
Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2013). Why bilateral damage is worse than Wittgenstein, L. (2001). Philosophical investigations: The German text,
unilateral damage to the brain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, with a revised English translation 50th anniversary commemorative edi-
25(12), 2107 2123. tion. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilat- Woollams, A. M., Cooper-Pye, E., Hodges, J. R., & Patterson, K.
eral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and (2008). Anomia: A doubly typical signature of semantic dementia.
Psychiatry, 20, 11 21. Neuropsychologia, 46(10), 2503 2514.

K. CONCEPTUAL SEMANTIC KNOWLEDGE

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061


To protect the rights of the author(s) and publisher we inform you that this PDF is an uncorrected proof for internal business use only by the author(s), editor(s), reviewer(s), Elsevier
and typesetter MPS. It is not allowed to publish this proof online or in print. This proof copy is the copyright property of the publisher and is confidential until formal publication.

NON-PRINT ITEM

Abstract
Semantic memory represents our knowledge about the meanings of words, objects, people, and all other ver-
bal and nonverbal stimuli that we encounter in the world. The semantic network in the human brain encodes all
of this information, reactivates it rapidly and effortlessly, and generalizes appropriately across different exem-
plars and situations to form coherent concepts. This chapter considers how coherent concepts are formed and
argues that the semantic network must include both modality-specific primary and association areas and a cru-
cial transmodal hub. Drawing on evidence from normal and disrupted semantic processing, we consider the
growing evidence that regions in the anterior temporal lobe (ATL) are the foundation of this representational
hub and assess the possible roles of left versus right and dorsal to ventral ATL subregions.
Keywords: Anterior temporal lobe; semantic dementia; hemispheric differences; embodied cognition; transcranial
magnetic stimulation

Hickok-1611153 978-0-12-407794-2 00061

View publication stats

You might also like