You are on page 1of 129

"A Dictionary of Narratology fills an important gap.

Prince knows the


subject well: he helped to invent it. In many cases, he has made terms
thai were opaque quite understandable. The book is lucid and easy to
use "—Seymour Chatman. author of Story and Discourse. "Gerald
Prince's work has always been marked by soundness, expertise, ob-
jectivity. and precision. A Dictionary of Narratology establishes stan-
dards of meaning which wiil be of great help to narratologists often
struggling in a Babe! of contradictions and synonyms."—Harold F.
Mosher, Jr.. editor of Style.

History, literature, religion, myth, film, psycnology. theory, and daily


conversation all rely heavily upon narrative. So it is hardly surprising
at studies of the art of narra{jdfe|bpund. Narrative has a language
patterns, deeply established
retive codes, whether in a
£S many disciplines, nar-
t ge. and only recently
;e it to permit one expert
[tology, Gerald Prince
concepts that are de-
ies are keyed to arti-
'e exemplified.
'nary of Narratology
r
s. and scholars in many

al the University of Pennsylva-


fology: The Form and Functioning of

8«780a03«2S7!4
— « sb. j z & S . — j o l —

Gerald Prince

Dictionary of Narratology

UNIVERSITY O F NEBRASKA PRESS: LINCOLN 4 LONDON


Copyright © 1987 by the
University of Nebraska Press
All rights reserved
Manufactured in the United
States of America

First paperback edition: 1989


Most recent printing indicated
by the last digit below:
10 9 8 765432

The paper in this book meets


the minimum require-
ments of American National
Standard for Infor-
mation Sciences - Permanence
of Paper for
Printed Library Materials,
ANSI Z39.46-1984.

Library of Congress Cataloging


in Publication Data

Prince, Gerald.
A dictionary of narratology.

Bibliography: p.
1. Discourse analysis, Narra-
tive - Dictionaries.
I. Title.

P302.7.P75 1987
808.3*00141 87-4998
ISBN 0-8032-3673-6
alkaline paper
ISBN 0-8032-8714-3 (pbk.)
i-_—r--7TMf-j«niJr»ri'*Kii. "THE mJ 3 Q JElgSi JWPs.

VII PREFACE

1 A DICTIONARY OF NAFIRATOLOGY

105 BIBLIOGRAPHY
reface

In this dictionary, i define, explain, and illustrate terms that are specific to nar-
ratology (e.g., narreme, extradiegetic); terms whose narratologica! acceptation
differs from their other ones {e.g., voice, transformation); and terms whose "ordi-
nary" or technical meaning belongs to a semantic domain that is prominent in or
essential to narratological description and argumentation (e.g., code, rewrite rule).
My list is not exhaustive. 1 have retained only terms that enjoy wide currency
in narratology—terms that are used and can be used by narratologists with differ-
ent theoretical or methodological preferences—as well as a few terms that I
consider heipful and that might gain currency and a few other terms that are no
longer very fashionable but once were. Furthermore, I have concentrated on terms
used in connection with verba! rather than nonverbal narratives: I believe that this
bias reflects the biases of narratology itself. I have attempted not to neglect any
important movement: I have drawn on the Anglo-Saxon tradition originating with
Henry James and Percy Lubbock, the German tradition of Lammed or Stanzel,
the Russian Formalists and the Russian semioticians, the French structuralists,
and the Tel Aviv poeticians; I have taken into account the narratological labors of
linguists, psychologists, anthropologists, historians, and students of artificial intelli-
gence; and I have not forgotten Aristotle. Nevertheless, I have been partial to what
constitutes perhaps the most influential narratological work of the past twenty
years, that of "French" or "French-inspired" narratologists. Finally, I have left out a
large number of terms which are no doubt pertinent to the analysis of narrative but
which I regard as belonging more appropriately in dictionaries of rhetoric, semiot-
ics, linguistics, or literature (e.g., cooperative principle and allegory or novel and
romance).
If my list of terms is not exhaustive, neither are my explanations of these
terms complete. In the first place, I have not attempted to provide a thorough sur-
vey of definitions of or opinions on any given term; rather,! have sought to provide
an overview, and I have given what I consider to be the most important and useful
definitions or opinions. In the second place, I have repeatedly opted for short for-
mulations, in the belief that a dictionary should (and can) only be a helpful starting
point. (The few cases in which I have supplied more lengthy formulations are in-
tended to suggest the richness of the discussions that a particular term and the
notions it designates have provoked.) I have tried to keep technical language to a
minimum, and I have been spare with examples (paradoxically, they often lead to
confusion rather than clarification); but I have not avoided repetitions, since I have
a lot of faith in their pedagogical effectiveness.

The terms selected are presented in a single alphabetical listing. I have made
generous use of cross-references, in spite of the anxiety they might create, to
indicate relations, parallels, and contexts and to point to further examples or clarifi-
cations: when a term characterized in the dictionary appears in the body of an
entry, it is printed in SMALL CAPITALS. The few exceptions to this practice pertain to
such recurrent terms as narrator or character: a cross-reference seemed, at times,
superfluous. Furthermore, whenever I thought that the characterization (or knowl-
edge) of one or more other terms in the dictionary would enrich the user's under-
standing of a particular entry, I have added a statement of the form "See also
SUCH AND SUCH" at the very end of the entry, i have also given bibliographical refer-
ences at the end of most entries in order to allow the user of the dictionary to
investigate the topics further and in order to identify at least some of the sources
on which my formulations are based. Again, the few exceptions to this practice—
e.g., conjoining, constitutional model, mediator—are cases where the context,
cross-references, and/or cross-referential statements make bibliographical indica-
tions superfluous. When an entry has two or more definitions (e.g., competence,
contact, plot) and if pertinent, the formulation of the subentries points to the biblio-
graphical item(s) that are particularly relevant to each definition. The references
are collected in the bibliography to be found at the end of the volume.
When I started to prepare this dictionary,! wanted it to give a sense of narra-
tology as a discipline and to constitute a space for indicating some of the agree-
ments, compatibilities, and divergences obtaining in a field that has undergone
remarkable growth since the 1960s and the heyday of structuralism, i also—and
even more so—wanted it to constitute a simple guide to many of the terms, con-
cepts, and ambitions characterizing narratological study as well as a stimulant to
the development, sharpening, and refining of narratological tools. I hope that I
have been at least partly successful.

I would like to thank the University of Pennsylvania for granting me a leave in the
fall of 1985 which allowed me to write much of this dictionary. I would also like to
thank Ellen F. Prince for patient and invaluable suggestions. I feel indebted to the
readers of the manuscript for the University of Nebraska Press: their comments
were of great benefit.
abruptive dialogue. A DIALOGUE in which
the speakers' utterances are not accompa-
n i e d by TAG CLAUSES: " — H O W a r e y o u

today? —I feel great! and you? —I feel


fine." HGenette 1980.
absent narrator. A maximally COVERT NAR-
RATOR; a n IMPERSONAL NARRATOR; a

narrator presenting situations and events


with minimum narratoriai mediation and in
no way referring to a narrating self or a
narrating activity. Absent narrators are
c h a r a c t e r i s t i c o f BEHAVIORIST NARRATIVES

("Hills Like White Elephants"). HChatman


1 9 7 8 . S e e also MEDIATED NARRATION, NON-

NARRATED NARRATIVE, SHOWING,

abstract. The part of a NARRATIVE which


summarizes it and encapsulates its POINT,
or main thrust. If a narrative is taken to
constitute a series of answers to certain
questions, the abstract is that constituent
of it answenng the questions "What was
this narrative about?" and "Wny was this
narrative told?" ULabov 1972; Pratt 1977.
achronie structure. A sequence of events,
as opposed to an isolated event or two,
characterized by ACHRONY (cf. Marcel's
walks toward Meseglise and toward
Guermanies in the first part of Remem-
brance of Things Past). HGenette 1980.
S e e also ORDER, SYLLEPSIS.

achrony. An event deprived of any temporal


connection with other events; a dateless
event. Robbe-Grillet's Jealousy abounds in
achronies. 1iBal 1985;Genette 1980. See
also ACHRONIC STRUCTURE, ORDER,

act. 1. Along with the HAPPENING, one of


two possible kinds of narrated EVENTS; a
change of state brought about by an AGENT
and manifested in discourse by a PROCESS
STATEMENT in t h e m o d e of do; an ACTION.

"Mary solved the problem" represents an


act, whereas "It rained yesterday" does
not. 2. A syntagmatic constituent of an
action, an action being made up of several
acts. HChatman 1978; Greimas and
C o u r t e s 1 9 8 2 . See also NARRATIVE STATE-

WENT.

actant. A fundamental ROLE at the level of


• n a r r a t i v e DEEP STRUCTURE ( c o r r e s p o n d i n g

t o S o u r i a u ' s FUNCTION, P r o p p ' s DRAMATIS

PERSONA, a n d L o t m a n ' s ARCHIPERSONA).

The term was introduced into narratology


by Greimas, following the linguist Tesnifere,
who had used it to designate a type of
syntactic unit. By reworking the role typolo-
gies proposed by Souriau and Propp,
G r e i m a s a r r i v e d at s n ACTANTIAL MODEL

originally consisting of six actants: SUBJECT


( S o u r i a u ' s LION, P r o p p ' s HERO), OBJECT

(Souriau's SUN, Propp's SOUGHT-FOR PER-


SON), SENDER ( S o u r i a u ' s BALANCE, P r o p p ' s

DISPATCHER), RECEIVER ( S o u r i a u ' s EARTH),

HELPER ( S o u r i a u ' s MOON, P r o p p ' s HELPER

a n d DONOR), a n d OPPONENT (Souriau's


I I I I

MARS, P r o p p ' s VILLAIN a n d FALSE HERO). In Scholes 1974. See also CHARACTER.
a more recent version of Greimas's actan- actantial model. The structure of relations
tial model, Helper and Opponent are taken obtaining among ACTANTS. According to
to be AUXILIANTS and n o t actants. " A n a c - Greimas, narrative is a signifying whole be-
tant can occupy a certain number of cause it can be grasped in terms of such a
s p e c i f i e d p o s i t i o n s or ACTANTIAL ROLES structure. HThe original actantial model in-
a l o n g its NARRATIVE TRAJECTORY. T h e S u b - volved six actants: SUBJECT (looking for the
ject, for instance, can be established as OBJECT), Object (looked for by the Sub-
such by the Sender, qualified (made com- ject), SENDER (of the Subject on its quest
petent) along the axis of ability, realized as for the Object), RECEIVER (of the Object to
a successful performer, and rewarded for be secured by the Subject), HELPER (of the
its performance. Furthermore, at the level Subject), and OPPONENT (of the Subject). It
o f n a r r a t i v e SURFACE STRUCTURE, o n e a c - is often represented by the following dia-
tant can be represented by several gram:
different ACTORS, and several actants can
Sender Receiver
be represented by one and the same actor.
Thus, in an adventure story, the Subject
\
Subject^ /
> Object'
may have several enemies, all of whom
Helper'' Opponent
function as Opponent; and in a simple love
story, the boy may function as both Subject In accounting for the actantial structure of
and Receiver while the girl functions as Madame Bovary, for instance, the model
both Object and Sender. Finally, not only might yield something like this: Subject—
human actors but also animals, things, and Emma; Object—happiness; Sender-
concepts can fulfill the fundamental roles Romantic literature; Receiver—Emma;
constituting the actantial model: a diamond Helper— L§on, Rodolphe; Opponent-
can represent the Object of the Subject's Charles, Yonville, Hodolphe, Homais,
quest and an ideological imperative can Lheureux. HA more recent version of the
function as the Sender. HThough the term model involves only four actants: Subject,
actant is most often used with reference to Object, Sender, and Receiver (with Helper
the basic roles that are played by entities and Opponent functioning as AUXILIANTS).
in the world of the situations and events HAdam 198'!; Courtes 1976; Culler 1975;
recounted, it is also used sometimes to re- Greimas 1970,1983a, 1983b.
f e r to t h e roles of NARRATOR a n d actantial ro!a. A formal position occupied by
NARRATEE: these are actants of communi- a n ACTANT a l o n g its NARRATIVE TRAJEC-
cation as opposed to actants of narration TORY; a particular state assumed by an
(Subject, Object, Sender, or Receiver). actant in the logical unfolding of a narra-
liCourtes 1976; Culler 1975; Greimas tive. In its trajectory, for example, the
1970,1983a, 1983b; Greimas and Courtis SUBJECT is instituted as such by the
1982; Hamon 1972; Henault 1983; SENDER and can be modalized (qualified,
made competent) along the axes of desire, autonomous figure of the narrative world.
ability, knowledge, and obligation, realized UThe actor need not appear as an anthro-
as a performing Subject, recognized as pomorphic being: it might, for instance,
one, and rewarded, f l h e different actantial take the shape of a flying carpet, a lable,
roles in a given trajectory are sufficiently or a corporation. Moreover, the actor can
autonomous to be incarnated by different be individual (John, Mary) or collective (a
ACTORS. In other words, the actant, which subway crowd), figurative (anthropo-
constitutes a fundamental role at the DEEP morphic, zoomorphic, etc.) or noncurative
STRUCTURE level, is specified through a se- (Fate). Finally, one actor can represent
ries of actantial roles along a narrative several different actants, and several ac-
trajectory and further specified as one or tors can represent one and the same
m o r e a c t o r s at t h e SURFACE STRUCTURE actant. In romances, for example, the pro-
level. HChabrol 1973; Greimas 1970, tagonist often functions as both SUBJECT
1983a; Greimas and Courtes 1982; H6n- and RECEIVER; and in adventure stories,
a u l t 1983. S e e a l s o AUXILIANT, MODALITY, the various enemies of the hero or heroine
THEMATIC ROLE. all function as OPPONENT. HAdam 1985;
action. 1. A series of connected events ex- Greimas 1970, 1983a, 1983b; Greimas
hibiting unity and significance and moving and Court es 1982; Henault 1983; Mathieu
t h r o u g h a BEGINNING, a MIDDLE, a n d a n 1974; Scholes 1974. See also CHARACTER.
END; a syntagmatic organization of ACTS. In actorial narrative type. The class ot HOMO-
Aristotelian terms, an action is a process DIEGETIC o r HETEROOIEGETIC NARRATIVES
from bad to good fortune or the reverse. c h a r a c t e r i z e d by INTERNAL FOCALIZATION
Two actions can, of course, constitute a (Hunger, The Ambassadors). Along with
larger action. 2. In Barthes's terminology, the AUCTORIAL a n d t h e NEUTRAL NARRATIVE
a group of FUNCTIONS subsumed under the TYPES, it is one of three basic classes in
same ACTANT(S): for instance, functions in- Lintvelt's typology. HGenette 1983; Lintvelt
volving the SUBJECT in its movement 1981. S e e a l s o POINT OF VIEW.
toward the OBJECT would constitute the ac- addresses. One of the fundamental constit-
tion we call QUEST 3. An act. HAristotle uents of any act of (verbal) communication:
1968; Barthes 1975; Brooks and Warren the ( i n t e n d e d ) RECEIVER, t h e ENUNCIATEE.
1959; Chatman 1978; Genot 1979; The addressee receives a MESSAGE from
Greimas and Courtfe 1982. the ADDRESSER. UK. Buhler 1934; Jakob-
actor. The concretization of an ACTANT at son 1 9 6 0 . S e e a l s o CONATIVE FUNCTION,
t h e level of n a r r a t i v e SURFACE STRUCTURE. CONSTITUTIVE FACTORS OF COMMUNICATION.
The actor, which results from the conjunc- addresser. One of the fundamental constitu-
tion of at l e a s t o n e ACTANTIAL ROLE a n d ents of any act of (verbal) communication:
o n e THEMATIC ROLE, is r e p r e s e n t e d b y a the SENDER, t h e ENUNCIATOR.The a d d r e s -
unit equivalent to a noun phrase and indi- ser s e n d s a MESSAGE to t h e ADDRESSEE.
viduated in such a way as to constitute an HK. Biihlsr 1934; Jakobson 1960. See also
CONSTITUTIVE FACTORS OF COMMUNICATION, ogy. Whereas patients are affected by
EMOTIVE FUNCTION. certain processes, agents initiate these
advance mention, A narrative element the processes and, more specifically, influence
significance of which becomes clear only the patients, modify their situation (improv-
(well) after it is first mentioned; a narrative ing or worsening it), or maintain it (for the
"seed" the importance of which is not rec- good or the bad). In the set of influences,
ognized when it tirst appears. A character we find informers and dissimulators, se-
is casually introduced in the first chapter of ducers and intimidators, obligators and in-
a novel, for example, and begins to play a terdictors; in the set of modifiers, we find
decisive role only in chapter 20; an ordi- improvers and degraders; in the set of
nary living-room couch is mentioned in maintainers, we find protectors and frustra-
passing and, very much later, turns out to tors. ^Bremond 1973; van Dijk 1973;
conceal crucial secrets; the mere opening Scholes 1974; Todorov 1981. See also
of a window proves to have incalculable PRATTON.

consequences after a year goes by. UThe algebrlzatioa The opposite of DEFAMILIARI-
advance mention is not to be confused ZATION. Whereas the latter, for Shklovsky
with the ADVANCE NOTICE. The former does and the Russian Formalists, results from
not constitute an example of PROLEPSIS; techniques (sets of devices) that make the
the latter does. The former in no way re- familiar strange by impeding automatic, ha-
fers or alludes to what will happen; the bitual ways of perceiving, algebrization
latter does so explicitly. HGenette 1980. overautomatizes perception and allows for
See also FORESHADOWING. the greatest economy of perceptive effort.
advance notice. A narrative unit referring in ULemon & Reis 1965; Shklovsky 1965a.
advance to situations and events that will altomotif. A MOTIF occurring in a particular
occur and be recounted at a later point; a motifemic context; a motif manifesting a
r e p e a t i n g PROLEPSIS; a n ANTICIPATION:
specific MOTIFEME. Given a situation in
"That I was one day to experience a grief which it is forbidden to gather apples, for
as profound as that of my mother, we shall instance, a motif such as "The princess
see in the course of this narrative"; "We gathered apples" would be said to consti-
shall see also that, on the contrary, the tute an allomotif of the motifeme "violation."
Duchesse de Guermantes did associate Allomotifs are to motifemes what allo-
with Odette and Gilberte after the death of phones (variants of the same distinctive
Swann." fGenette 1980. See also AD- sound class) are to phonemes (distinctive
VANCE MENTION, ANACHRONY.
sound classes) and allomorphs to mor-
agent. 1. A human or humanized being phemes. HDundes 1964.
p e r f o r m i n g a n ACTION o r ACT; a CHARACTER alteration. An isolated change in FOCALIZA-
who acts and influences the course of TION;,A momentary infraction to the
events. 2. Along with the PATIENT, one of localization code governing a narrative.
two fundamental ROLES in Bremond's, typol- There are two types of alteration: giving
more information (PARALEPSIS) or giving c e r t a i n EXTENT o r AMPLITUDE ( t h e y cover a

less information (PARALIPSIS) than should certain amount of STORY TIME) as well as a
be given in terms of the governing code. certain REACH (the story time they cover is
IIGenette 1980. at a certain temporal distance from the
alternation. A combination of narrative SE- "present" moment): in "Mary sat down.
QUENCES (recounted in the same Four years later she would have the very
NARRATING INSTANCE o r i n d i f f e r e n t o n e s ) same impression and her excitement would
such that units of one sequence are made last for a whole month," the anachrony has
to alternate with units of another sequence; an extent of one month and a reach of four
an INTERWEAVING of sequences. A narra- years. fBa! 1985; Chatman 1978; Genette
tive like "John was happy, and Mary was 1980; Mosher 1980. See also ORDER.
unhappy; then John got divorced, and anagnorisis. See RECOGNITION. HAristotie
Mary got married; then John became un- 1968.
happy, and Mary became happy" can be analepsis. An ANACHRONY going backio the
said to result from the alternation of one past with respect to the "present" moment;
unit from "John was happy; then John got an evocation of one or more events that
divorced; then John became unhappy" and occurred before the "present" moment (or
one unit from "Mary was unhappy; then moment when the chronological recounting
Mary got married; then Mary became of a sequence of events is interrupted to
h a p p y . " HAIong with LINKING a n d EMBED- make room for the analepsis); a RETRO-
DING, alternation is one of the basic ways SPECTION; a FLASHBACK; " J o h n b e c a m e
of combining narrative sequences. TOucrot furious and, though he had vowed, many
and Todorov 1979; Prince 1973, 1982; To- years before, never to lose his temper, he
dorov 1966,1981. See also COMPLEX began to shout hysterically." HAnalepses
STORY. have a certain EXTENT as well as a certain
amplitude. See EXTENT. HChatman 1978. REACH: in "Mary could not face it. Yet she
anachrony. A discordance between the or- had spent several hours preparing for it the
der in which events (are said to) occur and day before," the anafepsis has an extent of
the order in which they are recounted: a several hours and a reach of one day.
beginning IN MEDIAS RES followed by a re- ^Completing analepses, or RETURNS, fill in
turn to earlier events constitutes a typical earlier gaps resulting from ELLIPSES in the
example of anachrony. 1!in relation to the narrative. Repeating analepses, or RE-
"present" moment, the moment when the CALLS, tell anew already mentioned past
chronological recounting of a sequence of events. HGenette 1980; Rimrnon 1976. See
events is interrupted to make room for a l s o ORDER, PROLEPSIS.

them, anachronies can go back to the past analysis. A technique whereby the thought
(RETROSPECTION, ANALEPSIS, FLASHBACK) and impressions of a character are re-
or forward to the future (ANTICIPATION, counted by the narrator in his or her own
PROLEPSIS, FLASHFORWARO). T h e y h a v e a name and language. IGenette 1980. See
a l s o INTERNAL ANALYSIS, NARRATIZED DIS- sification of a series of events or effects.
COURSE. HBrooks and Warren 1959. See also
a n a l y t i c a u t h o r . A n OMNISCIENT NARRATOR CLIMAX.

(The Red and the Black, Vanity Fair). antidonor. The opposite of the DONOR. The
^Brooks and Warren 1959. antidonor is a homologue of the OPPONENT.
anisochrony. A variation in narrative SPEED; TOreimas and Courtes 1932.
an acceleration or a slowdown in TEMPO. antihero. An unheroic HERO; a hero defined
T h e c h a n g e f r o m SCENE t o SUMMARY o r by negative or less than admirable attri-
summary to scene constitutes an aniso- butes; a PROTAGONIST whose character-
chrony. HGenette 1980. See also istics are antithetical to those traditionally
ISOCHRONY. associated with a hero. Bardamu in Ce-
antagonist. The major opponent of the PRO- line's Journey to the End of the Night, Jim
TAGONIST. A narrative articulated in terms Dixon in Kingsley Amis's Lucky Jim, and
of an interpersonal CONFLICT involves two Yossarian in Heller's Catch-22 are anti-
major characters with opposite goals: the heroes. ilScholes and Kellogg 1966.
protagonist (or the HERO) and the antago- antinarrativs. A (verbal or nonverbal) text
nist, or enemy. HFrye 1957. See also adopting the trappings of narrative but sys-
ANTISUBJECT, COUNTERPLOT. tematically calling narrative logic and
anterior narration. A NARRATION preceding narrative conventions into question; an AN-
in time the narrated situations and events; TISTORY. Robbe-Grillet's Jealousy and
a PRIOR NARRATING. A n t e r i o r n a r r a t i o n is Beckett's Molloy are antinarratives. HChat-
c h a r a c t e r i s t i c of PREDICTIVE NARRATIVE. man 1978.
'Prince 1982. antisender. The opposite of the SENDER.
a n t i c i p a t i o n . A PROLEPSIS, a FLASHFOR- The latter sends the SUBJECT on its quest
WARD, an ANACHRONY going forward to the and imparts a set of values to it; the Anti-
future with respect to the "present" mo- sender represents an opposite set of val-
ment (or moment when the chronological ues and sends the ANTISUBJECT on a quest
recounting of a sequence of events is in- that is at cross-purposes with that of the
terrupted to make room for the anticipa- Subject. HGreimas and Courtes 1982; Hen-
tion). HChatman 1978; Genette 1980; Latn- ault 1983.
mert 1955; Prince 1982. See also ADVANCE antistory. See ANTINARRATIVE. IChatman
NOTICE, ORDER. i 1978.
anticlimax. An event or series of events (es- antisubject. The opposite of the SUBJECT.
pecially at the end of a narrative or The Antisubject has aims that are at cross-
narrative SEQUENCE) noticeably and sur- purposes with those of the Subject. It
prisingly less important than the events should not be viewed as a mere OPPONENT
leading up to it; an effect turning out to be incidentally coming into conflict with or rep-
strikingly less significant or intense than resenting a momentary obstacle for the
expected; a break in the progressive inten- Subject in the letter's pursuit of its goal.
Like the latter, it is a quester, and the nar- (governed by operators o! permission, pro-
rative is articulated in terms of their hibition, and obligation), axiological
conflicting quests: in "The Final Problem," (governed by operators of goodness, bad-
Holmes represents the Subject and Mor- ness, and indifference), and epistemic
iarty the Antisubject. Hlf the Subject is (governed by operators of knowledge, ig-
c o n c r e t i z e d a s t h e PROTAGONIST a t t h e norance, and belief). Given a string of
l e v e l of n a r r a t i v e SURFACE STRUCTURE, t h e motifs analyzable as Lack (of some
Antisubject is concretized as the ANTAGO- value)—Liquidation of Lack, for example,
NIST. IIGreimas and Courtis 1982; Renault the string would constitute an axiological
1983; Rastier 1973. See also ANTISENDER. atomic story. UDolezel 197G. See also
a p p e l l a t i v e f u n c t i o n . T h e CONACTIVE FUNC- COMPOUND STORY, MODALITY, MOLECULAR

TION. IK. Buhler 1934. See also STORY.

CONSTITUTIVE FACTORS OF COMMUNICATION, a t t e m p t . I n STORY GRAMMARS, a c h a r a c t e r ' s

FUNCTIONS OF COMMUNICATION. effort t o r e a c h a GOAL or SUBGOAL. At-

archipersona. An ACTANT; a fundamental tempts usually consist of ore or more


narrative ROLE. ILotman 1977. EVENTS o r of a n e n t i r e EPISODE. f T h o r n -

argument 1. The summary of a narrative dyke 1977.


(usually consisting of the most important attribute. 1. A character TRAIT. 2. In
KERNELS making up that narrative). 2. In Propp's terminology, an external (as op-
Aristotelian terms, the set of events signifi- posed to functional) quality of a fairy-tale
cantly involved in the ACTION of a play or character, specifying his or her age, status,
epic. Some of these events may lie outside s e x , a p p e a r a n c e , e t c . T w o HEROE S c a n

the PLOT proper of the epic or play: they have very different attributes (though they
may occur before its BEGINNING, for exam- fulfill the same functions), and so can two
ple. In other words, argument is a larger DONORS o r too VILLAINS. UGarvey 1S78;

concept than plot: the murder of Laius is Propp 1968.


pari of the argument of Oedipus Rex but attributive discourse. The discourse ac-
not part of its plot. HAristotle 1968; Barthes companying a character's (direct)
1975. discourse and specifying the act of the
aspect. The VISION in terms of which a story speaker or thinker, identifying him or her,
is p r e s e n t e d ; FOCALIZATION; POINT OF VIEW. and (sometimes) indicating various dimen-
UTodorov 1966. sions or features of the act, the character,
atomic story. A string of MOTIFS governed the setting in which they appear, etc.:
by modal homogeneity: all modal formulas "—How are you? inquired John in a sono-
in an atomic story are constructed with op- rous voice white opening the door to the
erators pertaining to one and only one back room." Attributive discourse in a nar-
modality. Atomic stories can be alethic rative is equivalent to the set of T/VG
(governed by operators of possibility, im- CLAUSES in that narrative. ^Prince 1978;
possibility, and necessity), deontic Shapiro 1984.
L

auctorial narrative type. The class of HOMO- authentication function. The function in
DIEGETIC Or HETERODIEGETIC NARRATIVES terms of which a given MOTIF or NARRATIVE
characterized by ZERO FOCALIZATION (Moby STATEMENT is authenticated (assigned the
Dick, Bella, Eugenie Grandet, Tom Jones). status of a fact, given the value "authentic"
Along with the ACTORIAL and the NEUTRAL as opposed to "nonauthentic"). In THIRD-
NARRATIVE TYPES, it is one of three basic PERSON NARRATIVE, for example, motifs in-
classes in Lintvelt's typology. liGenette troduced by the narrator's discourse are
1983; Lintvelt 1981. See also POINT OF (conventionally) taken lo be authentic; on
VIEW. I the other hand, motifs introduced by the
auktoriala Erzfihlsituation. See AUTHORIAL characters' discourse are not: depending
NARRATIVE SITUATION. t S t a n z e l 1 9 6 4 , 1 9 7 1 , on the narrator's declarations (and the
1 9 8-1-4 f e f c - course of the action), they can turn out to
Au»«a9«, On® Of two linguistic subsystems, be authentic or nonauthentic. UDolezel
ger, who opposes it to 1980; Martinez-Bonati 1981; Ryan 1984.
rtOHAte ERZAHLEN (lie- S e e a l s o NARRATIVE WORLD.

usge (statement) author. The maker or composer of a narra-


^ p f e o B c a l . and prag- tive. This real or concrete author is not to
be confused with the IMPLIED AUTHOR of a
^ ^ ^ ^ g e u r r t n g , for narrative or with its NARRATOR and, unlike
them, is not immanent to or deducible from
l l f a r a a l (or the narrative. Nausea and "Erostratus," for
Vorigi- instance, have the same author—Sartre—
but different implied authors as well as dif-
ferent narrators. Similarly, a narrative can
have two or more real authors and one im-
plied author or one narrator (Naked Came
fie Stranger, the novels of Delly, Ellery
Qusan, etc.). IBeardsley 1958; Booth
1S83; Chatman 1978; Gibson 1950; Kayser
1853; Lintvelt 1981; Schmid 1973; Tillot-
| ® n 1959.

jWfiSrlal discourse. A narrative discourse


||8p)aylng signs of its NARRATOR or AUTHOR
Jjf his or her sovereign authority. As a
|©tjrs!ve mode, authorial (or auctorial)
HIOMras corresponds to the AUTHORIAL
JSATLVE SITUATION. It i s t h e d i s c o u r s e of

ted OMNISCIENT NARRATORS a n d c h a r -

t f t e e such novels as Tom Jones,

s:
Fathers and Sons, and Eugenie Grandet. INTRUSIVE NARRATOR.

HGenette 1960. author's s e c o n d self. See IMPLIED AUTHOR.


authorial narrative situation. A NARRATIVE TTillotson 1959.
SITUATION characterized by the omniscience autodiegetic narrative. A FIRST-PERSON
of a NARRATOR who is not a participant in NARRATIVE the NARRATOR of which is also
the situations and events recounted (Tom the PROTAGONIST or t h e HERO; a v a r i e t y o f

Jones, A Tale of Two Cities, Vanity Fair, HOMODIEGETIC NARRATIVE SUCh that the

Eugenie Grandet). Along with the FIRST- narrator is also the main character (Greer
PERSON a n d t h e FIGUHAL NARRATIVE SITU- Expectations, Kiss Me Deadly, The
ATIONS, the authorial narrative situation Stranger). ^Genette 1980; Lanser 1981.
(AUKTORIALE ERZAHLSITUATION) is o n e of See also DIEGETIC.
three basic types in Stanzel's classification. autonomous monologue, IMMEDIATE DIS-
UStanzel 1964, 1971,1984. See also AU- COURSE (Les Lauriers sont coupes). As
THORIAL DISCOURSE, OMNISCIENT NARRATOR, opposed to the''QU0TED MONOLOGUE, w h i c h
VISION, ZERO FOCALIZATION. is introduced by a narrator, the autono-
authority. The extent of a narrator's knowl- mous monologue is free of all narratorial
edge of the narrative situations and events. mediation or patronage. TCohn 1978,
A n OMNISCIENT NARRATOR ( T o m Jones, The 1981. S e e a l s o INTERIOR MONOLOGUE.

Red and the Black) has more authority auxiliant. A n ACTANTIAL ROLE whereby the
than one who does not provide an INSIDE SUBJECT is quaiilied along the axis of ability
VIEW of the characters ("Hills Like White El- (modalized as being able or not being a b l e
ephants"). HChatman 1978. See also to d o ) . A t t h e SURFACE STRUCTURE level,

PRIVILEGE. the auxiliant can be represented by the


author's intrusion. 1. An intervention by same ACTOR as the one representing the
the NARRATOR in the form of a comment on unmodalized Subject or by a different ac-
the situations and events presented, their tor. When the latter obtains, and depending
presentation or its context; a commentarial on the positive or negative nature of the
excursus by the narrator (Blin): "I know auxiliant, the actor functions as a HELPER
not, be it remarked by the way, whether or an OPPONENT. UGreimas and Courtes
this is not the same cell, the interior of 1982. See also MODALITY.
which may still be seen through a small
square aperture o n the east side, at about
the height of a man, on the platforms from
which the towers rise." 2. in fiction, a
passage felt to engage the responsibility of
the AUTHOR as opposed to that of the nar-
rator, a passage taken to betray the real background. The narrative space, SETTING,
author's hand. HBanfield 1982; Blin 1954; or c o l l e c t i o n of EXISTENTS a n d EVENTS
Genette 1980. See also COMMENTARY, against which other existents and events
emerge and come io the fore. ^iChatman and abstains from direct commentary and
1S78; Liddell 1947; Weinrich 1964. See interpretation. "SN. Friedman 1955b;
a l s o FIGURE, FOREGROUND, GROUND. Genette 1980; Lintvelt 1981; Prince 1982;
balance. One of six fundamental ROLES iso- Romberg 1962; Souvage 1965. See also
lated by Souriau in his study of the possi- DRAMATIC MODE, NEUTRAL NARRATIVE TYPE,

bilities of drama. The Balance (analogous POINT OF VIEW, VISION.

to Propp's DISPATCHER and Greimas's besprochene Welt. According to Weinrich,


SENDER) is the arbiter or rewarder, the at- one of two distinct and complementary cat-
tributor of the good, the imparter of values. egories of textual worlds, comprising such
*Scholes 1974; Souriau 1950. See also forms as dialogue, lyric poetry, the critical
ACTANT. essay, the political memorandum, and the
baring the device. See LAYING BARE. scientific report, and—in English—signaled
liTomashevsky 1965. by the use of the present, the present per-
beginning. The incident initiating the pro- fect, and the future. In the besprochene
cess of change in a PLOT or ACTION. This Welt (commented world) category—as op-
incident does not necessarily follow but is posed to the ERZAHLTE WELT (narrated
necessarily followed by other incidents. world) category—the ADDRESSER and AD-
'Students of NARRATIVE have emphasized DRESSEE are directly linked to and con-
that the beginning, which corresponds to cerned by what is described. Weinrich's
the passage from quiescence, homogene- distinction between besprochene Welt and
ity, and indifference to irritation, hetero- erzahlte Welt is analogous to Benveniste's
geneity and difference, provides narrative distinction between DISCOURS and HISTOIRE
with a forward-looking intention. It gives and is related to Hamburger's distinction
rise to a certain number of possibilities, b e t w e e n AUSSAGE a n d FIKTIONALE ERZAH-

and reading (processing) a narrative is, LEN. tRicoeur 1985; Weinrich 1964. See
among other things, wondering which will also TENSE.
and which will not be realized and finding block characterization. A (relatively) thor-
out. HAristotle 1968; Brooks 1984; Martin ough (physical and psychological)
1986; Prince 1982; Said 1975. See also description of a CHARACTER upon one ol
!
END, MIDDLE, NARRATIVITY. his or her first appearances, a set-piece
behaviorist narrative. An OBJECTIVE NARRA- presentation of a character's TRAITS.
TIVE; a narrative characterized by EXTERNAL HSouvage 1965. See also CHARACTERIZA-
FOCALIZATION and thus limited to the co'n- TION.
i
veyance of the characters' behavior (words b o u n d motif. A CARDINAL FUNCTION; a NU-
and actions but not thought or feelings), CLEUS; a KERNEL. F o r T o m a s h e v s k y a n d

their appearance, and the setting against the Russian Formalists, bound motifs (as
which they come to the fore ("The Killers"), opposed to FREE MOTIFS) are logically es-
fin this type of narrative, the narrator tells sential to the narrative action and cannot
less than one or several characters know be eliminated without destroying its causal-
chronological coherence. ITomashevsky ically essential to the narrative action, and
1 9 6 5 . S e e a l s o MOTIF. their elimination does not destroy its
causal-chronological coherence: rather
than constituting crucial nodes in the ac-
tion, they fill in the narrative space
between these nodes. IBarthes 1975. See
a l s o FUNCTION.

catastrophe. The precipitating final stage of


c a m e r a . O n e of e i g h t t y p e s of POINT OF VIEW a play; the scene bringing the dramatic
according to Friedman, who regards it as conflict to an end. The term usually desig-
the ultimate in narratorial exclusion and nates the unhappy DENOUEMENT of
gives as an example of it the opening sec- tragedy. IjFreytag 1894. See also FREY-
tion of Isherwcod's Goodbye to Berlin: "I TAG'S PYRAMID, PLOT.

am a camera with its shutter open, quite causality. A relation of cause and effect be-
passive, recording, not thinking. Recording tween (sets of) situations and/or events.
the man shaving at the window opposite ICausality can be explicit ("Mary liked to
and the woman in the kimono washing her read because she was smart") or implicit
hair. Someday, all this will have to be de- ("It was raining, and John got wet"). When
veloped, carefully printed, fixed.' The implicit, it is inferrable on logical, necessary
camera or CAMERA EYE (presumably) re- grounds ("All gamblers are sad. Susan
cords, without ostensible organization or was a gambler. She was sad") or on prag-
selection, whatever is before it. matic, probabilistic grounds: if one event
UN. Friedman 1955b. follows another event in time and is (plau-
camera eye. A technique whereby the situ- sibly) relatable to it, the second event is
ations and events conveyed (presumably) taken to be caused by the first unless the
"just happen" before a neutral recorder narrative specifies otherwise (compare
and are transmitted by it (U.S.A. trilogy); "Jane insulted Nancy, and Nancy felt bad"
CAMERA. IChatman 1978; N. Friedman with "Jane insulted Nancy, and Nancy felt
1955b; Magny 1972. bad, but it had nothing to do with Jane's
c a r d i n a l f u n c t i o n . A KERNEL; a NUCLEUS; a behavior"; or "Peter ate an apple and got
BOUND MOTIF. A s o p p o s e d t o CATALYSES, sick" with "Peter ate an apple and got sick,
cardinal functions are logically essential to but his doctors determined that the sick-
the narrative action and cannot be elimi- ness was not caused by the apple").
nated without destroying its causal- According to Barthes (following Aristotle),
chronological coherence. HBarthes 1975. the confusion between consecutiveness
S e e a l s o FUNCTION. and consequence, chronology and causal-
c a t a l y s i s . A SATELLITE; a FREE MOTIF; a m i - ity, constitutes perhaps the most powerful
nor event in a plot. As opposed to motor of NARRATIVITY: narrative would rep-
CARDINAL FUNCTIONS, c a t a l y s e s a r e n o t l o g - resent the systematic exploitation of the
POST HOC ERGO PROPTER HOC FALLACY, terms of their conformity to standard ROLES
whereby what-comes-after-X is interpreted (the eiron or self-deprecator, the alazon or
as what-is-caused-by-X. Whether implicit braggart, the ingenue, the femme fatale,
or explicit, causa! links may reflect a psy- the cuckold) or TYPES; and in terms of their
chological order (for example, a character's correspondence to certain SPHERES OF AC-
actions are the cause or consequence of TION (that of t h e HERO or t h a t of t h e VILLAIN,

his personality or her state of mind), a for instance) or their concretizing certain
philosophical order (every event exempli- ACTANTS (the SENDER, the RECEIVER, t h e

fies the theory of universal determinism, for SUBJECT, the OBJECT). fThough the term
instance), a social order, a political one, character is most often used with refer-
and so forth. HAristotle 1968; Barthes ence to existents in the world of the
1975; Chatman 1978; Pratt 1977; Prince situations and events recounted, it is also
1982; Todorov 1981. See also METONYMY, used sometimes to refer to the NARRATOR
NARRATIVE, PLOT. and the NARRATEE. 2. An actor; an exis-
c e n t r a ! c o n s c i o u s n e s s , FOCALIZER; REFLEC- tent engaged in an action. 3. In Aristotelian
TOR; CENTRAL INTELLIGENCE; h o l d e r of terms, and along with THOUGHT (DIANOIA),
POINT OF VIEW. The central consciousness one of two qualities that an AGENT (or
is the consciousness through which situ- PRATTON) has. C h a r a c t e r (ETHOS) is t h e

ations and events are perceived. element in accordance with which agents
HH. James 1972. See also FOCALIZATION, can be said to be of a certain type. It is a
FOCUS OF NARRATION, PERSPECTIVE. secondary element, consisting of the type
central intelligence. See CENTRAL CON- traits added to the agent in order to char-
SCIOUSNESS. HH. James 1972. acterize it. Whereas thought is revealed by
character. 1. An EXISTENT endowed with the agent's statements as well as by his or
anthropomorphic traits and engaged in an- her thinking and arguing, character is re-
thropomorphic actions; an ACTOR with vealed by the agent's choices, decisions,
anthropomorphic attributes. HCharacters and actions, and by the way they are per-
can be more or less major or minor (in formed. HAIexandrescu 1974; Aristotle
terms of textual prominence), dynamic 1968; Barthes 1974; Bourneuf and Ouellet
(when they change) or static (when they do 1975; Bremond 1973; Chatman 1978;
not), consistent (when their attributes and Ducrot and Todorov 1979; Forster 1927;
actions do not result in contradiction) or in- N. Friedman 1975; Frye 1957; Garvey
consistent, and FLAT (simple, two- 1978; Hamon 1972, 1983; Harvey 1965;
dimensional, endowed with very few traits, Hochman 1985; Lotman 1977; Scholes
highly predictable in behavior) or ROUND and Kellogg 1966; Todorov 1969; Zeraffa
(complex, multidimensional, capable of sur- 1969. S e e also ANTAGONIST, CHARACTERI-

prising behavior). They are also classable ZATION, PROTAGONIST, STOCK CHARACTER.

in terms of their actions, or their words, c h a r a c t e r - ! . T h e " I " of a CHARACTER w h o

their feelings, their appearance, etc.; in also functions as the NARRATOR of the situ-
ations and events in which he or she plays observes chronological order, whereas
a part. In "I ate s hamburger," the "I" who "Harry slept alter he worked" does not.
ate is the character-l and the "I" who tells ^Chronological order is very much privi-
about the eating the NARRATOR-I. ^Prince leged by positivistic historiography. ^Prince
1 3 8 2 . See a l s o FIRST-PERSON NARRATIVE, 1 9 7 3 . S e e a l s o FABULA, OROER, STORY.

HOMODIEGETIC NARRATIVE. ehronotope. The nature of and relationship


characterization. 1. The set of techniques between represented temporal and spatial
resulting in the constitution of CHARACTER. categories. The term designates and em-
Characterization can be more or less direct phasizes the utter interdependence of
(a character's TRAITS are reliably stated by space and time in (artistic) representations:
the narrator, the character herself, or an- it literally means "time-space." STexts and
other character) or indirect (dedudble from classes of texts model reality and create
the character's actions, reactions, thoughts, world pictures according to different chron-
emotions, etc.); it can rely on a set-piece otopes (different kinds of time-space
presentation of the character's (main) attri- complexes) and are definable in terms of
butes (BLOCK CHARACTERIZATION) or favor them. For example, the "adventure" chron-
their introduction one at a time; it can em- otope, exemplified by such Greek
phasize their permanence or undedine romances as Daphnis and Chloe or Ae-
their mutableness; it can privilege typicality thiopica, features a thoroughly abstract
(making the character conform to a certain time (removed from historical and bio-
TYPE) or, on ihe contrary, individualization; graphical times, consisting or unrelated and
and so forth. 2. in Aristotelian terms, the reversible moments, involving no biological
assignment of type traits to an AGENT or psychological transformation) that com-
(PRATTON). Characterization observes four bines in an external manner with an
principles: the agent should have a certain equally nonspecific and nondetermining
moral elevation (chreston); s/he should be geographical space (the adventures de-
endowed with traits appropriately related to picted can occur in any number of
the action (harmotton); s/he should have locations and are in no way affected by
idiosyncrasies and be like an individual them). HBakhtin 1981; Clark and Holquist
(homoios); and s/he should be consistent 1984.
(homalon). lAristotle 1968; Booth 1983; classeme. In Greimassian terminology
Chatman 1978; Ducrot and Todorov 1979; (adapted from Bernard Pottier's), a contex-
Garvey 1978; Hamon 1972,1983; Lubbock tual SEME as opposed to a nuclear or basic
1921; Margolin 1983; Scholes and Kellogg one; a seme educed by the context in
1966; Wellek and Warren 1949. See also which it recurs. According to Greimas, the
EMBLEM, MASK, STOCK CHARACTER. noun roar, for instance, which may be said
chronological order. The arrangement of to contain the nuclear seme "kind of cry,"
situations and events in the order ol their has the contextual seme "animal" in "the
occurrence. "Harry washed, then he slept" lion's roar was scary" and the contextual
seme "human" in "the policeman's roar tin 1986; Prince 1982. See also
was scary." <iClassemes provide texts with CONSTITUTIVE FACTORS OF COMMUNICATION,

coherence. IGreimas 1983b; Greimas and METALINGUAL FUNCTION, NARRATIVE CODE.

Courtes 1982. See also ISOTOPY, SEMEME. commentary. A commentarial excursus by


climax. The point of greatest tension; the the NARRATOR; a n AUTHOR'S INTRUSION; a

culminating point in a progressive intensifi- narratorial intervention going beyond the


cation. In traditional PLOT structure, the identification or description of EXISTENTS
climax constitutes the highest point of the and the recounting of EVENTS. In commen-
RISING ACTION. liSrooks and Warren 1959; tary, the narrator explains the meaning or
Freytag 1894; Tomashevsky 1965. See significance of a narrative element, makes
a l s o ANTICLIMAX, FALLING ACTION', FREY- value judgments, refers to worlds tran-
TAG'S PYRAMID. scending the characters' world, and/or
coda. A statement indicating that a NARRA- comments on his or her own narration.
TIVE is over: "and they lived happily ever ^Commentary can be simply ornamental; it
after" is a common coda. ^ILabov 1972; can fulfill a rhetorical purpose; and it can
Pratt 1977. function as an essential part of the dra-
code. 1. One of the fundamental 'constitu- matic structure of the narrative. HBooth
ents of any act of (verbal) communication. 1983; Chatman 1978; Lintveit 1981; Wein-
The code is the system of norms, rules, rich 1964. See also DESCRIPTION,
and constraints in terms of which the MES- DESCRIPTIVE PAUSE, DISCOURSE, EVALUA-

SAGE signifies. It is at least partially TION, INTRUSIVE NARRATOR, NARRATION.

c o m m o n t o t h e ADDRESSER a n d ADDRESSEE c o m m u n i c a t i o n . S e e CONSTITUTIVE FACTORS


of the message. HThe opposition between OF COMMUNICATION.
code and message is analogous to but competence. 1 . S e e NARRATIVE COMPE-
more general than the famous Saussurean TENCE. 2. In Greimassian terminology,
opposition between LANGUE (language sys- that which makes action possible and,
tem) and PAROLE (individual utterance): just more specifically, the qualification of the
as the language system governs the pro- SUBJECT along the axes of desire (wanting
duction (and reception) of the individual to do) and/or obligation (having to do) and
utterance, the code governs the production the axes of knowledge (knowing how to
(and reception) of the message. 2. One do) and/or ability (being able to do). HAdam
ol the "voices" (models of the already- 1984, 1985; Greimas 1970,1983a; Grei-
known, models of reality) out of which a mas and Courtes 1976, 1982; H6nault
narrative is woven. According to Barthes, 1983; Prince 1981, 1981-82. See also MO-
narrative and its constitutive units signify in DALITY, NARRATIVE SCHEMA, PERFORMANCE.
terms of one or more such voices or codes complex story. A story combining two or
(PROAIRETIC, HERMENEUTIC, REFERENTIAL,
m o r e (MINIMAL) STORIES o r NARRATIVES
SEMIC, SYMBOLIC, etc.). HBarthes 1974, t h r o u g h LINKING, EMBEDDING, o r ALTERNA-
1981a; Culler 1975; Jakobson 1960; Mar- TION. "John was rich and Mary was poor;
then Mary won the lottery and she became strings of MOTIFS governed by a different
rich; then John squandered his money and MODALITY); a MOLECULAR STORY. I D o l e z e l

he became poor" represents a complex 1976.


story that can be said to obtain from the conative function. One of the FUNCTIONS OF
embedding of "Mary was poor; then Mary COMMUNICATION in terms of which any
won the lottery, and she became rich" into communicative (verbal) act may be struc-
"John was rich; then John squandered his tured and oriented; the APPELLATIVE
money, and he became poor." UPrince FUNCTION. When the communicative act is
1 9 7 3 . S e e a l s o SEQUENCE. centered on the ADDRESSEE (rather than on
complicating action. In Labov's terminol- o n e of t h e o t h e r CONSTITUTIVE FACTORS O F

ogy, the part of a NARRATIVE that defines it COMMUNICATION), it (mainly) has a conative
as such. The complicating action follows function. More specifically, those passages
t h e ORIENTATION a n d l e a d s to t h e RESULT in narrative focusing on the NARRATEE c a n
or RESOLUTION. It is the MIDDLE of a n AC- be said to fulfill a conative function: "You
TION, t h e COMPLICATION, the b r i d g e will do the same, you, my reader, now
between the initial situation and its final holding this book in your white hands, and
modification. If a narrative is taken to con- saying to yourself in the depths of your
stitute a series of answers to certain easy chair: I wonder if it will amuse me!"
questions, the complicating action is that HJakobson 1960; Prince 1982.
constituent of it answering the question conceptual point of view. The world view
"Then what happened?" TiLabov 1972; or conceptual system in terms of which a
Pratt 1977. situation or event is considered. tChaiman
complication. 1. The part of a narrative 1 9 7 8 . S e e a l s o PERCEPTUAL POINT OF VIEW,

following the EXPOSITION and leading to the POINT OF VIEW.

DENOUEMENT; t h e MIDDLE of a n ACTION; conflict. The struggle in which the ACTORS


t h e COMPLICATING ACTION; the RAVELLING. are engaged. The latter can fight against
2. In t r a d i t i o n a l PLOT s t r u c t u r e , t h e RISING Fate or destiny, against their social or
ACTION (from EXPOSITION to CLIMAX). 3 . in physical environment, or against one an-
Proppian terms, FUNCTIONS Vlli-XI: villainy other (external conflict), and they can fight
or lack, mediation, beginning counteraction, against themselves (internal or inner con-
and departure. 4. In Aristotelian terms, flict). HBrooks and Warren 1959. See also
the situation obtaining before the BEGIN- NARRATIVITY.

NING of the action; desis. F Aristotle 1983; c o n j o i n i n g . S e e LINKING.

Brooks and Warren 195S; Freytag 1894; conjunction. Along with DISJUNCTION, one of
P r o p p 1 9 6 8 . S e e a l s o FREYTAG'S PYRAMID. the two basic types of JUNCTION, or rela-
composition. See MOTIVATION. HWellek and t i o n , b e t w e e n t h e SUBJECT a n d t h e OBJECT

("X is with Y," "X has Y"). tGreimas and


Warren 1949.
C o u r t s 1982; H<§nault 1983.
compound story. A story composed of two
consonance. The fusion between a NARRA-
or more ATOMIC STORIES ( t w o o r m o r e
TOR and the character's consciousness he a six-factor schema including the ad-
or she narrates (Portrait of the Artist as a dresser (the sender or encoder of the MES-
Young Man). Consonance is characteristic SAGE), the addressee (the receiver or
of the relationship between narrator and decoder of the message), the message it-
PROTAGONIST in a FIGURAL NARRATIVE SITUA- self, the CODE (in terms of which the
TION. H C o h n 1978. S e e also DISSONANCE. message signifies), the context (or REFER-
constative. An utterance that reports events ENT to which the message refers), and the
or states of affairs in certain worlds and CONTACT (the psychophysiological connec-
therefore has the properly of being "either tion between the addresser and the
true or false" in these worlds: "Napoleon addressee):
won the battle of Austertitz" and "The earth Context
is flat" are constatives. liThe theory of
Message
SPEECH ACTS o r i g i n a t e s i n J. L. A u s t i n ' s
Addresser Addressee
distinction between constatives and PER-
FORMATIVES (utterances like "I promise to Contact

come tomorrow" that are used to do rather Code


than to say something, to perform an act
rather than to state that something is or is Some theorists (Hymes, for example) pre-
not the case). However, as Austin goes on fer to speak of seven factors and replace
to argue, constatives are themselves per- context with topic (what is communicated
formatives, since saying (asserting, stating, about) and setting (the scene, the situation,
reporting) that something is or is not the the context of the communicative act). ITo
case constitutes a kind of doing. I l f narra- each of the factors corresponds a particu-
tive can be said to "constate," to report lar FUNCTION OF COMMUNICATION, a n d a n y
that certain situations and events are the communicative act fulfills one or more of
case in certain worlds, it can also be said these functions. UK. Buhler 1934; Hymes
to perform (at the very least) the act of re- 1970; Jakobson 1960.
porting. HAustin 1962; Lyons 1977; Pratt constitutive model. The Greimassian model
1977. S e e also ILIOCUTIONARY ACT. describing the elementary structure of sig-
constitutional model. See CONSTITUTIVE nification, the model accounting for the
MODEL. basic articulations of meaning within a se-
constitutive factors of communication. mantic micro-universe. It is represented
The elements entering into any act of (ver- v i s u a l l y b y t h e SEMIOTIC SQUARE. I G r e i m a s

bal) communication and essential to its 1970, 1983b; Greimas and Courtis 1982.
operation. «;Buhler had isolated three such contact. 1. One of the fundamental constit-
e l e m e n t s : the ADDRESSER, t h e ADDRESSEE, uents of any act of (verbal) communication.
and the CONTEXT. Jakobson, in what has The contact is the physical channel and
proven to be the most influential model of psychological connection that allows the
communication in NARRATOLOGY, proposed ADDRESSER a n d t h e ADDRESSEE t o e n t e r
and stay in communication. 2. The rela- on singing. The bells kept on ringing. J o h n

tion between the NARRATOR and the suddenly got up and went into the bed-

NARRATEE (Lanser). Along with STANCE and room," the first two sentences are coor-

STATUS, contact is one of three basic rela- dinate clauses. ULabov and Waletzky 1967.

tions in terms of which POINT OF VIEW is S e e a l s o FREE CLAUSE, NARRATIVE CLAUSE,

structured. Wakobson 1950; Lanser 1981. RESTRICTED CLAUSE.

S e e a l s o CONSTITUTIVE FACTORS OF COMMU- c o u n t e r p l o t . A unified set of actions di-


NICATION, PHATIC FUNCTION. rected toward a result opposite the result
content. Following Hjelmslev, one of the two intended by the actions of the (main) PLOT:
planes of any semiotic system: the "what" the ANTAGONIST'S actions and goals can be
that is signified as opposed to the "way" it taken to make up a counterplot. HSouvage
is signified. Like the EXPRESSION plane, the 1965.
content plane has a FORM and a SUB- covert narrator. A n effaced NARRATOR; a
STANCE. W h e n used in connection with non-iNTRUSivE a n d UNDRAMATIZED NARRA-

narrative, content can be said to be equiv- TOR; a narrator presenting situations and
alent t o STORY (as o p p o s e d to DISCOURSE). events v/ith a minimum amount of narrato-
HChatman 1978; Hjelmslev 1954, 1961; rial mediation ("The Dead," The Spoils of
Prince 1973. Poynton). Covert narrators are typical of

context. One of the fundamental constitu- positivistic historiography. fiChatman 1978.

ents of any act of (verbal) communication. S e e a l s o ABSENT NARRATOR, DRAMATIZED

The context or REFERENT is that which the NARRATOR, OVERT NARRATOR.

MESSAGE refers to, that which it is about. crisis. The TURNING POINT, the decisive m o -
HJakobson 1960. See also CONSTITUTIVE ment on which the plot will turn. ?Holman
FACTORS OF COMMUNICATION, REFERENTIAL 1972; Madden 1979.
FUNCTION. c u l t u r a l c o d e . T h e REFERENTIAL CODE.

contract. 1 . S e e NARRATIVE CONTRACT. 11AII of the codes or models of the already-

2. In the Greimassian model, an agree- known are culturally determined; but the

ment between the SENDER and the so-called referential or cultural code is the

SUBJECT. It provides the latter with a pro- most obviously cultural among them.

gram to realize and can thus be said to HBarthes 1974; 1981a.

constitute the mainspring of (canonical) c u t b a c k . A n ANALEPSIS, a FLASHBACK, a RET-

NARRATIVES. TThe Subject can fulfill (or fail ROSPECTION, a SWITCHBACK. ^ B r o o k s a n d

to fulfill) the contract and be rewarded (or Warren 1S59. See also ORDER.
punished). HAdam 1984; Barthes 1974;
Greimas 1983a, 1983b; Greimas and
Courtes 1982. See also MANIPULATION,
SANCTION.

coordinate ciauses. Clauses having identi-


cal DISPLACEMENT SETS. In "The birds kept
I S 1 _ . S " K _ ... — I — — c —

TRANENIYE) captures the purpose of


(literary) art: the promotion of awareness.
ULemon and Reis 1965; Shklovsky 1965a.
S e e a l s o ALGEBRIZATION.

decisive test. One of the three TESTS char- deictic. Any term or expression which, in an
acterizing the movement of the SUBJECT utterance, refers to the context of produc-
) in t h e c a n o n i c a l NARRATIVE SCHEMA. P r e - tion (ADDRESSER, ADDRESSEE, time, place)
s u p p o s e d b y t h e GLORIFYING TEST a n d of that utterance: "here," "now," "yester-
p r e s u p p o s i n g t h e QUALIFYING TEST, t h e day," "i," "you," etc., are deictics, and in a
decisive (or main) test results in the CON- statement like "She saw him yesterday,"
JUNCTION of Subject and OBJECT. IIGreimas the adverb helps to locate what is reported
1983a, 1983b; Greimas and Courtes 1982; relative to the addresser (in terms of his or
Henauit 1983. See also PERFORMANCE. her present, what is reported occurred the
deep structure. The abstract underlying day before). IlKate Hamburger noted that,
structure of narrative; the MACROSTRUC- in narrative fiction, deictic (temporal) adver-
TURE of narrative. The deep structure bials—which, in statements about reality,
consists of global syntactico-semAntic rep- refer in terms of a present—are often
resentations determining the meaning of linked to past tenses: consider "Mary
the narrative and is converted into SUR- learned that John was in town. She now
FACE STRUCTURE b y a set of o p e r a t i o n s or faced a crucial decision" or "He got angry.
of TRANSFORMATIONS. In t h e G r e i m a s s i a n Yesterday, fie had accepted everything,
model of narrative, for example, whereas but he wasn't going to take it anymore."
ACTANTS and actantial relations would be She interpreted this as evidence that,
elements of the deep structure, ACTORS rather than signifying real time, rather than
and actorial relations would be found at the labelling the situations and events narrated
surface-structure level. In other models of as pertaining to a former time, past tenses
narrative, whereas the deep structure in narrative fiction designate these situ-
might be said to correspond to STORY, the ations and events as fictive and as
surface structure might be said to corre- "occurring" in the characters' fictive and
spond to OISCOURSE. 1FThe term and "time-less" present. TOenveniste 1971;
concept were adapted from Chomsky and Hamburger 1973; Palmer 1981. See also
generative-transformational grammar. DEIXIS, EPIC PRETERITE, SHIFTER, TENSE.
liChomsky 1965; van Dijk 1972; Fuger deixis. The general phenomenon of the oc-
1972; Johnson and Manaler 1980. See currence of DEICTICS; the set of references
also NARRATIVE GRAMMAR. to the situation (interlocutors, time, place)
defamiliarization. Making the familiar of an utterance. ®Benveniste 1971; Palmer
strange by impeding automatic, habitual 1981.
ways of perceiving. For Shklovsky and the d e n o u e m e n t The outcome or untying of the
Russian Formalists, defamiliarization (os- PLOT; t h e UNRAVELLING o f t h e COMPLICA-
TION; the END. HEjxenbaum 1971a. See a DESCRIPTION. UNot ail pauses are de-
alSO CATASTROPHE, FALLING ACTION, RESO- scriptive pauses: some are the result of
LUTION. COMMENTARY. Furthermore, not every d e -
description. The representation' of objects, scription occasions a pause in the
beings, situations, or (nonpurposeful, non- narrative: "The hall. . . was rather shal-
volitional) happenings in their spatial rather low in proportion to its length, and opened
than temporal existence, their topological in great arched bays into a sort of lobby
rather than chronological functioning, their surrounding it, in which serving-tables were
simultaneity rather than succession. It is placed" constitutes a descriptive pause be-
traditionally distinguished from' NARRATION cause it does not correspond to any
and from COMMENTARY. Wny description passage of time in the worid represented
can be said to consist of a theme designat- (by The Magic Mountain). On the other
ing the object, being, situation, or happen- hand, "After the fish followed an excellent
ing described (e.g., "house") and a set of meat dish, with garnishings, then a sepa-
subthemes designating its component parts rate vegetable course, then roast fowl, a
(e.g., "door," "room," "window," "wall"). pudding . . . and lastly cheese and fruit"
The theme or subthemes can be charac- does not constitute a descriptive pause (in
terized qualitatively (in terms of their the same narrative). HGenette 1980.
qualities: "the door was beautiful," "the determination. The temporal limits of an IT-
wall was green") or functionally (in terms of ERATIVE NARRATIVE; t h e s p a n of t i m e in
their function or use: "the room was only which an event (or a set of events) is said
used for special occasions"). HA descrip- to recur: " I went to summer camp every
tion can be more or less detailed and year, from 1959 to 1954" has a determina-
precise; objective or subjective; typical and tion of five years. HGenette 1980.
stylized or, on the contrary, individualizing; diachronic anaiysis. The study of changes
decorative or explanatory/functional (estab- in (linguistic) systems or parts thereof
lishing the tone or mood of a passage, across time. ^iSaussure 1966. See also
conveying plot-relevant information, con- SYNCHRONIC ANALYSIS.
tributing to characterization, introducing or dialogic narrative. A narrative characterized
reinforcing a theme, symbolizing a conflict by the interaction of several voices, con-
to come); and so on and so forth. ^Bal sciousnesses, or world views, none of
1977, 1983, 1985; Bonheim 1982, Bour- which unifies or is superior to (has more
neuf and Ouellet 1975; Debray-Geneite authority than) the others; a POLYPHONIC
1980, 1982; Genette 1976, 1983; Hamon NARRATIVE. In d i a l o g i c a s o p p o s e d t o MONO-

1981, 1982; Ricardou 1967, 1971, 1973, LOGIC NARRATIVE, t h e n a r r a t o r ' s v i e w s ,

1978; Riffaterre 1972,1972-73. See also judgments, and even knowledge do not
DESCRIPTIVE PAUSE, SET DESCRIPTION, SET- constitute the ultimate authority with re-
TING. spect to the world represented but only

descriptive pause. A PAUSE occasioned by one contribution among several, a contri-


JRES ESESS

bution that is in dialogue with and tradiegetic narrator); and they can appear
frequently less significant and perceptive in a METADIEGETIC o r HYPODIEGETIC NARRA-

than that of (some of) the characters. Ac- TIVE (a narrative embedded within the
cording to Bakhtin, Dostoevsky's fiction diegetic or intradiegetic narrative). Further-
(say, The Brothers Karamazov) provides more, narrators can also be characterized
particularly good examples of dialogic nar- in terms of the role (or lack thereof) they
o rative. ^Bakhtin 1981, 1984; Pascal 1977. play in the diegesis they present; a HOMO-
dialogue. The representation (dramatic in DIEGETIC NARRATOR is o n e w h o is a

type) of an oral exchange involving two or character in the situations and events s/he
more characters. In dialogue, the charac- recounts (when s/he is the protagonist of
ters' speeches are presented as they these situations and events, an AUTODI-
(supposedly) were uttered and may or may EGETIC NARRATIVE o b t a i n s ) ; a HETERO-

not be accompanied by TAG CLAUSES. DIEGETIC NARRATOR, on the other hand, is


HGifowinski 1974; Stanzel 1984. See also one who is not a character in the situations
ABRUPTIVE DIALOGUE, DIRECT SPEECH, and events s/he recounts. Finally, when a
MONOLOGUE, REPORTED SPEECH, SCENE. SECOND-DEGREE NARRATIVE i s b r o u g h t up

dianoia. See THOUGHT. HAristotle 1968. to the level of the primary narrative (when
diegesis. 1. The (fictional) world in which a metadiegetic narrative functions as if it
the situations and events narrated occur (in were a diegetic one), a PSEUDO-OIEGETIC
French, didgese). 2. TELLING, recounting, or REDUCED METADIEGETIC NARRATIVE o b -

as opposed to SHOWING, enacting (in tains. HGenette 1980, 1983; Rimmon 1976.
French; d i e s i s ) . HAristotle 1968; Genette diegetic level. The level at which an exis-
1980, 1983; Plato 1968. See also OIE- tent, event, or act of recounting is situated
GETIC, MIMESIS. with regard to a given DIEGESIS (dtegese).

diegetic. Pertaining to or part of a given DIE- In Manon Lescaut, for example, M. de Re-

GESIS (diigese) and, more particularly, that noncourt's recounting of his memoirs

diegesis represented by the (PRIMARY) NAR- occurs at an EXTRADIEGETIC level; the situ-

RATIVE. ^Narratives, narrators and ations and events recounted in these

narratees, existents and events, are char- memoirs (including Des Grieux's telling of

acterizable in diegetic terms. Existents, for his and Manon's adventures) occur at the

example, can be part of different diegeses, OIEGETIC or INTRADIEGETIC level; and these

or they can belong to the same one (they adventures take place in a METADIEGETIC

are then said to be ISODIEGETIC). Similarly, or HYPODIEGETIC NARRATIVE. ^ G e n e t t e

narrators can be described according to 1980, 1983.

DIEGETIC LEVEL. T h e y c a n b e EXTRADI- direct discourse. A TYPE OF DISCOURSE

EGETIC (not part of, external to, any whereby a character's utterances or

diegesis); they can be OIEGETIC or IN- thoughts are given or quoted in the way

TRADIEGETIC (belonging to the diegesis the character (presumably) formulated

presented in a primary narrative by an ex- them, as opposed to INDIRECT DISCOURSE:


compare "John said: —I am doing it" and FORM (it consists of a connected set of
"John said that he was doing it." In NARRATIVE STATEMENTS that state the story
TAGGED DIRECT DISCOURSE, t h e s e formula- and, more specifically, determine the OR-
tions are accompanied by TAG CLAUSES DER of presentation of situations and
characterizing some of their qualities, iden- events, the POINT OF VIEW governing that
tifying the speaker (or thinker), etc.: "—It's presentation, the narrative SPEED, the kind
the larynx, isn't it? Hans Castorp asked, in- o f COMMENTARY, a n d s o o n ) . " T h e m a n a t e ,

clining his head in answer"; "—And what then he slept" and "The man slept after he
kind of emissary are you, may I ask? Hans ate" have the same discourse substance
Castorp thought. Aloud he said: —Thank (the written English language) but different
you, Professor Naphta." Sometimes, the discourse forms. 2. According to Benven-
formulations are not accompanied by a tag iste, and along with history or story
clause, but narratorial mediation is indi- (HISTOIRE), one of two distinct and comple-
cated by such signs as quotation marks, mentary linguistic subsystems, in discourse
dashes, etc.:"—How are you? —Very (DISCOURS), a link is established between a
w e l l ! a n d y o u ? " In FREE DIRECT DISCOURSE, state or event and the situation in which
no tag clause is used and neither are other that state or event is linguistically evoked.
signs of narratorial mediation. HChatman Discours ihus involves some reference to
1978; Genette 1980, 1983; Lanser 1981; t h e ENUNCIATION a n d i m p l i e s a SENDER a n d

Todorov 1981. See also REPORTED DIS- a RECEIVER. Histoire, on the other hand,
COURSE. does not. Compare "He has gone" or "I've

direct speech, DIRECT DISCOURSE, especially told you about it hundreds of times" with

direct discourse whereby a character's "He went" or "She told her about it

utterances <as opposed to thoughts) are hundreds of times." HBenveniste's distinc-

represented. ^Chatman 1978. See also tion between histoire and discours is

DIALOGUE. analogous to Weinrich's distinction be-

d i r e c t s t y l e . S e e DIRECT DISCOURSE. t w e e n ERZAHLTE WELT a n d BESPROCHENE

discours. See DISCOURSE. UBenveniste WELT and reminiscent of Hamburger's dis-

1971. t i n c t i o n b e t w e e n FIKTIONALE ERZAHLEN a n d

discourse. 1. The EXPRESSION plane of AUSSAGE. UBenveniste 1971; Chatman

NARRATIVE a s o p p o s e d to its CONTENT 1978; Genette 1976, 1980,1983. See also

plane or STORY; the "how" of a narrative TENSE.

as opposed to its "what"; the NARRATING discourse time. The time taken by the rep-
a s o p p o s e d to t h e NARRATED; the NARRA- resentation of the NARRATED; the time of
TION a s o p p o s e d t o t h e FICTION (in t h e NARRATING; ERZAHLZEIT. HChatman
Ricardou's sense of the terms). ^Discourse 1 9 7 8 . S e e a l s o DURATION, STORY TIME,
h a s a SUBSTANCE ( a m e d i u m of MANIFESTA- TENSE.
TION: oral or written language, still or discovery. See RECOGNITION. HAristotle
moving pictures, gestures, etc.) and a 1968.
disjunction. Along with CONJUNCTION, one ous the details provided about the narrated
of two basic types of JUNCTION, or relation, situations and events, the smaller the dis-
b e t w e e n t h e SUBJECT a n d the OBJECT ( " X tance that is said to obtain' between them
is not with Y," "X does not have V"). a n d t h e i r NARRATION, MIMESIS or SHOWING,

^Greimas and Courtes 1982; Henault for example, is taken to institute less dis-
1983. tance than QIEGESIS or TELLING. 2. The
dispatcher. One of the seven fundamental (metaphorical) space between NARRATOR,
ROLES that a character may assume (in a CHARACTERS, situations and events nar-
fairy tale), according to Propp. The dis- rated, and NARRATEE. The distance can be
patcher (analogous to Greimas's SENDER temporal (I narrate events that happened
and Souriau's BALANCE) sends the HERO off two hours or two years ago); it can be in-
on his adventures. HPropp 1968. See also tellectual (the narrator of The Sound and
ACTANT, DRAMATIS PERSONA, SPHERE OF AC- the Fury is far more intelligent than Benjy),
TION. mora! (Sade's Justine is certainly more vir-
displacement set. The set consisting of (1) tuous than the characters in her story),
a given clause c in a sequence and (2) the emotional (the narrator of "A Simple Heart"
clauses before and after which c can be is not as moved by Virginie's death as Fili-
placed in that sequence without altering ate is), and so on. Furthermore, a given
the semantic interpretation. In "John went distance can vary in the course of a narra-
to greet the couple. The man stopped talk- tive: at the end of Tom Jones the narrator
ing and the woman started to srnile. John and the narratee are emotionally closer
decided they were nice," the displacement than at the beginning. HBooth 1961, 1983;
set of "The man stopped talking" is "The Genette 1980,1983; Prince 1980, 1982.
man stopped talking and the woman S e e a l s o COVERT NARRATOR, MODE, MOOD,

started to smile." liLabov and Waletzky TONE.

1 9 6 7 . S e e a l s o COORDINATE CLAUSES, FREE donor. One of the seven fundamental ROLES


CLAUSE, NARRATIVE CLAUSE, RESTRICTED that a character may assume (in a fairy
CLAUSE. tale), according to Propp. The donor (anal-

dissonance. The narrator's distancing of the ogous to Greimas's HELPER and Souriau's

character's consciousness he or she nar- MOON) provides the HERO with some agent

rates ("Death in Venice"). Dissonance is (usually magical) that allows for the even-

characteristic of the relationship between tual liquidation of misfortune. HPropp 1968.

NARRATOR a n d PROTAGONIST in a n AUTHOR- S e e a l s o ACTANT, ANTIDONOR, DRAMATIS

IAL NARRATIVE SITUATION. " i C o h n 1 9 7 8 . S e e PERSONA, SPHERE OF ACTION.

a l s o CONSONANCE, DISTANCE. double focalization. The concurrence of


distance. 1. Along with PERSPECTIVE, one two different FOCALIZATIONS in the render-
of two major factors regulating narrative in- ing of a particular situation or event.
formation (Genette). The more covert the IDouble focalization is not infrequently
narratorial mediation and the more numer- used in film: in Suspicion, for example,
when Una reads the telegram johnnie sent dramatic mode. One of eight possible
to tell her that he is going to attend the POINTS OF VIEW a c c o r d i n g t o F r i e d m a n ' s

Hunt Ball, the end of the reading is shot so classification. When the dramatic mode is
a s t o reflect b o t h h e r o w n POINT OF VIEW adopted—as in the so-called OBJECTIVE or
and the more "objective" point of view of SEHAVIORIST NARRATIVE ("Hills Like White
the camera. IGenette 1980. Elephants," The Awkward Age)—the infor-
double logic of narrative. The two organiz- mation provided is largely limited to what
ing principles in terms of which (many a) the characters do and say, and there is no
NARRATIVE deploys itself, according to direct indication of what they perceive,
some narratologists. One principle empha- think, or feel. I N . Friedman 1955b. See
sizes the primacy of event over meaning a l s o EXTERNAL FOCALIZATION, EXTERNAL

(insists upo n event as the origin of mean- POINT OF VIEW.

ing); the other stresses the primacy of.. d r a m a t i c m o n o l o g u e . INTERIOR MONO-

meaning arad its requirements (insists upon LOGUE. In dramatic monologue, the
event as the effect of a will to meaning). character's inner life is presented directly,
The first principle emphasizes the (logical) without narratorial mediation. UThe dra-
priority of FABULA rather than SJU2ET; the matic or interior monologue in narrative
second stresses the reverse (and makes should be distinguished from the dramatic
fabula the product of sjuzet). Each princi- monologues composed by a Browning or a
ple functions through the exclusion of the Tennyson: the latter are addressed to a
other, but paradoxically, both are neces- possible interlocutor and have speech
sary to the deployment of (many a) rather than thought as their formal base.
narrative, and the contradictory tension be- HBanfield 1982; Scholes and Kellogg 1966.
tween them constitutes an important motor dramatic treatment. In Jamesian terminol-
of narrative force or NARRATIVITY. Wrooks ogy, the scenic rendering of situations and
1984; Culler 1981. events and, more particularly, of the char-
double plot. A PLOT involving two concur- acters' speech and behavior. 1H. James
rent ACTIONS of (more or less) equal 1 9 7 2 . S e e a l s o DRAMA, PANORAMA, PICTO-

importance. UEmpson 1960. See also RIAL TREATMENT.

COUNTERPLOT, SUBPLOT. dramatis persona. In Proppian terminology,


d o u b l e v i s i o n . S e e DOUBLE FOCALIZATION. a fundamental ROLE (in a fairy tale) assum-

liRogers 19S5. able by a character. Propp isolated seven

drama, SCENE; scenic rendering of speech such roles, each corresponding to a partic-
u l a r SPHERE OF ACTION: t h e VILLAIN, t h e
(or thought) and behavior. The distinction
made by James and Lubbock between DONOR (provider), the HELPER, the princess
( a SOUGHT-FOR PERSON) a n d her father, t h e
drama and PANORAMA is analogous to the
DISPATCHER, t h e HERO ( s e e k e r o r victim),
distinction between scene and SUMMARY or
a n d t h e FALSE HERO. U P r o p p 1 9 6 8 . S e e
SHOWING a n d TELLING. HH. J a m e s 1 9 7 2 ;
a l s o ACTANT.
Lubbock 1965. See also PICTURE.
dramatized narrator. A NARRATOR charac- discourse time (the time taken by the
terized in more or less detail as an "I." representation of story time) is difficult, not
Though a dramatized narrator can be rela- to say impossible, to measure: it is not
tively effaced (Madame Bovary), it is more equivalent to the (variable) time it takes to
often provided with numerous physical, read or write a narrative nor is it the same
mental, and.'or moral attributes (Tom as the time a given narration is said to
Jones). In fact, it is frequently represented have taken (imagine a three-page narration
at the level of characters (in FIRST-PERSON ending with "I started my account at nine
or HOMODIEGETIC NARRATIVE) a s a m e r e o'clock and it is now twelve," or a three-
observer or witness ("A Rose for Emily"), a hundred-page narration ending with the
minor participant in the action (A Study in same sentence). This has led many nana-
Scarlet), a relatively important participant tologists to consider the study of SPEED or
(The Great Gatsby), or a protagonist (The TEMPO preferable to (more fruitful than) that
Confessions of Zeno, Great Expectations, of duration. HChatman 1978; Genette
Kiss Me Deadly). HBooth 1983. See also 1980; Metz 1974; Prince 1982. See also
COVERT NARRATOR, OVERT NARRATOR, UN- PACE, RHYTHM.

DRAMATIZED NARRATOR.

dual-voice hypothesis. The hypothesis ac-


c o r d i n g t o w h i c h FREE INDIRECT DISCOURSE

results from the mixing of two voices or


language situations—that of a narrator and
that of a character. UPascal 1977.
Du-Form. SECOND-PERSON NARRATIVE form earth. One of six fundamental ROLES or
(you-form). ^Fuger 1972. See also ICH- FUNCTIONS isolated by Souriau (in his study
FORM, ER-FORM. of the possibilities of drama). The Earth
duplication. The repetition, at the level of (analogous to Greimas's RECEIVER) stands
the NARRATED, of one or more (sequences to benefit from the work of the LION.
of) events: "Joan attempted to climb the liScholes 1974; Souriau 1950. See also
mountain and failed. She tried a second ACTANT.

time and succeeded." ^Suleiman 1980. editorial omniscience. One of eight possi-
See also TRIPLICATION. ble POINTS OF view according to Fried-
duration. The set of phenomena pertaining man's classification: editorial omniscience
to the relation between STORY TIME and characterizes the heterodiegetic, omni-
DISCOURSE TIME. The former can be greater s c i e n t , a n d INTRUSIVE NARRATOR (War and

than the latter, equal to it, or smaller than Peace). UN. Friedman 1955b. See also
it. ^Duration is a problematic notion, partic- HETERODIEGETIC NARRATOR, NEUTRAL OMNI-

ularly in the case of written narrative. SCIENCE, OMNISCIENT POINT OF VIEW.

Even if story time is specified (this event effst de r£el. See REALITY EFFECT. TOarthes
lasted ten minutes and that one twenty), 1982.
ellipsis, A canonical narrative TEMPO; along then Jane met Peter, and she became un-
w i t h PAUSE, SCENE, STRETCH, a n d SUM- happy" can be said to result from the
MARY, one of the fundamental narrative embedding of "Susan was unhappy; then
SPEEDS. When there is no part of the nar- Susan met Flora, and she became happy"
rative (no words or sentences, for into "Jane was happy; then Jane met Pe-
example) corresponding to (representing) ter, and she became unhappy." Similarly,
narratively pertinent situations and events Manon Lescaut can be said to result from
that took time, ellipsis obtains. r.An ellipsis the embedding of Des Grieux's narrative
can be frontal and merely institute a break into the one recounted by M. de Renon-
in the temporal continuity (by skipping court. HAIong with LINKING and ALTER-
over one or several events, one or several NATION, e m b e d d i n g (or NESTING) i s o n e of
moments of time), or it can be lateral (PAR- the basic ways of combining narrative se-
ALIPSIS): in that case, it is not an quences. TOal 1981b; Berendsen 1981;
intervening event that goes unmentioned Bremond 1973; Ducrot and Todorov 1979;
but, rather, one or more components in a Prince 1973, 19B2; Todorov 1966, 1981.
situation that is being recounted. In other S e e a l s o METADIEGETIC NARRATIVE.
words, given a series of events e,, e% e3 emblem. A CHARACTERIZATION device
. . . e„ occurring at times /,, f a f 3 . . . t„, whereby a particular element in the world
respectively or taking place at time t, we represented is evoked with each mention
speak of ellipsis when one of the events is of a given CHARACTER and thus becomes
not mentioned. An ellipsis can also be ex- distinctive of the latter. UDucrot and Todo-
plicit (underlined by the narrator, as in "I rov 1979.
will not say anything about what happened ernic approach. An internal and functional
during that fateful week") or implicit (as opposed to an ETIC or external and tax-
(inferable from a lacuna in the chronology onomic) approach to the study of (human)
or a break in the sequence of events re- situations and productions. The emic ap-
counted). IChatman 1S78; Genette 1980, proach defines and describes the
Prince 1982. constituents or a system in terms of the
embedded narrative. A narrative within a position and function attributed to them in it
n a r r a t i v e ; a METADIEGETIC NARRATIVE. 1Ge- by its users. IKenneth Pike coined the
n e t t e 1 9 8 0 . S e e a l s o EMBEDDING, FRAME term emic by analogy with the t e r n pho-
NARRATIVE. nemic." HDundes 1962,1954; Pike 1967.
embedding. A combination of narrative SE- S e e a l s o ETIC APPROACH.

QUENCES (recounted in the same emotive function. One of the FUNCTIONS OF


NARRATING INSTANCE o r i n d i f f e r e n t o n e s ) COMMUNICATION in terms of which any com-
such that one sequence is embedded (set municative (verbal) act may be structured
within) another one. A narrative like "Jane a n d o r i e n t e d ; t h e EXPRESSIVE FUNCTION.

was happy, and Susan was unhappy; then When the communicative act is centered
Susan met Flora, and she became happy; on the ADDRESSER (rather than on one of
t h e o t h e r CONSTITUTIVE FACTORS OF COMMU- Should he or she tell the same story to
NICATION), it (mainly) has an emotive someone else, the latter is its enunciatee.
function. Wore specifically, those passages ^Greimas and Courtes 1976, 1982.
in narrative focusing on the NARRATOR can enunciation. 1. The traces in a discourse
be said to fulfill an emotive function: "I of the act (and its contextual dimensions)
really hate to evoke the events that hap- generating that discourse. In "I will now re-
pened then." HJakobson 1960; Prince count a beautiful story," the DEICTICS "I"
1982. and "now" are signs of the enuncia-
enchainment. A mode of combining TRIADS tion. 2. The act (and its contextual
such that the outcome of one constitutes dimensions) generating a discourse. HBen-
the opening situation of another one. A veniste 1971,1974; Ducrot and Todorov
narrative like "He was happy; then he met 1979; Greimas and Courtis 1976, 1982;
Peter; then, he became unhappy; then he Hutcheon 1985; Kerbrat-Orecchioni 1980;
met Paul; then he became happy" can be Lejeune 1975, 1982.
said to result from the enchainment of "He enunciator. An ADDRESSER. Should I tell
was happy; then he met Peter; then he be- someone a story, I am its enunciator, and
. came unhappy" and "He became unhappy; should he or she tell me a story in return,
then he met Paul; then he became happy." he or she is its enunciator. HGreimas and
HBremond 1973,1980. See also LINKING. Courtis 1976,1982.
e n d . T h e final i n c i d e n t in a PLOT o r ACTION. epic preterite. The preterite characteristic of
The end follows but is not followed by epic or fictional narrative. According to
other incidents and ushers a state of (rela- Kate Hamburger, this so-called epic preter-
tive) stability. fStudents of NARRATIVE have ite (EPISCHE PRAETERITUM) is a d i s t i n c t i v e

pointed out that the end occupies a deter- feature of fiction as opposed to nonfiction
minative position because of the light it (or of FIKTIONALE ERZAHLEN a s o p p o s e d tO
sheds (or might shed) on the yeaning of AUSSAGE): rather than signifying real time,
the events leading up to it. The end func- rather than labelling the situations and
tions as the (partial) condition, the events reported as past, it designates Shem
magnetizing force, the organizing principle as fictive (the past exists only for a real
of narrative: reading (processing) a narra- person; events in fiction are "time-less"
tive is, among other things, waiting for the and "occur" in the characters' fictive and
end, and the nature of the waiting is re- "time-less" present). For Hamburger, flhis
lated to the nature of the narrative. special status of the epic preterite is evi-
"Aristotle 1968; Benjamin 1969; Brooks denced by combinations between it an-d
1984; Genette 1968; Kermode 1967; Mar- DEICTIC (temporal) adverbials that wouSd be
tin 1986; Prince 1982; Ricoeur 1985. See unacceptable in statements about reality:
also BEGINNING, MIDDLE, NARRATIVITY. consider, for example, "He saw her, and
enunciatee. An ADDRESSEE. Should some- now he felt bad," in which an adverbial
one tell me a story, I am its enunciatee. form designating a present is combined
with a preterite. DSome students of narra- HOMOOIEGET1C NARRATIVE, t h e CHARACTER-I

tive (Bronzwaer, Chatman) have argued ( a s o p p o s e d to ERZAHLENDES ICH, the n a r -

that Hamburger's claim is excessive and, rating "I," the NARRATOR-I). In "I felt bad,"
more specifically, that the co-occurrence of the subject pronoun refers both to an er»e-
preterite tense forms and deictic (temporal) bendes Ich (the one who felt) and to an
adverbials does not define fictionality. Oth- erzahlendes Ich (the one who tells about
ers, however, have argued—not unlike the feeling); and in Nausea, Roquentin is
Hamburger—that, in fiction, the preterite an erlebendes Ich insofar as he longs to
constitutes a present with esthetic distance see Anrty again and an erzahlendes Ich in-
and expresses above all the fictional status sofar as he recounts the longing in his
of the world represented (Ingarden, Sartre, diary. ULammert 1955; Spitzer 1928.
Barthes). "Banfield 1982; Barthes 1968; e r l s b t e r e d e , FREE INDIRECT DISCOURSE.

Bronzwaer 1970; Chatman 1978; Ham- HThe term, first used by Etienne Lorck, lit-
burger 1973; Ingarden 1973; Pascal 1962; erally means "experienced speech." ULorck
Ricoeur 1985; Sartre 1965; Weinrich 1964. 1921; Pascal 1977.
epilogue. A final section in some narratives, e r z a h i e n d e s I c h . In HOMODIEGETIC NARRA-

coming after the DENOUEMENT and not to TIVE t h e " I " w h o n a r r a t e s , t h e NARRATOR-I

be confused with it. The epilogue helps to ( a s o p p o s e d to ERLEBENDES ICH, the " I "

realize fully the design of the work. ^Martin who experiences, the CHARACTER-I). In "I
1 9 8 6 . S e e also PROLOGUE. saw her come out of the room," the sutsject
e p i s c h e P r a e t e r i t u m . S e e EPIC PRETERITE. pronoun refers both to an erzahlendes 'ch
11 Hamburger 1973. (the one who tells about the seeing) and to
episode. A series of related events standing an erlebendes Ich (the one who saw); and
apart from surrounding (series of) events in Great Expectations, Pip is an erzah-
because of one or more distinctive features lendes Ich insofar as he recounts his
and having a unity. HBeaugrande 1980; adventures and an erlebendes Ich insofar
Brooks and Warren 1959. See also GOAL, as he lives them. ULammert 1955; Spitzer
STORY GRAMMAR. 1928.
episodic plot. A loosely woven PLOT; a plot erzahlte W e t t According to Weinrich, ore of
in which no strong causal continuity exists two distinct and complementary categories
b e t w e e n o n e EVENT o r EPISODE a n d the of textual worlds, comprising the various
next; a plot the events or episodes of kinds of verbal narrative and in English sig-
which have no necessary or probable rela- naled by the use of such forms as the
tion to each other, f Aristotle 1968; Brooks preterite, the imperfect, and the pluperfect.
and Warren 1959. With the erzahlte Welt (narrated world) cat-
E r - F o r m . THIRD-PERSON NARRATIVE f o r m (he- egory, as opposed to the SESPROCHENE
form). UDoleZel 1973; Fuger 1972. See WELT (commented world) category, the AO-

a l s o ICH-FORM, DU-FORM. DRESSER a n d ADDRESSEE do not (seem to)

erlebendes Ich. The experiencing "I" in consider themselves directly linked to and
concerned by what is described. HWein- nice," for example, the narrator's reflection
rich's distinction between erzahlte Welt and (emphasizing the unusual quality of the
besprochene Welt is analogous to Benven- happenings), the repetition (suggesting the
iste's distinction between HISTOIRE and importance of the event), the negative (un-
OISCOURS and is related to Hamburger's derlining what did obtain as opposed to
distinction between FIKTIONALE ERZAHLEN what could have), and the explicit state-
28 and AUSSAGE. ffiicoeur 1985; Weinrich ment of the point all function as evaluative
1964. See also TENSE. devices and are part of the evaluation.
Erzahlte Zeit The STORY TIME; the time 'Culler 1981; Labov 1972; Pratt 1977. See
span covered by the situations and events a l s o COMMENTARY, REPORTABILITY.

represented (as opposed to ERZAHLZEIT). event. A change of STATE manifested in DIS-


HMulter 1968. See also DURATION, SPEED. COURSE b y a PROCESS STATEMENT in t h e

Erzahlzeit. The DISCOURSE TIME; the time mode of Do or Happen. An event can be
taken by the representation of situations an ACTION or ACT (when the change is
and events (as opposed to ERZAHLTE ZEIT). brought about by an agent: "Mary opened
HMuller 1968. See also DURATION, SPEED. the window") or a HAPPENING (when the
ethos. See CHARACTER. 'Aristotle 1968. change is not brought about by an agent:
etic approach. An external and taxonomic "the rain started to fall"). lAlong with EXIS-
(as opposed to an emic or internal and TENTS, events are the fundamental
functional) approach to the study of (hu- constituents of the STORY. HChatman 1978;
man) situations and productions. Rather van Dijk 1974-75.
than defining and describing the constitu- existent. An ACTOR or an item of SETTING:
ents of a system from the point of view of the subject and object of "Susan looked at
one familiar with it, the etic approach uses the table" designate existents. lAlong with
criteria not intrinsic to the system to do it. EVENTS, existents are the fundamental con-
HKenneth Pike coined the term etic by stituents of the STORY. ^iChatman 1978.
analogy with the term phonetic. HDundes exposition. The presentation of the circum-
1962,1964; Pike 1967. See aiso EMIC AP- stances obtaining before the BEGINNING of
PROACH. the action. In many narratives, there is a

evaluation. In Labov's terminology, the set delayed exposition: the expository informa-

of features in a NARRATIVE that indicate or tion is provided after the beginning of the

suggest its POINT; the aspects of a narra- action has been set forth. HBrooks and

tive that show why the situations and Warren 1959; Freytag 1894; Sternberg

events narrated are worth narrating. In "I 1974, 1978; Tomashevsky 1965. See also

thought to myself that it was extremely bi- FREYTAG'S PYRAMID.

zarre," "The car stopped. The car stopped expression. Following Hjelmslev, one of the

and a woman got out," "He didn't say any- two planes of any semiotic system: the

thing: he just stood there," and "The point "way" something is signified as opposed to

of my story is that people are basically the "what" that is signified. Like the cow-
TENT plane, the expression plane has a perceiver). In a discussion of this problem,
FORM and a SUBSTANCE. W h e n used in Genette, who coined the term, specifies
connection with narrative, expression can that with external focalization, the FOCAL-
be said to be equivalent to DISCOURSE (as IZER is situated in the DIEGESIS (diegese)
opposed to STORY). ^IChatman 1978; but outside any of the characters, thereby
Hjelmslev 1954, 1961; Prince 1973. excluding the possibility of information on
expressive function. See EMOTIVE FUNC- any thoughts or feelings. 2. NONFOCALIZA-
TION. *]K. Buhler 1934; Jakobson 1960. TION or zero focalization in Rimmon-
extension. The duration of each constituent Kenan's terminology. IjBal 1977, 1983,
unit in an ITERATIVE NARRATIVE; the span of 1985; Genette 1980,1983; Lintvelt 1981;
time covered by an event (or set of events) Rimmon-Kenan 1983. See also DRAMATIC
that is said to recur: "I studied every day MODE, VISION.
from noon to midnight" is an iterative nar- external piot. A PLOT based on outer events
rative with an extension of twelve hours. and experiences, as in adventure stories.
HGenette 1980. 1IH. James 1972. See also INTERNAL PLOT.
extent. The duration or AMPLITUDE of an AN- external point of view. See EXTERNAL FO-
ACHRONY; t h e STORY TIME c o v e r e d by it. CALIZATION. liPrince 1982; Uspenskij 1973.
HGenette 1980. extradiegetic. External to (not part of) any
external action. What characters say and DIEGESIS {didgese). T h e NARRATOR of Eu-
do as opposed to what they think or feel genie Grandet is an extradiegetic narrator.
(INTERNAL ACTION), fBrooks and Warren More generally, the nanator of a PRIMARY
1959. NARRATIVE is always extradiegetic. Wn ex-
external tocalization. 1. A type of FOCALI- tradiegetic narrator is not equivalent to a
ZATION o r POINT OF VIEW w h e r e b y the HETERODIEGETIC one. Thus, in Arabian
information conveyed is mostly limited to Nights, Scheherazade functions as a het-
what the characters do anc." say and there erodiegetic narrator (since she does not teil
is never any direct indication of what they her own story) and as an INTRADIEGETIC
think or feel. External focalization is char- rather than extradiegetic one (since she is
acteristic of the so-called OBJECTIVE or a character in a framing narrative that she
BEHAVIORIST NARRATIVE ("Hills Like White does not tell). Conversely, in Gil Bias, the
Elephants"), and one of its consequences narrator is a HOMOQIEGETIC and extradi-
is that the NARRATOR tells less than one or egetic ona (he tells his own story, but as
several characters know. USeveral narratol- narrator, he is not part of any diegesis).
ogists have argued that external focaliza- HGenette 1S80, 1983; Lanser 1981; Rim-
tion is defined in terms of a criterion differ- m o n 1976. S e e a l s o DIEGETIC LEVEL.

ent from the one characterizing ZERO


FOCALIZATION or INTERNAL FOCALIZATION (na-

ture of what is perceived, of the information


conveyed, as opposed to position of the
i. _ i I

o p p o s e d t o NARRATION or DISCOURSE. HRi-

cardou 1967.
figural narrative situation. One of Stanzel's
three basic types of NARRATIVE SITUATION,
tabula. The set of narrated situations and a l o n g w i t h t h e AUTHORIAL NARRATIVE SITUA-

events in their chronological sequence; the TION (AUKTORIALE ERZAHLSITUATION) a n d

basic STORY materia! (as opposed to PLOT t h e FIRST-PERSON NARRATIVE SITUATION (ICH

or SJU2ET), in Russian Formalist terminol- ERZAHLSITUATION). The figural (or PER-


ogy. IChatman 1978; Ejxenbaum 1971b; SONAL) NARRATIVE SITUATION (PERSONALS

Erlich 1965. ERZAHLSITUATION) is c h a r a c t e r i z e d by IN-

falling action. Along with the RISING ACTION TERNAL FOCALIZATION ( a n d a NARRATOR

and the CLIMAX, one of the basic constitu- who is not a participant in the situations
ents of a (dramatic or closely knit) PLOT and events recounted: The Ambassadors).
structure. The falling action follows the cli- UStanzel 1964, 1971, 1984.
m a x a n d e x t e n d s t o the DENOUEMENT. figure. The entity or collection of entities fo-
HFreytag 1894. See also FREYTAG'S PYR- cused on or foregrounded. A figure comes
AMID. to t h e f o r e a g a i n s t a GROUND o r BACK-

false hero. One of the seven fundamental GROUND. HBeaugrande 1980; Chatman
ROLES that a character may assume (in a 1 9 7 8 . S e e a l s o FOREGROUND.

fairy tale), according to Propp. The false fiktionale Erzahlen. One of two linguistic
hero (analogous to Greimas's OPPONENT subsystems, according to Hamburger, who
and Souriau's MARS) pretends to have ac- opposes it to what she calls AUSSAGE
complished what, in fact, the HERO (statement). Fiktionale Erzahlen (fictional
accomplished. HPropp 1968. See also AC- recounting) consists of THIRD-PERSON NAR-
TANT, DRAMATIS PERSONA, SPHERE OF RATIVE fiction. Whereas the statements
ACTION. constituting Aussage are relatable to a real

ficelle. A term used by Henry James to des- (or feigned) l-Origo, a real (or feigned) ori-

ignate a CHARACTER whose main function ginary "I" and his or her subjectivity, in

is to throw light on the meaning or signifi- fiktionale Erzahlen, fictive characters intro-

cance of the situations and events duced as third persons are the subjects of

narrated. Henrietta Stackpole in The Por- the utterances, thoughts, feelings, and ac-

trait of a Lady and Maria Gostrey in The tions presented. Furlhermore, the basic

Ambassadors are ficelles. HThe term tense used in fiktionale Erzahlen—the pre-

means "string" in French, as well as "trick" terite—designates as fictive the situations

or "ruse" (cf. the strings with which a pup- and events reported rather than labelling

peteer controls his or her puppets). HBooth them as past (the past exists only for an I-

1983; H. James 1972; Souvage 1965. Origo; situations and events in fiction are

fiction, STORY, in Ricardou's terminology, as "time-less"). Finally, fiktionale Erzahlen has


the unique ability to portray the subjectivity one character is the FOCALIZER; a render-
of third persons qua third persons: it is the ing of situations and events in terms of one
only place where such a subjectivity can a n d o n l y o n e POINT OF VIEW ( T h e Ambas-

be presented, the only place where a third sadors). IIGenette 1980. See also
person's mind can be inspected from the FOCALIZATION.

inside. ^Hamburger's distinction between fixed internal p e i n i of view. See FIXED IN-
fiktionale Erzahlen and Aussage is analo- TERNAL FOCALIZATION. ^ P r i n c e 1 9 8 2 .

gous though by no means equivalent to f l a s h b a c k . A n ANALEPSIS; a RETROSPECTION;

Benveniste's distinction between HISTOIRE a CUTBACK; a SWITCHBACK. T h e t e r m is of-

and DISCOURS and Weinrich's distinction ten used in connection with cinematic
b e t w e e n ERZAHLTE WELT a n d BESPROCHENE narrative. (Citizen Kane, The Locket, Wild
WELT. ^Banfield 1982; Hamburger 1973. Strawberries). HChatman 1978; Prince
S e e a l s o EPIC PRETERITE. 1982; Souvage 1965. See also ANACH-
first-person narrative. A narrative the NAR- RONY, ORDER.
RATOR of which is a character in the f i a s h f o r w a r d . A PROLEPSIS: a n ANTICIPA-
situations and events recounted (and, in TION. The term is often used in connection
the latter capacity, is designated by an "I"). with cinematic narrative (The Anderson
Sartre's The Words is a first-person narra- Tapes; Petulia; They Shoot Horses, Don't
tive, and so are Joinville's Memoirs, They?). ^Chatman 1978; Prince 1982. See
Augustine's Confessions, and Robinson a l s o ADVANCE NOTICE, ANACHRONY, ORDER.
Crusoe's account of his adventures. flat character. A CHARACTER endowed with
1iGft>wiriski 1977; Prince 1982; Romberg one or very few TRAITS and highly predict-
1982; Rousset 1973; Tamir 1976. See also able in behavior. Mrs. Micawber in David
HOMODIEGETIC NARRATIVE, PERSON. Copperfieid is a fiat character. HForster
first-person narrative situation. One of 1 9 2 7 . S e e also ROUND CHARACTER.
Stanzel's three basic types of NARRATIVE focal character. The character in terms of
srTUATioN, a l o n g w i t h the AUTHORIAL NARRA- whose POINT OF VIEW the narrated situ-
TIVE SITUATION (AUKTORIALE ERZAHL- ations and events are presented; the
SITUATION) a n d the FIGURAL NARRATIVE SITU- c h a r a c t e r a s FOCALIZER; t h e VIEWPOINT
ATION (PERSONALE ERZAHLSITUATION). T h e CHARACTER. In The Ambassadors, Strether
first-person narrative situation (ICH ERZAHL- is the focal character,
SITUATION) is characterized by a narrator focaiization. The PERSPECTIVE in terms of
who is a participant in the situations and which the narrated situations and events
events recounted (Great Expectations, Moll are presented; the perceptual or concep-
Flanders, Lord Jim). "IStanzel 1964, 1971, tual position in terms of which they are
1 9 8 4 . S e e a l s o HOMODIEGETIC NARRATIVE. rendered (Genette). When such a position
fixed internal focalization. A type of INTER- varies and is sometimes unbeatable (when
NAL FOCALIZATION such that one and only no systematic conceptual or perceptual
constraint governs what may be pre- In a discussion of this problem, Genette
sented), the narrative is said to have ZERO specifies thai in the case of external focali-
FOCALIZATION or to be nontocalized: zero fo- zation, the FOCALIZER is situated in the
calization is characteristic of "traditional" or DIEGESIS (diegese) but outside any of the
"classical" narrative (Vanity Fair, Adam characters. ^Focalization—"who sees" or,
Bede) and associated with so-called OM- more generally, "who perceives (and con-
NISCIENT NARRATORS. When such a position ceives)"—should be distinguished from
is locatable (in one character or another) VOICE ("who speaks," "who tells," "who
and entails conceptual or perceptual re- narrates"). liBai 1977,1981a, 1983, 1985;
strictions (with what is presented being Genette 1930, 1983; Rimmon-Kenan 1983;
governed by one character's or another's Vitoux 1982. See also ASPECT, DOUBLE FO-
perspective), the narrative is said to have CALIZATION, FIXED INTERNAL FOCALIZATION,

INTERNAL FOCALIZATION (The Ambassadors, FOCALIZED, MULTIPLE INTERNAL FOCALIZA-

The Age of Reason, The Ring and the TION, NONFOCALIZATION, POINT OF VIEW,

Book). Internal focalization can be fixed VARIABLE INTERNAL FOCALIZATION, VISION.

(when one and only one perspective is focalized. The object of FOCALIZATION; the
adopted: The Ambassadors, What Maisie existent or event presented in terms of the
Knew), variable (when different perspec- FOCALIZER'S perspective. In "Jane saw Pe-
tives are adopted in turn to present ter leaning against the chair. He looked
different situations and events: The Age of strange to her," Peter is the focalized. IBal
Reason, The Golden Bowl), or multiple 1977, 1983,1985; Martin 1986; Vitoux
(when the same situations and events are 1982.
presented more than once, each time in focalizer. The subject of FOCALIZATION; the
terms of a different perspective: The Ring holder of POINT OF VIEW; the focal point
and the Book, The Moonstone, Rasho- governing the focalization. In "Jane saw
mon). Should what is presented be limited Peter leaning against the chair. He looked
to the characters' external behavior (words strange to her," Jane is the focalizer. HBal
and actions but not thoughts or feelings), 1977, 1983, 1985; Lanser 1981; Martin
their appearance, and the setting against 1986; Vitoux 1982. See also CENTRAL CON-
which they come to the fore, EXTERNAL FO- SCIOUSNESS, FOCAL CHARACTER, FOCALIZED.

CALIZATION is said to obtain ("The Killers"). focus of narration. The VOICE and POINT OF
Several narratologists have argued that ex- VIEW governing the situations and events
ternal focalization is characterized not so presented. Brooks and Warren distinguish
much by the perspective adopted as by the four NARRATIVE SITUATIONS, four narrational
information provided. Indeed, if a given types corresponding to four basic focuses
character's perspective is adopted (internal of narration: (1) first-person (a character
focalization), it may well happen that only tells his or her own story); (2) first-person
words and actions but not thoughts or feel- observer (a character tells a story which
ings are presented (external focalization). s/he has observed); (3)author-observer (a
HETEROOIEGETIC NARRATOR limits w h a t S/he STATEMENTS) that state the story and, more
tells to the characters' words and actions); specifically, determine the ORDER of pre-
(4) omniscient author (a heterodiegetic nar- sentation, the narrative SPEED, the kind of
rator tells what happens, and s/he has the COMMENTARY, and so on. liChatman 1978;
freedom to enter the characters' minds and Ducrot and Todorov 1979; Hjelmslev 1954,
to comment on the action). Types 1 and 2 1961.
c o r r e s p o n d to HOMODIEGETIC NARRATIVES frame. A set of related mental data repre-
w i t h INTERNAL FOCALIZATION, t y p e 3 to HET- senting various aspects of reality and
ERODIEGETIC NARRATIVES w i t h EXTERNAL enabling human perception and compre-
FOCALIZATION (BEHAVIORIST NARRATIVE, DRA- hension of these aspects (Minsky). A
MATIC MODE), a n d type 4 t o HETERO- "restaurant" frame, for example, is a net-
DIEGETIC NARRATIVES with ZERO FOCALIZA- work of data pertaining to the parts,
TION (OMNISCIENT NARRATOR). ^ B r o o k s a n d function, etc., that restaurants typically
Warren 1959. have. More generally, NARRATIVE can be
foreground. That which is focused on, un- considered a frame allowing Sor certain
derlined, emphasized; that which comes to kinds of organization and understandings
the fore against a BACKGROUND. HWeinrich of reality. "[Frames are often taken to be
1964. S e e a l s o FIGURE, GROUND. e q u i v a l e n t to SCHEMATA, PLANS, and

foreshadowing. The technique or device SCRIPTS, but certain suggestive distinctions


whereby some situation or event is hinted have been proposed: a serially ordered,
at in advance. For example, should a char- temporally bound frame is a schema; a
acter manifest extreme sensitivity to color goal-directed schema is a plan; and a ster-
as a child and then become a famous eotypical plan is a script. liBeaugrande
painter, the first event is said to fore- 1980; Goffman 1974; Minsky 1975;
shadow the second. HBrooks and Warren Schank and Abelson 1977; Winograd
1959; Chatman 1978; Souvage 1965. See 1975.
a l s o ADVANCE MENTION, SUSPENSE. frame narrative. A narrative in which an-
foreshortening. See SUMMARY. liBrooks and other narrative is embedded"; a narrative
Warren 1959; H.James 1972. functioning as a frame for another narrative
form. Following Hjelmslev, and as opposed by providing a setting for it. In Manon Les-
to SUBSTANCE, the relational system deter- caut, M . de Renoncourt's narrative is a
mining the units of the two planes of a frame narrative. HSee also EMBEDDED NAR-
semiotic system (the EXPRESSION plane RATIVE, EMBEDDING, METADIEGETIC

and the CONTENT plane), t i n the case of NARRATIVE.

narrative, the form of the content can be frae clause. A clause the DISPLACEMENT SET
said to be equivalent to the STORY compo- of which is equal to the entire narrative; a
nents (existents and events) and their c l a u s e u n a f f e c t e d b y TEMPORAL JUNCTURE

connections; and the form of the expres- and therefore displaceable without any re-
sion to the constr.uents (NARRATIVE sulting change in the semantic interpre-
tation. In "The birds kepts on singing. John free indirect discourse. A TYPEOF DISCOURSE
was happy; then he thought about Mary," representing a character's utterances or
"The birds kept on singing" is a free thoughts. Free indirect discourse (NAR-
clause. I l a b o v 1972; Labov and Waletzky RATED MONOLOGUE, REPRESENTED SPEECH

1967. S e e also COORDINATE CLAUSES, NAR- AND THOUGHT, STYLE INDIRECT LIBRE,

RATIVE CLAUSE, RESTRICTED CLAUSE. ERIEBTE REDE, SUBSTITUTIONARY NARRA-

free direct discourse. A TYPE OF DISCOURSE TION) has the grammatical traits of "normal"
whereby a character's utterances or INDIRECT DISCOURSE, b u t it d o e s n o t in-

thoughts are (presumably) given as the volve a TAG CLAUSE ("he said that," "she
character formulates them, without any thought that") introducing and qualifying
narratorial mediation (TAGS, quotation the represented utterances and thoughts.
marks, dashes, etc.). In "It was unbearably Furthermore, it manifests at least some of
hot, and she just stood there. I cah't stand the features of the character's ENUNCIATION
any of these people! She decided to (some of the features normally associated
leave," "I can't stand any of these people" with the discourse of a character presented
is an instance of free direct discourse. directly, with a first person's as opposed to
^Free direct discourse sometimes is also a third person's discourse: compare "He
made to cover those cases in which a became indignant. A man like him was a
character's perceptions are presented di- suspect now!" or "She smiled. Mary, bless
rectly as they occur in his or her j her soul, would be coming to relieve her
consciousness. HChatman 1978; Genette tomorrow" with "He became indignant: 'A
1980, 1983; Lanser 1981; Todorov 1981. man like me is a suspect now!'" or "She
See also DIRECT DISCOURSE, IMMEDIATE DIS- smiled: 'Mary, bless her soul, will be com-
COURSE, INTERIOR MONOLOGUE, STREAM OF ing to relieve me tomorrow' "). Free
CONSCIOUSNESS. indirect discourse—which is not linguisti-
tree direct speech, FREE DIRECT DISCOURSE, cally derivable from DIRECT DISCOURSE or
especially free direct discourse whereby a ( " n o r m a l " ) TAGGED INDIRECT DISCOURSE—is

character's utterances (as opposed to usually taken to contain mixed within it


thoughts) are presented. Whatman 1978. markers of two discourse events (a narra-
See a l s o FREE DIRECT THOUGHT. tor's and a character's), two styles, two

free direct style. See FREE DIRECT DIS- languages, two voices, two semantic and

COURSE. axiological systems. However, some theo-

free direct thought, FREE DIRECT DISCOURSE rists (e.g., Banfield) have argued—contra

whereby a character's thoughts (as op- this DUAL VOICE HYPOTHESIS—that it should

posed to utterances) are presented. It is be taken to be a speakerless (narratorless)

often called INTERIOR MONOLOGUE when in representation of one subjectivity or self.

extended form. HChatman 1978; Scholes THiere are a number of grammatical fea-

and Kellogg 1966. See also FREE OIRECT tures or indices that might characterize a

SPEECH. particular passage as free indirect dis-


course (back-shift of tenses, conversion of sober reality; Miss Havisham was going to
personal and possessive pronouns, OEIC- make my fortune on a grand scale").
TICS referring to the character's Though it may favor p»ast tenses, it can
spatiotemporal frame, etc.). These gram- and does make room for other tenses
matical features, however, do not appear in ("She shrugs her shoulders and goes out.
every instance of free indirect discourse She's not gonna fall for this two-bit punk").
(and do not by themselves guarantee its Though it often co-occurs with INTERNAL FO
being unequivocally distinguished from CAUZATION, it dearly does not appear in
NARRATIZED DISCOURSE). In o t h e r w o r d s , every passage presen ted in terms cf a
free indirect discourse is not definable in given character's perspective (consider, for
strictly grammatical terms. It is also (and example, "He saw John leaning against
perhaps more frequently) a function of the wall" or "She thought about her past
what might be called contextual features: and felt deeply moved"). Finally, though it
(1)formal features such as general markers occurs perhaps more frequently in written
of colloquialism (ejaculations, lexical fillers, language, it can and does occur in spoken
evaluative expressions, emotive elements, language. HThe category of free indirect
subjective indicators normally absent from discourse is sometimes extended to in-
narratorial discourse); more specific mark- clude discourse representing a character's
ers of a group or class to which a nonverbalized perceptions as they occur in
character belongs; even more specific h i s o r h e r c o n s c i o u s n e - s s (REPRESENTED

markers of a character's personal idiom PERCEPTION). Compare "Mary just stood


(distinctive words, registers, "intonations"); there. The man was crawling toward her"
or markers of social-role relationships (for and "Mary just stood there and saw the
example, appellations which would be used man crawling toward her." HBakhtin 1981;
only by particular characters with respect Bal 1977, 1985; Bally 1912; Banfield 1982;
to other characters); and (2) semantic fea- W. Biihler 1937; Chatrnan 1978; Cohn
tures (assessments, interpretations, 1978; Dillon and Kirchhoff 1976; Genette
judgments, "intended meanings" more 1980, 1983; Jespersen 1924; Lips 1926;
plausibly attributable to a character than to Lorck 1921; McHale 1978,1983; Pascal
the narrator). So dependent is free indirect 1977; Strauch 1974; Todorov 1981; Volo-
discourse on context that it appears more Sinov 1973.
readily in the vicinity of verbs of speech or f r e e i n d i r e c t s p e e c h , FREE INDIRECT DIS-
thought, or next to instances of direct or in- COURSE, especially free indirect discourse
direct discourse, or in the neighborhood of whereby a character's utterances (as op-
a foregrounded character. TThough free in- posed to thoughts) are represented.
direct discourse may have a special affinity H C h a t m a n 1 9 7 8 . S e e a l s o FREE INDIRECT
with the third person, it can and does occur THOUGHT.
in the first and second persons ("My dream
f r e e i n d i r e c t s t y l e . S e e FREE INDIRECT DIS-
was out; my wild fancy was surpassed by
COURSE.
f r e e i n d i r e c t t h o u g h t FREE INDIRECT DIS- Freytag's pyramid has often been used to
COURSE whereby a character's thoughts (as characterize (various aspects of) PLOT in
opposed to utterances) are represented. narrative. IFreytag 1894.
• i C h a t m a n 1 9 7 8 . S e e also FREE INDIRECT function. 1. An ACT defined in terms of its
SPEECH. significance for the course of the ACTION in
f r e e m o t i f . A CATALYSIS; a SATELLITE; a mi- which it appears; an act considered in
nor eyent in a PLOT. For Tomashevsky and terms of the role it plays at the action level;
the Russian Formalists, free motifs (as op- a MOTIFEME. Propp, who developed the no-
p o s e d to BOUNO MOTIFS) are not l o g i c a l l y tion in his study of the (Russian) folktale,
essential to the narrative action, and their showed that the same act can have differ-
elimination does not alter its causal-chron- ent roles (be subsumed by different
ological coherence. HDucrot and Todorov functions) in different tales ("John killed
1979; Tomashevsky 1955. See also MOTIF. Peter," for instance, might constitute a vil-
frequency. The relationship between the lainy in one tale and the hero's victory in
number of times an event happens and the another); conversely, different acts can
number of times it is recounted. For exam- have the same role (be subsumed by the
ple, I can recount once what happened same function) in different tales ("John
once or n times what happened n times killed Peter" and "The dragon kidnapped
(SINGL/LATIVE NARRATIVE); I c a n r e c o u n t n the princess," for example, might both con-
times what happened once (REPEATING stitute a villainy). For Propp, functions
NARRATIVE); and I can recount once what constitute the fundamental components of
h a p p e n e d n times (ITERATIVE NARRATIVE). the underlying structure of any (Russian)
IGenette 1980,1983; Rirnmon-Kenan fairy tale; furthermore, no function excludes
1983. any other and, however many of them ap-
Freytag's pyramid. Gustav Freytag's dia- pear in a single tale, they always appear in
grammatic representation of the structure the same order (given the set a, b, c, d, e,
of a tragedy. . . . . n, should b, c, and e appear in a
particular tale, they will appear in that or-
Climax
der); finally, their number is limited to
thirty-one, which Propp describes as fol-
lows:

I. One of the members of a family


absents himself from home (absen-
tation).
II. An interdiction is addressed to the
hero (interdiction).
III. The interdiction is violated (viola-
Inciting Moment Moment of Las! Suspense tion).
IV. The villain makes an attempt at re- XVI. The hero and the villain join in di-
connaissance (reconnaissance). rect combat (struggle).
V. The villain receives information XVII. The hero is branded (branding,
about his victim (delivery). marking).
VI. The villain attempts to deceive his XVIII. The villain is defeated (victory).
victim in order to take possession XIX. The initial misfortune or lack is liq-
of him or his belongings (trickery). uidated (liquidation of misfortune or
VII. The victim submits to deception lack).
and thereby unwittingiy helps his XX. The hero returns (return).
enemy (complicity). XXI. The hero is pursued (pursuit,
VIII. The villain causes harm or injury to chase).
a member of a famiiy (villainy). XXII. Rescue of the hero from pursuit
Villa. One member of a family either (rescue).
lacks something or desires to have XXIII. The hero, unrecognized, arrives
something (lack). home or in another country (unrec-
IX. Misfortune or lack is made known; ognized arrival).
the hero is approached with a re- XXIV. The hero, unrecognized, arrives
quest or command; he is allowed home or in another country (unrec-
to go or he is dispatched (media- ognized arrival).
tion, the connective incident). XXIV. A false hero presents unfounded
X. The seeker agrees to or decides claims (unfounded claims).
upon counteraction (beginning XXV. A difficult task is proposed to the
counteraction). hero (difficult task).
XI. The hero leaves home (departure), XXVI. The task is resolved (solution).
XII. The hero is tested, interrogated, at- XXVII. The hero is recognized (recogni-
tacked, etc., which prepares the tion).
way for his receiving either a magi- XXVIII. The false hero or villain is exposed
cal agent or helper (the first (exposure).
function of the donor). XXIX. The hero is given a new appear-
XIII. The hero reacts to the actions of ance (transfiguration).
the future donor (the hero's reac- XXX. T h e villain is punished (punish-
tion). ment).
XIV. The hero acquires the use of a XXXI. T h e hero is married and ascends
magical agent (provision or receipt the throne (wedding).
of a magical agent).
XV. The hero is transferred, delivered, Propp's functional account is often consid-
or led to the whereabouts of an ob- ered to mark the birth of modern
ject of search (spatial transference NARRATOLOGY a n d t h e STRUCTURAL ANALY-

between two kingdoms, guidance). sis OF NARRATIVE and it has constituted a


starting point for many influential models of REPRESENTATIVE, APPELLATIVE, AND EX-

narrative structure. Thus, Greimas's NAR- PRESSIVE. jakobson, in what has proven to
RATIVE SCHEMA ultimately r e s u l t s f r o m a be the most influential model of (verbal)
reanalysis of Proppian functions, and Bre- communication in NARRATOLOGY, proposed
mond's characterization of narrative as a schema involving six functions: (1) the
c o m b i n a t i o n s of SEQUENCES o r TRIADS i s EMOTIVE FUNCTION, r e l a t e d to an e m p h a s i s

articulated along functional lines. 2. A o n t h e ADDRESSER; (2) t h e CONATIVE FUNC-

narrative unit or MOTIF metonymically re- TION, related to an emphasis on the


lated to other units in the same sequence ADDRESSEE; (3) t h e REFERENTIAL FUNCTION,

or action (linked to them in terms of con- related to an emphasis on the CONTEXT or


secution or consequence). Barthes REFERENT; ( 4 ) t h e PHATIC FUNCTION, r e l a t e d

contrasted the function and the INDEX, t o a n e m p h a s i s on the CONTACT; (5) t h e


i
POETIC FUNCTION, r e l a t e d to a n e m p h a s i s
which is metaphorically rathpr than meto-
o n t h e MESSAGE for its o w n s a k e ; a n d (6)
nymically related to other units (linked to
t h e METALINGUAL FUNCTION, r e l a t e d t o a n
them in terms other than chronological or
e m p h a s i s o n t h e CODE:
causal: for instance, thematic ones), and
he distinguished two kinds of function:
Referential
CARDINAL FUNCTIONS a n d CATALYSES. H T h e Poetic
same unit cln constitute a function and an Emotive Conative

index. 3. In Greimas's early model of nar- Phatic


Metalingual
rative, a dynamic PREDICATE (as opposed
to the QUALIFICATION or static predi-
UK. Biihler 1934; Jakobson 1960.
cate). 4. A f u n d a m e n t a l ROLE or, ACTANT,

in Souriau's terminology. The six functions


are t h e LION, t h e SUN, t h e EARTH, MARS, t h e

BALANCE, and t h e MOON. TiBarthes 1 9 7 5 ,

Bremond 1973, 1980,1982; Culler 1975;


Greimas 1970,1983a, 1983b: Greimas
and Courtes 1982; Hsnault 1983; Propp
1968; Scholes 1974; Souriau 1950. See generative trajectory. The general econ-

a l s o METONYMY, MOVE. omy of the component parts of a semiotic

functions of communication. The direc- theory, according to Greimassian terminol-

tions orienting any act of (verbal) ogy. In a generative trajectory [parcours

communication and determining the roles it girieratif), the component parts are articu-

fulfills. Each function corresponds to one of lated along a path that goes from the

the CONSTITUTIVE FACTORS OF COMMUNICA- simple to the complex and the abstract to

TION, and any communicative act fulfills the concrete. For instance, Greimas's

one or more functions. HBuhler had iso- model of narrative—whereby two basic

lated three functions of language: components (syntactic and semantic) are


generated (assigned a description) along STORY GRAMMARS t a k e a s t o r y to consist o f

two narrative levels (deep and surface) and a series of EPISODES which bring a charac-
one discursive level—can be represented ter closer t o or farther from the goal
by the diagram below (to be read from top through the reaching or not reaching of a
to bottom). <IGreimas and Courtes 1382; SUBGOAL. ^Beaugrande 1980; Black and
Henault 1983. Bower 1980; Rumelhart 1975; Thorndyke
glorifying test. One of the three TESTS char- 1977.
acterizing the movement of the SUBJECT in g o a l - s t a t e . S e e GOAL.

t h e c a n o n i c a l NARRATIVE SCHEMA. P r e s u p - ground. The entity or collection of entities


p o s i n g t h e DECISIVE TEST w h i c h in turn against which another entity or collection of
p r e s u p p o s e s t h e QUALIFYING TEST, the glo- entities (a FIGURE) emerges and comes to
rifying test results in the recognition of the the fore. HBeaugrande 1980; Chatman
Subject's accomplishment and (usually) 1978. S e e a l s o BACKGROUND, FOREGROUND

takes place when the decisive test has oc-


curred in secret. <lGreimas 1983a, 1983b;
Greimas and Courtes 1982; Henault 1983.
S e e a l s o SANCTION.

gnarus. A Latin word meaning "knowing,"


"expert," "acquainted with," and deriving
from the Indo-European root gna ("to happening. Along with the ACT or ACTION,
know"). Such words as NARRATOR, narrate, one of two possible kinds of narrated
NARRATIVE, etc., are related to it: etymolog- EVENTS; a change of state not brought
ically speaking, the narrator is the one who about by a n AGENT and manifested in dis-
knows. ^Mitchell 1981. c o u r s e b y a PROCESS STATEMENT in the

g n o m i c c o d e . S e e REFERENTIAL CODE. mode of Happen. "It started to rain" and


HBarthes 1974. "Mary was hit by a falling rock" represent
goal. A desired final state for a character. happenings. *Chatman 1978. See also

GENERATIVE TRAJECTORY

Syntactic Component Semantic Component

Semio-NarTative Deep Level Fundamental Syntax Fundamental Semantics


Structures
Surface Level Narrative Surface Syntax Narrative Semantics

Discursive Discursive Syntax Discursive Semantics


Structures Discursivization Thematizaton

I Figurativizaiion

Temporalization

Actorialization Spatialization
NARRATIVE STATEMENT. or solution; or if it constitutes that answer
helper. 1. One of the seven fundamental or solution, represents a contribution to it,
ROLES that a character may assume (in a or acts as an obstacle to it. ^Barthes 1974,
fairy tale), according to Propp; one of six 1981a; Culler 1975; Prince 1982. See also
ACTANTS in Greimas's early versions of the HERMENEUTEME.

ACTANTIAL MOOEL. The Helper (analogous hero. 1. The PROTAGONIST or central char-
to Souriau's MOON) helps the HERO or SUB- acter in a narrative. The hero (or heroine)
JECT. 2. In Greimas's more recent model usually represents positive values. 2. One
of narrative, a positive AUXILIANT that is of the seven fundamental ROLES that a
represented, at the surface structure level, character may assume (in a fairy tale), ac-
by an ACTOR different form the one repre- cording to Propp. The hero (analogous to
senting the Subject. liGreimas 1370, Greimas's SUBJECT and Souriau's LION)
1983b; Greimas and Courtes 1982; Ren- suffers from the action of the VILLAIN or
ault 1983; Propp 1968. See also DRAMATIS from some kind of lack and/or liquidates
PERSONA, SPHERE OF ACTION. his or another character's misfortune or
hermeneuteme. A unit of the HERMENEUTIC lack. IHamon 1972,1983; Propp 1968.
CODE; an element in terms of which the S e e a l s o ACT ANT, ANTIHERO, DRAMATIS PER-

path from an enigma to its solution is artic- SONA, SPHERE OF ACTION.

ulated. Barthes isolated the following heterodiegetic narrative. A narrative the


hermeneutemes: thematization (underlining NARRATOR of which is not a character in
what will be the object of the enigma); pro- the situations and events recounted; a nar-
posal (signaling the existence of an rative with a HETERODIEGETIC NARRATOR:
enigma); formulation (of the enigma); The Iliad, Tom Jones, A Tale of Two Cities,
promise or request for an answer; snare and The Decline and Fall of the Roman
(false lead, deliberate evasion of truth); Empire are heterodiegetic (as opposed to
equivocation (mixture of truth and snare); HOMODIEGETIC) NARRATIVES. l i G e n e t t e
jamming (admitting the insolubility of the 1980,1983. See also DIEGETIC, EXTRADI-
enigma); suspended answer; disclosure or EGET1C, INTRADIEGETIC, PERSON, THIRD-
decipherment. HBarthes 1974. See also PERSON NARRATIVE.
SUSPENSE. heterodiegetic narrator. A NARRATOR who
hermeneutic code. The CODE or "voice" ac- is not part of the DIEGESIS (diegese) s/he
cording to which a narrative or part thereof presents; a narrator who is not a character
can be structured as a path leading from a in the situations and events s/he recounts.
question or enigma to its (possible) answer The narrators in Eugenie Grandet, the Il-
or solution. SA passage can signify in iad, and Carlyle's French Revolution are
terms of the hermeneutic code if if sug- heterodiegetic. ^Genette 1980,1983; Lan-
gests or asserts that there is a question to ser 1981. See also EXTRADIEGETIC,
be asked or an enigma to be solved; if it HETERODIEGETIC NARRATIVE, HOMODIEGETIC
formulates that question or enigma; if it an- NARRATOR.
nounces or alludes to a (possible) answer histoire. See STORY. liBenveniste 1971.
homodiegetic narrative. A narrative the 1955b. See also FIRST-PERSON NARRATIVE,
NARRATOR of which is a character in the " I " AS WITNESS.

situations and events recounted; a narra- " ! " as witness. One of eight possible
tive with a HOMODIEGETIC NARRATOR. Gil POINTS OF VIEW according to Friedman's
Bias, The Great Gatsby, All the King's classification. When it is adopted (Lord
Men, and Kiss Me Deadly are homodi- Jim, The Great Gatsby), the information
egetic (as opposed to HETEROOIEGETIC) provided is limited to the perceptions, feel-
NARRATIVES. HGenette 1980, 1983. See ings and thoughts of a NARRATOR who is a
a l s o DIEGETIC, EXTRADIEGETIC, FIRST-PER- secondary character in the situations and
SON NARRATIVE, INTRADIEGETIC, PERSON. events recounted. Because the narrator as
homodiegetic narrator. A NARRATOR who is witness is not a PROTAGONIST, the action is
part of the DIEGESIS (diegese) s/he pre- viewed from the periphery rather than from
sents; a narrator who is a character in the the center. UN. Friedman 1955b. See also
situations and events s/he recounts. Gil FIRST-PERSON NARRATIVE, " I " AS PROTAGO-

Bias in the novel by the same name, Jack NIST.

Burden in All the King's Men, and Pablo Ich erzahlsituation. See FIRST-PERSON NAR-
Ibbieta in "The Wall" are homodiegetic. RATIVE SITUATION. IStanzel 1964,1971,
IGenette 1980, 1983; Lanser 1981. See 1984.
a l s o AUTOOIEGETIC NARRATIVE, HETEROOI- Ich-Form. FIRST-PERSON NARRATIVE form (I-
EGETIC NARRATOR, HOMODIEGETIC form). fDolezel 1973; Fuger 1972; Leib-
NARRATIVE, INTRADIEGETIC. fried 1972. See also DU-FORM, ER-FORM.
hypodiegetic narrative. See METADIEGETIC illocutionary act. An act performed in say-
NARRATIVE. liBal 1977. Rimmon-Kenan ing something, to accomplish some
1983. purpose: in uttering "I promise to be there
tomorrow," for instance, I perform the illo-
cutionary act of promising. Along with a
LOCUTIONARY ACT a n d ( p o s s i b l y ) a PERLO-

CUTIONARY ACT, an illocutionary act is


involved in the performance of a SPEECH,
ACT. In the case of so-called indirect
" I " as protagonist. One of eight possible speech acts, an illocutionary act is per-
POINTS OF VIEW according to Friedman's formed indirectly by way of the
classification. When it is adopted (Great performance of another illocutionary act:
Expectations, The Catcher in the Rye), the thus, taken literally, "I wish you would open
information provided is limited (o the'per- the window" makes an assertion about the
ceptions, feelings, and thoughts of a addresser's feelings; in particular contexts,
NARRATOR w h o is a PROTAGONIST in t h e sit- however, it can (and does) perform the illo-
uations and events recounted. The latter cutionary act of making a request. HThe
are then viewed from a fixed center rather accomplishment of an illocutionary act de-
than from the periphery. UN. Friedman pends on the fulfillment of so-called felicity
or appropriateness conditions. For exam- i m m e d i a t e d i s c o u r s e , FREE DIRECT DIS-

ple, the conditions for the illocutionary act COURSE. With immediate discourse (as
of asking a question would include the fol- o p p o s e d t o REPORTED DISCOURSE), t h e

lowing: (1) the ADDRESSER does not know character is given the floor without any
the answer; (2) s/he believes that the AD- narratorial introduction, mediation, or pa-
DRESSEE may know the answer; (3) s/he tronage (Les Lauriers sont coupes; the
'2 wants to know the answer; (4) it is not monologues of Benjy, Quentin, and Jason
clear that the addressee will give the an- in the first three sections of The Sound
swer without being asked. Should (one of) and the Fury; Molly Bloom's monologue in
these conditions not be met, the question Ulysses). liGenette 1980. See also AUTON-
would be said to be infelicitous or inappro- OMOUS MONOLOGUE, TYPES OF DISCOURSE.

priate (as a question). HAmong the many immediate speech. See IMMEDIATE DIS-
attempts to classify illocutionary acts, John COURSE. l l G e n e t t e 1 9 8 0 .
Searle's is perhaps the best known: there impersonal narrator. A maximally COVERT
are representatives (undertaken to repre- NARRATOR; a narrator with no individuating
sent a state of affairs, e.g., stating, • property other than the fact that he or she
reporting, telling, suggesting, insisting, or is narrating. HRyan 1981. See also ABSENT
swearing that something is the case); di- NARRATOR, NONNARRATED NARRATIVE.
rectives (undertaken to get an addressee i m p l i e d a u t h o r . T h e AUTHOR'S SECOND SELF,
to do something, e.g., requesting, com- mask, or PERSONA as reconstructed from
manding, or pleading); commissives the text; the implicit image of an author in
(committing the addresser to doing some- the text, taken to be standing behind the
thing, e.g., promising or threatening); scenes and to be responsible for its design
expressives (expressing the addresser's and for the values and cultural norms it ad-
psychological attitudes, e.g., thanking, wel- heres to (Booth). fThe implied author of a
coming, or deploring); and declarations text must be distinguished from its real AU-
(bringing about the state of affairs they re- THOR. In the first place, the same real
fer to, e.g., baptizing, marrying, blessing, or author (Fielding, Sartre) can write two or
arresting). Wiewed as a speech act, one more texts, each conveying a different pic-
NARRATIVE could therefore be said to in- ture of an implied author (Amelia and
volve the illocutionary act of threatening, Joseph Andrews, Nausea and "Erostra-
another one that of deploring, and still an- tus"). In the second place, one text
other one that of suggesting. More (having, like all texts, one implied author)
generally, any tellable or reportable narra- can have two or more real authors (Naked
tive could be said to have the illocutionary Came the Stranger, the novels of Ellery
status of an exclamatory assertion. UAustin Queen, Delly, or Erckmann-Chatrian). fThe
1962; Chatman 1978; van Dijk 1977; implied author of a narrative text must also
Lyons 1977; Pratt 1977; Searte 1969, be distinguished from the NARRATOR: the
1975,1976. See also PERFORMATIVE, RE- former does not recount situations and
PORTABILITY. events (but is taken to be accountable for
their selection, distribution, and combina- index. A narrative unit linked to other units
tion); furthermore, he or she is inferred i n t h e s a m e SEQUENCE o r ACTION i n t e r m s

from the entire text rather than inscribed in other than chronological or causal (say,
it as a teller. Though the distinction can be thematic). Barthes contrasted the index
problematic (e.g., in the case of an ABSENT (which implies metaphoric relata) and the
o r m a x i m a l l y COVERT NARRATOR: " T h e Kill- FUNCTION (which is metonymically rather
ers," "Hills Like White Elephants"), it is than metaphorically related to other units:
sometimes very clear (e.g., in the case of linked to them in terms of consecution or
m a n y HOMODIEGETIC NARRATIVES: Great consequence), and he distinguished two
Expectations, "Haircut"). HBal 1981a; kinds of indices: the index proper (which
Booth 1983; Bronzwaer 1978; Chatman refers to an atmosphere, a philosophy, a
1978; Genette 1983. feeling, a personality trait, and signifies im-
i m p l i e d reader. The audience presupposed plicitly) and the INFORMANT (which provides
by a text; a real READER'S second self explicit bits of information about the time
(shaped in accordance with the IMPLIED AU- and space represented). fiThe same unit
THOR'S values and cultural norms). The can constitute an index and a function.
implied reader of a text must be distin- HBarthes 1975.
guished from its real reader. In the first i n d i r e c t d i s c o u r s e . A. TYPE OF DISCOURSE

place, the same real reader can read texts whereby a character's utterances or
presupposing different audiences (and let thoughts are integrated into another utter-
himself or herself be shaped in accordance ance or thought (usually but not always)
with different implied authors' values and through a back-shift of tenses and a shift
norms). In the second place, one text (hav- from first-person to third-person pronouns.
ing, like all texts, one implied reader) can These thoughts or utterances are reported
have two or more real readers. TThe im- with more o r less literal fidelity (as opposed
plied reader of a narrative text must also t o DIRECT DISCOURSE, w h e r e a c h a r a c t e r ' s

be distinguished from the NARRATEE: the utterances o r thoughts are given or quoted
former is the audience of the implied au- in the way t h e character presumably for-
thor and is inferrable from the entire text, mulated them): "iviary said: 'I have to go' "
whereas the latter is the audience of the becomes "Mary said that she had to go";
NARRATOR and is inscribed as such in the " 'I want to take a look at it,' I said" be-
text. Though the distinction can be prob- comes "I said that I wanted to see i t " ; " 'I
lematic (for example, in the case of a have killed m y father,' cried out Oedipus"
maximally covert narratee: "Hills Like becomes "Oedipus cried out that he had
White Elephants"), it is sometimes very murdered his father." HA distinction can be
clear (for example, in the case of a narra- m a d e b e t w e e n " n o r m a l , " o r TAGGED, INDI-

tive where the narratee is also a character: RECT DISCOURSE ( w h i c h i n v o l v e s a TAG

Isa, in Vipers' Tangle}. UBooth 1933; Ge- CLAUSE—"he said that," "she thought
nette 1983; Gibson 1950; Iser 1974, 1978; that"—introducing and qualifying the repre-
Rabinowitz 1977. sented utterances and thoughts) and FREE
INDIRECT DISCOURSE (which does not and or thoughts, delineating some of their qual-
which manifests at least some of the fea- ities, and identifying the speaker or thinker.
tures of the character's ENUNCIATION). In "Mary said she was tired" and "How are
!IBanfield 1982; Chatman 1978; Genette you? replied Nancy," the italicized words
1980, 1983; Mendilow 1952; Todorov constitute inquit ("one says") formulas.
1 9 8 1 . S e e also TRANSPOSED DISCOURSE. •iBonheim 1982; Chatman 1978; Prince
indirect speech, INDIRECT DISCOURSE, 1 9 7 8 . S e e a l s o ATTRIBUTIVE DISCOURSE,

especially indirect discourse whereby a VERBUM DICENDI.

character's utterances (as opposed to inside view. The representation of a charac-


thoughts) are represented. liChatman ter's mind. liBooth 1983. See also
1
1978. AUTHORITY.

indirect style. See INDIRECT DISCOURSE. intercalated narration. A type of NARRATION


informant. A type of INDEX. AS opposed to whereby a NARRATING INSTANCE is tempo-
indices proper (which signify implicitly and rally situated between two moments of the
refer to an atmosphere, a philosophy, a ACTION; a n INTERPOLATED NARRATING. Inter-

feeling, a personality trait), informants pro- calated narration is characteristic of epis-


vide explicit bits of information about the tolary narratives (Pamela) and diary narra-
time and space represented. liBarthes tives (Doctor Glas, Diary of a Country
1975. Priest). HGenette 1980; Prince 1982.
in medias res. The method of starting a intercalation. See EMBEDDING. HGreimas
narrative (and, more specifically, an epic) and Courtis 1982.
with an important situation or event (rather interest point of view. The consideration of
than with the first situation or event in narrated situations and events in terms of
time). Homer opens the Iliad in medias res the character's interests they (most) evoke
(in the midst of things) rather than ab ovo and concern. In "Though he didn't realize
(with an account ot Helen's birth! for exam- it, these developments were disastrous for
ple). liThe in medias res method, is now John," the interest point of view is John's.
usually taken to constitute a principle of or- •iChatman 1978.
dering situations and events (a beginning interior monologue. The nonmediated pre-
in the middle of things is followed by a re- sentation of a character's thoughts and
turn to an earlier period of time). Originally, impressions or perceptions; an extended
however, it referred to a principle of selec- s t r e t c h of FREE DIRECT THOUGHT (Les Lau-

tion (Horace): the narrator starts with the riers sont coupes, Molly Bloom's mono-
situation pertinent to his or her account logue in Ulysses). ^Interior monologue
(and takes its constituents to be already (monologue int4rieur, stiller Monolog)
well-known). "IHorace 1974; Sternberg is now frequently taken to subsume
1978. | STREAM OF CONSCIOUSNESS a s a p a r t i c u l a r

inquit formula. A TAG CLAUSE; a clause ap- variant. However, it has sometimes been
pended to the representation of utterances opposed to stream of consciousness: inte-
rior monologue would present a character's ^internal focalization can be fixed (when
thoughts rather than impressions or per- one and only one perspective is adopted:
ceptions, and stream of consciousness The Ambassadors, What Maisie Knew,
would present both impressions and Robert Montgomery's The Lady in the
thoughts; or else, the former would respect Lake), variable (when different perspec-
morphology and syntax, whereas the latter tives are adopted in turn to present
would not and would thus capture thought different situations and events: The Age of
in its nascent stage, prior to any logical or- Reason, The Golden Bowl), or multiple
ganization. On the other hand, the two (when the same situations and events are
terms have often been used interchangea- presented more than once, each time in
bly; in fact, Dujardin—whose Les Lauriers terms of a different perspective: The Ring
sont coupes probably constitutes the most and the Book, The Moonstone, Rasho-
famous example of a text written entirely in mon). HBal 1985; Genette 1980. See also
FREE DIRECT DISCOURSE—stressed stylistic FIXED INTERNAL FOCALIZATION, INTERNAL

criteria and effects associated with stream POINT OF VIEW, MULTIPLE INTERNAL FOCALIZA-

of consciousness in his definition of interior TION, PERSONAL NARRATIVE SITUATION,

monologue. UBickerton 1967; Bowling VARIABLE INTERNAL FOCALIZATION, VISION.

1950; Chatman 1978; Cohn 1978, 1981; internal plot. A PLOT focusing on internal
Dujardin 1931; Francoeur 1976; M. Fried- feelings and movements, as in psychologi-
man 1955; Genette 1980; Humphrey 1954; cal novels. HH. James 1972. See also
Scholes and Kellogg 1966. See also AU- EXTERNAL. PLOT.
TONOMOUS MONOLOGUE, DRAMATIC internal p o i n t of view. See INTERNAL FOCALI-
MONOLOGUE, TYPES OF DISCOURSE. ZATION. UPrince 1982; Uspenskij 1973.
internal action. What characters think and interpolated narrating. See INTERCALATED
feel as opposed to what they say and do NARRATION. HGenette 1980.
(EXTERNAL ACTION). I B r o o k s a n d W a r r e n interspersed narration. See INTERCALATED
1959. NARRATION. HLanser 1981.
interna! analysis. A narrator's account, in intertext. 1. A text (or set of texts) that is
his or her own words, of a character's cited, rewritten, prolonged, or generally
thoughts and impressions; a NARRATIVE RE- transformed by another text and that
PORT of thoughts and impressions in words makes the latter meaningful. Homer's Od-
that are recognizably the narrator's (as op- yssey is one of the intertexts of Joyce's
p o s e d t o NARRATED MONOLOGUE); a Ulysses, and INTERTEXTUALITY obtains be-
PSYCHONARRATION. ^Bowling 1950; Chat- tween the two. Hln Riffaterre's influential
man 1978; Cohn 1978. See also ANALYSIS. view, a text and its intertext are homolo-

internal focalization. A type of FOCALIZA- gous, and the latter leaves in the former

TION whereby information is conveyed in traces controlling its decipherment. 2. A

terms of a character's (conceptual or per- text insofar as it absorbs and binds to-

c e p t u a l ) POINT OF VIEW or PERSPECTIVE. gether a multiplicity of other texts (Jenny).


In this acceptation, Joyce's Ulysses would Lescaut, M. de Renoncourt, the extradi-
be the intertext absorbing such texts as egetic narrator of the primary narrative, is
Homer's Odyssey. More generally, any text intradiegetic insofar as he functions as a
could be taken to constitute an inter- character in the diegesis he presents; and
text. 3. A set of texts that are inter- Des Grieux is intradiegetic as a character
textually linked (Arrive): given that intertex- in M. de Renoncourt's narrative (and META-
tuality obtains between Homer's Odyssey DIEGETIC as a character in his own narra-
and Joyce's Ulysses, the two texts would tive). HAn intradiegetic narrator is not
be said to constitute an intertext. HArrive equivalent to a HOMODIEGETIC one. Thus,
1973; Barthes 1981b; Jenny 1982; Morgan in Arabian Nights, Scheherazade functions
1985; Riffaterre 1978,1980, 1983. a s a HETERODIEGETIC NARRATOR ( s i n c e she

intertextuality. The relation(s) obtaining be- does not tell her own story) and as an in-
tween a given text and other texts which it tradiegetic rather than extradiegetic one
cites, rewrites, absorbs, prolongs, or gen- (since she is a character in a framing nar-
erally transforms and in terms of which it is rative that she does not tell). Conversely,
intelligible. *iThe notion of intertextuality in Gii Bias, the narrator is a homodiegetic
was formulated and developed by Kristeva and extradiegetic one (he tells his own
(inspired by Bakhtin). In its most restricted story, but as a narrator, he is not part of
acceptation (Genette), the term designates any diegesis). HGenette 1980, 1983; Lan-
the relation(s) between one text and other ser 1981; Rimmon 1976. See also
ones which are demonstrably present in it. DIEGETIC LEVEL.

In its most general and radical acceptation intrigue. The PLOT; the aggregate of motifs
(Barthes, Kristeva), the term designates characterizing the characters' machina-
the relations between any text (in the tions, conflicts, and struggles.
broad sense of signifying matter) and the ITomashevsky 1965.
sum of knowledge, the potentially infinite intrusive narrator. A NARRATOR comment-
network of codes and signifying practices ing in his or her own voice on the situa-
that allows it to have meaning. *iBarthes tions and events presented, their presenta-
1981b; Culler 1981; Genette 1982; Jenny tion, or its context; a narrator relying on
1982; Kristeva 1969,1984; Morgan 1985; and characterized by commentarial excur-
Ricardou 1971; Riffaterre 1978, 1980, suses or intrusions (Euginie Grandet,
1 9 8 3 . S e e also INTERTEXT. I Barchester Towers, Tom Jones). IBIin
interweaving. See ALTERNATION. <IDucrot 1954; Genette 1980; Prince 1982. See

and Todorov 1979. a l s o AUTHOR'S INTRUSION, COMMENTARY,

intradiegetic. DIEGETIC; pertaining to or part OVERT NARRATOR,

of the DIEGESIS (diegese) presented (in a inverted content. The thematic situation

PRIMARY NARRATIVE) by a n EXTRADIEGETIC whose TRANSFORMATION into a contrary (or

narrator. In Pere Gcriol, for example, Ras- contradictory) situation marks the comple-

tignac is intradiegetic. Similarly, in Marion tion of a narrative SEQUENCE. ^Narrative


can be viewed as correlating a temporal iterative n a r r a t i v e . A narrative or part
opposition (before/after, initial situation/final thereof with a FREQUENCY whereby what
situation) and a thematic one (inverted happens n times is recounted once: "Every
c o n t e n t / R E S O L v E D CONTENT). l i C h a b r o l Sunday, w e went to the beach." An itera-
1973; Greimas 1970; Rastier 1973. tive narrative (or iterative series) can have
isochrony. 1. The steadiness of narrative a DETERMINATION (the span of time in
SPEED. An isochronous narrative is one the which an event or set of events is said to
speed of which is constant, as in "Susan recur), a SPECIFICATION (the rhythm of re-
wrote for an hour, then she drank for an currence o f the event or set of events), and
hour, then she slept for an hour." 2. The an EXTENSION (the duration of the recurring
equality between the duration of a situation event or set of events). In "During a period
or event and the duration of its representa- of eight weeks, I ran once a week for an
tion. HBal 1985; Genette 1S80. See also hour," the series has a determination of
ANISOCHRONY, DURATION. eight weeks, a specification of one day out

isodiegetic. Part of the same DIEGESIS of seven, a n d an extension of one hour.

(diggese). !n Nausea, the Self-Taught Mar HGenette 1980.

and Anny are isodiegetic; in Heart of Dark-


ness, Marlow's mates on the Nellie and
Kurz are not. HGenette 1980. See also DIE-
GETIC.

Isotopy. The repetition of semiotic features


that institutes the coherence of a text. In
"Everybody was beautifully dressed. John joining. T h e connecting of two TRIADS each
and Mary were led to a magnificent table of which presents the same process but
in the middle of a splendidly decorated considers it from a different point of view
room and were offered champagne," the (say, that of the protagonist in the first a n d
clustering of terms capable of evoking lux- that of the antagonist in the second) and
ury—"beautifully dressed," "magnificent," thus constitutes a different set of FUNC-
"splendidly decorated," "champagne"—can TIONS. liBremond 1973,1980.
be said to constitute a "luxury" isotopy. Uln junction. A relation linking the SUBJECT and
its more restricted acceptation, the term is t h e OBJECT a n d y i e l d i n g STASIS STATE-

taken to designate the repetition of seman- MENTS. There are two basic types of
tic units in a text (or part thereof). In its junction: CONJUNCTION ("X is with Y," "X
broadest sense, its designates the repeti- h a s Y " ) a n d DISJUNCTION ( " X is not w i t h

tion of units at any and all textual levels Y," "X does not have Y"). ^Greimas and
(phonetic, stylistic, rhetorical, syntactic, Courtes 1982; Henault 1983.
prosodic, etc). liAdam 1985; Eco 1979,
1984; Greimas 1983b; Greimas and
Court6s 1982; Rastier 1973.
toSMf' ' "BSSH^ TSSHS^"

Shklovsky 1965b; Tomashevsky 1965.


leitmotif. A frequently recurring MOTIF, re-
lated to and expressive of a character,
situation, or event. Vinteuil's little phrase in
k e r n e l . A BOUND MOTIF; a CARDINAL FUNC- Remembrance of Things Past functions as
TION; a NUCLEUS (noyau). As opposed to a leitmotif. TThe term was originally used in
SATELLITES, kernels are logically essential connection with Wagnerian music. HDucrot
to the narrative action and cannot be elimi- and Todorov 1979; Tomashevsky 1965.
nated without destroying its causal- lexia. A textual unit or unit of reading of vari-
chronological coherence. liBarthes 1975; able dimension constituting the best space
Chatman 1978. in which meaning can be observed.
liBarthes 1974.
limited point of view. A FOCALIZATION or
POINT OF VIEW that is subject to conceptual
or perceptual constraints (as opposed to
OMNISCIENT POINT OV VIEW). The Ambassa-

dors is told in terms of a limited point of


langue. The language system or CODE gov- view, and so is "Bliss." IN. Friedman
erning the production (and reception) of 1955b; Stanzel 1984.
individual utterances (PAROLE) in a given linking. A combination of narrative SE-
language. According to Saussure, who ar- QUENCES (recounted in the same
ticulated the distinction, langue rather than NARRATING INSTANCE o r in d i f f e r e n t o n e s )

parole constitutes the main object of lin- such that one sequence is conjoined with
guistic study. By analogy with (Saussurean) (placed after) another or such that the end
linguistics, NARRATOLOGY attempts to char- of one sequence constitutes the beginning
acterize "narrative langue" (the code or set of another. A narrative like "John was
of principles governing the production of all happy, then he got divorced, then he be-
and only narratives) rather than to study in- came unhappy and Mary was unhappy,
dividual narratives (equivalent to parole). then she got married, then she became
^ISaussure 1966. See also NARRATIVE COM- happy" can be said to result from the link-
PETENCE. ing of "John was happy, then he got

laying bare. Calling attention to a device, divorced, then he became unhappy" and

technique, or convention. According to To- "Mary was unhappy, then she got married,

mashevsky and the Russian Formalists, then she became happy." Similarly, a nar-

laying bare a device, or BARING THE DEVICE rative like "She was in good health, then

(as opposed to MOTIVATION), underlines the she ate a rotten apple, then she became

convention-governed character of a text, its ill, then she took some medicine, then she

fictionality, its literary nature. HDucrot and felt very well" can be said to result from

Todorov 1979; Lemon and Reis 1965; the linking of "She was in good health,
then she ate a rotten apple, then she be-
came ill" and "She became ill, then she
took some medicine, then she felt very
well." HAIong with EMBEDDING and ALTER-
NATION, linking, or CONJOINING, is o n e of macrostructure. The abstract underlying
the basic ways of combining narrative se- s t r u c t u r e o f a text; t h e DEEP STRUCTURE of

quences. ffiremond 1973; Ducrot and a text defining its global meaning. The
Todorov 1979; Prince 1973, 1S82; Todorov macrostructure is converted into the
1 9 6 6 , 1 9 8 1 . S e e a l s o COMPLEX STORY, EN- MICROSTRUCTURE (or SURFACE STRUCTURE)

CHAINMENT, THIAD. b y a set o f o p e r a t i o n s o r TRANSFORMA-

lion. One of six FUNCTIONS or fundamental TIONS. W a n Dijk 1972, 1974-75, 1976a.
ROLES isolated by Souriau (in his study of S e e a l s o NARRATIVE GRAMMAR.

the possibilities of drama). The Lion (anal- main narrator. The NARRATOR introducing
ogous to Greimas's SUBJECT and Propp's the entire narrative (including all the mini-
HERO) is the force oriented in terms of the narratives composing it or parts of it); the
SUN (or OBJECT) and works for the benefit narrator ultimately responsible for the
o f t h e EARTH (or RECEIVER). HScholes whole narrative (including title, epigraphs,
1974; Souriau 1950. See also ACTANT. etc.) HPrince 1982.
locutionary act. An act of saying, of produc- m a i n t e s t S e e DECISIVE TEST.

ing a grammatical utterance. When ! say m a n i f e s t a t i o n . T h e SUBSTANCE of the DIS-

"The earth is round," for instance, I per- COURSE o r of t h e EXPRESSION plane of

form the locutionary act of making a narrative (as opposed to its FORM); the me-
sentence in accordance with the rules of dium (verbal, cinematic, balletic, etc.) of
English. Along with an ILLOCUTIONARY ACT narrative representation. A cinematic repre-
a n d (possibly) a PERLOCUTIONARYACT, a sentation of a man eating, then sleeping
locutionary act is involved in the perfor- and a verbal representation of it can con-
mance of a SPEECH ACT. HAustin 1962; stitute two different manifestations of the
Lyons 1977; Searle 1969. same form of the discourse (or set of NAR-
logos. Subject matter; topic; thought; argu- RATIVE STATEMENTS). H C h a t m a n 1978. S e e

ment. For Aristotle, the imitation of a real a l s o NARRATIVE MEDIUM.

action, or PRAXIS, constitutes an argument, manipulation. In the Greimassian account


or logos, providing the basis for the MY- of canonical narrative structure, the action
THOS, or PLOT. fThe distinction between o f t h e SENDER o n t h e SUBJECT t o m a k e t h e

logos and mythos is suggestive of that be- latter execute a given program. HAdam
t w e e n STORY a n d OISCOURSE o r FASULA 1984, 1985; Greimas 1983a; Greimas and
and SJU2ET. UAristotle 1968; Chatman C o u r t e s 1 9 8 2 . S e e a l s o CONTRACT, NARRA-

1978. TIVE SCHEMA, SANCTION.

Mars. One of six FUNCTIONS or fundamental


ROLES isolated by Souriau (in his study of
prologue. An initial section in some narra- story of his love for Manon, proceeded to
tives, preceding and not including the narrate that story as if another narrator had
EXPOSITION or (part of) t h e COMPLICATION. not recounted it to him, a pseudodiegetic
TTomashevsky 1965. See also EPILOGUE. o r REDUCED METADIEGETIC NARRATIVE w o u l d

prop. An EXISTENT that is not active in the obtain. liGenette 1980. See also DIEGETIC
situations and events recounted. As op- LEVEL.

posed to a PARTICIPANT, a prop constitutes pseudo-iterative frequency. A type of FRE-


part of the SETTING. ^Grimes 1975. QUENCY whereby events that (presumably)
proposition. An elementary story unit con- could have happened only once are re-
stituted by a subject and a PREDICATE; a counted iteratively, as if they had hap-
MOTIF. Propositions describe states ("X is pened many times. ^Genette 1980. See
Y") or events ("X does Y"). Some are logi- a l s o ITERATIVE NARRATIVE.

cally essential to the narrative action and p s y c h o n a r r a t i o n . A NARRATIZED DISCOURSE

its causai-chronological coherence, representing a character's thoughts (as op-


whereas others are not. They combine into posed to utterances), in the context of
SEQUENCES and can be related temporally, THIRD-PERSON NARRATIVE; INTERNAL ANALY-

spatially, causally, transformationally, and SIS. ^ICohn distinguishes three basic


so on. fTodorov 1981. See also TRANS- techniques for rendering consciousness:
FORMATION. psychonarration (the most indirect one),
p r o s p e c t i o n . A n ANTICIPATION, a FLASHFOR- NARRATED MONOLOGUE, a n d QUOTED MONO-

WARD, a PROLEPSIS. f T o d o r o v 1 9 8 1 . S e e LOGUE (the most direct one). tCohn 1966,


a l s o ANACHRONY, ORDER. 1 9 7 8 . S e e also SELF-NARRATION.

protagonist. The main CHARACTER; the


character constituting the chief focus of in-
terest. A narrative articulated in terms of an
interpersonal CONFLICT involves two major
characters with opposite goals: the protag-
o n i s t ( o r t h e HERO) and t h e ANTAGONIST.

IN. Friedman 1975; Fiye 1957; Toma- qualification. 1. In Greimas's early model
shevsky 1965. See also ANTIHERO,, of narrative, a static PREDICATE (as op-
SUBJECT. posed to the FUNCTION, or dynamic
I
predicate). 2. The consequence of the
pseudodiegetic narrative. A SECOND-
QUALIFYING TEST in G r e i m a s ' s a c c o u n t of
DEGREE NARRATIVE b r o u g h t u p t o t h e l e v e l
the canonical NARRATIVE SCHEMA. Qualifica-
of the PRIMARY NARRATIVE a n d t a k e n i n
tion corresponds to the SUBJECT'S
charge by its narrator; a METADIEGETIC NAR-
acquisition of COMPETENCE along the axes
RATIVE f u n c t i o n i n g a s if it w e r e a DIEGETIC
of ability (being able to do or be) and/or
one: if, in Marion Lescaut, M. de Renon-
knowledge (knowing how to do or be).
court, after des Grieux had told him the
HGreimas 1970, 1983a, 1983b; Greimas
and Courtis 1982; Henauit 1983.
qualifying test. One of the three fundamen-
tal TESTS characterizing the movement of
t h e SUBJECT in t h e c a n o n i c a l NARRATIVE r a v e l l i n g , COMPLICATION; COMPLICATING AC-

SCHEMA. P r e s u p p o s e d by t h e DECISIVE TION; MIDDLE ( a s o p p o s e d to BEGINNING or

TEST, which is in turn presupposed by the END). HBrooks and Warren 1959. See also
GLORIFYING TEST, the qualifying test results UNRAVELLING.

in the Subject's QUALIFICATION. "iGreimas reach. The temporal distance between the
1970,1983b; Greimas and Courtes 1982; STORY TIME c o v e r e d b y a n ANACHRONY a n d

Henauit 1983. the "present" moment (or moment when


q u a s i - d i r e c t d i s c o u r s e . S e e FREE INDIRECT the chronological recounting of a sequence
DISCOURSE. HVolo§inov 1973. of events is interrupted to make room for
quasi-direct speech, QUASI-DIRECT DIS- the anachrony). HGenette 1980.
COURSE, especially quasi-direct discourse reader. The decoder or interpreter (of a writ-
whereby a character's utterances (as op- ten narrative). This real or concrete reader
posed to thoughts) are represented; FREE is not to be confused with the IMPLIED
INDIRECT SPEECH. <|Bakhtin 1 9 8 1 . READER of a n a r r a t i v e or with its NARRATEE

quest. The figuration, at the discursive level, and, unlike them, is not immanent to or de-
of the movement of the desiring SUBJECT ducible from the narrative. Heart of Dark-
toward the OBJECT desired. The goal of a ness and Vipers' Tangle, for example,
quest is the CONJUNCTION of Subject and have different implied readers as well as
Object. HGreimas 1970, 1983a, 1983b; different narratees, but they can have the
Greimas and Courtds 19e2; Henauit 1983. same real reader. Furthermore, a narrative
quoted monologue. A verbatim quotation of with only one implied reader and oniy one
a character's mental language, in the con- narratee ("The Wall") can have two or
t e x t o f THIRD-PERSON NARRATIVE; a n more real readers. HBooth 1983; Chatmar,
INTERIOR MONOLOGUE; a REPORTED DIS- 1978; Eco 1979; Prince 1982; Rabinowitz
COURSE representing a character's thoughts 1977.
(as opposed to utterances). HCohn distin- readerlv t e x t A text that can be read (or
guishes three basic techniques for decoded) in terms of well-defined con-
rendering consciousness: quoted mono- straints, conventions, and codes; a text
logue (the most direct one), NARRATED adapted to (more or less established) read-
MONOLOGUE, a n d PSYCHONARRATION (the ing strategies. The readedy text ftexte
most indirect one). HCohn 1966, 1978. See lisible) is a moderately polysemous text, a
a l s o AUTONOMOUS MONOLOGUE, DIRECT DIS- parsimoniously plural text, a partially
COURSE, SELF-OUOTED MONOLOGUE. closed text, as opposed to the WRITERLY
TEXT (texte scriptible), which is infinitely po-
the possibilities of drama). Mars (ana- metadiegetic. Pertaining to or part of a DIE-
logous to Greimas's OPPONENT and GESIS (cVegese) that is embedded in
P r o p p ' s VILLAIN a n d FALSE HERO) is t h e AN- another one and, more particularly, in that
TAGONIST, or enemy, of the LION. HScholes of t h e PRIMARY NARRATIVE. ^ G e n e t t e 1 9 8 0 .

1974; Souriau 1950. See also ACTANT, 1 9 8 3 . S e e a l s o DIEGETIC LEVEL, METADI-

ANTISUBJECT. EGETIC NARRATIVE.

mask. A CHARACTERIZATION device whereby metadiegetic narrative. A narrative embed-


the physical features (and'or clothes, fur- ded within another narrative and, more
nishings, names, etc.) of a character are in particularly, within the PRIMARY NARRATIVE;
harmony with his or her personality. UTo- a HYPODIEGETIC NARRATIVE. T h e s i t u a t i o n s

mashevsky 1965. and events recounted by Des Grieux in


mediated narration. A narration in which Manor) Lescaut are METADIEGETIC in rela-
the narrator's presence makes itself felt; a tion to those recounted by M. de
narration featuring an OVERT rather than Renoncourt (which are DIEGETIC or INTRA-
COVERT NARRATOR; a narration in which DIEGETIC). W h e n a metadiegetic narrative
OIEGESIS, TELLING, or recounting, rather functions as a diegetic one (when its meta-
than MIMESIS, SHOWING, or enacting, are diegetic status is forgotten, as it were), it is
dominant. ^Chatman 1978. See also AB- s a i d to b e a PSEUDO-DIEGETIC NARRATIVE

SENT NARRATOR. (the Theaetetus). HGenette 1980, 1983.


mediation. The process or operation ef- S e e a l s o DIEGETIC LEVEL, EMBEDDING.

fected by a MEDIATOR and correlating metalanguage. A (natural or artificial) lan-


(initial and final sets of) situations in myth guage used to describe another language
and narrative; the intratextual TRANSFOR- (the object language). For example, the
MATION linking two (sets of opposite) language used by grammarians to describe
situations. ^L6vi-Strauss 1963; Kongas- the functioning of English is a metalan-
Maranda and Maranda 1962. guage. 1iBy extension, any language used
mediator. The ACTOR or character through to describe a given domain constitutes a
whom a MEDIATION is effected. The media- m e t a l a n g u a g e : NARRATIVE GRAMMAR c a n be

tor is, at first, linked to actions in opposition considered to be the metalanguage char-
to the antagonist but then proves capable acterizing the form and functioning of
of engaging in the same (kind of) actions narrative. ULyons 1977.
undertaken by that antagonist, metalepsis. The intrusion into one OIEGESIS
message. One of the fundamental constitu- (dieg&se) of a being from another diegesis;
ents of any act of (verbal) communication. the mingling of two distinct DIEGETIC LEV-
The message is the text (the signifying ma- ELS. Should an EXTRADIEGETIC narrator
terial, the set of signs to be decoded) sent suddenly enter the world of the situations
by t h e ADDRESSER t o t h e ADDRESSEE. HJa- and events recounted, for instance, a me-
kobson 1960. See also CODE, CONSTITUTIVE talepsis obtains. IGenette 1980, 1983.
FACTORS OF COMMUNICATION, POETIC FUNC- mataiingus! function. One of the FUNC-
TION. TIONS OF COMMUNICATION in terms of which
any communicative (verbal) act may be plicitly comments on a narrative unit x and
structured and oriented; the METALINGUIS- provides an answer to such questions as
TIC FUNCTION. When the communicative act "What does x mean in the (sub-)code ac-
is centered on the CODE (rather than on cording to which the narrative is developed?,"
o n e o f t h e o t h e r CONSTITUTIVE FACTORS OF "What is x in the (sub-)code used?," or
COMMUNICATION), it (mainly) has a metalin- "How does x function in the (sub-)code ac-
gual function. More specifically, those cording to which the narrative can be
passages in a narrative focusing on and understood?" In "John punched Jim, and
explaining the language constituting it can Jim punched him back. This fight only
be said to fulfill a metalingual function: "In lasted a few seconds," for example, "this
Anjou, the frippe, a colloquial word, desig- fight" constitutes a metanarrative sign and,
nates what goes with bread, from butter more specifically, a metaproairetic one: it
spread on toast—the commonest kind—to explicitly comments on the meaning of
peach preserves, the most distinguished of "John punched Jim, and Jim punched him
all the Srippes." IJakobson 1960; Prince back" in terms of a PROAIRETIC CODE.
1982. UPrince 1977, 1982. See also METANARRA-
metalinguistic function. See METALINGUAL TIVE, NARRATIVE CODE.

FUNCTION. metaphor. A figure of speech through which


metanarrative. About narrative; describing a term designating a notion, A, is substi-
narrative. A narrative having (a) narrative tuted for or identified with another term
as (one of) its topic(s) is (a) metanarrative. designating another notion, B, thereby as-
More specifically, a narrative referring to it- cribing to B one or more of the qualities of
self and to those elements by which it is A or investing it with qualities associated
constituted and communicated, a narrative with A (consider "A woman is a rose"
discussing itself, a SELF-REFLEXIVE NARRA- where "rose" is identified with "woman" or
TIVE, is metanarrative. Even more specifi- "The winter of my life is fast approaching"
cally, the passages or units in a narrative where "winter" is substituted for something
that refer explicitly to the CODES or sub- like "final pari"). HJakobson argued in an
codes in terms of which the narrative important essay that two processes stand
signifies are metanarrative and constitute at the heart of verbal activity: the meta-
METANARRATIVE SIGNS. W a n t o n 1 9 7 7 ; phorical process, where one discourse
Hutcheon 1984; Prince 1982. See also topic leads to another through relations of
NARRATIVE CODE. similarity (involving selection and substitu-

metanarrative sign. In a narrative, a sign tion), and the metonymical process, where

explicitly referring to (one of) the CODES (or one discourse topic leads to another

one of the subcodes) in terms of which the through relations of contiguity. Following

narrative signifies; a sign predicated on an- and expanding Jakobson, who emphasized

other sign considered as an element in the the importance of the metonymical process

code framing the narrative in which they in realistic fiction, narratologists have

both appear. TThe metanarrative sign ex- tended to treat NARRATIVE as primarily me-
tonymic: they have argued that MOTIFS and and FUNCTIONS are integrated into SE-
FUNCTIONS are integrated into SEQUENCES QUENCES primarily through relations of
mainly through relations of contiguity. Yet it contiguity (the narrated situations and
can also be argued that narrative is—in an events constitute logico-temporal chains).
important way—a function of the meta- liCuller 1981; Jakobson 1956; Lodge 1977.
phoric process: in a narrative sequence, See also METAPHOR.
the last situation or event constitutes a par- m i c r o s t r u c t u r e . T h e SURFACE STRUCTURE of

tial repetition of the first; in other words, a text; the particular way the MACROSTRUC-
there is a relation of similarity between the TURE (or DEEP STRUCTURE) of a t e x t is

two. "^Culler 1981;Jakobson 1956; Lodge realized. The microstructure is related to


1 9 7 7 . S e e a l s o METONYMY, TRANSFOHMA- the macrostructure by a set of operations,
TION. or TRANSFORMATIONS. W a n Dijk 1972,
metonymy. A figure of speech whereby a 1974-75, 1976a. See also NARRATIVE
term designating a notion, A, is used for GRAMMAR.

another term designating another notion, B, middle. The set of incidents in a PLOT or AC-
related to A as cause and effect, container TION b e t w e e n t h e BEGINNING a n d t h e END.

and thing contained, or part and whole The middle follows and is followed by other
(consider "In the sweat of thy face shalt incidents. UStudents of narrative have
thou eat bread" where "sweat" as effect is pointed out that the middle is doubly ori-
substituted for "labor" as cause,, or "She ented (prospectively from beginning to end
smoked a whole pack" where "pack" as and retrospectively from end to beginning),
container is substituted for "cigarettes" as that it paradoxically progresses toward the
thing contained). 1|ln an influential essay, end while, at the same time, postponing
Jakobson argued that two processes stand the reaching of the end, and that it consti-
at the heart of verbal activity: the meto- tutes a (more or less prolonged) situation
nymical process, where one discourse of deviance from the "normal" (the non-
topic leads to another through relations of NARRATABLE). UAristotle 1968; Brooks
contiguity (involving CAUSALITY and inclu- 1 9 8 4 . S e e a l s o COMPLICATING ACTION, COM-

sion), and the metaphorical process, where PLICATION, NARRATIVITY, RAVELLING.

one discourse topic leads to another mimesis. In narratology, SHOWING, enacting


through relations of similarity. Following (as opposed to TELLING, recounting). HPIato
and expanding Jakobson, who emphasized distinguished between two poetic modes:
the importance of the metonymical process with mimesis (imitation), the poet delivers a
in realistic fiction (the narrator often di- speech as if s/he were someone else (a
gresses metonymically from CHARACTER to given character), whereas with DIEGESIS
SETTING and from plot to atmosphere), (narration, di£g6sis), the poet delivers a
many narratologists have treated NARRA- speech in his or her own name. While mi-
TIVE as predominantly metonymic. mesis therefore involves no (or minima!)
Specifically, they have argued that MOTIFS narratorial mediation, such mediation is
characteristic of diegesis. For Aristotle, senting only a single EVENT: "She opened
according to whom all art is imitation and the door." 2. A narrative containing a sin-
g l e TEMPORAL JUNCTURE ( L a b o v ) : " S h e a t e
the various arts differ depending on object,
then she slept." HGenette 1983; Labov
means, and manner or mode, the two
1 9 7 2 . See a l s o COMPLEX STORY, MINIMAL
modes as well as the so-called mixed STORY.
mode (which is formed of the other two
and illustrated by Homer, for example) minimal story. A narrative recounting only
constitute three varieties of mimesis. In Ar- two STATES and one EVENT such that (1)
istotelian terms, verbal narrative could then one state precedes the event in time and
be characterized as the imitation of an ac- the event precedes the other state in time
tion (mimesis praxeos), using linguistic (and causes it); (2) the second state con-
means and adopting any one of the three stitutes the inverse (or the modification,
modes. 1jln a discussion of Aristotle's Poet- including the "zero" modification) of the
ics and its pertinence for understanding first. "John was happy, then he saw Peter,
narrative, Ricoeur develops a threefold then, as a result, he was unhappy" is a
model of mimesis as imitation whereby minimal story. HPrince 1973. See also
PLOT, which provides the means that allow COMPLEX STORY, MINIMAL NARRATIVE, PRO-
us to grasp and make sense of human CESS, STORY.
time, is viewed as a temporal configuration mise en abyme. A miniature replica of a
mediating between the time prefigured in text embedded within that text; a textual
the practical field (the field of life and hu- part reduplicating, reflecting, or mirroring
man action) and the time refigured through (one or more than one aspect of) the tex-
the reception of the narrative. UPerhaps no tual whole. In The Counterfeiters, Edouard's
concept has exerted a more powerful influ- writing of a novel entitled The Counterfeit-
ence than imitation in the Western critico- ers constitutes a mise en abyme. TThe
literary tradition, whether through encour- term comes from heraldry: a figure in an
aging the accurate representation of life or escutcheon is said to be en abyme when it
through fostering the imitation of classic constitutes a miniature of that escutcheon.
works and ancient masters or—most gen- HDallenbach 1977.
erally—through promoting the view that the modality. The qualification of a statement or
work of art, by holding a mirror up to na- set of statements by a modal operator (cf.
ture (and not merely being a mirror itself), "John was sick" and "John did not know
reveals the presence of the generic in the that he was sick"). The operator may, for
specific, the universal in the particular, the example, be alethic (express the modalities
essential in the phenomenal. lAristotie of possibility, impossibility, and necessity),
1968; Frye 1957; Genette 1980; Plato deontic (express the modalities of permis-
1S68; Ricoeur 1984,1S85. See also NAR- sion, prohibition, and obligation), axiological
RATIVE. (express the modalities of goodness, bad-
mlraimal narrative. 1, A NARRATIVE repre- ness, and indifference), or epistemic
(express the modalities of knowledge, ig- rior in kind to both); (2) romance (superior
norance, and belief). ^Various modal in degree to both); (3) high mimesis (supe-
c o n s t r a i n t s g o v e r n NARRATIVE DOMAINS rior in degree to others but not to the
and, more generally, determine what "hap- environment); (4) low mimesis (equal to
pens" in a narrative by establishing what is both others and the environment); (5) irony
or could be the case in the world repre- (inferior to others or to the environment).
sented, regulating the characters' liFrye 1957; Todorov 1966,1981.
knowledge, setting their values, obligations, molecular story. A story consisting of two
and goals, and in general guiding their o r m o r e ATOMIC STORIES; a COMPOUND

course of action. Indeed, it has been ar- STORY. U D o l e z e l 1 9 7 6 . S e e a l s o MODALITY.

gued that narratives develop along modal monologic narrative. A narrative character-
axes and represent transitions from certain ized by a unifying voice or consciousness
states on these axes to other ones (going, superior to other voices or conscious-
for example, from what has to be done to nesses in that narrative {Eugenie Grandet,
what can be done; from what is bad to A Tale of Two Cities). In monologic as op-
what is good; and/or from what is not p o s e d t o DIALOGIC NARRATIVE, t h e n a r r a t o r ' s

known to what is known). 1iln the Greimas- views, judgments, and knowledge consti-
sian model of narrative, modalizations tute the ultimate authority with respect to
along the axes of ability (being able to do the world represented. HBakhtin 1981,
or be), desire (wanting to do or be), knowl- 1984; Pascal 1977.
edge (knowing how to do or be), and monologue. A long discourse produced by
obligation (having to do or be) are the most one character (and not addressed to other
important. liDolelel 1976; Greimas 1970, characters). Should the monologue be un-
1971; Greimas and Courtes 1982; Pavel spoken (should it consist of the character's
1980, 1985; Ryan 1985. See also ACTAN- verbalized thoughts), it constitutes an INTE-
TIAL ROLE, ATOMIC STORY, COMPETENCE. RIOR MONOLOGUE. S h o u l d it b e s p o k e n , it

mode. 1. DISTANCE. The extent of narrato- constitutes an exterior monologue or solilo-


rial mediation characterizes the mode of a quy. IHolman 1972. See also DIALOGUE.
n a r r a t i v e : SHOWING a n d TELLING a r e t w o montage. A technique whereby the meaning
different modes. <]Along with PERSPECTIVE of a given series of situations and events
or POINT OF VIEW, m o d e c o n s t i t u t e s t h e c a t - comes from their juxtaposition rather than
egory of narrative MOOD. 2. A fictional from their constituent features (cf. the
world considered from the point of view of "Newsreels" in Dos Passos's U.S.A.). The
the hero's power of action in relation to hu- term is particularly associated with motion
man beings and to their environment. Frye pictures. HMetz 1974; Souvage 1965.
argues that the hero can be superior, mood. The set of modalities—namely, DIS-
equal, or inferior in kind or in degree to TANCE or MODE a n d PERSPECTIVE o r POINT

others and/or to the environment and he OF VIEW—regulating narrative information.


characterizes five modes: (1) myth (supe- The mood of a narrative will vary depend-
i n g o n w h e t h e r SHOWING o r TELLING i s in Dundes 1964; Tomashevsky 1965. See
evidence, for example; it will also vary de- a l s o ALLOMOTIF.

p e n d i n g on w h e t h e r INTERNAL o r EXTERNAL motiferne. A FUNCTION (in the Proppian


FOCALIZATION is adopted. HGenette 1980. sense). Dundes, who borrowed the term
moon. One of six fundamental ROLES or from Pike, suggested its adoption to desig-
FUNCTIONS isolated by Souriau (in his study nate the fundamental structural unit of a
of the possibilities of drama). The Moon folktale: a motiferne is specified or mani-
( a n a l o g o u s t o P r o p p ' s DONOR a n d HELPER fested by a MOTIF and is to the latter as a
as well as to Greimas's HELPER) assists function is to an ACTION, a phoneme to a
t h e LION o r HERO. H S c h o l e s 1 9 7 4 ; S o u r i a u phone, or a morpheme to a morph. UDole-
1 9 5 0 . See a l s o ACTANT. 2el 1972; Dundes 1964; Pike 1967.
motif. 1. A minimal thematic unit. When a motivation. 1. The network of devices jus-
motif recurs frequently in a given text, it is tifying the introduction of a MOTIF, a
called a LEITMOTIF. UA motif should not be complex of motifs, or, more generally, a
confused with a THEME, which constitutes a constituent feature of a (literary) text; the
more abstract and more general semantic reason for the use of a given textual ele-
unit manifested by or reconstructed from a ment; COMPOSITION. fTomashevsky
set of motifs: if glasses are a motif in Prin- distinguished between compositional moti-
cess Brarnbilla, vision is a theme in that vation (referring to the usefulness of the
work. A motif should also be distinguished motif), realistic motivation (stressing the
from a TOPOS, which is a specific complex lifelikeness, realism, or authenticity of the
of motifs that frequently appears in (liter- motif), and artistic motivation (justifying the
ary) texts (the wise fool, the aged child, the introduction of the motif in terms of the re-
locus amoenus, etc.) 2. A minimal narra- quirements of "art"). 2. The complex of
tive unit at the syntactic level; a NARRATIVE circumstances, reasons, purposes, and im-
STATEMENT. For Tomashevsky, motifs can pulses governing a character's actions
be static (designate a STATE) or dynamic (and making them plausible), © r o o k s and
(designate an EVENT). Furthermore, they Warren 1959; Ducrot and Todorov 1979;
can be logically essential to the narrative Genette 1968; Propp 1968; Rimmon-
action and its causal-chronological coher- Kenan 1933; Tomashevsky 1965; Wellek
e n c e (BOUND MOTIFS), or t h e y c a n b e a n d W a r r e n 1 9 4 9 . S e e a l s o LAYING BARE,

logically inessential to it (FREE MOTIFS). 3. NATURALIZATION, VERISIMILITUDE.

An element fulfilling or manifesting a MOTI- move. 1. Any sequence of FUNCTIONS pro-


FEME. A motif is to a motiferne as a phone ceeding from a Villainy or a Lack to a
(a sound of language) is to a phoneme (a DENOUEMENT. According to Propp, every
distinctive sound class), a morph to a mor- tale (sk&zka) consists of one or more
p h e m e , or a n ACTION t o a FUNCTION. moves. HMoves can be combined through
^IBremond 1982; Daemmrich and Daemm- EMBEDDING, ALTERNATION, a n d LINKING, or

rich 1986; Ducrot and Todorov 1979; CONJOINING. 2 . A CARDINAL FUNCTION; a


NUCLEUS; a NARREME. i n P a v e l ' s NARRATIVE common kind. Thus, the Oedipus myth re-
GRAMMAR, a Move is an action called tor by lates the opposition non-autochtonous
a Problem (e.g. "John's life was threat- origin of man / autochtonous origin of man,
ened"), representing an effort toward its to the more acceptable opposition overesti-
Solution (e.g., "John left the country"), and mation of kinship ties / underestimation of
bringing about another Move or the end of kinship ties. !Frye 1957; Greimas 1970;
the story. "iPavel 1985; Propp 1S68. See Jolles 1956; Levi-Strauss 1963, 1965-71;
a l s o NARRATIVE DOMAIN. I Scholes and Kellogg 1966.
multiple internal focalization. A type of IN- mytheme. The fundamental constituent unit
TERNAL FOCALIZATION Or POINT OF VIEW of MYTH. l i L e v i - S t r a u s s 1 9 6 3 .

whereby the same situations and events mythos. A PLOT, an arrangement of inci-
are presented more than once, each time dents. For Aristotle, mythos consists in the
in terms of a different focalizer (The Ring selection and possible rearrangement of
and the Book, The Moonstone, Rasho- the units constituting LOGOS (the imitation
mon). HGenette 1980. See also of a real action, or PRAXIS). VThe distinction
FOCALIZATION. between mythos and logos is suggestive of
multiple Internal point of view. See MULTI- t h a t b e t w e e n DISCOURSE a n d STORY or

PLE INTERNAL FOCALIZATION. U P r i n c e 1 9 8 2 . SJU2ET a n d FABULA. HAristotle 1968; C h a t -

multiple selective omniscience. One of m a n 1 9 7 8 . S e e a l s o THOUGHT.

e i g h t possible POINTS OF VIEW a c c o r d i n g t o

Friedman's classification: multiple selective


omniscience characterizes the HETERODI-
EGETIC NARRATOR a d o p t i n g VARIABLE

INTERNAL FOCALIZATION (TO the Light-

house). UN. Friedman 1955b. See also


SELECTIVE OMNISCIENCE. narratable. That which is worthy of being
myth. A traditional narrative, usually associ- told; that which is susceptible of or calls for
ated with religious belief and ritual, that narration. ^Brooks 1984; Miller 1981. See
expresses and justifies an exemplary as- a l s o MIDDLE, REPORTABILITY.

pect of the way things are. ^According to narrated. 1, The set of situations and
L6vi-Strauss, the structure of myth can be events recounted in a narrative; the STORY
expressed through a four-term homology (as o p p o s e d to t h e DISCOURSE). 2. T h e

relating two pairs of opposite MYTHEMES (A signs in a narrative representing the situ-
and B; C and D): A:B: :C:D (A is to 8 as ations and events narrated (as opposed to
C is to D). This formula presumably ac- t h e NARRATING). <:Prince 1982.

counts for the meaning of myth, whereby n a r r a t e d m o n o l o g u e , FREE INDIRECT DIS-

one kind of irreconcilability (contradiction, COURSE in t h e c o n t e x t of THIRD-PERSON

opposition) is made simpler to contend with NARRATIVE. With narrated monologue (as
through being related to another, more o p p o s e d t o PSYCHONARRATION), the a c -
count of the character's discourse is mainly clear (for example, in the case of a narra-
in words that are recognizabSy the charac- tive where the narratee is also a character:
ter's. HCohn 1966, 1978. See also QUOTED Isa, in Vipers' Tangle). HGenette 1980,
MONOLOGUE, SELF-NARRATED MONOLOGUE. 1983; Mosher 1980; Piwowarczyk 1976;
narratee. The one who is narrated to, as in- Prince 1980, 1982. See also DISTANCE,
scribed in the text. There is at least one NARRATING INSTANCE, PERSON.

(more or less overtly represented) narratee narrating. 1. The telling or relating of one
per narrative, located at the same OIEGETIC or more events. 2. The DISCOURSE (as
LEVEL a s the NARRATOR a d d r e s s i n g h i m or opposed to the STORY). 3. The signs in a
her. In a given narrative, there may, of narrative representing the narrating activity,
course, be several different narratees, its origin, its destination, and its context (as
each addressed in turn by th-e same narra- opposed to the NARRATED). 'Genette 1980;
tor (Vipers' Tangle) or by a different one Prince 1982. See also INTERPOLATED NAR-
{The Immoralist). TlLike the narrator, the RATING, PRIOR NARRATING, SIMULTANEOUS

narratee may be represented as a charac- NARRATING, SUBSEQUENT NARRATING.

ter, playing a more or less important role in narrating instance. The act of recounting a
the situations and events recounted (Vi- series of situations and events and, by ex-
pers' Tangle, The Immoralist, Heart of tension, the spatio-temporal context
Darkness, A Change of Heart). Very often, ( i n c l u d i n g t h e NARRATOR a n d t h e NARRA-

however, the narratee is not represented TEE) of that act. IThere can be several
as a character (Tom Jones, Eugenie Gran- distinct narrating instances in a single nar-
det, Crime and Punishment). IThe nar- rative, each involving the same narrator
ratee— a purely textual construct—must (Vipers' Tangle, Diary of a Superfluous
be distinguished from the real READER or Man, Doctor Glas) or a different one (The
RECEIVER. After all, the same real reader Immoralist, Manon Lescaut). fiGenette
can read different narratives (each having 1 9 8 0 , 1 9 8 3 . S e e a l s o PRIMARY NARRATIVE,

different narratees); and the same narra- VOICE.

tive (which always has the same set of narration. 1. A NARRATIVE; a discourse
narratees) can have an indefinitely varying representing one or more events. Narration
set of real readers. TThe narratee must is traditionally distinguished from DESCRIP-
also be distinguished from the IMPLIED TION and from COMMENTARY but usually
READER: the former constitutes the narra- incorporates them within itself. 2. The
tor's audience and is inscribed as such in production of a narrative; the recounting of
the text; the latter constitutes the IMPLIED a series of situations and events. A POSTE-
AUTHOR'S audience (and is inferable from RIOR NARRATION follows the narrated
the entire text). Though the distinction can situations and events in time (and is char-
be problematic (for example, in the case of acteristic of "classical" or "traditional"
a maximally covert narratee: "Hills Like n a r r a t i v e ) ; a n ANTERIOR NARRATION pre-
White Elephants"), it is sometimes very cedes them in time (as in PREDICTIVE
NARRATIVE); a SIMULTANEOUS NARRATION tion, some narratologists (Labov, Prince,
(presumably) occurs at the same time as Rimmon-Kenan) have defined it as the re-
they do ("Jim is now walking down the counting of at least two real or fictive
street; he sees Joan and greets h e r . . . "); events (or one situation and one event),
finally, a n INTERCALATED NARRATION is tem- neither of which logically presupposes or
porally situated between two moments of entails the other. In order to distinguish it
the action recounted and is characteristic from the recounting of a random series of
of epistolary narratives (Pamela) and diary situations and events, narratologists
narratives (Nausea). 3. TELLING, in Todo- (Danto, Greimas, Todorov) have also ar-
rov's terminology: narration is to REPRE- gued that narrative must have a continuant
SENTATION a s telling is t o SHOWING. subject and constitute a whole. <TThe narra-
4. DISCOURSE, in Ricardou's terminology: tive media of representation are diverse
narration is to FICTION as discourse is to (oral, written, and sign language, for exam-
STORY. HGenette 1980; Prince 1982; Ricar- ple, still or moving pictures, gestures,
dou 1967; Todorov 1966,1981. music, or any ordered combination
narrative. The recounting (as product and thereof). So are the forms narrative can
process, object and act, structure and take (in the domain of verbal narrative
structuration) of one or more real or ficti- alone, we find novels and romances, no-
tious EVENTS communicated by one, two, vellas and short stories, history, biography
or several (more or less overt) NARRATORS and autobiography, epics, myths, folktales,
to one, two, or several (more or less overt) legends and ballads, news reports, sponta-
NARRATEES. Such (possibly interesting) neous accounts in ordinary conversation,
texts as "Electrons are constituents of at- and so on). As for its distribution, narrative
oms," "Mary is tall and Peter is small," "All appears in every human society known to
men are mortal; Socrates is a man; Socra- history and anthropology, indeed, all (aver-
tes is mortal," and "Roses are red / Violets age) human beings know how to produce
are blue / Sugar is sweet / And so are and process narrative at a very early age.
you" do not constitute narratives, since HConsidered as a structure or product, and
they do not represent any event. Moreover, following Labov's well-known characteriza-
a dramatic performance representing tion, narrative can be said to exhibit at
(many fascinating) events does not consti- l e a s t a COMPLICATING ACTION a n d ( w h e n

tute a narrative either, since these events, "complete" or "fully developed") as many
rather than being recounted, occur directly as six basic macrostructural elements: AB-
on stage. On the other hand, even such STRACT, ORIENTATION, c o m p l i c a t i n g a c t i o n ,

possibly uninteresting texts as "Tl^e man EVALUATION, RESULT o r RESOLUTION, a n d

opened the door," "The goldfish died," and CODA. More specifically, and following the
"The glass fell on the floor" are narratives, famous two-tier structuralist model, narra-
according to this definition. *5ln order to dis- tive can be said to have two parts: STORY
tinguish narrative from mere event descrip- and DISCOURSE. IThe story always involves
temporal sequence (it consists of at least on its QUEST and, as a result of a series of
one modification of a state of affairs ob- TESTS (PERFORMANCE), fulfills or fails to ful-

taining at time t0 into another state of fill the contract and is (justly) rewarded or
affairs obtaining at time („), and this is its (unjustly) punished (SANCTION). TThe
most distinctive feature. Of course, tem- "same" given story can be told differently
poral relations between the situations and in narratives adopting different discourses,
events making up a story are not the only and conversely, different stories can be
ones possible: these situations and events told in terms of the same discourse (with
may be related causally, for example. the same chronological arrangement of
Moreover, in a "true" narrative as opposed events, for instance, the same FOCALIZA-
to the mere recounting of a random series TION, SPEED, FREQUENCY, a n d DISTANCE,

of changes of state, these situations and and the same kind of inscription of narrator
events also make up a whole, a SEQUENCE and narratee in the narrative text). HThe
the first and last major terms of which are very depiction of a narrator recounting situ-
partial repetitions of each other, a structure ations and events to a narratee
having—to use Aristotle's terminology—a emphasizes the fact that nanative is not
BEGINNING, a MIDDLE, a n d an END. I l f the only a product but also a process, not
Aristotelian account of story structure has merely an object but also an act which oc-
been exceedingly influential, the most sem- curs in a certain situation because of
inal account of that structure in modem certain factors and with a view of fulfilling
NARRATOLOGY has probably been that of certain functions (informing, diverting atten-
Vladimir Propp, who developed the notion tion, entertaining, persuading, etc.). More
of FUNCTION, argued that every (Russian specifically, narrative is a context-bound
fairy) tale consists of one or more MOVES, exchange between two parties, an ex-
and categorized tale participants in terms change resulting from the desire of (at
of the fundamental ROLES they may as- least one of) these parties, and the "same"
sume. HFurther investigation into the nature story can have a different worth in different
of functions and roles have led Greimas situations (A wants to know what hap-
and his school to arrive at what is another pened, but B does not; A understands an
influential account of story structure, ac- account in one way and B in another). This
cording to which canonical narrative is the sheds light on the tendency of many narra-
representation of a series of events ori- tive texts to underline the contract between
ented in terms of a goal (equivalent to a narrator and narratee, that contract on
JUNCTION b e t w e e n SUBJECT a n d OBJECT). which the very existence of the narrative
Specifically, after a CONTRACT between depends: I will tell you a story if you prom-
SENDER a n d SUBJECT (MANIPULATION) ise to be good; 1 will listen to you if you
make it valuable; or, more literarily, a tale
whereby the latter acquires COMPETENCE
for a day of survival (Arabian Nights), a
and undertakes to attain an Object (for the
story for a night of love ("Sarrasine"), a di-
benefit of a RECEIVER), the Subject goes on
ary for redemption (Vipers' Tangle). This redefinition and effects a mediation be-
also explains why unsolicited narratives, in tween the law of what is and the desire for
particular, must awaken and maintain de- what may be. Most crucially, perhaps, by
sire in the audience by relying on the marking off distinct moments in time and
d y n a m i c s o f SURPRISE a n d SUSPENSE; w h y setting up relations among them, by dis-
narrators try to make it clear that their nar- covering meaningful designs in temporal
rative has a POINT; and why the very series, by establishing an end already
shape of a narrative is affected by the situ- partly contained in the beginning and a be-
ation in which it occurs and the goal which ginning already partly containing the end,
it seeks to attain, with the sender of the by exhibiting the meaning of time and/or
message giving certain kinds of informa- providing it with meaning, narrative deci-
tion, disposing it in a certain way, adopting phers time and indicates how to decipher
one kind of focalization as opposed to an- it. In sum, narrative illuminates temporality
other, underscoring the importance or and humans as temporal beings. W a r n
strangeness of certain details, so that the 1984,1985; Aristotle 1968; Bal 1985;
receiver can better process the information Barthes 1974, 1975; Benjamin 1969; Booth
in terms of certain imperatives and ends. 1983; Bremond 1973, 1980; Brooks 1984;
r o t the many functions that narrative can Chafe 1980; Chambers 1984; Chatman
have, there are some that it excels at or is 1978; Courtis 1976; Culler 1981; Danto
unique in fulfilling. By definition, narrative 1965; Van Dijk 1976a, 1976b; Dole2el
always recounts one or more events; but, 1973, 1976; Freytag 1894; Genette 1980,
as etymology suggests (the term' narrative 1983; Genot 1979, 1984; Greimas 1970,
is related to the Latin GNARUS), it also rep- 1983a, 1983b; Grimes 1975; Henault
resents a particular mode of knowledge. It 1983; Janik 1973; Johnson and Mandler
does not simply mirror what happens; it 1980; Kermode 1967; Kloepfer 1980; La-
explores and devises what can happen. It bov 1972; Larivaille 1974; Lemon and Reis
does not merely recount changes of state; 1965; Matejka and Pomorska 1971; Martin
it constitutes and interprets them as signi- 1986; Mink 1969-70, 1978; Mitchell 1981;
fying parts of signifying wholes (situations, Pavel 1985; Prince 1982; Propp 1968; Ri-
practices, persons, societies). Narrative coeur 1984,1985; Rimmon-Kenan 1983;
can thus shed light on individual fate or Sacks 1972; Scholes and Kellogg 1366;
group destiny, the unity of a self or the na- Segre 1979; Smith 1981; Stanzel 1984;
ture of a collectivity. Through showing that Stein 1982; Todorov 1966, 1978, 1981;
apparently heterogeneous situations and White 1973. See also CAUSALITY, DOUBLE
events can make up one signifying struc- LOGIC OF NARRATIVE, FREYTAG'S PYRAMIO,

ture (or vice versa) and, more particularly, NARRATION, NARRATIVITY, ORDER, PLOT,

through providing its own brand of order POST HOC ERGO PROPTER HOC FALLACY.

and coherence to (a possible) reality, it fur- narrative clause. A clause the displacement
nishes examples for its transformation or of which across a TEMPORAL JUNCTURE
leads to a change in the semantic interpre- ing its very shape: an act of narration
tation of the original narrative sequence. In supplies something which is (to be) ex-
"John went to greet the couple. The man changed for something else (I will tell you
stopped talking, and the woman started to a story if you promise to be good; I will lis-
smile. John decided they were nice." both ten to you if you make it valuable; or, more
"John went to greet the couple" and "John literarily, a taie for a day of survival as in
decided they were nice" are narrative Arabian Nights, a story for a night of love
clauses. ^Narrative clauses constitute the as in "Sarrasine," adiar^for redemption
skeleton of the narrative, and as opposed as in Vipers' Tangle). UBarthes 1974;
to FREE CLAUSES a n d RESTRICTED CLAUSES, Brooks 1984; Chambers 1984.
they are locked in a certain position in the narrative domain. The set of MOVES per-
narrative sequence. liLabov 1972; Labov taining (mainly) to a given character (and
and Waletzky 1967. See also COORDINATE his or her allies). From a semantic point of
CLAUSES, DISPLACEMENT SET. view, a narrative domain is governed by a
narrative closure. A conclusion giving the number of maxims or ruEes establishing
feeling that a narrative or narrative SE- what is or could be the case, regulating the
QUENCE has come to an END and providing character's knowledge, setting his or her
it with an ultimate unity and coherence, an priorities, and, most generally, guiding him
end creating in the receiver a feeling of ap- or her in assessing a situation and reacting
propriate completion and finality. liHamon to it. HA narrative in which all the narrative
1975; Kermode 1967; Miller 1981; Smith domains are governed b7 identical sets of
1968; Torgovnick 1981. See also CODA. maxims and rules is said to be semanti-

narrative code. The system of norms, rules, cally homogeneous. When the sets are not

and constraints in terms of which a narra- identical, the narrative is semantically het-

tive MESSAGE signifies. This system is not erogeneous or partitioned. Should only

monolithic: it conjoins, combines, and or- certain classes of maxims and rules—say,

ders a set of CODES or subcodes (the oniological and epistemological ones—be

HERMENEUTIC CODE, t h e PROAIRETIC CODE, in force in all the narrative domains, the

the SYMBOLIC CODE, etc.). tBarthes 1974, narrative would be said to be ontologically

1981a; Prince 1977, 1982. and epistemologically homogeneous. In

narrative competence. The ability to pro- other words, a narrative >could be ontologi-

duce and understand narratives. One of cally homogeneous but axiologically

the goals of NARRATOLOGY is to character- partitioned, or epistemologically homoge-

ize narrative competence, lirlamon 1S81; neous but axiologically p artitioned, and so

Prince 1981-82. See also LANGUE. on. <iPavel 1980, 1985. See also MODALITY.

narrative contract. The agreement between narrative grammar. A series of statements

t h e NARRATOR a n d t h e NARRATEE, t h e teller


and formulas interrelated by an ordered set

and his or her audience, underlying the of rules and accounting for (structural as-

very existence of a NARRATIVE and affect- pects of) a particular set of narratives or
the set of all and only possible narratives. n a r r a t i v e d i s c o u r s e (FREQUENCY, RHYTHM,

liAmong the various narrative grammars SPEED, natatorial intrusions, etc.); (4) a
that have been developed, the STORY pragmatic component (specifying the cog-
GRAMMARS devised by students of cognitive nitive and communicative factors which
psychology and artificial intelligence have affect the processing, TELLABIIITY, and suit-
been very influential: they attempt to spec- ability of the output of the first three parts
ify the basic constituents of the NARRATED of the grammar); and (5) an expression
and to describe their interrelations, and component, allowing for the translation of
they help to investigate the effects of struc- the information provided by the other com-
ture and content variables on memory and ponents into a given medium of repre-
the comprehension of texts. ^Narrative sentation (for example, written English).
grammars developed along structuralist HBIack and Bower 1980; Bruce 1978;
and text-linguistic lines and trying to spec- Bruce and Newman 1978; Chabrol 1973;
ify the syntax and semantics of plot Colby 1973; van Dijk 1972,1976a, 1980;
(Pavel), the macrostructural elements of Fuger 1972; Genot 1979, 1984; Glenn
narrative and their articulation (van Dijk), or 1978; Georges 1970; Hendricks 1973;
the constituents of both story and dis- Kintsch and van Dijk 1975; Lakoff 1972;
course and their interrelations have also Mandler and Johnson 1977; Pavel 1976,
been proposed. Such grammars (often) 1985; Prince 1973, 1980, 1982; Rumelhart
aim at completeness (accounting for all 1975; Ryan 1979; Schank 1975; Thorn-
and only narratives), explicitness (indicat- dyke 1975; Todorov 1969; Wilensky 1978.
ing, with a minimum of interpretation left to S e e a l s o METALANGUAGE.

their users, how a narrative can be pro- n a r r a t i v e l e v e l . S e e DIEGETIC LEVEL.

duced and/or understood by utilizing a narrative medium. The SUBSTANCE of the


specific set of rules), and empirical plausi- EXPRESSION plane of narrative; the medium
bility (being in line with what is known in terms of which the narrative manifests it-
about cognitive and social determinants). self. With written narrative, for example,
HA grammar of narrative might ultimately that medium is written language; with oral
consist of the following interconnected narrative, it is oral language. ";Chatman
parts: (1) a finite number of (REWRITE) 1 9 7 8 . S e e a l s o MANIFESTATION.

RULES g e n e r a t i n g t h e MACRO- a n d MICRO- narrative program. A SYNTAGM at the level


STRUCTURES of all and only sequences of o f n a r r a t i v e SURFACE STRUCTURE r e p r e s e n t -

narrated situations and events; (2) a se- ing a change of state effected by an ACTOR
mantic component interpreting these and affecting another (or the same) actor.
structures (characterizing both the global Narrative programs can be simple (when
macrostructural and the local microstruc- they do not require the realization of an-
tural content); (3) a finite set of (TRANS- other narrative program for their own
FORMATIONAL) RULES o p e r a t i n g o n the i n - realization) or complex (when they do).
terpreted structures and accounting for ^Greimas 1970,1983a; Greimas and
Courtes 1982; Henault 1983. See also u s i n g t h e c a t e g o r i e s of PERSON, MODE, a n d

ACT, NARRATIVE TRAJECTORY. PERSPECTIVE. (Is there a HETERODIEGETIC

narrative proposition. See PROPOSITION. o r a HOMODIEGETIC NARRATOR? A r e the

narrative report. A narrator's account, in his narrated situations and events presented
or her own words, of a character's utter- panoramically—say, in terms of an OVERT
ances or thoughts. ^Chatman 1978. NARRATOR—or scenicaily, in terms of a
narrative schema. A general FRAME in Charader-REFLECTOR? Is t h e POINT OF VIEW

terms of which NARRATIVE is organized. located in the story's PROTAGONIST or its


HAccording to the canonical narrative center of action, or is it located outside t h e
schema, after a given order of things is stony or its center of action?) He isolated
disturbed, a CONTRACT is established be- three fundamental narrative situations: AU-
t w e e n t h e SENDER a n d t h e SUBJECT t o THORIAL (AUKTORIALEERZAHLSITUATION), i n

bring about a new order'or reinstate the which external perspective dominates; FIG-
o l d o n e (MANIPULATION). T h e Subject, w h o URAL or PERSONAL (PERSONALE ERZAHL-

has been qualified through the contract SITUATION), in which a reflector mode domi-
along the axes of desire, obligation, knowl- n a t e s ; arid FIRST-PERSON (ICH ERZAHL-

edge, and/or ability (COMPETENCE), goes SITUATION), in which a homodiegetic narra-


through a number or TESTS to fulfill its part tor dominates. *J!n a more recent discus-
of the c o n t r a c t (PERFORMANCE) a n d is re- sion in which Genette defines narrative sit-
warded (or punished) by the Sender uation according to person (homodiegetic
(SANCTION). HGreimas 1970, 1983a; Grei- or heterodiegetic narrator), FOCALIZATION
mas and Courtes 1982; Henault 1983; (zero, external, or internal), and NARRATIVE
Larivaille 1974. LEVEL (EXTRADIEGETIC o r INTRADIEGETIC),

narrative sentence. A sentence that refers Genette characterizes twelve different nar-
to at least two temporally distinct situations rative situations. He also underlines the
or events but describes (is about) only the fact that if such categories as DISTANCE or
earliest one. "Emperor Napoleon was born temporal relation of NARRATION and nar-
in 1769" constitutes a narrative sentence: it rated were taken into account, many more
refers both to an event occurring in 1769 narrative situations could be characterized.
and to a situation obtaining between 1804 TCohn 1981; Genette 1983; Lintvelt 1981;
and 1815 (when Napoleon was emperor) Stanzel 1964, 1971,1984. See also FOCUS
but it describes only the former. Nanative OF NARRATION.

sentences are important signs of the teleo- narrative statement. An elementary constit-
logical determination of narrative. *!Danto uent of DISCOURSE independent of the
1965. See also END. particular medium of narrative MANIFESTA-
narrative situation. The mediating process TION: the discourse can be said to state
through which the NARRATED is presented. the story through a connected set of narra-
Stanzel characterized the kinds and de- tive statements. HThere are two basic kinds
grees of "mediacy" found in narrative by of n a r r a t i v e s t a t e m e n t : PROCESS STATE-
MENTS (in the mode of Do or Happen) and ics and narratology: the former develops
STASIS STATEMENTS (in the m o d e of IS). models or grammars accounting for (the
^Chatman 1978. See also ACT, EVENT, structure of) narrative; the latter uses these
HAPPENING, STATE. models or grammars to study particular
narrative strategy. In recounting a narra- narratives. HGenot 1979; Ihwe 1972. See
tive, the set of narrative procedures a l s o NARRATIVE GRAMMAR.

followed or narrative devices used to narrativity. The set of properties character-


achieve some specific goal. <iSouvage izing NARRATIVE and distinguishing it from
1965. nonnarrative; the formal and contextual
narrative trajectory. A set of logically con- features making a narrative more or less
n e c t e d NARRATIVE PROGRAMS. A n a r r a t i v e narrative, as it were. TThe degree of narra-
trajectory involves the same ACTANT, and tivity of a given narrative depends partly on
each one of its constituent narrative pro- the extent to which tfcjat narrative fulfills a
g r a m s c o r r e s p o n d s to an ACTANTIAL ROLE. receiver's desire by representing oriented
in its canonical narrative trajectory, for ex- temporal wholes (prospectively from BEGIN-
ample, the SUBJECT is established as such NING to END and retrospectively from end
by the SENDER, qualified (made competent) to beginning), involving a CONFLICT, con-
along the axes of desire, ability, knowl- sisting of discrete, specific, and positive
edge, and obligation, realized as a situations and events, and meaningful in
pedorming Subject, recognized as one, terms of a human(ized) project and world.
and rewarded IGreimas and Courtes 1982. HBrooks 1984; Genot 1979; Greimas 1970;
narrative world. The set or collection of MO- Kloepfer 1980; Prince 1982. See also DOU-
TIFS in a given narrative (or part thereof) BLE LOGIC OF NARRATIVE, MIDDLE, POST HOC

that are authenticated and thus given the ERGO PROPTER HOC FALLACY.

status of facts. HA distinction can be made narratized discourse. A TYPE OF DISCOURSE

between an actual (or absolute) narrative whereby a character's utterances or verbal


world and a possible (or relative) one: the thoughts are represented, in words that are
former constitutes the sphere of "reality" the narrator's, as acts among other acts;
for the individuals in a nanative; the latter a discourse about words uttered (or
would result from world-creating and/or thoughts) equivalent to a discourse not
world-representing acts by these individu- about words. For example, should a given
als, such as forming beliefs, wishing, character have said at one point, "Well!
dreaming, predicting, or imagining (Ryan). that's settled then! I'll meet you at the sta-
*!Dolezel 1976; Ryan 1985. See also AU- tion!," narratized discourse might render it
THENTICATION FUNCTION, as "She made an appointment to meet
narrativics. NARRATOLOGY. f T h e term, intro- h e r . " HAIong w i t h REPORTED DISCOURSE

duced by Ihwe, has not gained much (DIRECT DISCOURSE) a n d TRANSPOSED DIS-

cunency. Some students of narrative at COURSE (INDIRECT DISCOURSE), n a r r a t i z e d

times make a distinction between narrativ- (or narrated or narrativized) discourse is, in
Genette's view, one of the three basic GRAMMAR, PAROLE.

ways of representing characters' utter- narrator. The one who narrates, as in-
ances and verbal thoughts. HGenette 1980, scribed in the text. There is at least one
1983. narrator per narrative, located at the same
n a r r a t i z e d s p e e c h , NARRATIZED DISCOURSE, DIEGETIC LEVEL a s t h e NARRATEE h e or s h e

especially narratized discourse whereby a is addressing. In a given narrative, there


character's utterances (as opposed lo may, of course, be several different narra-
thoughts) are represented, f Genette 1980, tors, each addressing in turn a different
1983. narratee or the same one. HA narrator may
narratology. 1. The (structuralist-inspired) be more or less overt, knowledgeable,
theory of NARRATIVE. Narratology studies ubiquitous, self-conscious, and reliable,
the nature, form, and functioning of narra- and s/he may be situated at a greater or
tive (regardless of medium of represen- lesser DISTANCE from the situations and
tation) and tries to characterize NARRATIVE events narrated, the characters, and/or the
COMPETENCE. More particularly, it examines narratee. This distance can be temporal (I
what all and only narratives have in com- narrate events that occurred three hours or
m o n (at t h e l e v e l o f STORY, NARRATING, and three years ago), discursive (I narrate in
their relations) as well as what enables them my own words what a character said, or I
to be different from one another, and it at- use his or her own words), intellectual (I
tempts to account for the ability to produce am intellectually superior to my narratee, or
and understand them. HThe term was pro- equal or inferior to him or her), moral (I am
posed by Todorov. 2. The study of narra- more or less virtuous than the characters),
tive as a verbal mode of representation of and so on. ^Whether or not s/he is overt,
temporally ordered situations and events knowledgeable, self-conscious, or reliable,
(Genette). In this restricted sense, narratol- the n a r r a t o r m a y b e EXTRADIEGETIC, or IN-

ogy disregards the level of story in itself (it TRADIEGETIC. Furthermore, the narrator
does not attempt to formulate a grammar of may be heterodiegetic or homodiegetic
stories or plots, for instance) and focuses and, in the latter case, function as a PRO-
on the possible relations between story and TAGONIST in the events recounted (Great
narrative text, NARRATING and narrative text, Expectations, Jouney to the End of the
and story and narrating. Specifically, it in- Night, Kiss Me Deadly), an important char-
vestigates problems of TENSE, MOOD, and acter (All the King's Men, The Great
VOICE. 3. The study of given (sets of) nar- Gatsby), a minor one (A Study in Scarlet),
ratives in terms of models elaborated by so- or even a mere observer ("A Rose for Em-
called NARRATIVICS (Genot). This accepta- ily"). HThe narrator, who is immanent to the
tion of the term is rare. UBal 1977,1985; narrative, must be distinguished from the
Genette 1983; Genot 1979; Mathieu-Colas real or concrete AUTHOR, who is not:
1936; Pavel 1985; Prince 1981-82,1982; Nausea, "Intimacy," "The Wall," and
T o d o r o v 1 9 6 9 . S e e also LANGUE, NARRATIVE "Erostratus" have the same author—
Sartre—but different narrators. tfT'ne narra- nothing is known beyond the fact of his or
tor must also be distinguished from the her existence (The Brothers Karamazov).
IMPLIED AUTHOR: the latter does not recount ^Ducrot and Todorov 1979. See also NAR-
situations and events (but is taken to be RATOR-AGENT.

accountable for their selection, distribution, narreme. In Dorfman's terminology, a CARDI-


and combination); moreover, s/he is in- NAL FUNCTION, a KERNEL, a NUCLEUS.

ferred from the entire text rather than UDorfman 1969; Wittmann 1975.
inscribed in it as a teller. Though the dis- naturalization. The network of devices
tinction can be problematic (for example, in through which the receiver of a narrative
the case of an ABSENT or maximally relates it to an already known model of
COVERT NARRATOR: "Hills Like White Ele- reality and thus reduces its strangeness.
phants"), it is sometimes very clear (for W h e r e a s MOTIVATION is author-oriented,
example, in the cases of such HOMODI- naturalization is reader- or receiver-ori-
EGETIC NARRATIVES as Great Expectations ented. ^Chatman 1978; Culler 1975;

or "Haircut"). HBal 1981a; Booth 1983; Rimmon-Kenan 1983. See also VERISIMILI-
i
TUDE.
Chatman 1978; Ducrot and Todorov 1979;
natural narrative. A narrative occurring
Friedemann 1910; Genette 1980, 1983;
spontaneously in "normal," everyday con-
Kayser 1958; Prince 1982; Scholes and
versation. The term is supposed to
Kellogg 1966; Suleiman 1980; Tacca 1973.
distinguish narratives produced without de-
S e e a l s o GNARUS, HETERODIEGETIC NARRA-
liberation ("naturally") from narratives that
TOR, HOMODIEGETIC NARRATOR, OVERT
have a "constructed" character and appear
NARRATOR, PERSON, RELIABLE NARRATOR,
in specific story-telling contexts. W a n Dijk
SELF-CONSCIOUS NARRATOR, VOICE.
1974-75; Prait 1977.
narrator-agent. A NARRATOR who is a char-
nesting. See EMBEDDING. HBarthes 1974.
acter in the situations and events
neutral narrative type. A type of narrative
recounted and has some measurable effect
c h a r a c t e r i z e d b y EXTERNAL FOCALIZATION.
in them: Gil Bias, in the novel by the same
Along with the AUCTORIAL and ACTORIAL
name, is a narrator-agent. HDucrot and To-
NARRATIVE TYPES, the neutral narrative type
dorov 1979. See also HOMODIEGETIC
("The Killers") is one of three basic classes
NARRATOR, NARRATOR-WITNESS.
in Lintvelt's typology. UGenette 1983; Lint-
n a r r a t o r - l . T h e " I " of a HOMODIEGETIC NARRA-
velt 1S81. See also BEHAVIORIST NARRA-
TOR in his or her role as NARRATOR and not
TIVE, 0RAMATIC MODE, POINT OF VIEW.
as CHARACTER. In "I drank a glass of
beer," the "I" who tells about the drinking neutral omniscience. One of eight possible

is the narrator-l, whereas the "I" who drank POINTS OF VIEW according to Friedman's

is the CHARACTER-I. UPrince 1982. See also classification: neutral omniscience charac-

FIRST-PERSON NARRATIVE terizes the heterodiegetic and omniscient

n a r r a t o r - w i t n e s s . In a HOMODIEGETIC NARRA- b u t n o n i n t r u s i v e , IMPERSONAL NARRATOR

TIVE, a NARRATOR of whom practically (Lord of the Flies). I N . Friedman 1955b.


S e e also EDITORIAL OMNISCIENCE. Rossum-Guyon 1970. See also DRAMATIC
n o n f o c a i i z a t i o n . S e e ZERO FOCALIZATION. MODE, SUBJECTIVE NARRATIVE.

nonfocalized narrative. A narrative having omnipresent narrator. A ubiquitous narra-


ZERO FOCALIZATION (Vanity Fair, Adam tor; a narrator with the capacity to be in
Bede). r B a l 1977, 1981a; Genette 1980, two or more different spaces at the same
1983; Rimmon-Kenan 1983; Vitoux 1982. time or to move freely back and forth be-
nonnarrated narrative. A narrative with an tween scenes occurring in different places.
ABSENT NARRATOR; a narrative presenting HOmnipresent narrators are typical of histo-
situations and events with a minimum riography and are not necessarily omni-
amount of narratorial mediation ("Hills Like scient. Conversely, OMNISCIENT NARRA-
White Elephants"). HChatman 1978. See TORS are not necessarily omnipresent: the
a l s o MIMESIS, SHOWING. narrator of Mrs. Dalloway is, at times, o m -
n u c l e u s . A BOUND MOTIF; a CARDINAL FUNC- niscient but not omnipresent. liChgitman
TION; a KERNEL (noyau). As opposed to 1978.
CATALYSES, nuclei are logically essential to omniscient narrator. A narrator who knows
the narrative action and cannot be elimi- (practically) everything about the situations
nated without destroying its causal- and events recounted (Tom Jones, The Mill
chronological coherence. HBarthes 1975; on the Floss, Eugenie Grandet). Such a
Chatman 1978. n a r r a t o r h a s a n OMNISCIENT POINT OF V I E W

and tells more than any and all the charac-


ters know. 1 Booth 1983; Chatman 1978;
N. Friedman 1955b; Genette 1980; Prince
1982; Todorov 1981. See also ANALYTIC
AUTHOR, AUTHORIAL NARRATIVE SITUATION,

OMNIPRESENT NARRATOR, POINT OF VIEW.

object. An ACTANT or fundamental ROLE at omniscient point of view. The POINT OF


the level of deep narrative structure, in the VIEW a d o p t e d b y a n OMINISCIENT NARRA-

Greimassian model. The Object (analogous TOR; VISION from behind. Analogous to
to Propp's SOUGHT-FOR PERSON and Sour- ZERO FOCALIZATION, omniscient point of
iau's SUN) is looked for by the SUBJECT. view is characteristic of traditional or clas-
^Greimas 1970,1983a, 1983b; Greimas sical narrative (Adam Bede, Tom Jones,
and Court6s 1982; Henault 1983. See also Vanity Fair). "[Booth 1983; Chatman 1978;
ACTANTIAL MODEL. N. Friedman 1955b; Genette 1980; Prince
objective narrative. 1. A narrative charac- 1 9 8 2 . S e e a l s o LIMITED POINT OF VIEW.

terized by the narrator's attitude of opponent. 1. An ACTANT or fundamental


detachment toward the situations and ROLE at the level of deep structure, in Grei-
events recounted. 2. A BEHAVIORIST NAR- mas's early model of narrative. The
RATIVE. ^Brooks and Warren 1959; Hough Opponent (analogous to Propp's VILLAIN
1970; Magny 1972; Romberg 1962; van a n d FALSE HERO a n d t o S o u r i a u ' s MARS)
opposes the SUBJECT. 2. In Greimas's to certain questions, the orientation is that
more recent model of nanative, a negative constituent of it answering the questions
AuxiiiANT that is represented, at the sur- "Who?" "When?" "What?" and "Where?"
face level, by an ACTOR different from the <!Labov 1972; Pratt 1977.
one representing the SUBJECT. The Oppo- o s t r a n e n i y e . S e e DEFAMILIARIZATION.

nent, who comes into conflict with the overt narrator. A NARRATOR presenting situ-
Subject incidentally and/or represents a ations and events with more than a
momentary obstacle for it, should not be minimum of narratorial mediation; an IN-
confused with the ANTISUBJECT, who is a TRUSIVE NARRATOR (Eugenie Grander,

quester, like the Subject, and has aims that Barchester Towers, Tom Jones, Tristram
are at cross purposes with those of the Shandy). HChatman 1978. See also CO-
Subject. HGreimas 1983a, 1983b; Greimas VERT NARRATOR, DRAMATIZED NARRATOR,

and Courtis 1982; Renault 1983. See also MEDIATED NARRATION.

ACTANTIAL MODEL, ANTIDONOR.

order. The set of relations between the or-


der in which events (are said to) occur and
the order in which they are recounted.
^Events can be recounted in the order of
their occurrence: in "Joan ate, then she
w e n t o u t , " CHRONOLOGICAL ORDER is o b - pace. The regulating of SPEED; the propor-
served. On the other hand, there can be tioning of TEMPOS in a narrative. r Brooks
discordances between the two orders, as and Warren 1959. See also DURATION,
in "Joan went out after she ate"; ANACH- RHYTHM.

RONIES (RETROSPECTIONS Or ANTICIPATIONS, panorama. The rendering of situations and


ANALEPSES Or PROLEPSES, FLASHBACKS Or events from a distance, in nonscenic terms
FLASHFORWARDS) then obtain. In some ( a s o p p o s e d to DRAMA); SUMMARY. H u b -

cases an event may be deprived of any bock 1965; Souvage 1965. See also
temporal connection with other events (it PICTORIAL TREATMENT, PICTURE, SCALE,

may be dateless): the result is an ACH- SCENE.

RONY. In other cases, the unfolding of paradigm. A class of elements all of which
events may obey a nonchronological prin- can occupy the same position in a given
ciple rather than a chronological one: the context. Two units u and u' (say, man and
result is a SYLLEPSIS. ^Chatman 1978; Ge- boy) belong to the same paradigm if there
nette 1980; Prince 1S82. See also FABULA, exist two syntagmatic chains tuv and tu'v
PLOT, SJU2ET, STORY. (consider The man ate and The boy ate).
orientation. In Labov's terminology, the part liDucrot and Todorov 1979; Greimas and
of a NARRATIVE which identifies the (initial) Courtis 1982; Saussure 1966. See also
spatiotemporal situation in which the SYNTAGM.

events recounted took place. If a narrative pars'spsis. An ALTERATION that consists in


is taken to constitute a series of answers giving more information (not less, as in
PARALIPSIS) than should presumably be having some effect on them (as opposed
given in terms of the FOCALIZATION code to a PROP). ^Grimes 1975.
governing a narrative. Should EXTERNAL FO- patient. Along with the AGENT, one of two
CALIZATION be adopted, for instance, and fundamental ROLES in Bremond's typology.
should the thoughts of a character sud- Whereas patients are affected by certain
denly be reported, a paralepsis is said to processes (and, more specifically, may
obtain. <!Genette 1980. constitute victims or beneficiaries), agents
paralipsis. An ALTERATION that consists in initiate these processes and influence the
giving less information (not more, as in patients, modify their situation (improving
PARALEPSIS) than should presumably be or worsening it), or maintain it (for the good
given In terms of the FOCALIZATION code or the bad). r Bremond 1973; Scholes
governing a narrative; a lateral ELLIPSIS 1974.
whereby it is not ao intervening event that pattern. A significant arrangement of repeti-
goes unmentioned but rather one or more tions (in the narrated situations and
components in the situation that is being events). E. M. Forster characterized a
recounted. In The Murder of Roger Ack- number of PLOT patterns, such as the
royd, for example, the FOCALIZER is also "hour-glass" [Thais, The Ambassadors) or
the murderer, yet until the end, this fact— the "grand chain" (Roman Pictures).
which he knows perfectly well—is omitted HBrooks and Warren 1959; Forster 1927;
from his thoughts and thus concealed from Frye 1957; Souvage 1965.
the reader. HGenette 1980. pause. A canonical narrative TEMPO; along
parole. The individual utterance or speech with ELLIPSIS, SCENE, SUMMARY, a n d
act (as opposed to the LANGUE or language STRETCH, one of the fundamental narrative
system which it manifests and which SPEEDS. When some part of the narrative
makes it possible). TThe Saussurean op- text or some DISCOURSE TIME corresponds
position between langue (which constitutes to no elapsing of STORY TIME, pause ob-
the proper object of linguistics) and parole tains (and the narrative can be said to
is analogous to that between CODE and come to a stop). HA pause can be occa-
MESSAGE, SCHEMA a n d u s e , or c o m p e t e n c e sioned by a description or by a narrator's
and performance. It has had a profound in- commentarial excursuses. IChatman 1978;
fluence on the study of signifying systems Genette 1980; Prince 1982. See also COM-
and, more particularly, on NARRATOLOGY: MENTARY, OESCRIPTIVE PAUSE, DURATION.
the latter can be said to study the langue perceptual point of view. The physical per-
of narrative, the system of rules and norms ception through which a situation or event
accounting for the production and under- is apprehended. IChatman 1978. See also
standing of individual narratives (equivalent CONCEPTUAL POINT OF VIEW, POINT OF VIEW.
to parole). HGreimas and Courtes 1982; performance. In Greimassian terminology,
Saussure 1966. t h e NARRATIVE PROGRAM of a SUBJECT t h a t
participant. An ACTOR; an EXISTENT involved has acquired COMPETENCE. Performance
in the situations and events recounted and consists in the transformation of a given
state of affairs and, more specifically, cul- ^Austin 1962; Lyons 1977; Pratt 1977. See
minates in the CONJUNCTION of Subject and a l s o ILLOCUTIONARV ACT.
OBJECT. W a r n 1984, 1S85; Greimas p e r i p e t e i a . S e e PERIPETY.
1983a, 1983b; Greimas and Courtis 1976, peripety. The inversion (REVERSAL) from one
1 9 8 2 . S e e also DECISIVE TEST, NARRATIVE state of affairs to its opposite. For example,
SCHEMA. an action seems destined for success but
performative. An utterance that is used to suddenly moves toward failure, or vice
do rather than to say something, to per- versa. ^According to Aristotle, peripety
form an act by means of language rather (PERIPETEIA) is, a l o n g w i t h RECOGNITION
than to state that something is or is not the (ANAGNORISIS), the most potent means of
case: "I promise to come at five" and "I ensuring the tragic effect. HAristotle 1968.
bet you a dollar that it will rain tomorrow" perlocutionary act. An act performed by
are performatives, and by uttering them, means ot saying SQmething and describa-
the speaker actually makes a promise or a ble in terms of the effect which the
bet. More specifically, they are explicit per- iiLOCUTiONARY ACT performed in saying
formatives (performing the very illocu- that something has on the ADDRESSEE.
tionary act to which they refer) as opposed When I say to someone "I promise to be
to such implicit or primary performatives as there," I (may) accomplish, through my
"I'll be there at five" and "A dollar," which promising, the perlocutionary act of con-
do not contain a verb or expression nam- vincing him or her of my good faith. Along
ing the act but can be used to make a with a LOCUTIONARY ACT and an illocution-
promise or a bet. fThe theory of SPEECH ary act, a perlocutionary act is (possibly)
ACTS originates in Austin's distinction be- involved in the performance of a SPEECH
tween performatives and CONSTATIVES ACT. ILittle progress has been made in the
(utterances like "Napoleon won the battle study of perlocutionary acts, and they have
of Austeriitz" that report events or states of been increasingly absent from explorations
affairs in certain worlds and consequently in speech act theory. However, when NAR-
are "either true or false" in these worlds). RATIVES are viewed as speech acts, they
However, as Austin goes on to argue, con- are sometimes said to accomplish certain
statives themselves are performatives, perlocutionary acls (e.g., convincing, fright-
since saying (asserting, stating, reporting) ening, or entertaining their addressees).
that something is or is not the case consti- TAustin 1962; Lyons 1977; Pratt 1977.
tutes a kind of doing. Indeed, any ' person. The set of relations between the
utterance or set of utterances can be NARRATOR ( a n d NARRATEE) a n d t h e story

viewed as performative. Ilf narrative can narrated. HA distinction is commonly made


i
b e t w e e n FIRST-PERSON NARRATIVES (the
be said to "constate," to report that certain
narrator of which is a character in the situ-
situations and events are the case in cer-
ations and events recounted) and THIRD-
tain worlds, it can also be said to perform
PERSON NARRATIVES ( t h e n a r r a t o r of w h i c h
(at the very least) the act of reporting.
_55a _2se .m Jsm j m ^ a " * M l — ^

is not a character in the situations and phatic function. HJakcbson 1960; Mali-
events recounted). Another category is that nowski 1953; Prince 1982.
of t h e SECOND-PERSON NARRATIVE (the nar- pictoriai treatment. In Jamesian terminol-
rates of which is the main character in the o g y a n d a s o p p o s e d to DRAMATIC

situations and events recounted). f Bal TREATMENT, a nonscenic presentation of


1985; Cohn 1978; Genette 1980, 1983; some character's view of situations and
Prince 1982; Rimmon-Kenan 1983; Stan- events; PICTURE. 1IH. James 1972; Lub-
zel 1984; Tamir 1976. See also VOICE. bock 1965.
persona. In the criticism of narrative fiction, picture. A nonscenic rendering of some
a term that is used to refer to the IMPLIED character's consciousness of a situation. In
AUTHOR but that is also commonly used to The Ambassadors, Strether's first view o f .
refer to the NARRATOR. ilThe term was a Chad at the play constitutes a picture. I l n
Latin word designating the actor's mask in Jamesian terminology, picture is contrasted
classical theater. liBooth 1983; Holman with DRAMA (which renders scenically tfie
1972; Souvage 1965. characters' speech and behavior).
persona! narrative situation. See FIGURAL 1H. James 1972; Lubbock 1965.
NARRATIVE SITUATION. UStanze! 1964, 1971, plan. A global semantic framework repre-
1984. senting various aspects of reality pertinent
personale Erzahlsituatio.n. See FIGURAL to a planner or being advancing toward a
NARRATIVE SITUATION. UStanzel 1964, 1971, GOAL. Narrative frequently consists of sets
1984. of interacting plans. HPIans are often taken
p e r s p e c t i v e , FOCALIZATION; POINT OF VIEW. t o b e e q u i v a l e n t t o FRAMES, SCHEMATA, a n d

Along with DISTANCE, perspective is one of SCRIPTS, but certain suggestive distinctions
two main factors regulating narrative infor- have been proposed: a serially ordered,
mation. IGenette 1980, 1983; Rimmon temporally bound frame is a schema; a
1976. goal-directed schema is a plan; and a ster-
phatic function. One of the FUNCTIONS OF eotypical plan is a script. UBartlett 1932;
COMMUNICATION in terms of which any com- Beaugrande 1980; Bruce and Newman
municative (verbal) act may be structured 1976.
and oriented. When the communicative act plot. 1. The main incidents of a NARRATIVE;
is centered on the CONTACT (rather than the outline of situations and events
o n e of t h e o t h e r CONSTITUTIVE FACTORS OF (thought of as distinct from the CHARAC-
COMMUNICATION), it (mainly) has a phatic TERS i n v o l v e d in t h e m o r t h e THEMES

function. More specificaliy, those passages illustrated by them). HThese incidents can
in narrative focusing on the psychophysio- constitute a structure the main parts of
logical connection between narrator and which are cftaracterizable in iernis of FREY-
narratee ("Reader, are you still following TAG'S PYRAMID. 2 . T h e a r r a n g e m e n t of

me, or are you overwhelmed by the details i n c i d e n t s ; MYTHOS; SJU2ET; the situations

I'm providing?") can be said to fulfil! a and events as presented to the receiver.
liThe Russian Formalists made an influen- uation: The Brothers Karamazov), plots of
tial distinction between sjuzet and FABULA character (involving a change in the pro-
(or basic STORY material). 3. The global tagonist's moral character: The Portrait ol a
dynamic (goal-oriented and forward-mov- Lady), and plots of thought (involving a
ing) organization of narrative constituents change in the protagonist's thought and
which is responsible for the thematic inter- feeling: Mariusthe Epicurean). 'Friedman
est (indeed, the very intelligibility) of a proposes a more detailed classification, by
narrative and for its emotional effect. 4. A making further distinctions as to whether
narrative of events with an emphasis on the protagonist succeeds or fails, as to
causality, as opposed to story, which is a whether s/he is responsible and attractive
narrative of events with an emphasis on or not, and as to how this complex of fac-
chronology (Forster). "The king died, and tors is supposed to affect the receiver's
. then the queen died" is a story, whereas feelings: 1. Plots of Fortune: (a) the ac-
"The king died, and then the queen died of tion plot (organized around a problem and
grief" is a plot. KAristotle 1968; Brooks a solution and frequent in popular litera-
1984; Brooks and Warren 1959; Chatman ture: Treasure Island): (b) the pathetic plot
1978; Crane 1952; Egan 1978; Forster (an attractive but weak protagonist fails,
1927; N. Friedman 1955a, 1975; Frye and the unhappy ending inspires pity: Tess
1957; Martin 1986; O'Grady 1965; Pavel of the D'Urbervilles); (c) the tragic plot (an
1985; Ricoeur 1984; Scholes and Kellogg attractive protagonist is responsible for his
1966; Shklovsky 1965b; Tomashevsky or her misfortune, and catharsis is experi-
1 9 6 5 . S e e a l s o DOUBLE PLOT, PLOT TYPOL- enced: Oedipus Rex, King Lear); (d) the
OGY, SUBPLOT, punitive plot (an antipathetic though partly
plot typology. The systematic determination admirable protagonist fails: Richard III, The
of PLOT types according to structural or Treasure of the Sierra Madre); (e) the sen-
other similarities. For example, plots can timental plot (an attractive but weak or
be euphoric (fortunate: things change for passive protagonist succeeds in the end:
the better) or dysphoric (fatal: things Bleak House, Anna Christie); (f) the admi-
change for the worse), external (based on ration plot (an attractive and responsible
outer events and experiences) or internal protagonist succeeds and arouses respect
(based on inner feelings and transactions), and admiration: Tom Sawyer, Mister Rob-
simple (lacking PERIPETY and/or RECOGNI- erts]|; 2. Plots of Character: (a) the
TION) or complex, epic (episodic, loosely maturing plot (an attractive but naive pro-
woven) or dramatic (closely knit), and so tagonist acquires maturity: Portrait of the
on. ^lAmong the modern attempts to devise Mist as a Young Man, Great Expecta-
a typology of plots, Crane s and Fried- tions, The Portrait of a Lady); (b) the
man's are particularly noteworthy from the reform plot (an attractive protagonist is re-
point of view of NARRATOLOGY. Crane of- sponsible for his or her misfortunes but
fers a tripartite classification: plots of action changes for the better: The Scarlet Letter,
(involving a change in the protagonist's sit- The Pillars of the Community); (c) the test-
ing plot (a protagonist fails repeatedly and essential matter it is getting at (Labov).
renounces his or her ideals: The Sea Gull, The point of a narrative is indicated or sug-
Uncle Vanya)\ (d) the degeneration plot gested by a set of evaluative features that
(an attractive protagonist changes for the show why the situations and events nar-
worse after some important crisis: The Irn- rated are worth narrating: whereas a
moralist)\ 3. Plots of Thought: (a) the pointless narrative might be greeted with a
education plot (the thought of an attractive remark like "So what?," a pointed one
protagonist improves, but the effect of this would be greeted with an acknowledge-
improvement on his or her behavior is not ment of its REPORTABIUTY. c Labov 1972;
shown: Huckleberry Finn)-, (b) the revela- Polanyi 1979; Prince 1983. See also AB-
tion plot (the protagonist gets to know his STRACT.

or her own condition: Roald Dahl's "Be- point of view. The perceptual or conceptual
ware of the Dog"), (c) the affective plot position in terms of which the narrat.ed situ-
(the protagonist changes in attitudes and ations and events are presented;
feelings but not in philosophy: Pride and FOCALIZATION; PERSPECTIVE; VIEWPOINT.

Prejudice); (dj the disillusionment plot (the The point of view adopted may be that of
protagonist loses his or her ideals as well a n OMNISCIENT NARRATOR w h o s e position

as the receiver's sympathy and ends In de- varies and is sometimes unbeatable
spair or death: The Great Gatsby, The Sot- and who is (by and large) not subject to
Weed Factor). HAristotle 1968; Chatman perceptual or conceptual restrictions
1968; Crane 1952; Ducrot and Todorov (OMNISCIENT POINT OF VIEW: Vanity Fair,

1979; N. Friedman 1955a, 1975; Pavel Adam Bede). Or else, it may be situated in
1985; A. Wright 1982. the diegesis and, more specifically, in a
poetic function. One of the FUNCTIONS OF c h a r a c t e r (INTERNAL POINT OF VIEW: every-

COMMUNICATION in terms of which any com- thing is presented strictly in terms of the
municative (verbal) act may be structured knowledge, feelings, and perceptions of the
and oriented. When the communicative act same character or different ones). In this
is c e n t e r e d o n t h e MESSAGE for its o w n case, it may be fixed (the perspective of
sake (rather than on one of the other CON- one and only one character is adopted:
STITUTIVE FACTORS OF COMMUNICATION), it What Maisie Knew), variable (the perspec-
(mainly) has a poetic function. More specif- tive of several characters is adopted in turn
ically, those passages in narrative focusing to present different sequences of events:
on the message and underlining its tangi- The Golden Bowl, The Age of Reason), or
bility (drawing attention to its structure, its multiple (the same event or sequence of
shape, etc.: "Peter Piper picked a peck of events is narrated more than once, each
pickled peppers") can be said to fulfill a time in terms of a different perspective:
poetic function. HJakobson 1960; Prince The Moonstone, The Ring and the Bock).
1982. Finally, it may emanate from a focal point
situated in the diegesis but outside any of
point. The raison d'etre of a NARRATIVE, the
the characters (any thinking or feeling
reason for which it is recounted and the
being); it thereby excludes all information HGrimes, who speaks of viewpoint, also
on feelings and thoughts and is limited to distinguishes four basic categories: (1) om-
registering the characters' words and ac- niscient viewpoint (equivalent to Brooks
tions, their appearance, and the setting and Warren's omniscient author); (2) first-
against which they come to the fore (EX- person participant viewpoint (HOMODI-
TERNAL POINT OF VIEW: "Hills L i k e W h i t e EGETIC NARRATIVE with internal point of
Elephants"). ^According to this narrow def- view); (3) third-person subjective viewpoint
inition (inspired by Genette), point of view (HETERODIEGETIC NARRATIVE with i n t e r n a l

("who sees") should be distinguished from point of view); (4) third-person objective
VOICE ("who speaks"): it is not equivalent viewpoint (external point of view). UPouil-
to expression but institutes the perspective lon, who prefers to speak of VISION
governing expression. Yet, especially since (followed by Todorov, who spoke of AS-
the work of Lubbock on nanative tech- PECT), has a three-term classification:
nique, point of view has often been taken (1) vision from behind (similar to ZERO FO-
to involve not only a perceptual or concep- CALIZATION or omniscient point of view; the
tual apparatus but also factors like the narrator tells more than any and all of the
NARRATOR'S overtness, the types of treat- characters know: Tess of the D'Urber-
ment favored (DRAMA or PANORAMA), and vilies): (2) vision with (similar to internal
t h e TYPES OF DISCOURSE a d o p t e d . M o r e point of view; the narrator tells only what
generally, it has been taken to spring from one or several characters know: The Am-
the relations between narrator and NARRAT- bassadors, The Age of Reason); (3) vision
ING, narrator and NARRATEE, and narrator from without (similar to external point of
and NARRATED (Lanser). t Several typologi- view; the narrator tells less about certain
cal descriptions of nanative based on point situations than one or several characters
of view (in the broader rather than nar- know: "The Killers"). HFriedman proposes
rower sense) have been proposed. Thus, an eight-term classification, in order of
Brooks and Warren (who use the term FO- narratorial prominence: (1) EDITORIAL OM-
CUS OF NARRATION) offer a quadripartite NISCIENCE (heterodiegetic, omniscient, and
classification based on two distinctions: be- INTRUSIVE NARRATOR: Tess of the D'Urber-

t w e e n FIRST-PERSON a n d THIRD-PERSON vilies, War and Peace): (1) NEUTRAL


NARRATIVE and between internal depiction OMNISCIENCE (heterodiegetic and omniscient
and external observation of events: (1) first b u t n o n i n t r u s i v e , IMPERSONAL NARRATOR:

p e r s o n (AUTODIEGETIC NARRATIVE: Great Point Counterpoint, Lord of the Flies):


Expectations): (2) first-person observer ( 3 ) " I " AS WITNESS (the n a r r a t o r is a s e c -

(the narrator is a secondary character in ondary character in the situations and


the story recounted: The Great Gatsby): events presented, and the latter are viewed
(3) author-observer (external point of view: from the periphery rather than from the
"Hills Like White Elephants"); (4) omnis- center: The Good Soldier, The Great
cient author (Tess of the D'Urbervilies). Gatsby); (4) " t " AS PROTAGONIST ( t h e n a r -
rator is the protagonist in the action re- tween what he calls internal and external
counted, and the latter is viewed from the point of view (is the perceptual or concep-
center: Great Expectations, Huckleberry tual position inside or outside the diegesis?
Finn, The Catcher in the Rye): (5) MUL- does the information conveyed result from
TIPLE SELECTIVE OMNISCIENCE (HETERO- an inner view or an outer one?). UDolezel
DIEGETIC NARRATOR with variable internal arrives at a six-term categorization based
point of view: The Age of Reason, To the on a distinction between the ICH-FORM and
Lighthouse); (6) SELECTIVE OMNISCIENCE the ER-FORM (first-person and third-person
(heterodiegetic narrator with fixed internal narrative) and on a further distinction be-
point of view: The Ambassadors, A Portrait tween three narrative modes: objective (the
Of the Artist as a Young Man)] (7) the DRA- narrator views the situations and events
MATIC MODE (heterodiegetic narrator with from the periphery rather than from the
external point of view: The Awkward Age); center and does not evaluate or comment
(8) the CAMERA (the situations and events on them), rhetorical (the narrator views the
narrated presumably "just happen" before situations a n d events from the periphery
a neutral recorder and are transmitted by it but is intrusive), and subjective (situations
without ostensible organization or selec- and events are viewed from the center).
tion: Goodbye to Berlin). UStanzel ^Finally, Lintvelt proposes a five-term clas-
distinguishes between three main types of sification: (1) heterodiegetic AUCTORIAL
NARRATIVE SITUATIONS: t h e AUKTORIALE ER- NARRATIVE TYPE (the point of view is that of
ZAHLSITUATION (characterized by an the heterodiegetic narrator: Tess of the
omniscient narrator: Tess of the D'Urber- D'Urbervilles, Tom Jones)', (2) heterodi-
villes, Tom Jones)', the PERSONALE e g e t i c ACTORIAL NARRATIVE TYPE (the

ERZAHLS ITUATION (heterodiegetic narrator narrator is heterodiegetic but the point of


with internal point of view: The Ambassa- view is that of a character: The Ambas-
dors); and the ICH ERZAHLSITUATION (first- sadors)', (3) heterodiegetic NEUTRAL
person narrative: Great Expectations, Nau- NARRATIVE TYPE (similar to Friedman's dra-
sea). To these three categories (or ones matic mode: "The Killers," Moderato
that are essentially similar), Romberg adds Cantabile)', (4) homodiegetic auctorial
that of the narrator as behaviorist observer narrative type (the point of view is that
("Hills Like White Elephants"). HUspenskij of the homodiegetic narrator as narrator:
considers that point of view manifests itself Moby Dick); (5) homodiegetic actorial
on four different planes—ideological, phra- narrative type (the point of view is that
seological, spatiotemporal (narrator's of the homodiegetic narrator as charac-
spatial perspective on and temporal dis- ter: The Hunger). IBal 1977, 1983,
tance from narrated), and psychological 1985; Brooks and Warren 1959; Chat-
(narrator's psychological distance from or man 1978; Cohn 1981; Dole^el 1973; N.
affinity with narrated)—and he makes a Friedman 1955b; Fuger 1972; Genette
fundamental distinction on each plane be- 1980, 1983; Grimes 1975; H. James
1972; Lanser 1981; Leibfried 1972; Lint- ble rearrangement of the units constituting
velt 1981; Lubbock 1965; Pouillon 1946; LOGOS (the imitation of praxis). liAristotle
Prince 1982; Rimmon-Kenan 1983; Rom- 1968; Chatman 1978.
berg 1962; van Rossum-Guyon 1970; predicate. In a PREPOSITION or statement,
Schmid 1973; Stanzel 1964, 1971, 1984; that which asserts something about the
Todorov 1981; Uspenskij 1973; Weimann subject of the proposition or statement.
1973. TThere are static predicates ("Mary was
point-of-view character. See FOCALIZER. sad") and dynamic ones ("Mary ate a loaf
point-of-view narrative. A narrative with IN- of bread"). Furthermore, there are base
TERNAL POINT OF VIEW. " . P a s c a l 1 9 7 7 . predicates ("Mary walked three miles
polyphonic narrative. See DIALOGIC NARRA- every day") and transformed predicates
TIVE. HBakhtin 1981,1984. (resulting from the simple or complex
posterior narration. A NARRATION following TRANSFORMATION of a given predicate;
in time the narrated situations and events, "Jane thought that Mary walked three
a SUBSEQUENT NARRATING. P o s t e r i o r nar- miles every day"). HDucrot and Todorov
ration is characteristic of "classical" or "tra- 1979; Greimas and Courtes 1982; Todorov
ditional" narrative. IPnnce 1982. 1981.
post hoc ergo propter hoc fallacy. A con- predictive narrative. A narrative in which
fusion, denounced by scholasticism, t h e NARRATION p r e c e d e s t h e NARRATED in

between consecutiveness and conse- time; a narrative characterized by ANTE-


quence. According to Barthes (following RIOR NARRATION: "YOU will kill your father,
Aristotle), the mainspring of NARRATIVITY is and you will marry your mother." HGenette
related to an exploitation of this confusion, 1980; Todorov 1969.
what-comes-after-X in a nanative being primary narrative. A narrative the NARRAT-
processed as what-is-caused-by-X: given ING INSTANCE of which introduces one (or
"It started to rain, and Mary became nos- more than one) other narrating instance
talgic," for example, Mary's nostalgia tends and is not itself introduced by any: in
to be understood as caused by the weather Mancn Lescaut, for example, M. de Re-
conditions. ^Aristotle 1968; Barthes 1975. noncourt's narrative is primary, while Des
See also CAUSALITY. Grieux's is "secondary." Of course, a pri-
postulated reader. See IMPLIED READER. mary narrative is not necessarily more
HBooth 1983. important or interesting than the one(s) it
pratton. An AGENT. For Aristotle, the agent, introduces; indeed, the opposite is often
or pratton, can be endowed with ETHOS true (Manon Lescaut, Canterbury Tales).
(CHARACTER, the type traits characterizing HGenette 1980, 1983; Rimmon 1976. See
it) a n d DIANOIA (THOUGHT). l A r i s t o t l e 1 9 6 8 . a l s o DIEGETIC LEVEL, EMBEDDING, EXTRADI

See also CHARACTERIZATION. EGETIC, VOICE.

praxis. A real ACTION. For Aristotle, MYTHOS prior narrating. A NARRATING that precedes
(PLOT) consists in the selection and possi- the situations and events narrated, an AN-
TERIOR NARRATION. ^ G e n e t t e 1 9 8 0 . S e e g o u s to a MINIMAL STORY). A n n - s t a t e

also PREDICTIVE NARRATIVE. process (where r. > 2) constitutes a com-


privilege. A narrator's special right or ability. plex process. UGenot 1979.
The narrator may be more or less privi- p r o c e s s s t a t e m e n t . A NARRATIVE STATE-
leged in knowing what cannot be known by MENT in the mode of do or happen; a
strictly "natural" means: for example, an statement presenting an EVENT and, more
OMNISCIENT NARRATOR h a s c o m p l e t e privi- specifically, an ACT or a HAPPENING. Along
lege. TOooth 1983; Chatman 1978; Prince w i t h t h e STASIS STATEMENT, it is o n e o f t w o
1982. S e e a l s o AUTHORITY. kinds of statements with which the DIS-
proairetic code. The CODE, or "voice," ac- COURSE s t a t e s t h e STORY. H C h a t m a n
cording to which a narrative or part thereof 1978.
can be structured as a series of ACTION prolepsis. An ANACHRONY going forward to
sequences which themselves can be com- the future with respect to the "present"
bined into larger sequences, etc.; the code moment; an evocation of one or more
regulating the folding of actions into larger events that will occur after the "present"
actions or their unfolding into smaller ones; moment (or moment when the chronologi-
the code governing the construction of cal recounting of a sequence of events is
PLOT. 11A passage can signify in terms of interrupted to make room for the prolep-
the proairetic code if it introduces (integral sis); a n ANTICIPATION, a FLASHFORWARD, a
elements of) an initial situation to be modi- PROSPECTION: "John became furious. A
fied in the world of the NARRATED, or if it few days later, he would come to regret his
presents activities (which may combine into attitude, but now, he did not think of the
larger activities) modifying the (modified) consequences and he began to scream."
initial situation, or if it reports activities UProlepses have a certain EXTENT, or AM-
causing, resulting from, or pertinent to the PLITUDE (they cover a certain amount of
modifying activities, or if it recounts (inte- STORY TIME), a s w e l l a s a c e r t a i n REACH
gral elements of) a modified situation. (the story time they cover is at a certain
HBarthes 1974,1981a; Culler 1975; Prince temporal distance from the "present" mo-
1982. ment): in "Mary did not seem to notice it.
p r o a i r e t i s m . A unit of t h e PROAIRETIC CODE. Yet, the day after, she would think about it
fBarthes 1974. for several hours," the prolepsis has an ex-
problem. A situation making the fulfillment tent of several hours and a reach of one
of a GOAL (or SUBGOAL) u n c e r t a i n . T h e day. ^Completing prolepses fill in later
term is often used in artificial intelligence- gaps resulting from ELLIPSES in the narra-
inspired accounts of narrative structure. tive. Repeating prolepses, or ADVANCE
HBeaugrande 1980. NOTICES, recount ahead of time events that
process. The transformation from one state will be recounted again. HGenette 1980;
to another. A two-state process constitutes Rimmon 1976. See also ANALEPSIS, OR-
a simple or minimal process (and is analo- DER.
lysemous, triumphantly plural, perfectly most effective when closely allied to peri-
open. Narrative texts are reacierly if only pety. HAristotle 1968,
because they signify in terms of a logic of recounted discourse. See NARRATIZED DIS-
a c t i o n { t h e PROAIRETIC CODE a n d its v a r - COURSE. UTodorov 1981.
ious constraints). IBarthes 1974. reduced metadiegetic narrative. See PSEU-
reality effect. A seemingly functionless de- DODIEGETIC NARRATIVE. TOenette 1980.
tail presumably reported just "because it is referent. See CONTEXT. HJakobson 1960.
there (in the world of the NARRATED)," a referential code. The CODE, or "voice," in
detail presumably mentioned for no other terms of which a narrative or part thereof
reason than the fact that it is part of the refers to a given cultural background, to
reality represented. Reality effects (effets various stereotypic bodies of knowledge
de r6el) are exemplary connotators of the (physical, psychological, literary, artistic,
real (they signify "this is real"), and an philosophic, historic, medical, etc.) and cul-
abundance of them characterizes realistic tural objects. HAn important function of the
. narrative. HBarthes 1882. referential code is to activate models of
recall. A repeating ANALEPSIS; an analepsis what is vraisemblable (verisimilar, lifelike).
telling anew already mentioned past UBarthes 1974, 1981a. See also CULTURAL
events. HGenette 1980. See also RETURN. CODE, VERISIMILITUDE.
receiver. 1. An ACTANT or fundamental referential function. One of the FUNCTIONS
ROLE at the level of deep narrative struc- OF COMMUNICATION in terms of which any
ture, in the Greimassian model. The communicative (verbal) act may be struc-
Receiver (analogous to Souriau's EARTH) is tured and oriented; the REPRESENTATIVE
the one who (eventually) receives the OB- FUNCTION. When the communicative act is
JECT l o o k e d for by t h e SUBJECT. 2. A n c e n t e r e d o n t h e REFERENT o r CONTEXT
ADDRESSEE. A distinction is sometimes (rather than on one of the other CONSTITU-
made between the addressee and the TIVE FACTORS OF COMMUNICATION), it
mere receiver (who may not be the AD- (primarily) has a referential function: "John
DRESSER'S intended addressee). TCreimas is intelligent and handsome." More specifi-
1970, 1983b; Greimas and Courtes 1982; cally, those passages in a narrative mainly
H ^ n a u l t 1 9 8 3 . See a l s o ACTANTIAL MOOEL. focusing on (this or that feature of) the sit-
recognition. In Aristotelian terminology, a uations and events narrated can be said to
change from ignorance to knowledge expe- fulfill a referential function. HJakobson
rienced by a PROTAGONIST, brought about 1960; Prince 1982.
by the events in the PLOT and resulting in a reflector. In Jamesian terminology, the FO-
turning of the action. ^According to Aristo- CAUZER, t h e FOCUS OF NARRATION, t h e
tle, r e c o g n i t i o n (DISCOVERY, ANAGNORISIS) holder of POINT OF VIEW, t h e CENTRAL CON-
is, a l o n g w i t h PERIPETY (PERIPETEIA, RE- SCIOUSNESS o r CENTRAL INTELLIGENCE.
VERSAL), the most potent means of HH. James 1972. See also FOCALIZATION.
securing the tragic effect Furthermore, it is reliable narrator. A NARRATOR behaving in
a c c o r d a n c e w i t h t h e IMPLIED AUTHOR'S character's utterances (as opposed to
norms. Mike Hammer, in I, The Jury, is a thoughts) are represented. HGenette 1980,
reliable narrator. HBooth 1983. See also 1983. S e e a l s o DIALOGUE, DIRECT SPEECH.

UNRELIABLE NARRATOR. representation, SHOWING, in Todorov's ter-


repeating narrative. A narrative or part minology: representation is to NARRATION
thereof with a FREQUENCY such that what as showing is to TELLING. fTodorov 1966.
happens once is recounted n times (with or representative function. The REFERENTIAL
without stylistic variations): "At two o'clock FUNCTION. I K . Buhler 1934. See also CON-
Wary saw Nancy! At two o'clock Mary saw STITUTIVE FACTORS OF COMMUNICATION,

Nancy! She felt really good." HGenette FUNCTIONS OF COMMUNICATION.

1980. represented perception. A TYPE OF DIS-


reportabitlty. The quality that makes situ- COURSE whereby the narrator, instead of
ations and events reportable, worthy of presenting the external worid, presents a
being told. Situations and events that are character's perceptions of it, presumably
(shown to be) extraordinary, wonderful, as they occur in his or her consciousness
bizarre (as opposed to ordinary, common- and without suggesting that the character
place, humdrum), are reportable. *!A has verbalized them. Whereas "—Here
reportable assertion can be said to have comes Mary, said John" would constitute a
the force of an exclamatory one, and nar- speech about perception rather than a
rators usually underline the reportability transcription of perception and whereas
(TELLABILITY) of their assertions through "John saw Mary coming toward him" would
evaluative devices. HLabov 1972; Pratt constitute a report about perception, "John
1 9 7 7 . S e e a l s o EVALUATION, NARRATABIE. "just stood there. Mary was coming toward
r e p o r t e d d i s c o u r s e , DIRECT DISCOURSE. him" provides an example of represented
A l o n g w i t h TRANSPOSED DISCOURSE (INDI- perception (substitutionary perception, style
RECT DISCOURSE) a n d NARRATIZED indirect libre de perception, erlebte Wahr-
DISCOURSE, reported discourse is, in Ge- nehmung, erlebte Eindruck). liBanfield
nette's view, one of the three basic ways of 1982; Brinton 1980; W. Buhler 1937; Fehr
representing characters' utterances and 1 9 3 8 ; L i p s 1 9 2 6 . S e e a l s o FREE INDIRECT

verbal thoughts. ^Reported discourse is DISCOURSE.

formally distinguished from IMMEDIATE DIS- represented speech and thought. See
COURSE (FREE DIRECT DISCOURSE) by t h e FREE INDIRECT DISCOURSE. HBanfield 1 9 8 2 .
presence of a TAG CLAUSE (or some other resolution. 1, In Aristotelian terminology,
form of narratorial mediation) introducing that part of the plot which goes from the
the characters' words or thoughts. ^Ge- beginning of the change in fortune to the
nette 1980,1983. See also TYPES OF END. In that sense, resolution (lusis) should
DISCOURSE. not be confused with DENOUEMENT. 2.
r e p o r t e d s p e e c h , REPORTED DISCOURSE, es- See RESULT. HAristotle 1968; Labov 1972.
pecially reported discourse whereby a resolved content. The thematic situation re-
suiting from the TRANSFORMATION of its ELLIPSIS in the narrative. HGenette 1930.
contrary (or contradictory) and marking the S e e a l s o RECALL.

completion of a narrative SEQUENCE. 1iNar- r e v e r s a l . S e e PERIPETY.

rative can be viewed as correlating a rewrite ruie. A rule of the form X->Y (to be
temporal opposition (before / after, initial read "Rewrite X as Y" or "X consists of
situation / final situation) and a thematic Y") and allowing for the replacement of a
o n e (INVERTED CONTENT / r e s o l v e d con- given element in a string by one or several
tent). HChabrol 1973; Greimas 1970; other elements. For example, the fact that
Rastier 1973. a sentence consists of a noun phrase and
restricted clause. A clause the DISPLACE- a verb phrase would be captured by a rule
MENT SET of which is greater than that of a such as Sentence—>Noun Phrase + Verb
NARRATIVE CLAUSE but smaller Shan that of Phrase (the symbol + indicates the combi-
a FREE CLAUSE. A restricted clause can be nation of elements in sequential order);
displaced over a large part of the narrative similarly, the fact that a MINIMAL STORY
i
consists of one event following a state of
without any resulting change in the seman-
affairs obtaining at time t 0 and preceding
tic interpretation, but it cannot be displaced
another state of affairs obtaining at time f,
over the entire narrative: in "it was five
would be captured by a rule such as Mini-
o'clock. The birds started to sing. At ten
mal Story-»State at t0 + Event + State at
past five, Mary got up," "It was live
f,. HRewrite rules were imported into NAR-
o'clock" is a restricted clause. ULabov
RATOLOGY from generative grammar, and
1972; Labov and Waletzky 1967. See also
they play an important part in STORY GRAM-
COORDINATE CLAUSES.
MARS. UChomsky 1957, 1962; Pavel 1985;
restriction of field. The subjecting of POINT
Pnnce 1973, 1982; Thorndyke 1977. See
OF VIEW to conceptual or perceptual con-
a l s o NARRATIVE GRAMMAR, TRANSFORMA-
straints. HBlin 1954. See also
TIONAL RULE.
FOCALIZATION, LIMITED POINT OF VIEW.
rhythm. A recurrent pattern in narrative
result. In Labov's terminology, the outcome
SPEED and, more generally, any pattern of
of the events constituting the COMPLICATING
repetition with variations. The most com-
ACTION; t h e ENO. If a NARRATIVE is t a k e n t o
mon rhythm in classical narrative results
constitute a series of answers to certain
from the regular alternation of SCENE and
questions, the result, or RESOLUTION, is
SUMMARY. TOrown 1950; Genette 1980;
that constituent of it answering the ques-
T. Wright 1985.
tion "What finally happened?" 1i!n a, "fully
rising action. Along with the FALLING ACTION
developed" narrative, the resolution1 is fol-
1 and the CLIMAX, one of the fundamental
lowed by a CODA. ULabov 1972.
constituents of a (dramatic or closely knit)
r e t r o s p e c t i o n . An ANALEPSIS, a FLASHBACK,
PLOT structure. The rising action proceeds
a CUTBACK, a SWITCHBACK. T T o d o r o v 1 9 8 1 .
from the EXPOSITION and culminates in the
S e e a l s o ANACHRONY, ORDER.
climax. HFreytag 1894. See also FREYTAG'S
return. A completing ANALEPSIS; an analep-
PYRAMID.
sis filling in a gap resulting from an earlier
role. A typical set of FUNCTIONS peiiormable conversely, one actor or character can ful-
by and ATTRIBUTES attachable to an entity. fill several different roles. HBremond 1973;
IFThere have been several typologies of Ducrot and Todorov 1979, Greimas 1970,
roles proposed, of which some have 1983a, 1983b; Greimas and Courtes 1982;
proven particularly influential in NARRATOL- Propp 1968; Scholes 1974; Souriau 1950.
OGY. Thus, Propp isolated seven DRAMATIS S e e also STOCK CHARACTER, TYPE.

PERSONAE, or basic functional roles, in his round character. A complex, multidimen-


account of the structure of the fairy tale, sional, unpredictable CHARACTER, who is
each corresponding to a certain SPHERE OF capable of convincingly surprising behav-
ACTION: t h e VILLAIN, t h e DONOR (provider), ior. Charlus in Remembrance of Things
t h e HELPER, t h e p r i n c e s s ( a SOUGHT-FOR Past is a round character. UForster 1927.
PERSON) a n d h e r f a t h e r , t h e DISPATCHER, S e e a l s o FLAT CHARACTER.

t h e HERO, a n d t h e FALSE HERO. ^ S o u r i a u

distinguished six basic roles, or dramatic


functions: the LION (the Oriented Thematic
F o r c e , t h e SUBJECT, t h e HERO), the SUN

(the Representative of the desired OBJECT,


of the orienting value), the EARTH (the po-
tential Obtainer of this Object, the one for sanction. In the Greimassian account of c a -
whose benefit the Oriented Thematic Force nonical narrative structure, that part of the
is u l t i m a t e l y w o r k i n g , t h e RECEIVER), MARS action whereby the SUBJECT who has ful-
( t h e OPPONENT), t h e BALANCE (the arbiter or filled (failed to fulfill) a CONTRACT is (justly)
rewarder, the attributor of the good, the im- rewarded or (unjustly) punished by the
parter of values, the SENDER), and the SENDER. HAdam 1984,1985; Greimas
MOON (the rescuer, the helper). <iGreimas 1970,1983a; Greimas and Courtes 1982.
d e v i s e d a n ACTANTIAL MODEL r e p r e s e n t i n g S e e a l s o GLORIFYING TEST, NARRATIVE
the structure of relationships obtaining SCHEMA.
among ACTANTS, or fundamental roles, at s a t e l l i t e . A FREE MOTIF; a CATALYSIS; a mi-
the level of deep narrative structure. The nor plot event. As opposed to KERNELS,
original model involved six actants: Sub- satellites are not logically essential to the
ject, Object, Sender, Receiver, Helper, and narrative action, and their elimination does
Opponent. In a more recent version of the not destroy its causal-chronological coher-
model, the Helper and the Opponent no ence: rather than constituting crucial nodes
longer constitute actants. HBremond devel- in the action, they fill in the narrative space
oped an intricate typology by making a between these nodes. HBarthes 1975;
fundamental distinction between PATIENTS Chatman 1978. See also FUNCTION.
(victims or beneficiaries) and AGENTS (in- scale. The relative amount of detail used to
fluences, modifiers, and maintainers). represent a particular set of situations and
HOne role can be fulfilled by several differ- events; the length of a narrative (or part
e n t ACTORS or CHARACTERS, a n d
thereof) relative to the situations and
events recounted. "Brooks and Warren elements of which are viewed as instruc-
1 9 5 9 . See also DURATION, SPEED. tions about the proper fulfillment of certain
scene. A canonical narrative TEMPO; along roles (Schsnk and Abelson). A "restaurant"
w i t h ELLIPSIS, PAUSE, STRETCH, a n d SUM- script, for example, contains instructions for
MARY, one of the fundamental narrative the customer, the waiter, the cashier, and
SPEEDS. When there is some sort of equiv- so on. fThough scripts are frequently con-
alence between a narrative segment and sidered equivalent to FRAMES, PLANS, and
t h e NARRATED it represents (as in DIA- SCHEMATA, they are more properly de-
LOGUE, for instance), when the DISCOURSE scribed as stereotypical, goal-directed
TIME is ( c o n s i d e r e d ) equal to t h e STORY schemata. liBeaugrande 1980; Schankand
TIME, scene obtains. TThe conventional Abelson 1977.
equivalence between narrative segment second-degree narrative. See METADI-
and narrated is usually marked (in English) EGETIC NARRATIVE. H G e n e t t e 1 9 8 0 , 1983.

by the (relative) absence of narratorial me- second-person narrative. A narrative the


diation, the emphasis on moment-by- NARRATEE o f w h i c h is t h e PROTAGONIST in

moment action, the careful detailing of spe- the story s/he is tola. Butor's A Change of
cific events, the use of the preterit rather Heart is a second-person narrative. fGe-
than the imperfect, the preference for nette 1983; Morrissette 1965; Prince 1982.
point-action verbs rather than durational S e e also PERSON.

ones, etc. llScene (DRAMA) is traditionally selective omniscience. One of eight possi-
contrasted with summary (PANORAMA). b l e POINTS OF VIEW a c c o r d i n g to

IChatman 1978; Genette 1980; Prince Friedman's classification: selective omnis-


1 9 8 2 . S e e a l s o DURATION, RHYTHM. cience characterizes the HETERCDIEGETIC
schema. A global semantic framework rep- NARRATOR a d o p t i n g FIXED INTERNAL FOCAU-

resenting various aspects of reality and ZATION (A Portrait of the Artist as a Young
guiding perception and comprehension of Man). UN. Friedman 1955b. See also MUL-
these (or related) aspects (Bartlett). HSche- TIPLE SELECTIVE OMNISCIENCE.

mata are often taken to be equivalent to seif-conscious narrator. A NARRATOR who


FRAMES, PLANS, a n d SCRIPTS, b u t c e r t a i n is aware that s/he is narrating; a narrator
suggestive distinctions have been pro- who discusses and comments on his or
posed: a schema is a serially ordered, her narrating chores. Jacques Revel in
temporally bound frame (a "house" Passing Time and Holden Caulfield in The
schema, for instance, would represent the Catcher in the Rye are self-conscious nar-
order in which houses are built or else the rators; Meursault in The Stranger and the
order in which people visit or inspect narrator in "Haircut" are not. HBooth 1983.
them); a plan in a goal-directed schema; self-narrated monologue. A NARRATED MON-
and a script is a stereotypical plan. HBart- OLOGUE in a FIRST-PERSON NARRATIVE.

lett 1932; Beaugrande 1980. H C o h n 1S78. S e e a l s o FREE INDIRECT DIS-

script. A representation of knowledge the COURSE. .


self-narration. A PSYCHONARRATION in a without a mate during the breeding time,"
FIRST-PERSON NARRATIVE. I C o h n 1S78. etc. HGreintas 1983b; Greimas and
seif-quoted monologue. A OUOTED MONO- Courtes 1S82; Pottier 1964; Rastier 1973.
LOGUE i n a FIRST-PERSON NARRATIVE. I C o h n semic code. The CODE, or "voice," in terms
1978. of which a narrative or part thereof allows
self-reflexive narrative. A narrative taking for the construction of CHARACTERS (and
itself and-'or those narrative elements by SETTINGS). "iBarthes 1974,1981a. See also
which it is constituted and communicated SEME.

(NARRATOR, NARRATEE, NARRATION, etc.) a s semioiic square. The visual representation


a subject of reflection. Tristram Shandy of the logical articulation of any semantic
and Passing Time are self-reflexive narra- category or, in other words, the visual rep-
tives; I, the Jury and Germinal are not. r e s e n t a t i o n of the CONSTITUTIVE MODEL

^Chambers 1984; Dallenbach 1977; describing the elementary structure of sig-


Hutcheon 1984. nification. In the Greimassian model, given
seme. 1. An elementary semantic feature a unit of sense s, (e.g., rich), it signifies in
(Greimas); a minimal unit of meaning. The terms of relations with its contradictory s7
meaning of the word colt, for example, is (not rich), its contrary s 2 (poor), and the
the product of such semes as 'equine', contradictory of s2 (sj, not poor):
'young', 'male', etc. 2. A unit of the SEMIC
CODE (Barthes); a connotative SIGNIFIED;
an element connoting a certain character
(or setting) trait. Given a male character
who has long eyelashes and a soft voice
and who bites and scratches when he where
fights, the length of the eyelashes, the soft- < >: relation of contradiction
ness of the voice, and the biting and < - - > : relation of contrariety
scratching can be said to function as >: relation of complementarity
semes of femininity. IBarthes 1974; Chat- (s; and s i imply s, and s 2 respectively).
man 1978; Culler 1975; Greimas 1983b; ^According t o Greimas, the (semantic)
Greimas and Courtes 1982; Rastier 1973. course of a narrative can be said to corre-
sememe. 1. The set of SEMES recognizable spond to a movement along the semiotic
in a given word or morpheme (Pottier). square: the narrative deploys itself in terms
2. A particular acceptation of a given word of operations (transformations) leading
(Greimas). The word bachelor, for in- from a given unit to its contrary (or contra-
stance, brings together several sememes, dictory). For instance, the course of "John
since it means "young knight," "one who was lull of life. One day, he became very
has received the first academic degree sick and fell into such a deep coma that
conferred by a college or university," "a he was thought to be dead. But something
man who has not married," "a male animal in him refused to die, and he was miracu-
» . Far. zm sm' M3S sm ms a

lously restored to normal life" could bo rep- sequence, A component unit of NARRATIVE
resented by the following diagram (to be that is itself capable of functioning as a
read following the direction of the ar- narrative; a series of situations and events
rows and starting from A): of which the last one in time constitutes a
(A) life death partial repetition or TRANSFORM of the first
one. In "Jane was happy, and Susan was
unhappy; then Susan met Flora, and she
became happy; then Jane met Peter, and
nondeath ; nonlife she became unhappy," "Susan was un-
Similarly, the course of (Perrault's) "Cin- happy; then Susan met Flora, and she
derella," in which (1) the heroine finds became happy" constitutes a sequence
herself in a state that is not rightly hers and so does "Jane was happy; then Jane
(she has lost her favorable position after met Peter, and she became unhappy." The
her widowed father's remarriage); (2) with combination of sequences through LINKING,
the help of a fairy, she appears as a beau- EMBEDDING, a n d ALTERNATION y i e l d s ever

tiful lady at a ball; (3) on the stroke of m o r e COMPLEX STORIES. HAn e l e m e n t a r y

midnight, she no longer has that appear- sequence—or (Bremorid) TRIAD—is made
ance; and (4) she is recognized by the up of three terms or FUNCTIONS corre-
prince for the worthy person she rea/ly is, sponding to the three stages in any
could be represented by the following dia- process: virtuality (situation opening a pos-
gram (starting from A): sibility), actualization of possibility, and
result. IBarthes 1975; Bremond 1973;
appearing ; :t being
Ducrot and Todorov 1979; Greimas 1970,
1971, 1S83b; Greimas and Courtes 1982;
Prince 1982; Todorov 1981. See also
(A) not being !•' • N i not appearing
PROPOSITION, TRANSFORMATION.

HAdam 1984,1985; Bremond 1973; Grei- set description. A DESCRIPTION that is not
mas 1970,1983b; Greimas and Courtes developed in terms of a character's POINT
1982. OF VIEW or actions. HChatman 1978.
sender. 1. A n ACTANT o r f u n d a m e n t a l ROLE salting. The spatiotemporal circumstances
at the level of deep narrative structure, in in which the events of a narrative occur.
the Greimassian model. The Sender (anal- USeiting may be textually prominent or
ogous to Souriau's BALANCE arid Propp's negligible, consistent (when its features are
DISPATCHER) is the imparter of values and not contradictory) or inconsistent, vague or
sends the SUBJECT on its quest for the OB- precise, presented objectively or subjec-
JECT. 2. A n ADDRESSER. H G r e i m a s 1 9 7 0 , tively, presented in an orderly fashion (the
1983a, 1983b; Greimas and Courtes 1982; facade of a house is described from left to
H & i a u i t 1983. S e e also ACTANTIAL MODEL, right, a door is depicted from top to bottom,
ANTISENDER. a castle is shown from the inside to the
ESKS BEim !HragH" IOTHE*"

outside, or vice versa) or in a disorderly signified. The conceptual dimension of a


one, and so on. Furthermore, it can be SIGM. A signified (signifiij is linked with a
utilitarian (every part of it has a function in SIGNIFIER (signifiant) and does not exist
the action), symbolic (of a conflict to outside of its relation with it. liSaussure
come, of a character's feelings), "irrele- 1966.
vant" ("realistic": it is presented simply signifler. The perceptual dimension of a
because it is there, as it were), and so SIGN. A signifier (signifiant) is linked with a
forth. Finally, its features may be intro- SIGNIFIED (sigriifie) and does not exist as
duced contiguously (a DESCRIPTION can such outside of its relation with it. liSaus-
then be said to obtain) or scattered one by sure 1966.
one through the narrative. HCnatman 1978; simultaneism. The concurrent rendering,
Grimes 1975; Hamon 1981, 1982; Liddell through intercutting and INTERWEAVING, of
1947; Prince 1982. See also EXISTENT, two or more sets of situations and events
REALITY EFFECT, SPACE. occurring simultaneously (The Reprieve,
shifter. A term or expression whcse referent Manhattan Transfer, the l/.S.A. trilogy).
is determinable only with respect to the sit- HBeach 1932; Magny 1972.
uation (ADDRESSER, ADDRESSEE, time, simultaneous narrating. A NARRATING that
place) of its utterance (Jakobson): "I" and is simultaneous with the situations and
"Dad" are shifters. HBenveniste 1971; Duc- e v e n t s n a r r a t e d ; a SIMULTANEOUS NARRA-

rot and Todorov 1979; Jakobson 1971. TION. liGenette 1880.


showing. Along with TELLING, one o? two simultaneous narration. A NARRATION con-
fundamental kinds of DISTANCE regulating temporaneous with the situations and
narrative information; MIMESIS. HAs op- e v e n t s n a r r a t e d ; a SIMULTANEOUS NARRAT-

posed to telling or DIEGESIS (di6g£sis), ING (The Unnameabla). HPrince i 982.


showing is a MODE characterized by the singulative rias-rstivs. A narrative or part
detailed, scenic rendering of situations and thereof with a FREQUENCY such that what
events and by minimal narratorial media- happened once is recounted once (or what
tion: DIALOGUE constitutes a good example happened n times is recounted n times):
of showing. HChatman 1978; Genette "At fen past six, Mary got up and left."
1380,1983; H. James 1972; Lubbock IGenette 1330.
1 9 6 5 . S e e a l s o SCENE. s i n g u l a r n a r r a t i v e . S e e SINGULATIVE NARRA-

sign. 1. in Saussurean terminology, a so- TIVE. IGenetts 1980.


cially constituted entity linking a perceptible sjuzet In Russian formalist terminology, the
image (or SIGNIFIER) and a concept (or SIG- set of narrated situations and events in the
NIFIED) neither of which exists outside of its order of their presentation to the receiver
relation with the other. The sign dog, for in- (as opposed to FABULA); the arrangement
stance, links a series of visible marks with of incidents; MYTHOS; PLOT. HChatman
the concept "dog." 2. An entity stand- 1978; Ejxenbaurn 1971b; Erlich 1965.
ing for another entity. HSaussure 1966. skaz. A narrative devised as specifically oral
in terms of style; a narrative fashioned to tween the above-mentioned places can be
give the illusion of spontaneous speech. significant and function thematically, struc-
Skaz (from Russian skazat'/skazyvat', "to turally, or as a CHARACTERIZATION device.
tell, relate") is told in language that is typi- Should a narrator narrate from a hospital
cal of the fictional NARRATOR (as opposed bed, for instance, it may mean that she is
to the AUTHOR) and is firmly set in a com- near death and has to rush in order to
munication framework. The manner of complete the NARRATION. Furthermore, one
telling (the distinctive features and pecu- can easily conceive of narratives in which
liarities of the narrator's speech) is as the space of the narrating instance is sys-
important to the effect of the narrative as tematically constrasted with that of the
the situations and events recounted. "Hair- NARRATED (I narrate from a prison cell
cut" and Huckleberry Finn would be events that took place in wide-open
examples of skaz, whereas Robinson Cru- spaces); or narratives-in which the former
soe and David Copperiield would not. is progressively more (or less) distant and
liBakhtin 1984; Banfield 1982; Lemon and different from the latter and in which, con-
Reis 1965; Titunik 1963; Vinogradov 1980. sequently, the narration is more (or less)
slow motion. A cinematic manifestation of precise (I start narrating in Philadelphia
STRETCH (cf. Peckinpah's The Wild Bunch events occurring in New York; I continue
or Penn's Bonnie and Clyde). With slow my narration in Princeton; and I finish it in
motion, an action takes less time than its New York); or narratives in which the var-
I
ious places where the events narrated
representation, which proceeds at less
occur are represented in more or less de-
than usual speed. ^Chatman 1978.
tail, according to different points of view;
sought-for person. One of the seven
and so on. HBal 1977,1985; Bonheim
fundamental ROLES that a character may
1982; Bourneuf and Ouellet 1975; Chat-
assume (in a fairy tale), according to
man 1978; Hamon 1981, 1982; Prince
Propp. The sought-for person (analogous
1982; Zoran 1984. See also DESCRIPTION.
to Souriau's SUN and Greimas's OBJECT) is
spatial form. An arrangement obtaining in
usually represented by a princess.«! Propp
narrative when the usual loaico-iemporal
1 9 6 8 . S e e also ACTANT, DRAMATIS PER-
modes of narrative organization are aban-
SONA, SPHERE OF ACTION.
doned in favor of modes traditionally
space. The place or places within which the
privileged by (nonnarrative) poetry. With
situations and events represented (SET-
spatial form, the temporal movement of an
TING, story space) and the NARRATING
episode stops; attention is drawn to rela-
INSTANCE(S) occur. ^Though it is possible to
tions of symmetry, antithesis, gradation,
narrate without referring to the story space,
repetition, etc., between the episode con-
the space of the narrating instance, or the
stituents, and meaning springs from these
relations between them ("John ate; then he
relations, as in the country-fair scene in
slept"), space can play an important role in
Madame Bovary. "IFrank 1945.
narrative; and the features of or links be-
specification. The rhythm of recunence of act. Thus, taken literally, "I wish you would
the event or set of events in an ITERATIVE open the window" is an assertion about the
NARRATIVE. In "John took a shower once a ADDRESSER'S feelings; yet, in particular
week" the series has a specification of one contexts, it can (and does) perform the illo-
day out of seven. ^Specification can be in- cutionary act of making a request. tiThe
definite ("John often took a cold shower") theory of speech acts originates in J. L.
or definite ("John took a cold shower every Austin's distinction between CONSTATIVES
Monday"). It can also be simple ("Mary (utterances like "Napoleon won the battle
went to the movies every other day") or of Austerlitz" or "The earth is flat" that re-
complex (when two or more patterns of re- port events or states of affairs in certain
currence are combined "Every summer, on worlds and, consequently, are either true
Sundays, Mary went to the movies"). HGe- or false in these worlds) and PERFORMA-
nette 1980. TIVES (utterances iike "I promise to come"
speech act. An utterance considered as a or "I now pronounce you husband and
goal-directed act. The performance of a wife" that are used to do rather than to say
speech act involves that of a LOCUTIONARY something, to perform an act rather than to
ACT (an act of saying, of producing a gram- state that something is or is not the case).
matical utterance), that of an However, as Austin goes on to argue, con-
ILLOCUTIONARY ACT (performed in saying statives are themselves performatives,
something, to accomplish some purpose: since saying (asserting, stating, reporting)
make a promise, an assertion, a request, that something is or is not the case consti-
give a warning, issue a command, etc.), tutes a kind of doing. Indeed, any
and (possibly) that of a PERLOCUTIONARY utterance or set of utterances can be
ACT (performed by means of saying some- viewed as performative and can be re-
thing and describable in terms of the effect garded as a speech act. 5INARRATIVE can,
which the illocutionary act, on the particular of course, be taken to constitute a speech
o c c a s i o n o f use, h a s on the ADDRESSEE: act, a kind of complex or global one sub-
persuading someone to do something, suming the more local ones of the
convincing someone that something is the narrator(s) and the character(s). ^Austin
case, etc.). For example, uttering "I prom- 1962; Chatman 1978; van Dijk 1977; Lan-
ise to be there" in a given context involves ser 1981; Lyons 1977; Pratt 1977;Searle
the locutionary act of making a sentence 1969, 1975, 1976.
according to the rules of English, the illocu- speed. The relationship between the dura-
tionary act of promising, and (possibly) the tion of the NARRATED—the (approximate)
perlocutionary act of convincing the ad- amount of time (presumably) covered by
dressee of one's good faith. In the case of the situations and events recounted—and
so-called indirect speech acts, an illocu- the length of the narrative (in words, lines,
tionary act is performed indirectly by way or pages, for example). UNarrative speed
of the performance of another illocutionary can vary considerably and its canonical
forms—the major narrative TEMPOS—are character may be involved in several
(in descending order, from infinity to zero) spheres of action. HPropp 1968. See also
ELLIPSIS, SUMMARY, SCENE, STRETCH, a n d ACTANT.

PAUSE. liGenette 1980; Prince 1982. See stance. The relation between the NARRATOR
a l s o ANISOCHRONY, DURATION, RHYTHM. a n d t h e NARRATED. A l o n g w i t h CONTACT
sphere of action. The set of FUNCTIONS cor- and STATUS, stance is one of three basic
responding to a particular ROLE or r e l a t i o n s in t e r m s of w h i c h POINT OF VIEW is
DRAMATIS PERSONA ( P r o p p ) . S e v e n s p h e r e s structured. ULanser 1981.
of action can be distinguished: (1) the stasis statement. A NARRATIVE STATEMENT
sphere of action of the VILLAIN: villainy, in the mode of is, one presenting a STATE
struggle, pursuit; (2) the sphere of action of and, more specifically, establishing the ex-
the DONOR: first function of the donor istence of entities by identifying them or
(preparation f<pr the transmission of a magi- qualifying them (cf. "Mary was an engi-
cal agent), provision of a magical agent; neer" and "Mary was happy"). Along with
(3) the sphere of action of the HELPER: t h e PROCESS STATEMENT, it is o n e of t w o
spatial transference of the hero, liquidation kinds of statements with which the DIS-
of misfortune or lack, rescue, solution, COURSE s t a t e s t h e STORY. f C h a t m a n 1 9 7 8 .
transfiguration; (4) the sphere of action of state. The condition of a system (or part
a p r i n c e s s ( a SOUGHT-FOR PERSON) a n d h e r thereof) at a given point of operation; a.set
father: branding, difficult task, exposure, of elements characterized by a number of
recognition, punishment, wedding (the prin- properties and relations at a given time or
cess and her father are not easy to i place; a situation, NARRATIVE is the repre-
distinguish from each other in terms of sentation of one or more changes of state.
functions; usually, it is the father who, pro- UBeaugrande 1980; van Dijk 1974-75;
poses difficult tasks to the HERO and who G e n o t 1 9 7 9 . S e e a l s o EVENT, MOTIF,
p u n i s h e s t h e FALSE HERO, a n d it is t h e p r i n - STASIS STATEMENT.
cess who marries the hero); (5) the sphere status. The relation between the NARRATOR
of action of the DISPATCHER: mediation; (6) and the act of NARRATING. Along with CON-
the sphere of action of the hero: departure, TACT and STANCE, s t a t u s is o n e of three
reaction, wedding (the first function—de- basic relations in terms of which POINT OF
parture on a search—is distinctive of the VIEW is structured. Hlanser 1981.
hero as seeker rather than of the hero as stock character. A conventional CHARACTER
victim); (7) the sphere of action of the traditionally associated with a given (narra-
FALSE HERO: departure, reaction, and—spe- tive) genre or form; a TYPE. The cruel
cific to the false hero—unfounded claims. stepmother and the prince charming are
HA sphere of action may correspond ex- stock characters in fairy tales. Woiman
actly to one CHARACTER or be distributed 1972.
among several characters. Conversely, one
s t o c k situation. A conventional situation; a
standard set of states and events. Stock t i o n a n d i m p l i e s a SENDER a n d a RECEIVER,

situations go from the particular (e.g., the story or history (HISTOIRE) does not. Com-
birthmark that reveals kinship) to the gen- pare "He has eaten" cr "I've reminded you
eral (e.g., the rags-to-riches kind of PLOT). of it many times" with "He ate" or "She re-
Some are considered to be more arche- minded him of it many times." HBenveniste's
typal than merely conventional (e.g., the distinction between histoire and discours is
death and rebirth kind of story). Wolman analogous to Weinrich's distinction be-
1972. t w e e n ERZAHLTE WELT a n d BESPROCHENE

story. 1. T h e CONTENT p l a n e of NARRATIVE WELT and reminiscent of Hamburger's dis-


as opposed to its EXPRESSION plane or DIS- t i n c t i o n b e t w e e n FIKTIONALE ERZAHLEN a n d

COURSE; the "what" of a narrative as AUSSAGE. IBeaugrande 1930; Benveniste


opposed to its "how"; the NARRATED as op- 1971; Chaiman 1978; Dolezel 1976; Fors-
p o s e d t o t h e NARRATING; t h e FICTION as ter 1927; Genette 1980; Prince 1973,
opposed to the NARRATION (in Ricardou's 19S2; ShkJovsky 1965b; Stein 1982; T o -
sense of the terms); the EXISTENTS and mashevsky 1965. See also COMPLEX
EVENTS represented in a narrative. 2. The STORY, MINIMAL STORY.

FABULA (or basic material arranged into a story grammar. A grammar or series of
PLOT) a s o p p o s e d t o t h e SJUJET o r statements and formulas interrelated by an
plot. 3. A narrative of events with an em- ordered sel or rules and accounting for
phasis on chronology, as opposed to plot, (the structure of) a set of STORIES; a gram-
which is a narrative of events with an em- mar specifying the "natural" constituents of
phasis on causality (Forster): "The king (a set of) stories and characterizing their
died, and then the queen died" is a story, relations. '"Stcry grammars take a story to
whereas "The king died, and then the consist of a series of EPISODES which bring
queen died of grief" is a plot. 4. A causal a CHARACTER closer to or farther from a
sequence of events pertinent to a charac- GOAL through the reaching or not reaching
ter or characters seeking to solve a of a SUBGOAL. In the grammar for simple
problem or reach a goal. As such, though stories devised by Thorndyke, for instance,
every story is a narrative (the recounting of each rule in the ordered set is of the form
one or more events), not every narrative is X->Y (to be read "Rewrite X and Y" or "X
necessarily a stop/ (consider, for instance, consists cf Y"); parentheses are used to
a narrative merely recounting a temporal enclose optionally chosen items; different
sequence of events that are not causally possible rewritings of the same item or dif-
related). 5. According to Benveniste, and ferent items that yield the same rewriting
along with discourse (DISCOURS), one of are listed within braces; an asterisk (') in-
two distinct and complementary linguistic dicates that an element may be repeated;
subsystems. Whereas discourse involves and the symbol + indicates the combina-
some reference to the situation of enuncia- tion of elements in sequential order:
(1) Story—i- Setting + Theme + Plot + thieu and his acquaintances or relations
Resolution constitute another one. In many narratives,
(2) Setting-* Characters + Location + a predominant or main story-line can be
Time distinguished from subsidiary ones. IRim-
(3) Theme-* (Event)' + Goal mon-Kenan 1983.
(4) Plot—» Episode* story time. The period of time in which the
(5) Episode-* Subgoai + Attempt* + NARRATED OCCUrS; ERZAHLTE ZEIT. liChat-

Outcome m a n 1978. S e e a l s o DISCOURSE TIME,

DURATION.
(6) Attempt-* I
stream of consciousness. A kind of FREE
DIRECT DISCOURSE or INTERIOR MONOLOGUE
(7) Outcome-* }
attempting to give "a direct quotation of the

(8) Resolution^'} mind" (Bowling); a mode of representation


of human consciousness focusing on the
0 3 random flow of thought and stressing its il-
(9) { Q ^ ' } - Desired State
logical, "ungrammatical," associative

(Characters]
Location
Time J
State nature (Molly Bloom's monologue in Ulys-
ses). HThough interior monologue and

I stream of consciousness have often been


UDeveloped by students of cognitive considered interchangeable, they have also
psychology and artificial intelligence, frequently been contrasted: the former
story grammars have influenced would present a character's thoughts rather
decisively the study of the effects of than impressions or perceptions, while the
structure and content variables on latter would present both impressions and
memory and the comprehension of j thoughts; or else, the former would respect
narrative texts: they are attempts to morphology and syntax, whereas the latter
capture the abstract structural schemes would not (punctuation is then absent,
allowing for the retention and grammatical forms truncated, short incom-
understanding of narrative. "Back and plete sentences numerous, neologisms
Bower 1980; van Dijk 1980; Glenn 1978; frequent) and would thus capture thought
Mandler and Johnson 1977; Rumelhart in its nascent stage, prior to any logical
1975; Schank 1975; Thomdyke 1975; connection. fThe term was coined by
Wilensky 1978. See also NARRATIVE j William James to describe the way

GRAMMAR, REWRITE RULE. consciousness presents itself. ^Bowling

story-line. The set of events in a story that 1950; Chatman 1978; Cohn 1978;

involve the same individuals. In The Re- M. Friedman 1955; Genette 1980; Hum-

prieve, for example, the events involving phrey 1954; W. James 1890; Scholes and

Milan Hlinka and his wife Anna constitute Kellogg 1966.

one story-line and the events involving Ma- stretch. A canonical narrative TEMPO (Chat-

i
m a n ) ; a l o n g w i t h ELLIPSIS, PAUSE, SCENE, 1980; Rumelhart 1975; Thorndyke 1975.
and SUMMARY, one of the fundamental nar- S e e a l s o STORY GRAMMAR.

r a t i v e SPEEDS. W h e n DISCOURSE TIME is subject. An ACTANT or fundamental ROLE at


(taken to be) greater than STORY TIME, the level of deep narrative structure, in the
when a narrative segment is (felt to be) too Greimassian model. The SUBJECT (analo-
lengthy for the NARRATED it represents, gous to Propp's HERO and Souriau's LION)
when a relatively long (part of the) narra- looks for the OBJECT. At the level of narra-
tive text corresponds to a relatively short tive surface structure, it is concretized as
narrated time (to a narrated action that is the PROTAGONIST. HGreimas 1970, 1983a,
usually completed in a short time), stretch 1983b; Greimas and Courtes 1982; Hen-
obtains ("Occurrence at Owl Creek ault 1983. See also ACTANTIAL MODEL,
Bridge"). W summary covers the range of ACTANTIAL ROLE, ANTISUBJECT, AUXILIANT,

speeds between scene and ellipsis, stretch NARRATIVE SCHEMA, NARRATIVE TRAJEC-

covers the range of speeds between pause TORY.

and scene. HChatman 1978; Genette 19S0; subjective narrative. 1. A narrative char-
Prince 1982. See also DURATION, SLOW MO- acterized by a n OVERT NARRATOR whose
TION. feelings, beliefs, and judgments color the
structural analysis of narrative. The analy- treatment of t h e situations and events pre-
sis of narrative in terms of its STRUCTURE. sented. 2. A narrative in which the
In accounting for (a given) narrative, struc- thoughts or feelings of one or more char-
tural analysis gives priority to (syntactico- acters are presented (as opposed to
semantic) relations (as opposed to, say, BEHAVIORIST NARRATIVE). I B r o o k s a n d W a r -

origin, function, or substance), <iBarthes r e n 1 9 5 9 . S e e a l s o OBJECTIVE NARRATIVE.

1975. subplot. A unified set of actions coincident


structure. The network of relations obtaining with but subordinate to the (main) PLOT.
between the various constituents of a HSouvage 1965. See also DOUBLE PLOT.
whole as well as between each constituent subsequent narrating. A narrating that fol-
and the whole. Should narrative be defined lows the situations and events narrated; a
a s c o n s i s t i n g of STORY a n d DISCOURSE, for POSTERIOR NARRATION. Subsequent narrat-
example, its structure would be the net- ing is characteristic of most narratives.
work of relations obtaining between story HGenette 1980.
and discourse, story and narrative, and substance. Following Hjelmslev, and as op-
discourse and narrative. IjChatman 1978; posed to FORM, the (material or semantic)
Greimasand Courtes 1982; Piaaet 1980. reality constitutive of the two planes of a

style indirect libre. See FREE INDIRECT DIS- semiotic system (the EXPRESSION plane

COURSE. HBally 1912. and the CONTENT plane). HJn the case of

subgoal. An intermediate STATE in a (char- narrative, the substance of the expression

acter's) plan to reach a desired GOAL. can be said to b e equivalent to the medium

HBeaugrande 1980; Black and Bower of narrative MANIFESTATION (language, film,


etc.) and the substance of the content to is realized: surface structure is related to
the set of possible entities and events that DEEP STRUCTURE b y a s e t of o p e r a t i o n s or
can be represented by a narrative. TiChat- TRANSFORMATIONS; t h e MICROSTRUCTURE of
man 1978; Ducrotand Todorov 1979; narrative. In the Greimassian model of nar-
Hjelmslev 1954, 1961. See also DIS- rative, for example, whereas ACTANTS and
COURSE, NARRATIVE MEDIUM, STORY. actantial relations would be elements of the
s u b s t i t u t i o n a r y n a r r a t i o n . S e e FREE INDI- deep structure, ACTORS and actorial rela-
RECT DISCOURSE. ^ F e h r 1 9 3 8 ; H e r n a d i tions would be found at the surface
1972. | structure level. In other models of narra-
summary. A canonical narrative TEMPO; tive, whereas the deep structure might be
a l o n g with ELLIPSIS, PAUSE, SCENE, a n d said to correspond to STORY, the surface
STRETCH, one of the fundamental narrative structure might be said to correspond to
SPEEDS. W h e n DISCOURSE TIME is ( t a k e n t o DISCOURSE (or the stating of the story).
be) smaller than STORY TIME, when a nar- ?The term and concept were adapted from
rative segment is (felt to be) too brief for Chomsky and generative-transformational
the NARRATED it represents, when a rela- grammar. HChomsky 1965; van Dijk 1972;
tively short (part of the) narrative text Johnson and Mandler 1980. See also NAR-
corresponds to a relatively long narrated RATIVE GRAMMAR.
time (to a narrated action that it usually surprise. The emotion obtaining when ex-
takes a long time to complete), summary pectations about what is going to happen
obtains: it covers the range of speeds be- are violated by what in fact does happen.
tween scene and ellipsis. HSummary (or The production of surprise is considered
PANORAMA) is traditionally contrasted with particularly effective when, although what
scene (or DRAMA) and, in classical narra- in fact does happen violates expectations,
tive, constitutes the connective between it is well grounded in what happened ear-
scenes as well as the background against lier. TThe interplay of surprise and
which they come to the fore. HBentley SUSPENSE traditionally constitutes an im-
1946; Chatman 1978; Genette 1980; portant feature of good plotting. UChatman
P r i n c e 1 9 8 2 . S e e also DURATION, RHYTHM. 1978.
s u n . O n e of s i x f u n d a m e n t a l ROLES or FUNC- suspense. An emotion or state of mind aris-
TIONS isolated by Souriau (in his study of ing from a partial and anxious uncertainty
the possibilities of drama). The Sun (analo- about the progression or outcome of an
gous to Greimas's OBJECT and P'ropp's action, especially one involving a positive
SOUGHT-FOR PERSON) is t h e r e p r e s e n t a t i o n character. Suspense obtains, for instance,
of the desired object and orients the action when a certain result is possible but
of the LION. IScholes 1974; Souriau 1950. whether it will actually come to pass is not
S e e a l s o ACTANT. clear or when -a given outcome is known
surface structure. The particular way the but how and when it will occur is not.
deep or underlying structure of a narrative ^Suspense often depends on FORESHAD-
OWING and, more generally, on the form a syntagrn and so do the sounds /dz/,
thematizations, snares, and suspended /el/, a n d In/ whereas the word "Jane" a n d
answers structured in terms of the HERME- the sound lb/ do not. t i n an early version
NEUTIC CODE. TiBal 1985; Barthes 1974; of the Greimassian model of narrative,
Chatman 1978; Rabkin 1973; Sternberg three types or fundamental narrative syn-
1 9 7 8 . S e e a l s o HERMENEUTEME, SURPRISE. tagms were identified: performative
s w i t c h b a c k . A n ANALEPSIS; a FLASHBACK; a (relating to tests and struggles), contractual
RETROSPECTION; a CUTBACK. ^ S o u v a g e (pertaining to the establishing and breaking
1 9 6 5 . S e e a l s o ANACHRONY, ORDER. of contracts), and disjunctional (involving
syllepsis. A grouping of situations and various kinds of movements and displace-
events governed by a nonchronological ments, of departures and returns). liDucrot
principle (spatial, thematic, etc.) rather than and Todorov 1979; Greimas 1970; Grei-
by a chronological one. in "He remem- mas a n d Courtes 1982; Saussure 1966.
bered that time very well. He had dmnk S e e a l s o PARADIGM.

huge amounts of cola; he had dated a lot;


and he had read a little," the representa-
tion of the events remembered constitutes
a syllepsis. HGenette 1980. See also OR-
DER.

symbolic code. The CODE, or "voice," in


terms of which a narrative or part thereof tag. S e e TAG CLAUSE. liChatman 1978.
can acquire a symbolic dimension; the tag clause. A clause ("he said," "she
code governing the production/reception of thought," "she asked," "he replied") ac-
symbolic meaning. Given a series of anti- companying a character's discourse
thetical terms in a text, they can—through (speech or verbalized thought) and specify-
associations and extrapolations regulated ing the act of the speaker or thinker,
by the symbolic code—be taken to repre- identifying him or her, and (sometimes) in-
sent more abstract, fundamental, and dicating various aspects of the act, the
general oppositions and meanings. character, the setting in which they appear,
HBarthes 1974,1981a. etc. TTag clauses (or TAGS) can accom-
synchronic analysis. The study of a (lin- p a n y DIRECT DISCOURSE ( " — W h a t are y o u

guistic) system as it appears at one and doing here? he asked with a smile") or IN-
the same moment (without bringing into DIRECT DISCOURSE ( " S h e s a i d that s h e w a s

play factors or elements belonging to other e x h a u s t e d " ) . I n ABRUPTIVE DIALOGUE a s

moments). liSaussure 1966. See also w e l l a s i n FREE DIRECT DISCOURSE a n d

DIACHRONIC ANALYSIS. FREE INDIRECT DISCOURSE (excepting p a r -

syntagm. A rule-governed sequence of two entheticais and as opposed to TAGGED


or more units of the same type. In "Jane INDIRECT DISCOURSE), n o t a g clauses a c -

blushed," the words "Jane" and "blushed" company the utterance or thought.
HChatman 1978; Page 1973; Prince 1978. NARRATED a n d NARRATING. 2. In g r a m m a r ,
S e e a l s o ATTRIBUTIVE DISCOURSE, INQUIT a form indicative of a time distinction. Psy-
FORMULA, VERBUM DICENDI. chologists, linguists, and students of fiction
tagged direct discourse. A DIRECT DIS- and literature (Buhler, Benveniste, Wein-
COURSE a c c o m p a n i e d by a TAG CLAUSE. rich) have frequently argued that tenses
^Chatman 1978. can be grouped in two main categories:
tagged indirect discourse. An INDIRECT DIS- tenses related to the DEICTIC system of "I-
COURSE a c c o m p a n i e d by a TAG CLAUSE. here-now," to the situation of ENUNCIATION
^Chatman 1978. (e.g., the present perfect—"he has ea-
tellability. See REPORTABILITY. ten"—which connects a past occurrence
telling. Along with SHOWING, one of two fun- with the present time), and tenses not re-
damental kinds of DISTANCE regulating lated to it (e.g., the preterite—"he ate"—
narrative information; DIEGESIS (diGgesis). which refers to a past occurrence without
TTelling is a MODE characterized by more connecting it to the present time). Narrative
narratorial mediation and a less detailed privileges the members of the second
rendering of situations and events than group (e.g., the preterite but also the im-
SHOWING (or MIMESIS): NARRATIZED DIS- perfect and the pluperfect, as opposed to
COURSE constitutes a good example of the present, the present perfect, and the
telling. HChatman 1978; Genette 1980, future). HBenveniste 1971; Bronzwaer
1983; H. James 1972; Lubbock 1965. 1970; K. Buhler 1934; Ducrot and Todorov
i
:empo. A rate of narrative SPEED. ELLIPSIS, 1979; Genette 1980; Todorov 1966; Wein-

SUMMARY, SCENE, STRETCH, a n d PAUSE a r e rich 1964. See also BESPROCHENE WELT,

the five major tempos in narrative. HBentley EPIC PRETERITE, ERZAHLTE WELT.

1946; Chatman 1978; Genette 1980. test. A narrative SYNTAGM characterizing the

emporal juncture. The temporal separation movement of the SUBJECT toward its goal

obtaining between two NARRATIVE CLAUSES. and involving a polemical or transactional

Given a sequence of temporally ordered confrontation (a struggle for a given object

clauses, the displacement of clauses or an exchange of objects), a domination

across a temporal juncture leads to a (by the Subject), and its consequence. Hln

change in the semantic interpretation of the the canonical NARRATIVE SCHEMA devel-

original sequence: compare "John had oped by Greimas and his school, the

lunch; then John went to bed" and "John Subject undergoes a QUALIFYING TEST, a

went to bed; then John had lunch." HFor DECISIVE TEST, a n d a GLORIFYING TEST.

L a b o v , a MINIMAL NARRATIVE is o n e c o n - W a r n 1984; Greimas 1970, 1983a,

taining a single temporal juncture. ULabov 1983b; Greimas and Courtes 1982; Hen-

1972; Labov and Waletzky 1967. auit 1983; Larivaille 1974.

anse. 1. The set of temporal relations— thematic role. A set of attributes and behav-

SPEED, ORDER, DISTANCE, e t c . — b e t w e e n iors which, in conjunction with at least one

the situations and events recounted and ACTANTIAL ROLE, defines an ACTOR. There

their recounting, STORY and DISCOURSE* are professional roles (the physician, the
teacher, the farmer, the priest, etc.), famil- Ducrot and Todorov 1979; Falk 1967; N.
ial ones (the father, the stepmother, the Friedman 1975; Fn/e 1957; Prince 1985;
older brother, etc.), psychosocial ones (the Rimmon-Kenan 1985; Wellek and Warren
pedant, the snob, the paranoiac, etc.), and 1949; Zholkovsky 1384.
so on. fin the Greimassian model of narra- thesis. The doctrine or ideological context of
tive, the thematic role constitutes an a text; the (philosophical, moral, political)
intermediate category between ACTANT and views advanced by that text. The thesis of
actor: it helps to specify the former and is a work could be distinguished from its
in turn specified by the latter. UGreimas THEME: it promotes an answer instead of
1983a; Greimas and Courtes 1982; Hamon raising questions and asks to be agreed
1972, 1983; Henault 1983. with rather ihan thought about. Thus, the
theme. A semantic macrostrjctural category theme of a given novel might be the de-
or FRAME extractable from (or allowing for cline of Southern aristocracy whereas its
the unification of) distinct (and discontin- thesis might be that this decline was most
uous) textual elements which (are taken to) regrettable. liBeardsley 1958; Chatnnan
illustrate it and expressing the more gen- 1983; Suleiman 1983.
eral and abstract entities (ideas, thoughts, third-person narrative. A narrative whose
etc.) that a text or part thereof is (or may narrator is not a character in the situations
be considered to be) about. HA theme and events recounted; a HETERODIEGETIC
should be distinguished from other kinds of NARRATIVE; a narrative that "is about" third
macrostructural categories or frames that persons ("he," "she," "they"). "He was
also connect or allow for the connecting of happy; then he lost his job, and he be-
textual elements and express what a text came unhappy" is a third-person narrative,
or segment thereof is (partly) about: it is an and so are Sons and Lovers, The Trial,
"idea" frame rather than, for example, an and One Hundred Years ot Solitude. HGe-
action frame (PLOT) or an existent frame nette 1983; Prince 1982. See also PERSON.
(CHARACTER, SETTING). H M o r e o v e r , a t h e m e thought. 1. Along with CHARACTER
should be distinguished from a MOTIF, (ETHOS), one of two fundamental qualities
which is a more concrete and specific unit t h a t a n AGENT (or PRATTON) h a s , according

manifesting it, and from a TOPOS, which is to Aristotle. Thought (DIANOIA) is an agent's
constituted (rather than illustrated) by a vision of the world, an agent's conception
specific complex of motifs. HFinaily, the of things; it is revealed by his or her emo-
theme of a work could be distinguished tions, beliefs, statements, and
from its THESIS (the doctrine it supports). reasonings. 2. The THEME or, more gen-
Unlike the latter, the former does not pro- erally, the meaning of a literary work,
mote an answer but helps to raise according to Frye. In narrative, thought (di-
questions: it is contemplative rather than anoia) can b e viewed as the MYTHOS in
assertive. *Barthes 1974; Beardsley 1958; stasis (and mythos would be dianoia in
Bremond 1985; Chatman 1983; van Dijk movement) ^Aristotle 1968; N. Friedman
1977; Daemmrich and Daemmrich 1986; 1975; Frye 1957.
time. The period or periods during which the S e e a l s o TRANSFORMATIONAL RULE.

situations and events presented (STORY transformation. An operation relating two


TIME, t i m e o f t h e NARRATED, ERZAHLTE ZEIT) strings or two structural levels within the
and their presentation (DISCOURSE TIME, same text or two (strings within) different
time of the NARRATING, ERZAHLZEIT) occur. texts. ^According to Todorov, for example,
<!Chatman 1978; Mendilow 1952; Metz a transformation is a relation obtaining be-
1974; Muller 1968; Prince 1982. See also tween two PROPOSITIONS that have a
DURATION, SPEED, TENSE. PREDICATE in common, and it can be sim-
tone. The narrator's attitude toward the NAR- ple (involving the addition of an operator—
RATEE and/or the situations and events of modality, negation, etc.—to a base
presented, as implicitly or explicitly con- predicate: "X eats a hamburger every day"
veyed by his or her narration. UTone can -> "X does not eat a hamburger every
be taken to be a function of DISTANCE. day") or complex (involving the grafting of
11 Brooks and Warren 1959; Richards a predicate onto a base predicate: "X eats
1950. a hamburger every day" —»"X [or Y] says
topos. Any of a stable disposition of MOTIFS that X eats a hamburger every day").
that frequently appears in (literary) texts. Among simple transformations, there are
Such topo'i as those of the wise fool, the transformations of mode ("X must eat a
aged child, and the locus amoenus are hamburger every day"), intent ("X tries to
very common in Western literature HCurtius eat a hamburger every day"), result ("X
1973; Ducrot and Todorov 1979. See also manages to eat a hamburger every day"),
THEME. manner ("X hastens to eat a hamburger

trait. A CHARACTER'S quality or feature that every day"), aspect ("X continues to eat a

recurs in a series of situations and events. hamburger every day"), and status ("X

HChatman 1978. See also ATTRIBUTE, does not eat a hamburger every day").

SEME. Among complex transformations, there are

transform. The string resulting from the ap- transformations of appearance ("X [or YJ

plication of a TRANSFORMATION to a certain claims that X eats a hamburger every

(set of) string(s). Given a string of situ- day"), knowledge ("X [or Y] knows that X

ations and events "A—then—B—then— eats a hamburger every day"), description

C," for example, "C—before C—A—-then— ("X [or Y] says that X eats a hamburger

B" could be said to be a transform of it, re- every day"), supposition ("X [or Y] sus-

sulting from the application of an order pects that X eats a hamburger every day"),

transformation to the original string (cf. "She subjectivization ("X [or Y] believes that X

had been poor; then she won the lottery; eats a hamburger every day"), and attitude

then she became rich" and "She became ("X [or Y] likes the fact that X eats a ham-

rich. Before she became rich, she had been burger e v e r / day"). For a narrative

poor, then she won the tottery"). TThe temn SEQUENCE to be complete, it must contain

transform was borrowed from generative- two distinct propositions in a transforma-

transformational grammar. ^Prince 1973. tional relation. ^According to Greimas also,


transformations link intratexiual strings at tons. According to Propp, for example, the
the same structural level. They are equiva- specific actions concretizing the FUNCTIONS
lent So CONJUNCTION anci DISJUNCTION that constitute the fundamental compo-
operations between SUBJECT and OBJECT, nents of any fairy tale can change from
and more generally, they lead from an ini- one tale (or set of tales) to another, and
tial state to a final one constituting its the changes (which may be considered to
contrary or contradictory (its inversion or follow a historically determined evolution,
negation). Hlf, for Todorov and Greirnas, with the marvelous becoming rational, for
transformations are intratextual and obtain instance, o r the heroic humoristic) are
at the same structural level, for some stu- called transformations. Similarly, according
dents of narrative—in particular, those who to Levi-Strauss, for whom a myth consists
have been influenced by generative-trans- of all its versions, these versions can be
formational grammar (van Dijk.-Pavel, said to be related by transformations. Wan
Prince, etc.)—transformations are intratex- Dijk 1980; Ducrot and Todorov 1979; Grei-
tual but (usually) relate two different rnas 1970, 1971,1983b; Greirnas and
structural levels (the underlying or DEEP Courtes 1982; HSnault 1983; Kongas-Mar-
STRUCTURE of a n a r r a t i v e a n d its SURFACE anda and Maranda 1971; Levi-Strauss
STRUCTURE). Specifically, they perform cer- 1963, 1955-71; Mandler and Johnson
tain changes (permutations of elements, 1977; Pave! 1976, 1985; Prince 1973,
additions, deletions, etc.) in certain deep- 1982; Propp 1968, 1984; Todorov 1978,
structure strings (or their TRANSFORMS). For 1 9 8 1 . S e e a l s o NARRATIVE GRAMMAR,

instance, given a string analyzable as "A— TRANSFORMATIONAL RUI.E.

then—B—then—C," an order transforma- transformational rule. A rule allowing for


tion operating on it might yield "C—before the performance of certain changes in cer-
C—A—then—B," and a repetition transfor- tain strings provided these strings have a
mation might yield "A—then—B—then— certain structure. The first part of a trans-
C—repeat A" (cf. "He had been very formational rule is a structural analysis
happy; then he left his hometown; then he (SA) specifying the kind of string to which
became unhappy," "He became unhappy. the rule applies; the second part specifies
Before he became unhappy, he had been the structural change (SC) by means of
very happy; then he left his hometown," numbers referring to the elements in SA
and "He had been very happy; then he left For instance, to show that an event, A, can
his hometown; then he became unhappy. appear after another event, B, even though
He had been very happy"). Two or more it precedes it in time (as in "She ate a
different surface structures might thus be hamburger; before that, she had eaten a
derived form the same underlying struc- pizza"), there might be a transformational
ture. TOn the other hand, some students of rule such as:
narrative (and, more particularly, of folk-
tales and myths) take transformations to be SA: A—then—B

intertextual rather than intratextual opera-


S C : 3—before 3—1
^Transformational rules were imported into other sequence might be made up of
narratology from generative-transforma- "villainy, intervention of the hero, failure."
tional grammar and play an important role W i t h i n a triad, a posterior term implies an
in some grammars of narrative. HChomsky anterior one but not vice versa: there is an
1957,1962, 1965; Prince 1973,1982. See intervention of the hero, for instance, only if
alSO NARRATIVE GRAMMAR, TRANSFORMA- there was a villainy, and there is a success
TION. only if there was an intervention. On the
transposed discourse, INDIRECT DIS- other hand, every anterior term offers a
COURSE. A l o n g with REPORTED DISCOURSE consequent alternative: a villainy might
(DIRECT DISCOURSE) a n d NARRATIZED DIS- lead to an intervention or nonintervention
COURSE, transposed discourse is, in of the hero, and an intervention might end
Genette's view, one of three basic ways of in success or failure. UTriads can combine
representing characters' utterances and to yield more complex-sequences, and ac-
verbal thoughts. HGenette 1980,1983. See cording to Bremond, the most charac-
a l s o TYPES OF DISCOURSE. teristic modes of combination are ENCHAIN-
transposed speech, TRANSPOSED DIS- MENT ("back-to-back" succession, bout A
COURSE, especially transposed discourse bout: the outcome of one sequence consti-
whereby a character's utterances (as op- tutes the situation's opening a possibility in
posed to thoughts) are represented. another sequence), EMBEDDING (enclave:
HGenette 1980, 1983. See also INDIRECT one sequence is embedded into another
SPEECH. and specifies or details one of its first two
trebling. See TRIPLICATION. ^Propp 1968. functions), and JOINING (accolement: the
same sequence, considered from two dif-
triad. A series of three units or FUNCTIONS ferent points of view, consists of two
corresponding to the three fundamental different sets of functions depending on the
stages in the unfolding of any process—(1) point of view adopted):
virtuality (a situation's opening a possibil-
A, A, A, = B,
ity); (2) actualization or nonactualization of
i 1 1 1
the possibility; (3) achievement or nona-
A2 B, A2 = B;
chievement—and constituting the elemen-
i 1 1 1
tary (minimal, atomic) narrative SEQUENCE:
A3 = B, B2 A 3 = B;

I
I 1
(achievement
b2 B3
1 1
nonactualization nonachievement B3 A2
1
More specifically, a given elementary se-
A3
quence might consist of "villainy,1
intervention of the hero, success," and an- Bremond 1973,1980.
triplication. The double repetition, at the
level of the NARRATED, of one or more (se-
quences of) events; TREBLING. A character
may, for instance, violate three interdictions
or perform three difficult tasks. Triplication u n d r a m a t i z e d n a r r a t o r . A COVERT NARRA-

is common in folk literature. "iSee also DU- TOR. U Booth 1983. See also DRAMATIZED
PLICATION. NARRATOR.

turning point. The ACT or HAPPENING that is unrave'iing. See DENOUEMENT. USee also
decisive in making a goal reachable or not. RAVELLING.

UBeaugrande 1980. See also CRISIS. unreliable narrator. A NARRATOR whose


type. A static character whose attributes are norms and behavior are not in accordance
very few and who constitutes a paradigm with the IMPLIED AUTHOR'S norms; a narra-
case of a given quality, attitude, or role tor whose values (tastes, judgments, moral
(the miser, the braggart, the femtne fatale, sense) diverge from those of the implied
the hypochondriac, etc.) HDucrot and To- author's; a narrator the reliability of whose
dorov 1979; Scholes and Kellogg 1966. account is undermined by various features
types of discourse. The basic modes of of that account ("Haircut," The Fall, Hitch-
representation of a character's thoughts cock's Stage Fright). UBooth 1983;
and (spoken or written) utterances. The fol- C h a t m a n 1978. S e e also RELIABLE NARRA-

lowing categories are usually distinguished TOR.

on a scale of decreasing narratorial media-


t i o n : (1) NARRATIZED DISCOURSE; (2)

TAGGED INDIRECT DISCOURSE ( o n e variety of

INDIRECT o r TRANSPOSED DISCOURSE); (3)

FREE INDIRECT DISCOURSE (another variety

of indirect or transposed discourse); (4)


(TAGGED) DIRECT DISCOURSE (REPORTED DIS- v a r i a b l e i n t e r n a l f o c a l i z a t i o n . A t y p e o f IN-
COURSE); (5) FREE DIRECT DISCOURSE TERNAL FOCALIZATION Or POINT OF VIEW
(IMMEDIATE DISCOURSE). U C h a t m a n 1978; whereby different FOCALIZERS are used in
Genette 1980, 1983; McHale 1978; Rim- turn to present different situations and
mon-Kenan 1983. events (The Age of Reason, The Golden
Bowl). tGenette 1980. See also FOCALIZA-
TION.

variable internal p o i n t of view. See VARI-


ABLE INTERNAL FOCALIZATION. KPrince 1 9 8 2 .

verbum dicendl. A verb that can appear in


a TAG CLAUSE. Verba dicendi (literally,
"verbs of saying") may occur with DIRECT
o r INDIRECT DISCOURSE a n d c o n s t i t u t e a
class that is usually taksn to include not fairy tale), according to Propp. The villain
only verbs of linguistic communication (analogous to Greimas's OPPONENT and
("say," "ask," "reply," "swear," "shout," Souriau's MARS) opposes the HERO and,
etc.) but also verbs of belief, reflection, and more specifically, causes his misfortune or
emotion ("think," "believe," "feel," etc.) that of another character. UPropp 1968.
and, most generally, verbs that are viewed S e e a l s o ACTANT, DRAMATIS PERSONA,

as specifying the act of the speaker or SPHERE OF ACTION.

thinker ("—How are you? he smiled").. vision. The POINT(S) OF VIEW in terms of
liBanfield, 1982; Page 1973; Pnnce 1978. which the narrated situations and events
S e e a l s o ATTRIBUTIVE DISCOURSE, INOUIT are presented. Pouillon devised a three-
FORMULA. i term categorization: (1) vision from behind
verisimilitude. The quality of a text resulting (vision par derriere, analogous to ZERO FO-
from its degree of conformity to a set of CALIZATION or OMNISCIENT POINT OF VIEW;

"truth" norms that are external to it: a text the narrator tells more than any and all of
has (more or less) verisimilitude (gives the characters know: Tess of the D'Urber-
more or less of an illusion of truth) depend- viltes); (2) vision with (vision avec,
ing on the extent to which it conforms to a n a l o g o u s to INTERNAL FOCALIZATION; t h e

what is taken to be the case (to "reality") narrator tells only what one or several
and to what is made suitable or expected characters know: The Ambassadors, The
by a particular generic tradition. HCuller, Age of Reason); and (3) vision from with-
1975; Genette 1968; Todorov 1981. See out (vision du dehors, analogous to
a l s o MOTIVATION, NATURALIZATION, REFER- EXTERNAL FOCALIZATION; the narrator tells
ENTIAL CODE. less about certain situations and events
i e w p o i n t . FOCALIZATION; POINT OF VIEW. than one or several characters know: "Hills
Grimes distinguishes four basic categories: Like White Elephants"). HGenette 1980;
omniscient viewpoint (ZERO FOCALIZATION), Pouillon 1945; Prince 1982; Todorov 1981.
first-person participant viewpoint (HOMOOI- See also ASPECT.
EGETIC NARRATIVE With INTERNAL FOCAL- voice. The set of signs characterizing the
IZATION), third-person subjective viewpoint NARRATOR and, more generally, the NAR-
(HETEROOIEGETIC NARRATIVE with internal RATING INSTANCE, a n d g o v e r n i n g t h e

focalization), and third-person objective relations between NARRATING and narrative


v i e w p o i n t (EXTERNAL FOCALIZATION). text as well as between narrating and NAR-
(IGrimes 1975. i RATED. HVoice has a much larger extension
ewpoint character. See FOCAL CHARAC- than PERSON and, though often amalgam-
TER. ated or confused with POINT OF VIEW,

Ilain. 1. A wicked ANTAGONIST; an enemy should be distinguished from it: the latter

of the hero, capable or guilty of evil j provides information about who "sees,"

doings. 2. One of the seven fundamental who perceives, whose point of view gov-

ROLES that a character may assume (in a erns the narrative, whereas the former
provides information about who "speaks,"
; who the narrator is, what the narrating in-
! stance consists of. HGenette 1980, 1983;
i
j Rimmon 1976.
zero focalization. A type of FOCALIZATION or
POINT OF VIEv/ w h e r e b y t h e NARRATED is

presented in terms of a noniocatafale, inde-


terminate perceptual or conceptual
position. Zero focalization (or NON FOCALIZA-
TION) is characteristic of "traditional" or
"classical" narrative (Vanity Fair, Eugenie
i w e l l - s p o k e n narrator. A narrator whose Grandet) and associated with OMNISCIENT
i mode of expression is a standard (or even NARRATORS. HGenette 1980. See atso AU-
j elegant) one and functions as a norm in THORIAL NARRATIVE SITUATION, OMNISCIENT

| terms of which the characters' modes of POINT OF VIEW, VISION.

| expression are situated. ^According to


] Hough, the contrast between the well-spo-
| ken narrator's diction and the characters'
j diction is characteristic of the novel as op-
I posed to the epic. HHough 1970.
| writer!-/ text. A text that cannot be read (or
i decoded) in terms of well-defined con-
| straints, conventions, and codes; a text
| that is not adapted to (more or less) estab-
j lished decoding strategies; a text that is (to
i be) written rather than (already) read. The
| writerly text (texte scriptible) is a text sigrii-
i fying in infinitely many ways (in any and all
] ways). As opposed to the READERLY TEXT
(texte lisible), it is triumphantly plural and
totally open. ^Strictly speaking, the term
designates a (paradoxical) ideal and can-
not characterize narrative texts, if only
because they signify in terms of a logic of
a c t i o n ( t h e PROAIRETIC CODE a n d its v a r -

ious constraints). Yet it has increasingly


come to be used in connection with uncon-
ventional texts, including unconventional
narratives. HBarthes 1974.
Jwibliography

Adam, Jean-Michel. 1984. Le F!6cit. Paris:


Presses Universitaires de France.
. 1985. Le Texte narratif. Paris: Fer-
nand Nathan.
Alexandrescu, Sorin. Logique du person-
nage. Tours: Mame.
Aristotle. 1968. Poetics. In Aristotle's Poet-
ics: A Translation and Commentary for
Studenis of Literature. Trans. Leon
Golden. Commentary by 0. B. Hardison,
Jr. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall.
Arrive, Michel. 1973. "Pour une iheorie dss
textes poly-isotopiques."Langages, no.
31: 53 - 6 3 .
Austin, J. L. 1962. How to Do Things with
Words. New York: Oxford University
Press.
Bakhtin, Mikhail. 1981. The Dialogic Imagi-
nation. Ed. Michael Holquist. Trans. Caryl
Emerson and Michael Holquist. Austin:
University of Texas Press.
. 1984. Problems of Dostoevsky's Po-
etics. Ed. and trans. Caryl Emerson.
Minneapolis: University of Minnesota
Press.
Bal, Mieke. 1977. Alarratologie. Paris: Klinck-
sieck.
. 1981 a. "The Laughing Mice; or, On
Focalization." Poetics Today 2:202-10.
. 1981b. "Notes on Narrative Embed-
ding." Poetics Today 2:41-59.
—. 1983. "The Narrating and the Focal-
izing: A Theory of the Agents in Narra-
tive." S!y/e 17:234-69.
—. 1985. Narratology: Introduction to
the Theory of Narrative. Trans. Christine
van Boheernen. Toronto: University of
Toronto Press.
Bally, Charles. 1912. "Le Style indirect libre
en franfais moderne." Gerrnanisch-ro-
manische Monatsschrift 4:549-56, 597-
606.
Banfield, Ann. 1982. Unspeakable Sen-
tences: Narration and Representation in
the Language of Fiction. Boston: Rout-
ledge and Kegan Paul.
Barthes, Roland. 1968. Writing Degree Zero.
Trans. Annette Lavers and Colin Smith.
New York: Hill & Wang.
. 1974. S / Z Trans. Richard Miller.
New York: Hill & Wang.
. 1975. "An Introduction to the Struc-
tural Analysis of Narrative." New Literary
History 6:237-62.
. 1981a. "Textual Analysis of Poe's
'Valdemar.' " In Robert Young, ed., Unty-
ing the Text: A Post-Structuralist Reader,
pp. 133-61. London: Routledge and Ke-
gan Paul.
. 1981b. "Theory of the Text." Ibid.,
pp. 31-47.
. 1982. "The Reality Effect." InTzve-
tan Todorov, ed., French Literary Theory
Today, pp. 11-17. Trans. R. Carter.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, Bonheim, Helmut W. 1982. The Narrative
and Paris: Editions de la Maison des Sci- Modes: Techniques of the Short Story.
ences de I'Homme. Cambridge, Eng.: D. S. Brewer.
Barllett, Frederic C. 1932. Remembering: A Booth, Wayne C. 1961. "Distance and Point
Study in Experimental and Social Psy- of View." Essays in Criticism 11:60-79.
chology. New York: Macmillan. . 1983. The Rhetoric of Fiction. 2d ed.
3each, Joseph Warren. 1932. The Twen- Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
tieth-Century Novel: Studies in Boumeuf, Roland, and Real Ouellet. 1975.
Technique. New York: Appleton-Century- L'Univers du roman. Paris: Presses Uni-
Crofts. versitaires de France.
3eardsley, Monroe. 1958. Aesthetics: Prob- Bowling, Lawrence E. 1950. "What Is the
lems in the Philosophy of Criticism. New Stream of Consciousness Technique?"
York: Harcourt, Brace. PMLA 55:333-45.
Beaugrande, Robert de. 1980. Text, Dis- Bremond, Claude. 1973. Logique du recit.
course, and Process: Toward a Paris: Seuil.
Multidisciplinary Science of Texts. Nor- . 1980. "The Logic of Narrative Possi-
wood, N.J.: Ablex. bilities." New Literary History 11:398-
Benjamin, Walter. 1969. Illuminations. Trans. 411.
Harry Zohn. New York: Schocken Books. . 1982. "A Critique of the Motif." In
3entley, Phyllis. 1946. Some Obsenations Todorov, French Literary Theory, pp.
on the Art of Narrative. London: Home & 125-46. See Barthes.1982.
Van Thai. . 1985. "Concept et theme." Poetique,
ienveniste, Emile. 1971. Problems in Gen- no. 64:415-23.
eral Linguistics. Trans. Mary Elizabeth Brinton, Laurel. 1980. '"Represented Per-
Meek. Coral Gables, Fla.:'University of ception': A Study in Narrative Style."
Miami Press. Poetics 10:363-81.
. 1974. Probl&mes de linguistique Bronzwaer, W.J.M. 1970. Tense in the
gintrale. II. Paris: Gallimard. Novel: /in Investigation of Some Poten-
Serendsen, Marjet. 1981. "Format Criteria of tialities of Linguistic Criticism. Groningen:
Narrative Embedding," Journal of Literary Wolters-Noordhoff.
Semantics 10:79-94. . 1978. "Implied Author, Extradiegetic
•ickerton, Derek. 1967. "Modes of Interior Narrator, and Public Reader: Gerard Ge-
Monologue: A Formal Definition." Modem nette's Narratological Model and the
Language Quarterly 28:229-39. Reading Version of Great Expectations."
lack, John B „ and Gordon H. Bower. 1980. Neophilologus 52:1-18.
"Story Understanding as Problem-Solv- Brooks, Cleanth, and Robert Penn Warren.
ing." Poetics 9:223-50. 1959. Understanding Fiction. 2d ed. New
lin, Georges. 1954. Stendhal et les pro- York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
blemes du roman. Paris: Corti. Brooks, Peter. 1984. Reading for the Plot:
Design and Intention in Narrative. New . 1952. "A Transformational Approach
York: A. A. Knopf. to Syntax." In A. A . Hill, ed., Proceedings
Brown. E. K. 1950. Rhythm in the Novel. To- of the 1958 Conference on Problems of
ronto: University of Toronto Press. Linguistic Analysis in English, pp. 1 2 4 -
Bruce, Bertram. 1978. "What Makes a Good 58. Austin: University of Texas Press.
Story?" Language Arts 55:460-66. . 1965. Aspects of the Theory of Syn-
Bruce, Bertram, and Denis Newman. 1973. tax. Cambridge: MIT Press.
"Interacting Plans." Cognitive Science Clark, Katerina, and Michaei Holquist. 1984.
2:195-233. Mikhail Bakhtin. Cambridge: Harvard
Buhler, Karl. 1934. Sprachtheorie: Die Dar- University Press.
steliungfunktion der Sprache. Jena: Cohn, Dorrit. 1966. "Narrated Monologue:
Gustav Fischer. Definition of a Fictional Style." Compara-
Buhler, Willi. 1337. Die 'erlebte Rede' im en- tive Literature 18:97-112.
glischen Roman: Ihre Vorstufen und ihre . 1978. Transparent Minds: Narrative
Ausbiidung im Werke Jane Austens. Zu- Modes for Presenting Consciousness in
rich : Max Niehaus Verlag. Fiction. Princeton: Princeton University
Chabrol, Claude. 1973. "De quelques pro- Press.
blames de grammaire narrative et . 1981. "The Encirclement of Narra-
textuelle." In Claude Chabrol, ed., Semio- tive: On Franz Stanzel's Theorie des
tique narrative et textuelle, pp. 7-28. Erzahlens." Poetics Today 2:157-82.
Paris: Larousse. Coiby, Benjamin N. 1973. "A Partial Gram-
Chafe, Wallace, ed. 1980. The Pear Stories: mar of Eskimo Folktales." American
Cultural, Cognitive, and Linguistic As- Anthropologist 75:645- 62.
pects of Narrative Production. Norwood, Courtes, Joseph. 1976. Introduction a is se-
N.J.: Ablex. miotique narrative et discursive. Paris:
Chambers, Ross. 1984. Story and Situation: Hachette.
Narrative Seduction and the Power of Crane, Ft. S. 1S52. "The Concept of Plot and
Fiction. Minneapolis: University of Minne- the Plot of Tom Jones." In R. S. Crane,
sota Press. ed., Critics and Criticism: Ancient and
Chatman, Seymour. 1978. Story and Dis- Modern, pp. 616-47. Chicago: University
course: Narrative Structure in Fiction and of Chicago Press.
Film. Ithaca: Cornell University Press. Culler, Jonathan. 1375. Structuralist Poetics:
. 1983. "On the Notion of Theme in Structuralism, Linguistics, and the Study
Narrative." In John Fisher, ed., Essays in of Literature. Ithaca: Cornell University
Aesthetics: Perspectives on the Work oI Press.
Monroe C. Beardsley, pp. 161-79. Phila- . 1981. The Pursuit of Signs: Semiot-
delphia: Temple University Press. ics, Literature, Deconstruction. Ithaca:
Chomsky, Noam. 1957. Syntactic Struc- Cornell University Press.
tures. The Hague: Mouton. Curtius, Ernst R. 1973. European Literature
and the Latin Middle Ages. Trans. Wil- . 1976. "Narrative Semantics." PTL
lard R. Trask. Princeton: Princeton 1:129-51.
University Press. . 1980. "Truth and Authenticity in Nar-
Daemmrich, Horst S., and Ingrid Daernmrich. rative." Poetics Today 1:7-25.
1986. Themes and Motifs in Western Lit- Dorfman, Eugene, 1968. The Narreme in the
erature: A Handbook. Tubingen: Francke. Medieval Romance Epic: An Introduction
Dallenbach, Lucien. 1977. Le Recit specu- to Narrative Structures. Toronto: Univer-
late. Paris: Seuii. sity of Toronto Press.
Danto, Arthur C. 1965. Analytical Philosophy Ducrot, Oswald, and Tzvetan Todorov. 1979.
of History. Cambridge: Harvard University Encyclopedic Dictionary of the Sciences
Press. of Language. Trans. Catherine Porter.
Debray-Genette, Raymonde. 1980. "La Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University
Piefre descriptive." Poetique, no. 43: Press.
293-304. Oujardin-, Edouard. 1931. Le Monologue ln-
. 1982. "Traversees de I'espace des- tfrieur. Paris: Messein.
criptif." Poetique, no. 51:359-68. Dundes, Alan. 1962. "From Eticto Ernie
van Dijk, Teun A. 1972. Some Aspects of Units in the Structural Study of Folk-
Text Grammars: A Study in Theoretical tales." Journal of American Folklore
Linguistics and Poetics. The Hague: 75:95-105.
Mouton. . 1964. The Morphology of North
. 1974-75. "Action, Action Descrip- American Indian Folktales. Helsinki:
tion, and Narrative." Poetics 5:287-338. Suomalainen Tiedeakatemia.
. 1976a. "Narrative Macro-Structures: Eco, Umberto. 1979. The Role of the
Logical and Cognitive Foundations." PTL Reader. Bloomington: Indiana University
1:547-68. Press.
. 1976b. "Philosophy of Action and . 1984. Semiotics and the Theory of
Theory of Narrative." Poetics 5:287-338. Language. Bloomington: indiana Univer-
. 1977. Text and Context: Explora- sity Press.
tions in the Semantics and Pragmatics of Egan, Kieran. 1978. "What Is a Plot?" New
Discourse. London: Longman. Literary History 9:455-73.
. 1980. "Story Comprehension: An In- Ejxenbaum, Boris M. 1971a. "O'Henry and
troduction." Poetics 9:1-21. the Theory of the Short Story." In Ladis-
Dillon, George, and Frederick Kirchhoff. lav Matejka and Krystyna Pomorska,
1976. "On the Form and Function of Free eds., Readings in Russian Poetics, pp.
Indirect Style." PTL 1:431-40. 227-70. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Oolezel, Lubomir. 1S72."From Motifemes to . 1971b. "The Theory of the Formal
Motifs." Poetics, no. 4:55-90. Method." Ibid., pp. 3 - 3 7 .
. 1973. Narrative Modes in Czech Lit- Ernpson, William. 1960. Some Versions of
erature. Toronto: Univeristy of Toronto Pastoral. New York: New Directions.
Press. Erlich, Victor. 1965. Russian Formalism: His-
tor/, Doctrine. 2d ed. The Hague: Narrative." Poetics 7:63-78.
Mouton. Genette, Gerard. 1368. "Vraisemb lance et
Falk, Eugene H. 1965. Types of Thematic motivation." Communications, no. 11: 5 -
Structure. Chicago: Univeristy of Chicago 21.
Press. — — . 1976. "Boundaries of Narrative."
Fehr, Bernhard. 1938. "Substitutionary Nar- New Literary History 8:1-15.
ration and Description: A Chapter in . 1980. Narrative Discourse: An Essay
Stylistics." English Studies 20:97-107. in Method. Trans. Jane E. Lewin. Ithaca:
Forster, E. M. 1927. Aspects of the Novel. Cornell University Press.
London: Methuen. — . 1982, Palimpsestes: La Litterature
Francoeur, Louis. 1976. "Le Monologue in- au socond degre. Paris: Seuil.
terieur narratif." Etudes Utteraires 9:341- . 1983. Nouveau Discours du recit.
65. Paris: Seuil.
Frank, Joseph. 1945. "Spatial Forrn in Mod- Genot, Gerard. 1979. Elements of Narrativ-
ern Literature." Sev/anee Review es: Grammar in Narrative, Narrative in
53:221-46, 433-56. Grammar. Hamburg: Helmut Buske Ver-
Freytag, Gustav. 1894. Technique of the lag.
Drama. Trans. E. J. McEwan. Chicago: . 1984. Grammaire et recit: Essai de
Scott. linguistique textuelle. Universite Paris
Friedemann, Kate. 1910. Die Rolle des Er- X—Nanterre: Documents du CRLLI 32.
zahlers in der Epik. Leipzig: H. Haessel Georges, Robert A. 1970. "Structure in Folk-
Verlag. tales: A Generative-Transformational
Friedman, Melvin. 1955. Stream of Con- Approach." The Conch 2:4-17.
sciousness: A Study in Literary Method. Gibson, Walker. 1950. "Authors, Speakers,
New Haven: Yale University Press. Readers, and Mock Readers." College
Friedman, Norman. 1955a. "Forms of the English 11:265-69.
Plot." Journal of General Education Glenn, Christine G. 1978. "The Role of Epi-
8:241-53. sodic Structure and Story Length in

. 1955b. "Point of View in Fiction: The Children's Recall of Simple Stories."

Development of a Critical Concept." Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal

PMLA 70:1160-84. Behavior 17:229-47.

. 1975. Form and Meaning in Fiction. Glowinski, Michal. 1974. "Der Dialog im Ro-

Athens: University of Georgia Press. man." Poetica 8:1-16.

Frye, Northrop. 1957. Anatomy of Criticism. . 1977. "On the First-Person Novel."
Princeton: Princeton University Press. New Uterary History 9:103-14.
Fuger, Wilhelm. 1972. "Zur Tiefenstruktur Goffman, Erving. 1974. Frame Analysis: An
des Narrativen: Prolegomena zu einer Essay on the Organization of Experience.
generativen 'Grammatik' des Erzahlens." Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Poetica 5:268-92. Greirnas, A. J. 1970. Du sens: Essais semio-
Garvey, James. 1978. "Characterization in tiques. Paris: Seuil.
—. 1971. "Narrative Grammar: Units tique generals. Paris: Presses
and Levels." MLN 86:783-806. Universitaires de France.
. 1983a. Du sens II: Essais semio- Hendricks, William 0 . 1973. Essays on Se-
tiques. Paris: Seuil. miolinguistics and Verbal Art. The
. 1983b. Structural Semantics: An At- Hague: Mouton.
tempt at a Method. Trans. Daniele Hernadi, Paul. 1972. "Dual Perspective:
McDowell, Ronald Schleifer, and Alan Free Indirect Discourse and Related
Velie. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Techniques." Comparative Literature
Press. 24:32-43.
Greimas, A. J., and Joseph Courtes. 1976. Hjelmslev, Louis. 1954. "La Stratification du
"The Cognitive Dimension of Narrative langage," Word 10:163-88.
Discourse." New Literary History 7:433- . 1961. Prolegomena to a Theory of
47. Language. Trans. Francis J. Whitfield.
. 1982. Semiotics and Language: An Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.
Analytical Dictionary. Trans. Larry Crist Hochrnan, Baruch. 1985. Character in Litera-
et al. Bloomington: Indiana University ture. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.
Press. Holman, Hugh C. 1972. A Handbook to Lit-
Grimes, Joseph. 1975. The Thread of Dis- erature. Based on the original by William
course. The Hague: Mouton. Flint Thrall and Addison Hibbard. 3d ed.
Hamburger, Kate. 1973. The Logic of Litera- Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merriil.
ture. Trans. Marilyn J. Rose, Horace. 1974. The Art of Poetry. Trans. Bur-
Bloomington: Indiana University Press. ton Raffs!. Albany: State University of
Hamon, Philippe. 1S72. "Pour un statut se- New York Press.
miologique du personnage." Litterature, Hough, Graham. 1970. "Narrative and Dia-
no. 6:86-110 logue in Jane Austen." Critical Quarterly
. 1975. "Clausules." Poetique, no. 12:201-29.
24:495-526. Humphrey, Robert. 1954. Stream of Con-
. 1977. "Texte litteraire et metalan- sciousness in the Modern Novel.
gage." Podtique, no. 31:261-34. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University ot
. 1981. Introduction a I'analyse du California Press.
descriptif. Paris: Hachette. Hutcheon, Linda. 1984. Narcissistic Narra-
. 1982. "What Is a Description?" In tive: The Metafictional Paradox. London:
Todorov, French Literary Theory, pp. Methuen. •
147-78. See Barthes 1982. . 1985. A Theory of Parody: The
. 1983. Le Personnel du roman. Ge- Teachings of Twentieth-Century Art
neva: Droz. Forms. London: Methuen.
Harvey, W. J. 1965. Character and the Hymes, Dell. 1970. "The Ethnography of
Novel. London: Chatto & Windus. Speaking." In Joshua A. Fishman, ed..
Henauit, Anne. 1983. Narratologie: Semio- Readings in the Sociology of Language,
pp. 99-138. The Hague: Mouton. Johnson, Nancy S., and Jean M. Mandler.
Ingarden, Roman. 1973. The Literary Work 1980. "A Tale of Two Structures: Undei
of Art. Trans. George G. Grabowicz. lying and Surface Forms in Stories."
Evanston: Northwestern University Press. Poetics 9:87-98.
Iser, Wolfgang. 1974. The implied Reader: Jolles, Andre. 1956. Einfache Formeri. Hal!
Patterns of Communication in Prose Fic- M. Miemayer.
tion from Bunyan to Beckett. Baltimore: Kayser, Wolfgang. 1958. "Wererzahlt den
Johns Hopkins University Press. Roman?" In Wolfgang Kayser, Die Vor-
. 1978. The Act of Reading: A Theory tragsreise: Studien zur Literatur, p p . 82
of Aesthetic Response. Baltimore: Johns 101. Bern: Francke Verlag.
Hopkins University Press. Kerbrat-Orecchioni, Catherine. 1980. L'Eno
Jakobson, Roman. 1956. "Two Aspects of ciation: De la subjectivity dans ie
Language and Two Types of Aphasic langage. Paris: Armand Colin.
Disturbances." In Roman Jakobson and Kermode, Frank. 1967. The Sense of an
Morris Halle, Fundamentals of Language, Ending. Mew York: Oxford University
pp. 53-82. The Hague: Mouton. Press.
- — — . 1960. "Closing Statement: Linguis- Kintsch, Walter, and Teun A, van Dijk, 197:
tics and Poetics." In Thomas A. Sebeok, "Recalling and Summarizing Stories."
ed., Style in Language, pp. 350-77. New Language 40:98-116.
York: Wiley. Kloepfer, Rolf. 1980. "Dynamic Structures ii
. 1971. "Shifters, Verbal Categories, Narrative Literature: 'The Dialogic Princi-
and the Russian Verb." In Roman Jakob- ple.' " Poetics Today 1:115-34.
son, Selected Writings, 2:130-47. The Kongas-Maranda, Elii, and Pierre Waranda.
Hague: Mouton. 1971. Structural Models in Folklore end
James, Henry. 1972. "The Art of Fiction, Transformational Essays. The Hague:
etc." In James E . Miller, Jr., ed., Theory Mouton.
of Fiction: Henry James. Lincoln: Univer- Kristeva, Julia. 1969. Semeiotike: Fte-
sity of Nebraska Press. cherches pour une semanalyse. Paris:
James, William. 1890. The Principles of Psy- Seuii.
chology. 2 vols. Nsw York: H. Holt. . 1984. Revolution in Poetic Lan-
Janik, Dieter. 1973. Die Kommunikation- guage. Trans. Margaret Waller. New
strucktur des Erzahlwerks: Em York: Columbia University Press.
semiologisches Model. Bebenhausen: Labov, William. 1972. Language in the Mm
Lothar Rotsch. City. Philadelphia: University of Pennsyl-
Jenny, Laurent. 1982. "The Strategy of vania Press.
Form." In Todorov, French Literary The- Labov, William, and Joshua Waletzky. 1967
ory, pp. 34-63. See Barthes 1982. "Narrative Analysis: Oral Versions of Pe
Jespersen, Otto. 1924. The Philosophy of sonai Experience." In June Helm, ed.,
Grammar. London: Allen & Unwin. Essays on the Verbal and Visual Arts:
Proceedings of the 19S6 Annual Spring Writing: Metaphor, Metonymy, and the
Meeting, pp. 12-44. Seattle: University Typology of Literature. Ithaca: Cornell
of Washington Press, University Press.
koff, George. 1972. "Structural Complexity Lorck, Jean Etienne. 1921. Die erlebte
in Fairy Tales." Study of Man 1:128-50. Rede: Eine sprachliche Untersuchung.
mmert, Eberhart. 1955. Bauformen des Heidelberg: Carl Winter.
Erzahlens. Stuttgart: J. B. Metzlersche Lotman, Jurij. 1977. The Structure of the Ar-
Verlag. tistic Text. Trans. Gail Lenhoff and
riser, Susan Sniader. 1981. The Narrative Ronald Vroon. Michigan Slavic Contribu-
Act: Point of View in Fiction. Princeton: tions, no. 7. Ann Arbor: University of
Princeton University Press, Michigan.
rivaille, Paul. 1974. "L'Analyse (morpho) Lubbock, Percy. 1965. The Craft of Fiction.
logique du recit." Poetique, no. 19: 368- London: Jonathan Cape.
88. Lyons, John. 1977. Semantics. 2 vols. Cam-
bfreid, Erwin. 1970. Kritische Wissen- bridge: Cambridge University Press.
schaft vom Text: Manipulation, Reflexion, McHale, Brian. 1978. "Free Indirect Dis-
transparente Poetologie. Stuttgart: J. B, course: A Survey of Recent Accounts."
Metzler. PTL 3:249-288.
eune, Philippe. 1975. Le Facte autobio- . 1983. "Unspeakable Sentences, Un-
graphique. Paris: Seuil. natural Acts: Linguistics and Poetics
. 1982. "The Autobiographical Con- Revisited." Poetics Today 1:17-45.
tract." In Todorov, French Literary Madden, David. 1980. A Primer of the Novel:
Theory, pp. 192-222. See Barthes 1982. For Readers and Writers. Metuchen,
mon, Lee T., and Marion J. Reis, eds. N.J.: Scarecrow Press.
1965. Russian Formalist Criticism. Lin- Magny, Claude-Edmonde. 1972. The Age of
coln: University of Nebraska Press. the American Novel. Trans. Eleanor
/i-Strauss, Claude. 1963. Structural An- Hochman. New York: Ungar.
thropology. Trans. Claire Jacobson and Malinowski, Bronislaw. 1953. "The Problem
Brooke G. Schoepf. New York: Basic of Meaning in Primitive Languages." In
Books. C. K. Ogden and I. A. Richards, eds.,
. 1965-71. Mythologlques. 4 vols. The Meaning of Meaning, pp. 296-336.
Paris: Plon. New York: Oxford University Press.
dell, Robert. 1947. A Treatise on the j Mandler, Jean M„ and Nancy S. Johnson.
Novel. London: J. Cape, 1977. "Remembrance of Things Parsed:
rvelt, Jaap. 1981. Essai de typologie nar- Story Structure and Recall." Cognitive
rative: Le 'point de vue.' Paris: Corti. Psychology 9:11-151.
3, Marguerite. 1926. Le Style indirect Margolin, Uri. 1983. "Characterization in
fibre. Paris: Payot. Narrative: Some Theoretical Prolego-
ige, David. 1977. The Modes of Modern mena." Neophilologus 67:1-14.
Martin, Wallace. 1986. Recent Theories ol Mitchell, W.J.T., ed. 1981. On Narrative. Chi-
Narrative. Ithaca: Cornell University cago: University of Chicago Press.
Press. Morgan, Thai's E. 1985. "Is There an Inter-
Martfnez-Bonati, Felix. 1981. Fictive Dis- text in This Text? Literary and
course and the Structure of Uterature: A Interdisciplinary Approaches to Intertex-
Phenorvenological Approach. Trans. tuality." American Journal of Semiotics
Philip W. Silver. Ithaca: Cornell University 3:1-40.
Press. Morrissette, Bruce. 1965. "Narrative 'You' in
Matejka, Ladislav, and Krystyna Pornorska, Contemporary Literature," Comparative
eds. 1971. Readings in Russian Poetics. Literature Studies 2:1-24.
Cambridge: MIT Press. Mosher, Harold F. 1980. "A New Synthesis
Mathieu, Michel. 1974. "Les Acteurs du re- of Narratology." Poetics Today 1:171-86.
cit." Podtique, no. 19: 357-67. Muller, Gunther. 1968. Morphologische Poe-
Mathieu-Colas, Michel. 1986. "Frontieres de tik. Tubingen: Max Niemeyer.
la narratologie." Poetique, no. 65:91-110. O'Grady, Walter. 1965. "On Plot in Modern
Mendilow, A. A. 1952. Time and the Novel. Fiction: Hardy, James, and Conrad."
London: P. Nevill. Modern Fiction Studies 11:107-15.
Metz, Christian. 1974. Film Language: A Se- Page, Norman. 1973. Speech in the English
miotics of the Cinema. Trans. Michael Novel. London: Longman.
Taylor. New York: Oxford University Palmer, F. R. 1971. Semantics. 2d ed. Cam-
Press. bridge: Cambridge University Press.
Miller, D. A. 1981. Narrative and Its Discon- Pascal, Roy. 1962. "Tense and Novel."
tents: Problems of Closure in the Modern Language Review 57:1-11.
Traditional Novel. Princeton: Princeton . 1977. The Dual Voice: Free Indireci
University Press. Speech and Its Functioning in the Nine-
Mink, Louis 0 . 1 9 6 9 - 7 0 . "History and Fic- teenth-Century European Novel.
tion as Modes of Comprehension." New Manchester: Manchester University
Literary History 1:541-58. Press.
. 1978. "Narrative Form as a Cogni- Pavel, Thomas. 1976. La Syntaxe narrative
tive Instrument." In Robert H. Canary and des tragedies de Comeille. Paris: Klinck-
Henry Kozicki, eds., The Writing of His- sieck.
tory: Literary Form and Historical . 1980. "Narrative Domains." Poetics
Understanding, pp. 129-49. Madison: Today 1:105-14.
University of Wisconsin Press. . 1985. The Poetics of Plot: The Case
Minsky, Marvin. 1975. "A Framework for ol English Renaissance Drama. Minneap-
Representing Knowledge." In Patrick olis: University of Minnesota Press.
Winston, ed., The Psychology of Com- Piaget, Jean. 1970. Structuralism. Trans.
puter Vision, pp. 211-77. New York: Chaninah Maschler. New York: Basic
McGraw-Hill. Books.
Pike, Kenneth L. 1967. Language in Rela- sage, and Point." Poetics 12:527-36.
tion to a Unified Theory of the Structure . 1985. "Th6matiser." Poetique, no.
of Human Behavior. The Hague: Mouton. 64:425-33.
Piwowarczyk, Mary Ann. 1976. "The Narra- Propp, Vladimir. 1968. Morphology of the
tee and the Situation of Enunciation: A Folktale. 2d ed. Trans. Laurence Scott.
Reconsideration of Prince's Theory." Austin: University of Texas Press.
Genre 9:161-77. . 1984. "Transformations of the Won-
Plato. 1968. The Republic. Trans. Allan dertale." in Vladimir Propp, Theory and
Bloom. New York: Basic Books. History of Folklore, pp. 82-99. Ed. Ana-
Polanyi, Livia. 1979. "So What's the Point?" toly Liberman. Trans. Ariadna Y. Martin
Semiotica 25:207-41. and Richard P. Martin. Minneapolis: Uni-
Pottier, Bernard. 1964. "Vers une seman- versity of Minnesota Press.
tique moderne." Travaux de linguistique Rabinowitz, Peter. 1977. 'Truth in Fiction: A
et de litt£rature 2:107-37. Reexamination of Audiences." Critical In-
Pouillon, Jean. 1946. Temps et roman. quiry 4:121-41.
Paris: Gallimard. Rabkin, Eric S. 1973. Narrative Suspense:
Pratt, Mary Louise. 1977. Toward a Speech "When Slim Turned Sideways . . . ."
Act Theory of Literary Discourse. Bloom- Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
ington: Indiana University Press. Rastier, Franfois. 1973. Essais de semio-
Prince, Gerald. 1973. A Grammar of Stories. tique discursive. Tours: Mame.
The Hague: Mouton. Ricardou, Jean. 1967. Problemes du nou-
. 1977. "Remarques sur les signes veau roman. Paris: Seuil.
meianarratifs." Degrds, nos. 11-12:e1- . 1971. Pour une theorie du nouveau
e10. roman. Paris: Seuil.
. 1978. "Le Discours attributif et le r& . 1973. Le Nouveau Roman. Paris:
cit." Poetique, no. 35:305-13. Seuil.
. 1980. "Introduction to the' Study ef — — . 1978. Nouveaux Problemes du ro-
the Narratee." In Jane P. Tompkins, ed., man. Paris: Seuil.
Reader-Response Criticism, pp. 7-25. Richards, I. A. 1950. Practical Criticism: A
Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Study of Literary Judgment. New York:
Press. Harcourt, Brace.
. 1981. "Reading and Narrative Com- Ricoeur, Paul. 1984. Time and Narrative.
petence." L'Esprit Creaieur 21:81-88. Vol. 1. Trans. Kathleen McLaughlin and
. 1981-82. "Nanative Analysis and David Pellauer. Chicago: University of
Narratology." New Literary History Chicago Press.
13:179-88. . 1985. Time and Narrative. Vol. 2.
. 1982. Narratology: The Form and Trans. Kathleen McLaughlin and David
Functioning of Narrative. Berlin: Mouton. Pellauer. Chicago: University of Chicago
. 1983. "Nanative Pragmatics, Mes- Press.
Riffaterre, Michael. 1972. "Systeme d'un in Narratology." Semiotica 28:127-55.
genre descriptif." Poetique, no. 9:15-30. . 1981. "The Pragmatics of Personal
. 1S72-73. "Interpretation and De- and impersonal Fiction." Poetics 10:517—
scriptive Poetry." New Literary History 39.
4:229-56. . 1984. "Fiction as a Logical, Ontolog-
. 1978. Semiotics of Poetry. Bloom- ical, and Illocutionary Issue," Style
ington: Indiana University Press. 18:121-39.
. 1980. "La Trace de I'intertexte." La . 1985. "The Modal Structure of Nar-
Pensie, no. 215:4-18. rative Universes." Poetics Today 6:717-
. 1983. Text Production. Trans. 55.
Terese Lyons. New York: Columbia Uni- Sacks, Harvey. 1972. "On the Analyzability
versity Press. of Stories by Children." In John J. Gum-
Rimmon, Shlomith. 1976. "A Comprehensive perz and Dell Hymes, eds., Directions in
Theory of Narrative: Genette's Figures III Sociolinguistics, pp. 325-45. New York:
and the Structuralist Study of Fiction." Holt, Rinenart and Winston.
PTL 1:33-62. Said, Edward. 1975. Beginnings: Intention
Rimmon-Kenan, Shiomith. 1983. Narrative and Method. New York: Basic Books.
Fiction: Contemporary Poetics. London: Sartre, Jean-Paul. 1965. What Is Literature?
Methuen. Trans. Bernard Frechtman. New York:
. 1985. "Qu'est-ce qu'un thfeme?" Harper & Row.
Politique, no. 64:397-406. Saussure, Ferdinand de. 1966. Course in
Rogers, B. G. 1965. Proust's Narrative General Linguistics. Trans. Wade Baskin.
Techniques. Geneva: Droz. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Romberg, Bertil. 1962. Studies in the Narra- Schank, Roger. 1975. "The Structure of Epi-
tive Technique of First-Person Novel. sodes in Memory." In Daniel G. Bobrow
Lund: Almqvist & Wiksell. and Allan Collins, eds., Representation
van Rossum-Guyon, Frangoise. 1970. "Point and Understanding: Studies in Cognitive
de vue ou perspective narrative." Poe- Science, pp. 237-72. New York: Aca-
tique, no. 4:476-97. demic Press.
Rousset, Jean. 1973. Narcisse romancier: Schank, Roger, and Robert Abeison. 1977.
Essai sur la premiere personne dans le Scripts, Plans, Goals, and Understand-
roman. Paris: Corti. ing. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum
Rumelhart, David E. 1975. "Notes on a Associates.
Schema for Stories." In Daniel G. Bob- Schmid, Wolf. 1973. Der Textaufbau in den
row and Allan Collins, eds., Erzahlungen Dostoevskijs. Munich: Fink.
Representation and Understanding: Scholes, Robert. 1974. Structuralism in Liter-
Studies in Cognitive Science, pp. 211- ature: An Introduction. New Haven: Yale
36. New York: Academic Press. University Press.
Ryan, Marie-Laure. 1979. "Linguistic Models Scholes, Robert, and Robert Kellogg. 1966.
The Nature of Narrative. New York: Ox- Stanzel, Franz. 1964. Typische Formen des
ford University Press. Roman. Gottingen: Vandenhoeck & Ru-
Searie, John. 1969. Speech Acts. Cam- precht.
bridge: Cambridge University Press. . 1971. Narrative Situations in the
. 1975. "Indirect Speech Acts." In Pe- Novel: "Tom Jones," "Moby-Dick," "The
ter Coles and Jerry L. Morgan, eds., Ambassadors," "Ulysses." Trans. James
Syntax and Semantics ill: Speech Acts, P. Pusack. Bloomington: Indiana Univer-
pp. 59-82. New York: Academic Press. sity Press.
. 1976. "A Classification of lllocution- . 1984. A Theory of Narrative. Trans.
ary Acts." Language in Society 5:1-23. Charlotte Goedsche. Cambridge: Cam-
Segre, Cesare. 1979. Structure and Time: bridge University Press.
Narration, Poetry, Models. Trans. John Stein, Nancy L. 1982. "The Definition of a
Meddemmen. Chicago: University of Chi- Story," Journal of Pragmatics 6:487-
cago Press. 507.
Shapiro, Marianne. 1984. "How Narrators Steinberg, Gunter. 1971. Erlebte Rede: Ihre
Report Speech." Language and Style Eigenart und ihre Formen in neuerer
17:67-78. deutscher, franzosischer und englischer
Shklovsky, Victor. 1965a. "Art as Tech- Erzahlliteratur. 2 vols. Goppingen: A.
nique." In Lemon and Reis 1965, pp. 3 - Kiimmerle.
24. Sternberg, Meir. 1974. "What is Exposition?"
. 1965b. "Sterne's Tristram Shandy: !n John Haiperin, ed., The Theory of the
Stylistic Commentary." Ibid., pp. 25-57. Novel. New York: Oxford University
Smith, Barbara Herrnstein. 1968. Poetic Clo- Press.
sure: A Study of How Poems End. . 1978. Expositional Modes and Tem-
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. poral Ordering in Fiction. Baltimore:
. 1981. "Narrative Versions, Narrative Johns Hopkins University Press.
Theories." In W.J.T. Mitchell, e d „ On Strauch, G6rard. 1974. "De quelques inter-
Narrative, pp. 209-32. Chicago: Univer- polations r^centes du style indirect
sity of Chicago Press. libre." Recherches Anglaises et Ameri-
Souriau, Etienne. 1950. Les Deux Cent Mille caines 7:40-73.
Situations dramatiques. Paris: Flamma- Suleiman, Susan R. 1980. "Redundancy and
rion. the 'Readable' Text." Poetics Today
Souvage, Jacques. 1965. An Introduction to 1:113-42.
the Study of the Novel, with Special Ref- . 1983. Authoritarian Fictions: The
erence to the English Novel. Ghent: E. Ideological Novel as a Literary Genre.
Siory-Scientia P.V.B.A. New York: Columbia University Press.
Spitzer, Leo. 1928. "Zum Sill Marcel Tacca, Oscar. 1973. Las Voces de la no-
Proust." Stilstudien II, pp. 365-497. Mu- vela. Madrid: Editorial Grados.
nich: Max Hueber. Tamir, Nomi. 1976. "Personal Narration ami
Its Linguistic Foundation." PTL 1:403-29, Vitoux, Pierre. 1982. "Le Jeu de la focalisa-
Thorndyke, Perry W. 1977. "Cognitive Struc- tion," Poetique, no. 51:359-68.
tures in Comprehension and Memory of VoloSinov, V. N. 1973. Marxism and the Phi-
Narrative Discourse." Cognitive Psychol- losophy ol Language. Trans. Ladislav
ogy 9:77-110. Matejka and I. R. Titunik. New York:
Tillotson, Kathleen. 1959. The Tale and the Seminar Press.
Teller. London: Rupert Hart-Davis. Weimann, Robert. 1973. "Point of View in
Titunik, liwin Robert. 1963. The Problem ol Fiction." In Gaylord C. Leroy and Ursula
'Skaz' in Russian Literature. Ph.D. thesis. Beitz, eds., Preserve and Create: Essays
University of California—Berkeley. Repro- in Marxist Literary Criticism, pp. 54-75.
duced by microfilm-xerography by New York: Humanities Press.
University Microfilms International, 1976. Weinrich, Harald. 1964. Tempus, Besproch-
Todorov, Tzvetan. 1966. "Les Categories du ene und Erzahlte Welt. Stuttgart:
r6cit litteraire." Communications, no. 8: W. Kohlhammer.
125-51. Wellek, Rene, and Austin Warren. 194$.
. 1969. Crammaire du "Decameron." Theory of Literature. New York: Harcourt,
The Hague: Mouton. Brace.
. 1978. The Poetics ol Prose. Trans. White, Hayden. 1973. Metahistory: The His-
Richard Howard. Ithaca: Cornell Univer- torical Imagination in Nineteenth-Century
sity Press. Europe. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Uni-
. 1981. Introduction to Poetics. Trans. versity Press.
Richard Howard. Minneapolis: University Wilensky, Robert. 1S78. Understanding
of Minnesota Press. Goal-Based Stories. Research Report
Tomashevsky, Boris. 1965. "Thematics." In no. 140, Department of Computer Sci-
Lemon and Reis 1965, pp. 61-95. ence, Yale University, New Haven.
Torgovnick, Marianna. 1981. Closure in the Winograd, Terry. 1975. "Frame Representa-
Novel. Princeton: Princeton University tions and the Deciarative-Procedurai
Press. Controversy." In Daniel G. Bobrow and
Uspenskij, Boris. A Poetics of Composition: Allan Collins, eds., Representation and
The Structure of the Artistic Text and Ty- Understanding: Studies in Cognitive Sci-
pology of a Compositional Form. Trans. ence, pp. 185-210. New York: Academic
Valentina Zavarin and Susan Wittig. Press.
Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of Wittmann, Henri. 1975. "Theorie des nsr-
California Press. remes et algorithmes narratifs." Poetics
Vinogradov, V. V. 1980. "The Problem of 4:19-28.
Skaz in Stylistics." In Ellendea Proffer Wright, Austin. 1982. The Formal Principle in
and Carl R. Proffer, eds., The Ardis An- the Novel. Ithaca: Cornell University
thology of Russian Futurism. Ann Arbor: Press.
Ardis. Wright, Terence. 1985. "Rhythm in the
Novel." Modern Language Review 80:1-
15.
Zeraffa, Michel. 1969. Personne el person-
nage. Paris: Klincksieck.
Zhoikovsky, Alexander. 1984. Themes and
Texts: Toward a Poetics ot Expressive-
ness. Translated by the author and
edited by Kathleen Parte. Ithaca: Cornell
University Press.
Zoran, Gabriel. 1984. "Towards a Theory of
Space in Narrative." Poetics Today
5:309-35.
i
.1

i
!

You might also like