You are on page 1of 11

Language Textbook Selection: Using

Materials Analysis from the Perspective of


SLA Principles

Marie J. Guilloteaux

The Asia-Pacific Education


Researcher

ISSN 0119-5646

Asia-Pacific Edu Res


DOI 10.1007/s40299-012-0015-3

1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and
all rights are held exclusively by De La Salle
University. This e-offprint is for personal
use only and shall not be self-archived in
electronic repositories. If you wish to self-
archive your work, please use the accepted
author’s version for posting to your own
website or your institution’s repository. You
may further deposit the accepted author’s
version on a funder’s repository at a funder’s
request, provided it is not made publicly
available until 12 months after publication.

1 23
Author's personal copy
Asia-Pacific Edu Res
DOI 10.1007/s40299-012-0015-3

Language Textbook Selection: Using Materials Analysis


from the Perspective of SLA Principles
Marie J. Guilloteaux

 De La Salle University 2012

Abstract This paper outlines a procedure for language Introduction


textbook analysis from the perspective of second language
acquisition (SLA) principles as a preliminary procedure to Textbooks continue to be a central feature of language
evaluation for selection. The aim is to provide a tool that classrooms worldwide; their quality can, therefore, affect the
allows comparison of the potential of textbooks for sup- learning experiences of many students. Nonetheless, the
porting students’ language learning. To this end, ten gen- textbook selection process—which typically relies on pre-
eral principles representing a spectrum of SLA theories dictive evaluation (Ellis 1997a)—is not always efficient.
were drawn from the literature. Then five EFL textbooks According to Tomlinson (2003), it is not unknown for pub-
produced in South Korea were analyzed using a minimally lishers, teachers, and ministries to choose textbooks that only
modified version of Littlejohn’s (Materials development in create ‘‘an illusion of language learning’’ (Tomlinson 2008,
language tea, 1998, 2011) task analysis schedule. The low- p. 8). A potential avenue for increasing the efficiency of pre-
to-medium inference instrument enabled the analysis of all dictive evaluation is to use insights from the second language
the activities contained in the sampled units, yielding fre- acquisition (SLA) research since it can reveal some strengths
quency tallies. Final rankings indicated that the procedure and weaknesses in the construction of language learning
did discriminate between the sampled textbooks, revealing materials (Cook 2008). Indeed, several publications in the
a clear cluster of three books that reflected SLA principles materials development literature have suggested SLA-based
more closely. Although only in-use evaluation can deter- principles for materials design (e.g., Cook 1998; Richards
mine the extent to which these textbooks actually support 2006; Tomlinson 1998, 2010, 2011). However, the use of
language acquisition, the evidence suggests that they are instructed SLA research output is still an underexplored area
potentially more supportive than the lower scoring books. in materials evaluation (for an exception, see Aski 2003).
The procedure outlined in this paper could be of use to The present study seeks to offer a more efficient procedure
language educators, those involved in language materials for the selection of textbooks (used here interchangeably with
publishing, and government officials involved in the learning/teaching materials) by introducing a preliminary
approval of language materials. materials analysis phase into materials evaluation. It uses a
recognized task analysis schedule (Littlejohn 1998, 2011) as
Keywords Language materials  Textbook evaluation  an operational framework to evaluate whether textbooks
Second language acquisition  Korea reflect universal principles representing a spectrum of com-
peting SLA theories. The aim is to provide a tool that allows
comparison of the potential of textbooks for supporting the
students’ second language (L2) learning. Note that the term
second in SLA usually refers to either or both the second and/
M. J. Guilloteaux (&) or foreign language because the processes involved in both
Department of English Education, College of Education,
are fundamentally similar although they occur in different
Gyeongsang National University, 501 Jinju-daero,
Jinju 660-701, South Korea situations (Ellis 1997b; Ortega 2009). A caveat is in order,
e-mail: marie@gnu.ac.kr however. In order to make a complete predictive evaluation,

123
Author's personal copy
M. J. Guilloteaux

the kind of analysis proposed here needs to be complemented teachers’ and learners’ roles. Of particular interest here is
by a further step using criteria related to the local context, the second level because of its focus on activities as units of
teacher and learner factors (including age and level), available analysis. It enables analysts to describe every teaching/
resources, and curriculum (including teaching time and con- learning activity contained in a textbook and is composed of
tents) (Tomlinson 2003). This second evaluative phase is not three parts: What is the learner expected to do? (deals with
the focus of this paper. learners’ role, activity focus, and broad cognitive processes
involved); Who with? (deals with participation structure/
interaction); With what contents? (deals with input/output
Materials Selection types). These three areas can be informed by SLA research
and can, therefore, serve to assess the extent to which
A quality textbook should be a model of good teaching materials may support students’ language learning.
(Newton and Newton 2009) and should facilitate students’
language learning (Cook 1998; Tomlinson 2008, 2011;
Waters 2009). However, language practitioners often Materials Analysis and SLA
choose a textbook based on cursory impressionistic evalu-
ations (Ellis 1997a; Tomlinson 2011) and practical factors The field of SLA has reached a stage of maturity where
unrelated to pedagogy. Even if practitioners use one of the materials design and evaluation can benefit from input from
numerous checklists and questionnaires developed since the its research (Cook 1998). This does not mean, however, that
1970s to make their predictive evaluation more systematic there is consensus among SLA researchers about how
(for recent comprehensive reviews, see Mukundan and instruction can best benefit from SLA input. In fact, SLA
Ahour 2010; Tomlinson 2012), the selected materials may researchers have urged caution in applying SLA-derived
still not provide optimal conditions for language learning. principles to pedagogy (see Ellis 2005). But while the use of
This is because those instruments often rely on many vague, principles as narrow prescriptions is commonly discour-
locally specific, and sometimes dogmatic criteria. Criteria aged, it has been suggested that SLA-based principles can
include learners’ needs, goals, learning styles, and profi- be employed as predictions to inform pedagogical decisions
ciency levels; classroom contexts and processes; the made in or for the classroom (e.g., Ellis 1997b; Waters
potential of the materials for generating motivation, variety, 2009). Consequently, SLA principles could serve to explain
and interest; and language teaching methods (Rubdy 2003). and critique materials design features by identifying the
Additionally, the judgments elicited by such criteria tend to extent to which materials are ‘‘in harmony with how
be subjective and difficult to quantify, hence unreliable learners learn’’ (Ellis 1997c, p. 78). A few lists that include
(Littlejohn 1992; Tomlinson 2012). SLA-based principles can be found in relation to materials
One way of making textbook selection more objective is design (see Cook 1998; Richards 2006; Tomlinson 2003,
to build a materials analysis phase into predictive evalua- 2010), each representing the authors’ personal selections.
tion. According to Tomlinson (2003), who distinguishes However, these selections do not attempt to represent the
between materials evaluation and materials analysis, the spectrum of major competing theories in SLA—an
objective of predictive materials evaluations is to make approach close to what Swan (2006) would call subtractive
judgments about the effects that the materials may have on (i.e., emphasizing some particular theories or findings at the
their intended users; thus it is essentially subjective. In expense of others), as opposed to additive (i.e., embracing
contrast, materials analysis attempts to describe materials all possible theories unless there is considerable evidence
as they are against set criteria; it is, therefore, more against them). Moreover, the lists tend to also include
objective. Viewed this way, materials analysis can be pedagogical principles more context-relevant in nature.
considered as a preliminary step to materials evaluation What follows (Table 1) represents the author’s attempt to
(Littlejohn 1998). Regrettably, many checklists mix eval- select universal SLA-based principles (i.e., relevant to any
uation-type items (i.e., unipolar or bipolar items) and learner in any context: Tomlinson 2003). These principles can
analysis items (i.e., yes/no questions, frequency counts). be used to interpret the results of materials analyses carried
One exception is Littlejohn’s (1998, 2011) framework for out with Littlejohn’s (1998, 2011) scheme for analysis of
materials analysis (Tomlinson 2003). textbook activities. The approach used for selecting the
Littlejohn’s (1998, 2011) framework consists of three SLA principles listed in Table 1 was additive (Swan 2006).
levels of analysis requiring progressively more inference. That is, the selection aimed to embody the core elements
Level 1 assesses the physical aspects of the materials and presently known as representing potentially favorable (and
the main instructional steps within a typical unit. Level 2 unfavorable) conditions for instructed L2 learning from a
analyzes each textbook activity in detail. Level 3 requires psycholinguistic viewpoint. A psycholinguistic perspective
deduction of what is implied regarding the syllabus and the was favored here because it views ‘‘the learner as operating

123
Author's personal copy
Language Textbook Selection

and developing a relatively autonomous L2 system, and Table 1 Universal materials analysis criteria derived from SLA
see[s] interaction as a way of feeding that system with more or principles
less fine-tuned input data’’ (Mitchell and Myles 2001, p. 22). What supports language acquisition
In contrast, a sociolinguistic SLA views language learning • A predominant focus on meaning: mostly pragmatic meaning
essentially as a process in which ‘‘both the identity of the (i.e., what language forms convey in communication); but also
learner, and their language knowledge, are collaboratively semantic meaning (i.e., what language conveys, irrespective of
context, when language is treated as an object of study) (Ellis
constructed and reconstructed in the course of interaction’’ 2005)
(Mitchell and Myles 2001, p. 22). The latter process refers to
• Meaningful practice of unanalyzed formulaic expressions (as in
interaction between learners who are scaffolded into using a notional-functional approach), especially in the early stages:
new L2 forms and also socialized into a range of discourse The rote-memorized patterns will be available for analysis later
practices by expert speakers. Consequently, a sociolinguistic when the learners are developmentally ready, thereby
contributing to the ability to use language fluently and accurately
perspective is considered in this paper to be more relevant to
(Cook 1998; DeKeyser 2010; Ellis; Richards 2006)
the second language learning contexts (e.g., ESL) than to
• Participation in communicative activity (Tomlinson and
foreign language classrooms (e.g., EFL contexts) where Masuhara 2010) to boost learners’ implicit knowledge of the L2
learners often share the same first language and interact with (Ellis)
other learners of similar levels in the L2, and where the teacher • Focus on form (e.g., corrective feedback) and intensive focus on
may not even be an expert L2 user. Finally, the selection of pre-selected linguistic forms, by an inductive and/or deductive
approach (Ellis)
principles presented in Table 1 also represents a synthesis of
the main SLA principles proposed by Cook (1998), Ellis • Exposure to extensive language in use (Ellis 2005; Tomlinson
2010)
(2005), Richards (2006), Tomlinson (2010), and Tomlinson
• Opportunities for free production of output of a clause or more,
and Masuhara (2010). but not the type produced during controlled practice exercises
The sections that follow illustrate how Littlejohn’s (Ellis; Tomlinson 2010)
(1998, 2011) task analysis schedule could be used as an • Pair and group activities that promote interaction in the L2
operational framework to answer the following question: (Lantolf 2000; Long 1996), but excessive use of the L1 in
To what extent do sampled L2 textbooks reflect universal monolingual contexts may actually hinder it (Ellis)
SLA principles? • Engaging learners cognitively in a variety of a learning
activities (Ellis), and through high instructional clarity,
achievable challenges, awareness of different learning styles, and
strategy training, including experiential and learner training
activities (Ellis; Richards 2006; Tomlinson and Masuhara 2010)
Method
• Engaging learners affectively by providing relevant and
interesting contents, and giving opportunities to express own
Sample meanings and choose own topics (Richards 2006; Tomlinson and
Masuhara 2010)
The materials included five (out of eight) Middle School What seems to be of limited use in language acquisition
English I (Student Book) textbooks published in 2009 and • Mechanical drills requiring exclusive attention to form divorced
approved by the South Korean Ministry of Education, from meaning (De Keyser 2010); mechanical drills may even be
of no use at all (Wong and Van Patten 2003)
Science, and Technology. The books were selected ran-
domly because all of them had to follow similar basic
ministry-regulated formats, had passed the same stringent
term and intermediate-term memory, constructs that are
ministry-approval review process, and were thus assumed
seldom used nowadays in contemporary memory research
to be of similar quality in terms of supporting students’
(Ortega 2009).
learning. Following Littlejohn (1998), one unit represent-
Activities were delineated according to Littlejohn’s
ing about 10 % of each book was selected for analysis from
(1992) definition of a task as any activity that facilitates
the midsection of the books (see descriptions in Table 2).
learning and includes all of the following three elements:
‘‘(1) a process through which the learners are expected to
Materials Analysis Instrument go; (2) a mode of classroom participation concerning with
whom (if anyone) the learners are to work; (3) content
The materials in this study were analyzed using a mini- upon which learners are to engage’’ (p. 37). A change in
mally modified version of the second level of Littlejohn’s any of the three elements (e.g., in the mode of participa-
(1998, 2011) materials analysis framework, which con- tion) generates a new task only when it has a clear peda-
cerns what learners are expected to do during tasks (see gogical purpose. For instance, in this study (where task is
Tables 3, 4, 5, 6, 7). Simplifications were made such as always defined as in Littlejohn), simply reading aloud with
dropping items that made reference to retrieval from short- a partner the contents of a dialog that had first required

123
Author's personal copy
M. J. Guilloteaux

Table 2 Sampled materials and


Book Publishers of sampled Popularity Sampled Core textbook Tasks
number of tasks identified in
code Middle School English 1 rankingsa unit (%) identified
sampled lessons
(Student Book)

A Chunjae 5 6 10 29
B Doosan 1 6 8.33 26
a
The popularity rankings are C Joheunchaek-Sinsago 7 6 9.09 37
based on the sales figures (2011) D Jihaksa 6 6 8.33 26
for the third level in the series, E Biyuwasangjin 8 6 8.33 24
Middle School English 3

Table 3 Percentage of tasks


I. What is the learner expected Books Mean
having features from Section I
to do? (%)
A (TURN-TAKE) and textbook A% B% C% D% E%
scores (points) (points) (points) (points) (points)

A. TURN-TAKE
(?) Initiate language 24 (5) 15 (4) 5 (2) 4 (1) 13 (3) 12
• E.g., make a list; produce a
text based on a sample or
basic rhetorical frame
(?) Respond 76 (2) 81 (3) 92 (5) 92 (5) 75 (1) 83
• In narrowly defined
language/non-verbal
response (e.g., identify
picture; true/false; gap-
filler; sequence sentences)
(-) No action required 0 (-0) 4 (-3) (-2) 4 (-3) 13 (-5) 5
• E.g., read/listen—no other
activity

Subsection score 7 4 5 3 -1

some basic individual manipulation was considered to be frequency analysis of their features. The author coded
one pair-work task. Ambiguous tasks were delineated 100 % of the data once (i.e., 6672 judgments) and entered
according to their main purpose, which was inferred from the tallies into an SPSS file. The coding was repeated some
their contents. time later on 100 % of the data. The two assistants each
coded 50 % of the data. Intra- and inter-rater reliability was
Data Processing estimated with Cohen’s Kappa (a measure of reliability of
agreement between two raters which is corrected for chance
The author conducted the analysis of the materials inde- occurrence). The results for intra-rater reliability were
pendently with two assistants who were trained by the Kappa = .91, and for inter-rater reliability Kappa = .61
author in a two-hour seminar on Littlejohn’s (1998, 2011) and .62 for Assistant 1 and 2, respectively. Landis and Koch
framework for materials analysis and in a separate session (1977, p. 165) suggest that a Kappa coefficient of .81–1.00
during which sample materials were analyzed indepen- represents ‘‘almost perfect’’ agreement, and a coefficient of
dently. Results were then compared in plenary; the criteria .61 to .80 represents ‘‘substantial’’ agreement. Finally,
were finalized and illustrated with typical examples, because each unit in each textbook contained a different
resulting in the framework finally used in this study number of tasks (see Table 2), the raw frequency totals for
(Tables 3, 4, 5, 6, and 7 show the instrument and give some each textbook were divided by the number of tasks found in
definitions or examples). their respective units and multiplied by 100 to obtain
First, the author and one assistant independently divided comparable proportionate rates.
the materials from the selected units into tasks (as defined For the purpose of assessing the books so they could be
by Littlejohn 1992). Inter-rater agreement reached 97 % quickly compared, the books were awarded plus points (?)
(number of agreements divided by the sum of agreements when features were in line with SLA principles, or minus
and disagreements). Second, the materials were analyzed points (-) when they were not. The points for each feature
independently by the author and two assistants to obtain a were calculated according to the ranking of the materials

123
Author's personal copy
Language Textbook Selection

Table 4 Percentage of tasks


I. What is the learner expected to do? Books Mean (%)
having features from Section I B
(FOCUS) and textbook scores A (%) B (%) C (%) D (%) E (%)
(points) (points) (points) (points) (points)

B. FOCUS
(?) Language system 7 (1) 12 (2) 22 (4) 31 (5) 17 (3) 17
• E.g., practice forms/patterns;
apply rules
(?) Meaning 59 (5) 58 (3) 59 (5) 54 (2) 54 (2) 57
• E.g., comprehension exercises,
no extended L2 output
(?) Meaning/Form relationship 34 (5) 31 (4) 19 (2) 15 (1) 29 (3) 26
• E.g., consciousness-raising
activity; prepare a report for
presentation to the class

Subsection score 11 9 11 8 8

on the given feature (from 1 to 5, five being the number of In terms of opportunities to initiate language, textbook
books evaluated). When percentages were identical, rank- A seems to offer twice as many opportunities to initiate
ing points were awarded according to how close they were language than other textbooks. However, given that all its
to the mean: usually 3 points for close to the mean, 2 points task instructions and many prompts to initiate language in
for much below the mean, and 4 points for well above the Book A (as well as Books D and E) are in Korean and that
mean. The next section illustrates the method for calcu- students in Korea operate in a monolingual context, it is
lating points and scores. possible that Korean rather than English will be initiated.
Consequently, those three books fail to maximize L2 input.
In contrast, 84 % of the task instructions in Book C are in
Findings and Discussion English. Although Book C contains hardly any initiate-type
activities, its consistent use of English in task instructions
What is the Learner Expected to do? is probably more conducive to using English for commu-
nication within the classroom. This finding stresses the
Table 3 shows the learners’ expected role during tasks. In need to evaluate materials in use as well as before use
order to facilitate instructed language learning, students need (Ellis 1997a).
to interact in and with the L2; this is indicated by a plus sign Table 4 indicates the learners’ main focus of attention
(?). In contrast, activities requiring no production of output during tasks. The three areas listed merit plus points (?)
or no overt behavioral demonstration of engagement with because SLA principles stipulate that language learning
input should be avoided, as shown by the minus sign (-); this benefits from focusing primarily on meaning while not
is because students may simply let the L2 wash over them as neglecting form-focused work in general. In regards to
noise and not learn from it. In Book A, learners were always where learners need to focus their attention during tasks, the
required to be active in some way (‘‘No action required’’: results indicate that all books direct learners’ attention
0 %), and thus no point was deducted (-0). Book E, with a predominantly toward meaning as stipulated in SLA prin-
score of 13 % had the highest proportion of materials not ciples. However, focusing on form(s) seems to have been
requiring any action, thus incurring a score of -5. considered of little importance by Books A and B (7 and
The striking imbalance between the numbers of Initiate 12 %, respectively). One question now begs to be asked:
and Respond tasks reflects a preference for controlled How can middle school students who use materials con-
practice activities (at least three quarters of the tasks place taining a fairly high proportion of Initiate-type activities
learners in a Respond position, compared with less than a (e.g., Books A and B) initiate language when their texts
quarter of them requiring learners to initiate language). focus so little on form(s)? What provision is there for
These results echo those found regarding foreign ELT helping them abstract linguistic forms from texts so they
materials by Littlejohn (1992) and more recently by can use them to express their own meanings? One answer is
Sahragard, Rahimi, and Zaremoayeddi (2009), showing that learners will be able to produce English because form is
that not so much has changed in the field of materials over dealt with in additional materials (i.e., in ‘‘Activities
the past two decades. Books,’’ CD-Rom, and worksheets provided by publishers

123
Author's personal copy
M. J. Guilloteaux

Table 5 Percentage of tasks from Section I C: operation and textbook scores


What is the learner expected to do? Books Mean
(%)
A% B% C% D% E%
(points) (points) (points) (points) (points)

C. OPERATION
(?) Repeat/reproduce identically 0 (?0) 8 (?5) 5 (?3) 8 (?5) 0 (?0) 4
(-) Repeat/reproduce with substitution 7 (-4) 0 (-0) 0 (-0) (-0) 8 (-5) 3
• Mechanical drills
(?) Repeat/reproduce selectively 14 (?3) 12 (?1) 19 (?4) 27 (?5) 13 (?2) 17
• Meaningful drills
(?) Repeat/reproduce with transformation 0 (?0) 4 (?3) 11 (?5) 4 (?3) 4 (?3) 5
• E.g., sequence words to form a sentence
(?) Repeat/reproduce with expansion 21 (?5) 15 (?3) 19 (?4) 8 (?1) 8 (?1) 14
• E.g., use a sample dialog but change the setting
(?) Build text with recalled items 31 (?5) 31 (?5) 14 (?2) 8 (?1) 17 (?3) 20
• E.g., produce a list of words/an answer to a question from memory
(?) Apply general knowledge 14 (?2) 27 (?5) 19 (?3) 12 (?1) 21 (?4) 18
• E.g., use information learned in another subject
(?) Decode meaning 62 (?3) 54 (?1) 86 (?5) 62 (?3) 54 (?1) 64
• Match phrases with meanings in L1
(?) Extract gist, information, details 45 (?3) 46 (?4) 54 (?5) 31 (?1) 33 (?2) 42
(?) Categorize selected information 3 (?3) 0 (?0) 3 (?3) 4 (?5) 0 (?0) 2
(?) Hypothesize, infer meaning 7 (?2) 8 (?3) 14 (?4) 15 (?5) 4 (?1) 10
(?) Compare samples of language on the basis of contents (meaning) 3 (?1) 4 (?2) 11 (?4) (?5) 8 (?3) 9
or form
• E.g., true/false; select the correct version; whose story was the
most interesting?
(?) Analyze language form 0 (?0) 0 (?0) 0 (?0) 0 (?0) (?0) 0
• Examine the component parts of a text/word (e.g., find the
stressed syllable in a word)
(?) Formulate a language rule 0 (?0) 0 (?0) 0 (?0) 0 (?0) (?0) 0
• E.g., examine sentences and write partially formulated rule
(?) Apply a language rule 3 (?2) 8 (?5) 5 (?3) 8 (?5) 0 (?0) 5
(?) Negotiate to achieve something 3 (?3) 8 (?5) 0 (?0) 4 (?4) 0 (?0) 3
(?) Review own/peers’ L2 output 0 (?0) 12 (?5) 11 (?4) 4 (?3) 0 (?0) 5
(?) Attend to explanation/example 3 (?1) 8 (?2) 16 (?3) 23 (?5) 17 (?4) 13
• Students are required to be alert, perhaps notice something.

Subsection score 29 49 52 52 19

in the case of current materials in South Korean schools). section involve those related to comprehension. Theories of
The disadvantage of such practice is that it may lead to comprehension stress the importance of helping students
excessive decontextualization of forms presented and understand spoken or written texts by having them relate
practiced in isolated sentences. their schematic knowledge to new information contained in
In the ‘‘What learners are expected to do during tasks’’ a text, or by expanding that knowledge if necessary. Only
section, the third area (C: Operation) concerns mental 12–27 % of all the sampled tasks required learners to apply
processes. The results are presented in Table 5. All the general knowledge (M = 18 %). Although these results
listed processes are helpful for language acquisition except could be a function of the topics sampled, it seems that
for repeating/reproducing with substitution (i.e., mechani- textbooks could include more activities requiring students
cal drills). Hence the minus point sign (-) for that process. to make use of general knowledge. This would make
The features attracting the highest percentages in this materials less trivial.

123
Author's personal copy
Language Textbook Selection

Table 6 Percentage of tasks


II. Who with? Books Mean
having features from: ‘‘who
(%)
with?’’ and textbook scores A% B% C% D% E%
(points) (points) (points) (points) (points)

(?) Teacher $ Students 14 (?5) 4 (?3) – 4 (?3) 8 (?4) 6


• E.g., teacher-fronted question and answer
sessions
(?) Individual/Pair/Group $ Whole class – 4 (?5) – – – 1
• E.g., after presenting, a group answers
questions
(?) Choral work – 4 (?4) 5 (?5) 4 (?4) – 3
(?) Individual work 59 (?2) 46 (?1) 81 (?4) 73 (?3) 83 (?5) 68
(?) Presentation (e.g., act out a dialog, present a 7 (?4) 4 (?3) 3 (?2) 8 (?5) – 4
poster—individually, in pairs, or in groups)
• E.g., act out a dialog; present a poster
(?) Pair/group work, simultaneously 21 (?4) 38 (?5) 11 (?2) 12 (?3) 8 (?1) 18

Subsection score 15 21 13 18 10

Table 7 Percentage of tasks having features from: ‘‘with what contents?’’ and textbook scores
III. With what contents? Books Mean
(%)
A% B% C% D% E%
(points) (points) (points) (points) (points)

A. FORM
(a) Input to learners
(?) Non-verbal (e.g., pictures to sequence) 21 (?1) 58 (?5) 46 (?4) 46 (?4) 29 (?2) 40
(?) Written: words/phrases/short texts \50 words 52 (?1) 69 (?3) 78 (?4) 88 (?5) 67 (?2) 71
(?) Aural: words/phrases/short texts \50 words 21 (?1) 23 (?3) 22 (?2) 46 (?5) 29 (?4) 28
(?) Written: Coherent text [50 words with supra-sentential features 24 (?4) 23 (?3) 32 (?5) 12 (?2) 8 (?1) 20
(?) Aural: Coherent text [50 words with supra-sentential features 10 (?4) 4 (?3) 11 (?5) 4 (?3) 4 (?3) 7
(b) Expected learner outputa
(?) Non-verbal: e.g., match items, check correct picture, draw 21 (?1) 23 (?2) 49 (?5) 27 (?3) 29 (?4) 30
(?) Written words/phrases/single sentence: e.g., fill in the blanks 41 (?4) 31 (?2) 30 (?1) 46 (?5) 38 (?3) 37
(?) Oral words/phrases/single sentence: e.g., respond to a drill 31 (?4) 38 (?5) 16 (?3) 15 (?2) 13 (?1) 23
(?) Written: Coherent text with connected sentences [30 words 3 (?2) 4 (?5) 3 (?2) 4 (?5) 4 (?5) 4
(?) Oral: Coherent text with connected sentences [30 words 3 (?2) 4 (?4) – 8 (?5) 4 (?4) 4
B. SOURCE
(?) Materials (dialog/text). 72 (?2) 77 (?3) 100 (?5 81 (?4) 71 (?1) 80
(?) Teacher – – – – – –
(?) Learners (e.g., present results of own research) 28 (?4) 23 (?3) – 19 (?2) 29 (?5) 20
C. NATURE
(?) Metalinguistic information (e.g., a grammatical rule with example sentences; 24 (?2) 8 (?1) 27 (?3) 31 (?5) 29 (?4) 24
explanations about the use of certain expressions)
(?) Non-fiction 10 (?2) 15 (?5) 11 (?3) 8 (?1) 13 (?4) 11
(?) Fiction 45 (?2) 54 (?4) 59 (?5) 46 (?3) 25 (?1) 46
(?) Learners’ personal information/opinion 21 (?3) 23 (?4) 3 (?1) 15 (?2) 33 (?5) 19

Subsection total 39 55 48 56 49
a
Some totals are \100 % because some books contain tasks that do not require any output

Overall, Books B, C, and D appear to engage learners tasks, engaging learners in deeper and more meaningful
cognitively in more varied ways than the others. Moreover, processing of texts. Nonetheless, the most popular tasks
Books C and D contain more inference and comparison across the sample, representing about one-third to a half of

123
Author's personal copy
M. J. Guilloteaux

the tasks (M = 43 %), required learners to respond using Table 8 Final results and ranking of textbooks
some form of repetition (e.g., meaningful drills and repe- Summary of results Books
tition-type tasks followed by expansion). Such practice is
desirable from the perspective of skill acquisition theory, A B C D E
but conditions are attached. Controlled practice will only I. What is the learner expected to do?
support language learning as long as it is meaningful and A. TURN-TAKE 7 4 5 3 –1
follows the provision of relevant declarative knowledge B. FOCUS 11 9 11 8 8
such as explanations (see e.g., Ortega 2009). C. OPERATION 29 49 52 52 19
SLA research points toward the utility of producing II. Who with? 15 21 13 18 10
sustained output and negotiation for promoting language III. With what contents? 39 55 48 56 49
acquisition, but all the books are low in tasks that require Total points 101 138 129 137 85
negotiating; and Books C, D, and E vary in their require- Final Ranking 4 1 3 2 5
ments for learners to build texts out of items recalled from
memory. More opportunities for interaction and freer
production of extended output are needed. Regrettably, turns and extended written output. Finally, the findings
open-ended activities tend to be less popular in Korea reported in sections B and C indicate that, although the use
where teachers often do not feel confident about their of fiction may go some way toward engaging students on
proficiency in English, and single correct answer test-tak- an affective basis, textbooks should include more activities
ing practice activities are still currently preferred by stu- requiring personal input from learners to increase their
dents and parents. Feedback for closed activities is also motivation.
easier to prepare and give in large classes composed of
students of widely different levels. Even university stu- Final Results of Analysis
dents prefer English teachers to lecture about English
because they believe it is more time-efficient than a task- Table 8 presents the final ranking of the sampled text-
based approach (see e.g., Yim 2009). books, which was obtained by computing the points gained
on each subsection. The results indicate that the procedure
Who With? was efficient in discriminating between the sampled text-
books, revealing a clear cluster composed of three higher
Table 6 presents the results concerning the participation scoring books (Books B, D, and C), and two outliers with
structure that is recommended or inferred from tasks much lower scores (Books A and E). There is evidence to
instructions. Individual work predominates in Books C, D, suggest that Books B, D, and C reflect SLA principles more
and E, echoing closely than Books A and E, and may, therefore, poten-
Jeon’s (2005) findings that 80 % of activities in high tially offer more support for language learning than Books
school textbooks involved individual work, 15 % pair A and E. However, the extent to which textbooks actually
work, and 5 % group work. Book A, with fewer activities do support language acquisition can only be determined
requiring individual completion and 21 % requiring pair/ through in-use evaluation.
group work, seems to offer more opportunities for inter-
action and meaning negotiation in the L2. However, since
the learner groups are monolingual, students may interact Conclusion
in Korean.
The present study has illustrated how a minimally modified
With What Contents? version of Littlejohn’s (1998, 2011) task analysis schedule
could be used as an operational framework to evaluate
Table 7 shows the results regarding the contents of the whether textbooks reflect universal SLA criteria, which
tasks that are assigned to the learners. A staple of language were mainly derived from Ellis’ (2005) list of ten general
acquisition-rich classrooms is extensive input of language principles for instructed SLA and SLA-based principles for
in use, but in terms of input, all five books consist pre- materials design from Cook (1998), Richards (2006),
dominantly of written words, phrases, sentences, or brief Tomlinson (2010), and Tomlinson and Masuhara (2010).
texts. Similar results emerged for output. The most com- The procedure did discriminate between the sampled
mon type of required output found in the sample was of the textbooks, even though all five had been specially written
restricted, controlled kind. In order to support language according to ministry guidelines and had passed ministry-
acquisition, more extensive input of language in use is approval evaluation. The evidence suggests that three out
required in textbooks, as well as activities eliciting long of the five textbooks reflect SLA principles more closely

123
Author's personal copy
Language Textbook Selection

than the other books and are, therefore, potentially more development in language teaching (2nd ed., pp. 179–211).
helpful in terms of supporting students’ language learning Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Long, M. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second
in the classroom. However, the findings do not permit to language acquisition. In W. Ritchie & T. Bhatia (Eds.),
conclude that the three textbooks that belonged to the Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 413–468). San
higher scoring group in the analysis are more effective in Diego, CA: Academic Press.
supporting language acquisition—this could only be Mitchell, R., & Myles, F. (2001). Second language learning: Key
concepts and issues. In C. N. Candlin & N. Mercer (Eds.),
determined by in-use evaluation. Finally, to complete an English language teaching in its social context (pp. 11–27).
evaluation-for-selection process, the higher scoring books London, UK: Routledge.
in a preliminary materials analysis phase of the type Mukundan, J., & Ahour, T. (2010). A review of textbook evaluation
described here should be subjected to a materials evalua- checklists across four decades (1970–2008). In B. Tomlinson &
H. Masuhara (Eds.), Research for materials development in
tion phase for context relevance. Given the crucial role language learning: Evidence for best practice (pp. 326–352).
played by materials in students’ language development and New York, NY: Continuum.
the expensive nature of materials development, the pre- Newton, D. P., & Newton, L. D. (2009). A procedure for assessing
liminary materials analysis procedure delineated in this textbook support for reasoned thinking. The Asia Pacific
Education Researcher, 18(1), 109–115.
study could prove useful to anyone involved in materials Ortega, L. (2009). Understanding second language acquisition.
selection and materials design/publication, and to govern- London, UK: Hodder Arnold.
ment officials responsible for textbook authorization. Richards, J. C. (2006). Materials development and research—making
the connection. RELC Journal, 37(1), 5–26.
Rubdy, R. (2003). Selection of materials. In B. Tomlinson (Ed.),
Developing materials for language teaching (pp. 37–57).
London, UK: Continuum.
References Sahragard, R., Rahimi, A., & Zaremoayeddi, I. (2009). An in-depth
evaluation of Interchange series (3rd ed). Porta Linguarum, 12,
Aski, J. M. (2003). Foreign language textbook activities: Keeping 37–54.
pace with second language acquisition research. Foreign Lan- Swan, M. (2006). Two out of three ain’t enough: The essential
guage Annals, 36(1), 57–65. ingredients of a language course. In IATEFL 2006 harrogate
Cook, V. (1998). Relating SLA research to language teaching conference selections (pp. 45–54). Retrieved from http://www.
materials. Canadian Journal of Applied Linguistics (CJAL)/ mikeswan.co.uk/elt-applied-linguistics/Two-out-of-three-aint-
Revue Canadienne de Linguistique Applique´e (RCLA), 1(1-2), enough-the-essential-ingredients-of-a-language-course
9–27. Tomlinson, B. (1998). Introduction. In B. Tomlinson (Ed.), Materials
Cook, V. (2008). Second language learning and language teaching. development in language teaching (pp. 1–24). Cambridge, UK:
London, UK: Hodder Education. Cambridge University Press.
De Keyser, R. (2010). Practice for second language learning: Don’t Tomlinson, B. (2003). Materials evaluation. In B. Tomlinson (Ed.),
throw out the baby with the bathwater. International Journal of Developing materials for language teaching (pp. 15–36).
English Studies, 10(1), 155–165. London, UK: Continuum.
Ellis, R. (1997a). The empirical evaluation of language teaching Tomlinson, B. (2008). Language acquisition and language learning
materials. ELT Journal, 51(1), 36–42. materials. In B. Tomlinson (Ed.), English language learning
Ellis, R. (1997b). Second language acquisition. Oxford, UK: Oxford materials: A critical review (pp. 3–13). London: Continuum.
University Press. Tomlinson, B. (2010). Principles of effective materials development.
Ellis, R. (1997c). SLA and language pedagogy: An educational In N. Harwood (Ed.), English language teaching materials:
perspective. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 69–92. Theory and practice (pp. 81–108). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
Ellis, R. (2005). Principles of instructed language learning. System, University Press.
33(2), 209–224. Tomlinson, B. (2011). Introduction: Principles and procedures of
Jeon, I. (2005). An analysis of task-based materials and performance: materials development. In B. Tomlinson (Ed.), Materials
Focused on Korean high school English textbooks. English development in English teaching (2nd ed., pp. 1–34). Cambridge,
Teaching, 60(2), 87–109. UK: Cambridge University Press.
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer Tomlinson, B. (2012). Materials development for language learning
agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174. and teaching. Language Teaching, 45(2), 143–179.
Lantolf, J. (2000). Second language learning as a mediated process. Tomlinson, B., & Masuhara, H. (2010). Applications of the research
Language Teaching, 33, 79–96. results for second language acquisition theory and research. In B.
Littlejohn, A. (1992). Why are English language teaching materials Tomlinson & H. Masuhara (Eds.), Research for materials
the way they are? Doctoral dissertation, University of Lancaster, development in language teaching: Evidence from best practice
England. Retrieved from http://www.andrewlittlejohn.net/web (pp. 399–409). London, UK: Continuum.
site/books/phd.html Waters, A. (2009). Advances in materials design. In M. H. Long & C.
Littlejohn, A. (1998). The analysis of language teaching materials: J. Doughty (Eds.), Handbook of language teaching (pp.
Inside the Trojan Horse. In B. Tomlinson (Ed.), Materials 311–326). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
development in language teaching (pp. 190–216). Cambridge, Wong, W., & Van Patten, B. (2003). The evidence is IN: Drills are
UK: Cambridge University Press. OUT. Foreign Language Annals, 36(3), 403–423.
Littlejohn, A. (2011). The analysis of language teaching materials: Yim, S. H. (2009). South Korean teachers’ perceptions of TBLT.
Inside the Trojan Horse. In B. Tomlinson (Ed.), Materials TESOL Review, 1, 29–50.

123

You might also like