You are on page 1of 42

10

Strategies for Listening,


Talking, and Thinking in
Auditory-Verbal Therapy

Ellen A. Rhoades, Warren Estabrooks,


Stacey R. Lim, and Karen MacIver-Lux

285
286  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

rhythms, gaze, affect, and vocal behav-


Introduction iors are coordinated (Feldman, Magori-
Cohen, Galili, Singer, & Louzon, 2011).
Many language facilitative strategies This natural parent-child “interaction
have been studied specifically for synchrony,” however, may be disrupted
outcome effectiveness with children when the child does not respond appro-
who have typical hearing and lan- priately because of a hearing loss. It is
guage delays/disorders. Consequently, imperative, therefore, that parent-child
there are considerable evidence-based interaction synchrony be restored.
strategies targeting that large pediat- In general, the best evidence-
ric population. Yet, for children with informed practice is considered to be
hearing loss, evidence-based strate- naturalistic intervention. Many char-
gies to facilitate listening and spo- acteristics of this are embraced by AV
ken language skills remain relatively practitioners as it is child directed,
scarce. Nevertheless, there are many play based on the child’s interests,
evidence-informed strategies that the and involves incidental and responsive
auditory-verbal (AV) practitioner can teaching. Naturalistic intervention
use to facilitate listening and spoken includes strategies that encourage joint
language (Nevo & Slonim-Nevo, 2011). attention, turn-taking, and other recip-
The purpose of this chapter, therefore, rocal adult-child interactions (Dunst,
is to present these evidence-informed Raab, & Trivette, 2011; Snyder et al.,
strategies in detail. 2015). Thus, with the use of strategies
Evidence-informed practice is guided that facilitate listening, speech, and lan-
by research that the practitioner finds guage, AV practitioners coach and guide
in peer-review journals. AV practitio- parents to develop the same skills.
ners use the best available knowledge
and research to guide strategy selec-
tion and implementation, and they try
Strategies
to familiarize themselves with outcome
studies across related disciplines such
as psychology, general education, child A strategy generally refers to a plan of
development, neurobiology, reading, action or method designed to achieve a
and so forth. Research findings con- goal; it tends to be behavioral or men-
cerning all issues in communication tal in nature. Effective and appropriate
disorders, therefore, need to be appro- strategies are based on good knowl-
priately incorporated into AVT (Nevo edge of the situation/problem with
& Slonim-Nevo, 2011). Throughout this reasonable expectations of outcomes.
chapter, we refer to the AV practitio- A technique is a way of doing some-
ner although we also expect that the thing by using a particular skill or spe-
parents of children with hearing loss cial knowledge. There are sometimes
(as a result of ongoing coaching) will many techniques or ways in which a
learn, practice, and apply the strategies strategy can be executed. AV practi-
in daily life. tioners and parents may differ in their
Parents and children with typical techniques when implementing a strat-
hearing usually interact in tandem from egy, and the techniques may vary from
birth, meaning that their biological child to child. Nevertheless, everyone
Chapter 10    Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  287
n

needs to work in harmony to achieve clarity when passing through


the outcomes they want for the child. a barrier or an acoustic filter.
Sometimes it is difficult to agree on the High-frequency audibility for
correct term, so for the purpose of clar- children with hearing loss is
ity throughout this chapter, the word critical (Stelmachowicz, Pittman,
strategies includes both. Hoover, Lewis, & Moeller,
One historical strategy that has 2004). Speech directed to young
been associated with AVT is the hand children with hearing loss must
cue (the adult’s “hand over mouth” to not involve degraded spectral
eliminate speech reading). For many content (Zangl, Klarman, Thal,
compelling reasons, however, this is no Fernald, & Bates, 2005).
longer considered an effective strategy.
Evidence for this indicates that Some practitioners use “speech
hoops” (acoustic screens) to cover their
n Covering the mouth disrupts faces. Their reason for using these to
sensorimotor input during replace the “hand cue” is to avoid com-
infancy and may have negative promising the sound quality. However,
implications for the development preventing adult-child eye contact can
of speech motor control (Yeung negatively affect the child’s overall
& Werker, 2013). development. The speaker’s eyes give
n Covering-up visual cues (lip children important cues about the
movements) in an obvious way direction of visual attention as well as
can instigate stress in young an emotional and/or mental state gaze
children which, in turn, nega- necessary for joint attention, spoken
tively affects speech perception language, and social skills (Frischen,
(Wang, Lee, Sigman, & Dapretto, Bayliss, & Tipper, 2007; Nappa, Wessel,
2006). McEldoon, Gleitman, & Trueswell, 2009;
n Placing one’s “hand over mouth” Rigato, Menon, Johnson, Faraguna, &
is considered negative body Farroni, 2011). Consequently, the use of
language among adults (Fast, the hand cue (or any substitute for it) is
2002). no longer recommended in AVT.
n Covering the mouth can alter the A precept underlying all strategies
child’s visual learning and visual presented in this chapter is that sound
memory (Brockmole, Davoli, must be meaningful. The child with
Abrams, & Witt, 2013). hearing loss will learn to “tune out”
n Seeing the mouth purposefully sound if it is has no meaning since the
hidden from view can detract brain learns to ignore nonmeaningful
from full auditory attention, thus sounds (Kotz, Opitz, & Friedrici, 2007).
slightly delaying speech percep- A young child wearing hearing aids
tion (Musacchia, Sams, Nicol, & with ear molds that inadequately fit,
Kraus, 2006). for example, may hear a high-pitched
n Obstructing the mouth provides squeal and will eventually ignore the
an acoustic barrier for the acoustic feedback if it persists.
child with hearing loss. For Another example is when, after
example, high-frequency sounds activation and programming of the
tend to distort or diminish in child’s cochlear implant, an adult calls
288  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

the child’s name while he or she is hap- strategies used by the AV practitioner
pily playing. The child hears his or her to foster the growth of listening and
name and turns in the direction of the spoken language across the years 0–6.
adult. Subsequently, the adult becomes Table 10–1 provides an outline of
excited, gleefully remarking to the AV these SIX GOALS and strategies recom-
practitioner, “See, he heard me! He mended for each of them.
knows his name!” In fact, there was no
payoff for the child, and consequently
had no real value, except to indicate SIX GOALS of the
detection of a sound.
Auditory-Verbal
The AV practitioner coaches the
Practitioner (for
parent to call the child’s name and make
it meaningful by saying something such
Every AVT Session)
as: “Yes, you heard me call you. Great!
Come here and help me” or “Come There are essentially SIX GOALS that the
here! I want to show you this” Sound AV practitioner typically addresses in the
must be meaningful if it is to be pro- planning and delivery of every AVT ses-
cessed and retained by the brain. sion. Parents usually learn these goals
Learning how to become an effec- quickly as the practitioner creatively
tive communicator is a dynamic process coaches them on using various strate-
that also involves cumulative practice. gies to accomplish them. Some strate-
Listening, in part, involves a set of skills gies may be specific to AVT while some
that can be taught, developed, and are used to help children with a vari-
enhanced through the use of various ety of communication disorders. These
strategies (Graham, Santos, & Vander- strategies encourage children to listen
plank, 2011). This chapter, therefore, and talk in AVT and are not to be con-
identifies SIX GOALS of the AV prac- fused with general parent guidance and
titioner and discusses many effective coaching practices found in Chapter 12.

Table 10–1. Six Goals and Selected Strategies of the AV Practitioner

GOAL 1: CREATE A LISTENING ENVIRONMENT

Strategies
• Controlling the environment; setting the stage
• Speaking within earshot; leaning to the child’s better hearing side
GOAL 2: FACILITATE AUDITORY ATTENTION

Strategies
• Presenting a look of concentration with a verbal prompt
• Pointing to the ear and saying “I heard something!”
• Using auditory hooks
• Using visual distractors
• Preparing the child to “listen first and last”
Table 10–1.  continued

GOAL 3: ENHANCE AUDITORY PERCEPTION OF SPEECH

Strategies
• Speaking parentese
• Engaging in vocal play
• Associating sounds with objects and words
• Whispering
• Singing
• Stressing selected syllables, words, and phrases
GOAL 4: PROMOTE KNOWLEDGE OF LANGUAGE

Strategies
• Focusing on the “knowing” rather than the “using”
• Taking turns
• Imitating the child’s early vocalizations
• Verbalizing in synchrony with movement
• Speaking the language from the child’s angle
• Talking before, during, and after the action
• Pausing for grammatical spaces or emphasis
• Transitioning beyond the comfort zone
• Connecting the familiar to the unfamiliar
• Recasting, expanding, and expatiating on the child’s words
• Emphasizing actions, relations, and attributes
• Contrasting the meaning of words
GOAL 5: FACILITATE SPOKEN LANGUAGE AND COGNITION

Strategies
• Leaning forward with expectant looks
• Signaling with objects
• Providing self-statements
• Asking “What did you hear?”
• Promoting auditory-verbal closure
• Waiting for the child’s response
• Asking stage-appropriate questions
• Scaffolding for language production
GOAL 6:  STIMULATE INDEPENDENT LEARNING

Strategies
• Pretending objects are something else
• Creating the unexpected
• Talking with imaginary friends
• Accepting and making mistakes

289
290  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

SIX GOALS Controlling the Environment;


Setting the Stage
1. Create a Listening Environment Noise compromises speech perception
2. Facilitate Auditory Attention and language learning (Newman, Chat-
3. Enhance Auditory Perception of terjee, Morini, & Remez, 2015). All chil-
Speech dren, especially those with hearing loss,
4. Promote Knowledge of Language benefit greatly from quiet environments
5. Facilitate Spoken Language and with little reverberation (Smaldino &
Cognition Flexer, 2014). Since AVT sessions may
6. Stimulate Independent Learning take place in the child’s home, the AV
practitioner informs parents of variables
that create adverse listening conditions.
GOAL 1: Create a Parents, subsequently, can create the
Listening Environment most favorable listening and language
learning conditions possible. Parents
need to know that household appliances,
Strategies
televisions, music equipment, comput-
n Controlling the environment; ers, and other electronic devices add to
setting the stage background noise and their use needs
n Speaking within earshot; leaning to be minimized. High ceilings and tile
to the child’s better hearing side floors cause reverberation, so floor, win-
dow, and wall coverings are encouraged.
A child with a hearing loss may experi- Many AVT sessions, however, take place
ence auditory deprivation dating from in clinical rooms where controlling the
3 months before birth (Yang, 2006). The environment is more easily managed.
length of sensory deprivation depends
on the age of the child when he or she Speaking Within Earshot;
was first fitted with at least one effec- Leaning to the Child’s
tive hearing device. Once the child has Better Hearing Side
access to sound, it is critical to help the
child develop focused auditory atten- A child with hearing loss has a reduced
tion so that the child learns to listen as listening range (Anderson & Crowley,
quickly and efficiently as possible. 2002). Within this range, adults need to
Listening improves when the child speak clearly in a natural voice at nor-
is still and attends to the adult who is mal conversational levels. The nearer
talking (Schneider, Nelson, & Mooney, the AV practitioner talks into the micro-
2014). As the AV practitioner and par- phone of the child’s hearing device, the
ent help increase the child’s auditory more easily the child will understand
attention span (sustained attention), the speech signal because the clarity
the child is more likely to discriminate of spoken language improves as the
between meaningful and nonmeaning- distance between listener and speaker
ful sounds, while “tuning out” irrelevant decreases (Souza, 2014).
background sounds (Dalton & Fraen- Speaking within earshot means that
kel, 2012). the person talking is close enough to
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  291

the listener that all the sounds of speech children with hearing loss require the
are audible and/or intelligible. The best same opportunities for learning to lis-
understanding of speech occurs in a ten as babies with typical hearing. The
fairly quiet room and typically where the development of auditory attention is of
person talking is within 3 to 12 inches prime importance (Gomes, Wolfson, &
of the microphone of the child’s hear- Halperin, 2007). To develop the listen-
ing device (Ling & Ling, 1978). This ing attitude, the child learns that “lis-
strategy may reduce listening effort for tening must be a continuous activity”
the child (Gustafson, McCreery, Hoover, (Pollack, 1972, p. 29).
Kopun, & Stelmachowicz, 2014). The child’s attention must be di-
When introducing new vocabulary rected to focus on sounds in order to
and/or language patterns, the AV prac- develop listening skills. Auditory atten-
titioner is positioned up close and per- tion directs auditory learning (Halliday,
sonal to the child, approximately 3 to Moore, Taylor, & Amitay, 2011). There-
12 inches from the child’s microphone, fore, for the child to develop a listening
with the person who is talking on the attitude, the AV practitioner repeatedly
side of the child’s better ear (Hol- uses prompts to direct the child’s atten-
strum, Gaffney, Gravel, Oyler, & Ross, tion to sound (e.g., “Listen!”). These
2008). To further minimize distortion of prompts alert the child to pay atten-
speech, the practitioner avoids having tion to what is heard, and consequently
any obstruction between the speaker’s learn that what he or she hears really
mouth and the child’s microphone. matters. All adults need to expect that
the child will attend to the sounds of life
and, in particular, to what people say.
GOAL 2: Facilitate
Auditory Attention
Presenting a Look of
Concentration With a
Strategies Verbal Prompt
n Presenting a look of concentra- Facial expressions can prime language
tion with a verbal prompt acquisition, so they are important cues
n Pointing to the ear and saying for young children (Woumans et al., in
“I heard something!” press). The AV practitioner often makes
n Using auditory hooks facial expressions to indicate anticipa-
n Using visual distractors tion or “listening,” such as a furrowed
n Preparing the child to “listen first brow, tilting of the head, or leaning for-
and last” ward to the sound source. These often
capture the child’s attention and sig-
After the initial fitting of the hearing nal that something is going to happen
device(s) and while the child begins . . . and then a sound is made. After
adjusting to newly acquired auditory the sound is made, the practitioner
potential, the AV practitioner focuses points to his or her own ear and says,
on helping the child to develop a lis- “I heard that!” or “Did you hear that?”
tening attitude (Holstrum et al., 2008). or “I wonder what that was?” or “What
Whether toddlers or preschoolers, did you hear?” Depending on the child’s
292  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

response, the practitioner may direct or her that something exciting is com-
this strategy to the parent who, in turn, ing. Auditory hooks can be verbal
responds appropriately (e.g., with a encouragements that include a variety
head nod or pointing). The child typi- of words, phrases, and/or fillers, such
cally follows the parent’s cue by imitat- as “Hmm” “uhm . . . ” “Hey!” “Look!”
ing what the parent just said or did. “Wow!” “Listen to this!” “Oh my good-
ness!” “That’s interesting” “That’s cool!”
Pointing to the Ear and Saying “Mmmm, let’s see.” These auditory
“I heard something!” hooks direct the child’s attention to
either environmental sounds or spoken
Pointing to the ear is a visual cue that language, or to the joint activity itself
alerts the young child to speech and and they can promote longer episodes
environmental sounds that may be of shared or joint attention (Flom &
forthcoming. This gesture can also Pick, 2003).
alert the child to the speech or an envi-
ronmental sound that was just heard. Using Visual Distractors
Pointing to the ear facilitates auditory
attention (Varghese, Ozmeral, Best, & Visual distractors refer to prompts
Shinn-Cunningham, 2012). Pointing, used by the AV practitioner that direct
along with the verbal cue (e.g., “Listen, or redirect the child’s visual attention.
I heard something!”) is associated with Research findings show that when the
word comprehension (Caselli, Rinaldi, child’s visual attention is focused on a
Stefanini, & Volterra, 2012). Combining speaker’s face, the act of listening may
the gesture with the verbal cue facili- be compromised (Champoux, Lepore,
tates joint attention that is important Gagné, & Théoret, 2009). So, when a
for both social interaction and language child persists in “reading” the speaker’s
acquisition (Colonnesi, Stams, Koster, & face, visual distractors used by the prac-
Noom, 2010). titioner include
The AV practitioner primes the child
to get ready to listen by saying, “Lis- n pointing to an item, toward the
ten!” while pointing to his or her own window, or to another person;
ear. Used simultaneously with “Listen!” n placing a toy, book, or other
this gesture signals that some kind of interesting item in front of the
sound will soon be coming. Sometimes child; or
referred to as a set to listen task, this n subtle postural shifting so that
strategy is used when the child is not the child finds “reading” the
expecting a sound. Immediately after speaker’s face to be a bit more
hearing the sound, the practitioner difficult.
says, “I heard that!”
The short-term objective and the
Using Auditory Hooks child’s level of auditory and linguistic
functioning determine when and how
Auditory hooks are prompts that “hook visual distractors are used. For exam-
the child on listening” and alerts him ple, the AV practitioner introduces the
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  293

sound “meow” associated with a cat adult can continue narrating. In all
during the first year of learning to listen: cases, the child’s attention is diverted
so that hearing and listening happen
n The child looks directly at the before seeing.
face of the practitioner when the
practitioner says “meow.” Preparing the Child to
n The practitioner makes a show “Listen First and Last”
of looking at a book on the desk
that diverts the child’s visual Listen First and Last refers to the pre-
attention. (The book is the visual sentation of information — first, in the
distractor.) auditory-only condition (at least twice
n The child momentarily looks at and perhaps three times), followed by
the book. visual information, if required, and
n The practitioner repeats “meow” then ending the sequence with the
before showing the toy cat, at auditory-only information again. This
which point the child’s attention means the child first listens to the spo-
is reverted to the practitioner. ken language (auditory-only message).
n The practitioner then presents If the child does not understand the
the toy cat to the child. auditory-only message after a maxi-
n The child plays with the toy cat mum of three presentations by the AV
and the practitioner again says practitioner, then a visual reinforcer is
or sings, “meow.” used to help the child understand what
was said. Once the child understands,
Another visual distractor might be out- the practitioner always returns to the
side, for example, the AV practitioner auditory-only message (Estabrooks,
points to the window and says, “Lis- 2006; Rhoades, 2011).
ten, I hear some birds. They’re singing, Aside from the practice of hearing
tweet, tweet, tweet, tweet!” Following the same message repeatedly (sustained
this, the practitioner presents a toy bird auditory attentional focus), the process
to the child. of listening first and last enables the
When a child focuses more on child to fill in the missing pieces and
watching faces than listening, another then make accurate predictions based
visual distractor may be when the prac- on the visual cues provided (Astheimer
titioner moves his or her body so that & Sanders, 2009). The practitioner makes
the child’s visual attention is partially every effort to help the child under-
obstructed. For example, the practitio- stand what was said and may implement
ner can bend down to the side of the other strategies such as repetition and
child to retrieve a toy clock from the rephrasing. If, after using these strate-
floor. The child watches the movement gies, the child still does not understand,
of the adult’s body as she or he says then the adult needs to use a visual
“tick-tock, tick-tock, tick-tock.” When cue such as speech reading, or a ges-
the clock is placed on the table and ture, or a picture to enhance the child’s
the child plays with it, the child’s visual understanding. Finally, when the child
attention is directed to the toy and the does comprehend the complete spoken
294  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

message, the practitioner presents the ciated with word learning by toddlers
message again in the auditory-only con- (Bosch, 2011; Cristia, Seidi, Junge, Sod-
dition as it was initially presented. erstrom, & Hagoort, 2014). Even though
today’s hearing technology provides
auditory access to all speech sounds at
GOAL 3: Enhance Auditory conversational levels, some children in
Perception of Speech AVT require further assistance to clearly
hear some of these sounds. To increase
Strategies the acoustic salience of spoken lan-
guage, the AV practitioner uses appro-
n Speaking parentese priately selected acoustic highlighting
n Engaging in vocal play strategies that enhance audibility of
n Associating sounds with objects different sounds, words, or phrases
and words and subsequently speech perception is
n Whispering made a little easier (Bedore & Leonard,
n Singing 1995; Smith & Levitt, 1999).
n Stressing selected syllables,
words, and phrases Speaking Parentese
Speech consists of segmental informa- Parentese, also known as infant-directed
tion (features that differentiate one speech or motherese, is a special speech
sound or phoneme from another) and mode that is higher-pitched, hyper-
their suprasegmental properties (inten- articulated, repetitive, rhythmic, and
sity, stress, intonation, rhythm). Audi- melodic and has a slower tempo than
tory perception of speech is the process typical talking (Matsuda et al., 2011).
by which the sounds of a language are This distinctive speaking style, charac-
heard and understood. Newborn babies terized by exaggerated inflection and
with typical hearing are able to detect intensity as well as vowel prolongations,
some suprasegmental information and is universal and spoken by most caregiv-
to differentiate between phonemes like ers in every culture, regardless of gender
/ba/ and /pa/ under certain conditions (van Heugten & Johnson, 2012). Paren-
(McMurray & Aslin, 2005; Werker & tese has inherent acoustic cues and con-
Gervain, 2013). sequently stimulates the young child’s
Across the first few years of life, developing auditory system (Chang &
children’s speech perception is shaped Thompson, 2011). Children who hear
by their auditory system as well as parentese frequently become more effi-
their phonetic and linguistic knowl- cient in understanding spoken language
edge (Brooks & Kempe, 2012; Houston, and have larger vocabularies as they
2011; Johnson, 2012). We also know mature (Weisleder & Fernald, 2013).
that children’s speech perception is at Children are more likely to attend
least partially based on socially inter- to parentese (Ma, Golinkoff, Houston, &
acting with people who matter to them Hirsh-Pasek, 2011) and, in turn, parents
(Kuhl, Tsao, & Liu, 2003). tend to adjust their exaggerated inflec-
Early speech perception skills learned tional and pleasant speech according
during typical infancy seem to be asso- to the child’s developmental level. It is
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  295

known that parentese facilitates both unique to their mother tongue (Gold-
social interaction and language acquisi- stein & Schwade, 2008).
tion (Estes & Hurley, 2013; Xu, Chen, When appropriate vocal play is
Cheng, & Ma, 2011); therefore, in dif- heard by children with cochlear implants,
ferent ways, the AV practitioner coaches they perform like children with typical
parents to speak parentese. hearing in that they improve their own
vocalizations (Ertmer & Stoel-Gammon,
Engaging in Vocal Play 2008). Research findings demonstrate
that adults need to (a) reinforce and
Vocal play occurs during the prelin- imitate their children’s spontaneous
guistic or preverbal stage of early child- vocalizations, (b) repeat the vocal play
hood, before toddlers say their first to help their children make associations
meaningful or recognizable words between what was heard and what was
(Nathani, Ertmer, & Stark, 2006). Dur- said, and (c) vary their own vocal play
ing this stage, young children typically in order to facilitate speech perception
progress from intentional cooing and and production (Ertmer & Stoel-Gam-
vowel utterances to “blowing raspber- mon, 2008; Humes & Bess, 2014).
ries” and making sounds with their lips The sounds perceived and babbled
and tongues (Kuhl & Meltzoff, 1996). It by babies depend on the sounds they
also progresses from simple babbling hear in their linguistic environment
to more complex babbling that sounds (Kokkinaki & Vitalaki, 2013). Therefore,
more like the stress and intonation of as soon as hearing devices are fitted, it
their native language (Boysson-Bardies, is important that adults provide many
1999). Babbling happens when infants opportunities for the child to hear all
repeatedly alternate vowels with con- the sounds used in their native lan-
sonants such as “bababa” and “dada- guage (Bass-Ringdahl, 2010).
boode” (Pettito, 2005). Finally, as children Toward that end, the AV practitio-
come to “know” their mother tongue, ner coaches parents on how to engage
they use jargon that is babbling inter- in vocal play with their children and, at
spersed with some recognizable words the same time, parents learn the impor-
(Saffran, Werker, & Werner, 2006). tance of delivering an “auditory pho-
Over the course of a year or so, neme schema” for specific sounds in
vocal play results in increasingly com- their language (Pollack, 1972, p. 164).
plex, phonetically diverse, and speech- This means the child’s brain receives a
like vocalizations prior to speaking a clear auditory imprint of those sounds
language (Nathani et al., 2006). Be- that can be babbled directly into the
cause babies tend to match their vocal- child’s ear while the parent carries the
izations to those they hear, this vocal child and dances with him or her, all
play becomes language-specific in that the while ensuring speaking within
it varies from culture to culture (Kuhl & earshot. Playful babbling of isolated
Meltzoff, 1996). The prelinguistic stage sounds can take place several times
is important for building “internal pho- throughout the day, for a few minutes
nological representations” (Narr, 2006). each time.
Adult feedback to infant babbling Subsequently, some children may
facilitates learning the speech patterns babble and others may not ( Jones,
296  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

2007). The child’s babbling is not, how- sound (Bernhardt, Kemp, & Werker,
ever, the primary goal. The primary goal 2007; Monaghan, Shillcock, Christian-
is to provide the child’s brain with the sen, & Kirby, 2014).
experience of hearing native speech For example, repeatedly saying
sounds in isolation (Sharma et al., “/b/b/b/” each time the child plays with
2004). The child is then much more a toy bus (the object) or sees a real
likely to differentiate between sounds bus, helps the child associate meaning
and to vocalize them in return (Werker with the phoneme /b/. Over a relatively
& Gervain, 2013). short period of time, the child learns to
differentiate /b/ from other phonemes
Associating Sounds With such as /ah/ which was associated with
Objects and Words a different object (an airplane). The AV
practitioner can also associate particu-
Associative learning is one mecha- lar sounds with actions that are par-
nism for enhancing speech perception ticularly meaningful to the child, such
(Kahana-Kalman & Walker-Andrews, as “/k/k/k/” with the act of coughing.
2001). For example, young infants learn Because many of these sounds can be
the relationship between vocal affect easy for the young child to hear and
and facial expressions (Kahana-Kalman say, they help the child to communicate
& Walker-Andrews. 2001). Older infants as early as possible. These are often re-
learn to associate complex strings of ferred to as the Learning to Listen Sounds
speech sounds such as words with (Estabrooks, 2006) and/or Sound-Object
objects and actions; cognitive mecha- Associations (Rhoades, 2007).
nisms affect their auditory perception Rather than introducing sounds/
of speech (Houston, 2011). objects in random order, it is helpful
Sound-object association activi- to group sound into similar categories,
ties help infants and preschoolers to such as animal sounds versus vehicle
(a)  strengthen auditory attentional sounds. Young infants categorize newly
focus, (b) develop an auditory imprint learned concepts (Hasegawa & Miyas-
or brain schema for specific sounds, hita, 2002), and the earlier they estab-
(c) recognize that these sounds or pho- lish a link between sounds or labels
nemes are different, and (d) maintain and categories, the more words they
their interest while enhancing speech will learn; this important link facilitates
perception (Ertmer & Stoel-Gammon, cognition and further word learning
2008; MacKenzie, Graham, & Curtin, (Ferguson, Havy, & Waxman, 2015).
2011). Furthermore, when children are Because the adult is within ear-
repeatedly exposed to these activities, shot every time specific sounds are
familiarity facilitates sound differentia- associated with specific objects, the
tion; this, in turn, enhances speech per- child is primed for verbal learning.
ception (Minagawa-Kawai, Mori, Naoi, The more sounds with which the child
& Kojima, 2006). Finally, this form of becomes familiar, the more quickly he
symbolic learning promotes language or she will understand the words. The
comprehension because the child sounds selected need to be specific to
learns to associate meaning with each the child’s mother tongue and culture
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  297

(the sound used for a barking dog var- pering select sounds and words is an
ies from culture to culture). When the important strategy in AVT.
child spontaneously produces any of
these sounds, it gives adults important Singing
information on how the sounds are per-
ceived by the child. Singing rhymes and songs is a univer-
sally pleasurable experience for chil-
Whispering dren and their parents (Trehub, 2015).
Singing facilitates listening skills, lan-
Babies and toddlers enjoy having adults guage, speech perception and produc-
whisper in their ears. Aside from the tion, as well as language and such cog-
enjoyment, whispering can make cer- nitive skills as memory (Wang, Trehub,
tain sounds more acoustically salient Volkova, & van Lieshout, 2013). Singing
(Kohlberger & Strycharczuk, 2015). Whis- can also help lengthen the child’s atten-
pering a few inches from the child’s tion span and spontaneous imitation
microphone is as loud as a shout from (Bergeson & Trehub, 2002) as well as
a distance of a few yards (Ling, 1976, facilitate parent-child interactions and
p. 233). For example, whispering spe- social bonding (Cirelli, Wan, & Trainor,
cific sounds, words, and/or phrases can 2014).
help children hear them more easily, The AV practitioner and parent
including high-frequency consonants encourage children with hearing loss
such as /h/, /s/, /t/, /k/, /f/, /p/, /sh/, to sing, even when they cannot carry a
and /th/. tune. The rhythmic nature of songs may
These high-frequency sounds tend play a vital role in speech fluency (Tier-
to be more difficult to hear for some ney & Kraus, 2015). Music can enhance
children who wear hearing aids (Narr, the perception of pitch, and the use of
2006), particularly those not wearing intonation (Marques, Moreno, Castro, &
digital ones. Some children with hear- Besson, 2007). Young children in AVT
ing loss benefit from hearing these can enjoy the rhythmic hand motions
high-frequency consonants whispered and gestures in such songs as “Twinkle,
in isolation and within earshot. First, Twinkle, Little Star” and “The Wheels
consonants seem to have greater per- on the Bus” (Estabrooks & Birkenshaw-
ceptual importance because they are Fleming, 2003). Across the preschool
essential to the intelligibility of words years, listening to and rehearsing songs
(Owren & Cardillo, 2006). Second, vow- may help the child to memorize the
els, when voiced, carry more energy so words and subsequently remember all
they sound louder than high-frequency the verses while singing rhythmically.
consonants, tending to mask those Restated, singing songs can facilitate
sounds. Third, when a sound/phoneme language learning (Ludke, Ferreira, &
is not audible, the child cannot develop Overy, 2014). Singing is typically inte-
an auditory-based internal representa- grated into AVT sessions where children
tion, and the likelihood of effortless can follow their parents’ lead in singing
phonemic production is diminished and through cumulative practice they
(Narr, 2006). For these reasons, whis- memorize the lyrics, similar to children
298  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

with typical hearing (Driscoll, Gfeller. learning and cognition (Mastropavlou,


Tan, See, Cheng, & Kanemitsu, 2015). 2010; Pedale & Santangelo, 2015).

Stressing Selected Morphemes, GOAL 4: Promote


Syllables, Words, and Phrases
Knowledge of Language
Stressed morphemes, syllables, words,
and phrases are longer, louder, and Strategies
higher pitched than those that are
unstressed. Babies perceive stress as n Focusing on the “knowing”
an acoustic cue to learning about word rather than the “using”
boundaries and figuring out the pat- n Taking turns
terns of spoken language (Thiessen n Imitating the child’s early
& Saffran, 2007). The AV practitioner vocalizations
refers to this strategy as another form of n Verbalizing in synchrony with
acoustic highlighting because it helps movement
the child to hear particular features n Speaking the language from the
of language more clearly. This assigns child’s angle
“perceptual salience” to the morpheme, n Talking before, during, and after
syllable, word, or phrase selected by the action
the practitioner; the linguistic element n Pausing for grammatical spaces
is brought to the child’s focused atten- or emphasis
tion (Pruden, Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff, & n Transitioning beyond the
Hennon, 2006). comfort zone
Examples of linguistic elements that n Connecting the familiar to the
AV practitioners often stress include unfamiliar
n Recasting, expanding, and
n the /s/ morpheme in Mario’s to expatiating on the child’s words
help the child learn about the n Emphasizing actions, relations,
possessive feature; and attributes
n the first syllable in brontosaurus n Contrasting the meaning of
to help the child understand words
how this word differs from
brachiosaurus; The goal of promoting familiarity with
n the word /this/ versus /that/ a spoken language can occur only if
to help the child recognize the the child demonstrates focused audi-
difference between the two tory attention. Historically, parents were
articles within the same gram- often given the generic advice of talk-
matical category; and ing all the time to their children, but
n the phrase /no more/ after the evidence shows this is not enough. How
child has finished eating. parents interact with their child also
matters. The AV practitioner helps par-
Just as stressing phonemes enhances ents by supporting them in their child-
speech perception (Narr, 2006), stress- directed verbal input — both in quality
ing linguistic features enhances language and quantity (Rowe, 2012). Likewise,
Chapter 10    Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  299
n

knowing when not to talk is important. Zelazo, Sharpe, & Mashari, 2014). Rather
Adult-controlled pauses are known to than expressive language, early recep-
structure and regulate language learn- tive language skills are good predictors
ing. Toward that end, the effective use of later language performance (Hay-
of interactive silences is significant in McCutcheon, Kirk, Henning, Gao, &
AVT. The following strategies used by Qi, 2008).
the AV practitioner are only part of an Some children with hearing loss,
exhaustive list that might be shared however, are often unfortunately judged
with parents. by what they say rather than by what
they understand. Young children’s com-
Focusing on the “Knowing” prehension of spoken language is far
Rather Than the “Using” more important than their production
of it (Duff, Reen, Plunkett, & Nation,
Linguistic competence is typically at- 2015; Rescorla, 2009). It is critical that
tained by children between 3 and 4 the AV practitioner continuously guide
years of age (Pinker, 1994). This means parents to understand that “knowledge
that very young children with typical always precedes use” and that the num-
hearing understand their native lan- ber of words a child comprehends far
guage by the time they are of preschool exceeds the number of words produced.
age. As long as the adult’s vocabulary In fact, a problem presents itself when
level is kept to the child’s known vocab- a child says all that he knows; he or she
ulary level, an adult can easily converse will then have no reservoir of words
with preschoolers. Young children are from which to draw. Moreover, this
able to independently detect and fig- reflects atypical, unnatural language
ure out the patterns of spoken language development ( Junge, Koojiman, Hag-
(Aslin & Newport, 2012), even when gort, & Cutler, 2012). Children typi-
they might not have intelligible speech. cally accumulate a considerable “stor-
When a child knows a spoken lan- age bank” of word knowledge before
guage, he or she comprehends it. Typical they withdraw from it and start using
2-year-old children know about three it. Indeed, many children with typical
times as many words as they actually hearing do not say more than a few
say (Tomasello, 2003). The child who words until they are 2 years of age, yet
demonstrates linguistic competence is they “know” syntactic structure when
able to understand complex sentences listening to spoken language (Bernal,
and stories and to follow multistep Dehaene-Lambertz, Millotte, & Chris-
verbal directions. Moreover, the pre- tophe, 2010).
schooler with typical hearing intuitively The AV practitioner ensures that
understands the appropriate placement parental expectations are high, realis-
of grammatical categories having to do tic, and fair. In general, during the first
with pronouns, prepositions, adjectives, year of hearing with hearing technol-
verb tenses, and articles (Arunachalam ogy, children internalize the sounds
& Waxman, 2010). Day-to-day com- of their native language and the rules
munication is possible. Of course, as of word order (Benasich, Choudhury,
children mature, their knowledge of Realpe-Bonilla, & Roesler, 2014). Dur-
grammar becomes more complex (Gao, ing the second year, the child usually
300  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

figures out the grammatical patterns the child moves. This turn-taking, mea-
of the language that is heard; this is sured in milliseconds, reflects mutual
when young children begin producing reciprocity or parent-infant coordina-
words combinations (Ferguson, Graf, & tion known as interactional synchrony.
Waxman, 2014; Yang, 2006). When par- Taking turns gives the child the power
ents understand this, they are far more of being, facilitates joint attention,
likely to focus on the “knowing” rather and minimizes interruptions (Stanton-
than the “using” of spoken language. Chapman & Snell, 2011). This occurs
Although this can be a difficult concept across all cultures (Leclère, Viaux,
for some parents who are anxious to Avril, Achard, Chetouani, Missonnier,
hear their child talk, the importance of & Cohen, 2014).
this strategy cannot be overstated. The child learns to embrace the
The AV practitioner and parent turn-taking skill of listening, then talk-
give voice to the child who has not yet ing, then listening, then talking. Taking
developed a spoken language. Perhaps turns is also one of the child’s first steps
the practitioner noticed a subtle silent toward attaining social competence
interaction that occurred between par- (Stanton-Chapman & Snell, 2011). Even
ent and child — that is, the child made a the toddler who does not have access
subtle eye or slight touch contact with to sound can take turns in all activities.
the parent who automatically (perhaps The child learns to wait for his turn after
unconsciously) responded to the child’s the AV practitioner presents an item in
cue by quietly doing something for a play activity. Equally important, the
the child. Because many parents new child also learns that when it is his turn,
to AVT are not aware of these “word- he will be able to freely manipulate the
less exchanges,” the AV practitioner item for at least a few minutes.
immediately informs the parents what
he or she just observed and then dem- Imitating the Child’s
onstrates alternative ways of respond- Early Vocalizations
ing to the child, always using spoken
language. Restated, the AV practitioner When the AV practitioner imitates what
promotes change by bringing “wordless the young child says, he or she pro-
exchanges” to the parent’s attention, in vides positive feedback by attending to
order to instill the habit of voicing each the child and showing the young child
interaction with the child. Although the that his communicative behavior is
child does not yet talk, this enables par- heard, acknowledged, and understood
ents to help their children “know” the (Bovey & Strain, 2005). Imitating (echo-
spoken language. ing) what the child says, whether it is
babbling or jargon, also promotes joint
Taking Turns attention and turn taking, leading to
social engagement (Farrant, Maybery,
Turn-taking between the parent and & Fletcher, 2011). Most importantly,
child with typical hearing begins at because adult imitations reinforce what
birth (Gratier, 2003). When the par- the child says, the child receives audi-
ent talks, the newborn child stills and tory feedback that encourages further
listens. When the parent stops talking, vocalizations and further adult imita-
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  301

tions. This ongoing turn-taking results When adults talk about what they
in verbal expansions and is sometimes do, children can learn language when
referred to as an expanding auditory- what they hear is time-synchronized
vocal spiral (Kuhl, 2004). with what they see, particularly in
Interestingly, imitations can also fa- terms of action-based behaviors (Mac-
cilitate language comprehension (Adank, rae, Duffy, Miles, & Lawrence, 2008).
Hagoort, & Bekkering, 2010). When This has been referred to as “language-
parent-child verbal interactions are action synchrony” (Rhoades, 2010). For
insufficient, the child may have a dif- example, when popping bubbles, the
ficult time figuring out the patterns AV practitioner might say, “Pop!” at
of language. In turn, this can lead to the same time that the child actually
the child exhibiting language delays pops the bubble. Such synchroniza-
(Barker et al, 2009). So, it is important tion may increase the child’s attention
then, that the AV practitioner demon- to what was said in addition to facili-
strates ways to respond, appropriately tating a pattern of recognition of the
and often, to the young child’s attempts acoustic event; this relates especially to
at communicating by imitating early action verbs (Bahrick & Lickliter, 2009;
vocalizations. Gogate, Bahrick, & Watson, 2000).

Verbalizing in Synchrony Speaking the Language


With Movement From the Child’s Angle
The brain’s mirror neuron system is tri- Every child’s behavior is a communica-
modal in that it integrates motor, visual, tive message that the adult verbalizes
and auditory information. The pro- for the child (Olson & Masur, 2013).
cessing of spoken language is linked To avoid having the child feel that
to motor system activity in the brain spoken language is irrelevant, the par-
( James & Maouene, 2009). Embedded ent’s language needs to be meaning-
in social interactions, the acquisition of ful, purposeful, and age-appropriate for
spoken language is typically a layered the child.
process (Vigliocco, Perniss, & Vinson, The AV practitioner puts into words
2014). Synchrony in the acquisition of what the child’s gestures, eye gazes,
information can facilitate more rapid and vocalizations are trying to commu-
learning (Lau & Salzman, 2009). nicate. This parallel talk, found to be a
Because this cross-modal learning predictor of later language functioning,
does not occur among young children means the adult talks about what the
without sufficient access to hearing, child wants to say rather than what the
their brains are reorganized differently adult wants the child to say. It is impor-
(Li et al,. 2012); hence, they learn dif- tant for the adult to try looking at the
ferently (Marschark, Spencer, Adams, world through the child’s eyes (Cruz,
& Sapere, 2011). Lack of integration Quittner, Marker, & DesJardin, 2013).
between seeing, hearing, and doing can By following the child’s lead, the
affect language and social development AV practitioner engages in “follow-in”
as well as learning in general (Le Bel, speech and strives to understand what
Pineda, & Sharma, 2009). the child has seen, heard, and/or done
302  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

and how the child feels. When the prac- (Lederberg & Spencer, 2009). The size
titioner recognizes the child’s interests, of a child’s lexicon is a predictor of lan-
he or she talks about how the child is guage and academic learning (Bishop &
experiencing the world, using words Adams, 2006). Therefore, practitioners
that give rich meaning to the child’s and parents need to promote vocabu-
experiences (Yoder, McCathren, War- lary expansion for children with hear-
ren, & Watson, 2001). As the child gains ing loss, as rapidly as possible.
listening and spoken language skills, Word learning, the process of ac-
the parent listens closely to the child’s quiring a lexicon, is an indicator of the
utterances, and indicates that what he child’s ability to understand or produce a
or she says is important. Parents learn word after repeated exposures in a sup-
not to interrupt when the child is talk- ported learning context (Bobzien et al.,
ing, even if they do not understand 2015; Gray, 2004). Repetition, a feature
what the child says. They also learn of parentese, is integral to vocabulary
to imitate the child’s sounds, actions, acquisition (Newman, Rowe, & Ratner,
words, or sentences, and then to wait in press). However, due to the effects of
and see what the child does next. background noise, this feature may be
During an AVT session, the child even more important for children with
may point to an object that is some- hearing loss since their prior exposure
what related to the activity. Rather than to words may have been compromised
ignoring what the child indicated, the (Blaiser, Nelson, & Kohnert, 2015).
AV practitioner takes advantage of the The AV practitioner talks before
spontaneous diversion by verbally relat- the action. This allows the child to
ing what was observed to the activity anticipate what the adult may be hold-
they are all doing. For example, during ing, what will happen, and when the
clean-up time, a feather in a box cap- activity will stop. Talking before acting
tures the child’s interest and she says, encourages the child to listen to the
“Bee!” Instead of ignoring the child’s adult’s spoken language without atten-
verbal attempt while cleaning up the tion being diverted elsewhere (South-
toys and putting them in the box, the gate, Chevalier, & Csibra, 2009). For
AV practitioner leans in and say, “Oh, example, the adult may have a toy in a
you think this is a bee? No, it’s not a paper bag and before opening it, says
bee; it’s just a feather! Do you want to “I have a toy in here.” The adult looks
blow the feather out of the box?” in the bag without letting the child see
it, and says, “Yes, my toy is in here. It’s
Talking Before, During, a wind-up monkey. Do you want to
and After the Action see it?” Then the child looks in the bag
and can play with the toy while the
Lexical knowledge (the corpus of words practitioner narrates what the child is
understood by a young child) is associ- doing. The adult’s repeated spoken lan-
ated with listening and word learning guage is given meaning. Similarly, the
(Florit, Roch, Altoe & Levorato, 2009; AV practitioner talks after the action is
Gray & Brinkley, 2011). The more words completed; this provides both repeti-
understood by a child, the more likely tion and support for the meaning of
the child will rapidly learn new words the message already heard. “We jumped
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  303

up and down, up and down. Jumping word or key phrase, or to wait in sus-
was so much fun. But I’m tired now. No pension for the next thought (Stahl,
more jumping.” 1994). Impact pauses act as stress mark-
ers by giving the child time to think
Pausing for Grammatical about what was or will be said. The
Spaces or Stress Markers child is not expected to provide a ver-
bal response. Stressed words may help
This refers to two specific types of delib- the child construct meaning from what
erate adult-controlled silences known was heard (Yeldham & Gruba, 2014).
as the phrasal intraturn pause and the The AV practitioner helps the child
impact pause. Both pauses occur within understand grammatical boundaries
a language-based activity, serving as lis- and promotes anticipation of a new
tening prompts that cue the child to pay word or linguistic unit just before ver-
attention to the message of the spo- balizing it (Read, Macauley, & Furay,
ken language. Both promote language 2014). For example, the practitioner
learning by having silences effectively tells the child, “This morning, I spilled
bracket meaningful language (Rowe my juice on the floor. (pause) I was so
& Rowe, 2006). In turn, this facilitates upset. (pause) The carpet was soiled
auditory attention, language growth, (stress new word and then pause) and
cognitive skills, and academic learning I  did not know what to do. (pause)
(Vassilopoulos & Konstantinidis, 2012). What do you think I did next?” This will
Neither type of pause is used to elicit be followed by a much longer pause.
vocal response from the child. An incidental benefit of these pauses is
The first type of pause reflects a that the child may use those moments
natural rest in the melody of spoken of silence to offer suggestions or ask
language to indicate grammatical spaces questions.
such as the end of linguistic units such
as phrases or sentences (Carey-Sargeant Transitioning Beyond
& Brown, 2003). The strategic place- the Comfort Zone
ment of this pause cues the child to pay
special attention to the word, phrase, The child’s comfort zone is the lan-
or sentence that follows, or to think guage level at which he or she currently
about what was just said. When delib- understands with relative ease (Mer-
erate silences serve as discourse mark- cer, Ryan, & Williams, 2012). Parents
ers, they can help the child understand typically talk within that comfort zone
what was heard (Yeldham & Gruba, because it is safe, easy, and quick. In
2014). Young children with hearing loss early sessions of AVT, the AV practitio-
tend to require longer pause lengths to ner’s language is also mostly tuned to
process linguistic information (Carey- the child’s comfort zone. Shortly there-
Sargeant & Brown, 2003). after, however, the AV practitioner guides
The second type of adult-controlled the parent to raise the bar or up the ante
silence is the impact pause. Often used (Chaiklin, 2003; Wasik, Bond, & Hind-
as a strategy when reading a story to man, 2006). Change forces growth. This
children, this deliberate silence helps means the language spoken to the child
them recognize an important or new must become a bit more complex so
304  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

the child can build his or her language Recasting, Expanding,


knowledge. For example, when a child and Expatiating on
demonstrates understanding of a phrase the Child’s Words
like “Throw it away,” the AV practitioner
demonstrates using a different linguistic It is important that adults provide feed-
unit or phrase such as “Please throw back in the form of good language mod-
this in the can over there.” This 1 plus 2 els when young children produce their
rule adds two new “bits” to the one “bit” first words, phrases, and sentences. As
that the child already knows. long as the child’s intended meaning is
If the child can remember two items preserved, adults can recast, rephrase,
when asked to get them from the basket paraphrase, or otherwise expand on
across the room, the parent adds more the child’s utterances to improve the
to the mix, so that the child remembers child’s knowledge and use of language
even more. If the child can take two (Proctor-Williams, Fey, & Loeb, 2001;
turns in conversation, the practitioner Strapp & Federico, 2000). This means
then demonstrates ways to take three that adults do not tell children that their
turns. And if the child can identify an sentences are wrong (White, Livesey, &
item by description, then the practi- Hayes, 2015).
tioner moves toward identification of For example, the child says, “dada
an item with more attributes. The AV bye-bye.” There are at least three ways
practitioner consistently encourages to provide the child with feedback:
parents to transition beyond their usual
ways of using language to reflect ongo- n Recast:  restate the child’s
ing change and growth. utterance into a different format,
such as a question format, e.g.,
Connecting the Familiar “Is daddy going bye-bye?”
n Expand:  repeat the child’s utter-
to the Unfamiliar
ance but in a more grammatical
The AV practitioner provides back-up and complete way without
support for the child’s acquisition of modifying the child’s word order
new words by connecting the old with or intended meaning, e.g., “Yes,
the new. Because the adult understands daddy is now going bye-bye.”
what the child already knows, the adult n Expatiate:  expand on the child’s
is the bridge between the known word utterance, but with the addition
and the unknown word and provides of new information, e.g., “Yes,
a “lexical hook.” The parent provides a daddy is going bye-bye. Daddy
new word for an already-known one. has to go to work. Daddy will
For example, a father might say, “Let’s come home later. We’ll miss him.
go in my car (child knows this word). See you later daddy!”
I got a new car. It’s a convertible (new
word to child). How about a drive in my Feedback can be in the form of add-
convertible?” Adults clarify or ‘bridge’ ing more information or just a word,
new words with the old. This helps or providing a complete grammatical
expand a child’s vocabulary (Callanan structure (Cruz et al., 2013; Dunst et al.,
& Sabbagh, 2004). 2011; Wasik et al., 2006). Regardless,
Chapter 10    Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  305
n

the adult provides a more complex Grammatically speaking, adjec-


form of language modeling than that tives tend to be dependent on nouns
of the child, without requiring the child and verbs. Nevertheless, adjectives
to repeat anything. are descriptive words that can matter
more to children, particularly when
Emphasizing Actions, they are easily observable or experi-
Relations, and Attributes enced (Booth & Waxman, 2009). For
example, the AV practitioner describes
Toddlers typically understand the gram- how things feel, sound, look, or move,
mar of their native language (Bernal for example, “Mmmmm, this feels so
et al., 2010). This means babies begin SOFT!” or “Wow, that is HOT!”
figuring out the grammatical rules so Prepositions, known as functional
they later know how to put words words, are typically understood before
together; this is often referred to as the end of infancy (Shi, 2014). The fre-
connected language. The three gram- quent use of prepositions occurs when
matical word categories that are criti- adults talk about the relation of the
cal for young children to understand child to something. This is referred to
are verbs, prepositions, and adjectives. as spatial relations. The AV practitioner
These word types seem universal in places a little more stress on preposi-
that they interest babies across all cul- tions when children are moving in, on,
tures (Tomasello, 2003). Furthermore, and around items, for example, “It’s
these word types reflect language that UNDER the chair.”
emphasizes words and concepts needed When adults model spoken lan-
for academic success (cognition), such guage for a child with hearing loss, both
as prepositions for spatial understand- quality and quantity matter. Children
ing and adjectives for categorical learn- learn by what they hear, remember, and
ing (Henninger, 2013; Weikart, Rogers, recall, and they discover their native
Adcock, & McClelland, 1971). language by figuring out the rules and
Action-based verbs are among the word order, like little investigators.
easiest category of words for young Children determine the linguistic pat-
children to understand, especially when terns when they hear the way adults
they are engaged in doing something use action verbs, prepositions, and
(Twomey, Lush, Pearce, & Horst, 2014; adjectives (Tomasello, 2003). Toward
Waxman, Fu, Arunachalam, Leddon, that end, rather than stressing nouns,
Geraghty, & Song, 2013). In AVT dur- parents are encouraged to acoustically
ing purposeful play, the adults use highlight action verbs, prepositions,
sentences that include labeling the action verbs, and adjectives (Yin &
child’s action while in progress (Syrett, Csibra, 2015).
Arunachalam, & Waxman, 2014). Because
language tends to be action based, and Contrasting the
the AV sessions are delivered through Meaning of Words
purposeful play, or real-life activities,
the adults in the child’s life have multi- Children come to understand the mean-
ple opportunities to model many verbs ing of words more easily and precisely
(Bergen & Wheeler, 2010). when polar opposites are brought to
306  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

their attention (Bellon-Harn, Credeur- frequency of use, which decreases over


Pampolina, & LeBoeuf, 2013). In other time, depend on the child’s stage of
words, a child can more easily under- development and his or her responses
stand the meaning of “hot” when con- to previously used prompts. Among
trasted to the word “cold” (Tomasello, these prompts is the use of interactive
2003). To fully understand a word’s silences; the types of deliberate silences
meaning, it helps to understand what used as strategies for this goal specifi-
it is and what it is not. So, the AV practi- cally structure and regulate verbal inter-
tioner makes concerted efforts to facili- actions between adults and children
tate word learning by using words that (Damron & Morman, 2011).
contrast in meaning, for example, “This
is so BIG; that one is NOT big. That one Leaning Forward With
is little.” and “My hair is STRAIGHT, but Expectant Looks
yours is CURLY.”
Leaning forward to the child and/or
parent and looking expectantly are non-
GOAL 5: Facilitate Spoken verbal prompts used by the AV prac-
Language and Cognition titioner to model listening behaviors,
encourage and establish joint attention,
Strategies and signal the child and/or parent that
a verbal response is expected. Leaning
n Leaning forward with expectant forward is a posture shift that is fre-
looks quently used at the beginning of a con-
n Signaling with objects versation (Cassell, Nakano, Bickmore,
n Providing self-statements Sidner, & Rich, 2001). If a child is look-
n Asking “What did you hear?” ing at a book and points at a picture of a
n Waiting for the child’s response favorite toy and looks at the parent, the
n Promoting auditory-verbal parent leans in and looks as if he or she
closure is waiting for the child to say something.
n Asking stage-appropriate This prompt is often used in con-
questions junction with eyebrow raising/lower-
n Scaffolding for language ing, and head nodding. Guaïtella and
production colleagues (2009) found that eyebrow
movements serve as an attention-
The AV practitioner uses a variety of getting device and play a role in turn
strategies to elicit targeted verbal behav- taking. Eyebrow raises also occur with
iors. Some strategies cited here consist questions, directions, and explanations,
of “actions” that are often referred to and signal that a verbal or nonverbal
as prompting or prompts (see Goal 2). response is expected (Flecha-Garcia,
Prompting is the act of assisting or 2010). The parent may say, “What hap-
encouraging a child to perform an pened to your shoes?” and wait. If the
action such as giving a verbal response. child does not respond, the parent
In general, language prompts are used leans forward, and looks expectantly
to initiate a series of interrelated lan- at the child.
guage-based actions (Hayes, 2014). The Adults make regular use of declara-
selection of types of prompts and their tive questions such as “Is this a ball?”
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  307

in their conversations with preschool Signaling With Objects


children (Estigarribia, 2010). Typi-
cally, this question is initially accom- As they learn language, young children
panied by three features: (a) prosodic are sensitive to the gestures of others
cues, such as the rising terminal pitch; in conversational situations (Iverson &
(b) contextual cues, such as the object Goldin-Meadow, 2005). This signaling
noted in the question; and (c) visual strategy involves using an object as
or facial cues such as raised eyebrows a physical cue or prompt to signal the
(Frota, Butler, & Vigário, 2014; Gunlog- child that it is his turn to talk. Some
son, 2003; Srinivasan & Massaro, 2003). examples are toy microphones or sym-
Without the presence of these three bolic representations such as cardboard
supplementary cues, preschoolers do tubes. This turn-taking prompt informs
not understand the question requires the child that a sound, word, or phrase
a yes/no response (Saindon, Trehub, needs to be imitated or initiated.
Schellenberg, & Van Lieshout, in press). For example, the AV practitioner
When the child responds appropri- uses one of the aforementioned prompts,
ately to declarative questions with all such as leaning forward and looking
features present, then the practitioner expectantly, to elicit a verbal response.
removes the visual cues. This facilitates But, if this is unsuccessful, the practitio-
the child’s transition to listening for ner repeats the prompt with the addi-
prosodic cues and comprehension of tion of a physical item that is directed
the question form. toward the parent, who models the
Verbal fillers such as “hmm,” “um,” targeted response. The physical item is
and “uh” may be used with facial or then directed to the child who typically
visual prompts. These “placeholders” imitates the parent’s model. Over time,
or “unglossable syllables” seem to help signaling with objects decreases.
children attend more to the words that
immediately follow (Peters, 2001). These Providing Self-Statements
fillers also serve a grammatical function
in conversation as well as benefit lan- Verbal self-disclosure has been high-
guage comprehension (Barr & Seyfeddi- lighted in the literature as an encour-
nipur, 2010). A verbal filler such as “um” aging way for individuals to share
can induce an anticipation effect, caus- their own thoughts and feelings, hence
ing listeners to expect new information. promoting verbal interaction between
For example, the AV practitioner people (Ignatius & Kokkonen, 2007).
knows that the child wants to play with In AVT, the child may make spontane-
a particular toy in the therapy room ous statements in response to the AV
because the child is looking at it with practitioner’s self-statements (Urry, Nel-
a look of anticipation. The AV practitio- son, & Padilla-Walker, 2011). Simply
ner rubs his chin and says, “Hmm” and described, the adult might begin the
waits expectantly. The child may then sentence with an “I” statement such as
say, “Bubbles!” But if not, the AV prac- “I feel like singing.”
titioner may murmur “Um” again to get For example, if a child points to a
the child’s attention and to let the child bottle of bubbles, and says, “Eh!”, the
know that the practitioner is waiting for AV practitioner may say, “You want
something to be said. those bubbles!” This provides a verbal
308  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

model that matches the gesture and process the information, and then
vocalization. Then the adult follows repeat those parts that were heard or
with a self-statement such as, “I want understood. In turn, this can help the
those bubbles too!” Or, at a slightly child use his or her linguistic knowl-
more advanced language level, the edge to “fill in the missing pieces.” This
adult might say, “I have that at home, can help the child to develop listening
and it’s one of my favorite toys!” skills that facilitate language compre-
This strategy is dependent on the hension, and make predictions based
adult’s willingness to trust that the child on key words he or she understood
will continue the conversation by mak- (Yeldham & Gruba, 2014).
ing an utterance. Increasing the use of
self-statements, and moving away from Promoting
direct questions, increases the child’s Auditory-Verbal Closure
willingness to converse with adults
(Power, Wood, Wood, & Macdougall, The development of spoken language
1990). Some children are passive com- is dependent on many factors, includ-
municators because they previously ing cognitive skills, including working
felt pressured to talk or because they memory (Wingfield, & Tun, 2007). Cog-
felt like they were often being tested. nitive skills play an even greater role
This strategy, like all those in this chap- for children with hearing loss because
ter, helps the child become an active they often need to understand spoken
communicator. language when the auditory input is
The difference between modeling compromised by background noise,
and providing self-statements is the tone conversations with people who speak
of voice and the intent of the speaker, very quietly, with imperfect diction or
with self-statements sometimes sound- with a dialect. Prior knowledge of lan-
ing more humorously argumentative. guage and the topic of conversation are
When adults share their own thoughts two factors that play a role in how well
and feelings, they promote the child’s speech is understood within a sentence
social skills (Moll & Meltzoff, 2011) and or longer linguistic units. These factors
the child is encouraged to think outside provide contextual and linguistic cues
of the self and to realize that others may for understanding (Shafiro, Sheft, Gygi,
have different perspectives. & Ho, 2012).
The redundancy of language per-
Asking, “What Did You Hear?” mits the child to engage in statistical
learning — that is, to figure out what
Sometimes the child with hearing loss makes sense given that word order is
quickly responds by saying, “What?” based on rules where specific words
when an adult says something that the are used repetitively. Practice in remem-
child does not fully understand. Rather bering sentences and then completing
than coaching the parent to automati- them when the sentences are incom-
cally repeat the stimulus, the AV prac- plete can help the child’s speech per-
titioner encourages the parent to ask, ception in noisy environments (Shafiro
“What did you hear?” This encourages et al., 2012). Although this strategy
the child to think about what was said, enhances speech perception (Goal 3),
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  309

it also facilitates spoken language and this task, then the practitioner redi-
cognition in that the child is prompted rects attention to the parent, repeats
to practice talking using a completed the rhyme or song in the same way,
linguistic unit. and the parent responds appropriately.
When the child is asked to infer The final step is that the AV practitioner
missing words within sentences, the repeats it with the child again. By this
semantic and syntactic context pro- time, the child typically “gets it” and
vide cues to the missing words. This is able to complete the task. Both par-
means the child is asked to complete ent and child end up feeling successful.
the perceptual whole from incomplete The AV practitioner can repeatedly use
language information (Gallun, Mason, this strategy of auditory-verbal closure
& Kidd, 2007). This is a modified ver- in relatively quiet environments. Over
sion of the cloze procedure used for time, background noise can be gradu-
the comprehension of printed language ally increased to make the task more
(e.g., Sharp, 2009). So, for purposes of difficult.
this goal, the strategy to facilitate sen-
tence completion is referred to as audi- Waiting for Child’s Response
tory-verbal closure: the child’s ability to
fill in missing portions of the spoken Wait-time occurs after the AV practitio-
message based on prior knowledge and ner asks a child a question. It is the
familiarity of the language and topic length of undisturbed time that an adult
(Hannemann, Obleser, & Eulitz, 2007). waits for a child’s response (Rowe,
This top-down processing strategy sig- 1996). The child’s response may come
nals the child that it is his or her turn in the form of a single word, phrase,
to finish the message, or connected language. Wait-times give
Toddlers with typical hearing can the child opportunities to decode the
be prompted with this auditory-verbal meaning of the question (Stephenson,
closure strategy in games such as “Peek- Carter, & Arthur-Kelly, 2011). Extended
a-boo. I see ---!” Preschoolers can com- wait-times tend to be associated with
plete the last part of many well-known positive responses and improved lan-
phrases, as in the story about the three guage learning (Brown & Wragg, 1993;
little pigs: “He huffed and he puffed Al-Balushi, 2009).
and he ---.” Children who complete such After a question is posed to the
tasks are able to predict the missing tar- child with typical hearing, the undis-
get word(s) or sound(s) based on con- turbed waiting time should not be less
text and prior knowledge of the spoken than 5 s (Gabel, 2004; Skinner, Pappas,
language (Pickering & Garrod, 2006). & Davis, 2005). The wait-time, however,
Similarly the AV practitioner can may need to be considerably longer for
sing a song familiar to the child, and some children with hearing loss (Rowe
then stop just before completing it. Con- & Rowe, 2006). The length of wait-time
currently, toward the end of the song, time is dependent on such factors such
the practitioner leans in toward the as linguistic complexity of the question
child and pauses. If the child remem- and expected response, the child’s devel-
bers the song, the child “fills in the opmental level, and auditory process-
blank.” If the child does not complete ing capacities as well as environmental
310  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

noise level (Maroni, 2011). Because as well as contextual and visual cues,
young children with hearing loss may children eventually learn when ques-
not hear or understand the entire ques- tions are being asked. When asking a
tion, they may need more wait-time to question, the AV practitioner usually
fill in the missing pieces of the lin- looks directly at the child (a sustained
guistic unit, known as “auditory per- gaze). Children with hearing loss and
ceptual restoration” (Winstone, Davis, well-programmed hearing devices can
& DeBruyn, 2012; Zekveld, Kramer, & also learn to recognize questions as a
Festen, 2011). vehicle for obtaining information early
When the AV practitioner directs in the process of learning to listen.
a question to a child, there is usually a The AV practitioner begins asking
prosodic cue (inflectional rise) at the open-ended questions during story
end; this imposes a social obligation for time at the 1- to 2-year age of language
the child to respond (Heeman, Lunsford, acquisition when the use of closed-end
Selfridge, Black, & van Santen, 2010). questions is considerably decreased.
A review of the literature shows that Open-ended questions require much
such self-controlled silences elicit many more from the child in that they neces-
positive outcomes that include sustained sitate more thoughtful, descriptive
auditory attention and increased linguis- responses. Parents typically ask ques-
tic-cognitive growth (Rhoades, 2013). tions to probe and, in doing so, seek
to improve their child’s thinking and
Asking Stage-Appropriate language skills (Rowland, Pine, Lieven,
Questions & Theakston, 2003; Weizman & Snow,
2001). The more open-ended questions
Questions can be viewed as either adults ask, the more likely there will
closed-ended or open-ended. An exam- be growth in the child’s use of spoken
ple of a simple closed-ended question language (Cruz et al., 2013).
is when an adult points at either a For example, when the AV prac-
picture or object, and asks the child titioner asks a preschooler an open-
“What is it?” Closed-end questions typi- ended question such as, “Why is the
cally request one-word responses, such wolf angry?” or “How did the frog get
as the name of something, or a yes/no hurt?” or “What do you think will hap-
response. Adults who frequently ask pen next?” the child is more likely to
closed-ended questions of children reply verbally in creative or descriptive
with hearing loss tend to take control ways than with a close-ended response.
of the verbal exchanges and the child When first using this strategy, par-
often feels pressure, and subsequently ents may need to respond for their
is less likely to engage in conversations children. When an open-ended ques-
(Power et al., 1990). tion is directed to the child, the child
Questions typically involve the pro- may look to the parent for help, and
sodic cue of rising intonation (Maroni, the parent will respond to the question.
2011). Infants with typical hearing are However, as soon as the child demon-
sensitive to prosodic cues that differen- strates improved language production,
tiate questions from statements (Frota the parent needs to stop speaking for
et al., 2014). Based on this information the child. The AV practitioner also
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  311

coaches the parent to avoid looking at next puzzle piece, the adult will put the
the child during such moments. Instead, piece next to the space with it turned
the parent learns to visually focus on askew. The child turns the piece slightly
the practitioner and not to “rescue” the and pushes it into place. With the fol-
child unless requested to do so. Using lowing piece, the adult places it next to
this strategy, the child has time to think the space, with the piece turned half-
and respond, correctly or not. way around. The child then turns the
piece around and pushes it into place.
Scaffolding for Language This process is repeated until the child
Production gains the confidence to complete the
puzzle independently. Restated, scaf-
Scaffolding is a process designed to folding is the part that the adult does
promote a deeper level of learning. It for the child until the child is able to
is support tailored to each child’s needs accomplish the task independently.
with the intention of helping that child This systematic strategy can occur on
achieve the session objectives and com- both verbal and nonverbal activities.
munication goals (Sawyer, 2006). Scaf- This strategy applies when the
folding involves interaction between adult encourages the child to use lon-
the adult and child, whereby the adult ger utterances. If the child only verbal-
can model and give subtle advice in izes part of a message, the AV practi-
helping the child build upon existing tioner models the missing part. Over
skills. Scaffolding can be in the form time, as the child gains more linguistic
of verbal or nonverbal prompts; it is skills, there is a gradual decrease in
temporary in the provision of help and adult modeling and in the amount of
then in the gradual weaning of adult scaffolding and prompting.
help. The goal is independent learning
(Bransford, Brown, & Cocking, 2000;
Reingold, Rimor, & Kalay, 2008). GOAL 6:  Stimulate
Scaffolding takes place when the Independent Learning
AV practitioner completes the first
parts of a complex or long sequence Strategies
of actions and then encourages the
child to perform only the last action. n Pretending objects are some-
After several turns at this, the practi- thing else
tioner begins doing less of the actions, n Creating the unexpected
encouraging the child to do a bit more n Talking with imaginary friends
each time until the entire sequence is n Accepting and making mistakes
completed by the child. For example, a
child may attempt to put a multishaped It is important that children want to
puzzle piece back into its place but has participate in AVT sessions. Therefore,
difficulty lining the piece up with the each AVT session includes activities that
shape of the space. The adult slowly are playful, intentional, and intrinsically
turns the piece so that it matches up motivating for young children. In gen-
with the space, and waits for the child eral, play stresses the means over the
to push the piece into place. With the ends, tending to involve self-selective,
312  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

variable, and nonstereotyped behav- Pretending Objects


iors (Pelligrini, Dupuis, & Smith, 2007). Are Something Else
Play is the “work” of children, and it is
the fundamental means by which chil- Symbolic play is the ability of children
dren gather and process information, to use objects, actions, or ideas to rep-
learn new skills, and practice old ones resent other objects, actions, or ideas
(Ginsburg, 2007). Regardless of hearing in their play activities. Symbolic play
status, play is enjoyable, meaningful, promotes cognitive development and
and goal oriented so that the child can is viewed as a precursor to literacy
discover solutions to problems (Con- (Potter, 1996; Smith, 2007). A banana
way, Bauernschmidt, Huang, & Pisoni, held up to the ear can be used as a
2010; Tomasello, 2003). Through play, make-believe telephone. A block being
adults can implement many of the strat- pushed around on the floor becomes
egies cited here to facilitate listening a train. Running on a broom becomes
and spoken language skills within the horseback riding. A child makes noises
contexts of daily life. with his baby toys by banging them
Guided play is child-directed and together and shaking them.
planned learning; it lies midway be- The AV practitioner incorporates
tween direct instruction and free play, symbolism into shared play to promote
presenting a learning goal, and scaf- joint attention, self-awareness, creativity,
folding the environment while allowing and spoken language (Lewis & Ramsay,
children to maintain a large degree of 2004). When parents actively participate
control over their learning (Weisberg, in pretending things are other things,
Hirsh-Pasek, & Golinkoff, 2013). This is higher levels of child pretend play tend
the means by which children learn how to follow (Brown & Remine, 2004).
to learn during AVT sessions. As stated
throughout this chapter, the AV practi- Creating the Unexpected
tioner uses and demonstrates strategies
that facilitate goal-directed problem A novel item, activity or event that defies
solving, referred to as advanced cog- expectations or occurs in unexpected
nitive capacities (Blair & Razza, 2007; ways is easily remembered, encourages
Bodrova & Leong, 2001). attentional focus, and is fun for the child
Guided and pretend play provide (Mather & Plunkett, 2012; Stahl & Fei-
wonderful opportunities for promot- genson, 2015). The element of surprise,
ing positive parent-child interactions a violation of expectations, enhances
(Brown & Remine, 2004). The AV prac- learning and exploration. Experiencing
titioner carefully observes how parents something out of the ordinary can facili-
play with their children and guides tate verbal language ( Jarvis, 2005). The
them to adjust their play styles to match disappearing coin or ball is a simple
the needs and skills of the child. Par- magic trick that exemplifies how a toy
ents learn to take advantage of sponta- elicits a surprise reaction from young
neous moments and are coached how children. The use of magic in play-based
to embed language learning strategies activities tends to facilitate cognitive
into playful interactions. growth (Woolley & Ghossainy, 2013).
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  313

For example, a child did not seem to not working! Ah, I see the plug is not
understand the preposition “under” no in the socket. Let’s put the plug in the
matter what his mother did, so one day socket. Turn on the TV now. Is the TV
his father told him they were going to working? Yes, the TV is working now.
eat lunch under a table. When they did, It’s not broken.”
the child was initially startled and, after Initially, the adult acts unaware of
his dad repeatedly noted they were eat- the problem, and waits for the child to
ing under a table, the child learned the react to it. When the child does make
meaning of that preposition. it known that something is wrong, a
Some AV practitioners refer to facil- parent-child conversation takes place
itating the unexpected as sabotage, the about the problem and how to fix it.
“accidentally-on-purpose messing up Children enjoy exploring the poten-
of something” (Robbins, 2000). The tial outcomes of problems. Examples
AV practitioner observes if the child of parent-created novel situations or
notices the error and then helps the sabotage are as follows:
child understand the violation of expec-
tation as well as verbalizes the situation. n Give the child a broken pencil.
Sabotage is simply one way to create n Pour cereal in a glass instead of
the element of surprise for something a bowl.
that was unexpected. n Forget where the book is at
For example, a preschooler may bedtime.
watch TV for an hour every day, while n Hide the child’s coat.
the parent is preparing dinner. One day, n Give the child an empty lunch
the parent decides to “create a language bag.
learning opportunity” by disconnecting
the TV when the child is not looking. Talking With Imaginary Friends
Later, the child anxiously runs into the
kitchen saying, “TV! TV!” The parent Assigning roles to inanimate objects is
responds, “Yes, the TV is in the living a form of symbolic play. Most young
room. Go turn it on.” The child franti- children enjoy interacting with stuffed
cally repeats what he just said, and the animals, dolls, or puppets as if they
parent continues faking it, repeating were alive; some preschoolers also cre-
what she just said. “I know, you said ate invisible friends who they know are
that. Turn the TV on. I know you want not real (Taylor & Mottweiler, 2008).
to watch it.” Children undergo significant growth
This exchange may occur a few in their understanding of invisibility
times, but before the child becomes during the preschool years (Woolley &
too frustrated, the parent says “Hmmm Brown, 2015). Research findings show
. . . something must be wrong. Let’s see that children with imaginary friends
what the problem is. OK, there’s the tend to be less shy, more social, and
TV. Now turn it on.” The child turns more competent in starting and main-
it on and nothing happens. The par- taining conversations as a result of
ent says, “Oh, you mean the TV is not practicing communication skills with
working! Oh my goodness, the TV is these special friends (Roby & Kidd,
314  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

2008; Wooley & Ghossainy, 2003). The ative ways. As a simple example, the
ultimate goal of this type of conver­ practitioner can set up a situation where
sational play is for the child to eventu- one puppet drops her food and cries
ally take the lead (Meltzer & Palermo, because she has nothing to eat. Another
in press). puppet sees her crying and says, “I’m
To promote conversational ex- sorry. What’s wrong?” When he learns
changes, the AV practitioner speaks why she is crying, he shares half of his
for the doll, stuffed animal, or pup- food with her and the child. The use
pet. Having conversational turn-taking of imaginary friends can also promote
with imaginary friends in AVT enables language and social skills when scenar-
the practitioner to incorporate differ- ios are scripted and practiced (Stanton-
ent mental states into the activity. It is Chapman & Brown, 2015).
important for the child to understand
how other people think and feel, and Accepting and
why these perspectives might be dif- Making Mistakes
ferent. Children who do not yet under-
stand another person’s perspective A “fear of failure” established during
have not acquired socioemotional com- early childhood can dramatically influ-
petence (Lewis & Ramsay, 2004). ence a child’s motivation, negatively
The AV practitioner has access to affecting the attitude toward learning
toys that include stuffed animals, pup- (Mouratidis, Vansteenkiste, Michou, &
pets, or dolls that wear assorted hear- Lens, 2013). It is crucial for the child to
ing devices. There are many advan- understand that making mistakes is a
tages to using such imaginary friends necessary part of the discovery process
wearing hearing devices in AVT ses- and learning.
sions: (a) hearing devices capture the The AV practitioner and parent
child’s interest, and hence, the imagi- must be sensitive to the child who dem-
nary friends serve as potential motiva- onstrates fear of providing the “wrong”
tors; (b) they can be effectively used as response. The child is reassured that
prompts; (c) they can demonstrate or everyone makes mistakes, and that mis-
model a language target; (d) they can takes help us learn. At times, both AV
engage the child in verbal turn-taking; practitioner and parent pretend to “not
(e) they provide opportunities for prac- know” a word, an answer, or how to
ticing conversational skills; (f) the child do something. Guessing activities fur-
may feel less pressure in performing ther promote the idea that children and
or talking; (g) imaginary friends show adults are not always “in the know.”
that making mistakes is part of learning
and thus are acceptable; and (h) imagi-
nary friends can help children problem
Conclusion
solve or work through some social or
emotional conflicts (Stanton-Chapman
& Brown, 2015). This chapter presented the concept that
The AV practitioner makes every an AV practitioner plans, delivers, and
attempt to use imaginary friends in cre- evaluates every AVT session with SIX
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  315

GOALS as the central theme. For each Classroom research in English language
of these SIX GOALS, a select number of teaching in Oman (pp. 2–7). Muscat,
evidence-based or evidence-informed Oman: Ministry of Education.
strategies were delineated. The AV Anderson, K. L., & Crowley, D. J. (2002).
When listening carefully is not enough:
practitioner implements each of these
12 steps to approaching speech lan-
effective strategies in isolation or in
guage pathology services for a child who
tandem with other strategies. Strategy is hard-of-hearing. Perspectives on Hear-
selection is based on the knowledge, ing and Hearing Disorders in Childhood,
experience, and expertise of the indi- 12(3), 33–36.
vidual practitioner, and influenced by Arunachalam, S., & Waxman, S. R. (2010).
the needs and skills of child and parent. Meaning from syntax: Evidence from
All strategies presented here are 2-year-olds. Cognition, 114(3), 442–446.
demonstrated by the AV practitioner Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (2012). Sta-
for the parents, who are viewed as the tistical learning from acquiring specific
primary client in AVT. Parents prac- items to forming general rules. Current
Directions in Psychological Science,
tice these strategies in AVT sessions
21(3), 170–176.
and then use them to generalize the
Astheimer, L. B., & Sanders, L. D. (2009).
objectives and goals in everyday life to Listeners modulate temporally selective
advance the child toward the expected attention during natural speech process-
outcomes of listening and talking. ing. Biological Psychology, 80, 23–34.
There is a growing body of research Bahrick, L. E., & Lickliter, R. (2009). Per-
by experimental psychologists and neu- ceptual development: Intermodal percep-
roscientists that indicate the brain of tion. In B. Goldstein (Ed.), Encyclopedia
the experienced practitioner may be a of perception (Vol. 2, pp. 753–756). New-
master predictor of child outcomes. In bury Park, CA: Sage.
the future, this research may be used to Barker, D, H., Quittner, A. L., Fink, N. E.,
Eisenberg, L. S., Tobey, E. A., Niparko,
help analyze these strategies even more
J. K., & The CDaCI Investigative Team.
extensively (Brown & Fenske, 2011).
(2009). Predicting behavior problems
Until such a time, it is hoped that more in deaf and hearing children: The influ-
researchers and AV practitioners will ences of language, attention, and parent-
collaborate in studies that can verify child communication. Development and
the effectiveness of more strategies with Psychopathology, 21(2), 373–392.
children who have hearing loss. Barr, D. J., & Seyfeddinipur, M. (2010). The
role of fillers in listener attributions for
speaker disfluency. Language and Cog-
nitive Processes, 25, 441–455.
References Bass-Ringdahl, S. M. (2010). The relation-
ship of audibility and the development
of canonical babbling in young children
Adank, P., Hagoort, P., & Bekkering, H. (2010). with hearing impairment. Journal of
Imitation improves comprehension. Psy- Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 15,
chological Science, 21(12), 1903–1909. 287–310.
Al-Balushi, S. N. M. (2009). An investiga- Bedore, L. M., & Leonard, L. B. (1995). Pro-
tion into how silent wait-time assists sodic and syntactic bootstrapping and
language learning. In S. Borg (Ed.), their clinical applications: A tutorial.
316  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

American Journal of Speech-Language to preschoolers with hearing loss. Infants


Pathology, 4(1), 66–72. and Young Children, 28, 262–280.
Bellon-Harn, M. L., Credeur-Pampolina, M. Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2001). The
E., & LeBoeuf, L. (2013). Scaffolded lan- Tools of the Mind project: A case study of
guage intervention: Speech production implementing the Vygotskian approach
outcomes. Communication Disorders in American early childhood and primary
Quarterly, 34(2), 120–132. classrooms. INNODATA Monographs — 7.
Benasich, A. A., Choudhury, N. A., Realpe- Switzerland: International Bureau of
Bonilla, T. & Roesler, C. P. (2014). Plastic- Education.
ity in developing brain: Active auditory Booth, A. E., & Waxman, S. R. (2009). A horse
exposure impacts prelinguistic acoustic of a different color: Specifying with pre-
mapping. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, cision infants’ mappings of novel nouns
13349–13363. and adjectives. Child Development, 80(1),
Bergen, B., & Wheeler, K. (2010). Grammati- 15–22.
cal aspect and mental stimulation. Brain Bosch, L. (2011). Precursors to language in
and Language, 112, 150–158. preterm infants: Speech perception abili-
Bergeson, T. R., & Trehub, S. E. (2002). ties in the first year of life. Progress in
Absolute pitch and tempo in mothers’ Brain Research, 189, 239–257.
song to infants. Psychological Science, Bovey, T., & Strain, P. (2005). Strategies
13(1), 72–75. for increasing peer social interactions:
Bernal, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Millotte, Prompting and acknowledgement. What
S., & Christophe, A. (2010). Two-year- Works Briefs, 17, 1–4.
olds compute syntactic structure on-line. Boysson-Bardies, B. (1999). How language
Developmental Science, 13(1), 69–76. comes to children: From birth to two
Bernhardt, B. M., Kemp, N., & Werker, J. years. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
F. (2007). Early word-object associations Bransford, J., Brown, A., & Cocking, R.
and later language development. First (2000). How people learn: Brain, mind,
Language, 27(4), 315–328. experience and school. Washington, DC:
Bishop, D., & Adams, C. (2006). A prospec- National Academy Press.
tive study of the relationship between Brockmole, J. R., Davoli, C. C., Abrams, R.
specific language impairment, phono- A., & Witt, J. K. (2013). The world within
logical disorders and reading retardation. reach: Effects of hand posture and tool-
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychia- use on visual cognition. Current Direc-
try, 31, 1027–1050. tions in Psychological Science, 22, 38–44.
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating Brooks, P. J., & Kempe, V. (2012). Language
effortful control, executive function, and development. London: Wiley.
false belief understanding to emerging Brown, G., & Wragg, E. C. (1993). Question-
math and literacy ability in kindergarten. ing. New York, NY: Routledge.
Child Development, 78, 647–663. Brown, J., & Frenske, M. (2011). The Win-
Blaiser, K. M., Nelson, P. B., & Kohnert, K. ner’s Brain. Boston, MA: Da Capo Press.
(2015). Effect of repeated exposures on Brown, P. M., & Remine, M. D. (2004). Build-
word learning in quiet and noise. Com- ing pretend play skills in toddlers with
munication Disorders Quarterly, 37, and without hearing loss: Maternal scaf-
25–35. folding styles. Deafness and Education
Bobzien, J. L., Richels, C., Schwartz, K., International, 6(3), 129–153.
Raver, Sharon A., Hester, P., & Morin, Callanan, M. A., & Sabbagh, M. A. (2004). Mul-
L. (2015). Using repeated reading and tiple labels for objects in conversations
explicit instruction to teach vocabulary with young children: Parents’ language
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  317

and children’s developing expectations tistical learning in language processing:


about word meanings. Developmental Word predictability is the key. Cognition,
Psychology, 40(5), 746–763. 114(3), 356–371.
Carey-Sargeant, C. L., & Brown, P. M. (2003). Cristia, A., Seidi, A., Junge, C., Soderstrom,
Pausing during interactions between M., & Hagoort, P. (2014). Predicting indi-
deaf toddlers and their hearing mothers. vidual variation in language from infant
Deafness and Education International, speech perception measures. Child De-
5(1), 39–58. velopment, 85, 1330–1345.
Cassell, J., Nakano, Y. I., Bickmore, T. W., Cruz, I., Quittner, A. L., Marker, C., & DesJar-
Sidner, C. L., & Rich, C. (2001). Non- din, J. L. (2013). Identification of effective
verbal cues for discourse structure. Asso- strategies to promote language in deaf
ciation for Computational Linguistics children with cochlear implants. Child
Joint EACL–2001 ACL Conference. Development, 84, 543–599.
Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., Stefanini, S., & Dalton, P., & Fraenkel, N. (2012). Gorillas
Volterra, V. (2012). Early action and ges- we have missed: Sustained inattentional
ture “vocabulary” and its relation with deafness for dynamic events. Cognition,
word comprehension and production. 124(2), 367–372.
Child Development, 83(2), 526–542. Damron, J. C. H., & Morman, M. T. (2011).
Chaiklin, S. (2003). The zone of proximal Attitudes toward interpersonal silence
development in Vygotsky’s analysis of within dyadic relationships. Human
learning and instruction. In A. Kozulin, Communication, 14, 183–203.
B. Gindis, V. Ageyev, & S. Miller (Eds.), Driscoll, V., Gfeller. K., Tan, X., See, R. L.,
Vygotsky’s educational theory in cultural Cheng, H-Y., & Kanemitsu, M. (2015).
context (pp. 39–64). Cambridge, UK: Family involvement in music impacts
Cambridge University Press. participation of children with cochlear
Champoux, F., Lepore, F., Gagné, J-P., & implants. Cochlear Implants Interna-
Théoret, H. (2009). Visual stimuli can tional, 16, 137–146.
impair auditory processing in cochlear Duff, F. J., Reen, G., Plunkett, K., & Nation,
implant users. Neuropsychologia, 47(1), K. (2015). Do infant vocabulary skills
17–22. predict school-age language and literacy
Chang, R. S., & Thompson, N. S. (2011). outcomes? Journal of Child Psychology
Whines, cries, and motherese: Their rela- and Psychiatry, 56, 848–856.
tive power to distract. Journal of Social, Dunst, C. J., Raab, M., & Trivette, C. M.
Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, (2011). Characteristics of naturalistic lan-
5(2) 10–20. guage intervention strategies. Journal of
Cirelli, L. K., Wan, S. J., & Trainor, L. J. (2014). Speech-Language Pathology and Applied
Fourteen-month-old infants use inter- Behavioral Analysis, 5, 8–16.
personal synchrony as a cue to direct Ertmer, D. J., & Stoel-Gammon, C. (2008).
helpfulness. Philosophical Transactions The conditioned assessment of speech
of the Royal Society Biological Science, production (CASP): A tool for evaluating
369, 1658. auditory-guided speech development in
Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. M., Koster, I., & young children with hearing loss. The
Noom, M. J. (2010). The relation between Volta Review, 108, 59–80.
pointing and language development: Estabrooks, W. (2006). Auditory-verbal ther-
A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, apy and practice. In W. Estabrooks (Ed.),
30(4), 352–366. Auditory-Verbal Therapy and practice
Conway, C. M., Bauernschmidt, A., Huang, (pp. 1–22). Washington, DC: A.G. Bell
S. S., & Pisoni, D. B. (2010). Implicit sta- Association.
318  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

Estabrooks, W., & Birkenshaw-Fleming, L. Frischen, A., Bayliss, A. P., & Tipper, S. P.
(2003). Songs for listening! Songs for life! (2007). Gaze cueing of attention: Visual
Washington, DC: A.G. Bell Association. attention, social cognition, and individ-
Estes, K. G., & Hurley, K. (2013). Infant- ual differences. Psychological Bulletin,
directed prosody helps infants map 133(4), 694–724.
sounds to meanings. Infancy, 18(5), Frota, S., Butler, J., & Vigário, M. (2014).
797–824. Infants’ perception of intonation: Is it
Estigarribia, B. (2010). Facilitation by vari- a statement or a question? Infancy, 19,
ation: right-to-left learning of English 194–213.
yes/no questions. Cognitive Science, 34, Gabel, D. (2004). Science. In G. Cawelti
68–93. (Ed.), Handbook of research on improv-
Farrant, B. M., Maybery, M. T., & Fletcher, ing student achievement (pp. 123–143).
J. (2011). Socio-emotional engagement, Arlington, VA: Educational Research
joint attention, imitation, and conversa- Service.
tion skill: Analysis in typical develop- Gallun, F. J., Mason, C. R., & Kidd, G., Jr.
ment and specific language impairment. (2007). The ability to listen with inde-
First Language, 31, 23–46. pendent ears. Journal of the Acoustical
Fast, J. (2002). Body language. Lanham, MD: Society of America, 122, 2814–2825.
M. Evans. Gao, H. H., Zelazo, P. D., Sharpe, D., &
Feldman, R., Magori-Cohen, R., Galili, G., Mashari, A. (2014). Beyond early linguis-
Singer, M., & Louzon, Y. (2011). Mother- tic competence: Development of chil-
infant coordinate heart rhythms through dren’s ability to interpret adjectives flex-
episodes of interaction synchrony. In- ibly. Cognitive Development, 32, 86–102.
fant Behavior and Development, 34(4), Ginsburg, K. R. (2007). The importance of
569–577. play in promoting healthy child devel-
Ferguson, B., Graf, E., & Waxman, S. R. opment and maintaining strong parent-
(2014). Infants use known verbs to learn child bonds. Pediatrics, 119(1), 182–191.
novel nouns: Evidence from 15- and Gogate, L. J., Bahrick, L. E., & Watson, J.
19-month-olds. Cognition, 131, 139. D. (2000). A study of multi-modal moth-
Ferguson, B., Havy, M., & Waxman, S. R. erese: The role of temporal synchrony
(2015). The precision of 12-month-old between verbal labels and gestures.
infants’ link between language and cat- Child Development, 71(4), 878–894.
egorization predicts vocabulary size at Goldstein, M. H., & Schwade, J. A. (2008).
12 and 18 months. Frontiers in Psychol- Social feedback to infants’ babbling
ogy, 6, 1319. facilitates rapid phonological learning.
Flecha-García, M. L. (2010). Eyebrow raises Psychological Science, 19, 515–523.
in dialogue and their relation to dis- Gomes, A., Wolfson, V., & Halperin, J. M.
course structure, utterance function and (2007). Is there a selective relationship
pitch accents in English. Speech Commu- between language functioning and audi-
nication, 52, 542–554. tory attention in children? Journal of
Flom, R., & Pick, A. D. (2003). Verbal encour- Clinical and Experimental Neuropsy-
agement and joint attention in 18-month- chology, 29(6), 660–668.
old infants. Infant Behavior and Devel- Graham, S., Santos, D., & Vanderplank,
opment, 26(2), 121–134. R. (2011). Exploring the relationship
Florit, E., Roch, M., Altoe, G., & Levorato, between listening development and strat-
M. C. (2009). Listening comprehension in egy use. Language Teaching Research,
preschoolers: The role of memory. Brit- 15, 435–456.
ish Journal of Developmental Psychology, Gratier, M. (2003). Expressive timing and
27, 935–951. interactional synchrony between moth-
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  319

ers and infants: Cultural similarities, implants. Audiology and Neurotology,


cultural differences, and the immigra- 13(6), 370–378.
tion experience. Cognitive Development, Hayes, D. (2014). The use of prompting as
18(4), 533–554. an evidence-based strategy to support
Gray S. (2004). Word learning by preschool- children with ASD in school settings
ers with specific language impairment: in New Zealand. Kairaranga, 14(2),
Predictors and poor learners. Journal of 52–56.
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, Heeman, P. A., Lunsford, R., Selfridge, E.,
47, 1117–1132. Black, L., & van Santen, J. (2010). Autism
Gray, S., & Brinkley, S. (2011). Fast map- and interactional aspects of dialogue.
ping and word learning by preschoolers Proceedings of SIGDIAL 2010: The 11th
with SLI in a supported learning context: Annual Meeting of the Special Interest
Effect of encoding cues, phonotactic Group on Discourse and Dialogue (pp.
probability and object familiarity. Jour- 249–252). University of Tokyo, Septem-
nal of Speech, Language, and Hearing ber 24–25, 2010.
Research, 54, 870–884. Henninger, M. L. (2013). Teaching young
Guaïtella, I., Santi, S., Lagrue, B., & Cavé, C. children: An introduction (5th ed.). New
(2009). Are eyebrow movements linked York, NY: Pearson.
to voice variations and turn-taking dia- Holstrum, W. J., Gaffney, M., Gravel, J, S.,
logue? An experimental investigation. Oyler, R. F., & Ross, D. S. (2008). Early
Language and Speech, 52, 207–222. intervention for children with unilateral
Gunlogson, C. (2003). True to form: Rising and mild bilateral degrees of hearing loss.
and falling declaratives as questions in Trends in Amplification, 12(1), 35–41.
English. New York, NY: Routledge. Houston, D. (2011). Infant speech percep-
Gustafson, S., McCreery, R., Hoover, B., tion. In R. Seewald & A. M. Tharpe (Eds.),
Kopun, J. G., & Stelmachowicz, P. (2014). Comprehensive handbook of pediatric
Listening effort and perceived clarity for audiology (pp. 47–62). San Diego, CA:
normal-hearing children with the use of Plural.
digital noise reduction. Ear and Hear- Humes, L. E., & Bess, F. H. (2014). Audiology
ing, 35, 183–194. and communication disorders: An over-
Halliday, L. F., Moore, D. R., Taylor, J. L., view (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Lippincott
& Amitay, S. (2011). Dimension-specific Williams and Wilkins.
attention directs learning and listening Ignatius, E., & Kokkonen, M. (2007). Factors
on auditory training tasks. Attention, contributing to verbal self-disclosure.
Perception, and Psychophysics, 73(5), Nordic Psychology, 59, 362–391.
1329–1335. Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005).
Hannemann, R., Obleser, J., & Eulitz, C. Gesture paves the way for language
(2007). Top-down knowledge supports development. Psychological Science, 16,
the retrieval of lexical information from 367–371.
degraded speech. Brain Research, 1153, James, K. H., & Maouene, J. (2009). Audi-
134–143. tory verb perception recruits motor sys-
Hasegawa, I. & Miyashita, Y. (2002). Catego- tems in the developing brain: An fMRI
rizing the world: Expert neurons look investigation. Developmental Science, 12,
into key features. Nature Neuroscience, F26–F39.
5(2), 90–91. Jarvis, M. (2005). The psychology of effective
Hay-McCutcheon, M. J., Kirk, K. I., Henning, learning and teaching. Cheltenham, UK:
S. C., Gao, S., & Qi, R. (2008). Using early Nelson Thornes.
language outcomes to predict later lan- Johnson, K. (2012). Acoustic and auditory
guage ability in children with cochlear phonetics (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Wiley.
320  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

Jones, S. S. (2007). Imitation in infancy: The Lau, B., & Salzman, C. D. (2009). The
development of mimicry. Psychological rhythms of learning. Nature Neurosci-
Science, 18, 593–599. ence, 12, 675–676.
Junge, C., Koojiman, V., Haggort, P., & Cutler, Leclère, C., Viaux, S., Avril, M., Achard, C.,
A. (2012). Rapid recognition at 10 months Chetouani, M., Missonnier, S., & Cohen,
as a predictor of language development. D. (2014). Why synchrony matters dur-
Developmental Science, 15, 463–473. ing mother-child interactions: A system-
Kahana-Kalman, R., & Walker-Andrews, A. atic review. PLoS ONE, 9(12), e113571.
S. (2001). The role of person familiar- Lederberg, A. R., & Spencer, P. E. (2009).
ity in young infants’ perception of emo- Word-learning abilities in deaf and hard-
tional expressions. Child Development, of-hearing preschoolers: Effect of lexi-
72, 352–369. con size and language modality. Journal
Kohlberger, M., & Strycharczuk, P. (2015). of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 14,
Voicing assimilation in whispered 44–62.
speech. Retrieved from https://www. Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2004). Develop-
internationalphoneticassociation.org/ ment of self-recognition, personal pro-
icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2015/Papers/ noun use, and pretend play during the
ICPHS1016.pdf 2nd year. Child Development, 75(6),
Kokkinaki, T., & Vitalaki, E. (2013). Com- 1821–1831.
paring spontaneous imitation in grand- Li, J., Li, W., Xian, J., Li, Y., Liu, Z., Liu, S., . . .
mother-infant and mother-infant interac- He, H. (2012). Cortical thickness analysis
tion: A three generation familial study. and optimized voxel-based morphome-
International Journal of Aging Human try in children and adolescents with pre-
Development, 77, 77–105. lingually profound sensorineural hearing
Kotz, S. A., Opitz, B., & Friederici, A. D. (2007). loss. Brain Research, 1430, 35–42.
ERP effects of meaningful and non- Ling, D. (1976). Speech and the hearing-
meaningful sound processing in anterior impaired child: Theory and practice.
temporal patients. Restorative Neurology Washington, DC: AG Bell Association for
and Neuroscience, 25, 273–284. the Deaf and Hard of Hearing.
Kuhl, P. (2004). Early language acquisi- Ling, D., & Ling, A. H. (1978). Aural habili-
tion: Cracking the speech code. Nature tation: The foundations of verbal learn-
Reviews Neuroscience, 5(11), 831–843. ing in hearing-impaired children. Wash-
Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). Infant ington, DC: AG Bell Association.
vocalizations in response to speech: Ludke, K. M., Ferreira, F., & Overy, K. (2014).
Vocal imitation and developmental Singing can facilitate foreign language
change. Journal of Acoustical Society of learning. Memory & Cognition, 42, 41–52.
America, 100, 2425–2438. Ma, W., Golinkoff, R. M., Houston, D., &
Kuhl, P. K., Tsao, F-M., & Liu, H-M. (2003). Hirsh-Pasek (2011). Word learning in
Foreign-language experience in infancy: infant- and adult-directed speech. Lan-
Effects of short-term exposure and social guage Learning and Development, 7,
interaction on phonetic learning. PNAS, 209–225.
100, 9096–9101. MacKenzie, H., Graham, S. A., & Curtin,
Le Bel, R. M., Pineda, J. A., & Sharma, A. S. (2011). Twelve-month-olds privilege
(2009). Motor-auditory-visual integra- words over other linguistic sounds in an
tion: The role of the human mirror associative learning task. Developmental
neuron system in communication and Science, 14(2), 249–255.
communication disorders. Journal of Macrae, C. N., Duffy, O. K., Miles, L. K.,
Communication Disorders, 42, 299–304. & Lawrence, J. (2008). A case of hand
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  321

waving: Action synchrony and person Mercer, S., Ryan, S., & Williams, M. (2012).
perception. Cognition, 109(1), 152–156. Psychology for language learning: In-
Maroni, B. (2011). Pauses, gaps and wait sights from research, theory and practice.
time in classroom interaction in primary New York, NY: St. Martin’s Press.
schools. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(7), Minagawa-Kawai, Y., Mori, K., Naoi, N., &
2081–2093. Kojima, S. (2006). Neural attunement
Marschark, M., Spencer, P. E., Adams, J., & processes in infants during the acqui-
Sapere, P. (2011). Evidence-based practice sition of a language-specific phonemic
in educating deaf and hard-of-hearing contrast. Journal of Neuroscience 27(2),
children: Teaching to their cognitive 315–321.
strengths and needs. European Journal Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2011). How does
of Special Needs Education, 26(1), 3–16. it look? Level 2 perspective-taking at 36
Marques, C., Moreno, S., Castro, S. L., & months of age. Child Development, 82,
Besson, M. (2007). Musicians detect 661–673.
pitch violation in foreign language bet- Monaghan, P., Shillcock, R. C., Christiansen,
ter than nonmusicians: Behavioral and M. H., & Kirby, S. (2014). How arbitrary
electrophysiological evidence. Journal of is language? Philosophical Transactions
Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1453–1463. of the Royal Society B: Biological Sci-
Mastropavlou, M. (2010). Morphophonolog- ences, 369(1651), 20130299.
ical salience as a compensatory means Mouratidis, A., Vansteenkiste, M., Michou,
for deficits in the acquisition of past A., & Lens, W. (2013). Perceived struc-
tense in SLI. Journal of Communication ture and achievement goals as predictors
Disorders, 43, 175–198. of students’ self-regulated learning and
Mather, E., & Plunkett, K. (2012). The role of affect and the mediating role of com-
novelty in early word learning. Cognitive petence need satisfaction. Learning and
Science, 36(7), 1157–1177. Individual Differences, 23, 179–186.
Matsuda, Y-T., Ueno, K., Waggoner, R. A., Musacchia, G. E., Sams, M., Nicol, T. G., &
Erickson, D., Shimura, Y., Tanaka, K., Kraus, N. (2006). Seeing speech affects
Cheng, K., & Mazuka, R. (2011). Process- acoustic information processing in the
ing of infant-directed speech by adults. human brainstem. Experimental Brain
NeuroImage, 54, 611–621. Research, 168, 1–10.
McDonald, J. D. (1985). Language through Nappa, R., Wessel, A., McEldoon, K. L., Glei-
conversation: A model for intervention tman, L. R., & Trueswell, J. (2009). Use of
with language-delayed persons. In S. F. speaker’s gaze and syntax in verb learn-
Warren & A. K. Rogers-Warren (Eds.), ing. Language Learning and Develop-
Teaching functional language: Gener- ment, 5(4), 203–234.
alization and maintenance of language Narr, R. A. F. (2006). Teaching phonological
skills (pp. 89–122). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. awareness with deaf and hard-of-hearing
McMurray, B., & Aslin, R. N. (2005). Infants students. Teaching Exceptional Children,
are sensitive to within-category varia- 38(4), 53–58.
tion in speech perception. Cognition, Nathani, S., Ertmer, D. J., & Stark, R. S.
95, B15–B26. (2006). Assessing vocal development in
Melzer, D. K., & Palermo, C. A. (in press). infants and toddlers. Clinical Linguistics
“Mommy, You are the princess and I am and Phonetics, 20, 351–369.
the queen”: How preschool children’s Nevo, I., & Slonim-Nevo, V. (2011). The myth
initiation and language use during pre- of evidence-based practice: Towards
tend play relate to complexity. Infant evidence-informed practice. British Jour-
and Child Development. nal of Social Work, 41(6), 1176–1197.
322  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

Newman, R. S., Chatterjee, M., Morini, G., eracy: Clarifying the links. Early Years,
& Remez, R. E. (2015). Toddler’s com- 16, 13–16.
prehension of degraded signals: Noise- Power, D. J., Wood, D. J., Wood, H. A., &
vocoded versus sine-wave analogs. Jour- Macdougall, J. (1990). Maternal control
nal of the Acoustical Society of America over conversations with hearing and
Express Letters, 138, EL311. deaf infants and young children. First
Newman, R. S., Rowe, M. L., & Ratner, N. Language, 10, 19–35.
(in press). Input and uptake at 7 months Proctor-Williams, K., Fey, M. E., & Loeb, D. F.
predicts toddler vocabulary: The role of (2001). Parental recasts and production
child-directed speech and infant process- of copulas and articles by children with
ing skills in language development. Jour- specific language impairment and typical
nal of Child Language. language. American Journal of Speech-
Olson, J., & Masur, E. F. (2013). Mothers Language Pathology, 10(2), 155–168.
respond differently to infants’ gestural Pruden, S. M., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff,
versus nongestural communicative bids. R. M., & Hennon, E. A. (2006). The birth
First Language, 33, 372–387. of words: Ten-month-olds learn words
Owren, M. J., & Cardillo, G. C. (2006). The through perceptual salience. Child Devel-
relative roles of vowels and consonants opment, 77, 266–280.
in discriminating talker identity versus Read, K., Macauley, M., & Furay, E. (2014).
word meaning. Journal of the Acoustical The Seuss boost: Rhyme helps children
Society of America, 119, 1727–1739. retain words from shared storybook
Pedale, T., & Santangelo, V. (2015). Percep- reading. First Language, 34, 354–371.
tual salience affects the contents of work- Reingold, R., Rimor, R., & Kalay, A. (2008).
ing memory during free-recollection of Instructor’s scaffolding in support of stu-
objects from natural scenes. Frontiers in dent’s metacognition through a teacher
Human Neuroscience, 9, 60. education online course — A case study.
Pelligrini, A. D., Dupuis, D., & Smith, P. K. Journal of Interactive Online Learning,
(2007). Play in evolution and develop­ 7, 139–151.
ment. Developmental Review, 27, 261–276. Rescorla, L. (2009). Age 17 language and
Peters, A. M. (2001). Filler syllables: What is reading outcomes in late-talking tod-
their status in emerging grammar? Jour- dlers: Support for a dimensional per-
nal of Child Language, 28, 229–242. spective on language delay. Journal
Petitto, L. (2005). How the brain begets of Speech, Language, and Hearing Re-
language. In J. McGilvray (Ed.), The search, 52, 16–30.
Cambridge companion to Chomsky (pp. Rhoades, E. A. (2007). Sound-object asso-
84–101). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge ciations. In S. Easterbrooks & E. Estes
University Press. (Eds.), Helping children who are deaf
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2006). Do and hard of hearing learn spoken lan-
people use language production to guage (pp. 181–188). Thousand Oaks,
make predictions during comprehen- CA: Corwin Press.
sion? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, Rhoades, E. A. (2010, July 25). Facilitating
105–110. auditory-verbal learning for young chil-
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. dren with impaired executive function-
New York, NY: HarperCollins. ing. Presentation at A.G. Bell Association
Pollack, D. (1972). Educational audiology Convention, Orlando, FL.
for the limited hearing infant. Spring- Rhoades, E. A. (2011). Listening strategies
field, IL: Charles C. Thomas. to facilitate spoken language learning
Potter, G. (1996). From symbolic play to among signing children with cochlear
symbolic representation in early lit- implants. In R. Paludneviciene & I. W.
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  323

Leigh (Eds.), Cochlear implants: Shifting Sawyer, R. K. (2006). The Cambridge hand-
perspectives (pp. 142–171). Washington, book of the learning sciences. New York,
DC: Gallaudet University Press. NY: Cambridge University Press.
Rhoades, E. A. (2013). Interactive silences: Schneider, D. M., Nelson, A., & Mooney, R.
Evidence for strategies to facilitate spo- (2014). A synaptic and circuit basis for
ken language in children with hearing corollary discharge in the auditory cor-
loss. The Volta Review, 113(1), 57–73. tex. Nature, 513(7517), 189–194.
Rigato, S., Menon, E., Johnson, M. H., Far- Shafiro, V., Sheft, S., Gygi, B., & Ho, K. T.
aguna, D., & Farroni, T. (2011). Direct N. (2012). The influence of environmen-
gaze may modulate face recognition in tal sound training on the perception of
newborns. Infant and Child Develop- spectrally degraded speech and environ-
ment, 20(1), 20–34. mental sounds. Trends in Amplification,
Robbins, A. M. (2000). Rehabilitation after 16, 83–101.
cochlear implantation. In J. K. Niparko, Sharma, A., Tobey, E., Dorman, M., Bharad-
K. I. Kirk, N. K. Mellon, A. M. Robbins, D. waj, S., Martin, K., Gilley, P., & Kunkel, F.
L. Tucci, & B. S. Wilson (Eds.), Cochlear (2004). Central auditory maturation and
implants: Principles and practices. New babbling development in infants with
York, NY: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. cochlear implants. Archives of Otolar-
Roby, A. C., & Kidd, E. (2008). The refer- yngology-Head & Neck Surgery, 13(5),
ential communication skills of children 511–516.
with imaginary companions. Develop- Sharp, A. (2009). Reading comprehension
mental Science, 11(4), 531–540. in two cultures. International Journal
Rowe, K., & Rowe, K. (2006). BIG issues of Learning, 16, 281–292.
in boys’ education: Auditory process- Shi, R. (2014). Functional morphemes and
ing capacity, literacy and behaviour. early language acquisition. Child Devel-
Retrieved from http://research.acer.edu​ opment Perspectives, 8(1), 6–11.
.au/boys_edu/2 Skinner, C. H., Pappas, D. N., & Davis, K.
Rowe, M. L. (2012). A longitudinal investiga- A. (2005). Enhancing academic engage-
tion of the role of quantity and quality ment: Providing opportunities for
of child-directed speech in vocabulary responding and influencing students
development. Child Development, 83, to choose to respond. Psychology in the
1762–1774. Schools, 42, 389–403.
Rowland, C. F., Pine, J. M., Lieven, E. V., Smaldino, J., & Flexer, C. (2014). Acoustic
& Theakston, A. L. (2003). Determinants accessibility: Room acoustics and remote
of the order of acquisition of wh- ques- microphone use in home and school
tions: Re-evaluating the role of caregiver environments. In J. R. Madell & C. Flexer
speech. Journal of Child Language, 30, (Eds.), Pediatric audiology: Diagnosis,
609–635. technology, and management (2nd ed.,
Saffran, J. R., Werker, J., & Werner, L. (2006). pp. 255–267). New York, NY: Thieme.
The infant’s auditory world: Hearing, Smith, P. K. (2007). Pretend play and chil-
speech and the beginnings of language. dren’s cognitive and literacy develop-
In R. Siegler and D. Kuhn (Eds.), Hand- ment: Sources of evidence and some
book of child development (pp. 58–108). lessons from the past. In K. Roskos & J.
New York, NY: Wiley. Christie (Eds.), Play and literacy in early
Saindon, M. R., Trehub, S. E., Schellenberg, childhood: Research from multiple per-
E. G., & Van Lieshout, P. (in press). Chil- spectives (2nd ed., pp. 3–19). Mahwah,
dren’s identification of questions from NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
rising terminal pitch. Journal of Child Smith, L. Z., & Levitt, H. (1999). Consonant
Language. enhancement effects on speech recogni-
324  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

tion of hearing-impaired children. Jour- opment of children with hearing loss.


nal of American Academy of Audiology, Archives of Otolaryngology-Head & Neck
10(8), 411–421. Surgery, 130, 556–562.
Snyder, P. A., Rakap, S., Hemmeter, M. L., Stephenson, J., Carter, M., & Arthur-Kelly, M.
McLaughlin, T. W., Sandall, S., & McLean, (2011). Professional learning for teachers
M. E. (2015). Naturalistic instructional without special education qualifications
approaches in early learning: A system- working with students with severe dis-
atic review. Journal of Early Interven- abilities. Teacher Education and Special
tion, 37(1), 69–97. Education, 34(1), 7–20.
Southgate, V., Chevallier, C., & Csibra, G. Strapp, C. M., & Federico, A. (2000). Imita-
(2009). Sensitivity to communicative tions and repetitions: What do children
relevance tells young children what to say following recasts? First Language,
imitate. Developmental Science, 12(6), 20(60), 273–290.
1013–1019. Syrett, K., Arunachalam, S., & Waxman, S. R.
Souza, P. (2014 June 29). Improving audibility: (2014). Slowly but surely: Adverbs sup-
The foundation for speech understanding. port verb learning in 2-year-olds. Lan-
In Maximizing Brain Adaptability. Pre- guage Learning and Development, 10(3),
sented at the A. G. Bell Association 2014 263–278.
Research Symposium, Orlando, FL. Taylor, M., & Mottweiler, C. M. (2008). Imag-
Srinivasan, R. J., & Massaro, D. W. (2003). inary companions: Pretending they are
Perceiving prosody from the face and real but knowing they are not. American
voice: distinguishing statements from Journal of Play, 1, 47–54.
echoic questions in English. Language Thiessen, E. D., & Saffran, J. R. (2007).
and Speech, 46, 1–22. Learning to learn: Infants’ acquisition of
Stahl, A. E., & Feigenson, L. (2015). Observ- stress-based strategies for word segmen-
ing the unexpected enhances infants’ tation. Language Learning and Develop-
learning and exploration. Science, 348​ ment, 3(1), 73–100.
(6230), 91–94. Tierney, A., & Kraus, N. (2015). Evidence
Stahl, R. J. (1994). Using “Think-Time” and for multiple rhythmic skills. PLoS ONE,
“Wait-Time” skillfully in the classroom. 10(9), e0136645.
ERIC Digest, ED370885. Retrieved from Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a lan-
http://www.ericdigests.org/1995-1/ guage: A usage-based theory of language
think.htm acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Stanton-Chapman, T. L., & Brown, T. S. University.
(2015). Facilitating commenting and Trehub, S. E. (2015). Cross-cultural conver-
requesting skills in 3-year-old children gence of musical features. Proceedings
with disabilities. Journal of Early Inter- of the National Academy of Sciences,
vention, 37, 103–118. 112(29), 8809–8810.
Stanton-Chapman, T. L., & Snell, M. E. Twomey, K. E., Lush, L., Pearce, R., & Horst,
(2011). Promoting turn-taking skills in J. S. (2014). Visual variability affects early
preschool children with disabilities: The verb learning. British Journal of Develop-
effects of a peer-based social commu- mental Psychology, 32(3), 359–366.
nication intervention. Early Childhood Urry, S. A., Nelson, L. J., & Padilla-Walker,
Research Quarterly, 26(3), 303–319. L. M. (2011). Mother knows best: Psy-
Stelmachowicz, P. G., Pittman, A. L., Hoover, chological control, child disclosure, and
B. M., Lewis, D. E., & Moeller, M. P. (2004). maternal knowledge in emerging adult-
The importance of high-frequency audi- hood. Journal of Family Studies, 17,
bility in the speech and language devel- 157–173.
Chapter 10  n  Strategies for Listening, Talking, and Thinking in AVT  325

van Heugten, M., & Johnson, E. K. (2012). Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A. (2013). Talk-
Infants exposed to fluent natural speech ing to children matters: Early language
succeed at cross-gender word recogni- experience strengthens processing and
tion. Journal of Speech, Language, and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science,
Hearing Research, 55(2), 554–560. 24(11), 2143–2152.
Varghese, L. A., Ozmeral, E. J., Best, V., & Weizman, Z. O., & Snow, C. E. (2001). Lexi-
Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (2012). How cal input as related to children’s vocabu-
visual cues for when to listen aid selective lary acquisition: Effects of sophisticated
auditory attention. Journal for Research exposure and support for meaning.
in Otolaryngology, 13(3), 359–368. Developmental Psychology, 37, 265–279.
Vassilopoulos, S. P., & Konstantinidis, G. Werker, J. F., & Gervain, J. (2013). Speech
(2012). Teacher use of silence in elemen- perception in infancy: A foundation for
tary education. Journal of Teaching and language acquisition. In P. D. Zelazo
Learning, 8, 91–105. (Ed.). The Oxford handbook of devel-
Vigliocco, G., Perniss, P., & Vinson, D. (2014). opmental psychology, Vol 1: Body and
Language as a multimodal phenomenon: mind (pp. 909–925). New York, NY:
implications for language learning, pro- Oxford University Press.
cessing and evolution. Philosophical White, F. A., Livesey, D. J., & Hayes, B. K.
Transactions of the Royal Society Biologi- (2015). Developmental psychology from
cal Sciences, 369(1651). infancy to adulthood (3rd ed.). Frenchs
Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapre- Forest NSW, Australia: Pearson.
tto, M. (2006). Developmental changes Wingfield, A., & Tun, P. A. (2007). Cogni-
in the neural basis of interpreting com- tive supports and cognitive constraints
municative intent. Social Cognitive and on comprehension of spoken language.
Affective Neuroscience, 1(2), 107–121. Journal of American Academy of Audiol-
Wang, D. J., Trehub, S. E., Volkova, A., & van ogy, 18, 548–558.
Lieshout, P. (2013). Child implant users’ Winstone, N., Davis, A., & De Bruyn, B.
imitation of happy- and sad-sounding (2012). Developmental improvements
speech. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(351), in perceptual restoration: Can young
1–8. children reconstruct missing sounds in
Wasik, N. A., Bond, M. A., & Hindman, A. noisy environments? Infant and Child
(2006). The effects of a language and Development, 21, 287–297.
literacy intervention on Head Start chil- Woolley, J. D., & Brown, M. M. (2015). The de-
dren and teachers. Journal of Educa- velopment of children’s concepts of invis-
tional Psychology, 98, 63–74. ibility. Cognitive Development, 34, 63–75.
Waxman, S., Fu, X., Arunachalam, S., Led- Woolley, J., & Ghossainy, M. (2013). Revisit-
don, E., Geraghty, K., & Song, H-j. (2013). ing the fantasy-reality distinction: Chil-
Are nouns learned before verbs? Infants dren as naive skeptics. Child Develop-
provide insight into a long-standing ment, 84(5), 1496–1510.
debate. Child Development Perspectives, Woumans, E., Martin, C. D., Bulcke, C. V.,
7(3), 155–159. Assche, E. V., Costa, A., Hartsuiker, R. J.,
Weikart, D. P., Rogers, L., Adcock, C., & McClel- & Duyck, W. (in press). Can faces prime
land, D. (1971). The cognitively oriented a language? Psychological Science.
curriculum. Urbana, IL: ERIC-NAEYC. Xu, W., Chen, A., Cheng, F., & Ma, H.
Weisberg, D. S., Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Golin­ (2011). A study on the prosodic features
koff, R. M. (2013). Guided play: Where of infant-directed speech in Mandarin
curricular goals meet a playful pedagogy. Chinese. Proceedings of the ICPhS XVII,
Mind, Brain, and Education, 7, 104–112. 2177–2180.
326  Auditory-Verbal Therapy

Yang, C. D. (2006). The infinite gift: How chil- tions in measuring maternal responses to
dren learn and unlearn the languages of communication in prelinguistic children
the world. New York, NY: Scribner. with disabilities. Communication Disor-
Yeldham, M., & Gruba, P. (2014). Toward ders Quarterly, 22(3), 135–147.
an instructional approach to developing Zangl, R., Klarman, L., Thal, D., Fernald, A.,
interactive second language listening. & Bates, E. (2005). Dynamics of word
Language Teaching Research, 18, 33–53. comprehension in infancy: Developments
Yeung, H. H., & Werker, J. F. (2013). Lip in timing, accuracy, and resistance to
movements affect infants’ audiovisual acoustic degradation. Journal of Cogni-
speech perception. Psychological Sci- tion and Development, 6, 179–208.
ence, 24, 603–612. Zekveld, A. A., Kramer, S. E., & Festen, J.
Yin, J., & Csibra, G. (2015). Concept-based M. (2011). Cognitive load during speech
word learning in human infants. Psycho- perception in noise: The influence of
logical Science, 26, 1316–1324. age, hearing loss, and cognition on the
Yoder, P. J., McCathren, R. B., Warren, S. F. pupil response. Ear and Hearing, 32,
& Watson, A. L. (2001). Important distinc- 498–510.

You might also like