You are on page 1of 14

Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Brain and Cognition


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c

What happens when children encounter an error?


Silvan F.A. Smulders a,⇑, Eric Soetens b, Maurits W. van der Molen a
a
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam 1018 XA, The Netherlands
b
Department of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Vrije Universiteit Brussels, Brussels B-1050, Belgium

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The current study presents the results of two experiments designed to assess developmental change in
Received 5 August 2015 post-error slowing (PES) across an age range extending from 5 to 25 years. Both experiments employed
Revised 11 February 2016 two-choice tasks and manipulated response-to-stimulus intervals (RSIs). The results showed that PES
Accepted 16 February 2016
decreased with advancing age; a disproportional developmental trend was observed in experiment 2
Available online 22 February 2016
while the age-related change in PES in experiment 1 was similar to the developmental decrease in basic
response speed. In both experiments, age and RSI effects on PES did not interact. This pattern of results
Keywords:
was interpreted to suggest that PES at long RSIs is due to increased caution and at short RSIs to a com-
Post-error slowing
Performance monitoring
bination of increased caution and the time it takes to orient toward the error. The developmental change
Child development in PES at longer RSIs was interpreted to suggest that as children grow older they are becoming more
Reaction time effective in setting appropriate response thresholds.
Error-related negativity Ó 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction typically slower than the average correct response. Rabbitt and
Rodgers (1977) considered several interpretations of PES. One is
The ability to adjust performance to a dynamically changing that response monitoring, i.e., the evaluation of whether the
environment is a hallmark of intelligent behavior. A key aspect of response matched the intended one, takes longer following an
this ability refers to error detection and remedial action to prevent error and may interfere with processing on the post-error trial. A
further errors. Typically, responses following an error are slower related interpretation suggested that participants, knowing that
and usually more accurate. This pattern has been observed in they committed an error, try to correct it by making the intended
humans (Laming, 1979; Rabbitt & Rodgers, 1977), monkeys response. Accordingly, PES would be due to interference between
(Jedema et al., 2011) and rodents (Narayanan, Cavanagh, Frank, & error correction and responding to the signal on the post-error
Laubach, 2013). Post-error slowing (PES) attracted various inter- trial. A third interpretation assumed that participants are dis-
pretations but the notion of ‘increased response caution’ is proba- tracted following an error, which negatively affects their speed of
bly most prominent (Dutilh et al., 2012). This interpretation is responding on the post-error trial. It should be noted, however,
readily integrated with various models of cognitive control (e.g., that these interpretations related to data obtained using very short
Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001). response-to-stimulus intervals (RSIs). Hence, Laming (1979) indi-
The seminal work on error processing (e.g., Laming, 1968; cated that these interpretations may not apply for RSIs longer than
Rabbitt, 1966, 1968; Rabbitt & Vyas, 1970; Welford, 1980) sug- half a second or so. For longer RSIs a fourth interpretation would be
gested that errors are not random events but, typically, represent more appropriate. That is, PES under those conditions is due to a
attempts to assess optimal performance limits in response to the re-adjustment of response boundaries, i.e., the criteria that must
instruction to perform as quickly and accurately as possible. Partic- be satisfied before a response is executed. Laming (1979) demon-
ipants do not know how fast they can respond until they commit strated that his data were consistent with the response caution
an error and then they have to slow down in order to prevent fur- interpretation and this has been confirmed by a recent application
ther errors. The tracking of performance may result in trial-by-trial of diffusion modeling to PES data derived from a lexical decision
speeding toward an error and a post-error response that is task (Dutilh et al., 2012).
The primary aim of the current study is to examine develop-
mental change in PES. To attain this goal, we will briefly review
⇑ Corresponding author at: The Hague University of Applied Sciences, The developmental or child studies of error processing with an eye
Netherlands. on age-related change in PES. At this point, it should be noted that
E-mail addresses: silvan.f.a.smulders@gmail.com, S.F.A.Smulders@hhs.nl (S.F.A.
most studies examining error processing in children focus on its
Smulders).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2016.02.004
0278-2626/Ó 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
S.F.A. Smulders et al. / Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47 35

neural concomitants. The developmental or child studies relevant relative to older age groups. Furthermore, sequences of correct
to PES build upon a large body of research employing electrocorti- RTs were not close to average but much slower. Finally, young chil-
cal indices of error processing (Overbeek, Nieuwenhuis, & dren made multiple errors in succession more frequently than
Ridderinkhof, 2005). The adult electrocortical studies revealed that other age groups. Collectively, these data suggest inaccurate error
error detection is associated with a negative brain potential, coined detection and inefficient performance adjustments in young
‘error negativity’, Ne (Falkenstein, Hohnsbein, Hoormann, & children.
Blanke, 1995) or ERN (Gehring, Goss, Coles, Meyer, & Donchin, Three other developmental studies examining PES used conflict
1993), followed by a positive brain potential, ‘error positivity’ rather than standard choice RT tasks. Jones, Rothbart, and Posner
(Pe), that has been associated with error awareness (2003) reported a developmental increase in PES examining 3- to
(Nieuwenhuis, Ridderinkhof, Blom, Band, & Kok, 2001). Brain imag- 4-years olds performing on a go-nogo task. They interpreted this
ing revealed that the ERN is generated within the posterior medial trend to suggest a developmental increase in cognitive control. It
prefrontal cortex (e.g., Ridderinkhof, van den Wildenberg, should be noted, however, that this conclusion is based on a lim-
Segalowitz, & Carter, 2004). Collectively, these findings have been ited number of trials (20 go vs. 20 nogo trials) and exceptionally
interpreted to suggest that the ERN and Pe are manifestations of high error rates (78%). Moreover, the age range under investigation
an error detection system that is linked to lateral regions of pre- was restricted to only one year. Schachar et al. (2004) examined
frontal cortex implicated in the implementation of strategic perfor- PES across a more extended age range, 7–16 years, using a stop-
mance adjustments, which may result in trading off speed for signal task. They obtained a positive correlation between advanc-
accuracy in order to prevent future errors (see review in ing age and response slowing following a failed inhibit. In contrast,
Ullsperger, Danielmeier, & Jocham, 2014). van de Laar, van den Wildenberg, van Boxtel, and van der Molen
The cognitive neuroscience studies of performance monitoring (2011), using a similar stop-signal task, observed that response
and adjustment provide the context for a rapidly growing litera- slowing following failed inhibits was age-invariant. Finally,
ture on developmental change in ERN and PES (for reviews Gupta, Kar, and Srinivasan (2009) employed a task-switching para-
Ferdinand & Kray, 2014; Tamnes, Walhovd, Torstveit, Sells, & digm including error feedback. They observed that PES decreased
Fjell, 2013). The ERN research, included in Table 1, revealed a across an age range between 6 and 11 years and interpreted this
developmental increase in ERN during childhood into adolescence pattern in terms of orienting toward the error signal (see Rabbitt
while PES showed little change with advancing age. It should be & Rodgers, 1977). The remaining performance studies of PES in
noted, however, that the majority of these studies focused on indi- children did not have a developmental focus. Two studies showed
vidual differences rather than developmental change. The develop- that PES was present in children aged between 8 and 11 years
mental increase in the ERN has been interpreted to reflect the (O’Connell et al., 2004; Ornstein et al., 2009) but one study failed
maturation of brain mechanisms implicated in error-monitoring to observe PES in children aged between 7 and 16 years
(i.e., the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex). The observation that (Yordanova et al., 2011). All in all, the performance studies inves-
PES was relatively age invariant suggested the idea that, initially, tigating PES in children yielded a heterogeneous pattern of results.
the mechanisms involved in error monitoring and performance The current, admittedly cursory, review of studies examining
adjustments are disconnected to become intertwined only later PES in children indicates that, given the paucity of developmental
in development (cf. Lyons & Zelazo, 2011). data, little definitive can be said about age-related change in PES.
A handful of performance studies, included in Table 1, are most Both developmental decrease and invariance have been observed
relevant for the current purpose and, thus, will be reviewed here in and a few studies reported even an increase in PES. The latter
somewhat greater detail. Fairweather (1978) was first in examin- observation, an age-related increase in PES, would be compatible
ing developmental change in PES. In contrast to the bulk of devel- with developmental neuroscience studies examining error moni-
opmental ERN studies, typically employing conflict tasks, his toring. These studies revealed a developmental increase in the
participants performed on a series of standard choice reaction ERN, an electrocortical manifestation of error detection or response
tasks. For the two-choice task, he observed a substantial decrease conflict (for reviews Crone, 2014; Ferdinand & Kray, 2014; Tamnes
in PES with advancing age, from about 600 ms in 5-year olds to et al., 2013). The developmental increase in ERN amplitude has
around 225 ms in 12-year olds. His findings led Fairweather been associated with the functional maturation of anterior cingu-
(1978) to conclude that the basic mechanisms involved in error- late cortex (ACC), a region of the medial frontal cortex that has
monitoring and performance adjustment are in place in young chil- been suggested to serve as an anatomical hub where
dren. He interpreted the developmental decrease in PES to suggest performance-monitoring information is integrated to inform the
that with advancing age the implementation of remedial action highly interconnected networks subserving subsequent action
becomes more efficient. selection (e.g., Luna, Marek, Larsen, Tervo-Clemmens, & Chahal,
The most detailed study of developmental change in PES was 2015). Within this framework, one would be led to predict a devel-
done by Brewer and Smith (1989). These authors examined error opmental increase in PES. On the hypothesis that the brain mech-
detection and performance monitoring in separate experiments anisms implicated in error monitoring are not yet fully developed
using four-choice reaction tasks. In the first experiment, partici- in young children most of their errors should go unnoticed and,
pants were asked to signal their errors by depressing a detection thus, they lack the information needed for performance adjust-
button when they felt they committed one. Error detection rate ments. Consequently, the speed of responding following an (unno-
was seen to increase rapidly from 38.5% in five-year olds to ticed) error should not differ from the speed of responding
91.1% in 11-year olds while error rates were similar across age following a correct response.
groups. These findings indicate that children do detect their errors In view of the heterogeneous pattern of age-related change in
although the youngest children are grossly inaccurate in doing so. PES, the primary goal of the present study was to perform a system-
The results of the second experiment showed response speeding atic assessment of developmental change in PES from childhood into
toward errors and PES in the adult participants. This pattern was adulthood. A standard two-choice RT task was used to generate PES
present also in young children, albeit less clear. The data seem to patterns. We employed a standard choice RT task rather than a con-
indicate that the performance tracking mechanism is present flict task that is most prominent in the developmental literature on
already from a very young age. But Brewer and Smith (1989) noted error processing. This was done for, primarily, two reasons. First,
also important differences between age groups. Young children standard choice tasks generated stable PES patterns in adults (e.g.,
continued fast responding following an error more frequently Laming, 1979). Secondly, the heterogeneous pattern that emerged
36 S.F.A. Smulders et al. / Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47

Table 1
Studies examining post-error slowing and/or error-related negativity (ERN) in different age groups. NR = not reported; NA = not applicable.

Author(S) Age range Study focus Trend PES


Albrecht et al. (2008) 8–15 ERN/ADHD NR NA
Albrecht et al. (2009) 8–15 EEG/Typical NR NA
Araujo et al. (2009) 7–18 ERN/Phenylketonuria Present NR
Araujo et al. (2013) 7–18 ERN/Phenylketonuria Present NA
Arbel and Donchin (2011) 8–10 ERN/Typical NR NA
Berwid et al. (2014) 3–6 Performance/ADHD Present NA
Burgio-Murphy et al. (2007) 7–13.5 ERN/ADHD, LD, ODD, Math NR NA
disorder
Brewer and Smith (1989) 5–23 Performance Present Decrease
Boucher et al. (2012) 9–13 ERN/Lead exposure NR NA
Brooker and Buss (2014a, 2014b) 4, 5 ERN/Fear Present in high fear NA
Brooker, Buss, and Dennis (2011) 4–8 ERN/Typical NR NA
Brooker et al. (2011) 4–8 ERN/Affective behaviors NR NA
Bruce, McDermott, Fisher, and Fox (2008) 4–7 ERN/Foster children Present NA
Carrasco et al. (2013) 10–17 ERN/OCD Present NA
Carrasco et al. (2013) 8–16 ERN/OCD, Anxiety Present Decrease
Davies et al. (2004a) 7–18 ERN/Typical NR NA
Davies, Segalowitz, and Gavin (2004b) 7–25 ERN/Typical Present No change
Eppinger, Mock, and Kray (2009) 10–12 and 19–24 ERN/Typical NR NA
Fairweather (1978) 5–12 Performance Present Decrease
Fitzgerald et al. (2010) 8–18 and 23–51 fMRI/typical NR NA
Groen et al. (2008) 10–12 ERN/ADHD and Autism NR NA
Groom et al. (2010) 16 ERN/ADHD NR NA
Groom et al. (2013) 9–15 ERN/ADHD NR NA
Gupta et al. (2009) 6–11 Performance Present Decrease
Hajcak, Franklin, Foa, and Simons (2008) 8–17 ERN/OCD Present NR
Hanna et al. (2012) 10–18 ERN/OCD Present No change
Henderson et al. (2006) 11 ERN/Autism NR NA
Hillman, Buck, Themanson, Pontifex, and Castelli (2009) 9 Performance/Aerobic fitness Present NA
Hogan, Vargha-Khadem, Kirkham, and Baldeweg (2005) 12–22 ERN/Typical Present only on difficult NR
task
Horowitz-Kraus (2011) 13 and 25 ERN/Dyslexia NR NA
Horowitz-Kraus and Breznitz (2014) 12 ERN/Dyslexia NR NA
Hum, Manassis, and Lewis (2013) 8–12 ERN/Anxiety NR NA
Jones et al. (2003) 3–4 Performance Present (in older) Increase
Jonkman, van Melis, Kemner, and Markus (2007) 10 ERN/ADHD Absent NA
Kamijo et al. (2014) 7–9 ERN/Obesity Present NA
Kim, Iwaki, Imashioya, Uno, and Fujita (2007) 7–11 and 21–25 ERN/Typical NR NA
Ladouceur, Dahl, Birmaher, Axelson, and Ryan (2006) 8–14 ERN/Anxiety Present NA
Ladouceur, Dhal, and Carter (2007) 8–13, 14–18, and 19–49 ERN/Typical Present No change
Ladouceur et al. (2012) 7–17 ERN/Depression Present NR
Ladouceur, Dahl, and Carter (2004) 9–17 ERN/Typical Present NR
Laurens et al. (2010) 9–12 ERN/Schizophrenic antecedents Present NA
Liotti, Pliszka, Perez, Kothmann, and Woldorff (2005) 9–11 ERN/ADHD NR NA
McDermott et al. (2009) 15 ERN/Anxiety NR NA
McDermott et al. (2013) 8 ERN/Foster care Present NA
Meyer et al. (2012) 8–13 ERN/Anxiety Present NR
O’Connell et al. (2004) 11 Performance/SCR/ADHD Present NA
Ornstein et al. (2009) 10 Performance/TBI Present NA
Pontifex et al. (2010) 9, 19, and 65 ERN/Typical NR NA
Pontifex et al. (2011) 10 ERN/Aerobic Fitness Present (?) NA
Richardson, Anderson, Reid, and Fox (2011) 7–9 ERN/Typical Present (minimal) NR
Rosch and Hawk (2013) 10–12 ERN/ADHD NR NA
Rubia, Halari, Mohammad, Taylor, and Brammer (2011) 10–15 fMRI/ADHD Absent NA
Santesso and Segalowitz (2008) 15–16 and 18–20 ERN/Typical Present Increase
Santesso, Segalowitz, and Schmidt (2006) 10 and 18–33 ERN/Typical Present No change
Schachar et al. (2004) 7–16 Performance/ADHD Present Decrease
Senderecka, Grabowska, Szewczyk, Gerc, and Chmylak 6–12 ERN/ADHD NR NA
(2012)
Shen, Tsai, and Duann (2011) 6–10 ERN/ADHD NR NA
Sokhadze et al. (2012) 9–21 ERN/Autism Present NA
South, Larson, Krauskopf, and Clawson (2010) 14 ERN/Autism Present NA
Spinelli et al. (2011) 10 fMRI/ADHD Present NA
Stieben et al. (2007) 8–12 ERN/Externalisation Present NA
Torpey, Hajcak, and Klein (2009) 5–7 ERN/Typical NR NA
Torpey, Hajcak, Kim, Kujawa, and Klein (2012) 5–7 ERN/Typical Present NR
Torpey et al. (2013) 6 ERN/Temperament Present NA
van de Laar et al. (2012) 7, 11, 20 and 75 Performance Present No change
van de Voorde, Roeyers, and Wiersema (2010) 9–10 ERN/ADHD Absent NA
van Meel, Heslenfeld, Oosterlaan, and Sergeant (2007) 8–12 ERN/ADHD Present NA
S.F.A. Smulders et al. / Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47 37

Table 1 (continued)

Author(S) Age range Study focus Trend PES


van Meel, Heslenfeld, Rommelse, Oosterlaan, and Sergeant 6–9, 10–12, and 18–26 ERN/Typical NR NA
(2012)
Vlamings, Jonkman, Hoeksma, van Engeland, and Kemner 10 ERN/Autism Present NA
(2008)
Wiersema, van der Meere, and Roeyers (2005) 7–13 ERN/ADHD Present NA
Wiersema et al. (2007) 7–8, 13–14, and 23–24 ERN/Typical Present No change
Wild-Wall, Oades, Schmidt-Wessels, Christiansen, and 8–18 ERN/ADHD Present NR
Falkenstein (2009)
Yordanova et al. (2011) 7–16 Performance/ADHD Absent NA
Zhang, Wang, Cai, and Yan (2009) 7–11 ERN/ADHD NR NA
Narayanan and Laubach (2008) Rat PES present
Moon et al. (2006) Mouse PES present
Ito, Stuphorn, Brown, and Schall (2003) Monkey Error detection
Jedema et al. (2011) Monkey PES present

from the child literature on error-processing might relate to the use 2.1. Method
of conflict tasks. The obvious advantage of using conflict tasks is a
pronounced error rate and, thus, this type of task is cost-effective 2.1.1. Participants
with regard to experimental time. Less trials are needed for obtain- Participants (N = 87) were recruited from three age groups;
ing sufficient errors and that is especially profitable when working between 5 and 25 years of age. There was one group of 42 children
with young children. A disadvantage, however, is that errors might between 5 and 12 (M = 8.5 (SD = 2.33) years; 21 girls). In addition,
be less salient compared to standard choice tasks, in particular for there was a group of 25 adolescents between the ages of 13 and 17
younger children. In this regard, the use of conflict tasks might (M = 14.4 (SD = 1.28) years; 16 females). Finally, a group of 20
obscure potential developmental trends in PES. Indeed, Brewer young adults between the ages of 18 and 25 (M = 21.9 (SD = 1.47)
and Smith (1989) used a four-choice task and observed that the years; 10 females) enrolled in the experiment. A preliminary v2-
influence of performance tracking was already visible in 5-year olds. analysis evaluating potential gender differences across age groups
Finally, it should be noted that we used a standard two-choice rather failed to produce a significant outcome (p > .50). The children and
than a standard multiple-choice task that has been standard in the adolescents were selected with the help of their schools and with
adult literature. We opted for a two-choice task to avoid potential permission of their caregivers. All children had average or above
confusion between responses, so as to reduce the demands on average intelligence based on teachers’ reports. The adolescents
response monitoring (e.g., Fairweather, 1978). were recruited from a high school. The young adults (18–25 yrs)
Consistent with the early adult literature on PES (e.g., Laming, were undergraduate psychology students. They were recruited by
1968; Rabbitt & Vyas, 1970), we manipulated RSI from 50 to flyers and received course credits for their participation. All partic-
1000 ms to assess whether the anticipated developmental change ipants reported to be in good health and had normal or corrected-
in PES is altered by RSI. It was predicted that PES would decrease to-normal vision. Informed consent was obtained from the adoles-
with a lengthening of RSI (e.g., Dudschig & Jentzsch, 2009) and this cent and adult participants and from the primary caregivers for the
trend should be more pronounced for young children compared to children. All procedures were approved by the Ethical Review
adults assuming that they experience more difficulty to resolve the Board of the University.
interference of immediate reactions to the error or implement
appropriate measures to ensure a more cautious response on the 2.1.2. Apparatus and task
subsequent response. Two experiments will be conducted to assess The experiment was run on 12-, and 15-in. screen PCs. Although
the robustness of the age-related change in PES should such a trend the screens were different in size, the task-relevant displays were
occur. The assessment of the robustness of developmental change identical across PCs. A vertical, black line was presented through
in PES seems warranted in view of the heterogeneous pattern that the center of the screen against a white background. The stimuli,
emerged from the literature available to date (see Table 1). red circles, were presented 5 cm to the left or right from the verti-
cal line (1.5 cm). Participants viewed the monitor from a distance
2. Experiment 1 of 40–60 cm, and responded to the stimuli by pushing the ‘z’ key
with their left-index finger or the ‘/’ key with their right-index fin-
Three age groups participated in the first experiment covering ger. These keys are on the bottom row of a ‘qwerty’ keyboard. Par-
an age-range between 5 and 25 years. This range includes the ages ticipants performed on a relatively standard two-choice task; a red
examined by Davies, Segalowitz, and Gavin (2004a) and Wiersema, circle presented to the left of the vertical line asked for a left-hand
van der Meere, and Roeyers (2007) who reported that developmen- button press while a red circle presented to the right of the vertical
tal change in PES is absent. The age range is also similar to the one line required a right-hand button press.
used by Brewer and Smith (1989) who observed that PES did occur
in young children but, unfortunately, did not test whether it chan- 2.1.3. Design and procedure
ged with advancing age. Fairweather (1978) performed such a test RT was recorded as the time between stimulus onset and the
and reported a significant developmental decrease in PES over a moment that one of the response keys was pushed. The stimulus
limited age range (5–12 years). The current study will add to his was response terminated. The response initiated the RSI, which
findings by examining a wider age range and by assessing whether was fixed within experimental blocks either at 50, 150, 200, 250,
developmental change in PES is sensitive to the manipulation of 500 or 1000 ms. Participants performed under each RSI condition.
RSI. Moreover, it will be tested whether the age-related differences The computer pseudo-randomized the order of RSI conditions
in PES, if they occur, are disproportional and, thus, are not resulting across participants. An experimental session consisted of 24 exper-
from basic response speed differences between age groups. imental blocks; each RSI condition consisted of 4 consecutive
38 S.F.A. Smulders et al. / Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47

Table 2
Error rate (total number of error trials), mean RT (ms) and N for each age group and RSI condition (Experiment 1).

Age group RSI


50 150 200
Error rate N RT Error rate N RT Error rate N RT
5–12 yrs 35.4 8.6 763.1 25.3 10.6 615.8 32.5 11.2 599.2
13–17 yrs 14.5 7.2 394.8 18.0 10.1 382.2 22.3 10.8 342.7
18–25 yrs 22.3 11.9 351.0 27.9 17.8 306.6 35.6 19.5 295.6
250 500 1000
Error rate N RT Error Rate N RT Error rate N RT
5–12 yrs 27.0 11.4 773.4 23.8 12.3 633.3 19.6 12.5 721.3
13–17 yrs 19.5 12.2 339.2 18.6 11.7 325.0 15.4 11.3 332.6
18–25 yrs 35.2 21.3 287.7 28.5 19.0 277.6 18.0 17.3 279.0

Abbreviations: yrs refers to years; RT to reaction time; RSI to response-to-stimulus interval (ms); N to the number of errors surrounded by correct responses (i.e., E 3, E 2,
E 1, E, E + 1, E + 2 E + 3).

blocks, with each block consisting of 100 trials. It was assumed that trial (E), and three subsequent trials (E + 1, E + 2, and E + 3)), as
a total of 2400 trials would be sufficient to generate stable PES pat- within Ss factors. The analysis showed a developmental increase
terns (e.g., Dutilh et al., 2012). Before each RSI condition, partici- in the speed of responding, F(2, 84) = 34.35, p < .001, g2p = .45,
pants performed a practice block of 50 trials. The first five trials and faster responses with a lengthening of RSI, F(5, 420) = 3.95,
in each experimental block were considered ‘warm-up’ and were p < .011, g2p = .17. Moreover, a developmental decrease in the RSI
excluded from statistical analyses. No error corrections were effect was found, F(10, 420) = 3.20, p < .011, g2p = .07. Importantly,
possible. the effect of Trial was highly significant, F(6, 504) = 14.25, p < .001,
Between blocks there was a 30-s rest period, and after three RSI g2p = .15, and was included in interactions with RSI, F(30, 2520)
conditions (12 blocks) there was a 2-min break. A white screen = 1.99, p < .002, g2p = .020, and Age group, F(12, 504) = 3.72,
crossed by the vertical black line initiated a new trial block. Partic- p < .001, g2p = .08. The higher order interaction including Age
ipants received an on-screen instruction before starting the exper- group, Trial and RSI was not significant, p > .97.
iment. They were instructed to respond to the left stimulus by Pre-Error RT. Pre-error change in the speed of responding was
using their left hand and to the right stimulus by using their right evaluated by comparing RTs associated with the correct trials that
hand. They were asked to respond quickly and to avoid errors. All immediately preceded the error (i.e., E 3, E 2, and E 1). The
participants were tested individually in a quiet laboratory or class- analysis indicated that the main effect of Trial was not significant
room, with similar ambient conditions (e.g., light-, noise- and tem- and was not altered by the effects of RSI or Age group, ps > .57.
perature levels). Participants spent approximately one hour in the Hence, the response speeding toward an error that has been
laboratory (psychology students) or classroom (children and ado- observed in some studies (e.g., Brewer & Smith, 1989; Shiels,
lescents), including instructions and breaks. Tamm, & Epstein, 2012; but see Laming, 1979) is absent in the cur-
rent data.
Error RT. The analysis aimed at the error trial, indicated that the
2.2. Results speed of responding was significantly faster on the error trial com-
pared to the average RT of the responses associated with the pre-
Error rates and mean RTs are presented in Table 2 for each age ceding trials (i.e., E 3, E 2, and E 1), F(1, 84) = 16.50,
group by RSI combination.1 The N in the table refers to the number p < .001, g2p = .16. This Trial effect discriminated between age
of trial sequences, consisting of an error surrounded by correct groups, F(2, 84) = 4.94, p < .009, g2p = .11, but not between RSI con-
responses, which entered into the analyses. ditions, p > .99. The higher-order interaction was not significant
Error rates. Error rates were square-root transformed for skew- either, p > .99. The RT difference (error vs. pre-errors) was larger
ness and submitted to ANOVA with Age group (3), as between Ss for children (184.58 ms) compared to adolescents (47.23 ms) and
factor, and RSI (6) as within Ss factor. Adolescents (M = 4.5%, adults (47.33 ms), ps < .04, gs2p > .084.2 Adolescents and adults
SD = 0.7%) make less errors than children (M = 6.8%, SD = 0.9%) did not differ in this regard, p > .98. It should be noted, however, that
and adults (M = 7.0%, SD = 1.0%), however, the ANOVA did not yield the interaction between Trials and Age group did not survive when
a significant main effect of Age group, p > .06. The main effect of RSI basic speed (i.e., age group RT) was included as a covariate, p > .93.
was not significant either, p > .14. Moreover, the interaction This result indicates that the errors of children were not dispropor-
between the effects of age group and RSI did not reach an accept- tionally faster compared to the two other age groups.
able level of significance, p > .21. Post-error RT. PES was evaluated by comparing RT on the correct
The RTs associated with trial sequences surrounding an error trial that immediately followed the error (i.e., E + 1) to the average
(i.e., 3 trials preceding the error, the error, and 2 trials following RT associated with the responses on the three trials preceding the
the error) are presented in Fig. 1. For each age group errors are fas- error (i.e., E 3, E 2, and E 1). The analysis indicated that, as
ter than correct responses on adjacent trials. The first response fol- anticipated, the speed of responding was considerably slower on
lowing an error is especially slow. Little change can be observed the post-error trial compared to the average speed of responding
between the responses preceding the error. before the error (644 ms vs. 414 ms, respectively), F(1, 84)
Complete RT sequence. Median RTs were subjected to ANOVA = 55.85, p < .001, g2p = .40. Most importantly, the analysis yielded
with Age group (3) as between Ss factor, and RSI (6) and Trials a developmental decrease in PES, F(2, 84) = 13.26, p < .001,
(7; three trials before the error (E 3, E 2, and E 1), the error g2p = .24, which is plotted in Fig. 2 (panel a). Subsequent analysis
revealed that each age group differed significantly from the other,
1
It should be noted that the total number of errors is slightly larger than the N ps < .003, gs2p > .13.
reported in the table. This is due to the requirement that all responses on trials
surrounding an error should be correct (see Table 2; see also Brewer and Smith
2
(1989)). All p-values associated with post-hoc comparisons are Bonferroni-corrected.
S.F.A. Smulders et al. / Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47 39

PES was influenced by RSI, F(5, 420) = 3.55, p < .006, g2p = .18. trend did not deviate from the age group differences in basic
Contrast analyses revealed that PES was significantly smaller for response speed. Our review identified only four studies that tested
RSI-1000 compared to the RSI-150 till RSI-500 ms, ps < .022, age-related change in PES (Carrasco, Harbin, et al., 2013; Carrasco,
gs2p > .06. Surprisingly, PES did not discriminate between the Hong, et al., 2013; Fairweather, 1978; Gupta, Kar, & Srinivasan,
shortest vs. longest RSI (see Fig. 2, panel b). The higher-order inter- 2009; Schachar et al., 2004) but none of these studies examined
action between Trial, Age group and RSI was not significant, p = .83. whether the observed age-related decrease in PES was dispropor-
Post-error recovery. An analysis focusing on post-error recovery tional. Experiment 2 was designed to re-address this issue. More
included the average of the three trials at the beginning of the specifically, the goal of this experiment was to test the robustness
sequence (i.e., E 3, E 2, and E 1) and the two trials subse- of the downward developmental trend in PES and to examine
quent to the post-error trial (i.e., E + 2 and E + 3). This analysis whether a substantial increase in observations would produce a
was done to evaluate the time it took for participants to attain a disproportional trend.
‘safe’ level of responding again. The effect of Trial was highly signif-
icant, F(2, 168) = 20.99, p < .01, g2p = .16. This effect was not
3. Experiment 2
altered by Age group or RSI condition, ps > .54. Subsequent analy-
ses showed that RTs associated with E + 2 and E + 3 were signifi-
The primary goal of this experiment was to assess the robust-
cantly longer than the average RT of the pre-error trials, ps < .01,
ness of the data-pattern that emerged from the previous experi-
gs2p > .21. Thus, it took at least two trials for participants to adjust ment. The same two-choice task was used but the number of
from an error.
RSIs was reduced to two conditions (50 vs. 500 ms) and number
Follow-up analyses. We conducted three follow-up analyses for a
of trials was increased from 4 blocks of 100 trials per RSI (previous
complete assessment of the apparent age-related change in PES.
experiment) to 24 blocks of 100 trials per RSI (current experiment).
First, although the v2-analysis on gender failed to produce a signif-
Each participant performed the task on four separate sessions. The
icant outcome, we included gender as a covariate in the analysis.
number of trials was increased in an attempt at obtaining a devel-
This did not impact the significant Age group  Trial type interac-
opmental PES pattern that would survive the rigorous test of con-
tion, F(2, 83) = 13.59, p < .001, gs2p > .25. Next, we included group
trolling for basic speed differences between age groups. The
RT as covariate. This analysis showed that the interaction between
increase in trial number served also the purpose of re-examining
Age group and Trial did not reach significance anymore, p > .64.
the somewhat erratic RSI  PES pattern that emerged from the first
Hence, the age-related decrease in PES does not differ from the
experiment. Although PES was reduced for RSI-1000 compared to
age-related decrease in the basic speed of responding. Thus, young
the shorter RSIs it did not differ from the shortest RSI-50. The cur-
children did not show a disproportional slowing following the
rent comparison between RSI-50 vs. RSI-500 was selected because
error. Finally, it should be noted that error rates generated by the
PES should be reduced from RSI-50 to RSI-500 and should not alter
standard choice task were relatively low and differed across indi-
significantly beyond RSI-500 (see Laming, 1979).
viduals. From the full sample, we selected those participants hav-
ing a minimum N of 5 per cell.3 The analysis of the data of this
restricted sample did not change the pattern obtained for the full 3.1. Method
sample. That is, a significant Age group  Trial interaction, F(2, 55)
= 20.47, p < .001, gs2p > .28, that remained when including gender 3.1.1. Participants
as covariate, F(2, 54) = 10.96, p < .001, gs2p > .29, but did not reach Participants (N = 63) were recruited from three age groups
significance when basic speed was included as covariate, F(2, 54) between 7 and 25 years of age. There were two groups of children;
= 3.13, p > .05, gs2p > .10. 16 children between 7 and 9 years of age (M = 7.9 (SD = 0.83)
years; 5 girls); 23 children between 10 and 12 years of age
2.3. Interim summary (M = 11.2 (SD = 0.78) years; 9 girls). Finally, a group of 24 young
adults between the ages of 18 and 25 (M = 19.8 (SD = 1.9) years;
The results that emerged from the first experiment are consis- 20 females) enrolled in the experiment. A v2-analysis evaluating
tent with previous reports examining developmental change in potential gender differences across age groups produced a signifi-
the speed of responding in showing (1) a developmental increase cant outcome, v2(2) = 13.69, p < .001, phi = .47. Thus, Gender will
in the speed of responding, (2) faster responses to longer RSIs, be included as covariate in subsequent statistical analyses.
and (3) a developmental decrease in the RSI effect (e.g., Smulders Although the specific age groups are somewhat different than
et al., 2005). In addition, consistent with previous reports on PES, those participating in the first experiment, the age-range under
the current results showed that (4) errors are particularly fast investigation is approximately similar.
and followed by response slowing. Furthermore (5) PES was smal-
ler to the longest RSI compared to the shorter RSIs (e.g., Rabbitt & 3.1.2. Task, design and procedure
Rodgers, 1977). However, (6) in contrast to expectations PES did Participants performed on the 2-choice task used in Experiment
not discriminate between the shortest and longest RSI and (7) 1. RSI was fixed within experimental blocks either at 50 or 500 ms.
response speeding on trials just preceding an error was not Participants performed under each RSI condition. The order of RSI
observed (e.g., Laming, 1979). Most importantly, the results conditions was counterbalanced across participants. An experi-
showed that (8) PES decreased with advancing age but (9) this mental session consisted of 12 experimental blocks; each RSI con-
developmental trend did not survive a test controlling for basic dition consisted of 6 consecutive blocks, with each block
speed differences across age groups. Finally, (10) it took at least containing 100 trials. Before each RSI condition, participants per-
two trials to recover from an error but this effect did not discrim- formed a practice block of 50 trials. Between blocks there was a
inate between age groups. The current finding of a developmental 30-s rest period, and after 12 blocks there was a 2-min break. Par-
decrease in PES should be qualified by the observation that this ticipants performed on a total of 4800 trials (i.e., 4 sessions  6
blocks  2 RSI conditions  100 trials), during four individual test-
3
ing sessions, scheduled within five consecutive days (i.e., the sec-
Total number of participants of the restricted sample was 58; 23 children (5–
12 yrs; 12 girls), 17 adolescents (13–17 yrs; 10 females), and 18 young adults (18–
ond and third sessions were separated by one day). The order of
25 yrs; 9 females). A v2-analysis evaluating potential gender differences across age RSIs was consistent across sessions. All other details were similar
groups failed to produce a significant outcome (p > .85). to Experiment 1.
40 S.F.A. Smulders et al. / Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47

Fig. 1. Reaction time difference (ms), including standard error bars, relative to group average for each pre- and post-error trial, error trial, and response-to-stimulus interval
(RSI) condition. Plots are presented for each age group, separately, in panel a, b, and c.

3.2. Results Error rates. Mean error rates (%) were submitted to a 2 (RSI;
within subjects)  3 (Age group; between-subjects) ANOVA. A
Error rates and mean RTs are presented in Table 3 for each age main effect of Age group was absent, p > .62. RSI did affect error
group by RSI combination (across session). rate significantly (4.3% and 4.8% for RSI-50 and RSI-500 ms respec-
S.F.A. Smulders et al. / Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47 41

Fig. 2. Panel a. Post-error Slowing (PES), including standard error bars, for each age group. ⁄Denotes significant differences in PES between indicated age groups. Panel b. Post-
error Slowing (PES) as a function of response-to-stimulus interval (RSI). ⁄Denotes significant differences between indicated RSI conditions. PES, both in a and b, has been
computed as the average reaction time difference (ms) between the mean RT for trials E 3, 2, 1 vs. trial E + 1.

tively), F(1, 60) = 4.66, p < .035, g2p = .072. The interaction between
Table 3
the effects of Age group and RSI did not reach significance, p > .19.
Error rate (total number of error trials), mean RT (ms) and N for each age group and
Complete RT sequence. The RTs associated with trial sequences RSI condition (across session) (Experiment 2).
surrounding an error (i.e., 3 trials preceding the error, the error,
Age group RSI-50 RSI-500 N N RT RT
and 3 trials following the error) are presented in Fig. 3. For each
Error rate Error rate RSI-50 RSI-500 RSI-50 RSI-500
age group errors are faster than correct responses on the adjacent
7–9 yrs 103.2 103.2 33.8 52.5 593.0 607.9
trials. The first response following an error is very slow. Median
10–12 yrs 93.6 132.0 52.0 75.7 424.2 360.2
RTs were subjected to ANOVA with Age group (3), as between Ss 18–25 yrs 110.4 110.4 57.8 83.7 385.7 324.3
factor, and RSI (2) and Trial (7; three trials before the error
Abbreviations: yrs refers to years; RT to reaction time; RSI to response-to-stimulus
(E 3, E 2, and E 1), the error trial (E), and three subsequent
interval (ms); N to the number of errors surrounded by correct responses (i.e., E 3,
trials (E + 1, E + 2, and E + 3), as within Ss factors. E 2, E 1, E, E + 1, E + 2 E + 3).
The analysis showed a developmental increase in the speed of
responding, F(2, 60) = 25.07, p < .001, g2p = .50, and faster
responses to longer RSIs, F(1, 60) = 14.82, p < .001, g2p = .23. The
RSI effect decreased with advancing age, F(2, 60) = 8.31, p < .001, the error (i.e., E 3, E 2, and E 1). The analysis indicated that
g2p = .25. The effect of Trial was highly significant, F(6, 360) the speed of responding did not differ significantly across trials,
= 70.66, p < .001, g2p = .58, and was included in interactions with p > .10 and was not altered by the effects of RSI or Age group,
RSI, F(6, 360) = 3.57 p < .002, g2p = .07, and Age group, F(12, 360) ps > .16.
= 5.03, p < .001, g2p = .17. The higher order interaction including Error RT. The analysis of the speed of responding on the error
Age group, Trial and RSI was not significant, p > .39. Collectively, trial indicated that the speed of responding was significantly faster
these findings are identical to the pattern of results obtained in on the error trial compared to the average RT associated with the
Experiment 1. three trials preceding the error trial (i.e., E 3, E 2, and E 1),
Pre-error RT. Pre-error speeding was evaluated by comparing F(1, 60) = 52.78, p < .001, g2p = .45. The Trial effect was not signifi-
RTs associated with the correct trials that immediately preceded cantly altered by the effects of Age group and RSI, ps > .23.
42 S.F.A. Smulders et al. / Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47

Fig. 3. Reaction time difference (ms), including standard error bars, relative to group average for each pre- and post-error trial, error trial, age group. Plotted for each
response-to-stimulus interval (RSI) condition, separately.

Post-error RT. PES was evaluated by comparing RT on the correct affect the Age group by Trial interaction; this interaction remained
trial that immediately followed the error (i.e., E + 1) to the average highly significant, F(2, 59) = 9.44, p < .001, gs2p > .24).
RT of the responses associated with the three trials at the begin- Importantly, the analysis using group RT as covariate revealed
ning of the sequence. The analysis indicated that the speed of that the Trial type by Age group interaction remained significant,
responding was considerably slower on the post-error trial com- p < .003, g2p = .17, indicating a disproportional age effect for PES.
pared to the average speed of responding on the three trials pre- The analysis on the restricted sample revealed that the perti-
ceding the error (619 ms vs. 409 ms, respectively), F(1, 60) nent Age group  Trial interaction remained significant, F(2, 42)
= 118.59, p < .001, g2p = .65 (PES effect). As anticipated, the analysis = 6.24, p < .004, gs2p > .23. Importantly, this interaction survived
yielded a developmental decrease in PES, F(2, 60) = 9.88, p < .001, when correcting for group differences in basic speed, F(2, 41)
g2p = .23, which is plotted in Fig. 4. Subsequent analysis revealed = 5.64, p < .007, gs2p > .21, and gender, F(1, 41) = 7.68, p < .001,
that each age group differed significantly from the others, gs2p > .27.
ps < .035, gs2p > .10. Finally, PES was influenced by RSI, F(1, 60) Finally, it could be argued that our developmental pattern of
= 15.00, p < .001, g2p = .19. As can be seen in Fig. 4, PES was signif- findings might be compromised by the effects of fatigue or bore-
icantly smaller for RSI-500 compared to the RSI-50 condition (i.e., dom in view of the considerable amount of trials. To address this
160 ms vs. 262 ms, respectively). The higher order interaction issue, we conducted an analysis including Session (4) as an addi-
between Trial, Age group and RSI was not significant, p > .47. tional within Ss factor. Fatigue or Boredom should be manifested
Post-error recovery. The analysis focusing on the speed of post- by an increase in RT across sessions. The results revealed the oppo-
error recovery (i.e., a comparison between the average RT of the site; the analysis showed a significant decrease in RT from 490 to
three trials preceding the error vs. the RTs of the 2nd and 3rd trials 422 ms, F(3, 180) = 4.50, p < .006, gs2p > .07. Thus, the overall pat-
following the error) yielded a highly significant main effect of Trial, tern suggests a beneficial effect of practice rather than a detrimen-
F(2, 120) = 19.24, p < .001, g2p = .29. This effect did not discrimi- tal effect of fatigue or boredom. The effect of Session was qualified,
nate between Age Groups and RSI conditions, ps > .27. Subsequent however, by a significant interaction with Age group, F(6, 180)
analyses showed that RTs associated with E + 2 and E + 3 differed = 2.99, p < .008, gs2p > .09. Young adults and older children showed
significantly from the average RT of the pre-error trials, ps < .002, a significant decrease with session, ps < .001, while RT was rela-
gs2p > .13. Thus, it took at least two trials for participants to adjust tively stable across sessions for the younger children, p > .92.
from an error. Hence, the current pattern of PES results observed for the younger
Follow-up analyses. We conducted four sets of follow-up analy- children is not influenced by time-on-task. For reasons of com-
ses. Similar to the previous experiment, we evaluated the potential pleteness we ran a similar analysis on error rates that showed a
influence of gender and group RT in assessing developmental significant effect of Session, F(3, 186) = 3.59, p < .015, gs2p > .05.
change in PES. And similar to the previous experiment, we evalu- However, error rate differed only between session 1 (3,9%) and 3
ated developmental change in PES for a restricted sample of partic- (5,1%), p < .006, not between other sessions, ps > .11. Session did
ipants; viz. only participants with a minimum N of 5.4 Finally, given not interact with Age group, p > .06.
the substantial amount of trials presented to our participants we
evaluated the potential influence of time on task.
Recall that gender differed significantly between age groups. 3.3. Interim summary
However, the analysis including gender as covariate did not really
The pattern of results that emerged from the second experiment
is largely consistent with expectations; (1) Errors were faster than
4
responses on preceding trials and the response on the trial imme-
The total number of participants for the restricted sample was 45; 7 young
children (7–9 yrs; 3 girls), 17 older children (10–12 yrs; 6 girls), and 21 young adults
diately following an error was particularly slow (PES), (2) PES was
(18–25 yrs; 18 females). A v2-analysis evaluating potential gender differences across now significantly more pronounced for the short RSI-50 compared
age groups produced a significant outcome, v2(2) = 10.97, p < .004, phi = .49. to the long RSI-500, (3) PES decreased with advancing age, (4), in
S.F.A. Smulders et al. / Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47 43

Fig. 4. Post-error Slowing (PES), including standard error bars, for each age group and response-to-stimulus interval (RSI) condition. PES has been computed as the reaction
time difference (ms) between the mean RT for trials E 3, 2, 1 vs. trial E + 1.

contrast to the results obtained in Experiment 1, this developmen- unexpectedly, not from the shortest RSI-50. Increasing trial num-
tal trend did survive a test controlling for basic speed differences bers in Experiment 2 yielded the expected pattern. That is, PES
across age groups, and finally (5) it took all participants at least 2 associated with RSI-50 was considerably larger compared to PES
trials to adjust to the error but this effect did not discriminate related to RSI-500. Thus, the RSI-PES pattern that emerged from
between age groups. Experiment 2 is consistent with the extant PES literature (e.g.,
Rabbitt & Rodgers, 1977). We anticipated that the RSI effect on
4. Discussion PES would be more pronounced for children compared to young
adults but both experiments showed that the higher order interac-
The current experiments yielded a consistent pattern of results tion between Trial type, Age group and RSI was far from significant.
consisting of a relatively fast error followed by PES. Response Our prediction was based on the idea that errors attract reactions
speeding toward the error was not observed but it should be noted to the error—error corrections (Rabbitt, 1966), error monitoring
that pre-error speeding is not a ubiquitous feature of the response (Welford, 1980), orienting (Notebaert et al., 2009)—that may inter-
pattern associated with errors. Although pre-error speeding seems fere with the response on the post-error trial. On the account that
part and parcel of the performance-tracking notion (e.g., Rabbitt & children are more sensitive to interference (van der Molen, 2000),
Rodgers, 1977) and is reported in some studies (e.g., Brewer & it was anticipated that they would exhibit disproportional slowing
Smith, 1989; Dudschig & Jentzsch, 2009), it has been noted already at short RSIs relative to young adults. These expectations were not
by Laming (1979) that pre-error speeding is not a prerequisite for borne out by the current data.
PES to occur (see also Rabbitt, 1966). The current data revealed an interesting pattern. Both experi-
Most importantly, with regard to our developmental perspec- ments yielded two-way interactions between Trial type and RSI
tive, both experiments yielded a decrease in PES with advancing and between Trial type and Age group in the absence of three-
age. The results of the second experiment indicated that this trend way Trial type  RSI  Age group interactions. This pattern sug-
was disproportional and, thus, cannot be interpreted in terms of gests that, at least, two mechanisms are involved in PES; one oper-
developmental change in the speed of responding per se. The cur- ating at short RSIs that is age invariant and another that is sensitive
rent findings are consistent with the results reported previously to development and is RSI invariant. The notion of two distinct
by Fairweather (1978) who interpreted PES in terms of interfer- mechanisms operating within different timeframes has been pro-
ence due to error-correction responses. Presumably, young chil- posed previously by Jentzsch and Dudschig (2009). A tentative
dren are more sensitive to this interference than older ones. It interpretation of the current findings assumes that PES is a joint
should be noted, however, that Fairweather (1978) used a rela- function of orienting and response caution at short RSIs while it
tively short RSI (200 ms) and, thus, his interpretation might not results from response caution only at longer RSIs. The orienting
apply to results obtained using longer RSIs. Brewer and Smith account of PES has been proposed by Notebaert et al. (2009) who
(1989), using a longer RSI, observed a pattern of response speeding obtained evidence to suggest that slowing is a function of the fre-
toward the error and slowing following the error for adults that quency of the preceding response rather than its accuracy. Orient-
was less clear in children. These findings led Brewer and Smith ing facilitates attention to the surprising event and is associated
(1989) to conclude that young children are less able in tracking with a temporary de-activation of the motor system (Lynn, 1966;
their performance and regulating their speed accordingly. Unfortu- Sokolov, 1963; see also Ursin, 2005). There is no reason to believe
nately, the apparent age-related changes in PES were not evaluated that hard-wired orienting is subject to age-related change. Orient-
statistically. ing is short-lived and dissipates over time. Hence, on long RSI tri-
The current RSI  PES patterns differed across the two experi- als, the ‘emergency brake’ following an error (or infrequent
ments. The RSI data from the first experiment showed that PES dif- event) is released prior to the occurrence of the new stimulus.
fered significantly between RSI-1000 vs. the shorter RSIs but, PES on those trials is then due to response caution only.
44 S.F.A. Smulders et al. / Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47

Two further issues remain to be addressed. The first is the suggested that implicit error detection, indexed by the ERN,
heterogeneous pattern of findings reported by studies examining matures at a slower rate compared to explicit error detection,
PES in children. It is difficult to present a unified account of these indexed by the Pe (Wiersema et al., 2007). Accordingly, adults
findings but task differences might be an important factor con- may rely on automatic error detection without the need for explicit
tributing to the heterogeneity. The few studies reporting a devel- monitoring while young children have to rely on conscious and fal-
opmental decrease in PES employed relatively standard choice lible error monitoring (Lyons & Zelazo, 2011). With advancing age,
reaction tasks (Brewer & Smith, 1989; Fairweather, 1978). children rely increasingly on implicit monitoring that is both less
Berwid, Halperin, Johnson, and Marks (2014), however, used a taxing and more accurate. Although the alleged developmental
two-choice task but reported stability rather than developmental shift from explicit to implicit error monitoring provides an elegant
change. It should be noted, however, that this age-invariance was explanation for the age-related increase in the ERN it remains to be
based on only 40 trials and about 13% errors. Thus, the limited seen how this shift may alter developmental change in the strate-
amount of observations might have prevented the detection of gic adjustments following an error. It could be hypothesized that,
developmental change. Indeed, the current data testify of the relative to explicit error monitoring, the more accurate and less
importance of the number of trials as the increase in the number taxing implicit monitoring might free-up resources for more effi-
of trials in Experiment 2 resulted in (1) a developmental trend in cient performance adjustments thereby decreasing PES. Future
PES going beyond the global age-related change in the speed of brain-potential research should address this hypothesis by exam-
responding and (2) a substantial reduction in PES with a lengthen- ining the relative amplitudes of ERN and Pe vis-à-vis developmen-
ing of RSI from 50 to 500 ms. tal change in PES.
The current review indicated that most previous studies exam-
ining performance monitoring in children used various types of
conflict tasks. Typically, these studies observed that PES was age- 5. Conclusion
invariant (see Table 1). There are a few notable exceptions, how-
ever. Gupta et al. (2009) observed a developmental decrease in Our review of developmental PES studies suggested a heteroge-
PES employing a task-switching paradigm. In contrast to most neous pattern of results. The majority of studies, mostly using con-
other studies, however, performance feedback was presented to flict paradigms, showed developmental stability of PES and few
signal errors. Jones et al. (2003) examined PES across a very limited studies, some using standard choice tasks, reported a developmental
age range (3–4 years) using a child-friendly go-nogo task. They decrease. Tentatively, we advanced the hypothesis that conflict
observed an age-related increase in PES. But the limited amount tasks induce greater caution that may obscure developmental differ-
of trials (i.e., 20 go vs. 20 nogo trials) and the huge differences in ences in PES. It would be of considerable interest to examine the
error proportions between age groups (i.e., 22, 76, 91%) presents interplay between micro- and macro speed-accuracy tradeoff (e.g.,
a serious challenge to the reliability of the developmental pattern Jentzsch & Leuthold, 2006; Wickelgren, 1977) in relation to develop-
of PES reported in this study. mental change in PES. Finally, on a methodological note, it should be
Two accounts, not mutually exclusive, might be offered for the added that trial numbers are important. A robust developmental
recurrent absence of developmental change in PES reported by trend in PES seems to require a substantial number of trials as evi-
studies using a conflict paradigm. One possibility would be that, denced by the current data (see Band, van der Molen, & Logan,
relative to standard choice tasks, errors are less easily detected in 2003, for a similar observation regarding the stop-signal paradigm).
conflict paradigms thereby obscuring developmental changes in The current results of Experiment 2 were consistent with previ-
PES. This possibility could be examined using a conflict paradigm ous reports showing that PES is most pronounced when RSIs are
in combination with an error-signaling requirement (e.g., short (e.g., Danielmeier & Ullsperger, 2011; Jentzsch & Dudschig,
Ullsperger et al., 2014). An alternative possibility would be that 2009). We suggested that errors elicit an orienting reaction
participants, when confronted with a conflict task, are more cau- (Notebaert et al., 2009) resulting in a short-lived de-activation of
tious than when asked to perform on a standard choice task. the motor system (e.g., Danielmeier, Eichele, Forstmann,
Enhanced caution has been observed by van de Laar et al. (2011) Tittgemeyer, & Ullsperger, 2011) contributing to PES at short RSIs.
when comparing RTs obtained using a standard choice task vs. It would be of interest to examine the temporal dynamics of the
RTs on go trials when stop-signal trials were inserted into the stan- de-activation of the motor system and its release using brain
dard choice task. The change in the macro speed-accuracy tradeoff potentials recorded over the primary motor cortex (van de Laar,
associated with conflict might obscure developmental changes in van den Wildenberg, van Boxtel, Huizenga, & van der Molen,
micro-speed accuracy tradeoff associated with performance moni- 2012). Although PES was smaller for the longer compared to the
toring. One notable exception, however, is provided by Schachar shorter RSI it was still substantial. Moreover, it should be noted
et al. (2004) who did obtain a developmental decrease in PES using that the RSI effect on PES did not differentiate between age groups.
a conflict task whereas other studies (see Table 1) did not, includ- Typically, PES observed for longer RSI’s is interpreted to result from
ing the one reported by van de Laar et al. (2011). We do not have a criterion adjustments (e.g., Dutilh et al., 2012; Jentzsch & Dudschig,
ready explanation for this apparent discrepancy. 2009; Laming, 1979); that is, the participant responds to an error
The other issue that needs to be addressed refers to the finding by increasing response thresholds on the subsequent trial so as
that PES can be observed in the absence of an ERN. Most studies, to ensure more accurate performance.
summarized in Table 1, observed that the ERN, but not PES, dis- Most importantly, the current results of Experiment 2 showed a
criminated between age groups. This observation seems to suggest disproportional decrease in PES with advancing age. This finding
that the mechanism thought to generate the ERN is not a conditio indicates that young children are capable of detecting their errors
sine qua non for PES to occur. Indeed, a lesion study showed alter- but suggests that they are less able to implement performance
ations in ERN while patients could still be aware of their errors adjustments. The current findings seem to suggest that it is not
(Stemmer, Segalowitz, Witzke, & Schonle, 2004). Furthermore, only a matter of efficiency, as the speed of PES recovery, indexed
Nieuwenhuis et al. (2001) reported, in typical participants, an by the speed of responding on the two trials following the post-
ERN on trials even when the error on those trials was not rated error trial, did not differ between age groups. The current findings
as erroneous. These and similar findings have been interpreted to suggest that when children encounter an error they have difficulty
suggest a dissociation between implicit vs. explicit error detection in regulating their speed of responding on the immediately subse-
(Ullsperger et al., 2014). With regard to development, it has been quent trial.
S.F.A. Smulders et al. / Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47 45

Some speculation is in order with regard to the putative brain Araujo, G. C., Christ, S. E., Grange, D. K., Steiner, R. D., Coleman, C., Timmerman, E.,
et al. (2013). Executive response monitoring and inhibitory control in children
mechanisms mediating the response to an error. Recently, Agam
with phenylketonuria: Effects of expectancy. Developmental Neuropsychology,
et al. (2013) examined the pattern of brain network activation over 38(3), 139–152.
a series of trials surrounding an error. They observed that Araujo, G. C., Christ, S. E., Steiner, R. D., Grange, D. K., Nardos, B., McKinstry, R. C.,
responses preceding an error and the error response itself were et al. (2009). Response monitoring in children with phenylketonuria.
Neuropsychology, 23(1), 130–134.
associated with activation of the brain default mode network that Arbel, Y., & Donchin, E. (2011). When a child errs: The ERN and the Pe complex in
has been linked to an internal processing focus (Buckner, Andrews- children. Psychophysiology, 48(1), 55–63.
Hanna, & Schacter, 2008) while the correct response following an Band, G. P. H., van der Molen, M. W., & Logan, G. D. (2003). Horse-race model
simulations of the stop-signal procedure. Acta Psychologica, 112, 105–142.
error was associated with the activation of the dorsal attention Berwid, O. G., Halperin, J. M., Johnson, R. E., Jr., & Marks, D. J. (2014). Preliminary
network that has been linked to effortful task-oriented attention evidence for reduced post-error reaction time slowing in hyperactive/
(Szczepanski, Pinsk, Douglas, Kastner, & Saalman, 2013). Interest- inattentive preschool children. Child Neuropsychology, 20, 196–209.
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict
ingly, the speed of performance adjustment following an error monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.
was correlated with greater microstructural integrity of the poste- Boucher, O., Burden, M. J., Muckle, G., Saint-Amour, D., Ayotte, P., Dewailly, E., et al.
rior cingulum cortex (PCC) suggesting its privileged position to (2012). Response inhibition and error monitoring during a visual go/no-go task
in inuit children exposed to lead, polychlorinated biphenyls, and
mediate network interactions around errors (cf. Agam et al., methylmercury. Environmental Health Perspectives, 120(4), 608–615.
2013). It was hypothesized that the PCC is a critical mechanism Brewer, N., & Smith, G. A. (1989). Developmental changes in processing speed:
mediating the balance of activity between the default mode and Influence of speed–accuracy regulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, 118, 298–310.
attention networks in response to others, and thereby reducing
Brooker, R. J., & Buss, K. A. (2014a). Harsh parenting and fearfulness in toddlerhood
internal focus, increasing task orientation resulting in more careful interact to predict amplitudes of preschool error-related negativity.
and thus slower responding to prevent future errors. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 148–159.
This brain-network framework might provide a useful, albeit Brooker, R. J., & Buss, K. A. (2014b). Toddler fearfulness is linked to individual
differences in error-related negativity during preschool. Developmental
speculative, account of the current pattern of findings. Our results Neuropsychology, 39(1), 1–8.
demonstrated that PES occurs in all age groups suggesting that Brooker, R. J., Buss, K. A., & Dennis, T. A. (2011). Error-monitoring brain activity is
children and adults do not differ in error detection. PES was larger associated with affective behaviors in young children. Developmental Cognitive
Neuroscience, 1(2), 141–152.
in children relative to adults suggesting that age groups differ in Bruce, J., McDermott, J. M., Fisher, P. A., & Fox, N. A. (2008). Using behavioral and
error adjustment. Within the context of brain networks interac- electrophysiological measures to assess the effects of a preventive intervention:
tions, slower error adjustment in children might arise from a diffi- A preliminary study with preschool-aged foster children. Prevention Science, 10
(2), 129–140.
culty to de-activate the default mode network and engage the task- Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default
oriented attention network. Recent studies of the functional archi- network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York
tecture of the maturing brain indicated that the negative correla- Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1–38.
Burgio-Murphy, A., Klorman, R., Shaywitz, S. E., Fletcher, J. M., Marchione, K. E.,
tion between default mode and task-oriented networks, typically Holahan, J., et al. (2007). Error-related event-related potentials in children with
seen in adult participants, is considerably less pronounced in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, reading
younger age groups (Chai, Ofen, Gabrieli, & Whitfield-Gabrieli, disorder, and math disorder. Biological Psychology, 75, 75–86.
Carrasco, M., Harbin, S. M., Nienhuis, J. K., Fitzgerald, K. D., Gehring, W. J., & Hanna,
2014). Furthermore, it has been observed that fractional aniso-
G. L. (2013). Increased error-related brain activity in youth with obsessive-
tropy, a putative measure of neural fiber coherence, diameter compulsive disorder and unaffected siblings. Depression and Anxiety, 30(1),
and myelination, of the cingulum bundle increase with advancing 39–46.
age in childhood and, importantly, is positively correlated with Carrasco, M., Hong, C., Nienhuis, J. K., Harbin, S. M., Fitzgerald, K. D., Gehring, W. J.,
et al. (2013). Increased error-related brain activity in youth with obsessive-
measures of executive functioning (Lebel et al., 2012; Peters compulsive disorder and other anxiety disorders. Neuroscience Letters, 541,
et al., 2014). Collectively, these findings may suggest that matura- 214–218.
tion of the PCC plays a pivotal role in the ability to adjust perfor- Chai, X. J., Ofen, N., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. (2014). Development of
deactivation oft he default-mode network during episodic memory formation.
mance in response to an error. It would be of considerable Neuroimage, 84, 932–938.
interest to apply a brain network analysis to developmental change Crone, E. A. (2014). The role oft he medial frontal cortex in the development of
in PES in order to obtain a more detailed view on the mechanisms cognitive and social-affective performance monitoring. Psychophysiology, 51,
943–950.
mediating the child’s exaggerated response to an error. Danielmeier, C., Eichele, T., Forstmann, B. U., Tittgemeyer, M., & Ullsperger, M.
(2011). Posterior medial frontal cortex activity predicts post-error adaptations
in task-related visual and motor areas. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1780–1789.
Acknowledgments
Danielmeier, C., & Ullsperger, M. (2011). Post-error adjustments. Frontiers in
Psychology, 2, 233.
This research was supported by an NWO-MaGW grant (490-22- Davies, P. L., Segalowitz, S. J., & Gavin, W. J. (2004a). Development of error-
monitoring event-related potentials in adolescents. In R. E. Dahl & L. P. Spear
218) from the Netherlands Science Organization. The technical
(Eds.), Adolescent brain development: Vulnerabilities and opportunities (Vol. 1021,
assistance of Bert van Beek was greatly appreciated. Furthermore, pp. 324–328).
we gratefully acknowledge Sander Dalenberg, Nathalie van der Davies, P. L., Segalowitz, S. J., & Gavin, W. J. (2004b). Development of response-
Kamp, Merel Kraan, Crista van Leijen, and Peggy Samson for their monitoring ERPs in 7-to 25-year-olds. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25(3),
355–376.
help in collecting a part of the data. The support of the Netherlands Dudschig, C., & Jentzsch, I. (2009). Speeding before and slowing after errors: Is it all
Institute for Advanced Studies in the Humanities and Social strategy? Brain Research, 1296, 56–62.
Sciences (NIAS) is also gratefully acknowledged. Dutilh, G., Vandekerckhove, J., Forstmann, B. U., Keuleers, E., Brysbaert, M., &
Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2012). Testing theories of post-error slowing. Attention,
Perception & Psychophysics, 74, 454–465.
References Eppinger, B., Mock, B., & Kray, J. (2009). Developmental differences in learning and
error processing: Evidence from ERPs. Psychophysiology, 46(5), 1043–1053.
Fairweather, H. (1978). Choice reaction times in children: Error and post-error
Agam, Y., Carey, C., Barton, J. J., Dyckman, K. A., Lee, A. K., Vangel, M., et al. (2013).
responses, and the repetition effect. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 26,
Network dynamics underlying speed–accuracy trade-offs in response to errors.
407–418.
PLoS ONE, 8, e73692.
Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoormann, J., & Blanke, L. (1995). Effects of
Albrecht, B., Brandeis, D., Uebel, H., Heinrich, H., Mueller, U. C., Hasselhorn, M., et al.
crossmodal divided attention on late ERP components II. Error processing in
(2008). Action monitoring in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder,
choice reaction tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 78,
their non-affected siblings, and normal control subjects: Evidence for an
447–455.
endophenotype. Biological Psychiatry, 64(7), 615–625.
Ferdinand, N. K., & Kray, J. (2014). Developmental changes in performance
Albrecht, B., Heinrich, H., Brandeis, D., Uebel, H., Yordanova, J., Kolev, V., et al.
monitoring: How electrophysiological data can enhance our understanding of
(2009). Flanker-task in children time-frequency analyses of response
monitoring. Journal of Psychophysiology, 23(4), 183–190.
46 S.F.A. Smulders et al. / Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47

error and feedback processing in childhood and adolescence. Behavioural Brain Laming, D. (1979). Choice reaction performance following an error. Acta
Research, 263, 122–132. Psychologica, 43, 199–224.
Fitzgerald, K. D., Zbrozek, C. D., Welsh, R. C., Britton, J. C., Liberzon, I., & Taylor, S. F. Laurens, K. R., Hodgins, S., Mould, G. L., West, S. A., Schoenberg, P. L. A., Murray, R.
(2010). Pilot study of response inhibition and error processing in the posterior M., et al. (2010). Error-related processing dysfunction in children aged 9 to 12
medial prefrontal cortex in healthy youth. Journal of Child Psychology and years presenting putative antecedents of schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 67
Psychiatry, 49(9), 986–994. (3), 238–245.
Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural Lebel, C., Gee, M., Camicioli, R., Wieler, M., Martin, W., & Beaulieu, C. (2012).
system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4, 385–390. Diffusion tensor imaging of white matter tract evolution over the lifespan.
Groen, Y., Wijers, A. A., Mulder, L. J., Waggeveld, B., Minderaa, R. B., & Althaus, M. Neuroimage, 60(1), 340–352.
(2008). Error and feedback processing in children with ADHD and children with Liotti, M., Pliszka, S. R., Perez, R., Kothmann, D., & Woldorff, M. G. (2005). Abnormal
Autistic Spectrum Disorder: An EEG event-related potential study. Clinical brain activity related to performance monitoring and error detection in children
Neurophysiology, 119(11), 2476–2493. with ADHD. Cortex, 41(3), 377–388.
Groom, M. J., Cahill, J. D., Bates, A. T., Jackson, G. M., Calton, T. G., Liddle, P. F., et al. Luna, B., Marek, S., Larsen, B., Tervo-Clemmens, B., & Chahal, R. (2015). An
(2010). Electrophysiological indices of abnormal error-processing in integrative model of the maturation of cognitive control. Annual Review of
adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Neuroscience, 38, 151–170.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(1), 66–76. Lynn, R. (1966). Attention, arousal, and the orientation reaction. Oxford: Pergamon
Groom, M. J., Liddle, E. B., Scerif, G., Liddle, P. F., Batty, M. J., Liotti, M., et al. (2013). Press.
Motivational incentives and methylphenidate enhance electrophysiological Lyons, K. E., & Zelazo, P. D. (2011). Monitoring, metacognition, and executive
correlates of error monitoring in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity function: Elucidating the role of self-reflection in the development of self-
disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(8), 836–845. regulation. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 40, 379–412.
Gupta, R., Kar, B. R., & Srinivasan, N. (2009). Development of task switching and McDermott, J. M., Perez-Edgar, K., Henderson, H. A., Chronis-Tuscano, A., Pine, D. S.,
post-error slowing in children. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 5, 38. & Fox, N. A. (2009). A history of childhood behavioral inhibition and enhanced
Hajcak, G., Franklin, M. E., Foa, E. B., & Simons, R. F. (2008). Increased error-related response monitoring in adolescence are linked to clinical anxiety. Biological
brain activity in pediatric obsessive–compulsive disorder before and after Psychiatry, 65(5), 445–448.
treatment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 116–123. McDermott, J. M., Troller-Renfree, S., Vanderwert, R., Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H., &
Hanna, G. L., Carrasco, M., Harbin, S. M., Nienhuis, J. K., LaRosa, C. E., Chen, P., et al. Fox, N. A. (2013). Psychosocial deprivation, executive functions, and the
(2012). Error-related negativity and tic history in pediatric obsessive- emergence of socio-emotional behavior problems. Frontiers in Human
compulsive disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Neuroscience, 7.
Psychiatry, 51(9), 902–910. Meyer, A. M., Klein, D. N., Torpey, D. C., Kujawa, A. J., Hayden, E. P., Sheikh, H. I., et al.
Henderson, H., Schwartz, C., Mundy, P., Burnette, C., Sutton, S., Zahka, N., et al. (2012). The additive effects of two dopamine genotypes on the error-related
(2006). Response monitoring, the error-related negativity, and differences in negativity (ERN) in children. Psychophysiology, 49. S57–S57.
social behavior in autism. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 96–109. Moon, J., Beaudin, A. E., Verosky, S., Driscoll, L. L., Weiskopf, M., Levitsky, D. A., et al.
Hillman, C. H., Buck, S. M., Themanson, J. R., Pontifex, M. B., & Castelli, D. M. (2009). (2006). Attentional dysfunction, impulsivity, and resistance to change in a
Aerobic fitness and cognitive development: Event-related brain potential and mouse model of fragile X syndrome. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120, 1367–1379.
task performance indices of executive control in preadolescent children. Narayanan, N. S., Cavanagh, J. F., Frank, M. J., & Laubach, M. (2013). Common medial
Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 114–129. frontal mechanisms of adaptive control in humans and rodents. Nature
Hogan, A. M., Vargha-Khadem, F., Kirkham, F. J., & Baldeweg, T. (2005). Maturation Neuroscience, 16, 1888–1895.
of action monitoring from adolescence to adulthood: An ERP study. Narayanan, N. S., & Laubach, M. (2008). Neuronal correlates of post-error slowing in
Developmental Science, 8(6), 525–534. the rate dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100,
Horowitz-Kraus, T. (2011). Does development affect the error-related negativity of 520–525.
impaired and skilled readers? An ERP study, Developmental Neuropsychology, 36 Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Blom, J., Band, G. P. H., & Kok, A. (2001). Error-
(7), 914–932. related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response
Horowitz-Kraus, T., & Breznitz, Z. (2014). Can reading rate acceleration improve errors: Evidence from an antisaccade task. Psychophysiology, 38, 752–760.
error monitoring and cognitive abilities underlying reading in adolescents with Notebaert, W., Houtman, F., Opstal, F. V., Gevers, W., Fias, W., & Verguts, T. (2009).
reading difficulties and in typical readers? Brain Research, 1544, 1–14. Post-error slowing: An orienting account. Cognition, 111, 275–279.
Hum, K. M., Manassis, K., & Lewis, M. D. (2013). Neural mechanisms of emotion O’Connell, R. G., Bellgrove, M. A., Dockree, P. M., & Robertson, I. H. (2004). Reduced
regulation in childhood anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(5), electrodermal response to errors predicts poor sustained attention performance
552–564. in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuroreport, 15(16), 2535–2538.
Ito, S., Stuphorn, V., Brown, J. W., & Schall, J. D. (2003). Performance monitoring by Ornstein, T. J., Levin, H. S., Chen, S., Hanten, G., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Dennis, M., et al.
the anterior cingulate cortex during saccade countermanding. Science, 302, (2009). Performance monitoring in children following traumatic brain injury.
120–122. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(4), 506–513.
Jedema, H. P., Carter, M. D., Dugan, B. P., Gurnsey, K., Olsen, A. S., & Bradberry, C. W. Overbeek, T. J. M., Nieuwenhuis, S., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Dissociable
(2011). The acute impact of ethanol on cognitive performance in rhesus components of error processing. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19, 319–329.
macaques. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 1783–1791. Peters, B. D., Voineskos, A. N., Szeszko, P. R., Lett, T. A., DeRosse, P., Guha, S., et al.
Jentzsch, I., & Dudschig, C. (2009). Why do we slow down after an error? (2014). Brain white matter development is associated with a human-specific
Mechanisms underlying the effects of post-error slowing. The Quarterly haplotype increasing the synthesis of long chain Fatty acids. The Journal of
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 209–218. Neuroscience, 34(18), 6367–6376.
Jentzsch, I., & Leuthold, H. (2006). Control over speeded actions: A common Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Johnson, C. R., Chaddock, L., Voss, M. W., Cohen, N. J.,
processing locus for micro- and macro-tradeoff effects? Quarterly Journal of et al. (2011). Cardiorespiratory fitness and the flexible modulation of cognitive
Experimental Psychology, 59, 1329–1337. control in preadolescent children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(6),
Jones, L. B., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Development of executive 1332–1345.
attention in preschool children. Developmental Science, 6(5), 498–504. Pontifex, M. B., Scudder, M. R., Brown, M. L., O’Leary, K. C., Wu, C.-T., Themanson, J.
Jonkman, L. M., van Melis, J. J. M., Kemner, C., & Markus, C. R. (2007). R., et al. (2010). On the number of trials necessary for stabilization of error-
Methylphenidate improves deficient error evaluation in children with ADHD: related brain activity across the life span. Psychophysiology, 47(4), 767–773.
An event-related brain potential study. Biological Psychology, 76(3), 217–229. Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1966). Errors and error correction in choice reaction tasks. Journal
Kamijo, K., Pontifex, M. B., Khan, N. A., Raine, L. B., Scudder, M. R., Drollette, E. S., of Experimental Psychology, 71, 264–272.
et al. (2014). The negative association of childhood obesity to cognitive control Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1968). 3 kinds of error-signaling responses in a serial choice task.
of action monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 24(3), 654–662. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 179–188.
Kim, E. Y., Iwaki, N., Imashioya, H., Uno, H., & Fujita, T. (2007). Error-related Rabbitt, P. M. A., & Rodgers, B. (1977). What does a man do after he makes an error?
negativity in a visual go/no-go task: Children vs. adults. Developmental An analysis of response programming. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Neuropsychology, 31, 181–191. Psychology, 29, 727–743.
Ladouceur, C. D., Dahl, R. E., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D. A., & Ryan, N. D. (2006). Rabbitt, P. M. A., & Vyas, S. M. (1970). An elementary preliminary taxonomy for
Increased error-related negativity (ERN) in childhood anxiety disorders: ERP some errors in laboratory choice RT tasks. Acta Psychologica, 33, 56–76.
and source localization. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(10), Richardson, C., Anderson, M., Reid, C. L., & Fox, A. M. (2011). Neural indicators of
1073–1082. error processing and intraindividual variability in reaction time in 7 and 9 year-
Ladouceur, C. D., Dhal, R. E., & Carter, C. S. (2007). Development of action monitoring olds. Developmental Psychobiology, 53(3), 256–265.
through adolescent into adulthood: EPR and source localization. Developmental Ridderinkhof, K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Segalowitz, S. J., & Carter, C. S.
Science, 10, 874–891. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal
Ladouceur, C. D., Dahl, R. E., & Carter, C. S. (2004). ERP correlates of action cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and
monitoring in adolescence. In R. E. Dahl & L. P. Spear (Eds.), Adolescent brain reward-based learning. Brain & Cognition, 56, 129–140.
development: Vulnerabilities and opportunities (Vol. 1021, pp. 329–336). Rosch, K. S., & Hawk, L. W. Jr., (2013). The effects of performance-based rewards on
Ladouceur, C. D., Slifka, J. S., Dahl, R. E., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D. A., & Ryan, N. D. neurophysiological correlates of stimulus, error, and feedback processing in
(2012). Altered error-related brain activity in youth with major depression. children with ADHD. Psychophysiology, 50(11), 1157–1173.
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(3), 351–362. Rubia, K., Halari, R., Mohammad, A.-M., Taylor, E., & Brammer, M. (2011).
Laming, D. (1968). Information theory of choice-reaction times. London: Academic Methylphenidate normalizes frontocingulate underactivation during error
press.
S.F.A. Smulders et al. / Brain and Cognition 104 (2016) 34–47 47

processing in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 70 Torpey, D. C., Hajcak, G., & Klein, D. N. (2009). An examination of error-related brain
(3), 255–262. activity and its modulation by error value in young children. Developmental
Santesso, D. L., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2008). Development of error-related ERPs in Neuropsychology, 34(6), 749–761.
middle to late adolescent males. Developmental Psychology, 44, 205–217. Ullsperger, M., Danielmeier, C., & Jocham, G. (2014). Neurophysiology of
Santesso, D. L., Segalowitz, S. J., & Schmidt, L. A. (2006). Error-related electrocortical performance monitoring. Physiological Reviews, 94, 35–79.
responses in 10-year-old children and young adults. Developmental Science, 9, Ursin, H. (2005). Press stop to start: The role of inhibition for choice and health.
473–481. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 1059–1065.
Schachar, R. J., Chen, S., Logan, G. D., Ornstein, T. J., Crosbie, J., Ickowicz, A., et al. van de Laar, M. C., van den Wildenberg, W. P., van Boxtel, G. J., Huizenga, H. M., &
(2004). Evidence for an error monitoring deficit in attention deficit van der Molen, M. W. (2012). Lifespan changes in motor activation and
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32(3), 285–293. inhibition during choice reactions: A Laplacian ERP study. Biological Psychology,
Senderecka, M., Grabowska, A., Szewczyk, J., Gerc, K., & Chmylak, R. (2012). 89, 323–334.
Response inhibition of children with ADHD in the stop-signal task: An event- van de Laar, M. C., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., van Boxtel, G. J. M., & van der
related potential study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 85(1), 93–105. Molen, M. W. (2011). Lifespan changes in global and selective stopping and
Shen, I. H., Tsai, S.-Y., & Duann, J.-R. (2011). Inhibition control and error processing performance adjustments. Frontiers in Psychology, 2.
in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: An event-related van de Voorde, S., Roeyers, H., & Wiersema, J. R. (2010). Error monitoring in children
potentials study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 81(1), 1–11. with ADHD or reading disorder: An event-related potential study. Biological
Shiels, K., Tamm, L., & Epstein, J. N. (2012). Deficient post-error slowing in children Psychology, 84(2), 176–185.
with ADHD is limited to the inattentive subtype. Journal of the International van der Molen, M. W. (2000). Developmental changes in inhibitory processing:
Neuropsychological Society, 18(3), 612–617. Evidence from psychophysiological measures. Biological Psychology, 54,
Smulders, S. F. A., Notebaert, W., Meijer, M., Crone, E. A., van der Molen, M. W., & 207–239.
Soetens, E. (2005). Sequential effects on speeded information processing: A van Meel, C. S., Heslenfeld, D. J., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2007). Adaptive
developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 90, 208–234. control deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): The role of
Sokhadze, E. M., Baruth, J. M., Sears, L., Sokhadze, G. E., El-Baz, A. S., & Casanova, M. error processing. Psychiatry Research, 151(3), 211–220.
F. (2012). Prefrontal neuromodulation using rTMS improves error monitoring van Meel, C. S., Heslenfeld, D. J., Rommelse, N. N., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A.
and correction function in autism. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 37 (2012). Developmental trajectories of neural mechanisms supporting conflict
(2), 91–102. and error processing in middle childhood. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37
Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the conditioned reflex. Oxford: Pergamon Press. (4), 358–378.
South, M., Larson, M. J., Krauskopf, E., & Clawson, A. (2010). Error processing in high- Vlamings, P. H. J. M., Jonkman, L. M., Hoeksma, M. R., van Engeland, H., & Kemner, C.
functioning Autism Spectrum Disorders. Biological Psychology, 85(2), 242–251. (2008). Reduced error monitoring in children with autism spectrum disorder:
Spinelli, S., Vasa, R. A., Joel, S., Nelson, T. E., Pekar, J. J., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2011). An ERP study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 28(2), 399–406.
Variability in post-error behavioral adjustment is associated with functional Welford, A. T. (1980). Choice reaction time: Basic concepts. In A. T. Welford (Ed.),
abnormalities in the temporal cortex in children with ADHD. Journal of Child Reaction times (pp. 73–128). New York: Academic Press.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(7), 808–816. Wickelgren, W. A. (1977). Speed-accuracy tradeoff and information processing
Stemmer, B., Segalowitz, S. J., Witzke, W., & Schonle, P. W. (2004). Error detection in dynamics. Acta Psychologica, 41, 67–85.
patients with lesions to the medial prefrontal cortex: An ERP study. Wiersema, J. R., van der Meere, J. J., & Roeyers, H. (2005). ERP correlates of impaired
Neuropsychologia, 42, 118–130. error monitoring in children with ADHD. Journal of Neural Transmission, 112(10),
Stieben, J., Lewis, M. D., Granic, I., Zelazo, P. D., Segalowitz, S., & Pepler, D. (2007). 1417–1430.
Neurophysiological mechanisms of emotion regulation for subtypes of Wiersema, J. R., van der Meere, J. J., & Roeyers, H. (2007). Developmental changes in
externalizing children. Development and Psychopathology, 19(2), 455–480. error monitoring: An event-related potential study. Neuropsychologia, 45(8),
Szczepanski, S. M., Pinsk, M. A., Douglas, M. M., Kastner, S., & Saalman, Y. B. (2013). 1649–1657.
Functional and structural architecture of the human dorsal frontoparietal Wild-Wall, N., Oades, R. D., Schmidt-Wessels, M., Christiansen, H., & Falkenstein, M.
attention network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(39), (2009). Neural activity associated with executive functions in adolescents with
15806–15811. attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). International Journal of
Tamnes, C. K., Walhovd, K. B., Torstveit, M., Sells, V. T., & Fjell, A. M. (2013). Psychophysiology, 74(1), 19–27.
Performance monitoring in children and adolescents: A review of Yordanova, J., Kolev, V., Albrecht, B., Uebel, H., Banaschewski, T., & Rothenberger, A.
developmental changes in the error-related negativity and brain maturation. (2011). May posterror performance be a critical factor for behavioral deficits in
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 6, 1–13. attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Biological Psychiatry, 70(3), 246–254.
Torpey, D. C., Hajcak, G., Kim, J., Kujawa, A. J., Dyson, M. W., Olino, T. M., et al. (2013). Zhang, J. S., Wang, Y., Cai, R. G., & Yan, C. H. (2009). The brain regulation mechanism
Error-related brain activity in young children: Associations with parental of error monitoring in impulsive children with ADHD – An analysis of error
anxiety and child temperamental negative emotionality. Journal of Child related potentials. Neuroscience Letters, 460(1), 11–15.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(8), 854–862.
Torpey, D. C., Hajcak, G., Kim, J., Kujawa, A., & Klein, D. N. (2012). Electrocortical and
behavioral measures of response monitoring in young children during a Go/No-
Go task. Developmental Psychobiology, 54(2), 139–150.

You might also like