You are on page 1of 3

Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation

journal homepage: www.archives-pmr.org


Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 2013;94:2562-4

BRIEF REPORT

Education Attenuates the Negative Impact of Traumatic


Brain Injury on Cognitive Status
James F. Sumowski, PhD,a,b Nancy Chiaravalloti, PhD,a,b,c Denise Krch, PhD,b,c
Jessica Paxton, PhD,a,b John DeLuca, PhDa,b,d
From the aNeuropsychology and Neuroscience, Kessler Foundation, West Orange, NJ; bDepartment of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation,
RutgerseNew Jersey Medical School, Newark, NJ; cTraumatic Brain Injury Laboratory, Kessler Foundation, West Orange, NJ; and dDepartment
of Neurology and Neurosciences, RutgerseNew Jersey Medical School, Newark, NJ.

Abstract
Objective: To investigate whether the cognitive reserve hypothesis helps to explain differential cognitive impairment among survivors of
traumatic brain injury (TBI), whereby survivors with greater intellectual enrichment (estimated with education) are less vulnerable to cognitive
impairment.
Design: Cross-sectional study.
Setting: Medical rehabilitation research center.
Participants: Survivors of moderate or severe TBI (nZ44) and healthy controls (nZ36).
Interventions: Not applicable.
Main Outcome Measures: Intellectual enrichment was estimated with educational attainment. Group was defined as TBI or healthy control.
Current cognitive status (processing speed, working memory, episodic memory) was evaluated with neuropsychological tasks.
Results: TBI survivors exhibited worse cognitive status than healthy persons (P<.001), and education was positively correlated with cognitive status
in TBI survivors (rZ.54, P<.001). Most importantly, regression analysis revealed an interaction between group and education (R2 changeZ.036,
PZ.004), whereas higher education attenuated the negative impact of TBI on cognitive status. TBI survivors with lower education performed
much worse than matched healthy persons, but this TBI-related performance discrepancy was attenuated at higher levels of education.
Conclusions: Higher intellectual enrichment (estimated with education) reduces the negative effect of TBI on cognitive outcomes, thereby
supporting the cognitive reserve hypothesis in persons with TBI. Future work is necessary to investigate whether intellectual enrichment can build
cognitive reserve as a rehabilitative intervention in survivors of TBI.
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 2013;94:2562-4
ª 2013 by the American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine

More than 200,000 survivors of moderate and severe traumatic cognitive outcomes. This inconsistent relation between neurologic
brain injury (TBI) are discharged from American hospitals insult and cognitive outcome is observed across neurologic pop-
annually.1 Many of these survivors will suffer chronic cognitive ulations, including Alzheimer disease (AD). For instance, many
impairment,2 with negative consequences for quality of life.3 older adults without dementia meet the neuropathologic criteria
Cognitive outcomes vary across survivors with similar injuries,2 for probable or definite AD at autopsy.4 This cognitive-pathologic
such that 2 persons with similar injuries often have discrepant discrepancy is explained in part by the cognitive reserve hypoth-
esis, which posits that higher lifetime intellectual enrichment
Supported by the New Jersey Commission on Brain Injury Research (grant no. (often estimated with education) protects against disease-related
CBIR11PJT020); the National Institute on Disability and Rehabilitation Research (grant no. cognitive decline.5 Indeed, high education protects against
H133A070037); and a fellowship training grant from the Department of Education, National
Institute on Disability and Rehabilitation Research (grant no. H133P090009).
dementia, despite substantial AD neuropathology.6,7 The cognitive
The content of this article does not necessarily represent the policy of the Department of reserve hypothesis has been extended to other neurologic diseases,
Education, and one should not assume endorsement by the federal government. including multiple sclerosis8,9 and pediatric10 and adult TBI.11,12
No commercial party having a direct financial interest in the results of the research supporting
this article has conferred or will confer a benefit on the authors or on any organization with which
Previous work with adult survivors of TBI has primarily shown
the authors are associated. positive correlations between intellectual enrichment and

0003-9993/13/$36 - see front matter ª 2013 by the American Congress of Rehabilitation Medicine
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.apmr.2013.07.023
Cognitive reserve in TBI 2563

cognitive outcomes (ie, a main effect),11 but herein we evaluate Results


the cognitive reserve hypothesis in TBI by investigating whether
educational attainment differentially impacts cognitive impair- Cognitive status was lower among TBI survivors ( .70.72) than
ment after TBI (ie, an interaction effect). healthy persons (.85.50, t78Z10.92, P<.001). Significant (all
Ps<.001) raw score differences also existed for each of the 3
Methods cognitive scores contributing to the cognitive status composite:
Symbol Digit Modalities Test (healthy: 64.399.76; TBI:
37.211.5), Letter-Number Sequencing (healthy: 12.442.57;
Participants
TBI: 9.252.3), and Selective Reminding Test (healthy:
141.445.7; TBI: 113.2723).
The sample included 44 survivors of moderate (nZ3) or severe The full regression model was significant (F5,74Z10.91,
(nZ41) TBI (mean age  SD, 39.310.6y; 12 women) and 36 P<.001, R2Z.691). After controlling for age, sex, group, and
healthy controls (mean age  SD, 43.711.3y; 24 women). education, there was an interaction between group and education
Survivors were at least 1 year postinjury (median, 5.9y; mean  (R2 changeZ.036, PZ.004). TBI survivors with lower education
SD, 9.07.1y). Glasgow Coma Scale data were only available for performed worse than healthy persons, but this TBI-related
a subset of TBI survivors (nZ20; median, 4; mean  SD, performance discrepancy was attenuated at higher levels of
5.22.9). The TBI sample was marginally younger (t78Z1.79, education (fig 1A).
PZ.078), with a higher proportion of men (72.7% vs 33.3%; The cognitive reserve hypothesis predicts that intellectual
c2Z12.42, P<.001). Age and sex were controlled in subsequent enrichment will be more related to cognitive status within
analyses. The Kessler Foundation Institutional Review Board neurologic samples than healthy persons. This is because among
approved data collection, and written informed consent was ob- healthy persons, there are no deleterious effects of neurologic
tained from all subjects. disease processes on cognitive status for intellectual enrichment to
protect against. Indeed, there was a strong positive relation
Cognitive reserve between education and cognitive status among TBI survivors
(rZ.538, P<.001), but there was no relation among healthy
Lifetime intellectual enrichment was estimated with educational persons (rZ.035, PZ.841). We provide the raw data demon-
attainment (healthy controls: 14.771.93y; TBI: 13.841.94y).5 strating this divergent relation (fig 1B).
Educational attainment was unrelated to time since injury
(rZ.074) or Glasgow Coma Scale scores (rZ.093) for TBI Discussion
survivors. As such, any protective effects of intellectual enrich-
ment are not explained/confounded by any chance relation
Higher education attenuated the deleterious effect of moderate to
between educational attainment and injury severity.
severe TBI on cognitive status, thereby supporting the cognitive
reserve hypothesis in TBI. This finding helps to explain the variable
Cognitive status cognitive outcomes across TBI survivors with similar injuries2
because survivors with higher education can sustain a given injury
The most common cognitive deficits among TBI survivors include with lesser cognitive decline than a person with less education. Our
slowed processing speed, poor working memory, and episodic findings extend earlier work linking higher premorbid intelligence
memory impairment.3 We assessed processing speed with the (another proxy of cognitive reserve) with better cognitive perfor-
Symbol Digit Modalities Test (oral version), working memory mance in adult survivors of TBI11 and lesser cognitive decline after
with Letter-Number Sequencing, and episodic memory with the penetrating head injuries12; however, we add to the literature by
Selective Reminding Test (total learning). Performance on each directly testing the cognitive reserve hypothesis with an interaction
measure was entered into a principal components analysis to between TBI and intellectual enrichment (education).
create a single cognitive status score used in subsequent analyses
(71.8% of variance). Study limitations

Statistical analysis Although limited by a relatively small sample size, our study
provides a proof of concept for cognitive reserve in TBI. The
We tested if intellectual enrichment (estimated with education) small sample size may limit the generalizability of our findings
moderates the deleterious effect of TBI on cognitive status because our small samples may not adequately represent the
(interaction effect) by performing a hierarchical regression pre- populations of healthy persons and survivors of TBI. For instance,
dicting cognitive status, with age, sex, group, and education in we confirmed our hypothesis that educational attainment would be
step 1, and the interaction between group and education in step 2. more related to cognitive status in survivors of TBI than in healthy
We expected a group by education interaction, where TBI survi- persons; however, one might expect at least a small positive
vors with lower education would show cognitive deficits relative correlation between education and cognitive status in healthy
to healthy persons, but such deficits would be attenuated in persons. This is one way in which our healthy sample may differ
patients with higher education. from the general population. Another limitation relates to our sole
use of educational attainment as an estimate of intellectual
List of abbreviations: enrichment because the construct validity of education as an
independent contributor to reserve has been questioned,9,13 in part
AD Alzheimer disease
because of the correlation between education and other proxies of
TBI traumatic brain injury
reserve (ie, intelligence). Finally, similar to work in AD6,7 and

www.archives-pmr.org
2564 J.F. Sumowski et al

help identify TBI survivors at greatest risk of cognitive impair-


ment (ie, survivors with lower cognitive reserve), who can then be
targeted for early intervention cognitive rehabilitation. Also,
encouraging survivors to engage in intellectually enriching
activities may help protect against cognitive decline, although
future experimental work is necessary to draw a causal link
between enrichment and neurocognitive protection.

Keywords
Brain injuries; Cognitive disorders; Cognitive reserve;
Neuropsychology; Rehabilitation

Corresponding author

James F. Sumowski, PhD, Neuropsychology and Neuroscience,


Kessler Foundation Research Center, 300 Executive Dr, Ste
70, West Orange, NJ 07052. E-mail address: jsumowski@
kesslerfoundation.org.

References

1. Hirtz D, Thurman DJ, Gwinn-Hardy K, Mohamed M, Chaudhuri AR,


Zalutsky R. How common are the “common” neurologic disorders?
Neurology 2007;68:326-37.
2. Millis SR, Rosenthal M, Novack TA, et al. Long-term neuro-
psychological outcome after traumatic brain injury. J Head Trauma
Rehabil 2001;16:343-55.
3. Hoofien D, Gilboa A, Vakil E, Donovick PJ. Traumatic brain injury
(TBI) 10-20 years later: a comprehensive outcome study of psychiatric
symptomatology, cognitive abilities and psychosocial functioning.
Brain Inj 2001;15:189-209.
4. Bennett DA, Schneider JA, Arvanitakis Z, et al. Neuropathology of
older persons without cognitive impairment from two community-
based studies. Neurology 2006;66:1837-44.
5. Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of
the reserve concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002;8:448-60.
6. Roe CM, Mintun MA, D’Angelo G, Xiong C, Grant EA, Morris JC.
Alzheimer disease and cognitive reserve: variation of education effect
with carbon 11-labeled Pittsburgh Compound B uptake. Arch Neurol
2008;65:1467-71.
7. Bennett DA, Wilson RS, Schneider JA, et al. Education modifies the
relation of AD pathology to level of cognitive function in older
persons. Neurology 2003;60:1909-15.
Fig 1 Plot of the interaction between group (TBI, healthy) and 8. Sumowski JF, Wylie GR, Chiaravalloti N, DeLuca J. Intellectual
enrichment lessens the effect of brain atrophy on learning and memory
educational attainment on the cognitive status component score.
in multiple sclerosis. Neurology 2010;74:1942-5.
Solid line indicates TBI group; dashed line, healthy control group (A).
9. Sumowski JF, Rocca MA, Leavitt VM, et al. Brain reserve and
Scatterplot of the relation between years of educational attainment cognitive reserve in multiple sclerosis: what you’ve got and how you
and cognitive status for TBI patients and healthy persons. Red indi- use it. Neurology 2013;80:2186-93.
cates TBI group; blue, health control group (B). 10. Farmer JE, Kanne SM, Haut JS, Williams J, Johnstone B, Kirk K.
Memory functioning following traumatic brain injury in children
multiple sclerosis,8,9 future research should investigate whether with premorbid learning problems. Dev Neuropsychol 2002;22:
intellectual enrichment moderates/attenuates the negative effect of 455-69.
disease-related neuropathologic changes (ie, cerebral atrophy) on 11. Green RE, Colella B, Christensen B, et al. Examining moderators of
cognitive recovery trajectories after moderate to severe traumatic brain
cognitive outcomes in TBI survivors.
injury. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2008;89(12 Suppl):S16-24.
12. Raymont V, Greathouse A, Reding K, Lipsky R, Salazar A, Grafman J.
Demographic, structural and genetic predictors of late cognitive
Conclusions decline after penetrating head injury. Brain 2008;131:543-58.
13. Satz P, Cole MA, Hardy DJ, Rassovsky Y. Brain and cognitive reserve:
Ultimately, consideration of lifetime intellectual enrichment mediator(s) and construct validity, a critique. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
through proxies, such as education or vocabulary knowledge, may 2011;33:121-30.

www.archives-pmr.org

You might also like