You are on page 1of 65

 

The  Power  of  Music:    


The  Composition  and  Perception  of  Emotion  in  Melody  
 
by  
 
Megan  Trenck,  B.M.  
 
A  Thesis  
 
In  
 
Music  Theory  
 
Submitted  to  the  Graduate  Faculty  
of  Texas  Tech  University  in  
Partial  Fulfillment  of  
the  Requirements  for  
the  Degree  of  
 
Master  of  Music  
 
Approved  
 
 
 
Dr.  Peter  Martens  
Chair  of  Committee  
 
 
 
Dr.  Michael  Berry  
 
 
 
Dr.  Jeff  Larsen  
 
 
 
Peggy  Gordon  Miller  
Dean  of  the  Graduate  School  
 
 
August,  2011  

 
 

 
 

©  Copyright  2011,  Megan  Trenck.  


 

 
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Acknowledgments  

I  would  first  like  to  thank  the  faculty  of  the  Music  Department  at  Texas  Tech  

University  for  the  continuous  support  and  inspiration  provided  throughout  my  

education.  I  give  special  thanks  to  Dr.  Peter  Martens  for  his  time  and  effort  

motivating  me  to  explore  the  field  of  music  and  emotion.  I  would  like  to  thank  Dr.  

Michael  Berry  for  first  introducing  me  to  the  field  of  music  cognition.  Thank  you  

both,  Dr.  Martens  and  Dr.  Berry,  for  being  supportive  mentors.  Also,  I  would  like  to  

thank  Dr.  Jeff  Larsen  for  his  assistance  in  the  statistical  analyses  and  for  providing  

insight  to  the  perception  of  emotion  in  music.  Additional  and  invaluable  thanks  go  

out  to  my  family  and  friends,  especially  my  mother  who  always  encouraged  me  to  

reach  higher  in  order  to  fulfill  my  dreams.  

ii  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Table  of  Contents  

 
Acknowledgments .................................................................................................................. ii  

Abstract .......................................................................................................................................v  

List  of  Tables............................................................................................................................ vi  

List  of  Figures ......................................................................................................................... vii  

I.  Introduction .......................................................................................................................... 1  

II.  Experiment  1 ....................................................................................................................... 8  

Method ................................................................................................................................. 8  

Participants.................................................................................................................. 8  

Procedure ..................................................................................................................... 9  

Results  and  Discussion ................................................................................................... 9  

Emotion/Mode  Correlation .................................................................................... 9  

First-­Order  Intervals...............................................................................................10  

Melodic  Spans............................................................................................................14  

Note  Density...............................................................................................................17  

Melodic  Arches..........................................................................................................17  

Post-­Skip  Reversal  and  Regression  to  the  Mean ...........................................20  

III.  Experiment  2 ....................................................................................................................24  

Method ...............................................................................................................................24  

Participants................................................................................................................24  

Stimuli..........................................................................................................................24  

Apparatus ...................................................................................................................25  
iii  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Procedure ...................................................................................................................25  

Results  1:  Happiness  and  Sadness ............................................................................26  

Discussion  1 ......................................................................................................................29  

Results  2:  Other  Emotions ...........................................................................................33  

Discussion  2 ......................................................................................................................38  

Structural  Features  and  Positive  Emotions.....................................................38  

Structural  Features  and  Negative  Emotions ...................................................40  

IV.  Conclusion .........................................................................................................................44  

V.  Future  Research................................................................................................................46  

Bibliography ...........................................................................................................................48  

Appendices  

A.  Melodies .................................................................................................................50  

B.  IRB  Exemption  Letter.........................................................................................54  

C.  T-­Tests.....................................................................................................................55  

D.  Correlations  Within  All  Melodies ..................................................................56  

E.  Correlations  by  Condition.................................................................................57  

iv  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Abstract  
 

To  investigate  some  specifics  of  what  makes  emotions  attributable  to  

melody,  a  combination  of  undergraduate  and  graduate  music  majors  at  Texas  Tech  

University  were  asked  to  compose  a  melody  depicting  either  happiness  or  sadness.  

No  restrictions  were  placed  on  the  use  of  time  signature,  key  signature,  or  tempo,  

but  melodies  were  restricted  to  one  monophonic  line  of  music.  Melodies  were  

analyzed  for  several  structural  features,  some  of  which  were  drawn  from  previous  

studies,  such  as  mode  and  note  density,  and  others  that  are  new  to  the  present  

project,  such  as  melodic  span,  and  melodic  contour.    

Next,  a  perceptual  study  was  conducted  to  help  determine  how  well  melodies  

portrayed  the  intended  emotions.  Forty-­‐nine  undergraduate  music  majors  rated  

their  perceptions  of  twelve  different  emotions  in  each  of  the  melodies;  six  emotions  

were  positively  valenced,  including  happy,  and  six  emotions  were  negatively  

valenced,  including  sad.  Perceptions  of  emotion  in  melody  were  dependent  on  

several  contributing  factors  ranging  from  those  widely  used  such  as  mode  and  note  

density,  to  those  not  often  connected  with  perceived  emotion  such  as  melodic  

contour  and  formal  design.  Overall,  however,  different  combinations  of  these  factors  

in  individual  melodies  demonstrated  that  no  single  musical  factor  can  determine  

emotional  content.  

v  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

List  of  Tables  

1.  Frequencies  of  First-­Order  Intervals,  with  Size  Groupings................................11  

2.  Mean  Interval  by  Melody  and  Condition...................................................................12  

3.  Minor  Second  Distribution ............................................................................................13  

4.  Frequencies  of  Melodic  Spans  by  Size  and  Groupings..........................................16  

5.  Note  Density  by  Melody..................................................................................................17  

6.  Frequencies  of  Post-­Skip  Reversal  and  Regression  to  Mean .............................22  

7.  Mean  Values  for  all  Structural  Features  by  Melody  and  Condition .................26  

8.  Perception  Ratings  for  Happy  Melodies ...................................................................33  

9.  Comparison  Order  of  Happy  Melodies ......................................................................34  

10.  Perception  Ratings  for  Sad  Melodies.......................................................................36  

11.  Comparison  Order  of  Sad  Melodies .........................................................................37  

vi  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

List  of  Figures  

1.  Melodic  Spans  Outlined ..................................................................................................14  

2.  Mean  Melodic  Arch  by  Condition.................................................................................19  

3.  Examples  of  Post-­Skip  Reversal  and  Regression  to  the  Mean ...........................21  

4.  Melody  with  Bracketed  Confirmations  and  Violations  of  Post-­Skip        


Reversal  (PSR)  and  Regression  to  the  Mean  (RTM)..............................................22  

5.  Mean  Melodic  Arches  of  Top  Five  Rated  Melodies  by  Condition ......................30  

6.  Happy  6 ................................................................................................................................38  

7.  Happy  7 ................................................................................................................................39  

8.  Sad  6 ......................................................................................................................................40  

9.  Sad  10 ...................................................................................................................................41  

vii  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Chapter  I  

Introduction  

  There  are  many  artistic  media  through  which  emotion  can  be  expressed.  The  

raw  materials  of  these  art  forms  are  constantly  manipulated  to  convey  both  general  

and  specific  emotions,  and  some,  such  as  music,  can  do  so  free  of  verbal  content.  The  

degree  to  which  music  can  reliably  and  accurately  express  emotion  is  a  matter  of  

considerable  debate,  but  there  is  wide  agreement  that  music  and  emotion  are  

linked.1  Indeed,  music  has  been  associated  with  emotional  states  since  antiquity.  

The  Epitaph  of  Seikilos,  the  oldest  complete  song,  is  inscribed  on  a  tombstone  with  

musical  notations  and  lyrics  that  read,  “While  you  live,  be  happy  [shine]  don't  suffer  

[be  sad]  anything  at  all;  [because]  life  is  short  and  time  demands  its  toll  [time  leads  

to  death].”2  Accounts  of  music  regulating  mood,  for  good  and  ill,  are  common  in  

antiquity  and  repeated  as  factual  well  into  the  modern  era,  notably  in  the  Baroque  

era’s  focus  on  the  affective  qualities  of  music.  With  the  19th-­‐century  advent  of  

experimental  psychology,  music  was  studied  as  a  human  endeavor  that  was  

uniquely  emotion-­‐laden.  Experiments  in  the  late  nineteenth  century  studied  music’s  

capacity  to  induce  emotion,3  as  well  as  perceived  emotional  content  in  music.4    

                                                                                                               
1  Patrik  N.  Juslin,  “Emotional  Responses  to  Music,”  in  The  Oxford  Handbook  of  Music  

Psychology,  ed.  Susan  Hallam,  Ian  Cross,  and  Michael  Thaut  (New  York:  Oxford  
University  Press,  2009),  131.  
2  “Seikilos  Epitaph  (Ancient  Greek),”  Original  Score  with  Commentary  (Greek  and  

English),  last  modified  Oct.  30,  2009,  


http://imslp.org/wiki/Seikilos_Epitaph_(Ancient_Greek).  
3  Benjamin  Ives  Gilman,  “Report  on  an  Experimental  Test  of  Musical  Expressiveness  

(Continued),”  The  American  Journal  of  Psychology  5,  no.  1  (Oct.  1892):  71.  
1  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

There  are  many  definable  parameters  within  music  that  could  contribute  to  

emotional  content.  The  semantic  meaning  of  lyrics  aside,  the  primary  parameters  

include  melody,  harmony,  and  rhythm.  Each  of  these  parameters  has  unique  

characteristics  and  tendencies  relative  to  emotional  expression.  Specific  individual  

notes  or  chords  do  not  automatically  lend  a  specific  emotional  cast  to  a  given  

composition;  it  is  the  relationship  and  arrangement  of  notes  that  contribute  

collectively  to  the  portrayal  of  an  emotion.  Looking  at  paintings,  beyond  the  literal  

images  on  the  page,  the  abstract  features  greatly  contribute  to  the  expression  of  

emotion.  Abstract  features  in  art  include  not  only  the  choices  of  color,  but  also  the  

type  of  paint,  type  and  size  of  brush,  and  the  particular  brush  stroke  the  painter  

employs.  For  instance,  a  watercolor  painting  using  pastel  colors,  a  thin  brush,  and  

short  brushstrokes  will  elicit  different  emotions  than  an  oil-­‐based  painting  using  

bright,  vibrant  colors  and  a  thicker  brush  with  wide  brushstrokes,  even  if  the  images  

depicted  in  the  paintings  are  identical.5  The  primary  parameters  of  music  similarly  

contribute  to  how  music  portrays  emotion.  Any  alterations  of  melodic  features,  

harmonies,  or  rhythms  can,  in  varying  degrees,  affect  the  emotions  portrayed  by  

music.  Because  of  the  various  ways  these  parameters  can  affect  emotions  in  music,  

close  examinations  are  needed  to  study  how  these  parameters  can  be  manipulated  

to  portray  emotions.  

                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                                         
4  June  E.  Downey,  “A  Musical  Experiment,”  The  American  Journal  of  Psychology  9,  no.  

1  (Oct.1897):  69.  
5  Barry  Nemett,  “Two-­‐Dimensional  Materials  and  Techniques,”  in  Images,  Objects,  

and  Ideas:  Viewing  the  Visual  Arts  (Fort  Worth:  Harcourt  Brace  Jovanovich  College  
Publishers,  1992).  
2  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Melody,  as  one  of  the  primary  parameters  of  music,  has  garnered  significant  

attention  with  respect  to  emotional  expression  in  music.  E.  T.  A.  Hoffmann,  a  well-­‐

known  critic,  composer,  and  author  from  the  turn  of  the  nineteenth  century,  states:  

[T]he  first  and  foremost  element  in  music  is  melody,  which  seizes  the  human  
imagination  with  magical  power.  It  cannot  be  said  often  enough,  that  without  
expressive,  singable  melody  any  instrumental  color  is  merely  glistering  
apparel,  not  adorning  any  living  body…6      
 

From  a  similar  premise,  Richard  Wagner  writes:  

Melody  is  the  redemption  of  the  poet’s  endlessly  conditioned  thought  into  a  
deep-­‐felt  consciousness  of  emotion’s  highest  freedom:  it  is  the  willed  and  
achieved  unwillful,  the  conscious  and  proclaimed  unconscious,  the  vindicated  
necessity  of  an  endless-­‐reaching  content,  condensed  from  its  farthest  
branchings  into  an  utmost  definite  utterance  of  feeling.7  
 
Hoffmann  and  Wagner  both  deemed  melody  to  be  the  most  important  aspect  

and  the  “shaping  organ”  of  music.8  Recent  research  has  attempted  to  discover  which  

features  in  melody  figure  most  prominently  in  the  depiction  of  emotion.  In  a  1990  

study,  Kastner  and  Crowder  reported  that  children  ages  three  to  twelve  rated  folk  

melodies  in  a  major  mode  as  happier  than  folk  melodies  in  a  minor  mode.  9  Kastner  

and  Crowder’s  study  helped  substantiate  the  correlation  of  emotion  and  mode  in  

music.  In  2001,  William  Collier  and  Timothy  Hubbard  focused  on  participants’  

                                                                                                               
6  E.  T.  A.  Hoffmann,  E.  T.  A.  Hoffmann’s  Musical  Writings.  Ed.  David  Charleton  Trans.  

Martyn  Clarke.  (New  York:  Cambridge  University  Press,  1989),  156.  


7  Richard  Wagner,  Wagner  on  Music  and  Drama:  A  Compendium  of  Richard  Wagner’s  

Prose  Works,  trans.,  H.  Ashton  Ellis,  selected  and  arranged  with  an  introduction  by  
Albert  Goldman  and  Evert  Sprinchorn  (New  York:  E.P.  Dutton  &  Co.,  Inc.,  1964),  209.  
8  Hoffmann,  E.  T.  A.  Hoffmann’s  Musical  Writings,  156.  
9  Marianna  Pinchot  Kastner  and  Robert  G.  Crowder,  “Perception  of  the  Major/Minor  

Distinction:  IV.  Emotional  Connotations  in  Young  Children,”  Music  Perception  8,  no.  2  
(Winter  1990):  198.  
3  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

reported  emotions  on  the  effects  of  differing  musical  structures.  One  of  their  main  

questions  in  this  experiment  was  if  a  minor  scale  could  be  manipulated  in  such  a  

way  that  it  would  receive  a  “happy”  rating  over  a  “sad”  rating,  and  vice  versa.  All  

ascending  scale  forms  were  given  higher  “happy”  ratings  than  descending  scales,  

leading  the  authors  to  conclude  that  listeners  rely  heavily  on  melodic  direction  and  

pitch  height  in  making  judgments  about  emotional  content,  rather  than  solely  on  the  

mode.  Collier  and  Hubbard  noted  “that  emotional  valence  may  be  based  more  on  the  

horizontal  rather  than  the  vertical  aspect  of  music.”10  

This  horizontal  component  of  music  may  be  a  definable  difference  between  

the  major  and  minor  scales,  however.    Those  acquainted  with  Western  tonal  music  

will  likely  associate  the  major  mode  with  happy  emotions  and  the  minor  mode  with  

sad  emotions.  Rudolf  Arnheim  explains  the  major-­‐happy  and  minor-­‐sad  correlation  

using  the  structure  of  the  scales.  Considering  the  major  and  natural  minor  scales  as  

comprised  of  two  tetrachords  separated  by  a  whole  step,  the  half-­‐step  placement  

determines  the  direction  of  melodic  energy.  The  preponderance  of  upward-­‐

propelling  motion  in  the  major  scale  creates  a  sense  of  liberation  and  of  overcoming  

weight,  while  the  resistance  to  upward  motion  in  the  minor  scale  is  experienced  as  

giving  in  to  gravity  and  as  a  passive  letting-­‐go.11  

                                                                                                               
10  William  Collier  and  Timothy  Hubbard,  “Musical  Scales  and  Evaluations  of  

Happiness  and  Awkwardness:  Effects  of  Pitch,  Direction,  and  Scale  Mode,”  The  
American  Journal  of  Psychology  113,  no.  3  (Autumn  2001):  357.  
11  Rudolf  Arnheim,  “Perceptual  Dynamics  in  Musical  Expression,”  The  Musical  

Quarterly  70,  no.  3  (Summer  1984).  


4  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

This  motion  and  mode  theory  as  proposed  by  Arnheim  figured  prominently  

in  Hunter,  Schellenberg,  and  Schimmack’s  2010  study  that  focused  on  the  

similarities  and  differences  in  listeners’  perceptions  of  emotions  when  listening  to  

30-­‐second  excerpts  of  pieces  composed  by  J.  S.  Bach.  Eight  excerpts  were  played  at  

both  a  slow  and  a  fast  tempo  (the  fast  tempo  was  exactly  twice  as  fast  as  the  slow  

tempo)  and  both  in  major  and  in  minor,  creating  four  different  versions  of  each  

excerpt.  Excerpts  with  two  predictors  for  happiness,  major  mode  and  fast  tempo,  

received  the  highest  happy  ratings,  while  the  sad  ratings  were  given  to  pieces  in  

minor  mode  and  at  a  slow  tempo.  Excerpts  that  had  conflicting  variables  for  

happiness  and  sadness  received  similar  ratings  for  both  emotions,  but  excerpts  with  

a  fast  tempo  in  the  minor  mode  received  higher  happy  ratings  than  did  excerpts  

with  a  slow  tempo  in  the  major  mode.  From  these  findings,  the  tempo  of  a  piece  of  

music  seemed  to  have  more  influence  on  the  perceived  emotional  content  than  did  

the  mode  of  the  piece.12  Note  that  in  using  the  same  pieces  played  at  two  different  

tempi,  the  authors  are  essentially  testing  the  effect  of  note  density  (mean  number  of  

notes  per  second).  Musical  tempo  -­‐  the  rate  of  a  regulating  musical  beat  -­‐  does  not  

provide  an  accurate  basis  for  comparing  the  number  of  attacks  per  second  across  

different  pieces.    

To  further  explore  how  emotions  are  portrayed  in  music  using  modern  

composers,  William  Forde  Thompson  and  Brent  Robitaille  examined  the  portrayal  

                                                                                                               
12  Patrick  G.  Hunter,  E.  Glenn  Schellenberg,  and  Ulrich  Schimmack,  “Feelings  and  

Perceptions  of  Happiness  and  Sadness  Induced  by  Music:  Similarities,  Differences,  
and  Mixed  Emotions,”  Psychology  of  Aesthetics,  Creativity,  and  the  Arts  4,  no.  1  
(2010):  51-­‐52.  
5  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

of  six  specific  emotions  –  joy,  sorrow,  excitement,  dullness,  anger,  and  peace  –  

through  newly  composed  melodies.  Five  highly  trained  composers  each  composed  a  

melody  to  depict  each  emotion,  and  listeners  then  rated  the  presence  of  each  

emotion  in  each  melody.  Thompson  and  Robitaille  observed  that  melodies  aimed  at  

depicting  joy  were  largely  tonal  while  melodies  composed  to  depict  sadness  and  

anger  used  chromaticism.13  Excitement  was  perceived  not  only  in  melodies  

intended  to  portray  excitement,  but  also  in  melodies  expressing  joy  and  anger.  

Based  on  these  results,  excitement  in  melodies  could  be  a  measure  of  arousal  

instead  of  valence,  capable  of  being  expressed  in  melodies  portraying  either  positive  

or  negative  emotions.    In  melodies  intended  to  convey  sorrow,  peace  ratings  were  

second  behind  sorrow  ratings,  and  yet  melodies  intended  to  convey  peace  received  

high  joy  ratings.  Peace,  like  excitement,  could  be  a  stronger  measure  of  arousal  

instead  of  valence  in  emotional  expression.  

In  “The  Relationship  between  Musical  Structure  and  Perceived  Expression”  

(2009),  Alf  Gabrielsson  provides  a  comprehensive  summary  of  many  reports  on  the  

emotional  effect  of  different  musical  parameters.  Gabrielsson  concludes  that  

“differences  between  major  and  minor  mode  are  thus  mainly  associated  with  

difference  in  valence,  positive  or  negative.”14  While  the  major  mode  is  not  a  

                                                                                                               
13  William  Forde  Thompson  and  Brent  Robitaille,  “Can  Composers  Express  Emotions  

through  Music?”  Empirical  Studies  of  the  Arts  10,  no.  1  (1992):  144.  
14  Alf  Gabrielsson,  “The  Relationship  between  Musical  Structure  and  Perceived  

Expression,”  in  The  Oxford  Handbook  of  Music  Psychology,  ed.  Susan  Hallam,  Ian  
Cross,  and  Michael  Thaut  (New  York:  Oxford  University  Press,  2009),  143.  
6  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

necessary  requirement  for  the  perception  of  happiness  in  music  and  minor  for  

sadness,  the  long-­‐established  correlation  holds  true.15  

In  order  to  discover  whether  these  implicit  correlations  exist  more  broadly,  

two  experiments  were  conducted  in  which  novice  composers  were  solicited  to  write  

a  melody  depicting  a  discrete  emotion.  These  melodies  were  analyzed  for  several  

structural  features  and  characteristics  that  have  been  implicated  in  recent  research  

on  music  and  emotion,  such  as  mode,  intervallic  content,  density,  and  contour,  as  

well  as  for  features  that  have  not  yet  been  investigated  such  as  melodic  spans,  

phrase  structure,  post-­‐skip  reversal,  and  regression  to  the  mean,  explained  fully  

below.  In  Experiment  2,  listeners  assessed  the  emotional  content  in  these  melodies.  

These  data  were  then  used  to  evaluate  the  success  of  the  composers  in  

communicating  their  intended  emotion,  and  to  further  explore  links  between  

structural  features  and  specific  emotions.  

                                                                                                               
15  cf.  Hunter  et  al.,  “Feelings  and  Perceptions  of  Happiness  and  Sadness  Induced  by  

Music.”  
7  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Chapter  II  

Experiment  1  

The  first  experiment,  aimed  at  determining  the  differences  of  structural  

features  in  melodies  composed  to  depict  a  specific  emotion,  used  happy  and  sad  as  

oppositional  emotions  to  highlight  these  structural  differences.  

Happiness  and  sadness,  two  basic  emotions,  function  as  umbrella  terms  for  

larger  families  of  emotions.  While  happiness  and  sadness  certainly  do  not  cover  the  

gamut  of  emotions,  they  are  two  commonly  referenced  emotions  that  provide  

measurable  contrasts  for  research  in  emotional  content  in  music.16  Findings  from  

other  researchers  agree  that  happiness  and  sadness  are  two  of  the  most  frequently  

occurring  emotions  in  music.17  These  findings  suggest  that  frequently  occurring  

emotions  in  music  are  not  completely  subjective  and  have  some  objective  qualities  

and  systematic  relationships  in  their  structures  that  can  be  modeled  successfully.18  

Method  

Participants  

Twenty  upper-­‐level  undergraduate  and  graduate  music  students  at  Texas  

Tech  University  were  solicited  as  fellow  students  of  the  author  to  compose  a  melody  
                                                                                                               
16  Paul  Ekman,  “An  Argument  for  Basic  Emotions,”  Cognition  and  Emotion  6  (3/4  

1992):  172.  
17  Researchers  include  Juslin  and  Laukka,  (2004);  Sloboda,  (1992);  Wells  and  

Hakanen,  (1991);  and  Zetner  et  al,  (2008).  


18  Patrik  N.  Juslin  et  al.,  “How  does  Music  Evoke  Emotions?”  in  Handbook  of  Music  

and  Emotion:  Theory,  Research,  and  Applications  ed.  Patrik  N.  Juslin  and  John  
Sloboda  (New  York:  Oxford  University  Press,  2010),  612.  
8  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

depicting  either  happiness  or  sadness.  The  mean  age  of  the  composer-­‐participants  

was  27,  ranging  from  20-­‐40,  with  a  mean  of  15  years  of  formal  musical  training.  

Eight  of  the  twenty  participants  were  familiar  with  non-­‐Western  musical  cultures.  

Seven  participants  were  female,  thirteen  male.  Participants  also  reflected  a  variety  

of  primary  instrument/vocal  types.  

Procedure  

Half  of  the  participants  were  instructed  to  compose  a  melody  portraying  

happiness  and  the  other  half  of  the  participants  were  instructed  to  compose  a  

melody  portraying  sadness  on  a  sheet  containing  a  treble  clef  staff.  There  were  no  

restrictions  placed  on  the  use  of  time  signature,  key  signature,  or  tempo.  The  

composition  was  limited,  however,  to  a  single  voice  or  instrument  with  no  harmonic  

accompaniment.  Biographical  information  was  gathered  on  the  same  sheet,  and  

participants  also  rated,  on  a  five-­‐point  scale,  how  strongly  they  thought  their  melody  

portrayed  the  intended  emotion  (see  Appendix  A  for  all  melodies).  

Results  and  Discussion  

Emotion/Mode  Correlation  

The  first  step  in  looking  at  the  structural  differences  within  the  melodies  was  

examining  the  correlation  of  emotion  and  mode.  All  ten  happy  melodies  were  

composed  in  the  major  mode  and  eight  of  the  sad  melodies  were  composed  

throughout  in  the  minor  mode  with  one  sad  melody  mixing  modes  between  phrases  

and  one  in  the  major  mode.  Participants  used  various  forms  of  the  minor  scale  in  the  

9  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

sad  melodies,  using  raised  leading  tones  where  expected  in  common  practice  

tonality.  Thus  the  construction  of  these  melodies  confirms  the  longstanding  

associations  of  mode  and  emotion.19    

First-­Order  Intervals  

Table  1  shows  the  numbers  and  percentages  of  first-­‐order  intervals  

(intervals  between  adjacent  notes)  by  emotional  condition.  There  were  no  intervals  

larger  than  an  octave.  Similar  intervals  are  grouped  and  subtotaled  in  the  table:  0-­‐2  

semitones  (unison  and  steps),  3-­‐7  semitones  (smaller  leaps),  0-­‐7  semitones  (all  

intervals  within  a  perfect  fifth),  and  8-­‐12  semitones  (larger  leaps  up  to  an  octave).    

                                                                                                               
19  cf.  Candace  Brower,  “A  Cognitive  Theory  of  Musical  Meaning,”  Journal  of  Music  

Theory  44,  no.  2  (Autumn  2000).  


10  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Table  1:  Frequencies  of  First-­Order  Intervals,  with  Size  Groupings  

 Interval  in   Happy   Happy   Sad   Sad  


 Semitones   Count   Percent   Count   Percent  
0   12   6.12%   13   6.60%  
1   39   19.90%   61   30.96%  
2   80   40.82%   72   36.55%  
0-­2   131   66.84%   146   74.11%  
3   22   11.22%   20   10.15%  
4   13   6.63%   13   6.60%  
5   18   9.18%   9   4.57%  
6   0   0.00%   0   0.00%  
7   5   2.55%   3   1.52%  
3-­7   58   29.59%   45   22.84%  
0-­7   189   96.43%   191   96.95%  
8   0   0.00%   4   2.03%  
9   2   1.02%   0   0.00%  
10   3   1.53%   1   0.51%  
11   0   0.00%   0   0.00%  
12   2   1.02%   1   0.51%  
8-­12   7   3.57%   6   3.05%  
Total   196       197      
 

The  vast  majority  of  first-­‐order  intervals  in  both  melody  conditions  were  a  

perfect  fifth  or  smaller  (0-­‐7  semitones).  The  comparisons  between  conditions  do  

little  to  support  the  general  notion  that  larger  melodic  intervals  convey  positive  

emotions  and  smaller  intervals  convey  negative  emotions.20  

As  seen  in  Table  2,  the  mean  first-­‐order  interval  in  semitones  was  also  

calculated  by  melody  and  by  condition,  and  difference  in  mean  interval  between  

conditions  was  as  expected  but  was  not  significant.  The  ten  melodies  within  each  

condition  were  arbitrarily  numbered  for  ease  of  reference  and  will  be  referred  to  

with  these  labels  below.  

                                                                                                               
20  cf.  Gabrielsson,  “Musical  Structure  and  Perceived  Emotion,”  144.  

11  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Table  2:  Mean  Interval  by  Melody  and  Condition  

Mean   Mean  
Melody   Interval   Melody   Interval  
Happy  1   3.94   Sad  1   2.25  
Happy  2   2.19   Sad  2   2.36  
Happy  3   2.40   Sad  3   1.83  
Happy  4   2.71   Sad  4   3.43  
Happy  5   4.83   Sad  5   2.17  
Happy  6   2.40   Sad  6   1.81  
Happy  7   2.17   Sad  7   2.28  
Happy  8   2.33   Sad  8   1.93  
Happy  9   2.34   Sad  9   2.70  
Happy  10   1.56   Sad  10   1.86  
Mean   2.59   Mean   2.22  
 

The  only  first-­‐order  interval  comparison  in  Table  1  that  is  statistically  

significant  is  the  minor  second  (p  =  .01).  Was  a  greater  amount  of  half  steps  

intentionally  used  to  portray  sadness,  or  did  the  greater  number  stem  from  the  scale  

structure  used  for  each  melody?  In  major  scales  there  are  two  occurrences  of  minor  

seconds,  between  mi  –  fa  and  ti  –  do.21  Minor  mode  melodies  were  frequently  

composed  using  the  harmonic  minor  collection,  which  contains  three  minor  

seconds,  between  re  –  me,  sol  –  le,  and  ti  –  do.  This  difference  in  scalar  structure  can  

explain  the  minor  second  difference  shown  in  Table  1,  as  detailed  in  Table  3.    

                                                                                                               
21  Solfège  based  on  a  movable-­‐do  system  for  both  the  major  and  minor  modes.  

12  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Table  3:  Minor  Second  Distribution  

    Happy   Happy   Sad   Sad    


    Count   Percentage   Count   Percentage  
ti  -­‐  do   17   41.46%   14   22.95%  
re  -­‐  me   0   0.00%   25   40.98%  
mi  -­‐  fa   24   58.54%   4   6.56%  
sol  -­‐  le   0   0.00%   15   24.59%  
other   0   0.00%   3   4.92%  
    41       61      
 

The  steps  between  mi  –  fa  and  ti  –  do  in  the  happy  category  account  for  all  of  the  

minor  seconds;  no  chromaticism  was  used.    The  higher  number  of  minor  seconds  in  

the  sad  melodies  was  split  somewhat  evenly  between  the  expected  scale  steps  with  

a  higher  number  of  steps  around  re-­‐  me.  The  exploitation  of  half  steps  in  sad  

melodies  is  partially  made  possible  by  the  additional  opportunities  for  half  steps.  

Not  only  do  sad  melodies  incorporate  more  kinds  of  diatonic  half  steps,  but  

additional  chromatic  half  steps  were  incorporated.  

Gabrielsson  observes  that  “the  minor  second  [is  perceived]  as  the  most  sad  

interval.”  22  The  greater  quantity  and  frequent  use  of  minor  seconds  in  sad  melodies  

may  help  in  the  depiction  of  sadness,  yet  this  conclusion  is  confounded  by  the  

inherent  structure  of  major  and  minor  scales.  Is  harmonic  minor  associated  with  

sadness  because  of  its  greater  opportunity  for  half  step  usage,  or  did  the  harmonic  

minor  collection  evolve  out  of  an  attempt  to  maximize  half-­‐step  motion  within  a  

diatonic  scale?  

                                                                                                               
22  Gabrielsson,  144.  

13  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Similar  patterns  of  first-­‐order  interval  distribution  can  be  seen  in  David  

Huron’s  assessment  of  folk  songs  from  several  nationalities.23  The  most  common  

interval  in  Western  traditions  is  the  major  second,  followed  by  the  perfect  fourth  

and  the  perfect  fifth,  as  in  Table  1.  24  The  percentage  of  minor  seconds  in  happy  

melodies  resembles  that  of  Huron’s  Western  folk  songs  (19.90%  vs.  approximately  

15%,  respectively),  while  the  minor  second  for  sad  melodies  does  not  (30.96%),  

likely  due  to  the  prevalence  of  the  major  mode  in  Western  folk  songs.    Yet  the  

overall  similarities  confirm  the  predominance  of  small  intervals  in  tonal  melodies.  

Melodic  Spans  

While  tallying  the  first-­‐order  intervals  sheds  light  on  the  note-­‐to-­‐note  

component  of  the  melodies,  comparing  larger  melodic  spans  can  provide  a  more  

musical  basis  from  which  to  explore  structural  differences  between  happy  and  sad  

conditions.  Melodic  spans  are  the  distance  music  travels  in  one  direction,  measured  

in  semitones.  Figure  1  shows  an  example  of  a  study  melody  with  brackets  outlining  

each  span  and  its  size.  

 
3    3              5                        3     3      5   7    2          2  
 
Figure  1:  Melodic  Spans  Outlined  

                                                                                                               
23  David  Huron,  Sweet  Anticipation  (Cambridge:  The  MIT  Press,  2006),  74,  Figure  

5.1.  
24  Western  nationalities  examined  include  American,  English,  and  German.  

14  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Note  that  pitches  at  the  melodic  peaks  and  valleys  serve  as  boundaries  for  the  spans  

on  either  side  of  them.  Note  repetitions  do  not  count  as  a  change  of  direction;  

therefore  they  are  considered  a  continuous  note  until  a  different  pitch  occurs.  Some  

of  the  melodic  span  counts  are  simultaneously  counts  of  first-­‐order  intervals.  All  

one-­‐semitone  spans  are  first-­‐order  intervals  by  definition,  since  that  is  the  minimal  

distance  music  can  move.  Additionally,  other  first-­‐order  melodic  leaps  surrounded  

by  changes  of  direction  are  also  counted  as  melodic  spans.  Approximately  20%  of  

the  first-­‐order  intervals  for  both  happy  and  sad  melodies  were  also  spans.  Table  4  

below  shows  the  distribution  of  melodic  spans  by  individual  sizes  and  by  groupings.  

15  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Table  4:  Frequencies  of  Melodic  Spans  by  Size  and  Groupings  

    Happy   Happy   Sad   Sad  


    Count   Percentage   Count   Percentage  
1   5   5.32%   9   9.78%  
2   8   8.51%   11   11.96%  
3   15   15.96%   15   16.30%  
4   6   6.38%   11   11.96%  
1-­4   34   36.17%   46   50.00%  
5   25   26.60%   19   20.65%  
6   0   0.00%   2   2.17%  
7   13   13.83%   8   8.70%  
8   7   7.45%   9   9.78%  
9   5   5.32%   4   4.35%  
10   4   4.26%   2   2.17%  
11   0   0.00%   0   0.00%  
5-­11   54   57.45%   44   47.83%  
12   5   5.32%   2   2.17%  
13   0   0.00%   0   0.00%  
14   0   0.00%   0   0.00%  
15   0   0.00%   0   0.00%  
16   0   0.00%   0   0.00%  
17   1   1.06%   0   0.00%  
12-­17   6   6.38%   2   2.17%  
Total   94       92      
 

The  greater  prominence  of  smaller  spans  in  sad  melodies  appears  to  be  a  

structural  difference  in  melodies  that  composers  used  to  help  depict  sadness,  

however,  the  difference  is  not  significant  (p  =  .10).  Happy  melodies  are  mainly  

comprised  of  spans  falling  in  the  middle  category,  predominantly  perfect  fourths  

and  perfect  fifths.  Happy  melodies,  therefore,  have  a  greater  average  span  size  

(5.57)  compared  to  that  of  sad  melodies  (4.73).  The  only  two  structural  features  that  

were  correlated  were  the  melodic  span  and  the  first-­‐order  intervals  (p  =  .005).  

16  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Note  Density  

  The  note  density  of  a  melody  is  calculated  as  the  mean  number  of  notes  per  

second.  Listed  below  in  Table  5  is  the  note  density  for  each  melody  and  the  mean  

density  by  condition.  Happy  melodies  had  a  greater  density  relative  to  sad  melodies  

(p  =  .01).    

Table  5:  Note  Density  by  Melody  

Note   Note  
Melody   Density   Melody   Density  
Happy  1   1.89   Sad  1   2.02  
Happy  2   3.03   Sad  2   1.50  
Happy  3   2.63   Sad  3   0.89  
Happy  4   1.88   Sad  4   1.16  
Happy  5   1.35   Sad  5   1.30  
Happy  6   5.13   Sad  6   0.59  
Happy  7   1.50   Sad  7   1.11  
Happy  8   2.17   Sad  8   1.44  
Happy  9   1.35   Sad  9   1.07  
Happy  10   2.08   Sad  10   1.01  
Mean   2.30   Mean   1.21  
 

Melodic  Arches  

Following  David  Huron’s  method  of  calculating  pitch  means  in  “The  Melodic  

Arch  in  Western  Folksongs,”  each  note  was  assigned  a  number  based  on  its  relation  

to  the  first  note.25  The  first  note  of  each  melody  was  assigned  “0,”  since  that  served  

as  the  basis  for  the  ensuing  interval  measurements.  Subsequent  notes  were  assigned  

a  number  based  on  their  distance  from  the  first  note,  measured  in  semitones,  with  a  

                                                                                                               
25  David  Huron,  “The  Melodic  Arch  in  Western  Folksongs,”  Computing  in  Musicology  

10  (1996):  5.  
17  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

positive  or  negative  value  based  on  whether  the  note  was  higher  or  lower  than  the  

first  note.    

To  accurately  compare  contours  across  melodies,  the  melodies  needed  to  be  

represented  with  an  equal  number  of  points.  In  addition  to  using  the  initial  and  final  

pitches,  eight  equally  distributed  points  were  selected  from  each  melody  to  

represent  the  melodic  shape.  Figure  2  shows  the  composite  melodic  shape  of  

melodies  by  condition.    The  percentages  used  on  the  x-­‐axis  represent  segments  of  

the  melodies  within  which  mean  pitch  height  was  calculated;  for  example,  the  data  

point  at  12.5%  is  mean  of  the  second  note  through  the  note  closest  to  12.5%  

through  the  melody.  The  point  at  25%  begins  with  the  next  note  after  the  first  

segment  and  continues  through  the  first  quarter  of  the  melody.    

18  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

5.00  

4.00  
Mean  Semitone  Distance  

3.00  

2.00  
Happy  Mean  
1.00   Sad  Mean  

0.00  

-­‐1.00  

-­‐2.00  

Melodic  Bins  
 

Figure  2:  Mean  Melodic  Arch  by  Condition  


 

  Looking  at  Figure  2,  differences  can  be  seen  in  the  composite  melodic    

arches  by  condition.  The  composite  melodic  arch  in  the  happy  category  is  more  

symmetrical  with  an  overall  convex  arch  shape  and  a  concave  dip  in  the  middle.  The  

composite  melodic  arch  in  the  sad  category  shows  a  greater  initial  increase  in  pitch  

height  during  the  first  eighth  of  the  melody  with  a  gradual  declination  to  the  end.  

Interestingly,  both  graphs  exhibit  declinations  below  the  eventual  final  note,  with  a  

small  rise  to  their  final  note.  David  Huron  finds  that  the  symmetrical  convex  contour  

type  comprises  nearly  40%  of  all  melodic  arch  shapes,  and  that  descending,  right-­‐

19  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

skewed  contour  types  make  up  nearly  30%  of  arch  shapes.26  Results  from  the  

present  study  support  Huron’s  findings  about  the  predominance  of  symmetrical  and  

right-­‐skewed  melodic  arch  shapes,  and  suggest  relationships  between  these  two  

predominant  melodic  arch  shapes  and  emotional  content.  Collier  and  Hubbard’s  

findings,  discussed  above,  indicated  that  listeners  gave  descending  melodic  lines  

higher  sad  ratings,  regardless  of  mode.  The  composite  melodic  shapes  in  Figure  2  

suggest  that  a  predominance  of  descending  motion  is  indeed  a  general  feature  of  sad  

melodies,  and  that  novice  composers  created  a  steep  initial  ascent  in  order  to  

maximize  the  possibility  for  overall  melodic  descent.  

Post-­Skip  Reversal  and  Regression  to  the  Mean  

Two  further  ways  to  analyze  melodic  motion  are  post-­‐skip  reversal  and  

regression  to  the  mean,  as  proposed  by  Huron  in  Sweet  Anticipation.  In  post-­‐skip  

reversal,  leaps  are  followed  by  a  change  in  direction.  Regression  to  the  mean  states  

that,  after  a  leap,  the  ensuing  note  will  move  toward  the  floating  mean,  whether  or  

not  that  constitutes  a  change  or  continuance  of  direction.27  Studies  by  Huron  have  

proven  that  listeners  tend  to  expect  post-­‐skip  reversal,  while  regression  to  the  mean  

actually  prevails  in  analysis.28  

The  string  of  floating  mean  values  in  each  melody  is  calculated  as  follows:  

The  first  mean  in  a  melody  was  the  average  of  the  pitch  heights  of  the  first  two  

notes.  The  next  mean  pitch  was  the  average  of  the  first  three  pitch  heights,  

                                                                                                               
26  David  Huron,  “Melodic  Arch,”  17.  
27  Huron,  Sweet  Anticipation,  80.  
28  Ibid.,  83.  

20  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

continuing  in  the  same  fashion  through  the  end  of  the  melody.  Any  single  value  in  

the  floating  mean  thus  represents  all  musical  events  that  have  occurred  prior  to  that  

point.  

As  seen  in  Figure  3,  five  possible  outcomes  can  arise  when  analyzing  the  note  

following  a  skip:  a)  confirmation  of  both  Post-­‐Skip  Reversal  and  Regression  to  the  

Mean,  b)  confirmation  of  Post-­‐Skip  Reversal  but  not  Regression  to  the  Mean,  c)  

confirmation  of  Regression  to  the  Mean  but  not  Post-­‐Skip  Reversal,  d)  confirming  

neither  Post-­‐Skip  Reversal  or  Regression  to  the  Mean,  and  e)  a  mean  landing  skip  

where  regression  to  the  mean  is  not  possible/allows  an  equal  opportunity  for  a  

change  or  continuance  of  direction,  but  where  post-­‐skip  reversal  states  that  a  

change  of  direction  should  occur.  

Mean    
Pitch  
  
             
 
  a.          b.                c.                    d.                          e.  
 
 
Figure  3:  Examples  of  Post-­Skip  Reversal  and  Regression  to  the  Mean  
(Middle  Line  is  Mean  Pitch)  

  Figure  4  depicts  a  melody  composed  for  Experiment  1  with  all  leaps  and  

continuations  bracketed  and  labeled  accordingly.  

21  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Figure  4:  Melody  with  Bracketed  Confirmations  and  Violations  of  Post-­Skip  
Reversal  (PSR)  and  Regression  to  the  Mean  (RTM)  

Shown  in  Tables  6a  and  6b  are  the  confirmations  and  violations  of  post-­‐skip  

reversal  and  regression  to  the  mean  in  all  melodies,  by  condition.  

Table  6:  Frequencies  of  Post-­Skip  Reversal  and  Regression  to  Mean  
a:  Occurrences  in  Happy  Melodies  

    no  RTM   RTM   on  mean      


no  PSR   12   9   1      
    21.05%   15.79%   1.75%      
PSR   2   32   1      
    3.51%   56.14%   1.75%      
Total               57  
 

b:  Occurrences  in  Sad  Melodies  

    no  RTM   RTM      
no  PSR   12   3      
    23.53%   5.88%      
PSR   5   31      
    9.80%   60.78%      
Total           51  
 
22  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

No  statistically  significant  differences  occur  in  any  of  the  categories  between  

conditions.  From  these  results  it  can  be  noted,  however,  that  “RTM/PSR”  and  “no  

RTM/no  PSR”  comparisons  show  particularly  little  difference  between  conditions.  

Within  the  mixed  categories,  happy  melodies  regress  toward  the  mean  more  

frequently  than  sad  melodies  while  sad  melodies  show  a  stronger  tendency  to  

follow  post-­‐skip  reversal.  While  the  small  numbers  of  occurrences  are  not  

conclusive  evidence  of  differences  between  conditions,  conclusions  can  be  made  

about  the  general  construction  of  melodies.  The  majority  of  leap  continuations  will  

confirm  both  post-­‐skip  reversal  and  regression  to  the  mean,  smaller  numbers  of  

leap  continuations  will  confirm  neither  post-­‐skip  reversal  nor  regression  to  the  

mean,  and  occasional  leap  continuations  will  confirm  either  post-­‐skip  reversal  or  

regression  to  the  mean.  

23  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Chapter  III  

Experiment  2  

After  analyzing  the  technical  construction  of  melodies  composed  to  depict  

happiness  or  sadness  from  the  first  experiment,  this  second  study  aimed  to  uncover  

how  well  these  intended  emotions  were  perceived  by  listeners.  Listener  ratings  

were  used  to  analyze  further  which  structural  differences  from  Experiment  1  helped  

to  depict  different  emotions.  

Method  

Participants  

  Participants  in  this  study  were  49  undergraduate  music  majors  enrolled  in  

three  sections  of  Aural  Skills  2  at  Texas  Tech  University.  The  participants’  ages  

ranged  from  17-­‐37,  with  a  median  of  19  years  old,  and  a  mean  of  9  years  of  formal  

music  training.  Participants  consisted  of  22  males  and  27  females.  Participants  also  

reflected  a  variety  of  primary  instrument/vocal  types.  

Stimuli  

  The  stimuli  were  MIDI  files  of  the  20  melodies  composed  for  Experiment  1,  

generated  using  the  Acoustic  Grand  Piano  sound  with  the  automatic  Finale  2009  

presets.  Pitches,  durations,  rests,  and  tempi  were  input  into  Finale  as  written  by  the  

composers,  but  any  other  markings  that  might  contribute  to  the  portrayal  of  

emotion,  such  as  articulation,  dynamics,  and  fermatas,  were  ignored.  

24  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Apparatus  

  Stimuli  were  presented  over  the  built-­‐in  classroom  stereo  sound  system  as  

participants  rated  the  melodies  on  the  forms  provided.  

Procedure  

The  melodies  were  presented  in  two  different  randomized  orders.  Twenty-­‐

seven  participants  listened  to  random  order  A  and  twenty-­‐two  participants  listened  

to  random  order  B.  After  listening  to  each  melody,  participants  were  asked  to  first  

rate  whether  or  not  they  perceived  any  of  the  twelve  different  emotions  in  each  

melody.  The  melodies  were  visually  separated  into  two  groups  of  emotions  by  

general  association;  following  Kate  Hevner’s  organization  of  emotions,  the  “positive”  

group  included  graceful,  happy,  joyful,  lively,  triumphant,  and  exciting  while  the  

“negative”  group  included  solemn,  melancholy,  tragic,  sad,  longing,  and  gloomy.29  If  

participants  did  perceive  a  given  emotion,  they  then  rated  how  strongly  they  

perceived  each  emotion,  on  a  scale  of  one  to  five  (1  =  mildly  and  5  =  extremely).  

Thirty  seconds  of  silence  were  placed  between  melodies  to  give  the  participants  

time  to  rate  the  perceived  emotions  before  proceeding  to  the  next  melody.  This  

response  method  allowed  participants  to  record  perceptions  of  multiple  emotions  

for  each  melody.  

                                                                                                               
29  Kate  Hevner,  “Experimental  Studies  of  the  Elements  of  Expression  in  Music,”  The  

American  Journal  of  Psychology  48,  no.  2  (April  1936):  249.  


25  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Results  1:  Happiness  and  Sadness  

Tables  7a  and  7b  list  the  melodies  in  order  of  highest  to  lowest  rated  target  

emotion.  Also  listed  are  the  means  for  each  structural  feature  and  the  composers’  

ratings  by  individual  melody  and  by  condition.  Data  from  these  tables  will  be  used  

throughout  this  section.  

Table  7:  Mean  Values  for  all  Structural  Features  by  Melody  and  Condition  
a:  Mean  Values  for  Happy  Melodies  

Happy   Sad   Mean   Composer  


Melody   Mean   Mean   Density   Interval   Span   Rating  
Happy  2   3.55   0.00   3.03   2.19   5.11   4.00  
Happy  7   3.45   0.00   1.50   2.17   6.25   4.00  
Happy  10   3.14   0.00   2.08   1.56   7.00   2.00  
Happy  3   3.02   0.04   2.63   2.40   4.00   3.00  
Happy  6   2.96   0.00   5.13   2.40   5.05   5.00  
Happy  5   2.88   0.00   1.35   4.83   11.60   4.00  
Happy  1   2.67   0.02   1.89   3.94   5.73   3.00  
Happy  4   2.67   0.08   1.88   2.71   5.75   4.00  
Happy  9   2.63   0.00   1.35   2.34   4.69   2.00  
Happy  8   2.02   0.00   2.17   2.33   7.00   4.00  
Mean   2.90   0.01   2.30   2.59   5.57   3.50  
 

26  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Table  7:  Mean  Values  for  all  Structural  Features  by  Melody  and  Condition  
cont’d  
b:  Mean  Values  for  Sad  Melodies  

Happy   Sad   Mean   Composer  


Melody   Mean   Mean   Density   Interval   Span   Rating  
Sad  6   0.00   3.37   0.59   1.81   4.22   4.00  
Sad  7   0.00   3.08   1.11   2.28   4.10   4.00  
Sad  4   0.00   2.81   1.16   3.43   6.00   3.00  
Sad  10   0.09   2.65   1.01   1.86   5.78   3.00  
Sad  9   0.00   2.35   1.07   2.70   5.40   4.00  
Sad  5   0.00   2.10   1.30   2.17   4.88   3.00  
Sad  2   0.06   1.88   1.50   2.36   4.00   4.00  
Sad  8   0.20   1.04   1.44   1.93   7.00   1.00  
Sad  3   0.71   0.94   0.89   1.83   3.44   3.00  
Sad  1   0.92   0.35   2.02   2.25   4.24   2.00  
Mean   0.20   2.06   1.21   2.22   4.73   3.10  
 

As  previously  noted,  happiness  and  sadness  are  basic  emotions  that  are  

central  to  larger  families  of  emotions.  Even  though  the  participants  rated  their  

perceptions  of  twelve  different  emotions  for  each  melody,  the  present  analysis  will  

focus  on  ratings  of  perceived  happiness  and  sadness.    

Structural  features  within  happy  and  sad  melodies  were  correlated  with  the  

perceptual  data  using  the  Pearson  correlation.  Not  surprisingly,  happy  melodies  

conveyed  more  happiness  than  sad  melodies  and  sad  melodies  conveyed  more  

sadness  than  happy  melodies  (p  <  .001  in  both  comparisons),  but  happy  melodies  

conveyed  their  target  emotion  better  than  sad  melodies  (p  =  .025).30  Both  happy  and  

sad  ratings  across  all  melodies  correlated  significantly  with  density  (happy  p  =  .006  
                                                                                                               
30  A  similar  discrepancy  in  overall  happiness  and  sadness  ratings  can  also  be  seen  in  

Hunter,  Schellenberg,  and  Schimmack’s  “Feelings  and  Perceptions  of  Happiness  and  
Sadness  Induced  by  Music:  Similarities,  Differences,  and  Mixed  Emotions”  and  
Thompson  and  Robitaille’s  “Can  Composers  Express  Emotion  Through  Music?”  
27  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

and  sad  p  =  .008).  The  mean  rating  for  happy  and  sad  melodies  was  not  correlated  

significantly  with  mean  interval  or  span,  but  the  mean  interval  and  mean  span  were  

correlated  (p  =  .005).  

Data  analyses  were  also  conducted  within  each  condition.  Happiness  ratings  

were  not  significantly  correlated  with  density,  mean  span,  or  mean  interval.  In  

addition,  no  structural  features  were  significantly  correlated  with  each  other  in  

happy  melodies.  Within  the  sad  melodies,  the  sadness  ratings  and  density  were  

correlated  (p  =  .028).  None  of  the  other  structural  features  were  correlated  with  

sadness  ratings  or  with  each  other.  Not  only  was  the  difference  in  density  between  

happy  and  sad  melodies  significant,  but  density  also  decreased  significantly  as  the  

sad  melodies  became  sadder.  

Based  on  the  perceptual  data,  composers  were  generally  successful  

composing  happy  melodies.  The  lowest  happy  rating  was  a  2.02  and  the  highest  was  

a  3.55,  a  difference  of  1.53.  On  the  other  hand,  the  sadness  ratings  of  the  sad  

melodies  ranged  from  .35  to  3.37,  a  difference  of  3.02.  The  greater  variance  of  

composers’  success  within  the  sad  condition,  however,  allowed  for  stronger  

correlations  between  ratings  and  structural  features.  (These  correlations  within  

conditions  will  be  explored  further  in  Discussion  1.)    

In  addition  to  examining  the  structural  features,  composers’  ratings  

evaluating  how  well  their  melody  portrayed  their  target  emotion  were  also  

examined.  Across  all  melodies,  the  composers’  ratings  correlated  significantly  with  

listeners’  ratings.  Composers  of  happy  melodies,  however,  did  not  accurately  predict  

28  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

how  much  happiness  their  melody  portrayed  to  the  listeners  (p  =  .88),  while  

composers  of  sad  melodies  did  (p  =  .027).    

Happy  melodies  that  were  given  higher  happiness  ratings  exhibited  clearer  

phrase  structures,  most  often  clearly  defined  periods.  As  the  happiness  rating  

decreased,  the  clearly  defined  period  structure  was  less  evident,  and  the  lower  rated  

happy  melodies  were  generally  a  single  continuous  phrase.  Not  many  sad  melodies  

were  composed  with  balanced  phrase  structures,  and  sad  melodies  did  not  show  a  

link  between  the  sadness  ratings  and  form.  Overall,  however,  high  perceptual  

complexity  in  form  is  associated  with  sadness  and  low  perceptual  complexity,  

happiness.31  

Discussion  1  

The  results  from  the  perceptual  test  do  not  provide  any  stronger  correlations  

between  the  structural  features  and  the  listeners’  ratings  than  those  between  the  

structural  features  themselves.  The  perceptual  data  do,  however,  allow  an  

investigation  of  which  structural  features  might  have  the  greatest  effect  on  

perceived  emotion.  Figure  5  depicts  the  mean  melodic  arches  for  the  five  highest  

rated  melodies  in  each  condition  –  the  “happiest”  and  “saddest”  melodies.    

                                                                                                               
31  Gabrielsson,  “Emotional  Expression  in  Music,”  145.  

29  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

6.00  

5.00  

4.00  

3.00  

2.00  

1.00  

0.00   Happiest  Mean  


Saddest  Mean  
-­‐1.00  

-­‐2.00  

-­‐3.00  

-­‐4.00  

-­‐5.00  

Figure  5:  Mean  Melodic  Arches  of  Top  Five  Rated  Melodies  by  Condition  
 

  The  clear  differences  between  conditions  seen  above  in  Figure  2  are  

exaggerated  in  the  mean  melodic  arches  in  Figure  5,  and  it  is  instructive  to  compare  

the  arches  between  Figures  2  and  5  within  conditions.  Note  that  the  mean  happiest  

contour  is  still  roughly  symmetrical,  but  has  a  greater  climax,  reaching  almost  five  

semitones  above  its  origin,  versus  a  peak  of  3.5  semitones  in  Figure  2’s  mean  happy  

contour.  Differences  also  occur  in  the  descent  of  these  two  happy  arches;  the  overall  

happy  arch  is  more  symmetrical  and  ends  close  to  where  it  began,  whereas  the  

happiest  arch  in  Figure  5  ends  roughly  1.5  semitones  higher  than  its  origin.  

Within  sad  melodies,  the  general  contour  is  similar  between  the  two  figures,  

but  the  melodic  arch  for  saddest  melodies  in  Figure  5  reaches  a  higher  peak  than  

30  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

that  in  Figure  2,  and  also  ends  over  three  semitones  lower  than  its  starting  pitch.  

This  melodic  arch’s  descent  spans  approximately  nine  semitones,  compared  to  a  

descent  of  just  over  five  semitones  in  Figure  2’s  mean  sad  contour.  The  extended  

melodic  descent  of  the  saddest  melodies  lends  further  evidence  to  the  association  of  

descending  motion  and  negative  valence.      

Recall  that  post-­‐skip  reversal  and  regression  to  the  mean  did  not  exhibit  any  

significant  differences  between  conditions,  but  that  trends  indicated  a  link  between  

decreased  happiness  ratings  and  more  mixed  confirmations  (no  PSR/RTM  and  

PSR/no  RTM);  the  three  happy  melodies  that  received  the  highest  happiness  ratings  

contain  no  instances  of  mixed  confirmations.  The  three  sad  melodies  that  received  

the  highest  sadness  ratings  contain  62.5%  of  the  mixed  confirmations  found  in  all  

ten  sad  melodies.  Thus  the  higher  occurrences  of  double  confirmation  or  non-­‐

confirmation  of  post-­‐skip  reversal  and  regression  to  the  mean  seem  to  be  associated  

with  higher  happiness  and  lower  sadness  ratings.  

The  greater  variance  of  listeners’  perception  of  sadness  in  sad  melodies  

suggests  that  there  are  certain  structural  features  without  which  sadness  is  more  

difficult  to  portray.  Three  sad  melodies  stand  out  for  having  received  higher  ratings  

in  some  positive  emotion  than  they  received  for  sadness.  Sad  3  received  the  second  

lowest  sad  rating  (.94).  Sad  3  was  the  only  sad  melody  composed  completely  in  the  

major  mode  and  has  a  clear  three-­‐phrase  structure.  Sad  3,  however,  has  the  second  

lowest  density  (.89);  in  this  case,  the  combination  of  mode  and  form  seemed  to  

31  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

prevail  over  density  in  determining  emotional  affect.32  Sad  1,  with  the  lowest  

sadness  rating  of  .35,  mixes  modes  between  phrases,  not  changing  to  minor  until  its  

last  quarter.  Sad  1  also  had  the  highest  density  of  the  sad  melodies  with  a  2.02,  

which  is  higher  than  that  of  five  happy  melodies.  Major  mode  and  high  density  likely  

made  it  more  difficult  to  perceive  sadness  in  Sad  1.  Sad  8,  receiving  the  third  lowest  

sadness  rating  (1.04),  also  goes  against  the  trends  exhibited  by  the  majority  of  the  

sad  melodies.  The  overall  density  of  the  minor-­‐mode  Sad  8  is  1.44,  one  of  the  highest  

densities  for  sad  melodies.  Moreover,  Sad  8  begins  with  a  quick  arpeggiation  

followed  by  a  rapid  stepwise  descent,  all  in  the  first  measure.  The  note  density  in  

this  first  measure  is  3.45,  much  higher  than  any  other  density  not  only  in  sad  

melodies,  but  also  in  the  majority  of  happy  melodies.  The  longer  notes  in  the  last  

three  measures  of  its  four-­‐measure  length  have  a  density  of  .77,  creating  the  overall  

density  of  1.44.  Sad  8  is  a  counterexample  to  Sad  3  of  density  having  a  pronounced  

effect  over  mode.  

Two  of  these  least  sad  melodies  exhibited  a  high  note  density.  Two  melodies  

incorporated  use  of  the  major  mode.  One  was  also  composed  using  a  clear  phrase  

structure,  uncommon  to  the  saddest  melodies.  The  major  mode,  higher  note  density,  

and  simple  formal  design  appear  to  be  characteristics  of  melodies  intended  to  

portray  happiness;  the  sad  melodies  that  contained  these  characteristics  received  

the  lowest  sadness  ratings.  Yet  there  was  a  lack  of  similar  variation  within  happy  

                                                                                                               
32  This  does  not  follow  the  general  findings  of  Hunter  et  al.  (2010)  as  discussed  

above.  
32  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

melodies,  which  makes  it  more  difficult  to  pinpoint  necessary  factors  for  expressing  

happiness  in  individual  pieces.  

Results  2:  Other  Emotions  

Table  8  lists  the  ratings  for  each  emotion  in  all  ten  happy  melodies,  with  the  

mean  values  for  each  structural  feature.  Melody  names  in  the  table  have  been  

abbreviated  to  H1  for  Happy  1,  etc.  Shaded,  boldface  cells  give  the  highest  rated  

emotion  for  each  melody.  

Table  8:  Perception  Ratings  for  Happy  Melodies  

  H1   H2   H3   H4   H5   H6   H7   H8   H9   H10  
Graceful   0.71   1.04   0.81   1.47   0.76   1.23   1.34   0.73   0.92   0.85  
Happy   2.67   3.55   3.02   2.67   2.88   2.96   3.45   2.02   2.63   3.19  
Joyful   2.39   4.00   2.83   1.94   2.94   3.02   3.69   2.29   1.73   2.77  
Lively   2.43   4.53   3.19   1.06   2.14   4.71   2.92   2.63   1.73   1.94  
Triumphant   2.18   3.22   1.51   1.58   1.10   1.60   3.15   1.08   1.20   1.38  
Exciting   1.63   3.37   2.08   0.62   1.20   4.47   1.79   1.38   0.90   1.04  
                     
Solemn   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.04   0.02   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.18   0.00  
Melancholy   0.00   0.06   0.00   0.10   0.06   0.00   0.00   0.08   0.06   0.00  
Tragic   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00  
Sad   0.02   0.00   0.04   0.08   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00  
Longing   0.06   0.00   0.04   0.22   0.15   0.00   0.00   0.04   0.33   0.00  
Gloomy   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00  
                     
Density   1.89   3.03   2.63   1.88   1.35   5.13   1.50   2.17   1.35   2.08  
Mode   Major   Major   Major   Major   Major   Major   Major   Major   Major   Major  
Mean  
Interval   3.94   2.19   2.63   2.71   4.83   2.40   2.17   2.33   2.34   2.08  
Mean  Span   5.73   5.11   4.00   5.75   11.60   5.05   6.25   7.00   4.69   7.00  
Composer  
Rating   3   4   3   4   4   5   4   4   2   2  
 

33  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

The  emotions  that  received  the  highest  ratings  for  individual  happy  melodies  

were  happy,  joyful,  and  lively.  The  ratings  for  lively  garnered  the  highest  rating  

values  of  the  entire  study.  The  four  melodies  that  received  lively  as  the  highest  rated  

emotion  also  had  the  four  highest  note  densities.  Three  of  the  four  lively  melodies  

had  higher  ratings  for  joyful  compared  to  their  happy  ratings.    

The  ordering  of  happy  melodies  ranked  by  happiness  ratings  (left  column)  

compared  to  the  ordering  by  any  emotion  (middle  column)  is  similar,  as  seen  in  

Table  9  below.  Melodies  sharing  a  box  indicate  a  tie  in  the  rating.  The  right  column  

lists  the  strongest  perceived  emotion  in  each  melody,  according  to  the  ordering  of  

the  middle  column.  The  most  drastic  difference  occurred  with  Happy  6,  which  

moved  from  fifth  place  in  the  Happy  Order  to  first  place  in  the  Overall  Order.  After  

Happy  6  moved  up,  the  ordering  of  the  next  four  melodies  remained  the  same.  Note  

that  the  top  five  and  bottom  five  melodies  were  consistent  between  orderings.  

Table  9:  Comparison  Order  of  Happy  Melodies  

Happy  Order   Overall  Order   Strongest  Emotions  


Happy  2   Happy  6   Lively,  Exciting,  Joyful  
Happy  7   Happy  2   Lively,  Joyful,  Happy  
Happy  10   Happy  7   Joyful,  Happy,  Triumphant  
Happy  3   Happy  10   Happy,  Joyful,  Lively  
Happy  6   Happy  3   Lively,  Happy,  Joyful  
Happy  5   Happy  5   Joyful,  Happy,  Lively  
Happy  1   Happy  9   Happy,  Joyful,  Lively  
Happy  4   Happy  8   Lively,  Joyful,  Happy  
Happy  9   Happy  1   Happy,  Lively,  Joyful  
Happy  8   Happy  4   Happy,  Joyful,  Triumphant  
 

34  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Comparing  the  top  three  perceived  emotions  in  each  melody  sheds  light  on  

the  portrayal  of  certain  emotions.  The  top  three  emotions  in  seven  of  the  happy  

melodies  were  happy,  joyful,  and  lively  (in  different  orders  and  varying  strengths).  

The  other  three  melodies  received  at  least  two  of  these  three  in  their  top  three,  with  

exciting  and  triumphant  as  the  third  emotion.  Notably,  all  ten  had  their  joyful  rating  

in  the  top  three  perceived  emotions.    

In  six  of  the  ten  happy  melodies,  the  value  for  the  highest  rated  emotion  was  

within  one  point  of  the  composer’s  rating.  In  three  of  the  four  melodies  where  the  

composer’s  rating  was  not  within  one  point  of  the  highest  perceived  emotion  value,  

the  composer’s  rating  was  higher  than  the  mean  listener  rating.  

As  seen  below,  Table  10  lists  the  ratings  for  each  emotion  for  the  sad  

melodies  and  the  corresponding  values  for  the  structural  features.  Melody  names  

were  also  abbreviated  here,  similar  to  that  of  the  happy  melodies.  

35  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Table  10:  Perception  Ratings  for  Sad  Melodies  

  S1   S2   S3   S4   S5   S6   S7   S8   S9   S10  
Graceful   1.33   0.65   2.00   0.74   1.31   0.31   0.71   0.49   0.49   1.49  
Happy   0.92   0.06   0.71   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.20   0.00   0.09  
Joyful   0.94   0.02   0.21   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.20   0.00   0.00  
Lively   1.39   0.10   0.15   0.00   0.04   0.00   0.00   1.59   0.00   0.07  
Triumphant   0.71   0.06   0.46   0.00   0.23   0.00   0.06   0.16   0.14   0.00  
Exciting   0.76   0.08   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.00   0.04   0.76   0.08   0.00  
                     
Solemn   0.47   1.57   1.61   2.50   2.59   3.02   2.75   0.90   1.88   2.31  
Melancholy   0.76   1.88   0.85   2.51   1.98   2.55   2.59   1.35   1.60   2.39  
Tragic   0.27   1.16   0.60   2.06   1.69   3.14   2.02   0.88   1.78   2.41  
Sad   0.35   1.88   0.94   2.81   2.10   3.37   3.08   1.04   2.35   2.65  
Longing   1.65   2.29   1.81   2.63   1.71   2.27   2.47   0.82   1.84   3.14  
Gloomy   0.20   1.37   0.65   2.21   1.69   3.71   2.60   1.10   2.61   1.69  
                     
Density   2.02   1.50   0.89   1.16   1.30   0.59   1.11   1.44   1.07   1.01  
Mode   M/m   minor   Major   minor   minor   minor   minor   minor   minor   minor  
Mean  
Interval   2.25   2.36   1.83   3.43   2.17   1.81   2.28   1.93   2.70   1.86  
Mean  Span   4.24   4.00   3.44   6.00   4.88   4.22   4.10   7.00   5.40   5.78  
Composer  
Rating   2   4   3   3   3   4   4   1   4   3  
 

The  highest  perceived  emotion  values  for  the  individual  sad  melodies  were  

more  varied  than  they  were  for  the  happy  melodies.  The  emotions  that  received  the  

highest  ratings  include  solemn,  sad,  longing,  and  gloomy,  plus  two  emotions  from  

the  positive  category,  graceful  and  lively.  As  seen  in  Table  10,  there  is  much  less  

consistency  in  the  strongest  perceived  emotions  across  sad  melodies.  Ratings  of  sad  

melodies  did  not  establish  connections  between  specific  emotions  and  density.  For  

instance,  the  four  least  dense  melodies  received  longing  and  gloomy  as  their  highest  

rated  emotion,  but  longing  also  was  also  highly  rated  in  melodies  that  had  the  

highest  densities.  

36  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Six  of  the  composers'  ratings  for  their  sad  melody  were  within  one  point  of  

that  melody’s  highest  rated  emotion,  similar  to  the  findings  for  the  happy  melodies.  

Sad  8  received  its  highest  rated  emotion  in  the  positive  category;  yet  with  the  self-­‐

rating  of  1,  the  composer  seemed  cognizant  of  his  melody’s  lack  of  success  

portraying  sadness.  

  Listed  below  in  Table  11  is  the  ordering  of  the  melodies  by  sadness  ratings  

(left  column)  compared  to  the  ordering  by  the  strongest  perceived  emotion  (middle  

column)  with  their  corresponding  strongest  perceived  emotion  (right  column).  

Table  11:  Comparison  Order  of  Sad  Melodies  

Sad  Order   Overall  Order   Strongest  Emotions  


Sad  6   Sad  6   Gloomy,  Sad,  Tragic  
Sad  7   Sad  10   Longing,  Sad,  Tragic  
Sad  4   Sad  7   Sad,  Solemn,  Gloomy  
Sad  10   Sad  4   Sad,  Longing,  Melancholy  
Sad  9   Sad  9   Gloomy,  Sad,  Solemn  
Sad  5   Sad  5   Solemn,  Sad,  Melancholy  
Sad  2   Sad  2   Longing,  Sad,  Melancholy  
Sad  8   Sad  1   Longing,  Lively,  Graceful  
Sad  3   Sad  8   Lively,  Melancholy,  Gloomy  
Sad  1   Sad  3   Graceful,  Longing,  Solemn  
 

  The  only  emotion  that  was  common  within  the  top  three  perceived  emotions  

was  sadness,  appearing  in  eight  melodies.  No  other  emotion  appeared  in  the  top  

three  of  more  than  four  melodies.  Two  sad  melodies  had  a  positive  emotion  as  their  

strongest  perceived  emotion,  Sad  8  and  Sad  3.  These  two  sad  melodies,  along  with  

Sad  1,  were  the  anomalies  discussed  above.  Note  that  the  strongest  perceived  

37  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

emotion  for  Sad  1  was  longing,  but  the  second  and  third  strongest  perceived  

emotions  were  lively  and  graceful,  both  emotions  from  the  positive  category.  

Discussion  2  

Structural  Features  and  Positive  Emotions  

  After  comparing  the  differences  between  the  Happy  Order  and  the  Overall  

Order,  a  close  examination  of  specific  melodies  can  illustrate  how  each  one  contains  

a  nuanced  portrayal  of  positive  emotions.  Figure  6  below  shows  Happy  6,  the  happy  

melody  that  most  strongly  portrays  a  positive  emotion  and  had  lively,  exciting,  and  

joyful  as  the  top  three  rated  emotions.  

Figure  6:  Happy  6  


 

  Happy  6  is  by  far  the  densest  melody,  and  is  clearly  divided  into  two  phrases  

forming  a  period.  Relative  to  the  other  happy  melodies,  Happy  6  does  not,  however,  

exhibit  a  large  mean  interval  or  span  (the  fifth  highest  mean  interval  and  eighth  

highest  mean  span).  Despite  the  relatively  small  mean  interval  and  span,  Happy  6  

contains  several  prominent  factors  for  the  portrayal  of  happiness:  major  key,  high  
38  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

note  density,  a  clearly  defined  period  structure,  and  a  melodic  contour  that  ends  

higher  than  where  it  began.  

  Figure  7  shows  Happy  7,  the  second  strongest  melody  depicting  happiness.  

The  most  strongly  perceived  emotions  for  Happy  7  were  joyful,  happy,  and  

triumphant.  

Figure  7:  Happy  7    

  Like  Happy  6,  Happy  7  contains  two  clear  phrases  that  constitute  a  period.  

Happy  7  also  has  a  relatively  small  mean  interval  (ninth  largest),  but  a  somewhat  

larger  span  (fourth  largest).  Happy  7  does  contain  several  factors  for  the  portrayal  

of  happiness,  including  the  major  mode  (based  on  the  pitch  collection,  not  the  key  

signature),  a  clear  period  structure,  and  a  symmetrical  arch  shape,  but  it  has  only  

the  eighth  highest  note  density  out  of  the  ten  happy  melodies.  In  this  case,  density  

was  not  necessary  to  convey  a  high  degree  of  happiness.  

  Clearly  the  major  mode  was  a  common,  even  necessary  factor  for  happiness  

in  these  melodies.  Additionally,  four  of  the  top  five  melodies  in  the  overall  rankings  

have  four  of  the  highest  densities,  making  density  another  major  contributing  factor  
39  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

for  the  portrayal  of  happiness.  Mean  interval  and  mean  span  were  not  major  

determinants  of  happiness.  For  instance,  Happy  5  had  the  highest  mean  interval  and  

span,  but  had  the  lowest  density,  perhaps  why  Happy  5  was  sixth  in  each  order.  

  As  noted  above,  the  four  melodies  with  the  highest  lively  ratings  also  had  the  

four  highest  densities;  thus  lively  seems  to  be  an  emotion  strongly  linked  to  musical  

motion.  Within  the  happy  melodies,  as  happy  ratings  increase,  ratings  for  joyful  also  

increase,  but  at  a  faster  rate.  Generally,  joyful  was  rated  higher  than  happy  in  the  

happiest  melodies  whereas  happy  was  rated  higher  than  joyful  in  the  least  happy  

melodies.  Exciting  and  lively  also  appear  to  have  a  similar  relationship,  in  that  when  

lively  ratings  increased  exciting  ratings  did  also,  but  at  a  similar  rate;  all  ten  ratings  

for  lively  were  higher  than  the  ratings  for  excitement.  Triumphant  and  graceful,  

however,  did  not  appear  to  have  any  strong  associations  with  other  emotions  or  

structural  features.    

Structural  Features  and  Negative  Emotions  

  Sad  6  is  depicted  below  in  Figure  8.  Not  only  did  Sad  6  receive  the  highest  

sadness  rating,  but  it  also  received  the  highest  negative  rating  in  the  Overall  Order.  

The  most  strongly  perceived  emotions  for  Sad  6  are  gloomy,  sad,  and  tragic.  

Figure  8:  Sad  6  

 
40  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

  Sad  6  was  composed  in  the  minor  mode  and  had  the  lowest  density.  Sad  6  

was  also  comprised  of  the  smallest  mean  interval  and  the  third  smallest  mean  span.  

Sad  6  is  a  single  continuous  phrase,  lacking  any  clearly  definable  internal  phrase  

structure.  Without  fully  investigating  the  perceived  emotional  content  of  different  

meters,  a  brief  look  at  the  use  of  5/4  for  Sad  6  might  unearth  some  repercussions  of  

using  an  uneven  meter.  Although  Sad  6  was  the  only  melody  that  specifically  used  

an  uneven  meter,  the  asymmetry  of  5/4  ties  in  to  other  asymmetrical  aspects  of  the  

sad  melodies;  no  instances  of  parallel  periodic  structures  appeared  in  any  sad  

melodies  and  internal  phrases  within  sad  melodies  were  irregular  in  length.  Three  

additional  sad  melodies,  Sad  2,  Sad  4,  and  Sad  8,  were  composed  with  tempi  and  

rhythmic  configurations  that  likely  made  it  difficult  for  listeners  to  grasp  a  regular  

meter.  

  Figure  9  shows  Sad  10,  which  received  the  second  highest  negative  emotion  

rating  in  the  Overall  Ordering.  The  most  strongly  perceived  emotions  in  Sad  10  were  

longing,  sad,  and  tragic.  

Figure  9:  Sad  10  


 
41  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

  Unlike  Sad  6,  Sad  10  does  portray  clearly  defined  phrases  that  constitute  a  

period  as  well  as  a  relatively  large  mean  span,  characteristics  more  common  to  

happy  melodies.  Yet  Sad  10  did  have  the  third  lowest  density,  was  also  composed  in  

the  minor  mode,  and  had  the  third  smallest  mean  interval.  In  this  melody,  these  

factors  seemed  more  important  in  listeners’  perceptions  of  negative  emotions.  

  Factors  that  are  frequently  employed  to  depict  sadness  in  melodies  are  the  

minor  mode  and  low  note  densities.  Two  of  the  three  melodies  that  received  the  

lowest  ratings  for  negative  emotions  were  at  least  partially  composed  in  the  major  

mode.  Four  of  the  top  five  melodies  in  the  overall  rankings  had  the  lowest  densities.  

While  the  mean  interval  and  mean  span  did  exhibit  some  direct  associations  with  

negative  emotions,  these  two  structural  features  were  not  superordinate  in  the  

portrayal  of  negative  emotions.  

Perhaps  surprisingly,  all  sad  melodies  received  at  least  one  nonzero  rating  

for  graceful,  a  positive  emotion.  Within  the  sad  melodies,  graceful  did  not  exhibit  

any  relationships  to  any  other  perceived  emotion  or  structural  feature.  Gracefulness  

could  be  largely  attributed  to  the  flowing  nature  of  many  melodies,  but  the  portrayal  

of  gracefulness  in  melodies  seems  to  have  underlying  characteristics  that  were  not  

uncovered  in  these  analyses.  Gracefulness  itself  may  be  of  neutral  valence,  and  so  

can  readily  be  associated  with  both  positive  and  negative  emotions.  

Even  discounting  the  prominent  graceful  ratings  and  the  two  melodies  that  

received  a  positive  emotion  as  the  strongest  perceived  emotion,  sad  melodies  

received  a  greater  number  of  ratings  in  happy  emotions  than  happy  melodies  did  in  

42  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

sad  emotions.  As  in  the  happy  melodies,  sad  melodies  of  higher  note  density  

generally  received  higher  ratings  for  liveliness.  The  one  exception  here  is  again  the  

major-­‐mode  Sad  3;  Sad  3  had  the  second  lowest  note  density  but  received  the  third  

highest  lively  rating  (.15).  In  the  presence  of  the  major  mode,  perhaps  density  is  not  

required  for  the  perception  of  some  liveliness.  When  comparing  the  major-­‐mode  

Sad  3  to  the  happy  melodies,  the  lively  rating  and  density  for  Sad  3  follow  the  

ordering  of  the  other  happy  melodies.  

Overall  rankings  of  negative  emotions  emerge  based  on  their  relative  

strengths  in  individual  melodies.  Sadness  tended  to  receive  the  highest  ratings,  

suggesting  that  sadness  was  easily  perceptible.  Ratings  for  melancholy  were  

generally  lower  than  sadness  ratings,  gloomy  ratings  were  lower  than  melancholy  

ratings,  and  ratings  for  tragic  tended  to  be  the  lowest  ratings  out  of  these  four  

emotions.  Gloomy  and  tragic  might  seem  to  be  extreme  versions  of  sad,  like  joyful  

was  to  happy,  but  no  clear  relationships  emerge  from  the  ratings  of  these  emotions.  

43  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Chapter  IV  

Conclusion  

  The  two  experiments  in  this  study  established  relationships  between  

different  structural  features  and  how  they  aid  in  the  depiction  of  different  emotions  

in  melody.  Based  on  these  findings,  the  strongest  correlations  exist  between  the  

main  target  emotions,  happy  and  sad,  and  both  mode  and  note  density,  confirming  

the  long-­‐standing  association  between  emotion  and  mode33  as  well  as  the  notable  

influence  note  density  has  on  perceived  emotion.34  The  melodic  contour  also  differs  

notably  between  conditions,  although  no  significance  tests  were  possible  with  the  

current  data  set.  A  weaker,  yet  still  important,  factor  exists  in  the  regularity  of  

formal  design,  especially  among  the  top  five  rated  melodies  in  each  condition.  

  Two  structural  features  that  were  not  significantly  correlated  with  emotion  

intent  or  perception  were  the  mean  interval  and  the  mean  span.  There  were,  

however,  some  observable  patterns  relating  larger  intervals  and  spans  to  happy  

melodies,  and  the  mean  interval  and  span  were  correlated  with  each  other.  The  lack  

of  significance  between  these  structural  features  and  the  mean  rating  may  suggest  

that  these  are  invariant  structural  features  of  tonal  melodies  that  do  not  contribute  

significantly  to  the  portrayal  of  emotions.  

  This  study  has  provided  a  detailed  account  of  how  well  individual  structural  

features  found  in  melody  contribute  to  the  depiction  of  emotion.  Other  music  and  

emotion  studies  account  for  differences  within  individual  primary  musical  


                                                                                                               
33  cf.  Kastner  and  Crowder,  “Perceptions  of  the  Major/Minor  Distinction.”  
34  cf.  Hunter  et  al,  “Feelings  and  Perceptions.”  

44  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

parameters,  but  they  do  not  investigate  the  detailed  structural  features  analyzed  

above.35  A  detailed  analysis  of  the  structural  features  within  melody  provides  those  

interested  in  the  portrayal  of  emotion  in  music  with  a  more  refined  view  of  the  

relationships  between  emotion  and  melody.  From  these  results,  we  can  also  

investigate  how  melodies  can  be  adapted  and  manipulated  to  portray  different  

emotions.  This  information  would  be  particularly  interesting  for  composers  and  

arrangers.  Performers  must  also  have  an  understanding  of  the  structural  features  

and  how  they  relate  to  the  portrayal  of  emotions  to  realize  their  musical  goals.  

While  performers  do  not  have  the  freedom  that  composers  do  to  incorporate  certain  

structural  features,  they  do  have  the  ability  to  manipulate  tempo,  and  thus  note  

density.  A  performer  may  opt  to  change  the  tempo  throughout  certain  sections  of  a  

piece  in  order  to  control  the  portrayal  of  emotion  and  carry  the  listener  through  a  

specific  emotional  journey  in  the  music.  

  Finally,  it  is  important  to  note  that  the  perceptual  data  explored  above  were  

obtained  with  expressionless  MIDI  files.  The  expression  of  a  human  performance  

would  undoubtedly  enhance  the  portrayal  of  emotion  in  these  melodies,  and  thus  

the  above  findings  might  be  viewed  as  especially  robust.  Yet  the  use  of  

computerized  sounds  controlled  for  the  effects  of  performance  expression,  allowing  

this  study  to  focus  solely  on  the  inherent  structural  features  of  the  melodies.  

                                                                                                               
35  See  Gabrielsson,  “Musical  Structure  and  Perceived  Emotions,”  and  Alf  Gabrielsson  

and  Patrik  N.  Juslin,  “Emotional  Expression  in  Music,”  in  Handbook  of  Affective  
Sciences,  ed.  Richard  Davidson,  Klaus  R.  Scherer,  and  H.  Hill  Goldsmith  (New  York:  
Oxford  University  Press,  2003).  
45  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Chapter  V  

Future  Research  

In  the  larger  area  of  music  and  emotion,  this  study  provides  a  basis  from  

which  to  investigate  other  prominent  parameters  of  music,  hopefully  culminating  in  

larger  studies  on  ways  music  and  emotion  are  linked.  To  examine  more  closely  how  

these  structural  features  contribute  to  the  portrayal  of  emotions,  these  findings  

should  be  replicated  and  expanded.  First  and  perhaps  most  obviously,  a  perceptual  

test  of  these  melodies  should  be  given  to  groups  of  non-­‐musicians.  Testing  non-­‐

musicians  would  provide  further  evidence  as  to  which  structural  features  help  

portray  each  emotion  and  that  the  present  results  are  not  merely  the  artifact  of  

formal  music  training.  

In  addition  to  correlating  mean  happy  and  sad  ratings  to  structural  features,  

statistical  analyses  could  be  done  relating  each  individual  emotion  to  structural  

features  and  to  other  emotions.  A  larger  number  of  melodies  could  possibly  

generate  stronger  correlations  between  structural  features  and  emotion  ratings,  as  

well  as  uncovering  stronger  effects  of  structural  features  themselves.  

To  make  stronger  correlations  between  mode  and  note  density,  the  melodies  

could  be  manipulated  following  the  methodology  from  Hunter,  Schellenberg,  and  

Schimmack.  Excerpts  in  their  study  were  altered  to  be  played  in  both  the  major  and  

minor  modes,  and  at  a  slow  and  a  fast  tempo  (the  fast  tempo  being  exactly  twice  as  

fast  as  the  slow  tempo).  Manipulating  each  melody  in  this  fashion  would  provide  

46  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

detailed  results  on  the  relative  strength  of  mode  and  note  density  in  the  perception  

of  emotion.  

  By  providing  a  treble  clef  on  the  composer’s  sheets,  this  study  controlled  

somewhat  for  variance  in  range.  Allowing  composers  to  choose  the  clef,  and  thus  the  

range,  certainly  would  have  allowed  for  additional  opportunities  for  emotional  

content.  Further  manipulations  of  these  melodies  into  different  ranges  would  be  an  

additional  method  to  study  more  precisely  emotional  content  in  melodies.  The  

instrumentation  of  the  melodies  could  also  be  altered  in  order  to  study  the  effect  of  

timbre.  In  particular,  the  melodies  that  contained  longer  note  values  might  benefit  

from  a  change  in  timbre  to  prevent  an  unmusical  decay  on  those  notes.  A  change  to  

an  instrument  that  sustains  note  values  could  provide  the  listeners  with  

continuously  sounding  melodies,  possibly  making  some  of  the  melodies  more  

idiomatic.  

47  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Bibliography  

Arnheim,  Rudolf.  “Perceptual  Dynamics  in  Musical  Expression.”  The  Musical  


Quarterly  70,  no.  3  (Summer  1984):  295-­‐309.  
 
Brower,  Candace.  “A  Cognitive  theory  of  Musical  Meaning.”  Journal  of  Music  Theory  
44,  no.  2  (Autumn  2000):  323-­‐379.  
 
Collier,  William  and  Timothy  Hubbard.  “Musical  Scales  and  Evaluations  of  Happiness  
and  Awkwardness:  Effects  of  Pitch,  Direction,  and  Scale  Mode.”  The  American  
Journal  of  Psychology  113,  no.  3  (Autumn  2001):  355-­‐375.  
 
Downey,  June  E.  “A  Musical  Experiment.”  The  American  Journal  of  Psychology  9,  no.  1  
(Oct.  1897):  63-­‐69.  
 
Ekman,  Paul.  “An  Argument  for  Basic  Emotions.”  Cognition  and  Emotion  6  (3/4  
1992):  169-­‐200.  
 
Gabrielsson,  Alf.  “The  Relationship  between  Musical  Structure  and  Perceived  
Expression.”  In  The  Oxford  Handbook  of  Music  Psychology,  edited  by  Susan  
Hallam,  Ian  Cross,  and  Michael  Thaut,  141-­‐150.  New  York:  Oxford  University  
Press,  2009.  
 
Gabrielsson,  Alf  and  Patrik  N.  Juslin.  “Emotional  Expression  in  Music.”  In  Handbook  
of  Affective  Sciences.  Edited  by  Richard  Davidson,  Klaus  R.  Scherer,  and  H.  Hill  
Goldsmith,  457-­‐480.  New  York:  Oxford  University  Press,  2003.  
 
Gilman,  Benjamin  Ives.  “Report  on  an  Experimental  Test  of  Musical  Expressiveness  
(Continued).”  The  American  Journal  of  Psychology  5,  no.  1  (Oct.  1892):  42-­‐73.    
 
Hevner,  Kate.  “Experimental  Studies  of  the  Elements  of  Expression  in  Music.”  The  
American  Journal  of  Psychology  48,  no.  2  (April  1936):  246-­‐268.  
 
Hoffmann,  E.  T.  A.  E.  T.  A.  Hoffmann’s  Musical  Writings.  Edited  by  David  Charleton.  
Translated  by  Martyn  Clarke.  New  York:  Cambridge  University  Press,  1989.  
 
Hunter,  Patrick  G.,  E.  Glenn  Schellenberg,  and  Ulrich  Schimmack.  “Feelings  and  
Perceptions  of  Happiness  and  Sadness  Induced  by  Music:  Similarities,  
Differences,  and  Mixed  Emotions.”  Psychology  of  Aesthetics,  Creativity,  and  the  
Arts  4,  no.  1  (2010):  47-­‐56.  
 
Huron,  David.  “The  Melodic  Arch  in  Western  Folksongs.”  Computing  in  Musicology  
10  (1996):  3-­‐23.  
———.  Sweet  Anticipation.  Cambridge:  The  MIT  Press,  2006.  

48  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

International  Music  Score  Library  Project.  “Seikilos  Epitaph  (Ancient  Greek).”  Last  
modified  Oct.  30,  2009.  
http://imslp.org/wiki/Seikilos_Epitaph_(Ancient_Greek).  
 
Juslin,  Patrik  N.  “Emotional  Responses  to  Music.”  In  The  Oxford  Handbook  of  Music  
Psychology,  edited  by  Susan  Hallam,  Ian  Cross,  and  Michael  Thaut,  131-­‐140.  
New  York:  Oxford  University  Press,  2009.  
 
Juslin,  Patrik  N.  and  Petri  Laukka.  “Expression,  Perception,  and  Induction  of  Musical  
Emotion:  A  Review  and  a  Questionnaire  Study  of  Everyday  Listening.”  Journal  
of  New  Music  Research  33,  no.  3  (2004):  217-­‐238.  
 
Juslin,  Patrik  N.,  Simon  Liljestrom,  Daniel  Vastfjall,  and  Lard-­‐Olov  Lundqvist.  “How  
does  Music  Evoke  Emotions?”  In  Handbook  of  Music  and  Emotion:  Theory,  
Research,  and  Applications,  Edited  by  Patrik  N.  Juslin  and  John  Sloboda,  605-­‐
644.  New  York:  Oxford  University  Press,  2010.  
 
Kastner,  Marianna  Pinochot  and  Robert  G.  Crowder.  “Perception  of  the  Major/Minor  
Distinction:  IV.  Emotional  Connotations  in  Young  Children.”  Music  Perception  
8,  no.  2  (Winter  1990):  189-­‐201.  
 
Nemett,  Barry.  “Two-­‐Dimensional  Materials  and  Techniques.”  In  Images,  Objects,  and  
Ideas:  Viewing  the  Visual  Arts.  New  York:  Harcourt  Brace  Jovanovich  College  
Publishers,  1992.  
 
Sloboda,  John.  “Empirical  Studies  of  Emotional  Response  to  Music.”  In  Cognitive  
Bases  of  Musical  Communication,  edited  by  Mari  Riess  Jones  and  Susan  
Holleran,  33-­‐46.  Washington  D.C.:  American  Psychological  Society,  1992.  
 
Thompson,  William  Forde  and  Brent  Robitaille.  “Can  Composers  Express  Emotions  
through  Music?”  Empirical  Studies  of  the  Arts  10,  no.  1  (1992):  79-­‐89.  
 
Wagner,  Richard.  Wagner  on  Music  and  Drama:  A  Compendium  of  Richard  Wagner’s  
Prose  Works.  Translated  by  H.  Ashton  Ellis.  Selected  and  arranged  with  an  
introduction  by  Albert  Goldman  and  Evert  Sprinchorn.  New  York:  E.  P.  
Dutton  &  Co.,  Inc.,  1964.  
 
Wells,  Alan  and  Ernest  A.    Hakanen.  “The  Emotional  Use  of  Popular  Music  by  
Adolescents.”  Journalism  Quarterly  68,  no.  3  (Fall  1991):  445-­‐454.  
 
Zetner,  Marcel,  Didier  Grandjean,  and  Klaus  R.  Scherer.  “Emotions  Evoked  by  the  
Sound  of  Music:  Characterization,  Classification,  and  Measurement.”  Emotion  
8,  no.  4  (2008):  494-­‐521.

49  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Appendix  A  
 
Melodies  

 
Happy  1  

 
 
Happy  2  

 
 
Happy  3  

 
 
Happy  4  

 
 
Happy  5  

 
 

50  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Happy  6  

 
 
Happy  7  

 
 
Happy  8  

 
 
Happy  9  

 
 
Happy  10  

 
51  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Sad  1  

 
 
Sad  2  

 
 
Sad  3  

 
 
Sad  4  

 
 
Sad  5  

 
 
Sad  6  

 
 

52  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Sad  7  

 
 
Sad  8  

 
 
Sad  9  

 
 
Sad  10  

53  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Appendix  B  
 
IRB  Exemption  Letter  

 
 

54  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Appendix  C  
 
T-­Tests  

55  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Appendix  D  
 
Correlations  Within  All  Melodies  

56  
Texas  Tech  University,  Megan  Trenck,  August  2011  

Appendix  E  
 
Correlations  by  Condition  

 
 
Valence  -­‐1  refers  to  sad  melodies  and  1  refers  to  happy  melodies.  

57  

You might also like