You are on page 1of 10

Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy

Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 5, 145±154 (1998)

Changes in Confidence and Craving


During Smoking Reduction
Kieron O'Connor* and Robert Langlois
Centre de recherche Fernand-Seguin, HoÃpital Louis-H. Lafontaine,
Montreal, Canada

This article reports two studies designed to explore patterns of change


in confidence about not smoking during gradual smoking reduction. In
the first study, trends in self-reported smoking, craving and confidence
were measured over baseline, 10 attendance sessions and at 2-month
follow-up in a group receiving situationally tailored coping strategies,
and two comparison groups receiving either a uniform coping strategy
(relaxation therapy) or no systematic coping strategy. The coping
strategy group showed a more consistent increase in confidence, but all
groups were able to cut out smoking, situation by situation, without
corresponding increases in craving in other situations. In a second
study, 60 smokers were assigned to one of three gradual reduction
groups emphasizing: self-efficacy or behavioural coping skills, or a
third control programme. Confidence in not smoking increased during
treatment in the self-efficacy and behavioural coping groups, and
initial situational differences in confidence had flattened out post-
treatment. Higher confidence at follow-up correlated significantly with
lower saliva cotinine level. In conclusion, situational reduction can
be accomplished systematically and is associated, in some subjects,
with an overall increase in confidence in coping with no smoking.
# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

INTRODUCTION smoking may become more attractive, as a con-


sequence of fewer cigarettes being smoked (Sobell
Health facts dictate cessation as the goal of choice
et al., 1990). An alternative view is that the smoker
for smokers. But, at the outset many smokers do not
may find reducing a less aversive way of quitting
consider this a realistic goal, and may fear the
(Hughes et al., 1984). Although there have been
commitment and the effects of abrupt cessation
studies showing comparable outcome in graded
(Owen et al., 1992). One obvious possibility is for
versus abrupt cessation (Glasgow et al., 1989), there
these smokers to cut down rather than stop com-
have, to our knowledge, been no studies examining
pletely and, with the increasing social margin-
cognitive changes relevant to quitting, during the
alization of smoking, more and more smokers are
actual process of smoking reduction. Clinically, for
becoming (or, are being forced to become) light
example, we have noted that successfully cutting
situation or period specific smokers. However, a
down may increase the smoker's confidence in the
major objection to smoking reduction as a means to
ability to cope with not smoking. If such confidence
cessation is that relapse is more likely and that
was greater after, than before, reduction, this result
could suggest that self-efficacy about coping with
*Correspondence to: K. O'Connor, Centre de recherche quitting had also increased. Self-efficacy (Bandura,
Fernand-Seguin, HoÃpital Louis-H. Lafontaine, 7331 Hochel-
aga, MontreÂal, QueÂbec, Canada H1N 3V2. 1982) has been reported as a main predictor of both
adherence and abstinence during a cessation pro-
Contract grant sponsor: Conseil QueÂbeÂcois de la Recherche
Sociale; Contract grant number: RS-1421-1653.
gramme (O'Leary, 1985; Yates and Thain, 1985).
Contract grant sponsor: Fonds de la Recherche en Sante du West and Schneider (1988) reported that lower self-
QueÂbec. efficacy was correlated with higher craving during

CCC 1063±3995/98/030145±10$17.50
# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
146 K. O'Connor and R. Langlois

cessation, and, according to Marlatt and Gordon (SDn718.8); uniform coping (relaxation), xÅ 39.4
(1985), self-efficacy is a central factor that can have a (SDn717.5); control, xÅ 40.5 (SDn7111.9). Cigarettes
major influence in relapse. per day: active coping, xÅ 32.1 (SDn7114.3); uni-
But, confidence in not smoking could be affected form coping, xÅ 29.8 (SDn719.4); control, xÅ 27.1
by perceived ability to cope with not smoking, and (SDn7110.7). Motivation to participate was assessed
so self-efficacy may also depend on what the smoker by initial interview, and rated on a scale of (0±10),
does whilst reducing and, in particular, whether on the basis of realistic expectations about the
coping strategies are adopted or not. There seem to programme, commitment to attend meetings and
be wide variations in the use of coping strategies, do homework exercises, and the absence of illness,
particularly during the early phases of cessation or difficulties or stress likely to impede participation:
fading (Shiffman, 1982, 1984, 1989). Coping with active coping, xÅ 7.2 (SDn711.2); relaxation,
craving might be managed more effectively during xÅ 7.3(SDn710.9); control xÅ 7.2 (SDn711.3).
reduction, with the help of coping skills than
without such skills, as has been shown in the case Measures
of preventing relapse after abrupt cessation (Hall Number of Cigarettes Smoked per Day
et al., 1984; Bliss et al., 1989; Stevens and Hollis, The number of cigarettes smoked per day was
1989). evaluated using a daily self-monitoring procedure.
In the present two studies, changes in cognitive Cards were specially printed for this purpose listing
appraisals of confidence about not smoking, whilst a column for trigger situation, time, and craving
gradually reducing smoking, were examined along- rating (1±5). The cards fitted into the cigarettes
side concomitant changes in craving to smoke, and packets. A baseline measure of 1 week's monitoring
self-report changes in arousal. The first study was taken at 1 month, and at 1 week before the
examined whether specific coping strategies were first session. Subjects continued self-monitoring
necessary to increase levels of confidence in not throughout the 10 weeks. Smoking status was also
smoking whilst reducing, and the second study evaluated by self-monitoring for a 1-week period at
evaluated the impact of a cognitive intervention, post-treatment, and at two follow-up sessions:
specifically targeting self-efficacy. 2 months and 6 months post-treatment. An hier-
The specific hypothesis of the first study was that archy of high craving cues and situations was
a group applying coping strategies would show a constructed individually for each smoker.
linear increase in confidence and a decrease in At baseline, after each group session throughout
craving during gradual reduction, and that neither a treatment, and at post-treatment, and at follow-up,
uniform coping approach which took no account of subjects completed the following three question-
coping specificity, nor an approach which gave no naires measuring: craving, confidence and arousal.
coping training at all, would show a consistent
trend in confidence or craving. Self-Report Craving Questionnaire
This questionnaire listed 12 categories of everyday
smoking cues and situations. These were: after a
meal, when drinking alcohol, taking a work break,
STUDY 1
when relaxing, when involved in a pleasurable
activity, when watching TV, when bored, when
Method
conversing, when doing complicated work, when
Recruitment waiting for something, when emotionally stressed,
Subjects were recruited via an internal advertise- when concentrating. Subjects were requested to rate
ment in a local hospital inviting staff who desire to smoke in each situation from 0 (never
were smokers to participate in a free 10 weeks smoke) to 100 (smoke always).
smoking cessation programme. The programme was
announced as suitable for those who wanted to cut Confidence About Not Smoking Questionnaire
down smoking gradually. Thirty-three smokers This questionnaire listed the same 12 items of the
were randomly assigned to three groups of 11. The Situational Questionnaire but subjects had to rate
groups were matched for age, sex, length of habit, from 0 (uncertain)±100 (certain) their degree of
motivation and type of situational preference, confidence in being able to not smoke in the
personality scores of extraversion and neuroticism different situations. (The scale was adapted from
(measured by the Eysenck Personality Question- the format validated by Condiotte and Lichtenstein,
naire) (Eysenck, 1980). Age: active coping, xÅ 42.0 1981; O'Connor and Langlois, 1992).

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 5, 145±154 (1998)
Confidence During Smoking Reduction 147

Thayer Stress Arousal Questionnaire manner. At the end of treatment, subjects com-
This was a 38-item adjective checklist which has pleted questionnaires detailing the types of strat-
been psychophysiologically validated (Thayer, egies offered in the programme, with ratings of how
1967; Mackay, 1980). The responses were divided useful they had found them. The active coping
into four categories of high and low stress, and group clearly used more active coping strategies
high and low arousal items. High stress and low than the other two groups, which confirmed the
arousal items included mood items reflecting mood differential effects of therapy. All 10 of the active
correlates of withdrawal (e.g. irritation, depression, coping group reported use of at least one beha-
stress) as given in Hughes and Hatsukami (1986). vioural coping strategy, of whom nine found it
useful, compared to six (67%) of the relaxation
Treatment group, of whom four found it useful, and two (25%)
Two weeks were spent on preliminary exercises of the control group, of whom one found it useful.
to enhance awareness of cues for smoking, and to All nine of the relaxation group had specifically
acclimatize the smoker to the rationale of the used what they considered to be relaxation, of
programme, and to the plan of graded cessation. whom eight found it useful compared to nine (90%)
The format of all treatments was a group format. of the active coping, of whom seven found it useful,
The smoking group leader (RL) was the same for all and five (62%) of the control group, of whom three
groups and was independent and impartial with found it useful. Only the relaxation group reported
regard to treatment modality. All groups consisted significant changes in stress and arousal over
of 10 weekly, 90 min reunions, followed by two treatment (see Results). Satisfaction with the group
follow-up sessions at 2 and 6 months post-treat- was also rated (0±5) at the end of treatment, by
ment. In each of the three groups, a gradual means of a questionnaire addressing expectations,
reduction approach was adopted. The experimental therapist style and the method used. Overall mean
group (A) involved tailoring active coping strategies ratings were comparable for the three groups
according to the smoking situation analysis. The (A ˆ 4.4; R ˆ 4.3; C ˆ 4.5).
person was encouraged to cope with high risk
smoking situations by stress management tech-
Analysis
niques, or by changing their habitual response
in the situation, or by replanning and restructuring At the final analysis, the complete data of
the task, or by physically, or mentally substituting 27 smokers was available. This comprised 10 in
the cigarette by another pleasurable activity. The the active coping group (A), nine in the relaxation
strategies chosen depended on the nature of the group (R), and eight in the control group (C). Data
situation and the needs and convenience for on number of cigarettes smoked was available for
the smoker. The coping strategies were rehearsed both baseline sessions (baseline 1 and baseline 2) for
in the group, and as homework, before being all 10 attendance sessions, and for 2-month, and
implemented in a systematic fashion over the 6-month follow-up. Data on arousal, craving, and
weeks of gradual cessation. The uniform strategy confidence were collected at baseline 2, and from
group (R) practised relaxation in all situations. The second to ninth sessions inclusive, and at 2-month
basic relaxation procedure was a Jacobsen type follow-up. The pre±post questionnaire measures
procedure including tensing and relaxing arm, leg, were collected at baseline 2, and at 2-month follow-
trunk and face muscles coupled with breathing and up. Repeated measures ANOVA, and regression
imagery instruction. After mastery of the procedure, analyses, are reported as F ratios. Where F-values
subjects learnt to apply relaxation strategies in vivo. refer to individual treatment groups they are
Each week the participants rehearsed, and imple- abbreviated for clarity of reference in the text to
mented, relaxation strategies in place of smoking, in F(A), F(R), F(C) respectively.
the targeted smoking situation. The control group (C)
did not rehearse any coping strategies. Some
Results
general strategies were however detailed including,
creating a delay, breathing, changing situations, There were no differences at baseline, between
avoiding cues, drinking water. These strategies groups, in any of the following self-reported
were given since it was suspected that smokers smoking characteristics: amount smoked; age of
would already be familiar with some of these aids. starting; length of habit; personality.
Some smokers in the control group did in fact Number of cigarettes was self-monitored at two
employ substitution strategies, but in a haphazard baseline levels (on first interview, and 1 week prior

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 5, 145±154 (1998)
148 K. O'Connor and R. Langlois

to treatment) and at each successive week of also a series of quadratic and cubic trends over
10 treatments, plus a 2-month (F2), and a 6-month sessions, which hence makes the results difficult to
follow-up. There was a non-significant decrease in interpret (see Table 1).
number of cigarettes recorded, from baseline 1 to The coping group showed an overall linear
baseline 2. Baseline 2 (B2) was considered as the increase in confidence in not smoking for six of
pre-treatment measure. All groups reported a linear the 12 situations (F(A) ˆ 7.95; p 5 0.01) (after a
reduction over sessions. (Active coping (F(1,9 meal (F(A) ˆ 5.82; p 5 0.02), concentration (F(A) ˆ
A) ˆ 18.65; p 5 0.01; relaxation F(1,8 R) ˆ 14.72; 8.83; p 5 0.01), during conversation (F(A) ˆ 4.78;
p 5 0.01; control group F(1,7 C) ˆ 7.07; p 5 0.05). p 5 0.04), doing complicated work (F(A) ˆ 6.94;
There were no differences between groups. Mean p 5 0.01), waiting for something (F(A) ˆ 5.92;
number of cigarettes reported smoked per group p 5 0.02) and under emotional stress (F(A) ˆ 13.94;
was as follows: group A, B2 xÅ ˆ 24.22 (SDn719.70), p 5 0.01). The control group showed a linear
F2 xÅ ˆ 7.72 (SDn715.99); group R, B2 xÅ ˆ 18.91 increase in confidence for three situations only
(SDn715.19), F2 xÅ ˆ 5.13 (SDn716.05); group C, B2 (F(C) ˆ 11.35; p 5 0.01) (after a meal (F(C) ˆ 16.26;
xÅ ˆ 19.17 (SDn715.77), F2 xÅ ˆ 6.67 (SDn717.67). At p 5 0.01), waiting for something (F(C) ˆ 6.56;
6-month follow-up four of the active coping p 5 0.03) and stress (F(C) ˆ 14.96; p 5 0.01)). The
group (40%), two of the relaxation group (22%) relaxation group showed a non-significant increase
and one of the control group (12.5%) reported they in confidence.
had stopped smoking. The stress-arousal adjective checklist was divided
Regression analysis measured the trends in into four subscales of high stress, low stress, high
situational craving from B2 through sessions 2±9 to arousal, and low arousal after Mackay (1980). High
F2 for the 12 items on the craving questionnaire. All stress included such items as, bothered, jittery,
groups showed a pronounced craving profile across nervous, fearful, which might be considered as
smoking situations at baseline which was not high stress. Only the relaxation group showed a
present at follow-up. Mean craving and confidence linear reduction in high stress over treatment
over all 12 situations for each group at B2 and at F2 (F(R) ˆ 3.98; p 5 0.05). Low stress included the
is given in Table 1. The relaxation group showed a opposite end of the stress dimension such as,
linear trend across sessions for reduced craving peaceful, relaxed, contended. There was no consist-
(F)R) ˆ 5.21, p 5 0.01). Reduction in craving was ent change across treatment sessions for any group.
significant for this group at final session High arousal included items such as, energetic,
(F(R) ˆ 13.12; p 5 0.01) and at F2 (F(R) ˆ 24.84; vigorous, alert, stimulated. The relaxation group,
p 5 0.01) as compared to B2. The coping group only, showed a significant linear increase
also showed a significant reduction at across sessions (F(R) ˆ 10.97; p 5 0.02). Low arousal
F2(F(A) ˆ 9.70 p 5 0.05) with a linear trend over included items such as, drowsy, tired, idle, sluggish,
sessions (F(A) ˆ 14.81; p 5 0.001). The control and showed no group differences, or trends over
group showed a reduction in craving at F2, but treatment.

Table 1. Mean change over 12 smoking situations and standard deviation (n 7 1) in % craving to smoke and %
confidence in not smoking for each treatment group over weekly treatment sessions and 2-month follow-up for Study 1

Active coping group Relaxation group Control group


(N ˆ 10) (N ˆ 9) (N ˆ 8)

Craving Confidence Craving Confidence Craving Confidence


(0±100) (0±100) (0±100) (0±100) (0±100) (0±100)
Pre-treatment 74.62 (5.43) 59.67 (19.27) 74.60 (15.67) 53.15 (16.58) 72.50 (9.61) 51.04 (17.58)
Session 2 73.17 (5.85) 55.54 (15.35) 72.92 (15.98) 56.76 (17.15) 58.02 (17.44) 63.44 (18.17)
Session 3 67.31 (9.73) 55.46 (11.83) 65.00 (16.29) 60.46 (13.16) 50.00 (18.36) 65.52 (16.12)
Session 4 61.66 (10.25) 60.74 (10.32) 57.13 (16.05) 63.39 (16.15) 56.17 (8.71) 57.67 (13.36)
Session 5 60.52 (7.94) 59.63 (9.97) 52.41 (15.00) 74.72 (4.23) 43.12 (19.10) 64.45 (18.75)
Session 6 50.77 (15.30) 62.86 (14.47) 51.96 (19.01) 66.79 (15.21) 46.76 (19.19) 62.20 (13.49)
Session 7 48.33 (22.56) 66.30 (18.05) 41.69 (22.33) 75.07 (12.83) 36.11 (20.17) 76.94 (14.47)
Session 8 48.28 (17.89) 67.45 (10.34) 38.17 (25.06) 77.83 (16.76) 47.74 (23.61) 69.64 (16.87)
Session 9 47.27 (22.61) 74.17 (17.95) 35.76 (20.34) 77.36 (14.29) 27.38 (24.36) 72.98 (31.35)
Follow-up 35.17 (28.48) 88.17 (35.64) 21.01 (22.80) 64.58 (39.79) 18.18 (22.18) 85.68 (15.01)

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 5, 145±154 (1998)
Confidence During Smoking Reduction 149

Discussion preference, age, sex, and motivation to quit. Age:


SET, xÅ 37.7 (SDn716.3), BST, xÅ 37.7 (SDn717.8), COT,
The principal hypothesis that smokers receiving
xÅ 40.6 (SDn716.2); cigarettes per day: SET, xÅ 20.11
coping strategies would show a more uniform
SDn716.7), BST, xÅ 24.6 (SDn716.9), COT, xÅ 19.3
increase in confidence, and decrease in self-reported
(SDn714.6). Sixty smokers began the treatment
craving, during a graded reduction programme,
programme (20 per group). Post-treatment data
than would a comparative group receiving either
was collected on 49 of these (SET ˆ 19, BST ˆ 15,
uniform strategies, or no specific strategies, did
COT ˆ 15) and 3-month follow-up data on 44
receive some support. The coping group reported a
(SET ˆ 16, BST ˆ 14, COT ˆ 14) and 9-month
linear increase in confidence in more situations,
follow-up on 42 (SET ˆ 15, BST ˆ 13, COT ˆ 14).
over the 10 sessions, than did the other groups.
Drop-out characteristics are discussed later.
However, all groups reported a linear reduction in
the amount smoked over sessions, and an overall
decrease in craving to smoke. Craving and reported Treatment
use did not increase in any group, in other Two subgroups of (initially) 10 subjects were run,
situations, when one situation was eliminated. by the same therapist, for all conditions. The
The focus of this study was on the self-reported programme lasted 10 weeks, and followed essen-
cognitive changes within each group during the tially the same structure and time schedule as the
process of reduction. The number of subjects was first study, but naturally there were differences in
too small and the self-report measures of smoking the way this programme was implemented in the
insufficient to permit any conclusion of the com- cognitive (SET) groups and behavioural (BST)
parative efficacy of the treatment modalities. groups. The behavioural skills group concentrated on
implementing behavioural dissociations and substi-
tutions in smoking situations, according to the
active coping rationale of the first study. In the
STUDY 2
SET group, coping with situations was approached,
Another intervention that might selectively increase exclusively, from the point of view of raising belief
confidence in not smoking, during smoking in ability to quit. The cognitive training concentrated
reduction, is self-efficacy training. The second on developing belief in personal competence to cope
study was designed to test this possibility, by with abstinence. Traditional cognitive methods,
comparing a group of smokers receiving a purely such as challenging irrational expectations, de-
behavioural coping skill package (BST), with a dramatizing outcome, and reality testing to dispel
group receiving cognitive therapy aimed at increas- automatic assumptions, were employed to focus on
ing self-efficacy, (SET) and, with another control situations where difficulties were anticipated. So a
group offered the standard package in the first typical self-efficacy training began by eliciting auto-
study without being exclusively focused either on matic doubts about the ability to control craving,
behavioural skill or efficacy (COT). The aim of and then analysed, in more detail, beliefs and
Study 2 was to examine the contribution of self- expectancies such doubts entailed, e.g. What exactly
efficacy training to improve confidence in coping did the person believe would happen if they did not
during smoking reduction. The hypothesis was that smoke? What were alternative possibilities to
increased self-efficacy training would result in negative expectations? The person was encouraged
reports of higher self-confidence in coping with to reality test beliefs in the situation and to separate
not smoking during reduction, as compared with a fact from fiction. The experiences of abstinence were
behavioural skill group which learnt coping skills, related to past experiences of successful problem
but without specific cognitive training aimed at solving. In particular, attempts were made to shift
enhancing efficacy. attributions from characteriological interpretations
of difficulties (e.g. I always fail) to behavioural
interpretation (e.g. I sometimes fail when faced with
Method
situations I didn't foresee) and to emphasize that
Recruitment causes of difficulties were personally changeable.
Sixty-six smokers were recruited from the general The smoking reduction hierarchy of situations was
population. Sixty (20 per group) participated and derived, exclusively, from ratings of confidence
the groups were matched for sex, age, smoker about not smoking in different situations. The
characteristics, and personality. All groups were smokers received no training in behavioural coping
comparable in terms of type of situational smoking skills.

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 5, 145±154 (1998)
150 K. O'Connor and R. Langlois

Measures validation of smoking status for both plasma and


The situational craving, confidence, and stress- saliva nicotine.
arousal instruments, and the self-monitoring
smoking cards, were identical to the first study.
Saliva nicotine samples were tested by measure- Results
ment of cotinine levels and were collected from all All groups showed a linear decrease in number of
smokers at all attendance sessions, but not all these cigarettes recorded, for all smoking situations, over
samples were analysed, and a complete analysis therapy sessions. There were no differences among
was limited to verifying smoking status post- groups in this decrease up to post-treatment (SP).
treatment. A cognitive scale measuring degree of However, at 3-month follow-up (F3) the control
belief in statements about smoking was adminis- group smoked more than the other groups, and
tered at baseline and post-treatment. All groups significantly more than the BST group (t30 ˆ 1.71;
also completed, post-treatment, a questionnaire on p 5 0.05). The mean number (xÅ ) of cigarettes
strategies used, and subjects rated degree of reported smoked was as follows: SET, SP xÅ ˆ 4.2
satisfaction with the programme. (SDn715.4), F3, xÅ ˆ 9.0 (SDn717.5); BST SP, xÅ ˆ 7.6
(SDn718.1). F3, xÅ ˆ 11.5 (SDn7110.1); COT SP,
xÅ ˆ 4.5 (SDn715.8), F3, xÅ ˆ 7.7 (SDn715.9). At F3,
Cotinine Analysis five of the SET, and five of the BST groups, and
Salivary levels of cotinine were measured by three of the COT group had quit smoking. At
high-performance liquid chromatography (HPLC) 9-month follow-up (F9), three of the SET group, two
with UV detection. Cotinine was extracted from of the BST group and two of the COT group, were
alkalinized saliva samples using ethyl acetate as the abstinent as determined by the cotinine analysis.
extraction solvent. After centrifugation, the aqueous No further subjects had quit.
phase was discarded and the organic layer was There were no significant differences at baseline,
evaporated to dryness under a gentle stream of in craving between groups, and each group
nitrogen. The dried residue was resuspended in contained smokers with preferences to smoke in a
200 ml of mobile phase of which approximately range of situations. All groups showed a significant
50 ml were used for the analysis. decrease in craving over all situations during
The analysis was performed with a Hewlett- treatment. The control group reported higher
Packard 1050 HPLC model. The delivery system craving in all situations than did the other two
was connected to a chromatographic column groups, but there were no post-treatment differ-
(12.5  0.46 cm i.d.) which was packed with 5 mm ences in craving between SET and BST groups.
ODS material (Hichrom). Cotinine and the internal Also, all groups showed a slight non-significant
standard were detected at 261 nm. increase in craving from post-treatment to F3 (see
The mobile phase was a mixture of acetonitrile: Table 2). But at F9, the reduction was maintained,
0.005 M NaH2PO4 pH 3.0 buffer (60:40) and the and mean craving over situations was: SET ˆ 40.74
flow rate was set at 1.0 ml/min. Under these (SDn717.28); BST ˆ 45.93 (SDn7112.82); COT ˆ 50.51
chromatographic conditions, cotinine and the (SDn7112.74).
internal standard eluted at the following retention Confidence about not smoking increased in all
times: 3.68 and 6.19 min, respectively. Nicotine did three groups during reduction. Significant differ-
not interfere with the analysis, its retention time ences in confidence ratings between individual
being 10.58 min. situations had disappeared at SP and at F3. The
Cotinine, which is a major metabolite of nicotine, SET group showed a significant increase over all
offers several advantages over other biochemical 12 situations, the BST group over 10, and the control
markers, since it is stable in body fluids, has a group over seven situations. The SET group had
long half-life and its concentration is not influenced less variation than the other two groups at SP, indi-
by diet, physical activity or environment. Further- cating greater between subjects uniformity in confid-
more, cotinine concentration in its serum forms in ence levels. At F9, mean confidence level and over
cigarette smokers seems directly related to nicotine situations was: SET ˆ 71.26 (SDn717.05); BST ˆ 62.59
intake (Rosa et al., 1992). Pomerleau et al. (1990) (SDn7111.62); COT ˆ 64.55 (SDn7111.17).
reported a strong relationship between nicotine The overall product±moment correlation between
tolerance as measured by self-report questionnaire mean confidence level over all situations and
and plasma cotinine. Cummings and Richard (1988) cotinine level across subjects was negative overall
provide the optimum cut-off points for biochemical (r(35) ˆ ÿ0.55; p 5 0.001) and for each group

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 5, 145±154 (1998)
Confidence During Smoking Reduction 151

Table 2. Craving (0±100) and confidence (0±100) in not smoking over each of 12 smoking situations and for treatment
group (SET, COT, BST) at baseline, post-treatment, and 3-month follow-up in Study 2 (SDn71 in parentheses)

Baseline (SO) Post-treatment (SP) 3-month follow-up

Craving Confidence Craving* Confidence Craving Confidence


After a meal SET 97.9 (5.4) 38.0 (31.4) 29.5 (42.7) 87.4 (20.0){ 56.9 (41.7) 74.4 (25.5)
COT 100.0 (0.0) 32.5 (31.4) 48.6 (42.6) 66.7 (34.5){ 63.3 (39.4) 56.0 (34.8)
BST 96.0 (15.5) 38.8 (31.7) 22.2 (38.4) 77.3 (36.3{ 51.5 (40.9) 68.5 (34.1)
With alcohol SET 82.9 (24.8) 44.5 (37.7) 23.9 (34.7) 88.4 (19.8){ 41.7 (39.5) 74.6 (26.5)
COT 92.7 (25.7) 32.4 (27.3) 50.7 (33.8) 61.3 (29.2){ 55.3 (40.0) 62.0 (34.9)
BST 83.7 (34.9) 31.0 (34.4) 27.3 (40.6) 76.0 (36.6){ 43.8 (40.9) 64.6 (36.0)
Taking a break SET 81.1 (18.8) 52.0 (29.5) 19.5 (29.1) 91.6 (16.7){ 36.9 (36.5) 80.0 (20.7)
COT 94.0 (11.8) 45.0 (27.7) 39.3 (35.8) 70.7 (34.7){ 45.3 (37.2) 70.7 (33.5)
BST 87.0 (21.7) 51.2 (32.3) 20.0 (27.2) 86.0 (26.9){ 43.1 (36.4) 68.5 (29.7)
Concentrating SET 61.6 (30.6) 65.3 (29.8) 17.4 (29.2) 92.7 (12.8){ 35.0 (36.0) 84.4 (20.3)
COT 77.7 (25.1) 59.2 (25.3) 36.4 (36.6) 77.3 (10.6){ 40.0 (32.5) 73.3 (28.2)
BST 56.0 (31.1) 79.2 (25.0) 19.3 (33.9) 80.0 (36.1) 31.9 (36.1) 83.9 (29.6)
In conversation SET 60.0 (27.9) 63.8 (27.7) 16.8 (23.6) 92.1 (15.1){ 28.7 (34.4) 82.2 (19.7)
COT 67.3 (14.4) 66.9 (14.9) 36.4 (30.5) 71.3 (34.0) 32.7 (26.6) 76.7 (32.9)
BST 70.7 (25.8) 70.4 (26.1) 13.3 (24.4) 87.7 (26.7){ 23.8 (28.1) 78.5 (26.4)
Relaxing SET 65.3 (26.7) 52.3 (29.1) 12.1 (23.2) 92.1 (16.5){ 30.3 (33.1) 84.4 (21.0)
COT 73.3 (20.6) 70.8 (22.9) 30.0 (33.5) 72.7 (35.1) 41.3 (32.9) 66.0 (32.9)
BST 77.0 (23.1) 59.2 (22.9) 20.7 (29.9) 91.7 (13.6){ 21.5 (33.0) 83.8 (23.3)
Bored SET 73.7 (31.5) 41.2 (37.6) 14.7 (24.1) 91.8 (16.4){ 36.2 (33.4) 73.7 (30.3)
COT 72.0 (17.8) 48.5 (25.8) 31.4 (34.6) 71.3 (34.6){ 44.0 (35.2) 67.3 (32.4)
BST 56.7 (27.4) 73.8 (25.7) 12.0 (24.0) 86.7 (25.2){ 16.9 (24.3) 85.4 (21.8)
Watching TV SET 72.6 (21.6) 55.9 (29.6) 11.6 (23.4) 94.2 (13.5){ 26.2 (34.8) 86.2 (21.2)
COT 73.7 (15.4) 63.1 (18.9) 26.4 (33.0) 73.3 (34.8) 42.7 (36.7) 66.7 (33.7)
BST 66.7 (17.6) 63.5 (17.7) 10.0 (17.3) 92.0 (15.7){ 20.7 (25.3) 82.3 (23.5)
Pleasurable activity SET 52.1 (28.8) 79.4 (17.5) 12.6 (20.8) 93.2 (15.6){ 28.1 (31.4) 84.7 (17.6)
COT 61.7 (22.6) 69.2 (20.2) 28.6 (34.4) 71.3 (34.4) 36.7 (31.1) 76.0 (32.5)
BST 60.7 (23.4) 74.6 (26.6) 6.0 (11.8) 94.0 (11.2){ 23.8 (31.2) 83.1 (24.3)
Doing complicated work SET 64.7 (31.9) 63.5 (29.6) 16.3 (27.9) 90.5 (16.1){ 30.6 (33.0) 85.6 (17.5)
COT 80.7 (22.3) 46.1 (31.5) 32.1 (33.3) 76.0 (30.7){ 40.7 (26.3) 69.3 (29.1)
BST 51.3 (32.9) 80.8 (23.3) 16.0 (32.7) 82.0 (32.3) 25.4 (30.2) 78.5 (28.2)
Waiting for something SET 76.6 (24.0) 57.1 (26.6) 17.9 (28.6) 89.5 (18.4){ 31.2 (34.0) 82.8 (20.6)
COT 81.7 (11.6) 52.3 (28.6) 23.6 (28.1) 74.7 (35.0){ 39.3 (35.1) 62.0 (34.1)
BST 73.3 (28.8) 58.5 (28.2) 11.3 (22.0) 88.7 (21.7){ 25.4 (29.6) 82.3 (22.4)
Emotionally stressed SET 85.8 (17.1) 32.6 (24.7) 25.8 (39.5) 81.4 (26.0){ 49.4 (41.7) 69.7 (36.1)
COT 94.3 (10.5) 42.3 (32.9) 45.7 (36.7) 64.7 (30.4){ 57.3 (38.4) 59.3 (30.6)
BST 79.0 (28.7) 39.2 (30.7) 29.3 (40.4) 80.7 (32.8){ 51.1 (39.7) 65.0 (31.1)
* All SO±SP differences in craving significant at p 5 0.01. {SO±SP differences significant at p 5 0.05; {SO±SP differences significant at
p 5 0.01.

(BST, r(11) ˆ ÿ0.40; SET, r(13) ˆ ÿ0.57; COT, higher levels at SP than the other groups. There were
r(11) ˆ ÿ0.86). The correlation between mean no significant trends in high or low arousal over
craving over all situations and cotinine levels was sessions. Reports of arousal were variable from week
positive overall (r(35) ˆ 0.62; p 5 0.001) and for each to week, and within group variation was large.
group (BST, r(11) ˆ 0.41; SET, r(13) ˆ 0.69; COT, Subjects completed, at SO and SP, a self-report
r(11) ˆ 0.78), so there was a consistent relationship scale measuring their degree of belief (0±5) in 13
between the objective measure of smokers' status statements about their habit. The scale was adapted
and subjective ratings of confidence and craving. from the attributional style measure of Anderson
Analysis of the stress-arousal questionnaire (1983), which assesses whether events are attributed
revealed a group effect for high stress to, internal/external, stable/unstable, charactero-
(F(2,39) ˆ 3.37; p 5 0.04). The SET group reported logical/behavioural, global/specific sources. The

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 5, 145±154 (1998)
152 K. O'Connor and R. Langlois

statements effectively measured if, and how, the this last analysis can only be considered tentative. It
person believed their smoking and other habits is, however, interesting that the smoking situations
could be controlled; the extent to which they felt where number of cigarettes smoked, was predicted
able to control the habit by themselves, or whether by level of confidence, all involved low levels of
they considered the habit was out of their control; activity and non-directed states.
and whether they believed the habit was dependent
on physical factors, on situational variation, or on Drop-Outs
other people. All three groups rated the belief that We examined baseline values, for all measures,
smoking was a physical dependence, less at SP than in those who dropped out of the study, and found
at SO (SET ˆ t18 ˆ 6.11, p 5 0.01; BST ˆ t14 ˆ 3.90, that this group showed marginally more high stress
p 5 0.01; COT ˆ t14 ˆ 4.74, p 5 0.01). Both BST and (t59 ˆ 1.61, p 5 0.07) and more low arousal
COT, but not the SET group, rated the belief, that (t59 ˆ 2.23, p 5 0.01) than the survivors. They were
smoking could be actively controlled by specific higher on neuroticism (t59 ˆ 2.49, p 5 0.01), and
strategies, higher at SP (COT ˆ t14 ˆ 2.66, p 5 0.05; scored lower on beliefs about ability to control life
BST ˆ t14 ˆ 2.21; p 5 0.05). The BST group increased habits in general (t59 ˆ 1.61, p 5 0.06) and on beliefs
their belief, that strategies to stop smoking required about the importance of situational variation in
effort (t14 ˆ 2.09, p 5 0.05) and this group felt more smoking (t59 ˆ 2.59, p 5 0.01). The drop-outs also
in control of their habits at SP (t14 ˆ 4.43, p 5 0.01). had significantly higher craving at outset when
The control group felt less ready at SP to invest stressed (t59 ˆ 2.19, p 5 0.01). There were no differ-
effort in stopping smoking (t14 ˆ 2.10, p 5 0.05). ences, between drop-outs and survivors, on number
At the end of the programme, strategies used by of cigarettes smoked, initial confidence levels, or
each group were divided into, cognitive coping expectations about quitting.
skills, behavioural coping skills, strategies aimed at
general changes in lifestyle, and cognitive strategies
Discussion
aimed at relapse prevention (e.g. not catastro-
phizing slips, restructuring stress, using self- The important finding, in both studies, is that
statements to maintain confidence). The SET group gradual reduction can be achieved systematically;
used significantly less behavioural strategies than can be accompanied by improved confidence; and
the BST or COT group (t32 ˆ 2.20; p 5 0.02). But, can be achieved; by some smokers, regardless of
interestingly, there was no difference between whether they adopt behavioural or cognitive coping
groups in cognitive coping skills used, suggesting strategies, or no strategies at all.
that even if cognitive strategies were not provided, In the first study, the use of specific coping
smokers invented them nonetheless. The SET group strategies allowed a more consistent increase in
used marginally more cognitive relapse prevention confidence during reduction, whilst the second
strategies (t32 ˆ 1.54; p 5 0.08). study indicated a slight advantage for the self-
In order to examine the effect of preliminary efficacy training group, since their increase in confi-
values on the process of reduction and outcome, dence was more consistent across all 12 situations
multiple stepwise regression was conducted on confi- and was less variable within situations than the
dence ratings, and on the self-reported number of other groups. Also, higher confidence at baseline
cigarettes smoked. Post-treatment (SP) and follow- and post-treatment predicted a lower consumption
up scores (F3, F9) were considered dependent of cigarettes at follow-up, particularly in low activity
variables in separate analyses, whilst scores on all situations, such as boredom, where other active
other weeks during the programme, and at base- coping strategies might be more difficult to imple-
line (SO), were considered independent variables. ment (O'Connor and Stravynski, 1982). Limitations
Number of cigarettes smoked at baseline was not a of the first study were the small number of subjects
predictor of amount smoked either at post-test, or at and the use of self-report measures of smoking only.
follow-up. Both confidence at SO and at SP were A limitation of both studies was that the same
significant predictors of the number of cigarettes therapist was used for all groups. The advantage
reported smoked at follow-ups in situations invol- here is that the same level of expertise and type of
ving: alcohol (b ˆ 0.32; T(SO) ˆ 2.8; p 5 0.01); relax- animation was applied in all groups. But obviously,
ation (b ˆ 0.37; T(SO) ˆ 3.0; p 5 0.01); boredom even though the therapist was genuinely impartial,
(b ˆ 0.33; T(SO) ˆ 2.1; p 5 0.04); when waiting for bias cannot be excluded. However, satisfaction
something (b ˆ 0.32; T(SO) ˆ 2.29; p 5 0.03). Owing ratings of all the groups were similar, and the fact
to the low subjects to variable ratio, the results of that there was no significant difference between

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 5, 145±154 (1998)
Confidence During Smoking Reduction 153

groups in outcome, post-treatment, supports the whom self-efficacy was stressed as part of treat-
assumption that groups benefited equally from ment. Hawkins (1992) concluded that, generally,
therapist input. Also, such criticisms, though self-efficacy was a predictor, but not a cause of
relevant to conclusions concerning treatment out- behaviour. There is controversy over the causal role
come, do not really bear on the major findings about of self-efficacy in enabling quitting, and Sperry and
the progressive changes in confidence found during Nicki (1991) for example suggest that self-efficacy
the process of reduction. An important limitation to reflects, but does not mediate smoking behaviour
our studies is that we did not measure puff volume, change. However, Stuart et al. (1994) found that
and hence cannot comment on whether reduction in although its impact may vary over the various
number of cigarettes led to increased puff volume, stages in the process of quitting, self-efficacy was
and nicotine compensation in those cigarettes positively related to successful attempts to quit. On
remaining. But, had this been the case, we would the other hand, the results in the present two
not have expected to find the overall significant studies are based on smokers who successfully
decrease in craving. Also, the cotinine concentra- reduced, and we cannot comment on the confidence
tions mirrored the reported smoking status which and craving levels of those who attempt reduction
would tend to exclude compensation. Rosa et al. unsuccessfully. Those smokers who dropped out of
(1992) noted that subjects smoking lower nicotine the study did report higher initial levels of craving,
cigarettes did not increase puff volume. The claim but were not less confident, than those who
for compensation is controversial, and both Epstein successfully reduced. The present research can, of
et al. (1981), and Hatsukami et al. (1990) found that course, draw no conclusions about the relative
the frequency with which cigarettes were smoked, in merits of graded reduction versus abrupt cessation,
a naturalistic situation, did not alter puff intensity. since the studies here focused specifically on factors
Alternative methods for reducing cigarette smok- affecting graded reduction. On the negative side, it
ing gradually include nicotine fading, where must be noted that although the higher self-rated
nicotine content of the cigarette is slowly decreased confidence levels were maintained at 9-month
(e.g. Brown et al., 1984), scheduled smoking at longer follow-up, this confidence did not apparently
and longer intervals throughout the day (Cinciripini translate into any further successes at quitting. In
et al., 1994), and paced puffing where nicotine future studies, it would be informative to look at
delivery is modified by puff parameters (Pomerleau long-term outcome in smokers who cut down.
et al., 1989). The advantage of the situational
approach to reduction is that it permits a functional
analysis of the smokers' behaviour (Axelrod, 1991). ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The role of situational factors and cues in eliciting
behaviour has been recognized not only for smoking The authors would like to acknowledge the help of
cigarettes (Niaura et al., 1992) but also for nicotine Dr Manon VeÂzina in the cotinine analysis. The
chewing gum (Parrott and Craig, 1995) and for study was in part supported by grant no. RS-1421-
smokeless tobacco use (Hatsukami et al., 1991). 1653 from the Conseil QueÂbeÂcois de la Recherche
Identification of smoking cues is the first step in Sociale. The first author was supported by a bourse
tailoring cue exposure and coping skills to aid in from the Fonds de la Recherche en Sante du QueÂbec.
reduction (O'Connor and Langlois, 1993) and
relapse prevention (Bliss et al., 1989; Marlatt, 1990).
The finding that gradual reduction can, in some REFERENCES
subjects, result in increased overall confidence in
coping with not smoking, adds weight to the Anderson, C. A. (1983). Motivational and performance
deficits in interpersonal settings. The effects of attribu-
viability of using reduction in those smokers who
tional style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
may be contemplating, but are not yet committed to 45, 1136±1147.
quitting, since higher confidence suggests the Axelrod, S. (1991). Smoking cessation through functional
person may be more, not less, prepared to quit analysis. Journal of Applied Behaviour Analysis, 24,
after reducing. Previous studies have also pointed 717±718.
to the role of efficacy in motivating the decision to Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanisms in human
change, and in preventing relapse. Sperry and Nicki agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122±147.
Bliss, R. E., Garvey, A. J., Heinold, J. W. and Hitchcock,
(1991) found a high correlation between self-efficacy J. L. (1989). The influence of situation and coping on
and smoking outcome during a nicotine fading relapse crisis outcomes after smoking cessation. Journal
programme, but no extra benefit for a group in of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57(3), 443±449.

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 5, 145±154 (1998)
154 K. O'Connor and R. Langlois

Brown, R. A., Lichtenstein, E., McIntyre, K. O. and O'Connor, K. P. and Langlois, R. (1992). The Effect of Self-
Harrington-Kostur, J. (1984). Effects of nicotine fading Efficacy Training on Adherence and Abstinence Rates
and relapse prevention on smoking cessation. Journal of During a Smoking Cessation Program Designed for Female
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52(2), 307±307. Smokers. Quebec Social Research Council Report
Cinciripini, P. M., Lapitsky, L. G., Wallfisch, A., Mace, R., RS-1653-089.
Nezami, E. and Van Vunakis, H. (1994). An evaluation O'Connor, K. and Langlois, R. (1993). Situational typing
of a multicomponent treatment program involving and graded smoking reduction. Psychological Reports,
scheduled smoking and relapse prevention pro- 72, 747±751.
cedures: initial findings. Addictive Behaviours, 19(1), O'Connor, K. P. and Stravynski, A. (1982). Evaluation of a
13±22. smoking typology by use of a specific behavioural
Condiotte, M. M. and Lichtenstein, E. (1981). Self-efficacy substitution method of self-control. Behaviour, Research
and relapse in smoking cessation programs. Journal of and Therapy, 20, 279±288.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 648±658. O'Leary, A. (1985). Self-efficacy and health. Behaviour
Cummings, S. R. and Richard, R. J. (1988). Optimum Research and Therapy, 23(4), 437±451.
cutoff points for biochemical validation of smoking Owen, N., Wakefield, M., Roberts, L. and Esterman, A.
status. American Journal of Public Health, 78(5), 574± (1992). Stages of readiness to quit smoking: population
575. prevalence and correlates. Health Psychology, 11(6),
Epstein, L. H., Ossip, D. J., Coleman, D., Hughes, J. and 413±417.
Wiist, W. (1981). Measurement of smoking topography Parrott, A. C. and Craig, D. (1995). Psychological
during withdrawal or deprivation. Behavior Therapy, 12, functions served by nicotine chewing gum. Addictive
507±519. Behaviors, 20(3), 271±278.
Eysenck, M. J. (1980). The Causes and Effects of Smoking. Pomerleau, C. S., Majchrzak, M. J. and Pomerleau, O. F.
London: Maurice Temple Smith. (1989). Paced puffing as a method for administering
Glasgow, R. E., Morray, K. and Lichtenstein, E. (1989). fixed doses of nicotine. Addictive Behaviors, 14, 571±575.
Controlled smoking versus abstinence as a treatment Pomerleau, C. S., Pomerleau, O. F., Majchrzak, M. J.,
goal. Behavior Therapy, 20, 77±91. Kloska, D. D. and Malakuti, R. (1990). Relationship
Hall, S. M., Rugg, D., Turnstall, C. and Jones, R. T. (1984). between nicotine tolerance questionnaire scores and
Preventing relapse to cigarette smoking by behavioral plasma cotinine. Addictive Behaviors, 15, 73±80.
skill training. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psych-
Rosa, M., Pacifici, R., Altieri, I., Pichini, S., Ottaviani, G.
ology, 52, 3, 372±382.
and Zuccaro, P. (1992). How the steady-state cotinine
Hatsukami, D. K., Anton, D., Callies, A. and Keenan, R.
concentration in cigarette smokers is directly related to
(1991). Situational factors and patterns associated with
nicotine intake. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics,
smokeless tobacco use. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,
52, 324±329.
14(4), 383±396.
Shiffman, S. (1982). Relapse following smoking cessation:
Hatsukami, D. K., Morgan, S. F., Pickens, R. and
a situational analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Champagne, S. E. (1990). Situational factors in cigarette
Psychology, 50, 1, 71±86.
smoking. Addictive Behaviors, 15, 1±12.
Hawkins, R. (1992). Self-efficacy: a predictor but not a Shiffman, S. (1984). Coping with temptations to smoke.
cause of behavior. Journal of Behavior and Experimental Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 261±267.
Psychiatry, 23, 4, 251±256. Shiffman, S. (1989). Trans-situational consistency in
Hughes, J. R. and Hatsukami, D. K. (1986). Signs and smoking relapse. Health Psychology, 8(4), 471±481.
symptoms of tobacco withdrawal. Archives of General Sobell, L. C., Sobell, M. B., Kozlowski, L. T. and Toneatto,
Psychiatry, 43, 189±194. T. (1990). Alcohol or tobacco research versus alcohol
Hughes, J. R., Higgins, S. T. and Bickel, W. K. (1984). and tobacco research. British Journal of Addictions, 85,
Nicotine withdrawal versus other drug withdrawal 263±269.
syndromes: similarities and dissimilarities. Addiction, Sperry, J. M. and Nicki, R. M. (1991). Cognitive appraisal,
89, 1461±1470. self-efficacy, and cigarette smoking behavior. Addictive
MacKay, C. J. (1980). The measurement of mood Behaviors, 16, 381±388.
and psychophysiological activity using self-report Stevens, V. J. and Hollis, J. F. (1989). Preventing smoking
techniques. In: I. Martin and P. H. Venables (Eds) relapse using an individually tailored skills-training
Techniques in Psycho-Physiology. Chichester: Wiley, technique. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
pp. 501±560. 57, 3, 420±424.
Marlatt, G. A. (1990). Cue exposure and relapse preven- Stuart, K., Borland, R. and McMurray, N. (1994). Self-
tion in the treatment of addictive behaviors. Addictive efficacy, health locus of control, and smoking cessation.
Behaviors, 15, 395±399. Addictive Behaviors, 19(1), 1±12.
Marlatt, G. A. and Gordon, J. R. (1985). Relapse Prevention: Thayer, R. E. (1967). Measurement of activation through
Maintenance Strategies in the Treatment of Addictive self-report. Psychological Reports, 20, 663±678.
Behavior. New York: Guilford Press. West, R. and Schneider, N. (1988). Craving for cigarettes.
Niaura, R., Abrams, D. B., Pedraza, M., Monti, P. M. and British Journal of Addiction, 3, 408±415.
Rohsenow, D. J. (1992). Smokers' reactions to inter- Yates, A. J. and Thain, J. (1985). Self-efficacy as a predictor
personal interaction and presentation of smoking cues. of relapse following voluntary cessation of smoking.
Addictive Behaviors, 17, 557±566. Addictive Behavior, 10, 291±298.

# 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Clin. Psychol. Psychother. 5, 145±154 (1998)

You might also like