You are on page 1of 8

Archival Report Biological

Psychiatry

D9-Tetrahydrocannabinol at Retrieval
Drives False Recollection of Neutral and
Emotional Memories
Manoj K. Doss, Jessica Weafer, David A. Gallo, and Harriet de Wit

ABSTRACT
BACKGROUND: It is well established that the main psychoactive constituent of cannabis, D9-tetrahydrocannabinol
(THC), impairs episodic memory encoding and modulates emotional processing, but little is known about the impact
of THC during the retrieval of emotional episodic memories. With the rise of cannabis to treat medical conditions,
including those characterized by emotional and episodic memory disturbances, there is an urgent need to
determine the effects of THC on memory accuracy and distortion. Here, we report the first study investigating the
effects of THC during retrieval of neutral and emotional episodic memories.
METHODS: Using a double-blind, placebo-controlled, within-subjects design, healthy volunteers (N = 23) viewed
negative, neutral, and positive pictures (emotional memory task) and lists of semantically related words (false
memory task). Forty-eight hours later, participants ingested a capsule containing either THC (15 mg) or placebo
and completed tasks to test their memories for the previously studied pictures and words.
RESULTS: THC during retrieval did not reduce the number of correct responses to studied items. Instead, it robustly
increased false recollection on both the emotional memory and false memory tasks. This effect was found for both
neutral and emotional items.
CONCLUSIONS: These findings show that THC has adverse effects during memory retrieval, distorting both neutral
and emotional memories. Coupled with THC’s known effects during encoding, these new retrieval findings are
important in light of the spreading acceptance of cannabis.
Keywords: Cannabis, Emotional memory, Episodic memory, False memory, False recollection, THC
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.04.020

Despite the rising acceptance of medical and recreational though many questions about the potential impact of THC at
cannabis, surprisingly little is known about the effects of D9- retrieval remain.
tetrahydrocannabinol (THC)—the primary psychoactive One open question is whether the potential impact of THC
ingredient of cannabis—on basic mechanisms of episodic during episodic memory retrieval depends on the emotional
memory in humans. Although it is well established that THC content of the material. Prior research indicates that THC
impairs episodic memory when it is administered during the specifically attenuates the encoding of emotional compared
encoding phase [for a review, see Curran et al. (1)], it is less with neutral episodic memories (5), and in general, THC has
clear how the drug affects episodic memory during retrieval. been found to impair emotional processing (7,8) and reduce
In many past studies the encoding and retrieval phases of amygdala activations to emotional stimuli (9). If THC during
memory were not distinguishable: Cannabis or THC was retrieval similarly impairs memory and emotional processes,
administered before encoding and then participants were one may expect THC to disproportionately impair the retrieval
tested soon afterward so that retrieval also occurred under of emotional compared with neutral recollections. Indeed, in-
the drug’s influence [e.g., (2,3)]. This timing of the drug dividuals with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), a disorder
administration makes it difficult to determine if memory im- characterized by aberrant negative recollections (10), are
pairments were related to encoding, retrieval, or both. To known to use cannabis at high rates (11,12), and anecdotally,
isolate encoding effects, we previously administered THC they report that the drug helps suppress the impact of
during encoding and tested memory 48 hours later in a drug- retrieving emotionally negative memories. Thus, it is possible
free state (4,5). This work confirmed that THC impairs that THC has effects during retrieval that are greater for
memory encoding. In contrast, a recent study examining the emotional memories.
effect of THC only during retrieval found a null effect of THC In addition to reducing memories for studied events, THC at
on the recall of neutral verbal stimuli (6). Taken together, this retrieval may increase false recollections of nonstudied events
work suggests that THC has a larger impact on encoding, (i.e., high-confidence memory errors). THC is known to disrupt

ª 2018 Society of Biological Psychiatry. 743


ISSN: 0006-3223 Biological Psychiatry November 15, 2018; 84:743–750 www.sobp.org/journal
Descargado para Anonymous User (n/a) en University Foundation of Health Sciences de ClinicalKey.es por Elsevier en febrero 02, 2020.
Para uso personal exclusivamente. No se permiten otros usos sin autorización. Copyright ©2020. Elsevier Inc. Todos los derechos reservados.
Biological
Psychiatry THC Distorts Emotional Memory Retrieval

processing in prefrontal cortex (13,14), an area important for Table 1. Demographic Data
retrieval monitoring processes used to avoid false memories Parameter Mean (SEM)
(15). This prefrontal disruption would predict that THC at Age, Years 22.74 (0.73)
retrieval may increase false recollection. In addition, THC and
Education, Years 15.30 (0.37)
other recreational drugs impact the ventral striatum (16), a
BMI, kg/m2 24.62 (0.72)
region involved in episodic memory retrieval (17,18), including
Caffeine, Cups/Day 1.37 (0.28)
emotional and false memory effects (19,20). Although the exact
Nicotine, Cigarettes/Day in the 5 Users 0.19 (0.15)
role of ventral striatum in episodic memory is still unclear,
recent studies indicate that other drugs impacting this region Alcohol, Drinks/Week 6.01 (1.32)
can increase memory distortion. Administering dextroam- Cannabis, Uses/Month 0.78 (0.26)
phetamine during retrieval increases recognition and recall Lifetime Uses of Cannabis 27.26 (5.25)
errors, especially for positive memories (21), and similar effects Last Use of Cannabis Before Placebo Session, Days 308.59 (189.59)
have been reported for 63,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine Last Use of Cannabis Before THC Session, Days 306.02 (189.71)
(22). A similar effect of THC would be problematic if the drug is BMI, body mass index; THC, D9-tetrahydrocannabinol.
used to treat PTSD.
Based on these considerations, we hypothesized that THC
during retrieval would reduce memory for studied events and and alcohol throughout the study, and they were advised that
increase false memory of nonstudied events. To test these each session would begin with a drug test. Positive tests led to
hypotheses, we administered THC during the retrieval phase of rescheduling or dismissal. Participants were advised to get
two memory tests. One task was an emotional memory test their normal amounts of sleep and not to eat for 2 hours before
sensitive to drug effects on emotional and false recollection retrieval sessions. Following completion of the study, partici-
(22). The other task was the Deese-Roediger-McDermott pants were fully debriefed and monetarily compensated. The
(DRM) illusion, a task involving episodic memory for emotion- study took place at the University of Chicago Medical Center
ally neutral words that has been used to study false memory and was approved by the institutional review board.
and drug effects [for a review, see Gallo (23)], including a
previous study with a THC at encoding manipulation (4). Drug
A total of 15 mg of THC (Marinol; Solvay Pharmaceuticals,
METHODS AND MATERIALS Brussels, Belgium) was placed in opaque size-00 capsules
Participants with dextrose filler. Placebo capsules contained only dextrose.
This dose of THC is within the range shown to affect memory in
Twenty-four healthy young adults (18–29 years of age, 12 men) previous studies (4,5).
with some experience using cannabis (4–100 lifetime occa-
sions) were recruited for the study, but 1 male participant was Design
excluded for not following instructions, leaving 23 participants.
Screening included a physical examination, an electrocardio- This study used a double blind, within-subjects, counter-
gram, and a semistructured interview by a clinical psycholo- balanced design in which participants were tested during
gist. Exclusion criteria included any current DSM-IV Axis I memory retrieval with THC or placebo. Each experimental arm
disorder, including substance dependence, current use of .5 consisted of an encoding session followed 48 hours later by a
cigarettes/day, history of psychosis or mania, less than a high retrieval session. Experimental arms were separated by at least
school education, lack of English fluency, a body mass index 5 days, and drug order was counterbalanced across partici-
above or below 19 to 33 kg/m2, high blood pressure (.140/90 pants. During the encoding session of each experimental arm,
mm Hg), abnormal electrocardiogram, daily use of any medi- participants viewed emotional stimuli and DRM stimuli. During
cation other than birth control, being pregnant, or lactating. the second session, memory for the emotional stimuli and
Women not taking hormonal contraceptives were tested during DRM stimuli was assessed. Participants completed additional
their follicular phase because hormonal fluctuations can influ- tasks on this session and during an additional brief visit, but
ence responses to drugs (24). Demographic and drug use in- these will be reported elsewhere.
formation were obtained during screening (Table 1).
Qualifying participants attended an orientation session to Stimuli
sign a consent form and practice memory tasks (i.e., partici- Stimuli for the emotional memory task consisted of 240 images
pants knew their memories would be tested). To minimize from the International Affective Picture System (25) and the
expectancy, participants were told that they could receive a Nencki Affective Picture System (26) and three- to five-word
stimulant, sedative, cannabinoid, or placebo. They were labels (e.g., “skinhead posing with Nazi flag,” “boy leaning
instructed to consume their normal amounts of caffeine and against wall,” “man on snowy mountain peak”) describing
nicotine before sessions but abstain from using alcohol, pre- these images. Pictures were selected with an effort to balance
scription drugs (except contraceptives), and over-the-counter semantic overlap between valence conditions, as this has been
drugs for 24 hours before the encoding sessions (i.e., the shown to be a factor in prior work (27). The images included
first and fourth sessions; see Design below), cannabis for 1 emotionally negative, neutral, and positive pictures (80 each),
week before the encoding sessions, and other illicit drugs for and these were divided into four comparable sets (A–D) for
48 hours before the encoding sessions (because of their faster counterbalancing across participants’ studied and nonstudied
clearance). They were also told to remain abstinent from drugs items (targets and lures, respectively; see Procedure below).

744 Biological Psychiatry November 15, 2018; 84:743–750 www.sobp.org/journal


Descargado para Anonymous User (n/a) en University Foundation of Health Sciences de ClinicalKey.es por Elsevier en febrero 02, 2020.
Para uso personal exclusivamente. No se permiten otros usos sin autorización. Copyright ©2020. Elsevier Inc. Todos los derechos reservados.
Biological
THC Distorts Emotional Memory Retrieval Psychiatry

Specifically, sets A and B were always administered during the response to allow sufficient time for a recollection response
first experimental arm and sets C and D were always adminis- (29). Then participants were asked whether they had seen the
tered during the second experimental arm. For half the partici- corresponding picture of each label (yes/no). Afterward, they
pants (balanced for drug order and sex), sets A and C were rated their confidence on a 5-point scale and were encouraged
targets and sets B and D were lures, and for the other half of to use the entire scale. After the cued recollection test, par-
participants sets B and D were targets and sets A and C were ticipants viewed each picture (again on screen for 1000 ms
lures. These sets had similar normed valences and arousals (see before a response could be made) and indicated if they had
Supplement). previously seen it (yes/no). When a picture was recognized,
DRM stimuli consisted of 30 lists of 10 semantically related they were asked if they remembered the picture or if they
words that converge on a critical lure that is only presented at simply knew that it was presented (29). They were instructed to
retrieval (e.g., “bed, rest, awake.” for critical lure “sleep”). give a “remember” response when they could recollect asso-
These were the same lists used in Ballard et al. (4). For ciated details from the event, such as thoughts during its
counterbalancing, these lists were split into two sets (A and B) presentation, and a “know” response when they simply knew
of 15 lists for each experimental arm. Half the participants that a picture had been presented without recollecting specific
(balanced for drug order and sex) received set A during the first details. Together, these memory tests lasted approximately 30
experimental arm and set B during the second experimental minutes.
arm, and the other half received the other order. In each set, 10 After the emotional memory tests, participants were tested
of the lists always served as targets and critical lures, and the on the DRM stimuli. This test was composed of 30 trials: 10
other five lists were used to draw unrelated lures. targets (first position in DRM list), 10 critical lures, and 10 un-
related lures (first position and critical lure from the five non-
Procedure studied DRM lists). On each trial, a word was presented (on
screen for 1000 ms before a response could be made), and
At the beginning of all laboratory visits, participants completed
participants indicated whether they had seen it (yes/no). In the
compliance measures including breath alcohol level (Alco-
instructions, participants were warned that there would be
Sensor III, Intoximeters, St. Louis, MO), a urine drug test
nonstudied related words, and they should only press “yes” if
(ToxCup, Branan Medical Co., Irvine, CA), a pregnancy test
they had actually seen the word in one of the lists. Prior work
(women only; Aimstrip, Craig Medical, Vista, CA), and baseline
with the DRM task has used this warning procedure to isolate
cardiovascular and mood measures. During the first session of
false recognition errors that are driven by a strong sense of
each experimental arm, participants encoded the emotional
false recollection, as opposed to familiarity-based guessing
stimuli consisting of 120 randomized labels, half of which were
(15). This test lasted approximately 5 minutes.
followed by their corresponding picture (each on screen for
After the DRM test, participants performed two other tasks
2000 ms). For each label, participants rated how arousing it
and then were allowed to relax with magazines and music.
was while it was on the screen (5-point scale). When a picture
Participants were allowed to leave 210 minutes after capsule
was presented, participants again rated its arousal while it was
ingestion if physiological and subjective measures had
on the screen. Between each trial was a random intertrial in-
returned to baseline.
terval between 1000 and 5000 ms. This task lasted approxi-
mately 15 minutes.
Next, participants encoded 10 DRM lists of 10 words each. Dependent Measures
The order of lists was randomized, but words within a list were Physiological and mood measures were obtained to monitor
presented in descending order relative to their semantic expected drug effects (Table 2). Heart rate and blood pressure
relatedness to the critical lure. Each word was presented for were measured using a portable blood pressure monitor (A&D
2500 ms, and participants were required to make a pleasant- Medical/Life Source, San Jose, CA). Mood measures included
ness rating (pleasant or unpleasant) while the word was still on the Addiction Research Center Inventory (30), the visual analog
the screen. Between each word was a 500-ms intertrial inter- scale (31), the Drug Effects Questionnaire (32), and an end-of-
val, and between each list was a screen that displayed “next session questionnaire. See the Supplement for descriptions of
list” for 3000 ms. This task lasted approximately 10 minutes, each scale.
and afterward participants left the lab. For the encoding phase, average arousal ratings and
Forty-eight hours later, participants returned to the lab and, response latencies for labels and pictures were calculated for
after compliance measures, consumed a capsule containing each valence and drug condition in each participant.
THC or placebo. Cardiovascular and mood measures were For the cued recollection test, hit and false alarm rates were
taken every 30 minutes for the next 120 minutes while the drug calculated for each valence and drug condition in each
was absorbed (28). During this time, participants were pro- participant. False alarms were subtracted from hits to compute
vided with magazines and music in furnished rooms. They memory accuracy. Finally, high-confidence hits, false alarms,
were not allowed to eat, sleep, or work, and they had no ac- and accuracy were calculated by only including responses with
cess to cell phones or Internet. the top two levels of confidence. High-confidence false alarms
After 120 minutes, participants were tested with cued are a hallmark of false recollection (15). See the Supplement for
recollection and picture recognition tests for the emotional picture recognition measures.
stimuli and a word recognition test for the DRM stimuli (all self- For the DRM recognition test, hit rates to targets and false
paced). For the cued recollection test, participants first viewed alarm rates to critical and unrelated lures were calculated. To
each label on screen for 1000 ms before they could make a correct for baseline responding, adjusted hit and false alarm

Biological Psychiatry November 15, 2018; 84:743–750 www.sobp.org/journal 745


Descargado para Anonymous User (n/a) en University Foundation of Health Sciences de ClinicalKey.es por Elsevier en febrero 02, 2020.
Para uso personal exclusivamente. No se permiten otros usos sin autorización. Copyright ©2020. Elsevier Inc. Todos los derechos reservados.
Biological
Psychiatry THC Distorts Emotional Memory Retrieval

Table 2. Physiological and Subjective Measures Supplement). For the DRM task, two-tailed t tests were con-
Measures Placebo THC ducted to compare placebo and drug condition hits, adjusted
Physiology
hits, and adjusted false alarms. False alarms were submitted to
a 2 (drug) 3 2 (lure type) ANOVA. When sphericity was violated,
Heart rate, bpm 27.00 (1.91) 0.23 (2.47)a
a Greenhouse-Geisser correction was applied to the degrees
Systolic BP, mmHg 23.45 (2.07) 1.05 (1.62)a
of freedom. See the Supplement for analysis of sex
Diastolic BP, mmHg 21.91 (1.71) 20.68 (1.81)
differences.
ARCI
Marijuana scale 0.41 (0.31) 4.50 (0.72)a Data Availability
VAS Data from this study are available from Doss (33).
Anxious 25.00 (6.14) 1.82 (2.94)
Stimulated 27.23 (5.46) 11.59 (6.53)
Sedated 21.77 (7.21) 25.00 (5.31) RESULTS
Elated 212.23 (5.47) 5.59 (5.71)
Cued Recollection
Insightful 210.41 (6.02) 8.45 (5.95)
Full cued recollection data can be found in Table 3. Cued
Sociable 210.68 (4.39) 11.23 (4.34) a
recollection hit rates were directly related to valence (hits
Confident 28.18 (5.16) 5.5 (5.00)
[F2,44 = 27.371, p , .001, hp2 = .554], high-confidence hits
Lonely 21.23 (4.65) 1.59 (2.23)
[F2,44 = 29.709, p , .001, hp2 = .575]). As expected, negative
Playful 210.14 (4.47) 13.05 (5.57)a
stimuli were remembered better than neutral and positive
Dizzy 24.00 (4.66) 2.00 (4.30) stimuli (Figures 1A and 2A). Relative to placebo, THC did not
Loving 22.64 (6.18) 2.64 (4.18) affect hits (F1,22 = 2.025, p = .169) or high-confidence hits
Friendly 28.91 (5.69) 13.45 (5.44)a (F1,22 = 0.062, p . .250), nor did it modulate the effect of
Restless 25.77 (8.19) 4.73 (5.52) valence (hits [F2,44 = 0.958, p . .250], high-confidence hits
DEQ [F1.582,34.798 = 0.437, p . .250]), inconsistent with the hy-
Feel drug effect 4.64 (1.67) 44.82 (6.50)a pothesis that THC at retrieval would reduce memory for
Like drug effect 12.23 (4.74) 33.45 (6.67)a emotional events.
Dislike drug effect 8.41 (3.88) 24.68 (5.71)a In contrast to hits, THC significantly increased false alarms
Feel high 3.72 (2.26) 41.00 (6.37)a (F1,22 = 10.269, p = .004, hp2 = .318) and high-confidence false
Want more drug 8.68 (4.08) 22.86 (5.14)a
alarms (F1,22 = 5.655, p = .027, hp2 = .204) relative to placebo
(Figure 1B and 2B). Valence did not modulate false alarms
ESQ, Percent Who Guessed Receiving
(F1.586,34.899 = 1.800, p = .186), high-confidence false alarms
Stimulant 4.35 8.70
(F2,44 = 0.114, p . .250), or the effect of THC (false alarm
Sedative 26.09 17.39
[F2,44 = 1.003, p . .250], high-confidence false alarms [F2,44 =
Cannabinoid 0 73.91
0.922, p . .250]). Because THC increased false alarms, this led
Placebo 69.57 0 to a marginal reduction in accuracy (F1,22 = 3.959, p = .059,
Mean (SEM) values are changes from precapsule to immediately hp2 = .153) but not in high-confidence accuracy (F1,22 = 1.814,
before retrieval (120 minutes). p = .192). There was also a main effect of valence on accuracy
ARCI, Addiction Research Center Inventory; BP, blood pressure; bpm,
(F2,44 = 29.753, p , .001, hp2 = .575) and high-confidence
beats per minute; DEQ, Drug Effects Questionnaire; ESQ, end-of-session
questionnaire; THC, D9-tetrahydrocannabinol; VAS, visual analog scale. accuracy (F2,44 = 26.471, p , .001, hp2 = .546), as memory
a
Values indicate significant differences (p , .05) between drug and was more accurate for negative compared with neutral or
placebo conditions (t test). positive items, and the drug-by-valence interaction was not
significant (accuracy [F2,44 = 1.642, p = .205], high-confidence
accuracy [F2,44 = 0.919, p . .250]).
rates can be calculated by subtracting false alarm rates to Although THC during retrieval significantly increased false
unrelated lures (15). alarms, this effect was not related to the emotional content of the
stimuli. Nevertheless, we examined this relationship more closely
Statistical Analysis because previous studies have found trends for other drugs to
The effects of THC on cardiovascular and subjective measures have greater effects on memory errors for positive material
(changes from baseline) were compared with placebo using (21,22). Indeed, the effect of THC on false alarms in the present
two-tailed t tests (see Supplement). For the emotional memory study appeared to be largest for positive items (negative [t22 =
task, picture and label arousal ratings and response latencies 2.853, p = .017, d = 0.538], neutral [t22 = 2.637, p = .015, d =
from the encoding phase were submitted to 2 (drug) 3 3 0.550], positive [t22 = 3.569, p = .002, d = 0.744]). This numerical
(valence) analyses of variance (ANOVAs) (see Supplement). trend was also found in high-confidence false alarms (negative
Cued recollection hits, false alarms, accuracy, and high- [t22 = 1.783, p = .088, d = 0.372], neutral [t22 = 2.193, p = .039, d =
confidence measures were also submitted to 2 3 3 ANOVAs. 0.458], positive [t22 = 2.702, p = .013, d = 0.563]).
Picture recognition hits, false alarms, accuracy, recollection
hits, recollection false alarms, recollection accuracy, familiarity DRM Recognition
hit estimates, familiarity false alarm estimates, and familiarity Full DRM recognition data can be found in Table 4. THC during
accuracy estimates were submitted to 2 3 3 ANOVAs (see retrieval did not affect hit rates (t22 = 0.000, p . .250), but as

746 Biological Psychiatry November 15, 2018; 84:743–750 www.sobp.org/journal


Descargado para Anonymous User (n/a) en University Foundation of Health Sciences de ClinicalKey.es por Elsevier en febrero 02, 2020.
Para uso personal exclusivamente. No se permiten otros usos sin autorización. Copyright ©2020. Elsevier Inc. Todos los derechos reservados.
Biological
THC Distorts Emotional Memory Retrieval Psychiatry

Table 3. Encoding Data, Cued Recollection Data, and Picture Recognition Data
Placebo THC
Negative Neutral Positive Negative Neutral Positive
Encoding
Label Aro 3.03 (0.14) 1.84 (0.09) 2.35 (0.12) 3.17 (0.14) 1.95 (0.10) 2.50 (0.11)
Picture Aro 1.80 (0.08) 1.05 (0.05) 1.49 (0.06) 1.83 (0.06) 1.05 (0.06) 1.43 (0.07)
Label RL 1.49 (0.03) 1.42 (0.04) 1.43 (0.04) 1.49 (0.04) 1.44 (0.04) 1.45 (0.04)
Picture RL 1.04 (0.05) 1.13 (0.04) 1.10 (0.04) 1.06 (0.04) 1.10 (0.04) 1.10 (0.04)
Cued Recollection
Hits 0.75 (0.03) 0.56 (0.05) 0.62 (0.05) 0.78 (0.03) 0.64 (0.03) 0.63 (0.04)
FAs 0.11 (0.02) 0.12 (0.02) 0.12 (0.02) 0.22 (0.03) 0.26 (0.04) 0.28 (0.05)
Accuracy 0.65 (0.04) 0.44 (0.05) 0.50 (0.04) 0.56 (0.04) 0.38 (0.05) 0.35 (0.04)
Hi Conf Hits 0.61 (0.04) 0.41 (0.04) 0.43 (0.05) 0.62 (0.04) 0.44 (0.04) 0.42 (0.04)
Hi Conf FAs 0.03 (0.01) 0.03 (0.01) 0.02 (0.01) 0.09 (0.03) 0.09 (0.03) 0.10 (0.03)
Hi Conf Acc 0.58 (0.04) 0.38 (0.04) 0.41 (0.05) 0.53 (0.04) 0.35 (0.04) 0.31 (0.04)
Picture Recognition
Hits 0.89 (0.02) 0.86 (00.04) 0.84 (0.04) 0.92 (0.02) 0.89 (0.03) 0.91 (0.02)
FAs 0.03 (0.01) 0.07 (0.01) 0.06 (0.01) 0.14 (0.03) 0.19 (0.03) 0.18 (0.04)
Accuracy 0.87 (0.02) 0.80 (0.04) 0.78 (0.04) 0.78 (0.04) 0.70 (0.03) 0.73 (0.04)
R Hits 0.67 (0.04) 0.51 (0.04) 0.50 (0.05) 0.67 (0.05) 0.53 (0.05) 0.54 (0.05)
R FAs 0.03 (0.00) 0.03 (0.00) 0.03 (0.00) 0.04 (0.01) 0.06 (0.01) 0.05 (0.01)
R Acc 0.65 (0.04) 0.48 (0.04) 0.48 (0.05) 0.64 (0.06) 0.47 (0.05) 0.49 (0.05)
IRK F Hits 0.68 (0.05) 0.76 (0.06) 0.70 (0.05) 0.74 (0.06) 0.76 (0.04) 0.79 (0.04)
IRK F FAs 0.04 (0.01) 0.08 (0.01) 0.07 (0.01) 0.14 (0.03) 0.16 (0.03) 0.17 (0.03)
IRK F Acc 0.64 (0.05) 0.68 (0.05) 0.63 (0.05) 0.60 (0.06) 0.60 (0.04) 0.63 (0.04)
Values are mean (SEM).
Acc, accuracy; Aro, arousal rating; F, familiarity; FA, false alarm; Hi Conf, high confidence; IRK, independence remember/know; R, recollection;
RL, response latency; THC, D9-tetrahydrocannabinol.

with the emotional memory tests, it significantly elevated false alarms on the cued recollection and DRM tests. These mea-
alarms (Figure 3) (F1,22 = 12.388, p , .002, hp2 = .360). This sures were then correlated with the change from baseline on
effect did not differ for critical lures or unrelated lures (F1,22 = the marijuana scale of the Addiction Research Center Inventory
0.862, p . .250). Because unrelated lure false alarms were during the THC session. False alarms on the cued recollection
elevated, adjusted hit rates were attenuated by THC (t22 = test (averaged across valences) were positively correlated with
3.393, p = .003, d = 0.708), but the concurrent elevation in false marijuana scale scores (r20 = .430, p = .046). Although this
alarms to critical and unrelated lures led to no significant dif- relationship was not significant for critical lures on the DRM
ference in drug conditions for adjusted false alarms (t22 = test (r20 = .014 p . .250), it was significant for unrelated lures
0.928, p . .250). (r20 = .516 p = .014). These correlations indicate that partici-
pants reporting a stronger cannabis-specific subjective effect
Correlations Between False Memory and Subjective were more sensitive to drug-related increases in false alarms.
Effects
DISCUSSION
Exploratory correlations were run to explore the association
between false memory and subjective effects of THC (for full This experiment is the first to find that selectively administering
correlation table, see Supplement). A change score was THC during episodic memory retrieval increases false recol-
calculated between THC and placebo conditions for false lection. Evidence for this effect was found across all emotional
valences in the emotional memory task and also for neutral
words in the DRM task. These findings are consistent with
Table 4. DRM Recognition Data
studies using emotionally neutral verbal stimuli showing that
Placebo THC THC increases false recognition (2,34) and recall intrusions
Hits 0.76 (0.04) 0.76 (0.04) (3,35). However, in those studies participants were intoxicated
Critical FAs 0.50 (0.04) 0.60 (0.04) during both encoding and retrieval. Although a recent study
Unrelated FAs 0.21 (0.03) 0.37 (0.05) failed to find an effect of THC during the recall of neutral verbal
Adjusted Hits 0.55 (0.04) 0.39 (0.05) stimuli (6), that study did not include measures of memory
Adjusted FAs 0.29 (0.04) 0.23 (0.04) intrusions.
Values are mean (SEM). Considered with our prior studies using these tasks, the
DRM, Deese-Roediger-McDermott; FA, false alarm; THC, D9- current results show that THC has different effects during
tetrahydrocannabinol. encoding and retrieval, highlighting the importance of drug

Biological Psychiatry November 15, 2018; 84:743–750 www.sobp.org/journal 747


Descargado para Anonymous User (n/a) en University Foundation of Health Sciences de ClinicalKey.es por Elsevier en febrero 02, 2020.
Para uso personal exclusivamente. No se permiten otros usos sin autorización. Copyright ©2020. Elsevier Inc. Todos los derechos reservados.
Biological
Psychiatry THC Distorts Emotional Memory Retrieval

Figure 1. Mean (A) hit and (B) false alarm rates on the cued recollection
task (all confidence responses included). Error bars indicate SEM. *p , .05. Figure 2. Mean high-confidence (A) hit and (B) false alarm rates on the
THC, D9-tetrahydrocannabinol. cued recollection task. Error bars indicate SEM. *p , .05. THC, D9-
tetrahydrocannabinol.
studies that separate the two phases. Specifically, selectively
administering THC during encoding primarily reduced memory A broad impairment of prefrontal monitoring processes also
for studied pictures and words in the emotional memory task may explain why THC increased false alarms for both critical
and the DRM task, respectively (4,5). By contrast, the current lures and unrelated lures in the DRM task.
study indicates that selectively administering THC during Our finding that THC at retrieval, like some other
retrieval primarily increased false recollection of nonstudied recreational drugs [dextroamphetamine (21), 63,4-
pictures and words in these tasks. methylenedioxymethamphetamine (22)], tended to dispropor-
It is possible that these THC effects on memory retrieval tionately increase emotionally positive false memories is
could be driven by a change in response bias, or a willing- consistent with work showing that positive compared with
ness to give an affirmative memory response at test in the negative memories are more susceptible to distortion (41–43). It
absence of memory retrieval. However, we believe that THC is possible that all of these drugs bias retrieval toward positive
during retrieval increased false recollections. The emotional
memory task we used encouraged participants to recollect
the pictures, and drug effects were found in both high-
confidence errors (cued recollection test) and subjective
recollection judgments (recognition test). Moreover, for the
DRM task, we warned participants during retrieval to avoid
falsely recognizing semantically related words. Thus, in
addition to increasing familiarity-based errors (see
Supplement), we found that THC increased false recollection
errors using two different memory tasks and two different
kinds of stimuli.
One potential mechanism by which THC during retrieval
could increase false recollections is by impairing prefrontal
functions (13,14), including retrieval monitoring processes that
help to suppress false recollections for words and pictures
(15). For example, THC is known to enhance mental imagery Figure 3. Mean hit and false alarm rates on the DRM task. Error bars
(36,37) and processing fluency (38,39), which together can indicate SEM. *p , .05. Crit, critical; DRM, Deese-Roediger-McDermott;
result in false recollections when retrieval monitoring fails (40). THC, D9-tetrahydrocannabinol; Unrel, unrelated.

748 Biological Psychiatry November 15, 2018; 84:743–750 www.sobp.org/journal


Descargado para Anonymous User (n/a) en University Foundation of Health Sciences de ClinicalKey.es por Elsevier en febrero 02, 2020.
Para uso personal exclusivamente. No se permiten otros usos sin autorización. Copyright ©2020. Elsevier Inc. Todos los derechos reservados.
Biological
THC Distorts Emotional Memory Retrieval Psychiatry

false memories because they impact the ventral striatum, a re- 3. Miller LL, McFarland D, Cornett TL, Brightwell D (1977): Marijuana and
gion involved in both positive memory retrieval (19) and general memory impairment: Effect on free recall and recognition memory.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav 7:99–103.
drug effects (16). Alternatively, drug effects on positive false
4. Ballard ME, Gallo DA, de Wit H (2012): Psychoactive drugs and false
memories could be related to a mood-congruent response bias. memory: Comparison of dextroamphetamine and delta-9-
Such a drug-induced increase in the processing of positive tetrahydrocannabinol on false recognition. Psychopharmacology
memories could influence the likelihood of using drugs even if (Berl) 219:15–24.
users lack awareness of such an effect. 5. Ballard ME, Gallo DA, de Wit H (2013): Pre-encoding administration of
These findings have potential implications for the wide- amphetamine or THC preferentially modulates emotional memory in
spread use of cannabis, including its medical use. First, all humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 226:515–529.
users should be aware that single doses of this drug can 6. Ranganathan M, Radhakrishnan R, Addy PH, Schnakenberg-
Martin AM, Williams AH, Carbuto M, et al. (2017): Tetrahydrocannab-
distort episodic memory retrieval at a level that may not be inol (THC) impairs encoding but not retrieval of verbal information.
perceptible to the user. Increased false recollections may Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 79:176–183.
contribute to cannabis’ known relationship with psychosis 7. Ballard ME, Bedi G, de Wit H (2012): Effects of delta-9-
(44). Second, these findings may be especially important for tetrahydrocannabinol on evaluation of emotional images.
PTSD. Individuals with PTSD are predisposed to having J Psychopharmacol 26:1289–1298.
distorted memories (45), and susceptibility to false memory 8. Hindocha C, Freeman TP, Schafer G, Gardener C, Das RK,
Morgan CJA, Curran HV (2015): Acute effects of delta-9-
formation is a complication in psychotherapy (46), especially
tetrahydrocannabinol, cannabidiol and their combination on facial
when adjunct drugs have been involved (47). Our findings, emotion recognition: A randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled
therefore, implicate a potential constraint on the use of THC study in cannabis users. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 25:325–334.
during psychotherapy. Notably, the finding that THC did not 9. Phan KL, Angstadt M, Golden J, Onyewuenyi I, Popovska A, de Wit H
suppress the retrieval of negative memories is at odds with (2008): Cannabinoid modulation of amygdala reactivity to social sig-
the proposal that cannabis can reduce emotional reactivity nals of threat in humans. J Neurosci 28:2313–2319.
10. Rubin DC, Berntsen D, Bohni MK (2008): A memory-based model of
to negative memories and PTSD symptomology more
posttraumatic stress disorder: Evaluating basic assumptions under-
generally (48,49), though THC may impact emotional pro- lying the PTSD diagnosis. Psychol Rev 115:985–1011.
cessing in ways not studied here. As cannabis becomes 11. Grant S, Pedersen ER, Neighbors C (2016): Associations of post-
increasingly accepted for its potentially beneficial properties, traumatic stress disorder symptoms with marijuana and synthetic
rigorous investigations of the impact of THC and other cannabis use among young adult U.S. veterans: A pilot investigation.
cannabinoids on memory and cognition will be important for J Stud Alcohol Drugs 77:509–514.
harm-reduction approaches, as well as for understanding the 12. Kevorkian S, Bonn-Miller MO, Belendiuk K, Carney DM, Roberson-Nay R,
Berenz EC (2015): Associations among trauma, posttraumatic stress dis-
factors that influence drug use.
order, cannabis use, and cannabis use disorder in a nationally represen-
tative epidemiologic sample. Psychol Addict Behav 29:633–638.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS AND DISCLOSURES 13. Bossong MG, Jager G, van Hell HH, Zuurman L, Jansma JM,
Mehta MA, et al. (2012): Effects of D9-tetrahydrocannabinol adminis-
This project was supported by Grant No. DA02812 (to HdW).
tration on human encoding and recall memory function: A pharma-
We thank lab personnel who recruited and screened participants and
cological fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci 24:588–599.
Royce Lee, M.D., for medical oversight.
14. Bossong MG, Jansma JM, van Hell HH, Jager G, Oudman E, Saliasi E,
HdW has received a GRAND research award from Pfizer, donation of a
et al. (2012): Effects of D9-tetrahydrocannabinol on human working
study drug from Indivior, support for a research study from Insys Thera-
memory function. Biol Psychiatry 71:693–699.
peutics, and consulting fees from Bristol-Myers-Squibb, Jazz Pharmaceu-
15. Gallo DA (2006): Associative Illusions of Memory: False Memory
ticals, Marinus, and Organon. None of these were related to the research
Research in DRM and Related Tasks. New York, NY: Psychology
presented here. The other authors report no biomedical financial interests or
Press.
potential conflicts of interest.
16. Yager LM, Garcia AF, Wunsch AM, Ferguson SM (2015): The ins and outs
ClinicalTrials.gov: Effects of THC on Emotional Memory Retrieval;
of the striatum: Role in drug addiction. Neuroscience 301:529–541.
https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT03471585; NCT03471585.
17. Scimeca JM, Badre D (2012): Striatal contributions to declarative
memory retrieval. Neuron 75:380–392.
ARTICLE INFORMATION 18. King DR, Chastelaine M, Elward RL, Wang TH, Rugg MD (2018):
Dissociation between the neural correlates of recollection and famil-
From the Departments of Psychology (MKD, DAG) and Psychiatry and iarity in the striatum and hippocampus: Across-study convergence.
Behavioral Neuroscience (JW, HdW), the University of Chicago, Chicago, Behav Brain Res 354:1–7.
Illinois.
19. Speer ME, Bhanji JP, Delgado MR (2014): Savoring the past: Positive
Address correspondence to Manoj K. Doss, M.Sc., M.A., Department of
memories evoke value representations in the striatum. Neuron 84:847–856.
Psychology, the University of Chicago, 5848 S University Ave, Beecher 308,
20. Abe N, Okuda J, Suzuki M, Sasaki H, Matsuda T, Mori E, et al. (2008):
Chicago, IL 60637; E-mail: mkdoss@uchicago.edu.
Neural correlates of true memory, false memory, and deception. Cereb
Received Jan 8, 2018; revised Apr 5, 2018; accepted Apr 26, 2018.
Cortex 18:2811–2819.
Supplementary material cited in this article is available online at https://
21. Ballard ME, Gallo DA, de Wit H (2014): Amphetamine increases errors
doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.04.020.
during episodic memory retrieval. J Clin Psychopharmacol 34:85–92.
22. Doss MK, Weafer J, Gallo DA, de Wit H (2018): MDMA impairs both the
REFERENCES encoding and retrieval of emotional recollections. Neuro-
1. Curran HV, Freeman TP, Mokrysz C, Lewis DA, Morgan CJ, Parson LH psychopharmacology 43:791–800.
(2016): Keep off the grass? Cannabis, cognition and addiction. Nat 23. Gallo DA (2010): False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the
Rev Neurosci 17:293–306. DRM illusion. Mem Cognit 38:833–848.
2. Ilan AB, Smith ME, Gevins A (2004): Effects of marijuana on neuro- 24. White TL, Justice AJ, de Wit H (2002): Differential subjective effects of
physiological signals of working and episodic memory. Psychophar- D-amphetamine by gender, hormone levels and menstrual cycle
macology (Berl) 176:214–222. phase. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 73:729–741.

Biological Psychiatry November 15, 2018; 84:743–750 www.sobp.org/journal 749


Descargado para Anonymous User (n/a) en University Foundation of Health Sciences de ClinicalKey.es por Elsevier en febrero 02, 2020.
Para uso personal exclusivamente. No se permiten otros usos sin autorización. Copyright ©2020. Elsevier Inc. Todos los derechos reservados.
Biological
Psychiatry THC Distorts Emotional Memory Retrieval

25. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN (1997): International Affective 37. Tart CT (1970): Marijuana intoxication: Common experiences. Nature
Picture System (IAPS): Technical Manual and Affective Ratings. 226:701–704.
Gainesville, FL: National Institute of Mental Health Center for the Study 38. Morgan CJ, Rothwell E, Atkinson H, Mason O, Curran HV (2010):
of Emotion and Attention. Hyper-priming in cannabis users: A naturalistic study of the effects
26. _
Marchewka A, Zurawski Ł, Jednoróg K, Grabowska A (2014): The of cannabis on semantic memory function. Psychiatry Res 176:213–
Nencki Affective Picture System (NAPS): Introduction to a novel, 218.
standardized, wide-range, high-quality, realistic picture database. 39. Schafer G, Feilding A, Morgan CJ, Agathangelou M, Freeman TP,
Behav Res Methods 46:596–610. Curran HV (2012): Investigating the interaction between schizotypy,
27. Gallo DA, Foster KT, Johnson EL (2009): Elevated false recollection of divergent thinking and cannabis use. Conscious Cogn 21:292–298.
emotional pictures in younger and older adults. Psychol Aging 24:981–988. 40. Doss MK, Bluestone MR, Gallo DA (2016): Two mechanisms of
28. Wachtel SR, ElSohly MA, Ross SA, Ambre J, de Wit H (2002): Com- constructive recollection: Perceptual recombination and conceptual
parison of the subjective effects of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol and fluency. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 42:1747–1758.
marijuana in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 161:331–339. 41. Bohn A, Berntsen D (2007): Pleasantness bias in flashbulb memories:
29. Yonelinas AP (2002): The nature of recollection and familiarity: A re- Positive and negative flashbulb memories of the fall of the Berlin Wall
view of 30 years of research. J Mem Lang 46:441–517. among East and West Germans. Mem Cognit 35:565–577.
30. Martin WR, Sloan JW, Sapira JD, Jasinski DR (1971): Physiologic, 42. Kensinger EA, Schacter DL (2006): When the Red Sox shocked the
subjective, and behavioral effects of amphetamine, methamphet- Yankees: Comparing negative and positive memories. Psychon Bull
amine, ephedrine, phenmetrazine, and methylphenidate in man. Clin Rev 13:757–763.
Pharmacol Ther 12:245–258. 43. Levine LJ, Bluck S (2004): Painting with broad strokes: Happiness and
31. Folstein MF, Luria R (1973): Reliability, validity, and clinical application the malleability of event memory. Cogn Emot 18:559–574.
of the Visual Analogue Mood Scale. Psychol Med 3:479–486. 44. Radhakrishnan R, Wilkinson ST, D’Souza DC (2014): Gone to pot – A
32. Morean ME, de Wit H, King AC, Sofuoglu M, Rueger SY, O’Malley SS review of the association between cannabis and psychosis. Front
(2013): The Drug Effects Questionnaire: Psychometric support across Psychiatry 5:54.
three drug types. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 227:177–192. 45. Strange D, Takarangi MKT (2015): Memory distortion for traumatic
33. Doss MK (2017): THC at retrieval drives false recollection of neutral events: The role of mental imagery. Front Psychiatry 6:27.
and emotional episodic memories [dataset]. Open Science Frame- 46. McNally RJ (2003): Recovering memories of trauma. Curr Dir Psychol
work. Available at: https://osf.io/e6b5k/. Accessed May 20, 2018. Sci 12:32–35.
34. Hart CL, Ilan AB, Gevins A, Gunderson EW, Role K, Colley J, Foltin RW 47. Piper A (1993): "Truth serum" and "recovered memories" of sexual
(2010): Neurophysiological and cognitive effects of smoked marijuana abuse: A review of the evidence. J Psychiatry Law 21:447–471.
in frequent users. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 96:333–341. 48. Tull MT, McDermott MJ, Gratz KL (2016): Marijuana dependence
35. Miller L, Cornett T, McFarland D (1978): Marijuana: An analysis of moderate the effect of posttraumatic stress disorder on trauma cue
storage and retrieval deficits in memory with the technique of reactivity in substance dependent patients. Drug Alcohol Depend
restricted reminding. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 8:327–332. 159:219–226.
36. Ames F (1958): A clinical and metabolic study of acute intoxication 49. Yarnell S (2015): The use of medicinal marijuana for posttraumatic
with cannabis sativa and its role in the model psychoses. J Ment Sci stress disorder: A review of the current literature. Prim Care Com-
104:972–999. panion CNS Disord 17:15r01786.

750 Biological Psychiatry November 15, 2018; 84:743–750 www.sobp.org/journal


Descargado para Anonymous User (n/a) en University Foundation of Health Sciences de ClinicalKey.es por Elsevier en febrero 02, 2020.
Para uso personal exclusivamente. No se permiten otros usos sin autorización. Copyright ©2020. Elsevier Inc. Todos los derechos reservados.

You might also like