You are on page 1of 9

doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01256.

Repeatability and heritability of reproductive traits in free-ranging


snakes

G. P. BROWN & R. SHINE


School of Biological Sciences A08, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia

Keywords: Abstract
inheritance; The underlying genetic basis of life-history traits in free-ranging animals is
life history; critical to the effects of selection on such traits, but logistical constraints mean
quantitative genetics; that such data are rarely available. Our long-term ecological studies on free-
reproduction; ranging oviparous snakes (keelbacks, Tropidonophis mairii (Gray, 1841),
reptile. Colubridae) on an Australian floodplain provide the first such data for any
tropical reptile. All size-corrected reproductive traits (egg mass, clutch size,
clutch mass and post-partum maternal mass) were moderately repeatable
between pairs of clutches produced by 69 female snakes after intervals of 49–
1152 days, perhaps because maternal body condition was similar between
clutches. Parent–offspring regression of reproductive traits of 59 pairs of
mothers and daughters revealed high heritability for egg mass (h2 ¼ 0.73,
SE ¼ 0.24), whereas heritability for the other three traits was low (< 0.37).
The estimated heritability of egg mass may be inflated by maternal effects such
as differential allocation of yolk steroids to different-sized eggs. High
heritability of egg size may be maintained (rather than eroded by stabilizing
selection) because selection acts on a trait (hatchling size) that is determined
by the interaction between egg size and incubation substrate rather than by
egg size alone. Variation in clutch size was mainly because of environmental
factors (h2 ¼ 0.04), indicating that one component of the trade-off between
egg size and clutch size is under much tighter genetic control than the other.
Thus, the phenotypic trade-off between egg size and egg number in keelback
snakes occurs because each female snake must allocate a finite amount of
energy into eggs of a genetically determined size.

that are produced during an organism’s lifetime. This


Introduction
strong link to fitness has stimulated an extensive litera-
Organisms display an immense variety in life-history ture on the adaptive significance of life-history variation,
traits such as reproductive modes, reproductive frequen- ranging from mathematical models that predict which
cies, the relative allocation of resources among offspring trait values will maximize fitness, through to field studies
size vs. number, and so forth (Charnov, 1982, 1993; that examine such predictions (Roff, 1992, 2002). How-
Clutton-Brock, 1991). In comparisons among individuals ever, any adaptationist interpretation of life-history
within a single population, such variation bears a direct variation necessarily makes a set of assumptions about
causal link to microevolutionary fitness, which by defi- the traits being studied. Chief among these assumptions
nition depends upon the numbers of surviving progeny is the idea that the observed phenotypic variation (in this
case, in life-history traits) has a genetic basis. If this is not
true – for example, if all variation in a phenotypic trait is
Correspondence: Richard Shine, School of Biological Sciences A08, Uni-
versity of Sydney, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia. because of direct impacts of the local environment
Tel.: +61-2-9351-3772; fax: +61-2-9351-5609; (incubation conditions, food supply, weather, parasites,
e-mail: rics@bio.usyd.edu.au etc.) – then there will be no cumulative change in

ª 2006 THE AUTHORS 20 (2007) 588–596


588 JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2006 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
Genetic basis of life-history variation 589

frequencies of life-history traits across generations (i.e. marked and released in our study area usually reach
no evolution) even if trait frequencies are modified by sexual maturity in less than 1 year (Brown & Shine,
selection (reflecting differential fitness) within each 2002) and they in turn can be captured while gravid and
generation. their clutches collected. Thus, within a relatively short
The functional link between empirical studies of span of time it is possible to acquire data on the clutches
phenotypic selection on the one hand, and evolutionary of female snakes and their daughters. Like most
models on the other, is the underlying genetical structure oviparous snakes, keelbacks lack parental care and a
of the traits in question. Quantifying the genetic bases mother’s investment in each egg is completed at ovula-
and correlations among selectively important traits in tion (Somma, 2003). Hence, there is no prolonged period
wild populations is an onerous but vital task for evolu- of close contact between the mother and her developing
tionary biologists (Lynch, 1999; Réale & Festa-Bianchet, offspring (such as gestation in viviparous animals) during
2000; Réale et al., 2003). The genetic basis of life-history which maternal behaviour or physiology could affect
traits is of special interest because such traits are directly offspring phenotypes. Thus, keelbacks are not subject to
linked to organismal fitness. For these reasons, we need many of the potential sources of maternal effects that are
empirical studies on natural populations (i.e. of free- present in studies of other taxa.
ranging rather than captive animals) to examine the
critical assumption of a genetic basis to within-popula-
Materials and methods
tion variation in life-history traits. Unfortunately, logis-
tical obstacles have meant that field studies of heritability
Study site and species
display a strong taxonomic bias. For example, Mousseau
& Roff’s (1987) review of heritabilities in natural popu- The study took place at the Fogg Dam Conservation
lations did not include any studies on reptiles. Since that Reserve (1234¢S, 13118¢E), 60 km SE of Darwin in the
time there has only been one (Sinervo & Doughty, 1996). wet–dry tropics of Australia’s Northern Territory. Fogg
Two main approaches have been used to assess the Dam is a 200-ha artificial impoundment formed by a
heritability of life-history traits. First, it is relatively easy 1300-m-long earthen wall approximately 3 m high. The
to measure repeatability: for example, the degree of area north of the dam wall is an extensive floodplain of
consistency of reproductive traits (egg size, number, etc.) the Adelaide River. The study area has been described
in successive clutches produced by the same female and illustrated previously (Madsen & Shine, 1996;
snakes. If successive clutches are no more similar to each Brown & Shine, 2002). Keelbacks (T. mairii) are
other (in the number and size of eggs, etc.) than they are medium-sized (female snakes grow to 80 cm snout-vent
to the clutches of other female snakes, then it is likely length, SVL) nonvenomous natricine snakes widely
that maternal genotype is not an important contributor distributed through coastal and near-coastal areas of
to overall life-history variation within the population. northern and north-eastern Australia (Cogger, 1996).
The second and more robust (but logistically demanding) Keelbacks are oviparous and in our study area nesting
method to quantify heritability of reproductive traits is to occurs throughout the drier months of the year (April–
examine inheritance directly (e.g. by comparing repro- November). Some female snakes produce two or more
ductive traits of mothers with those of their adult clutches during this period (Brown & Shine, 2002).
daughters). If genetic factors play an important role, we
would expect daughters to produce similar kinds of
Methods of data collection
clutches (e.g. a few large eggs vs. many small eggs) as
produced by their mothers. We patrolled the dam wall by car and on foot looking for
Our long-term mark-recapture study on an oviparous snakes during 1804 nights between May 1998 and
snake (the keelback, Tropidonophis mairii, Gray, 1841) in December 2003. Keelbacks were captured by hand and
the Australian wet–dry tropics provides an opportunity to returned to a field laboratory where they were marked,
utilize both of these methods to investigate the quanti- weighed and measured. If shelled eggs were evident by
tative genetics of female reproductive traits in free- palpation of a female snake’s abdomen, she was retained
ranging reptiles. Several aspects of the ecology of these in captivity. Gravid female snakes were housed indoors
snakes make them useful models for studies of quanti- in plastic cages containing a water dish and a container of
tative genetics in the wild. First, we are able to collect moist vermiculite for oviposition. On average, gravid
large numbers of female snakes as they move to nesting female snakes oviposited 6.5 days (SD ¼ 3.4) after cap-
sites, so that we can obtain data on their clutches laid ture. After oviposition, female snakes were reweighed
within a few days of capture (hence minimizing artefacts and then released at their capture location.
because of prolonged captivity). Second, we can incubate Eggs were measured and weighed within 12 h of being
the eggs under standardized conditions, thereby reducing laid. Each clutch was placed in a separate plastic bag
environmental sources of variance in phenotypic traits of containing damp vermiculite and incubated in an insu-
hatchlings (e.g. nest-site selection: Rhen & Lang, 1995; lated (Styrofoam) container at room temperature (aver-
Deeming, 2004). Third, hatchlings that are individually age 24.0 C, SD ¼ 3.0, range 16.0–32.0 C). Most eggs

ª 2006 THE AUTHORS 20 (2007) 588–596


JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2006 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
590 G. P. BROWN AND R. SHINE

were incubated on a mixture of 1:1 vermiculite to water (a) 3


by mass, but some were incubated on vermiculite-water
mixtures ranging in water content from 10 % to 600 %
2
as part of experiments on the effects of incubation

Offspring residual egg mass


moisture on hatchling phenotypes (Brown & Shine,
2004). Within 24 h of hatching, neonates were sexed, 1
measured (SVL, head length, tail length and mass) and
individually marked. They were then released at their
0
mother’s capture location.
The suite of reproductive traits we investigated com-
prised: (1) mean egg mass; (2) clutch size; (3) total clutch –1
mass; and (4) maternal post-partum body condition (i.e.
mass relative to snout-vent length). Because snakes
–2
continue to grow after maturation, a population typically –3 –2 –1 0 1 2 3
contains adult individuals that span a wide range of body Maternal residual egg mass
sizes (Andrews, 1982). A female snake’s body size
strongly affects her reproductive output: larger female
snakes typically produce more (and sometimes, larger) (b) 2
offspring (Seigel & Ford, 1987). Thus, analyses of
heritability need to be based on size-corrected measures;

Offspring residual clutch size


1
otherwise, the degree of similarity would depend upon
the respective body sizes of the two female snakes being
compared. To correct for body size effects, we calculated 0
residual scores from linear regressions of ln-transformed
clutch traits on ln-transformed maternal SVL. Residual
scores from the regressions of post-partum mass on –1
maternal SVL were also calculated, to provide estimates
of post-partum body condition. The residual scores from
–2
these regressions were then used in the calculation of
repeatabilities and heritabilities.
–3
–4 –3 –2 –1 0 1 2 3
Repeatabilities
Maternal residual clutch size
Measuring repeatability is often suggested as an initial
step for quantitative genetics studies. Significant repea- Fig. 1 Parent–offspring regressions for reproductive traits in free-
tability in a trait within individuals suggests that the trait ranging keelback snakes. The upper graph (a) shows residual egg
may have a genetic basis, and values of repeatability are mass (i.e. residual score from the linear regression of ln-mean egg
often thought to estimate an upper limit to heritability mass vs. ln-maternal snout-vent length and standardized to mean ¼
0, variance ¼ 1). This trait was significantly heritable; for example,
(but, see Dohm, 2002). Over the study period, we
mothers that produced unusually large eggs had daughters that also
captured a total of 69 individual female snakes that were
produced unusually large eggs. The lower graph (b) shows residual
gravid at more than one capture. Thus, we could clutch size (i.e. residual score from the linear regression of ln-clutch
calculate within-individual repeatability scores for the size vs. ln-maternal snout-vent length and standardized to mean ¼
clutch and maternal characteristics enumerated above, 0, variance ¼ 1). No significant heritability was evident for clutch
using intraclass correlations (Lynch & Walsh, 1998). size relative to maternal body size. The filled symbol represents an
outlying data point not included in the analysis.

Heritability
A total of 59 female neonatal keelbacks that hatched in ations (Lynch & Walsh, 1998). Thus, we standardized
the laboratory were later recaptured as gravid adults. size-corrected traits (such that mean ¼ 0, variance ¼ 1)
Thus, we could compare the clutch characteristics of before regression analyses were carried out. Standard-
these 59 daughters to those of their mothers using ized, size-corrected values of each reproductive trait for
parent–offspring regressions [a single outlying data point each daughter were regressed on those of her mother
for clutch size where a mother that produced a small using ordinary least squares. The slope of the linear
clutch had a daughter that produced a large clutch (see regression equation was multiplied by two to yield
Fig. 1b) was removed from analyses]. In parent–offspring narrow-sense heritability (h2), and the standard error of
regression analyses, it is important for variances of traits the slope was multiplied by two to provide the standard
to be equivalent between parental and offspring gener- error of h2 (Lynch & Walsh, 1998). h2 measures the

ª 2006 THE AUTHORS 20 (2007) 588–596


JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2006 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
Genetic basis of life-history variation 591

proportion of phenotypic variance explained by additive These significant repeatabilities suggest that reproduc-
genetic variance; low h2 values indicate a high degree of tive traits may have a genetic component, although we
environmental influence on phenotypes whereas a high emphasize that the repeatability could also be entirely
h2 indicates a high degree of additive genetic influence. because of environmental or nongenetic factors. If we
Quantitative genetic parameters were estimated using a found no repeatability it would be reasonable not to
bootstrap approach to estimate (with replacement) her- continue with heritability calculations (Postma & Van
itability and genetic correlations and their respective Noordwijk, 2005). The significant repeatability within
standard errors (program H 2 B O O T : Phillips, 1998). We individual keelbacks indicates that it is worth calculating
report estimates based on 10 000 bootstrap replicate actual heritabilities.
samples.
Heritability
Results
Table 2 presents calculated heritabilities of reproductive
traits for the 59 cases in which we recorded clutches from
Repeatability
female snakes and their adult daughters. The mean
The mean duration between successive clutches by the interval between the dates that clutches were produced
same female snake was 336 days (SE ¼ 19.6 days, by mothers vs. their daughters was 640 days (SE ¼
range ¼ 49–1152). Both maternal body size and clutch 31.3 days, range ¼ 323–1453). On average, the SVLs of
size increased significantly from the first to the second reproducing female snakes in the first generation were
clutch (respective means 62.0 vs. 67.3 cm SVL, paired larger than those of their daughters when they repro-
t68 ¼ 11.51, P < 0.0001; 9.9 vs. 12.3 eggs, paired t68 ¼ duced (mean SVLs 63.4 vs. 59.7 cm; paired t58 ¼ 3.47,
6.95, P < 0.0001), but mean egg mass remained rela- P ¼ 0.001) and as a result clutch sizes in the first
tively constant (2.84 vs. 2.80 g; paired t68 ¼ 0.70, P ¼ generation were larger than those in the second genera-
0.48). Most reproductive traits were relatively consistent tion (means of 10.95 vs. 8.95 eggs; paired t58 ¼ 3.65,
in comparisons between successive clutches produced by P < 0.001). However, the mean mass of eggs produced
the same individual female snakes (Table 1). Repeatabil- was similar in the two generations (mean egg masses
ities of egg mass and clutch size were similar (0.26 vs. 2.73 vs. 2.82 g; paired t58 ¼ 1.50, P ¼ 0.14). The clear
0.28). However, post-partum body condition was even result from our calculation of heritabilities is that only
more highly repeatable, suggesting that any environ- one of these four traits is strikingly similar between
mental effects related to maternal body condition would mothers and daughters: the size of eggs that they produce
have tended to generate high repeatability in clutch (Table 2). Based on 95 % confidence intervals, the
traits. In keeping with this possibility, post-partum lowest bound for the estimate of true heritability of egg
maternal condition after the first clutch was highly mass is 0.25. Raw measures of egg mass (i.e. not
correlated with pre-partum condition at the next clutch corrected for maternal SVL, or standardized to equal
(N ¼ 69, r ¼ 0.52, P < 0.0001). In other words, the variance) also are significantly heritable between moth-
energetic circumstances of a female snake apparently ers and their daughters (slope ¼ 0.54, SE ¼ 0.20;
did not vary much between her two successive clutches. P < 0.01). Despite the large standard error associated
with egg mass heritability, it is the only trait for which
the mother–daughter regression is statistically significant.
Heritability of the other traits is low, and nowhere close
Table 1 Repeatability of reproductive traits from 69 free-ranging
female keelback snakes that each produced two clutches of eggs
during our study. Table 2 Mother–daughter regression results for reproductive traits
from 59 pairs of female keelbacks and their daughters.
Trait F P Repeatability Pearson r
Trait F P r2 Slope (SE) h2 (SE)
Egg mass 1.72 0.014 0.26 0.28
Clutch mass 1.67 0.019 0.25 0.27 Egg mass 8.69 0.0046 0.13 0.36 (0.12) 0.73 (0.24)
Clutch size 1.77 0.01 0.28 0.34 Clutch mass 0.00 0.99 0.00 0.00 (0.12) 0.00 (0.25)
Post-partum condition 31.5 0.0001 0.54 0.57 Clutch size 0.007 0.93 0.00 0.02 (0.15) 0.04 (0.28)
Post-partum 1.82 0.18 0.03 0.18 (0.13) 0.37 (0.23)
All analyses are based on trait values corrected for the influence of condition
maternal body size (residuals of ln-trait regressed on ln-SVL). F, P
and repeatability values are derived from A N O V A s using maternal All analyses are based on trait values corrected for the influence of
identity number as the independent variable. Pearson correlation maternal body size (residuals of ln-trait regressed on ln-SVL). Trait
coefficients are based on correlations between values for clutch 1 values were then standardized to zero mean and unit variance.
and clutch 2. All traits are more similar between successive clutches Heritability values and standard errors were derived from 10 000
from the same female than would have been expected by chance. bootstrap samples. Of the four traits examined, only egg mass
Boldface font shows P < 0.05. appears to be significantly heritable. Boldface font shows P < 0.05.

ª 2006 THE AUTHORS 20 (2007) 588–596


JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2006 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
592 G. P. BROWN AND R. SHINE

to statistical significance. For example, the estimated during incubation or hatchling SVL, as well as original
heritability of clutch size is 0.04, with an upper 95% egg mass, as significant influences on subsequent egg
confidence limit of 0.58 (Table 2). Thus, it appears that sizes. In practice, this regression identified original egg
egg mass could respond rapidly to selection within this mass as the sole significant variable (F1,30 ¼ 14.97,
population whereas clutch size could not. P < 0.001): that is, the size of egg a female snake
produced depended upon the size of the egg from which
she originated, and not on the incubation conditions of
Maternal effects
that egg nor on her size as a hatchling. This has to be
The heritability of egg mass, estimated by parent– either a genetic effect, or a maternal effect of egg size per
offspring regression, may be inflated because it includes se (e.g. if hormone or nutrient levels consistently covary
nongenetic maternal effects (Lynch & Walsh, 1998). For with egg size) rather than a maternal effect acting
example, a tendency to produce large eggs could be through large size at hatching. Similar analyses on
transferred from mother to offspring simply because relative fecundity of daughters (clutch size relative to
larger eggs produce larger babies that in turn, grow maternal body size) revealed that factors such as the mass
quickly and tend to produce large eggs of their own. That of the egg from which a female keelback emerged, or the
is, some direct fitness consequence of hatchling size size of the clutch from which she came, were not
(rather than egg size per se) could generate a consistency significantly related to her subsequent fecundity (all
across generations in egg size. We could test this scenario F < 2.26, all P > 0.36).
by experimentally manipulating offspring size relative to
egg size, to break apart the usual correlation between
Trade-off between egg size and clutch size
these traits. One approach to achieve this aim would be
allometric engineering (yolk removal, follicle ablation: Our data set allows us to examine both phenotypic
Sinervo & Huey, 1990). However, a simpler method is correlations among reproductive traits, and the underly-
fortuitously available from our data: modification of ing genetic covariance. First, we consider phenotypic
incubation conditions to increase or reduce hatchling size correlations. Table 3 (above-diagonal values) reveals a
experimentally relative to egg mass. In keelbacks, significant negative phenotypic correlation ()0.24)
incubation on relatively dry substrates severely reduces between relative egg mass and relative clutch size among
hatchling size, because much of the yolk mass desiccates the 118 clutches produced in our mother–daughter data
and is left behind in the eggshell (Brown & Shine, 2004). set. A similar correlation coefficient is derived from the
We have data on moisture uptake (mass of egg at 138 clutches produced by the 69 ‘repeat’ female snakes
hatching vs. at oviposition) for 32 of the 59 maternal eggs (Pearson r ¼ )0.44, P < 0.0001) analysed earlier in this
from the mother–daughter data set analysed above. Thus, paper. Interpretation of most of the phenotypic correla-
we conducted stepwise multiple regression with the tions in Table 3 is straightforward. Female keelbacks that
daughter’s mean egg mass as the dependent variable and produce large eggs tend to produce fewer eggs; female
traits relating to the egg from which she originally snakes that invest more in their clutches tend to have
hatched (egg mass, water uptake of egg and incubation more and larger eggs; and female snakes that invest more
period) as well as her traits as a hatchling (SVL and mass) energy into their clutches also tend to be in a better body
as the independent variables. If the size of eggs produced condition after laying.
by a female keelback depend upon her own size at The consistent negative correlation between clutch
hatching rather than the mass of the egg from which she size and egg mass (after both variables are corrected for
emerged, such a regression should identify water uptake maternal body size) suggests a trade-off between these

Table 3 Phenotypic correlations (above


Relative clutch Post-partum
diagonal) and genetic correlations (below
Egg mass Clutch size mass condition
diagonal) between reproductive traits of
Egg mass – )0.24 0.38 0.27 keelbacks.
Clutch size )0.60 (1.35) – 0.75 0.31
(45)
Relative clutch mass 1.63 (2.18) )0.90 (3.82) – 0.49
(50) (73)
Post-partum condition 0.89 (0.95) )0.32 (2.14) 0.44 (2.30) –
(6) (50) (53)

Genetic correlation values are followed by standard errors in parentheses and the values
beneath represent the percentage of undefined estimates among 10 000 bootstrap samples.
Phenotypic correlations are derived from pooled data on the mother keelbacks and their
daughters (hence n ¼ 118). Boldface font shows P < 0.05.

ª 2006 THE AUTHORS 20 (2007) 588–596


JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2006 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
Genetic basis of life-history variation 593

two variables. Similar trade-offs have been documented (a) 3


in many reptile species (Seigel & Ford, 1987; Olsson & 2.5
Shine, 1997) but only one previous study has examined

Daughter residual egg mass


whether the trade-off has a genetic component, or is 2
simply forced by physical constraints (Sinervo & 1.5
Doughty, 1996). Unfortunately, calculating genetic 1
correlations from natural populations with relatively
0.5
small samples involves significant problems (Lynch,
1999; Réale & Roff, 2001). Standard errors were higher 0
than correlation estimates for all of the genetic correla- –0.5
tions that we calculated, and negative variance esti- –1
mates resulted in undefined values in up to 73% of
–1.5
bootstrap samples (Table 3), thus the estimates may be
specious. Nonetheless, the signs of the genetic correla- –2
tions match those of the phenotypic correlations in five –4 –3 –2 –1 0 1 2 3
of the six cases. The genetic correlation between egg Maternal residual clutch size
mass and clutch size ()0.60) has two components: one
(b) 2
derived from the relationship between a mother’s clutch
size and the daughter’s egg size, and a second relating 1.5

Daughter residual clutch size


the mother’s egg size to the daughter’s clutch size. 1
Looking at these components separately reveals no
0.5
tendency for mothers that produce large clutches to
have daughters that produce small eggs (Fig. 2a; egg 0
mass ¼ 0.00–0.09 · residual clutch size; r2 ¼ 0.008, –0.5
d.f. ¼ 1,57, P ¼ 0.51), or for mothers that produce –1
small eggs to have daughters that produce large clutches
–1.5
(Fig. 2b; residual clutch size ¼ 0.00 + 0.05 · residual
egg mass; r2 ¼ 0.002, d.f. ¼ 1,57, P ¼ 0.74). Thus, the –2
genetic relationship between the two traits remains –2.5
ambiguous. –3
–3 –2 –1 0 1 2 3
Discussion Maternal residual egg mass

For wild populations, often it is only feasible to investi- Fig. 2 Evidence for lack of genetic covariance between egg mass and
gate the heritability of hatchling phenotypes as it is clutch size in keelback snakes, based on relationships between trait
relatively easy to obtain litters of newborns. Investigating values in female keelbacks and their adult female offspring. All traits
the heritability of adult phenotypes (e.g. reproductive are standardized residuals (i.e. residual score from the linear
traits) is more difficult because this requires waiting for regression of ln-trait vs. ln-maternal snout-vent length and stan-
hatchlings of known relatedness to mature and express dardized to mean ¼ 0, variance ¼ 1). These data reveal no signifi-
the phenotype. These logistical constraints have discour- cant tendency (a) for mothers that produce large clutches to have
daughters that produce small eggs, or (b) for mothers that produce
aged attempts to estimate heritabilities for life-history
small eggs to have daughters that produce large clutches.
traits in free-living vertebrates (Roff & Mousseau, 1987)
and data are particularly scarce (virtually unavailable) for
nontraditional study organisms. for relative clutch size and offspring size, relative clutch
Lacking data on genetic relationships among free- mass and post-partum body condition (Madsen & Shine,
living animals, many investigators have adopted the 1992). In a sample of seven female smooth snakes,
more logistically feasible technique of estimating repeat- Luiselli et al. (1996) reported significant repeatability for
abilities of life-history traits for individuals recorded to several traits (offspring length and mass, RCM, %still-
reproduce on more than one occasion. Consistency in a births) but most of these effects disappeared after
trait within an individual suggests, but does not prove, a correction for female body size. In a larger oviparous
genetic basis (Postma & Van Noordwijk, 2005). Such snake species (water python, Liasis fuscus, Peters, 1873)
analyses on snakes have been conducted for two from the same study area as our keelbacks, successive
viviparous European species, the adder Vipera berus, clutches from 13 female snakes (1–2 years apart)
Linnaeus, 1766 (Madsen & Shine, 1992) and the smooth revealed significant repeatability in offspring size and
snake Coronella austriaca, Laurenti, 1768 (Luiselli et al., hatching success, but not in fecundity or maternal body
1996). Data were available for ‘repeat’ litters for only six condition (Madsen & Shine, 1996). In a laboratory study
female adders, but suggested high levels of repeatability on an oviparous African colubrid snake (Lamprophis

ª 2006 THE AUTHORS 20 (2007) 588–596


JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2006 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
594 G. P. BROWN AND R. SHINE

fuliginosus, Boie, 1827), Ford & Seigel (2006) reported selection will erode variance (i.e. any alleles coding for
repeatabilities of egg size and litter size among four nonoptimal trait values will be eliminated: Roff, 1992,
successive litters of 0.67 and 0.65 respectively. 2002). Thus, additive genetic variance (and hence,
Although repeatability is often considered to repre- heritability) for fitness-relevant traits will be low. In
sent an upper limit to heritability, Dohm (2002) this simplistic form, the hypothesis predicts that in our
identified situations in which repeatability could under- keelback study population, the trait with highest herit-
estimate heritability. In the present study, heritability of ability (egg size) should be relatively unimportant for
egg mass (0.73) was substantially higher than repeata- fitness. Our mark-recapture studies strongly falsify this
bility (0.26). Maternal effects may explain the discrep- prediction. Larger eggs tend to produce larger hatchlings
ancy between these estimates (Dohm, 2002). If the (Shine & Brown, 2002; Brown & Shine, 2004), and
repeatability of 0.26 represents the true upper limit of hatchling size is a primary determinant of a snake’s
heritability of egg mass, then a large maternal effect probability of surviving through its first year of life
must be conflating our heritability estimate of 0.73. We (Brown & Shine, 2004). In these short-lived animals,
demonstrated that this maternal effect is unrelated to survival for the first year (i.e. through to maturation:
hatchling size, but we cannot rule out more subtle Brown & Shine, 2002) must be responsible for a
maternal effects on hatchling composition. For example, significant proportion of the total variation in lifetime
larger eggs may contain higher levels of specific fitness among individuals within the population. How,
hormones, nutrients or minerals that affect some aspect then, can heritability be so high for a trait that is so
of physiology of the resultant hatchling, which in turn strongly associated with fitness?
allows them to produce larger eggs when they attain The answer to this paradox may lie in the fact that egg
reproductive age. Testing this hypothesis would require mass is not the only determinant of hatchling size. As
a detailed analysis of yolk composition in eggs of noted above, the size of a hatchling keelback depends
different sizes, as well as manipulations of yolk compo- primarily upon two factors: the mass of the egg from
sition to detect putative cross-generation effects. Such which it emerges, and the moisture content of the
work would be logistically challenging, but it is encour- substrate on which that egg has incubated. At any given
aging to note that in preliminary analyses of keelback egg mass, offspring size is enhanced by incubation in a
eggs we have found a significant correlation between moister rather than drier nest site (Shine & Brown, 2002;
egg size and the concentration of the sex hormone Brown & Shine, 2005a,b). Selection may thus work
dihydrotestosterone (R. Radder, G.P. Brown and R. intensely on genes for maternal nest-site selection,
Shine, unpublished data). eroding underlying genetic variance in that trait, and
In contrast to egg mass analyses, our estimates of coincidentally preserving significant genetic variation for
repeatability of the clutch size and relative clutch mass egg mass per se (Brown & Shine, 2005b).
were substantially higher than our estimates of herita- Although trade-offs between egg size and clutch size
bility for these variables. The high repeatability, despite a play a central role in life-history models (e.g. Smith &
low heritability, may be attributable to the ‘repeat’ Fretwell, 1974; Kaplan & Cooper, 1984; Olsson & Shine,
female snakes tending to be in similar energetic condition 1997) and have been demonstrated at the phenotypic
when they produced their two litters (i.e. repeatability of level in many organisms (including snakes: Seigel &
post-partum maternal condition ¼ 0.56). Repeatability is Ford, 1987), we know surprisingly little about the genetic
a much easier parameter to assess in natural conditions basis of this trade-off. Roff’s (2002) review of this topic is
than is heritability, but on its own may provide little based on only three empirical examples, involving lizards
insight into the genetic framework underlying the trait. (Sinervo & Doughty, 1996), fishes (Snyder, 1991) and
Nonetheless, if heritability data are available also (as in fruit flies (Schwarzkopf et al., 1999). All of these studies
the present study), estimates of repeatability may prove indicate (or at least, hint at) the existence of a negative
useful in interpreting the overall role of genetic, envi- genetic correlation between egg size and clutch size.
ronmental and maternal effects in engendering trait However, in a long-term field study on lesser snow geese
variation. (Anser caerulescens, Linnaeus, 1758), Lessells et al. (1989)
How heritable is egg mass in free-living keelbacks? found patterns similar to those seen in our keelback
Our estimate of heritability for this trait (h2 ¼ 0.73) may snakes. They reported high heritability of egg size (0.60)
be inflated by two effects. First, we removed a potential whereas heritability of clutch size was low (0.15) and the
source of environmental variation by incubating eggs two traits were correlated phenotypically but not gen-
under controlled conditions. Second, potential maternal etically. Furthermore, recent selection studies on insects
effects on egg yolk constitution (see above) may have suggest that the quality of the environment plays a major
exaggerated mother–daughter similarity in egg mass. role in determining whether or not genetic trade-offs
Regardless, it is clear that in keelback snakes, egg mass is between offspring size and number are detected (Czesak
more heritable than is clutch size (h2 ¼ 0.04). An & Fox, 2003; Blanckenhorn & Heyland, 2004). Similarly,
extensive literature predicts that traits with a large food supply affects whether or not a phenotypic trade-off
effect on fitness should have low heritability, because is apparent between offspring size and number in the

ª 2006 THE AUTHORS 20 (2007) 588–596


JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2006 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
Genetic basis of life-history variation 595

sand lizard, Lacerta vivipara, Jacquin, 1787 (Uller &


Acknowledgments
Olsson, 2005). Such studies demonstrate the complex-
ities of detecting genetic trade-offs, especially in natural We thank C. Shilton and the staff of Beatrice Hill Farm
populations. Nonetheless, free-living animals are the for assistance in a multitude of ways, and the Australian
vehicles through which the quantitative genetics pro- Research Council for funding. The comments of B.
cesses that are of interest to evolutionary ecologists Phillips and two anonymous reviewers substantially
occur. Therefore, documenting heritabilities and genetic improved the manuscript.
covariances in nonartificial settings provides necessary
benchmarks for interpreting the results of laboratory-
References
based studies.
Our analyses suggest that variation in egg size within Andrews, R.M. 1982. Patterns of growth in reptiles. In: Biology of
the keelback population has a strong genetic under- the Reptilia, Vol. 13 (G. Gans & F. H. Pough, eds), pp. 273–320.
pinning, whereas variation in clutch size is mainly Academic Press, New York.
determined by external factors. The contrast is even Blanckenhorn, W.U. & Heyland, A. 2004. The quantitative
genetics of two life history trade-offs in the yellow dung fly in
stronger if one looks at the raw data, without correcting
abundant and limited food environments. Evol. Ecol. 18: 385–
for variation in maternal body size. Mean egg mass shifts 402.
only slightly with maternal SVL (2.5–3.2 g, < 30 % Brown, G.P. & Shine, R. 2002. Reproductive ecology of a tropical
difference between smallest and largest female snakes, natricine snake, Tropidonophis mairii (Colubridae). J. Zool.
coefficient of variation ¼ 0.14) whereas clutch size (Lond.) 258: 63–72.
increases by about 500 % from small to large female Brown, G.P. & Shine, R. 2004. Maternal nest-site choice and
snakes (4–20 eggs, coefficient of variation ¼ 0.28; Brown offspring fitness in a tropical snake (Tropidonophis mairii,
& Shine, 2002). Thus, much of the phenotypic variance Colubridae). Ecology 85: 1627–1634.
in clutch size is generated by variation in female body Brown, G.P. & Shine, R. 2005a. Do changing moisture levels
size, in turn reflecting growth rates and age structures. In during incubation influence phenotypic traits of hatchling
snakes (Tropidonophis mairii)? Physiol. Biochem. Zool. 78: 524–
contrast, egg size may be largely determined by additive
530.
genetic variance. Brown, G.P. & Shine, R. 2005b. Female phenotype, life-history,
Lastly, we consider the mechanisms responsible for and reproductive success in free-ranging snakes (Tropidonophis
the observed phenotypic trade-off between egg size and mairii). Ecology 86: 2763–2770.
clutch size in keelbacks. For simplicity, we can envisage Charnov, E.L. 1982. The Theory of Sex Allocation. Princeton
three types of links between egg size and fecundity from University Press, Princeton.
a life-history perspective (actually, these are end points Charnov, E.L. 1993. Life History Invariants. Oxford University
on a continua of possibilities). First, both traits might be Press, Oxford.
under tight genetic control, so that their covariation is Clutton-Brock, T.H. 1991. The Evolution of Parental Care. Prince-
caused by genetic linkage. This scenario is supported by ton University Press, Princeton.
Cogger, H.G. 1996. Reptiles and Amphibians of Australia. Reed
the studies reviewed by Roff (2002), including the only
Books, Port Melbourne.
previous analysis on reptiles (Sinervo & Doughty, Czesak, M. & Fox, C.W. 2003. Evolutionary ecology of egg size
1996). Second, variation in both traits could be and number in a seed beetle: genetic trade-off differs between
generated by proximate environmental factors, so that environments. Evolution 57: 1121–1132.
their covariation results from some kind of constraint Deeming, D.C. 2004. Post-hatching phenotypic effects of incu-
(e.g. finite energy or abdominal space) combined with bation on reptiles. In: Reptilian Incubation. Environment, Evolu-
stochastic variation in both egg size and fecundity. tion and Behaviour (D. C. Deeming, ed.), pp. 229–251.
Third, one of the traits might be under strong genetic Nottingham University Press, Nottingham.
control whereas the other is not, in which case the Dohm, M.R. 2002. Repeatability estimates do not always set an
trade-off arises because one of the traits is determined upper limit to heritability. Funct. Ecol. 16: 273–280.
Du, W., Ji, X. & Shine, R. 2005. Does body-volume constrain
genetically and the other takes whatever value is
reproductive output in lizards? Biol. Lett. 1: 98–100.
possible under space or energy constraints (Olsson & Ford, N.B. & Seigel, R.A. 2006. Intra-individual variation in
Shine, 1997; Du et al., 2005). Our data on keelbacks clutch and offspring size in an oviparous snake. J. Zool. (Lond.)
clearly support the latter possibility. When a female 268: 171–176.
keelback reproduces, her egg size is largely determined Kaplan, R.H. & Cooper, W.S. 1984. The evolution of develop-
by her genetic inheritance whereas her clutch size is set mental plasticity in reproductive characteristics: an application
by whatever proximate cues determine her total energy of the ’adaptive coin-flipping principle’. Am. Nat. 123: 393–
allocation to reproduction. This conclusion accords well 410.
with the observation that despite significant annual Lessells, C.M., Cooke, F. & Rockwell, R.F. 1989. Is there a trade-
variation in traits such as clutch sizes and Relative off between egg weight and clutch size in wild Lesser Snow
Geese (Anser c. caerulescens)? J. Evol. Biol. 2: 457–472.
Clutch Masses, mean egg sizes remain the same among
Luiselli, L., Capula, M. & Shine, R. 1996. Reproductive output,
years in the keelback population (G.P. Brown & R. costs of reproduction, and ecology of the smooth snake,
Shine, unpublished data).

ª 2006 THE AUTHORS 20 (2007) 588–596


JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2006 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY
596 G. P. BROWN AND R. SHINE

Coronella austriaca, in the eastern Italian Alps. Oecologia 106: Roff, D.A. 2002. Life History Evolution. Sinauer Associates,
100–110. Sunderland, MA.
Lynch, M. 1999. Estimating genetic correlations in natural Roff, D.A. & Mousseau, T.A. 1987. Quantitative genetics and
populations. Genet. Res. 74: 255–264. fitness: lessons from Drosophila. Heredity 58: 103–118.
Lynch, M. & Walsh, B. 1998. Genetics and Analysis of Quantitative Schwarzkopf, L., Blows, M.W. & Caley, M.J. 1999. Life-history
Traits. Sinauer, Sunderland, MA. consequences of divergent selection on egg size in Drosophila
Madsen, T. & Shine, R. 1992. Determinants of reproductive melanogaster. Am. Nat. 154: 333–341.
success in female adders, Vipera berus. Oecologia 92: 40–47. Seigel, R.A. & Ford, N.B. 1987. Reproductive ecology. In: Snakes:
Madsen, T. & Shine, R. 1996. Determinants of reproductive Ecology and Evolutionary Biology (R. A. Seigel, J. T. Collins & S.
output in female water pythons (Liasis fuscus: Pythonidae). S. Novak, eds), pp. 210–252. Macmillan, New York.
Herpetologica 52: 146–159. Shine, R. & Brown, G.P. 2002. Effects of seasonally varying
Mousseau, T.A. & Roff, D.A. 1987. Natural selection and the hydric conditions on hatchling phenotypes of keelback snakes
heritability of fitness components. Heredity 59: 181–197. (Tropidonophis mairii, Colubridae) from the Australian wet–dry
Olsson, M. & Shine, R. 1997. The limits to reproductive output: tropics. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 76: 339–347.
offspring size versus number in the sand lizard (Lacerta agilis). Sinervo, B. & Doughty, P. 1996. Interactive effects of offspring
Am. Nat. 149: 179–188. size and timing of reproduction on offspring reproduction:
Phillips, P.C. 1998. H2BOOT: Bootstrap Estimates and Tests of experimental, maternal, and quantitative genetic aspects.
Quantitative Genetic Data. University of Oregon, USA. Software, Evolution 50: 1314–1327.
URL http://www.uoregon.edu/~pphil/software.html. Sinervo, B. & Huey, R.B. 1990. Allometric engineering: an
Postma, E. & Van Noordwijk, A.J. 2005. Genetic variation for experimental test of the causes of interpopulational differ-
clutch size in natural populations of birds from a reaction ences in locomotor performance. Science 248: 1106–1109.
norm perspective. Ecology 86: 2344–2357. Smith, C.C. & Fretwell, S.D. 1974. The optimal balance between
Réale, D. & Festa-Bianchet, M. 2000. Quantitative genetics of size and number of offspring. Am. Nat. 108: 499–506.
life-history traits in a long-lived wild mammal. Heredity 85: Snyder, R.J. 1991. Quantitative genetic analyses of life histories
593–603. in two freshwater populations of the threespined stickleback.
Réale, D. & Roff, D. 2001. Estimating genetic correlations in Copeia 1991: 526–529.
natural populations in the absence of pedigree information: Somma, L.A. 2003. Parental Behavior in Lepidosaurian and
accuracy and precision of the Lynch method. Evolution 55: Testudinian Reptiles. Krieger Publishing, Melbourne, FL.
1249–1255. Uller, T. & Olsson, M. 2005. Trade-offs between offspring size
Réale, D., Berteaux, D., McAdam, A.G. & Boutin, S. 2003. and number in the lizard Lacerta vivipara: a comparison
Lifetime selection on heritable life-history traits in a natural between field and laboratory conditions. J. Zool. (Lond.) 265:
population of red squirrels. Evolution 57: 2416–2423. 295–299.
Rhen, T. & Lang, J.W. 1995. Phenotypic plasticity for growth in
the common snapping turtle: effects of incubation tempera- Received 21 August 2006; revised 30 August 2006; accepted 30 August
ture, clutch, and their interaction. Am. Nat. 146: 726–747. 2006
Roff, D.A. 1992. The Evolution of Life Histories. Chapman & Hall,
New York.

ª 2006 THE AUTHORS 20 (2007) 588–596


JOURNAL COMPILATION ª 2006 EUROPEAN SOCIETY FOR EVOLUTIONARY BIOLOGY

You might also like