You are on page 1of 7

Journal of Clinical Neuroscience xxx (xxxx) xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Clinical Neuroscience


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jocn

Clinical study

The effects of dual-task in patients with Parkinson’s disease performing


cognitive-motor paradigms
Chang Hsiu-Chen a,b,c, Chen Chiung-Chu a,d,h, Liaw Jiunn-Woei e,f, Chiou Wei-Da b,g, Weng Yi-Hsin a,d,
Chang Ya-Ju d,b,e,1, Lu Chin-Song d,c,1,⇑
a
Division of Movement Disorders, Department of Neurology, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital at Linkou, Taiwan
b
School of Physical Therapy and Graduate Institute of Rehabilitation Science, College of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan
c
Professor Lu Neurological Clinic, Taoyuan, Taiwan
d
Neuroscience Research Center, Chang Gung Memorial Hospital at Linkou, Taiwan
e
Healthy Aging Research Center, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan
f
Department of Mechanical Engineering, College of Engineering, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan
g
Department of Physical Rehabilitation, Kaohsiung Armed Forces General Hospital, Kaohsiung, Taiwan
h
School of Medicine, College of Medicine, Chang Gung University, Taoyuan, Taiwan

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) exhibit impaired dual-task (DT) performance. A recent meta-
Received 4 October 2019 analysis confirmed that dual tasking severely affects walking performance in PD patients. However,
Accepted 5 January 2020 one report indicated that a cycling DT paradigm has facilitative effects on cognition. We investigated
Available online xxxx
the effects of dual tasking by using walking and cycling as motor tasks and revealed the clinical determi-
nants associated with DT performance. Twenty-seven eligible participants were enrolled for clinical,
Keywords: cognitive-walking, and cognitive-cycling DT paradigm investigations. The mean age and age at onset of
Parkinson’s disease (PD)
the patients were 59.87 ± 6.3 and 53.11 ± 8.4 years, respectively. Both the off- and on-state akinesia sub-
Dual-task (DT)
Dual-task interference (DTI)
scores were worse on the more-affected side than on the less-affected side. However, the DT effects on
Akinesia the cycling and gait outcomes on both the more-affected and the less-affected side showed no significant
differences. The DT effect on the two motor tasks and cognitive performance during a concurrent walking
task declined. Nevertheless, the DT effect on cognition improved during cycling. The present study also
revealed that the levodopa equivalent daily dosage was highly associated with cognitive-cycling perfor-
mance and that the akinesia subscore was the most relevant factor that contributed to cognitive-walking
performance. In conclusion, DT facilitation or interference might be mediated by the type of motor task
applied. The cognitive-cycling DT paradigm had a facilitative effect on cognition. Cycling exercise may
diminish motor dysfunction has been investigated. We suggest that cognitive-cycling DT training is a
potential adjuvant therapeutic strategy for patients with PD to promote motor and cognitive functions.
Ó 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction dual-task (DT) paradigms was largest for walking, intermediate


for cycling, and smallest for standing tasks [2]. A more recent
Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disorder pri- meta-analysis confirmed that dual tasking severely affects walking
marily caused by a loss of dopamine neurons, which in turn leads performance in PD patients [3]. However, one report showed that a
to a reduced number of dopamine neurotransmitters, and PD cycling DT paradigm had facilitative effects on the cognition of
patients present with impairment of motor function and auto- patients with PD [4]. Thus, the effects of DT on motor and cognition
maticity [1]. Patients with PD have difficulty performing motor might rely on the type of motor task being performed.
tasks, and their ability to carry out two tasks simultaneously is PD patients have difficulty in the ‘‘automatic execution of
impaired. Yogen-Seligmann et al. indicated that the effect of learned motor plans”, and this problem is likely due to impairment
of basal ganglia function [5]. In the early stage of PD, the ability to
perform automatic behaviors is already impaired. Symptoms, such
⇑ Corresponding author at: Professor Lu Neurological Clinic, 2F, No. 10, Aly. 41,
as akinesia, slowness of simple repetitive movements, reduced arm
Ln. 10, Wenhua 1st Rd., Guishan Dist., Taoyuan 333, Taiwan.
swing, decreased stride length, freezing of gait, may be associated
E-mail address: bob.cslu@gmail.com (L. Chin-Song).
1
Equally contributing authors. with impaired motor automaticity [6]. The severity of the clinical

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocn.2020.01.024
0967-5868/Ó 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article as: C. Hsiu-Chen, C. Chiung-Chu, L. Jiunn-Woei et al., The effects of dual-task in patients with Parkinson’s disease performing cog-
nitive-motor paradigms, Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocn.2020.01.024
2 C. Hsiu-Chen et al. / Journal of Clinical Neuroscience xxx (xxxx) xxx

symptoms of motor automaticity dysfunction has been shown to 2.2. Study procedures
correspond to the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale part
III (UPDRS III) motor scores. The akinesia subscores were signifi- Each eligible patient underwent clinical assessments and two
cantly different between the more-affected (MA) and less- DT paradigm tests (cognitive-cycling and cognitive-walking).
affected (LA) sides, particularly in PD patients with modified All clinical assessments were performed after 12 h of overnight
Hoehn and Yahr Stage (mHYS) of 1–1.5 who suffered from unilat- withdrawal from antiparkinsonian medications (24 h of with-
eral symptoms. Penko et al. observed asymmetric swing phase drawal was needed if the participants were taking prolonged
durations during gait in individuals with mild to moderate PD release dopaminergic agonists). The next morning, the UPDRS III
(Hoehn &Yahr 2–4) under DT conditions [7]. Previous studies and mHYS were administered in the off-state. Then, the medica-
reported conflicting results of asymmetric gait under DT conditions tions were self-administered by each patient. The mHYS, UPDRS
[8–11]. In addition, there are few studies that have investigated the III, Timed Up and Go (TUG) test, and cognitive-walking DT tests
difference between the MA and LA sides during a cycling DT para- were performed 40–60 min later in the patients’ best on-state.
digm. Therefore, we aimed to investigate the difference between After 2–3 days, the patients completed the cognitive-cycling DT
the LA and MA sides while PD individuals perform walking and test in their best on-state.
cycling DT paradigms.
The effect of dual tasking is often quantified for a motor task as 2.3. Dual-task paradigm
an index of the automaticity of motor control. Previous studies
demonstrated that impairment of the sensorimotor striatum in We adopted two types of DT paradigms, cognitive-cycling and
PD patients hinders automatic control, and the PD patients require cognitive-walking. Each DT paradigm contained three sections: 1)
more attentional control to perform daily motor behaviors [6,12]. motor single-task (ST), 2) cognitive ST, and 3) cognitive-motor
The relative change in performance associated with dual tasking DT without task prioritization. Under the cognitive ST and DT sec-
is referred to as the dual-task effect (DTE) or dual-task interference tions, the three cognitive tasks (i.e., calculation, spatial memory,
(DTI). DTI occurs when the simultaneous execution of a cognitive and Stroop color-word tasks) were randomly assigned.
and a motor task leads to a change in performance in one or both
tasks. DTI reflects the competing demands of the two tasks for 2.4. Cognitive task
access to limited processing resources within the brain [13]. The
effects of dual tasking depend on several factors, including motor In the calculation task, the participants had to subtract 3 from a
and cognitive function, cognitive reserve, compensatory abilities, number five times after they saw the instructions (e.g., ‘‘95 – 3 =
personality, expertise, task type, task difficulty, and task prioritiza- ?”). In the spatial memory task, a yellow block appeared every sec-
tion [14]. Several reports have noted that gait quality, falling, freez- ond. When a 3  3 grid with numbers appeared on the screen, the
ing, disability, and disease severity were strongly associated with participants were instructed to read the numbers in each yellow
DT performance in patients with PD [15–18]. Strouwen et al. con- block. In the Stroop color-word task, the participants answered
ducted a comprehensive study to investigate the determinants of with the color of the word (Supplementary 1). The cognitive per-
DT performance in PD patients. The authors illustrated that poorer formance outcomes were reaction time (milliseconds), accuracy
single-task (ST) performance was the most important determinant (%), the composite score, and DTI on the composite score (%).
of lower DT performance in motor and cognitive tasks [19]. To date,
few studies have explored the relationship between cycling and 2.5. Cognitive-cycling DT paradigm
gait performance and the clinical characteristics of PD patients.
We hypothesized that the effects of dual tasking would depend The patients completed the cognitive ST test on a stationary
on the complexity of the motor task being performed. There may bicycle located approximately 1.5 m from a 50-in LCD monitor
be differences in cycling and gait performance between the LA but did not pedal the bicycle. The patients were then required to
and MA sides. In addition, we anticipated that clinical features complete the ST cycling and DT cycling tests in a specific order.
would influence motor performance under DT conditions. Thus, The order of the ST cycling and DT cognitive-cycling tests was
the present study sought to investigate the effects of DT perfor- assigned randomly. The patients rested for 30 min after the ST test
mance by using walking and cycling as motor tasks and reveal and after the DT cycling test. Before cycling, the patients were
the clinical determinants associated with DT performance. instructed to pedal the bike at a self-selected, comfortable cadence
(Supplementary 2). Cycling cadence (measured in revolutions per
minute, rpm) was captured using Arduino I/O board hardware
2. Experimental procedures and Arduino IDE software (Arduino CC., Italy). The cycling perfor-
mance outcomes were cycling cadence (rpm) and DTI on cycling
The data in this study were collected as part of exercise and cadence (%).
cognitive-cycling DT training programs for patients with early
stage PD, which were approved by the Institutional Review Board 2.6. Cognitive-walking DT paradigm
of the Chang Gung Medical Foundation (IRB No. 104-8171A3 and
201600213A3) and were conducted in accordance with the decla- As in the ST walking section, the patients were instructed to
ration of Helsinki. Participants signed an informed consent form walk at ‘‘preferred speed” and ‘‘fast speed” twice. Afterwards, the
prior to participation. patients sat on a chair located approximately 6 m from a projection
screen to perform the ST cognitive section followed by the DT
walking section twice. After the ST cognitive and DT sections, the
2.1. Participants ST walking section was performed once. In addition, the patients
rested for 2 min between each cognitive task (Supplementary 3).
Individuals with PD were recruited from the Division of Move- Gait performance was analyzed using GAITRite (CIR Systems, Inc.,
ment Disorders in the Department of Neurology at the Chang Gung Franklin NJ, USA) on a 3.66-m-long and 0.9-m-wide instrumented
Memorial Hospital in Linkou, a referring medical center in Taiwan. walkway. The walking outcomes were the spatiotemporal gait
Individuals were eligible for the study if they fulfilled the inclusion parameters, including gait speed (cm/s), step length (cm), step
criteria described in our previous work [20]. width (cm), step time (s), double limb support time (DLST, s), DTI

Please cite this article as: C. Hsiu-Chen, C. Chiung-Chu, L. Jiunn-Woei et al., The effects of dual-task in patients with Parkinson’s disease performing cog-
nitive-motor paradigms, Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocn.2020.01.024
C. Hsiu-Chen et al. / Journal of Clinical Neuroscience xxx (xxxx) xxx 3

on gait speed (%), step length (%), step width (%), step time (%), and Table 1
DLST (%). Clinical characteristics and assessments.

Outcomes Mean ± SD Range

2.7. Data analysis Clinical characteristics


AAE, years 59.87 ± 6.3 47–68
Gender Female/Male = 9/18 
In this study, the effect of dual tasking was defined as the DTI
Education, years 15.07 ± 2.25 12–19
and calculated as the difference between ST and DT performance, AAO, years 53.11 ± 8.40 35–65
expressed as a percentage of ST performance, as follows: DD, years 6.75 ± 3.53 2.21–14.25
LEDD, mg/daily 628.74 ± 270.38 200–1242
DTperformance  STperformance MoCA 28.19 ± 1.44 26–30
DTIð%Þ ¼  100 Clinical subtype AR/Mixed = 11/16 –
STperformance
Clinical assessments
We measured the participants’ accuracies and reaction time and Off-state
computed a composite score that accounted for speed–accuracy mHYS 1.96 ± 0.41 1–2.5
UPDRS III 22.22 ± 7.05 6–36
trade-offs [21]. Thus, cognitive performance was described as a
Tremor subscores 0.67 ± 1.00 0–3
composite score: Rigidity subscores 4.33 ± 2.91 0–9
Akinesia subscores 9.78 ± 3.42 4–17
Accuracyð%Þ LA 3.78 ± 2.03 0–7
Compositescoreð%Þ ¼  100
ReactiontimeðmsÞ MA 6 ± 1.94* 3–10
PIGD subscores 1.63 ± 0.84 0–3
The data on cycling cadence and reaction time were analyzed by On-state
using custom LabVIEW scripts. MoCA 28.19 ± 1.44 26–30
mHYS 1.85 ± 0.43 1–2.5
UPDRS III 15.00 ± 5.96 3–28
3. Statistics Tremor subscores 0.37 ± 0.79 0–3
Rigidity subscores 3.19 ± 2.7 0–9
Akinesia subscores 6.30. ± 2.66 1–12
All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS (version 22.0, LA 2.11 ± 1.42 0–4
IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA) software. Descriptive statistics are MA 4.19 ± 1.96* 0–8
reported as the mean ± standard deviation. The Wilcoxon signed- PIGD subscores 1.22 ± 0.97 0–3
rank test was used to test paired samples. To identify the indepen- TUG, sec 7.42 ± 1.29 5.77–11.15
dent variables most relevant to the outcomes, a stepwise forward AR: Akinetic-rigid; AAE: Age at examination; AAO: Age at onset; DD: Duration of
procedure (often called ‘‘forward selection”) was applied. The pre- disease; LEDD: Levodopa equivalent daily dosage; MoCA: Montreal cognitive
dictors included in this study were age at onset (AAO, years), dura- assessment; mHYS: Modified Hoehn and Yahr; UPDRS III: Unified Parkinson’s Dis-
ease Rating Scale part III; Tremor subscores: item 20 (tremor at rest); Rigidity
tion of disease (DD, years), levodopa equivalent daily dosage
subscores: item 22 (rigidity); Akinesia subscores; items 23–26 (finger taps, hand
(LEDD, mg/day), clinical subtype (tremor dominant, akinetic- movements, rapid alternating movements of the hands, and leg agility); PIGD
rigid, and mixed) [22,23], MoCA score, TUG (sec); off- and on- subscores; items 29, 30 (gait, postural stability); TUG: Time Up and Go; MA: more-
state mHYS, UPDRS III score, and the tremor, rigidity, akinesia, affected side; LA: less-affected side; *: Wilcoxon signed-rank test (LA vs. MA),
and PIGD subscores. The TUG was excluded from the independent p < 0.001.

variables of the gait outcomes. The correlation coefficients


between the relevant factors and outcomes were assessed using
respectively. The cycling outcomes on both the MA and the LA side
simple linear regression (Pearson correlation for continuous vari-
exhibited no significant difference.
ables, Spearman’s correlation for ordinal variables, eta for categor-
The gait speed under the DT walking condition (85.93 ± 21.3,
ical variables). Statistical significance was defined as p < 0.05.
89.37 ± 22.95, and 92.63 ± 20.26 cm/s for calculation, spatial
memory, and Stroop color-word, respectively) was lower
4. Results than that under the ST walking condition at the preferred speed
(106.48 ± 16.9 cm/s). Under the ST walking condition at preferred
4.1. Clinical characteristics speed, the step time was longer on the MA side than on the LA side
(p = 0.017), but the DLST was longer on the LA side than on the MA
Table 1 presents the detailed clinical data of the participants. side (p = 0.03). In addition, the DLST was longer on the LA side than
Twenty-seven eligible participants (33.33% of whom were female) on the MA side (p = 0.013) during the Stroop color-word DT walk-
were enrolled in this study. The participants had a mean age of 59. ing condition. The DTI on the gait outcomes was similar between
87 ± 6.3 years and a mean AAO of 53.11 ± 8.40 years. The mean DD the MA and LA sides.
was 6.75 ± 3.53 years, the mean LEDD was 628.74 ± 270.38 mg/day, Fig. 1A illustrates the DTI on the processing speed of motor
and the mean MoCA score was 28.19 ± 1.44. The off- and on-state tasks. During the DT section, the DTI on cycling cadence was
UPDRS III scores were 22.22 ± 7.05 and 15.00 ± 5.96, respectively. 4.68% ± 11.73%, 0.62% ± 10.28%, and 1.74% ± 10.82%, and the
Both the off- and the on-state akinesia subscores were consider- DTI on gait speed was 19.84% ± 12.97%, 18.46% ± 16.27%, and
ably worse on the MA side than on the LA side. 13.67% ± 9.72% for the calculation, spatial memory, and Stroop
color-word tasks, respectively. Compared with the DTI on cycling
4.2. Motor task performance cadence, the DTI on gait speed was significantly affected.

Table 2 presents both cycling and gait performance. 4.3. Cognitive performance
The mean ST cycling cadence was 43.31 ± 11.15 rpm, and the DT
cycling cadences during the calculation, spatial memory, and The cognitive DT performance improved during the cycling DT
Stroop color-word tasks were 40.63 ± 9.71, 42.47 ± 10.04, and paradigm, and the composite score of the DT calculation task sig-
41.98 ± 10.15 rpm, respectively. The DTI on cycling cadence during nificantly increased (7.07 ± 2.42 vs. 7.89 ± 2.57, p = 0.013) com-
the calculation, spatial memory, and Stroop color-word tasks pared with that of the ST cognitive section. The DTI on the
were  4.68% ± 11.73%, 0.62% ± 10.28%, and 1.74% ± 10.83%, calculation, spatial memory, and Stroop color-word cognitive tasks

Please cite this article as: C. Hsiu-Chen, C. Chiung-Chu, L. Jiunn-Woei et al., The effects of dual-task in patients with Parkinson’s disease performing cog-
nitive-motor paradigms, Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocn.2020.01.024
4 C. Hsiu-Chen et al. / Journal of Clinical Neuroscience xxx (xxxx) xxx

Table 2
Motor task performance under DT paradigm.

Outcomes ST DT DTI, %
Preferred Fast speed Calculation Spatial Stroop color- Calculation Spatial Stroop color-
speed memory word memory word
Cycling cadence,
rpm
Overall 43.31 ± 11.15 ND 40.63 ± 9.71 42.47 ± 10.04 41.98 ± 10.15 4.68 ± 11.73 0.62 ± 10.28 1.74 ± 10.83
LA ND 40.26 ± 10.04 41.94 ± 10.26 41.5 ± 10.40 5.18 ± 14.33 1.33 ± 13.89 3.05 ± 12.64
MA ND 40.25 ± 10.06 41.97 ± 10.25 41.49 ± 10.45 5.83 ± 12.7 1.89 ± 12.29 2.32 ± 14.58
Gait parameters
Gait speed, cm/sec 106.48 ± 16.9 148.24 ± 23.5 85.93 ± 21.3 89.37 ± 22.95 92.63 ± 20.26 19.84 ± 12.97 18.46 ± 16.27 13.67 ± 9.72
Step length, cm
Overall 56.17 ± 6.41 67.99 ± 8.31 49.91 ± 8.15 49.41 ± 8.32 51.24 ± 6.96 10.96 ± 10.35 13.15 ± 11.61 8.84 ± 6.52
LA 56.3 ± 6.81 68.09 ± 8.75 49.97 ± 7.92 49.67 ± 8.4 51.81 ± 7 11.02 ± 9.59 12.73 ± 11.16 8.11 ± 6.64
MA 56.03 ± 6.53 67.89 ± 8.18 49.84 ± 9.2 49.15 ± 8.76 50.67 ± 7.88 10.97 ± 12.13 13.57 ± 12.73 9.64 ± 7.57
Step width, cm
Overall 10.23 ± 2.5 10.04 ± 2.34 11.96 ± 3.47 11.53 ± 3.3 11.19 ± 3.06 16.66 ± 21.27 12.37 ± 14.21 9.11 ± 15.83
LA 10.19 ± 2.53 10.03 ± 2.46 11.96 ± 3.49 11.52 ± 3.32 11.24 ± 3.04 17.33 ± 21.27 12.58 ± 14.49 10.69 ± 17.39
MA 10.28 ± 2.49 10.05 ± 2.26 11.96 ± 3.47 11.54 ± 3.29 11.15 ± 3.08 16.16 ± 21.94 12.41 ± 14.97 7.72 ± 15.37
Step time, sec
Overall 0.53 ± 0.05 0.46 ± 0.05 0.6 ± 0.1 0.57 ± 0.1 0.57 ± 0.07 12.4 ± 11.48 8.48 ± 12.78 6.32 ± 8.01
LA 0.53 ± 0.05 0.46 ± 0.05 0.6 ± 0.11 0.57 ± 0.1 0.56 ± 0.08 13.25 ± 11.84 8.66 ± 12.70 6.33 ± 7.89
MA 0.54 ± 0.04* 0.46 ± 0.05 0.6 ± 0.1 0.58 ± 0.1 0.57 ± 0.07 11.53 ± 12.03 8.26 ± 13.34 6.30 ± 8.73
DLST, sec
Overall 0.25 ± 0.05 0.17 ± 0.04 0.33 ± 0.11 0.31 ± 0.12 0.30 ± 0.08 28.85 ± 27.99 24.99 ± 32.98 15.38 ± 15.89
LA 0.26 ± 0.05* 0.17 ± 0.04 0.33 ± 0.12 0.31 ± 0.12 0.30 ± 0.08* 28.48 ± 27.81 25.03 ± 33.07 15.73 ± 15.71
MA 0.25 ± 0.05 0.17 ± 0.04 0.33 ± 0.11 0.31 ± 0.12 0.30 ± 0.08 29.28 ± 28.37 24.99 ± 33.13 15.06 ± 16.25

Mean ± SD, range; *: Wilcoxon-signed rank test (LA vs. MA), p < 0.05; ST: Single-task; DT: Dual-task; DTI: Dual-task interference; DLST: Double limb support time; MA: more-
affected side; LA: less-affected side; ND: Not done

Fig. 1. Dual-task interference (DTI) comparison between the two cognitive-motor paradigms. A. Both cognitive-motor paradigms yielded DTI on the motor task. The DT effect
on the processing speed of the motor task showed that gait speed was more affected than was cycling cadence. Values are Mean ± SD B. Dual-task facilitative effect on the
three cognitive tasks during cycling. The dual-task interference on the calculation and spatial memory cognitive tasks while walking. The DT interference on cognition was
clearly affected during the walking DT paradigm.

were 12.77% ± 25.42%, 9.19% ± 61.85%, and 14.55% ± 65.38%, 4.4. Relationship between motor performance and clinical
respectively. These findings revealed that cycling had a facilitative characteristics
effect on cognition. With the walking DT paradigm, the DT cogni-
tive performance declined, and the composite score of the DT cal- Table 3 shows that the AAO, LEDD, MoCA score, and akinesia
culation task significantly decreased (p = 0.044). However, the DT subscores contributed to motor performance. Under the cycling
Stroop color-word task performance significantly increased DT paradigm, a higher AAO contributed significantly to a lower
(p = 0.041) compared with that of the cognitive ST section. The ST cycling cadence (r = 0.598, p = 0.001), and LEDD was a key
DTI values on the calculation, spatial memory, and Stroop color- determinant of cycling DT performance. A higher LEDD corre-
word cognitive tasks were  6.48% ± 15.77%, 3.24% ± 21.11%, sponded to higher cycling cadence (calculation, r = 0.639,
and 5.23% ± 14.43%, respectively. These findings revealed that cal- p < 0.0001; spatial memory, r = 0.64, p < 0.0001; Stroop color-
culation and spatial memory cognitive performance deteriorated word, r = 0.615, p < 0.0001). Under the ST walking condition at
while walking (Supplementary 4). In contrast to the DTI on cogni- preferred and fast speeds, a higher MoCA score contributed consid-
tion during the cycling DT paradigm, the DTI on cognition was erably to a narrower step width; under the DT walking condition
affected considerably during the walking DT paradigm (Fig. 1B). (spatial memory and Stroop color-word tasks), a higher akinesia

Please cite this article as: C. Hsiu-Chen, C. Chiung-Chu, L. Jiunn-Woei et al., The effects of dual-task in patients with Parkinson’s disease performing cog-
nitive-motor paradigms, Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocn.2020.01.024
C. Hsiu-Chen et al. / Journal of Clinical Neuroscience xxx (xxxx) xxx 5

Table 3
Correlations between most relevant clinical characteristics and motor performance.

Outcomes ST DT
Preferred speed Fast speed Calculation Spatial memory Stroop color-word
Cycling DT paradigm
Cycling cadence AAO NA LEDD LEDD LEDD
r 0.598 NA 0.639 0.640 0.615
p 0.001 NA < 0.0001 < 0.0001 < 0.0001
Walking DT paradigm
Gait speed None None None None None
r – – – – –
p – – – – –
Step length None None None None None
r – – – – –
p – – – – –
Step width MoCA MoCA None Akinesia (on) Akinesia (on)
r 0.412 0.39 – 0.464 s 0.404 s
p 0.033 0.044 – 0.015 0.037
Step time None mHYS (off) None None None
r 0.377 s – – –
p 0.053 – – –
DLST None None None None
r – – – – –
p – – – – –

s: Spearmen correlation coefficient r; e: Eta correlation coefficient r; ST: Single-task; DT: Dual- task; NA: Not available; DLST: Double limb support time; AAO: Age at onset;
LEDD: Levodopa equivalent daily dosage; MoCA: Montreal cognitive assessment; mHYS: Modified Hoehn and Yahr; Akinesia subscores; items 23–26 (finger taps, hand
movements, rapid alternating movements of the hands, and leg agility).

subscore in the on-state determined a wider step width (r = 0.464, hypothesis that the effect of a DT paradigm depends on the com-
p = 0.015; r = 0.404, p = 0.037). plexity of the motor task being performed.
We observed a trend of cognition facilitative effects when the
patients with PD performed the cognitive-cycling DT section. Even
5. Discussion if cognitive performance improved under DT conditions, the possi-
bility of ‘‘posture second” strategy could not be obviated [31]. The
This study had several main findings. First, in agreement with individuals with PD might prioritize the cognitive task over main-
previous studies, we observed that the DTI was higher for walking tenance of motor task. During the cycling DT section, the average
than for cycling. In addition, the cognitive-cycling DT condition DTI on cycling cadence exhibited a minimal decline, but a facilita-
revealed facilitative effects on cognition in PD patients. As we tive effect was observed on cognition. A previous study suggested
hypothesized, the effect of a DT paradigm depended on the type that the arousal and attentional demands (AAD) model [32] can
of motor task employed in the DT paradigm. Second, the severity explain this facilitative effect on cognition while cycling. Addition-
of the clinical symptoms of motor automaticity dysfunction corre- ally, exercise-related arousal has been associated with increased
sponded to the UPDRS motor scores. The akinesia subscores were cognitive resources, improvement in processing speed [33–35],
significantly different between the MA and LA sides. However, and increased release of dopamine [36]. According to the AAD
the measured cycling cadence, most of the gait parameters, and model, DTI appears if the additional level of arousal cannot provide
DTI on motor performance exhibited no difference between the adequate cognitive resources to maintain DT performance. Con-
MA and LA sides. Third, a lower AAO and a higher LEDD con- trary to an increase in DTI, an increase in arousal when the
tributed significantly to cycling performance. Finally, the MoCA demands of a DT are less than anticipated might result in DT
score and akinesia subscores were closely correlated with gait benefits.
performance. The present results showed that the degree of automaticity dys-
During the DT section, the reduction in gait speed was exacer- function in the extremities (as shown by the akinesia subscores)
bated compared with that of cycling cadence. Locomotor rhythmic was markedly asymmetric; however, cycling performance in the
activity relies on the auto-activity of localized networks of neurons bilateral lower extremities showed similar results. The same
or central pattern generators within the nervous system [24]. How- results were observed for gait performance as well, except for
ever, walking is no longer considered merely an automated motor the step time and DLST gait parameters. Previous reports have
activity. A neuroimaging study demonstrated that patients with PD noted gait asymmetry (GA) in the early stages of PD [11], but the
required greater activation during imagined usual walking than degree of asymmetry was not associated with the level of asymme-
did healthy older adults [25]. Additionally, patients with PD exhib- try in motor symptoms [10,37]. Increased GA implies less bilateral
ited more brain activity during complex imagined walking condi- coordination. During usual walking, patients with advanced PD
tions (e.g., obstacle negotiation and turning) [25,26]. A number of with motor fluctuations exhibited increased GA while they were
investigations have demonstrated that walking relies on the use in the off-state, with higher GA being observed in patients with
of cognition. For example, poor executive function has been shown PD who suffered from freezing of gait [10]. These previous observa-
to be associated with gait dysfunction in patients with PD [27–30]. tions suggest that bilateral coordination becomes worse during the
These findings support the notion that walking relies on the use of course of PD progression. Poor bilateral coordination seems to
several cognitive domains. In contrast to lower limb cycling, walk- reflect the pathological processes of PD. In contrast, the current
ing represents a complex motor task requiring both bilateral coor- study recruited patients with early PD with a mHYS score of
dination and dynamic postural control, which are continuously 1–2.5 in the off-state. Compared with usual walking, patients with
monitored by cognitive resources. This result confirmed our PD exhibited significantly increased GA during the walking DT

Please cite this article as: C. Hsiu-Chen, C. Chiung-Chu, L. Jiunn-Woei et al., The effects of dual-task in patients with Parkinson’s disease performing cog-
nitive-motor paradigms, Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocn.2020.01.024
6 C. Hsiu-Chen et al. / Journal of Clinical Neuroscience xxx (xxxx) xxx

condition [37]. Penko et al. reported that PD patients presented Funding


lower extremity asymmetries during pedaling while cycling. The
authors also found that the asymmetries in pedaling were not cor- This work was supported by the Chang Gung Medical
related with the UPDRS subscores of postural instability and gait Foundation and Ministry of Science and Technology, Taiwan
disturbance [38]. Based on the previous observations, we con- (grant number CMRPG3F1341, CMRPG3F1342 to CS Lu, MOST
cluded that bilateral coordination worsens during the course of 108-2218-E-182-010 to YJ Chang, and CMRPD1G0041, MOST
PD; thus, asymmetric motor symptoms affect bilateral motor coor- 107-2218-E-182-003 to JW Liaw).
dination less in patients with early stage PD. The authors declare there are no financial or other conflicts of
Traditionally, levodopa has been the most efficient therapy to interest.
improve PD motor dysfunction. The improvement in motor func-
tion or physical activities was associated with the LEDD. A higher
LEDD contributed considerably to cycling DT performance. In con- Appendix A. Supplementary data
trast to the cycling performance in the cycling DT paradigm, motor
performance in the walking DT paradigm did not correspond to the Supplementary data to this article can be found online at
LEDD. Compared with walking, cycling is a simple motor task and https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocn.2020.01.024.
is considered a type of physical activity that may require a much
smaller amount of attentional resources. These findings imply that References
automatic walking might be a less efficient medical treatment for
patients with PD. Workman et al. reported that dopaminergic med- [1] Wu T, Hallett M. A functional MRI study of automatic movements in patients
with Parkinson’s disease. Brain 2005;128(Pt 10):2250–9.
ication did not improve gait automaticity of PD patients under DT [2] Yogev-Seligmann G, Giladi N, Gruendlinger L, Hausdorff JM. The contribution
conditions [39]. Thus, we were not surprised to observe that a of postural control and bilateral coordination to the impact of dual tasking on
higher LEDD contributed considerably to DT cycling performance. gait. Exp Brain Res 2013;226(1):81–93.
[3] Raffegeau TE, Krehbiel LM, Kang N, Thijs FJ, Altmann LJP, Cauraugh JH, et al. A
The present study showed that even during the ST walking section, meta-analysis: Parkinson’s disease and dual-task walking. Parkinsonism
lower MoCA scores were associated with a worse step width. This Related Disord 2019;62:28–35.
evidence indicates there is a relationship between cognitive func- [4] Hazamy AA, Altmann LJ, Stegemoller E, Bowers D, Lee HK, Wilson J, et al.
Improved cognition while cycling in Parkinson’s disease patients and healthy
tion and gait abnormalities. Walking relies on the use of several adults. Brain Cogn 2017;113:23–31.
cognitive domains, including executive-attentional function, visu- [5] Marsden CD. The mysterious motor function of the basal ganglia: the Robert
ospatial abilities, and even memory resources. Many studies have Wartenberg Lecture. Neurology 1982;32(5):514–39.
[6] Wu T, Hallett M, Chan P. Motor automaticity in Parkinson’s disease. Neurobiol
demonstrated an association between gait abnormalities and poor
Dis 2015;82:226–34.
executive/attention ability in older adults [5,6] and in patients [7] Penko AL, Streicher MC, Koop MM, Dey T, Rosenfeldt AB, Bazyk AS, et al. Dual-
with PD [27–29]. task interference disrupts Parkinson’s gait across multiple cognitive domains.
Our results indicated that disease progression–related motor Neuroscience 2018;379:375–82.
[8] Galna B, Lord S, Rochester L. Is gait variability reliable in older adults and
dysfunction impacts gait automaticity in patients with PD. During Parkinson’s disease? Towards an optimal testing protocol. Gait & Posture
the walking DT section, the akinesia subscore in the on-state was 2013;37(4):580–5.
the most relevant factor that contributed to gait performance. Aki- [9] Rochester L, Galna B, Lord S, Burn D. The nature of dual-task interference
during gait in incident Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience 2014;265:83–94.
nesia (or bradykinesia) is the primary feature contributing to [10] Plotnik M, Giladi N, Balash Y, Peretz C, Hausdorff JM. Is freezing of gait in
motor difficulties in PD patients, and it can affect almost all activ- Parkinson’s disease related to asymmetric motor function?. Ann Neurol
ities of daily living [40]. The severity of akinesia is correlated with 2005;57(5):656–63.
[11] Baltadjieva R, Giladi N, Gruendlinger L, Peretz C, Hausdorff JM. Marked
the striatal dopamine terminal integrity in vivo, with maximal alterations in the gait timing and rhythmicity of patients with de novo
involvement appearing in the posterior putamen [41]. Therefore, Parkinson’s disease. Eur J Neurosci 2006;24(6):1815–20.
the alleviation of akinesia symptoms has great clinical significance [12] Wu T, Liu J, Zhang H, Hallett M, Zheng Z, Chan P. Attention to automatic
movements in Parkinson’s disease: modified automatic mode in the striatum.
and should be highlighted in the treatment of patients with early Cereb Cortex 2015;25(10):3330–42.
stage PD. Our previous research demonstrated that cycling exercise [13] Al-Yahya E, Dawes H, Smith L, Dennis A, Howells K, Cockburn J. Cognitive
improved akinesia subscores by approximately 54% and 36% in the motor interference while walking: a systematic review and meta-analysis.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011;35(3):715–28.
off- and on-states, respectively [20].
[14] Yogev-Seligmann G, Hausdorff JM, Giladi N. Do we always prioritize balance
There are still several limitations that should be considered. when walking? Towards an integrated model of task prioritization. Movement
First, the cognitive-cycling paradigm was applied after 2–3 days Disord 2012;27(6):765–70.
of a wash-out period after the cognitive-walking paradigm, and [15] Kelly VE, Eusterbrock AJ, Shumway-Cook A. A review of dual-task walking
deficits in people with Parkinson’s disease: motor and cognitive contributions,
there may be learning effects on the cognitive task performance. mechanisms, and clinical implications. Parkinsons Dis 2012;2012:918719.
Second, cognitive-walking was performed using a 3.66 m (12 foot) [16] Heinzel S, Maechtel M, Hasmann SE, Hobert MA, Heger T, Berg D, et al. Motor
GAITRite walkway, which only allowed 1–3 trials for each cogni- dual-tasking deficits predict falls in Parkinson’s disease: A prospective study.
Parkinsonism Related Disord 2016;26:73–7.
tive task. Third, task prioritization influences DT performance. No [17] Spildooren J, Vercruysse S, Desloovere K, Vandenberghe W, Kerckhofs E,
instructions were provided regarding the prioritization of a task; Nieuwboer A. Freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: the impact of dual-
therefore, the strategy of specific-task priority might differ consid- tasking and turning. Movement Disord 2010;25(15):2563–70.
[18] Fuller RL, Van Winkle EP, Anderson KE, Gruber-Baldini AL, Hill T, Zampieri C,
erably between each PD individual. Fourth, we did not estimate the et al. Dual task performance in Parkinson’s disease: a sensitive predictor of
study sample size in advance, and the conclusions presented might impairment and disability. Parkinsonism Related Disord 2013;19(3):325–8.
be conservative. [19] Strouwen C, Molenaar EA, Keus SH, Munks L, Heremans E, Vandenberghe W,
et al. Are factors related to dual-task performance in people with Parkinson’s
In conclusion, DT facilitation or interference on both motor and disease dependent on the type of dual task?. Parkinsonism Related Disord
cognitive performance might be mediated by the type of motor 2016;23:23–30.
task applied. The cognitive-cycling paradigm had a facilitative [20] Chang HC, Lu CS, Chiou WD, Chen CC, Weng YH, Chang YJ. An 8-week low-
intensity progressive cycling training improves motor functions in patients
effect on cognition. In addition, the akinesia subscore was the most
with early-stage Parkinson’s disease. J Clin Neurol 2018;14(2):225–33.
relevant factor contributing to DT walking. Cycling exercise may [21] Dwolatzky T, Whitehead V, Doniger GM, Simon ES, Schweiger A, Jaffe D, et al.
diminish motor dysfunction has been investigated. We suggest Validity of a novel computerized cognitive battery for mild cognitive
that cognitive-cycling DT training is a potential adjuvant therapeu- impairment. BMC Geriatr 2003;3:4.
[22] Korchounov A, Schipper HI, Preobrazhenskaya IS, Kessler KR, Yakhno NN.
tic strategy for patients with PD to promote motor and cognitive Differences in age at onset and familial aggregation between clinical types of
functions. idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disord 2004;19(9):1059–64.

Please cite this article as: C. Hsiu-Chen, C. Chiung-Chu, L. Jiunn-Woei et al., The effects of dual-task in patients with Parkinson’s disease performing cog-
nitive-motor paradigms, Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocn.2020.01.024
C. Hsiu-Chen et al. / Journal of Clinical Neuroscience xxx (xxxx) xxx 7

[23] Kang GA, Bronstein JM, Masterman DL, Redelings M, Crum JA, Ritz B. Clinical disease and healthy adults: a neuro-behavioral model. PloS One 2015;10(5):
characteristics in early Parkinson’s disease in a central California population- e0125470.
based study. Movement Disord 2005;20(9):1133–42. [33] Lambourne K, Tomporowski P. The effect of exercise-induced arousal on
[24] Marder E, Calabrese RL. Principles of rhythmic motor pattern generation. cognitive task performance: a meta-regression analysis. Brain Res
Physiol Rev 1996;76(3):687–717. 2010;1341:12–24.
[25] Maidan I, Rosenberg-Katz K, Jacob Y, Giladi N, Deutsch JE, Hausdorff JM, et al. [34] Cooper CJ. Anatomical and physiological mechanisms of arousal, with special
Altered brain activation in complex walking conditions in patients with reference to the effects of exercise. Ergonomics 1973;16(5):601–9.
Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Related Disord 2016;25:91–6. [35] Wang CC, Huang CC, Shih CH, Chu CH, Chang YK. Effect of acute exercise on
[26] Peterson DS, Pickett KA, Duncan RP, Perlmutter JS, Earhart GM. Brain activity executive function: An event-related potential and brain-derived neurotropic
during complex imagined gait tasks in Parkinson disease. Clin Neurophysiol factor study. Phys Educ J 2016;49(S):47–60.
2014;125(5):995–1005. [36] Horvitz JC. Mesolimbocortical and nigrostriatal dopamine responses to salient
[27] Rochester L, Nieuwboer A, Baker K, Hetherington V, Willems AM, Kwakkel G, non-reward events. Neuroscience 2000;96(4):651–6.
et al. Walking speed during single and dual tasks in Parkinson’s disease: which [37] Yogev G, Plotnik M, Peretz C, Giladi N, Hausdorff JM. Gait asymmetry in
characteristics are important?. Movement Disord 2008;23(16):2312–8. patients with Parkinson’s disease and elderly fallers: when does the bilateral
[28] Yogev G, Giladi N, Peretz C, Springer S, Simon ES, Hausdorff JM. Dual tasking, coordination of gait require attention?. Exp Brain Res 2007;177(3):336–46.
gait rhythmicity, and Parkinson’s disease: which aspects of gait are attention [38] Penko AL, Hirsch JR, Voelcker-Rehage C, Martin PE, Blackburn G, Alberts JL.
demanding?. Eur J Neurosci 2005;22(5):1248–56. Asymmetrical pedaling patterns in Parkinson’s disease patients. Clin Biomech
[29] Amboni M, Barone P, Picillo M, Cozzolino A, Longo K, Erro R, et al. A two-year (Bristol, Avon) 2014;29(10):1089–94.
follow-up study of executive dysfunctions in parkinsonian patients with [39] Workman CD, Thrasher TA. The influence of dopaminergic medication on gait
freezing of gait at on-state. Movement Disord 2010;25(6):800–2. automaticity in Parkinson’s disease. J Clin Neurosci 2019;65:71–6.
[30] Amboni M, Barone P, Iuppariello L, Lista I, Tranfaglia R, Fasano A, et al. Gait [40] Hallett M. Bradykinesia: why do Parkinson’s patients have it and what trouble
patterns in Parkinsonian patients with or without mild cognitive impairment. does it cause?. Movement Disord 2011;26(9):1579–81.
Movement Disord 2012;27(12):1536–43. [41] Benamer HT, Oertel WH, Patterson J, Hadley DM, Pogarell O, Hoffken H, et al.
[31] Bloem BR, Grimbergen YA, van Dijk JG, Munneke M. The ‘‘posture second” Prospective study of presynaptic dopaminergic imaging in patients with mild
strategy: a review of wrong priorities in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci parkinsonism and tremor disorders: part 1. Baseline and 3-month
2006;248(1–2):196–204. observations. Movement Disord 2003;18(9):977–84.
[32] Altmann LJ, Stegemoller E, Hazamy AA, Wilson JP, Okun MS, McFarland NR,
Wagle Shukla A, Hass CJ. Unexpected dual task benefits on cycling in Parkinson

Please cite this article as: C. Hsiu-Chen, C. Chiung-Chu, L. Jiunn-Woei et al., The effects of dual-task in patients with Parkinson’s disease performing cog-
nitive-motor paradigms, Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocn.2020.01.024

You might also like