You are on page 1of 6

Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 18 (2012) 1067e1072

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Parkinsonism and Related Disorders


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/parkreldis

Neuropsychological profile in patients with early stage of Parkinson’s disease


in Taiwan
Rwei-Ling Yu a, Ruey-Meei Wu a, b, **, Chun-Hwei Tai b, Chin-Hsien Lin b, Ting-Wen Cheng b,
Mau-Sun Hua a, b, *
a
Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, Taipei 10617, Taiwan
b
Department of Neurology, National Taiwan University Hospital, College of Medicine, National Taiwan University, Taipei 10002, Taiwan

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Given the importance of early detection and intervention for disease management, determining the
Received 22 January 2012 vulnerable neuropsychological function in patients with early-stage Parkinson’s disease (PD) is a priority.
Received in revised form Here, we describe the neuropsychological pattern in early-stage PD patients with mild cognitive
30 April 2012
impairment (PD-MCI) and dementia (PDD) in Taiwanese population. The neuropsychological perfor-
Accepted 7 June 2012
mance of 94 patients with PD was compared with that of 84 healthy controls (HCs) and available
normative data, using a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment including tests of executive,
Keywords:
memory, psychomotor speed, attention, visuospatial, and language functions. Our results showed that PD
Parkinson’s disease
Mild cognitive impairment
patients performed significantly worse on executive function (i.e., category of card sorting) and
Neuropsychological function psychomotor speed (i.e., processing speed index). Up to 46.8% were classified as PD-MCI and the majority
of those having single-domain impairment (68.2%); 9.6% met the consensus diagnostic criteria for PDD.
Accordingly, we suggest that early-stage PD patients have cognitive dysfunction predominately in the
anterior brain. Further follow-up study to determine how many percent of PD-MCI develop PDD is
important. The effect of neurocognitive rehabilitation on executive function is also valuable in the
subsequence study.
Ó 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Although few studies have focused on PD patients with mild


disease severity, some researchers have suggested that cognitive
Patients with PD, a neurodegenerative disease involving frontal deficits can be present at an early stage of the disease and are more
lobe system [1], are twice as likely as healthy aging adults to heterogeneous than previously described [3,6,8]. Several studies
develop cognitive impairment [2e4]. A recent study reported that have shown that cognitive dysfunction is most prominent in the
a mean of 26.7% (range, 18.9%e32.8%) of non-demented patients memory [2,3,6] and executive function domains [3,6], while others
with PD have mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [5], including drug- have shown that visuospatial deficits can sometimes be the most
náive patients [2] and those with newly diagnosed PD [3,6]. One severe cognitive impairment [9]. Previous findings have also sug-
multicenter study that pooled data from Western cohorts revealed gested that MCI in PD patients with predominant posterior cortical
that the common cognitive dysfunctions in PD are attention/exec- dysfunction might have a more rapid progression to dementia
utive, memory, and visuospatial function [7]. The increased risk of associated with PD (PDD) [10,11]. However, methodological prob-
a more rapid cognitive decline and dementia in PD-MCI cases lems like variability in the neuropsychological function domains
underlines the importance of careful monitoring of cognitive investigated, the tests used to measure each of the domains, and
dysfunctions in PD patients without dementia. the determination of cutoff points for function impairment may be
at least partly responsible for the observed inconsistencies.
Another problem in accurately diagnosing PD with dementia
* Corresponding author. Department of Psychology, National Taiwan University, may be a predominant reliance on Diagnostic and Statistical
Taipei 10617, Taiwan. Tel.: þ886 2 3366 3101; fax: þ886 2 2362 9909. Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV), one of the
** Corresponding author. Department of Neurology, National Taiwan University most widely used diagnosis criteria for dementia. The DSM-IV
Hospital, Taipei 10002, Taiwan. Tel.: þ886 2 2312 3456/5337; fax: þ886 2 2341
states that memory impairment is a necessary neurocognitive
8395.
E-mail addresses: robinwu@ntu.edu.tw (R.-M. Wu), huams@ntu.edu.tw criterion for dementia; however, non-memory cognitive impair-
(M.-S. Hua). ments, such as executive dysfunction, predominate in PD patients.

1353-8020/$ e see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.parkreldis.2012.06.002
1068 R.-L. Yu et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 18 (2012) 1067e1072

On the basis of this, the Movement Disorders Society (MDS) [12] Table 1
proposed replacing the mandatory role of memory impairment Cognitive domains and neuropsychological tests.

with alternative, such as deficits of executive function, attention, Domain Neuropsychological tests
language, and visuospatial function in patients with PD. That is, Executive function MCST (number of categories achieved,
each of these cognitive deficits plays an equally weighted role in the perseverative and non-perseverative
dementia diagnosis. These two viewpoints are markedly different errors) [17]
Category fluency (fruit, fish, and
in terms of which is the essential neurocognitive deficit in the
vegetable)
dementia diagnosis in PD and require further exploration. Working memory indexa
Though neuropsychological testing heterogeneity can be of Similaritiesa
enormous value, little research has been done using a comprehen- Matrix reasoninga
sive neuropsychological test battery covering most cognitive Color trails test B [18]
Memory function Verbal: logical memoryb
function domains [3,6]. This is particularly a problem in Asian Nonverbal: visual reproductionb
countries. To our knowledge, only one study has investigated the 3-object recallc
neuropsychological function in a Han Chinese population [13]. Psychomotor speed Digit symbol substitutiona
However, the lack of norms and domain-specific cognitive Symbol searchinga
Processing speed indexa
batteries, and the small number of cognitive functions covered
Color trails test A [18]
limited researches studying the manifestations of early PD in Visuospatial function Block designa
cognitive profiles. In fact, most studies pooled PD patients with Pentagon copyc
different disease severities together, but this approach overlooks Attention Digit spana
the importance of disease heterogeneity and early detection of the Color trails-interference [18]
Serial 7sc
vulnerable cognitive function prior to development of full-brown Language Namingc
dementia. To address these issues, we focused on PD patients in Repetitionc
the initial disease stage. We hypothesize that PD patients develop Verbal comprehensionc
cognitive dysfunction in early stage and executive function is the Abbreviations: MCST, Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; WAIS-III [19],
most vulnerable cognitive function, especially in concept formation Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition; WMS-III [20], Wechsler Memory
ability and cognitive flexibility. Besides, the dementia criteria Scale-Third Edition; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination.
a
proposed by the MDS is more applicable for the PD population than Subtest of WAIS-III.
b
Subtest of WMS-III.
DSM-IV. Here, we explore the pattern of cognitive dysfunction in c
Subtest of MMSE.
early-stage patients by using comprehensive neuropsychological
measures encompassing most cognitive domains. In addition, the 2.3. Statistical analysis
proportion of patients with PD-MCI subtypes and PDD, and the
issue of the clinical utility of the two major diagnosis criteria of PDD Demographic and disease-related characteristics were summarized with
descriptive statistics. Chi-square, t, or ManneWhitney U tests were used for
were investigated.
comparison between groups, when appropriate.
To assess the pattern of cognitive dysfunction in PD patients, between-group
2. Methods comparisons were made using a t test for parametric variables or a ManneWhitney
U test for nonparametric variables. Subsequently, a logistic regression analysis was
2.1. Participants conducted to determine which neuropsychological measures differentiated patients
from HCs most effectively. Due to the nature of PD, it is possible that the motor deficits
Participants included 94 individuals with PD and 84 healthy controls (HCs) (e.g., bradykinesia and rigidity) will negatively influence the patient’s performance
(consisting of spouses, relatives of the patients, and community participants), who in processing speed tests. To minimize the effect of motor symptoms, stage 1 patients,
were gender-, age- and education-matched. The patients were recruited from the who have motor symptoms on only one side of their bodies, were investigated.
neurology outpatient clinics at National Taiwan University Hospital, and the clinical Of all the stage 1 patients, only three patients were documented as having an impaired
diagnosis of PD was made according to the UK PD Society Brain Bank criteria [14]. processing speed, and these patients had motor symptoms on their non-dominant
The inclusion criteria for patients were idiopathic PD with a mild level of disease side, which would not limit their processing speed test performance.
severity (Hoehn and Yahr stages, H&Y stage 1e2). Exclusion criteria were age of Additionally, the patients’ cognitive performances were compared with the
onset before 45 years, and history of brain surgery, other neurologic disorders, and available normative data, and an individual was identified as abnormal if the neu-
impairments of visual acuity or hearing ability. Taking into account the impact of ropsychological test result was 1.5 SD below the normative mean (or below the fifth
psychiatric symptoms on cognitive function [15], we also excluded the patients with percentile of normative samples). Meanwhile, a cognitive domain was defined as
psychiatric disorders, such as major depression and psychosis. In addition, patients “impaired” if any one of the test scores in that domain was abnormal. Patients with
with visual hallucination or fluctuating attention that had been diagnosed with PD were classified as having PD-MCI if they had impairment in at least one cognitive
dementia with Lewy body disease [16] were also excluded. Each patient underwent domain with normal social and occupation function. Furthermore, the DSM-IV
a neurological and a neuropsychological examination, during which no remarkable criteria and consensus criteria proposed by the MDS were used to classify patients
neuropsychiatric symptoms were observed. None of the HCs had global cognitive with or without dementia, respectively. Statistical significance was determined
deterioration (Mini-Mental State Examination, MMSE < 24), impairments of hearing when the probability value was less than 0.05. Commercially available software
and visual acuity, or any history of other neurologic or psychiatric disorders. All the (SPSS version 17.0; SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) was used for the statistical analyses.
participants provided written informed consent prior to enrollment, in accordance
with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki. The study
was approved by the Ethical Research Committee of National Taiwan University 3. Results
Hospital.
The demographic and clinical data of the study groups were
2.2. Neurological and neuropsychological assessment summarized in Table 2. There were no significant differences
between HCs and PD groups with respect to gender distribution,
A neurologic examination was performed on all patients to confirm the diag- handedness, age, and years of formal education, as well as pre-
nosis of PD. Information regarding the onset of disease, medical history, and levo-
dopa usage was obtained from a semistructured interview. The duration of disease
morbid verbal intellectual quotient (VIQ).
was defined as the time between the appearance of the first motor symptom as
reported by the patient, and the disease severity was determined using the H&Y 3.1. Profile of cognitive dysfunction in early-stage PD
staging rating scale. Each participant was interviewed by a licensed neuropsychol-
ogist and neuropsychological tests were administered to evaluate five cognitive
domains: executive function, memory, psychomotor speed, attention, and visuo- Patients with early-stage PD performed significantly worse
spatial and language abilities (Table 1). than controls on 19 of 30 neuropsychological measures (Table 3).
R.-L. Yu et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 18 (2012) 1067e1072 1069

Table 2
Demographic and clinical characteristics of cognitively intact, MCI, and dementia PD.a

HCs (n ¼ 84) All PD (n ¼ 94) Cognitively intact PD-MCI PDD (n ¼ 9)


PD (n ¼ 41) (n ¼ 44)
Gender, %Male 56 62.8 68.3 56.8 66.7
Handedness, %R/L/A 98.8/0/1.2 98.9/0/1.1 97.6/0/2.4 100/0/0 100/0/0
Age, y 61.00(7.71) 61.64 (6.54) 59.73(6.59) 62.18(5.44) 67.67(7.76)
Education, y 12.31(3.57) 11.90(4.25) 12.93(3.59) 10.95(4.78) 11.89(3.44)
premorbid VIQb 107.9(10.42) 105.05(13.08) 109.33(13.31) 102.56(11.99) 97.73(11.88)
Age of onset, y e 57.61(6.94) 56.15(6.43) 57.77(6.52) 63.44(8.69)
Disease duration, y e 4.03(2.18) 3.59(2.03) 4.41(2.26) 4.22(2.39)
Stages e 1.53(0.50) 1.34(0.48) 1.61(0.49) 2.00(0.00)
LED e 622.0(334.5) 545.7(243.8) 644.38(362.9) 575.2(194.8)
a
Mean and standard deviation.
b
Estimated by demographic variables and the vocabulary subtests.

The effect size, Cohen’s d (d ¼ 0.2e0.49 indicates small, 0.5e0.79 effectively differentiated patients from controls. The set of variables
medium, and S0.8 large), of each test was also shown in Table 3. correctly classified 70.4% of the cases (75.9% of PD patients and
To determine which tests had greatest ability to differentiate PD 62.7% of controls).
patients from controls, we conducted a logistic regression analysis.
Variables that were associated with cognitive decline in the 3.2. Proportion in patients with PD-MCI and PDD
univariate analyses were entered into a logistic regression as
independent variables, whereas the diagnosis (PD vs. control Proportions of cognitively intact PD, PD-MCI, and PDD were
group) was the dependent variable. The results of the logistic depicted in Fig. 1(A). By using DSM-IV criteria, eight (8.5%) patients
regression indicated that the MCST-C was measure that most were classified as having dementia. One (1.1%) patient has two

Table 3
Neuropsychological performances in the study groups.a

HCs (n ¼ 84) All PD (n ¼ 94) PD-MCI (n ¼ 44) pb pc Cohen’s d


Executive function
MCST-C 5.96(1.22) 4.72(1.98) 4.20(1.95) 0.00 0.00 0.75
MCST-P 2.33(2.75) 5.07(8.05) 4.66(5.22) 0.01 0.00 0.46
MCST-NP 5.35(2.76) 7.70(4.71) 9.77(4.96) 0.00 0.00 0.61
Category fluency 39.25(8.16) 35.38(8.43) 35.52(8.33) 0.00 0.02 0.47
WMI 111.13(12.88) 105.32(13.35) 102.56(13.7) 0.01 0.00 0.44
Similarities 11.79(2.34) 11.05(2.96) 10.55(2.71) 0.05 0.00 0.28
Matrix reasoning 11.86(2.46) 10.53(2.81) 9.65(2.52) 0.00 0.00 0.50
Color trails part B 95.75(24.43) 117.47(53.09) 121.43(39.53) 0.04 0.00 0.53
Memory function
LM-I(r.s.) 38.93(10.92) 32.91(14.22) 28.89(12.09) 0.00 0.00 0.47
LM-II(r.s.) 24.04(9.16) 19.51(11.26) 16.32(9.99) 0.02 0.00 0.44
LM-recognition(r.s.) 25.73(6.57) 23.20(4.85) 22.48(4.34) 0.02 0.00 0.44
VR-I(r.s.) 76.56(13.71) 68.68(20.89) 64.14(18.31) 0.03 0.00 0.45
VR-II(r.s.) 55.6(20.39) 51.36(24.79) 47.02(21.75) 0.44 0.03 0.19
VR-recognition(r.s.) 43.30(2.95) 41.54(4.49) 40.14(4.03) 0.02 0.00 0.46
MMSE-3 objects 2.57(0.64) 2.14(1.00) 2.07(1.02) 0.01 0.01 0.51
Psychomotor speed
DSS 11.85(2.28) 10.29(2.33) 9.70(2.19) 0.00 0.00 0.68
Symbol searching 11.98(2.11) 10.21(2.82) 9.86(2.53) 0.00 0.00 0.71
Processing speed index 110.47(10.89) 101.22(13.01) 98.79(11.73) 0.00 0.00 0.77
Color trails part A 47.88(15.68) 63.62(34.12) 70.71(35.09) 0.00 0.01 0.59
Visuospatial function
Block design 11.48(2.29) 9.87(2.45) 9.30(2.31) 0.00 0.00 0.68
Pentagon copy, %Error 0% 12.2% 22.7% 0.00 0.00 0.51
Attention
Digit span 12.26(3.03) 11.12(2.60) 10.64(2.77) 0.04 0.01 0.40
Digit span-forward 7.78(1.46) 7.76(1.27) 7.65(1.33) 0.66 0.38 0.01
Digit span-backward 5.49(1.61) 4.82(1.48) 4.70(1.58) 0.01 0.01 0.43
Digit span-difference 2.29(1.45) 2.95(1.43) 2.95(1.54) 0.00 0.02 0.46
Color trails-interference 1.10(0.52) 1.20(2.16) 0.98(0.83) 0.09 0.13 0.06
Serial 7s 4.62(0.75) 4.42(1.14) 4.57(1.04) 0.53 0.74 0.21
Language
Naming 2.00(0.00) 2.00(0.00) 2.00(0.00) 1.00 1.00 e
Repetition 1.00(0.00) 1.00(0.00) 1.00(0.00) 1.00 1.00 e
Verbal comprehension 2.97(0.16) 2.90(0.40) 2.89(0.443) 0.22 0.26 0.23

Abbreviations: see Table 1 and 2 and WMI, working memory index; r.s., raw score; LM-I, immediate memory of the logical memory; LM-II, delayed memory of the logical
memory; VR-I, immediate memory of the visual reproduction; VR-II, delayed memory of the logical memory; DSS, digit symbol substitution; MCST-C, MCST-P, and MCST-NP
indicate the achieved categories, perseverative, and non-perseverative errors in the modified card sorting test, respectively.
a
Mean and standard deviation.
b
Comparisons between healthy controls and all PD.
c
Comparisons between healthy controls and PD-MCI.
1070 R.-L. Yu et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 18 (2012) 1067e1072

Fig. 1. The proportion of (A) cognitively intact PD, PD-MCI, PDD and (B) PD-MCI subtype. Abbreviations: aMCI, amnestic mild cognitive impairment; non-aMCI, non-amnestic mild
cognitive impairment; s, single domain; m, multiple domains.

impaired non-memory cognitive domains (executive and The PD-MCI cases found in this study are heterogeneous in
psychomotor speed) plus social function impairment, but he was nature with a prevailing impairment in executive function, which
excluded from the dementia list based on the DSM-IV criteria. markedly differs from a primary deficit of memory most
commonly seen in the MCI of Alzheimer’s Disease (AD). In this
hospital-based population of early-stage PD patients, 9.6% met the
3.3. Pattern of cognitive impairment in patients with PD-MCI
consensus diagnostic criteria [12] for PDD and 46.8% had defects
in at least one cognitive domain. Our findings are compatible with
The proportions of subtype among PD-MCI were shown in
the literature [5,7,13] and suggest that single-domain MCI is more
Fig. 1(B). The percentage of impairment in PD-MCI and PDD were
common than multiple-domain MCI and that non-aMCI is more
shown in Fig. 2(A), and across different subtype of PD-MCI, the
common than aMCI in the PD population. Across the different
profile of impairment was depicted in Fig. 2(B).
types of PD-MCI, executive function was the most commonly
impaired domain. Moreover, the distribution of different domains
4. Discussion of cognitive impairment further revealed the heterogeneity of
PD-MCI.
Compared with either controls’ or normative data, our early- Similar to the findings described here, Caviness et al. [26] and
stage PD patients showed impaired performance, particularly in Janvin et al. [27] also found that large proportions of PD-MCI
executive and psychomotor cognitive functions, on a wide range of patients exhibited single-domain frontal/executive dysfunction.
standardized neuropsychological tests. Executive function is However, some of our patients manifested single-domain MCI
a complex construct, and the exact pattern of executive impairment characterized by visuospatial impairment. In fact, some studies
in PD is still being debated [21]. Our findings are consistent with have reported that single-domain non-aMCI is associated with later
other studies indicating that executive function deficits predomi- development of dementia [27]. Others have suggested that visuo-
nate in patients with PD [6,8] and are compatible with patho- spatial deficits rather than frontostriatal executive dysfunction play
physiological evidence suggesting that the frontostriatal loop an important role in dementia development in early PD patients
dysfunction develops at an early stage in the disease course [22,23]. [28]. If this is the case, visuospatial ability impairment could be
Results from the executive function measurements suggest that the a pivotal predictor of PDD.
MCST, conventionally thought to reflect dorsolateral prefrontal Based on the present study and most other available cross-
functioning [24], can differentiate patients with PD from control sectional and limited longitudinal study results, the issue of
participants more effectively than other neuropsychological tests. whether such a cognitive impairment is persistent in nature along
Patients do appear to benefit from executive function training the disease course remains undetermined. In this study, it seems
(i.e., mental shifting ability) tailored to their needs [25]. Our study that the PD-MCI is a transitional stage of PDD; however, our small
has revealed that a number of specific executive abilities, specifi- PDD sample size and cross-sectional study design limit this inter-
cally concept formation and cognitive flexibility, should be pretation. More extensive research is needed to determine whether
emphasized in rehabilitation programs designed for PD patients. PD-MCI is a precursor of PDD. Furthermore, the sense of function
Relatively few previous studies have focused on processing speed heterogeneity in patients does not only implicate the aspect of
function even though it is crucial for everyday functions. Our dysfunction, but also espouse normal function. Indeed, there were
findings of processing speed impairment are in agreement with 43.6% of our patients with sound cognitive function. It will be
Muslimovic et al. [6] However, further study using motor-free important and intriguing to further explore potential factors that
measures is needed to determine whether this impairment is one may protect cognitive function resilience in early-stage PD patients
of the core cognitive dysfunctions in early-stage PD patients. without MCI.
R.-L. Yu et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 18 (2012) 1067e1072 1071

Fig. 2. The percentage of impairment in (A) PD-MCI and PDD, and (B) different subtype of PD-MCI.

The current study revealed that PDD patients’ cognitive func- assessment (i.e., Neuropsychiatric Inventory). Although the role of
tions are closely associated with the frontostriatal circuitry rather neuropsychiatric symptoms in cognitive function remains a very
than the posterior cortical circuitry. Our finding is conflict with the promising area of research, this line of research was beyond the
results from other studies showing that posterior cortical scope of the current study.
dysfunction is closely related to dementia in PD [10,11]. The In summary, cognitive dysfunction is common in early-stage PD
discrepancy suggests that using DSM-IV criteria, which rely heavily patients, and specific neuropsychological testing of executive
on memory function to classify PDD patients, will result in false- function can effectively differentiate patients from normal controls.
negative errors thereby underestimating the disease prevalence. The current study suggests that early-stage PD patients have
In view of the different patterns of cognitive impairment between cognitive dysfunction predominantly in the anterior brain, and
AD and PD and the heterogeneity of cognitive dysfunction within comprehensive neuropsychological assessments should be
the PD population itself, our study favors application of the administered to patients in the initial disease stage. Furthermore,
dementia criteria proposed by the MDS. The MDS criteria include our findings favor the clinical applicability of the dementia criteria
most cognitive function domains and weigh each domain equally proposed by the MDS over that of the DSM-IV.
rather than predominantly focusing on memory function.
Although this study has provided novel insights into the
Acknowledgments
cognitive domain dysfunction profile of early-stage PD patients, it
does have some limitations. First, the MCI definition used here was
We are grateful to the participants involved in this study for
based entirely on cognitive tests. The original MCI clinical diagnosis
their support and the grant from the National Science Council,
requires the presence of a patient’s subjective cognitive complaints
Taipei, Taiwan (NSC 98-2410-H-002-026-MY3 and NSC 100-2420-
[29]; however, some evidence suggests that not all early-stage PD
H-002-019-DR) and National Taiwan University Hospital (NTUH
patients have preserved meta-memory/awareness of memory
101-S1889).
ability [30], which may lead to over or underreporting [2,26].
Hence, the presence or absence of subjective memory complaints
was not included as part of the MCI definition in this analysis. References
Second, a detailed motor rating scale (i.e., part III of the Unified
Parkinson Disease Rating Scale) was not used in this study, and [1] Zgaljardic DJ, Borod JC, Foldi NS, Mattis P. A review of the cognitive and
behavioral sequelae of Parkinson’s disease: relationship to frontostriatal
thus, cognitive performance could not be corrected for motor
circuitry. Cogn Behav Neurol 2003 Dec;16(4):193e210.
impact. Since different pathophysiological mechanisms among the [2] Aarsland D, Bronnick K, Larsen JP, Tysnes OB, Alves G. Cognitive impairment in
PD patients with various motor symptoms could contribute to incident, untreated Parkinson disease: the Norwegian ParkWest study.
different cognitive dysfunction [8,11], various motor disturbances Neurology 2009 Mar 31;72(13):1121e6.
[3] Foltynie T, Brayne CE, Robbins TW, Barker RA. The cognitive ability of an
should be taken into account in future studies. Last, participants incident cohort of Parkinson’s patients in the UK. The CamPaIGN study. Brain
recruited in this study did not receive a detailed neuropsychiatric 2004 Mar;127(Pt 3):550e60.
1072 R.-L. Yu et al. / Parkinsonism and Related Disorders 18 (2012) 1067e1072

[4] Hobson P, Meara J. Risk and incidence of dementia in a cohort of older subjects [16] McKeith IG, Dickson DW, Lowe J, Emre M, O’Brien JT, Feldman H, et al.
with Parkinson’s disease in the United Kingdom. Mov Disord 2004 Sep;19(9): Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: third report of the
1043e9. DLB consortium. Neurology 2005 Dec 27;65(12):1863e72.
[5] Litvan I, Aarsland D, Adler CH, Goldman JG, Kulisevsky J, Mollenhauer B, et al. [17] Nelson HE. A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects.
MDS task force on mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: critical Cortex 1976 Dec;12(4):313e24.
review of PD-MCI. Mov Disord 2011 Aug 15;26(10):1814e24. [18] D’elia LF, Satz P, Uchiyama CL, White T. Color trials test. USA: Psychologica;
[6] Muslimovic D, Post B, Speelman JD, Schmand B. Cognitive profile of patients Assessment Resources, Inc; 1996.
with newly diagnosed Parkinson disease. Neurology 2005 Oct 25;65(8): [19] Chen JH, Chen HY. Wechsler adult intelligence scale. 3rd ed. Taipei: Chinese
1239e45. Behavioral Science Corp; 2002 [Manual].
[7] Aarsland D, Bronnick K, Williams-Gray C, Weintraub D, Marder K, Kulisevsky J, [20] Hua MS, Chang BS, Lin KN, Yang JM, Lu SR, Chen SY. Wechsler memory scale.
et al. Mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease: a multicenter pooled 3rd ed. Taipei: Chinese Behavioral Science Corp; 2005 [Manual].
analysis. Neurology 2010 Sep 21;75(12):1062e9. [21] Kudlicka A, Clare L, Hindle JV. Executive functions in Parkinson’s disease:
[8] Kehagia AA, Barker RA, Robbins TW. Neuropsychological and clinical hetero- systematic review and meta-analysis. Mov Disord 2011 Nov;26(13):2305e15.
geneity of cognitive impairment and dementia in patients with Parkinson’s [22] Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of functionally
disease. Lancet Neurol 2010 Dec;9(12):1200e13. segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 1986;
[9] Kulisevsky J, Pagonabarraga J. Cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: 9:357e81.
tools for diagnosis and assessment. Mov Disord 2009 Jun 15;24(8): [23] Chudasama Y, Robbins TW. Functions of frontostriatal systems in cognition:
1103e10. comparative neuropsychopharmacological studies in rats, monkeys and
[10] Aarsland D, Bronnick K, Fladby T. Mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s humans. Biol Psychol 2006 Jul;73(1):19e38.
disease. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2011 Aug;11(4):371e8. [24] Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment. 4th ed. Oxford: Oxford University
[11] Williams-Gray CH, Evans JR, Goris A, Foltynie T, Ban M, Robbins TW, et al. The Press; 2004.
distinct cognitive syndromes of Parkinson’s disease: 5 year follow-up of the [25] Sammer G, Reuter I, Hullmann K, Kaps M, Vaitl D. Training of executive
CamPaIGN cohort. Brain 2009 Nov;132(Pt 11):2958e69. functions in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol Sci 2006 Oct 25;248(1e2):115e9.
[12] Emre M, Aarsland D, Brown R, Burn DJ, Duyckaerts C, Mizuno Y, et al. Clinical [26] Caviness JN, Driver-Dunckley E, Connor DJ, Sabbagh MN, Hentz JG, Noble B,
diagnostic criteria for dementia associated with Parkinson’s disease. Mov et al. Defining mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord
Disord 2007 Sep 15;22(12):1689e707. quiz 837. 2007 Jul 15;22(9):1272e7.
[13] Wu Q, Chen L, Zheng Y, Zhang C, Huang L, Guo W, et al. Cognitive impairment [27] Janvin CC, Larsen JP, Aarsland D, Hugdahl K. Subtypes of mild cognitive
is common in Parkinson’s disease without dementia in the early and middle impairment in Parkinson’s disease: progression to dementia. Mov Disord
stages in a Han Chinese cohort. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2012 Feb;18(2): 2006 Sep;21(9):1343e9.
161e5. [28] Williams-Gray CH, Foltynie T, Brayne CE, Robbins TW, Barker RA. Evolution of
[14] Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of cognitive dysfunction in an incident Parkinson’s disease cohort. Brain 2007
idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. Jul;130(Pt 7):1787e98.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatr 1992 Mar;55(3):181e4. [29] Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med
[15] Kulisevsky J, Pagonabarraga J, Pascual-Sedano B, Garcia-Sanchez C, 2004 Sep;256(3):183e94.
Gironell A. Prevalence and correlates of neuropsychiatric symptoms in [30] Yu RL, Wu RM, Tai CH, Lin CH, Hua MS. Feeling-of-knowing in episodic
Parkinson’s disease without dementia. Mov Disord 2008 Oct 15;23(13): memory in patients with Parkinson’s disease with various motor symptoms.
1889e96. Mov Disord 2010 Jun 15;25(8):1034e9.

You might also like