You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Occupational Health

Psychology
Looking Forward: How Anticipated Workload Change
Influences the Present Workload–Emotional Strain
Relationship
Michael J. DiStaso and Mindy K. Shoss
Online First Publication, September 3, 2020. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000261

CITATION
DiStaso, M. J., & Shoss, M. K. (2020, September 3). Looking Forward: How Anticipated Workload
Change Influences the Present Workload–Emotional Strain Relationship. Journal of Occupational
Health Psychology. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000261
Journal of Occupational Health Psychology
© 2020 American Psychological Association 2020, Vol. 2, No. 999, 000
ISSN: 1076-8998 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000261

Looking Forward: How Anticipated Workload Change Influences the


Present Workload–Emotional Strain Relationship

Michael J. DiStaso Mindy K. Shoss


University of Central Florida Saint Louis University

Many workers experience ebbs and flows of workload in concert with busy seasons, changing project
demands, and changes to the way tasks are completed. The present study examined how anticipating
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

workload changes influences emotional strain resulting from current workload. We extend conservation
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of resources theory (Hobfoll, 1989) by hypothesizing that anticipated changes in workload may be
viewed as a future opportunity to recover from current workload (in the case of anticipated workload
decrease) or as a threat of loss of resources (in the case of anticipated workload increase), and that
anticipated changes in workload would moderate the current workload– emotional strain relationship.
Using a sample of employees engaged in project-based work, we found that anticipation of a workload
increase exacerbated the current workload– emotional strain relationship and anticipation of a workload
decrease buffered this relationship. The results suggest that stressor–strain relations may be influenced
by anticipated change in stressor conditions. In other words, employees are more likely to tolerate
stressful circumstances if they see the proverbial “light at the end of the tunnel.” Implications for
occupational stress research and practice are discussed.

Keywords: anticipated change, emotional strain, future-oriented cognition, workload

With few exceptions, jobs impose some level of workload on more fatigue (Grech, Neal, Yeo, Humphreys, & Smith, 2009),
employees at all times. Workload represents a demand placed on negative affect (Ilies et al., 2007), and affective distress and blood
employees that requires continual exertion of energy to meet pressure (Ilies, Dimotakis, & De Pater, 2010) during high work-
(Bowling & Kirkendall, 2012). For this reason, high levels of load time periods than during low workload time periods.
workload have been found to relate to psychological strains, in- Although researchers have made strides in examining these
cluding indicators of emotional strain (Alarcon, 2011; Podsakoff, dynamics, we argue that these studies maintain a somewhat static
LePine, & LePine, 2007; Spector & Jex, 1998). perspective on workload from the employee’s point of view.
Workload has traditionally been studied as a static stressor that Specifically, the aforementioned studies capture reactions to the
varies as a function of the job. In other words, jobs were thought workload that the employee has experienced, whether that be in
to be characterized as having either a high or low workload general, over a month time frame, or over a given day. By asking
(Karasek, 1979; Mariani, 1999). In recent years, however, re- participants about their previously or recently experienced work-
searchers have begun to recognize the dynamic, changing nature of load, workload research has narrowly focused on the past and
workload. Workload can change gradually and permanently over present. In contrast, studies have not yet considered how employ-
time, as evidenced in the study of work intensification (Green, ees’ stress reactions are shaped by what employees anticipate will
2001). Workload can also change rapidly and temporarily. In this happen to their workload in the future.
regard, studies have generally found that employees experience We consider this to be a serious gap in our knowledge. Psy-
chologists argue that the ability to prospect into the future is one of
the defining hallmarks of humanity (Atance & O’Neill, 2001,
2005). Future-oriented cognitions include hopes, expectations, and
predictions about future states and conditions that may or may not
X Michael J. DiStaso, Department of Psychology, University of Central occur. Much of human behavior is motivated by future-oriented
Florida; Mindy K. Shoss, Department of Psychology, Saint Louis Univer- cognition, and people spend a great deal of time anticipating how
sity. their conditions might change (Aspinwall, 2005; Atance &
Mindy K. Shoss is now at Department of Psychology, University of O’Neill, 2001). In that vein, several conceptualizations of stress
Central Florida. feature elements of anticipation of an event (Hobfoll’s [1989]
A previous version of this article was presented at the 34th annual
notion of threat of resource loss; Lazarus and Folkman’s [1984]
conference of Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology in
National Harbor, Maryland.
notion of threat appraisal; Beehr and Bhagat’s [1985] discussion of
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Michael J. uncertainty; Aspinwall’s [2005] discussion of proactive coping).
DiStaso, Department of Psychology, University of Central Florida, 4000 Although there has been some research demonstrating that antic-
Central Florida Boulevard, Orlando, FL 32816. E-mail: michael.distaso@ ipated workplace events in particular impact well-being outcomes
ucf.edu (see Caplan & Jones, 1975; Casper, Sonnentag, & Tremmel, 2017;

1
2 DISTASO AND SHOSS

Eden, 1990; Shoss, 2017), this research has tended to not consider Workload and Emotional Strain
anticipated change in a circumstance separately from the circum-
stance itself. Thus, the question of how anticipating a change in a Workload refers to the amount or difficulty of job tasks that an
employee has at a given period of time (Spector & Jex, 1998).
stressor (i.e., workload) impacts reactions to the current levels of
High workload is characterized by an excessive level of job tasks
that stressor remains unresolved.
that cannot be managed within a time span. The relation between
With regard to workload, we argue that workload changes are
workload and well-being is supported theoretically by resource-
one of the many changes in job conditions that employees are
based stress theories such as the COR theory (Hobfoll, 1989).
aware of and anticipate. For example, accountants know that the
According to this theory, workload represents a stressor in the
spring is a busy season. Retail workers know in advance that
work environment that consumes psychological, physical, or other
weekends are particularly busy shopping days. Teachers learn over
valued personal resources. The consumption of resources, in turn,
time which classes are the most demanding and anticipate that
instigates the psychological stress process. This process produces
there is more to do at the beginning and end of the year than in the
negative emotional, cognitive, and behavioral outcomes of stress
middle. Technical support staff anticipate more difficult inquiries
called strains. In this vein, researchers have consistently found that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

when new, unfamiliar software is issued. These examples highlight an employee’s workload is associated negatively with well-being
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

an important question: How are our reactions to our current work- outcomes (Alarcon, 2011; Lee & Ashforth, 1996; Podsakoff et al.,
load changed when we anticipate increases or decreases in work- 2007; Spector & Jex, 1998).
load? Despite progress made in our understanding of workload as Emotional strain is a frequently studied strain resulting from
a stressor, there is a lack of theoretical and empirical research on workload (Alarcon, 2011; Demerouti, Bakker, Nachreiner, &
how anticipating workload changes affects reactions to current Schaufeli, 2001; Sonnentag, Kuttler, & Fritz, 2010; Spector & Jex,
workload levels. 1998). For example, Spector and Jex’s (1998) early meta-analysis
This article seeks to answer this question of whether expecta- found that workload is associated with affective strains (frustra-
tions about the future influence reactions to present workload. tion, anxiety), and Alarcon’s (2011) more recent meta-analysis of
Drawing from and building on the conservation of resources 231 work stress studies found that workload predicted emotional
(COR) theory (Hobfoll, 1989), we hypothesize that anticipated exhaustion and other facets of burnout. Because COR theory and
changes in workload will buffer or aggravate the relationship empirical evidence consistently indicate that workload is associ-
between current workload and exhaustion depending on the tra- ated with emotional strains, we hypothesize that workload will
jectory of the anticipated change (i.e., anticipated decrease or predict emotional strain in the current study. We offer this hypoth-
increase). When workload is anticipated to decrease, we hypoth- esis to replicate previous findings and to lay the groundwork for
esize that employees view this as an opportunity for recovery and the hypothesized moderating effect of anticipated changes in
react less negatively to their current workload. When workload is workload.
anticipated to increase, we hypothesize that employees process this
as a lack of opportunity to recover spent resources and a threat to Hypothesis 1: Workload is positively associated with emo-
future resources. As a result, they react more negatively to their tional strain.
current workload. Thus, by assessing both current workload and
anticipated changes in workload, we explore how current experi- The Changing Nature of Workload
ences interact with anticipated changes. The work environment is dynamic, and its conditions change
This study moves the workload and broader occupational health frequently. Within-person studies have shown that workload is a
psychology literature forward in several important ways. Specifi- dynamic stressor that varies within jobs (Ilies et al., 2010). More-
cally, we determine whether employees integrate expectations over, the study of work intensification shows that job demands
about the future into their emotional reactions to their current work have increased alongside economic and societal changes (Green,
conditions. This is important for both theory and practice, as both 2001; Green & McIntosh, 2001). The assignment of new tasks,
have yet to take into account people’s perceptions of future changes to the way tasks are carried out, the introduction of a new
changes in understanding their reactions to current workplace team member, and new technological advances all represent situ-
conditions. Indeed, our findings suggest that our understanding of ations that affect the level of workload that an employee must
emotional strain would benefit from considering not only how manage. Workload demands can change from day-to-day, week-
people perceive their current workload but also the extent to which to-week, and month-to-month. Workload that is highly demanding
they anticipate their workload levels will change. We also further at one point in time may become less demanding or more demand-
COR theory by suggesting that perceptions of current resource ing in the future.
expenditure (i.e., workload) function in an interactive manner with We argue that employees frequently anticipate changes in their
perceptions of opportunities for recovery (i.e., anticipated work- work environment as part of a human instinct to think about the
load change) to impact current levels of emotional strain. In this future. Workload ebbs and flows, and employees anticipate when
sense, we offer COR theory as an overarching metatheory that may changes to their circumstances will affect their workload. Teachers
help future research consider how current conditions interact with know in advance when the summer break begins. Emergency room
the anticipated change in those conditions to shape emotional staff know that their work will be more demanding during flu
outcomes. In the sections that follow, we briefly review the liter- season than other times of the year. Hospitality employees know
ature on workload and emotional strain before turning our atten- which months are their busy season. An employee whose team
tion to how anticipated workload change should moderate this member announces a vacation knows that they will have to man-
relationship. age that team member’s workload until they return. Our study is
LOOKING FORWARD 3

intended to address these cases where changes are foreseen, re- is the result of both the actual or perceived threat of resource loss.
gardless of whether the anticipated workload changes become We argue that the COR theory can be framed as a metatheory that
reality. unifies the aforementioned findings while guiding the hypotheses
of the present study. Hobfoll (1989, 2001) emphasized that both
actual and perceived resource loss instigate the psychological
COR Theory and Anticipated Workload Change
stress process. Further, the COR theory makes propositions about
The broad concept of anticipation has been addressed sporadi- the importance of opportunities for resource gain. The anticipation
cally in stress research. Laboratory research has found that indi- of circumstances that lead to the acquisition of more resources or
viduals experience a variety of stress-related responses when they replacement of expended resources is theorized to reduce psycho-
anticipate a stressful event. Outcomes of anticipating a stressful logical stress (Hobfoll, 1989). The COR theory’s emphasis not
experience include increased negative affect, heart rate, skin con- only on current resource loss but also on the importance of per-
ductance, and cortisol levels (Epstein & Roupenian, 1970; Gaab, ceived threats of loss and opportunities for gain makes it useful for
Rohleder, Nater, & Ehlert, 2005; Waugh, Panage, Mendes, & making predictions about the role of anticipated changes in work-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Gotlib, 2010). Research also suggests that those who anticipate load.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

positive outcomes in the future are more likely to use problem- According to the COR theory, current workload represents
focused coping strategies (Scheier, Weintraub, & Carver, 1986), current resource loss. As described earlier, workload requires the
appraise hypothetical future stressors as controllable (Peacock & expenditure of resources, such as time, energy, and effort. When
Wong, 1996), and have positive physical well-being (Scheier & workload is high, the environment requires greater expenditure of
Carver, 1987). These findings suggest that our beliefs about the personal resources, which leads to emotional strain. We advance
future have a substantial impact on stress-related outcomes. Yet, the COR theory by applying its concepts of future resource threat
with few exceptions (e.g., the notion of uncertainty in the role and resource gain to future-oriented cognition. First, we argue that
stressor literature; Harvey et al., 2017), people’s cognitions about anticipation of a workload increase reflects a threat of future
future have rarely been the focus on theory and research in occu- resource loss and the denial of an opportunity for resource recov-
pational stress literature. ery. Second, the anticipation of a workload decrease maps onto
Within occupational stress research specifically, Beehr (1998) Hobfoll’s (1989) concept of opportunity for future resource gain.
and Beehr and Bhagat (1985) theorized that perceptions of the We hypothesize that current workload and anticipated workload
future (uncertainty, importance, and perceived duration of imag- change operate in an interactive fashion. That is, the strength of the
ined future stressors) are key predictors of experienced stress. The relationship between workload and emotional strain will be influ-
importance of beliefs about the future is showcased in the study of enced by anticipated changes in workload. The nature of the
job insecurity, which is grounded primarily in resource-based moderation effect will depend on whether workload is anticipated
theories of stress. Job insecurity reflects anticipation of a potential to increase or decrease. We illustrate the expected pattern of
change in employment status (De Witte, 1999; Shoss, 2017), and relationships for those anticipating workload increases and de-
its relationship with emotional strain is well established (De creases in the sections in the following text.
Cuyper, Bernhard-Oettel, Berntson, De Witte, & Alarco, 2008; De
Witte et al., 2010). Job insecurity researchers frame job insecurity
Anticipated Workload Increase
as a threat to future resources and frequently use COR theory to
explain job insecurity’s effects (Benson et al., 2017; Cheng, When employees anticipate a workload increase, they are cued
Mauno, & Lee, 2014; De Cuyper, Mäkikangas, Kinnunen, Mauno, that their future resources are threatened. This can occur, for
& Witte, 2012; Jiang & Probst, 2014, 2017; Ünal-Karagüven, example, when an employee’s team member leaves the company,
2009). Particularly relevant to the current study, Shoss, Brummel, and the employee becomes responsible for her own tasks and her
Probst, and Jiang (2020) recently used COR to argue that job team member’s tasks for an indefinite period of time. An antici-
insecurity and job satisfaction interact to shape a variety of out- pated increase also signals that the employee may not have the
comes, suggesting that people’s anticipated change in job status opportunity to replenish or replace resources that have already
may shape how they respond to their current job conditions. been invested to address the current workload, suggesting that
Little research has examined future-oriented cognition related to current workload and anticipated changes in workload work in an
workload. Casper et al. (Casper & Sonnentag, 2020; Casper et al., interactive fashion. As current workload increases, anticipation of
2017) have examined daily workload anticipation and its effects on a further increased workload is viewed as more of a threat. These
behavior and well-being. They found that individuals engage in conditions prompt employees to continue mobilizing psychologi-
both productive (e.g., work-related planning) and counterproduc- cal resources, or even expend more resources, to address their
tive (e.g., worrying) coping in response to anticipating a high workload without the promise of recovery. Because both current
workload and that these anticipatory coping strategies predict and future resources are threatened, and there is no perceived
exhaustion and vigor. However, they did not examine how antic- opportunity to recover, we predict that the relationship between
ipated workload shapes reactions to current workload, nor did they current workload and emotional strain will be particularly strong
examine beliefs about how workload would change. Thus, our and positive when employees anticipate a workload increase.
focus is on anticipated change in workload and on the moderating
role of anticipated workload change on the current workload–
Anticipated Workload Decrease
emotional strain relationship.
The COR theory posits that individuals are motivated to acquire, Anticipated workload decrease occurs when an employee be-
maintain, and protect their resources and that psychological stress lieves that their task demands will become easier to manage. This
4 DISTASO AND SHOSS

occurs, for example, when an employee is joined by a new team could begin participation at any point, we performed several
member who will take on some of that employee’s workload. In follow-up analyses specifically looking at how workload, antici-
this case, the employee is not faced with the threat of future pated workload change, and emotional strain may have impacted
resource loss but with the promise of resource recovery. The responses. These analyses indicated that none of the three study
anticipated workload decrease signals that the employee will have variables predicted the length of time between first and second
an opportunity to cease investing personal resources on the current surveys (e.g., participants who responded to surveys in Months 1
workload and to replenish invested resources. This suggests an and 4 vs. in Months 1 and 2). Emotional strain, but not workload
interaction between current workload and the anticipated workload nor anticipated workload change, at the respondent’s initial survey
change. Specifically, we expect the anticipation of a workload predicted whether this respondent completed a second survey at
decrease to buffer the harmful effects of current workload on any point during the study. Finally, emotional strain, but not
current emotional strain by changing employees’ outlook on their workload nor anticipated workload change, during any survey
current workload. predicted whether a participant responded to the subsequent
In sum, our second hypothesis states that anticipated workload month’s survey. Together, results from these four analyses suggest
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

change moderates the relationship between current workload and that our missing data fall on the continuum between missing at
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

emotional strain by (a) promising the possibility of future resource random and missing not at random, with some analyses suggesting
recovery and/or (b) threatening future resources. missing not at random whereas others suggesting missing at ran-
dom. The type of missingness in the present study is best catego-
Hypothesis 2: Anticipated changes in workload moderate rized as stochastic range restriction (Newman, 2014) because
(buffer or aggravate) the positive relationship between current participants with high emotional strain during their first survey
workload and emotional strain such that workload and emo- were slightly less likely to respond to future surveys. Newman
tional strain have a weaker relationship when the employee explained that this can make it less likely for analyses to detect true
anticipates a workload decrease and a stronger relationship effects. Newman (2009) advocated for the use of maximum like-
when the employee anticipates a workload increase. lihood estimation in the cases where data are missing completely
at random or at random to mitigate any potential bias. Following
Method this advice, we use maximum likelihood estimation, which heavily
reduces the chance of biased estimates.
Procedure and Sample
Measures
This project received institutional review board approval from
the Saint Louis University institutional review board, Project Title Workload. Current workload was measured using six items
“Work and Well-Being Over Time,” Protocol Number 22852. from Caplan, Cobb, French, Harrison, and Pinneau (1980) and
Data were collected at a market research firm in the Midwestern adapted slightly based on communication with the organization’s
United States. Employees in this company work on a project basis leaders. Participants indicated their qualitative and quantitative
where their workload and work hours are based on the project workload during the past month. As recommended by Caplan et al.
needs. Management described employees’ workload as having (1980), the items (a) “How often does your job require you to work
peaks and valleys based on the different projects and stated that very fast?” and (b) “How often does your job require you to work
employees were able to anticipate future projects coming down the very hard?” were scored on a 5-point frequency Likert scale
pipeline. Projects tended to last about a month, and therefore ranging from 1 (rarely) to 5 (very often). The remaining four items
workload tended to change on a monthly basis. Employees were (“Over the past month, what quantity of work did others expect
invited to complete a brief survey each month for 1 year to you to do?”, “Over the past month, how much difficulty was
investigate the ebb and flow of work stress. Participants could start involved in the completion of your job duties?”, “Over the past
participating in the study at any time throughout the study’s month, how many lulls between heavy workload periods do you
duration and were eligible for compensation for each month that have?” [reverse coded], and “Over the past month, how many
they completed a survey. In total, 61 unique participants completed projects, assignments, or tasks did you have?”) were scored on a
at least one survey, and a total of 154 observations were made. The Likert scale ranging from 1 (hardly any) to 5 (a great deal).
sample’s mean age was 40.89 years, and 80.33% of participants Cronbach’s ␣ was .86. The workload was grand-mean centered for
were female. On average, participants had been working at the the analyses.
company for 3.85 years. Participants were enrolled in raffles for Anticipated workload change. Anticipated workload change
$10 Amazon gift cards as well as paid $2 for completion of each was measured using three items adapted from Caplan et al. (1980).
survey. The scale prompted the participants to reflect on what the changes
Due to incomplete data, 141 observations from 56 participants they expect to see in various aspects of their work over the course
(M ⫽ 2.48; SD ⫽ 2.27) were included in the analyses. We of the next month. The three items were as follows: “The amount
performed several analyses to investigate the nature of our missing of work you have to do,” “The difficulty of work you have to do,”
data (further details are available from the authors). As an over- and “The amount of time pressure you have.” Responses were
arching analysis, we predicted the number of survey responses per scored on the Likert scale ranging from 1 (decrease a lot) to 5
person from the person-level average of the main study variables (increase a lot). These items were then centered such that 0
and from demographics. There were no statistically significant represented an anticipation that workload would stay the same,
relationships between our study variables or demographics and with lower scores representing an anticipated decrease in workload
number of surveys completed. However, given that participants and higher scores representing an anticipated increase in workload
LOOKING FORWARD 5

to facilitate interpretation. Therefore, negative scores indicated Table 2


that participants expected a decrease in workload and positive Multilevel Regression Results Predicting Emotional Strain
indicated that participants expected an increase in workload. Cron-
bach’s ␣ was .82. 95% CI
Emotional strain. Emotional strain was measured using six Model parameter ␥ (SE) LL UL
items from Krischer, Penney, and Hunter (2010). Participants were
Constant 2.59 (.10)ⴱ 2.38 2.79
asked to indicate how frequently their job made them feel an Current workload 0.21 (.09)ⴱ 0.03 0.39
indicated emotion during the past month. These six items were as Anticipated workload change ⫺0.07 (.09) ⫺0.24 0.11
follows: “My job made me feel frustrated,” “My job made me feel Workload ⫻ Ant. Change 0.31 (.09)ⴱ 0.13 0.50
discouraged,” “My job made me feel frustrated,” “My job made Psuedo R2a .26
me feel gloomy,” “My job made me feel fatigued,” and “My job Note. CI ⫽ confidence interval; LL ⫽ lower limit; UL ⫽ upper limit;
made me feel miserable.” Responses were scored on a Likert scale Ant. ⫽ anticipated workload. All coefficients marked with an asterisk (ⴱ)
ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (extremely often). Cronbach’s ␣ was are significant at p ⬍ .05.
a
Psuedo R2 was calculated as the percentage decrease in residual variance
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.90.
from a null, intercept-only model predicting emotional strain.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Results change, and anticipated slight increase, respectively (i.e., ⫹1, 0,


and ⫺1) of anticipated workload change (see Figure 1). When
Descriptive statistics and correlations among study variables are
participants anticipated that workload would stay the same, there
displayed in Table 1. Current workload was positively associated
was a statistically significant relationship between workload and
with emotional strain (r ⫽ .26). Anticipated workload change was
emotional strain (␥ ⫽ .21; SE ⫽ .09, p ⬍ .05). When participants
not significantly associated with emotional strain (r ⫽ .05), which
anticipated an increase in workload, we predicted that the
fits with our contention that anticipated workload change may
workload– exhaustion relationship would be aggravated. This pre-
work in conjunction with current workload to impact emotional
diction was supported, as the relationship between workload and
strain.
emotional strain was potentiated and remained statistically signif-
We used multilevel modeling techniques using the STATA
icant (␥ ⫽ .52, SE ⫽ .10, p ⬍ .001). On the other hand, we
software’s xtmixed command to assess our hypothesis because of
predicted that the relationship is weaker when participants antici-
the nested nature of the data (e.g., individuals responding on
pate a decrease in their future workload. This prediction was
multiple occasions; Snijders & Bosker, 2012). This technique
supported, as workload anticipation buffered the relationship be-
corrects standard errors for nonindependence of observations and
tween workload and emotional strain, and this relationship became
uses maximum likelihood estimation for assessing regression ef-
nonsignificant (␥ ⫽ ⫺.10, SE ⫽ .16, p ⫽ .52). Thus, Hypothesis
fects. Before conducting our analyses, we grand-mean centered
2 was supported.
workload and calculated the interaction of the grand-mean cen-
tered workload variable and the anticipated workload change scale Discussion
centered at its midpoint (with 0 representing an anticipation that
workload would stay the same). Then, we regressed emotional The current study extends our understanding of the workload–
strain on workload, anticipated workload change, and the interac- emotional strain relationship by assessing employees’ anticipated
tion of workload and anticipated workload change. Multilevel change in workload. We suggested that workload changes could be
regression results are in Table 2. As seen there and in line with anticipated and influence how current workload impacts emotional
Hypothesis 1, workload predicted emotional strain (␥ ⫽ .21, SE ⫽ strain before the change even takes place. In particular, applying
.09, p ⬍ .05). Hypothesis 2 predicted that anticipated workload the concepts of resource threat and recovery from the COR theory
change would moderate the relationship between current workload (Hobfoll, 1989), we predicted that anticipated workload increase
and emotional strain. As expected, this interaction was statistically would exacerbate the relationship between current workload and
significant (␥ ⫽ .31, SE ⫽ 0.09, p ⬍ .001). emotional strain, whereas anticipated workload decrease would
We examined the nature of the current workload-anticipated buffer the relationship between current workload and emotional
changes in workload interaction by calculating the simple slopes of strain.
the relationship between workload and emotional strain levels The results supported our hypotheses. When workload was
corresponding to anticipated slight decrease, anticipated no anticipated to stay the same, there was a positive relationship
between current workload and emotional strain. However, when
workload was anticipated to decrease, the workload– emotional
Table 1 strain slope became nonsignificant. When workload was antici-
Means, Standard Deviations, Reliabilities, and Correlations pated to increase, the positive workload– emotional strain slope
Among Study 1 Variables was strengthened. Thus, current workload is associated with emo-
tional strain under the conditions that workload is anticipated to
Variable M SD 1 2 3 stay the same or increase, with the latter exacerbating the harmful
effects of current workload.
1. Current workload 3.75 0.72 (.86)
2. Anticipated workload change 0.31 0.67 ⫺.17ⴱ (.82)
3. Emotional strain 2.44 0.79 .26ⴱ ⫺.05 (.90) Theoretical Implications
Note. All correlations marked with an asterisk (ⴱ) are significant at p ⬍ This study has important theoretical implications for the study
.05. Internal consistency reliabilities are provided on the diagonal. of workload. Survey research on workload and other occupational
6 DISTASO AND SHOSS

3.5

Emotional Strain Decrease

2.5 Stay Same

Increase

2
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1.5
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1
Low Workload (-1 SD) High Workload (+1 SD)
Current Workload

Figure 1. The effect of current workload on emotional strain by levels of anticipated change in workload.

stressors primarily involve asking about participants’ experience resource loss and perceived threat of resource loss/perceived op-
with a recent or present stressor over a given period of time. portunity for resource recovery influence emotional strain in an
However, participants have not been asked how they believe that interactive fashion. We encourage future research to explore this
these conditions will change. This leaves a major gap in the type of interaction in shaping employees’ reactions to different
literature because workload levels change over time and employ- types of work stressors.
ees are able to anticipate future changes using information avail-
able to them in the present. Our study finds that participants make
Practical Implications
predictions about how stressors may change in the future and,
more importantly, that these predictions influence their emotional Our study also has practical takeaways. Our results suggest that
strain in the present. employees may be more willing to endure stressful circumstances
This study also supports and contributes to the broader psycho- if they anticipate that will have a chance to recover. In many
logical literature on future-oriented cognition. This literature has occupations (e.g., teaching), there are clear beginning and end
found that people invest considerable time and energy into think- points to busy periods, and employees can look forward to the
ing about and trying to anticipate the future (Atance & O’Neill, eventual break in workload. There is evidence of this in our data;
2001). The broadest implication of our findings is that future- we found that current workload was negatively (but weakly)
oriented cognitions interact with cognitions about present condi- associated with anticipated workload change (r ⫽ ⫺.17). This
tions to impact employee strain. finding seems to suggest that particularly high or low workload
Scattered research has examined future-oriented cognitions and periods were perceived as temporary. When high workload periods
their associations with stress (Casper & Sonnentag, 2020; Waugh do have ambiguous end points, leaders should take the initiative to
et al., 2010) and the effect of workload changes on well-being reduce employees’ emotional strain. Specifically, management
(Franke, 2015; Green, 2004). However, few studies have examined should both provide opportunities for recovery and also make sure
how anticipated changes in stressors interact with current levels of that employees are aware of when they will have recovery so that
stressors to impact emotional well-being. One of the reasons for they will look forward to it. They can do this by ensuring employ-
this limited research may have been a lack of theoretical frame- ees that the high workload period is temporary and communicating
work in which to understand these types of effects. In the current to employees when and how they can expect a workload decrease
studies, we extend Hobfoll’s (1989) COR theory to address antic- (e.g., by redistributing tasks) in the future. Of course, some work-
ipated change in stressors and the role anticipated change plays in load changes may be more permanent and ongoing in nature, such
shaping reactions to current stressors. The COR theory specifies as those posed by organizational restructuring or downsizings
three phenomena that induce stress: (a) actual resource loss, (b) (Green, 2004). Our results support Green’s (2004) discussion that
threat of future resource loss, and (c) lack of resource recovery continued intensification may have detrimental consequences for
following resource investment. Building from the COR theory, we well-being to the extent to which job intensification reflects both
framed threat of resource loss and perceived opportunity for re- high workload and high anticipated workload.
covery as future-oriented phenomena distinct from current re-
source loss. We advance the COR theory by hypothesizing and
Limitations and Future Research Opportunities
identifying an interaction between constructs representing these
phenomena. This interaction effect was not explicitly theorized by As with any study, this study has several limitations that should
Hobfoll (1989). However, our study provides evidence that current be acknowledged and addressed in future research. A limitation of
LOOKING FORWARD 7

this study is the lack of clarity regarding how well the study’s tant to note that the present study did not examine the perceived
findings would generalize to other occupations because the sample duration of anticipated changes, and the participants in the present
comes from a single company in a specific field. Employees in the study expected frequent workload changes. Although frequent and
present study were in market research, a field in which workload abrupt changes may be the norm for many jobs, some macrolevel
changes are common and expected. Because employees in this research (Green & McIntosh, 2001) has also suggested that work-
field are commonly engaged in project-based work, it is possible load in certain jobs and industries has increased gradually over
that they are better equipped to cope with workload changes than time. These changes are presumed to be permanent rather than
employees in occupations in which workload changes are less temporary. Some occupational stress theory suggests that the per-
frequent (Schneider, 1987). Future research might examine these ceived duration of negative changes is an important determinant of
hypotheses in other occupations and across various timeframes. stress (Beehr, 1998; Beehr & Bhagat, 1985). Longitudinal research
These job-level contextual factors may influence expectations re- would also be useful to examine the impact of anticipated duration
garding workload change, and these employees with different of workload changes as well as the consequences of mispredictions
expectations may react differently whenever workload changes do (e.g., anticipated increase but workload decreased; anticipated
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

occur. The COR theory does not make specific propositions about decrease but workload increased) on strain.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

how these contextual factors may influence perceptions of future


resource loss or opportunities for recovery, so more theoretical and Concluding Remarks
empirical development is needed in this area.
Some limitations relate to the nature of the data. In particular, Our study highlights the importance of considering future-
missingness presents a potential challenge to our data. Although oriented cognition in occupational stress research. Although re-
missingness did not appear to be driven by workload or anticipated searchers have applied the COR theory to occupational stress
change in workload, there was some evidence that emotional strain research in the past, we advanced the COR theory by mapping
influenced whether individuals responded to surveys. Newman some of its key propositions onto the concept of anticipated
(2014) discussed several challenges associated with missing data, workload change. Our study is unique in that it assessed both
including potential restriction in range, Type II errors (false neg- current perceptions of workload and anticipated changes in work-
atives) due to lower power, and concerns about generalizability. In load. When employees anticipated a workload increase, the rela-
our case, the type of missingness in the present study is best tion between workload and emotional strain was strengthened.
categorized as stochastic range restriction (Newman, 2014) be- When employees anticipated a workload decrease, however, the
cause participants with high emotional strain during their first relation between workload and emotional strain was buffered. Our
survey were slightly less likely to respond to subsequent surveys, results provide support for idea that employees are more likely to
although emotional strain did not predict the total number of tolerate stressful circumstances if they see the proverbial “light at
surveys an individual completed. The challenge with stochastic the end of the tunnel.”
range restriction is that it may make it more difficult to uncover
effects predicting the dependent variable, especially when pairwise References
or listwise deletion occurs. However, following Newman (2014),
Alarcon, G. M. (2011). A meta-analysis of burnout with job demands,
we used maximum likelihood estimation to mitigate any potential resources, and attitudes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 79, 549 –562.
bias. Still, we encourage future research to replicate this work in http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2011.03.007
both field and laboratory settings and to examine the extent to Aspinwall, L. G. (2005). The psychology of future-oriented thinking: From
which our findings generalize to other industries and types of jobs. achievement to proactive coping, adaptation, and aging. Motivation and
These results are based on self-reports, and the relationships Emotion, 29, 203–235. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11031-006-9013-1
among the key variables could be inflated slightly due to common- Atance, C. M., & O’Neill, D. K. (2001). Episodic future thinking. Trends
method bias (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, & Podsakoff, 2003). in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 533–539. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1364-
However, self-report correlations may be closer to true-score cor- 6613(00)01804-0
relations (Conway & Lance, 2010), and common-method variance Atance, C. M., & O’Neill, D. K. (2005). The emergence of episodic future
thinking in humans. Learning and Motivation, 36, 126 –144. http://dx
cannot create spurious interactions (Siemsen, Roth, & Oliveira,
.doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2005.02.003
2010). As recommended by Podsakoff et al. (2003), we took steps Beehr, T. A. (1998). An organizational psychology meta-model of occu-
to minimizing common-method bias by separating measures used pational stress. In C. L. Cooper (Ed.), Theories of organizational stress
in the present study into different survey subsections and leaving (pp. 6 –27). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press.
the primary purpose of the study ambiguous. Still, other studies Beehr, T. A., & Bhagat, R. S. (1985). Introduction to human stress and
should try to replicate these findings using objective measures of cognition in organizations. In T. A. Beehr & R. S. Bhagat (Eds.), Human
constructs when appropriate (e.g., measuring hours worked as a stress and cognition in organizations: An integrated perspective (pp.
proxy for current workload). Future studies could also extend our 3–19). New York, NY: Wiley.
findings by exploring the effects of workload and anticipated Benson, W. L., Probst, T. M., Jiang, L., Olson, K. J., & Graso, M. (2017).
workload change on physiological indicators of stress-related out- Insecurity in the ivory tower: Direct and indirect effects of pay stagna-
tion and job insecurity on faculty performance. Economic and Industrial
come variables in place of self-reported emotional strain. These
Democracy. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
objective indicators also have weaknesses (Spector, 2006) but will 0143831X17734297
be able to complement our understanding of the phenomena ex- Bowling, N. A., & Kirkendall, C. (2012). Workload: A review of causes,
amined in the present study. consequences, and potential interventions. In J. Houdmont, S. Leka, &
Finally, longitudinal work may be useful to uncover the effects R. R. Sinclair (Eds.), Contemporary occupational health psychology:
of other aspects of predictions about future workload. It is impor- Global perspectives on research and practice (Vol. 2, pp. 221–238).
8 DISTASO AND SHOSS

Chichester, United Kingdom: Wiley-Blackwell. http://dx.doi.org/10 Green, F. (2001). It’s been a hard day’s night: The concentration and
.1002/9781119942849.ch13 intensification of work in late twentieth-century Britain. British Journal
Caplan, R. D., Cobb, S., French, J. R. P., Jr., Harrison, R., & Pinneau, S. of Industrial Relations, 39, 53– 80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8543
(1980). Job demands and worker health: Main effects and occupational .00189
differences. Ann Arbor, MI: Institute for Social Research. Green, F. (2004). Work intensification, discretion, and the decline in
Caplan, R. D., & Jones, K. W. (1975). Effects of work load, role ambiguity, well-being at work. Eastern Economic Journal, 30, 615– 625.
and type a personality on anxiety, depression, and heart rate. Journal of Green, F., & McIntosh, S. (2001). The intensification of work in Europe.
Applied Psychology, 60, 713–719. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010 Labour Economics, 8, 291–308. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0927-
.60.6.713 5371(01)00027-6
Casper, A., & Sonnentag, S. (2020). Feeling exhausted or vigorous in Harvey, S. B., Modini, M., Joyce, S., Milligan-Saville, J. S., Tan, L.,
anticipation of high workload? The role of worry and planning during Mykletun, A., . . . Mitchell, P. B. (2017). Can work make you mentally
the evening. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, ill? A systematic meta-review of work-related risk factors for common
93, 215–242. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/joop.12290 mental health problems. Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 74,
Casper, A., Sonnentag, S., & Tremmel, S. (2017). Mindset matters: The 301–310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/oemed-2016-104015
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

role of employees’ stress mindset for day-specific reactions to workload Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources. A new attempt at con-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

anticipation. European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, ceptualizing stress. American Psychologist, 44, 513–524. http://dx.doi
26, 798 – 810. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1359432X.2017.1374947 .org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.3.513
Cheng, T., Mauno, S., & Lee, C. (2014). The buffering effect of coping Hobfoll, S. E. (2001). The influence of culture, community, and the
strategies in the relationship between job insecurity and employee well- nested-self in the stress process: Advancing conservation of resources
being. Economic and Industrial Democracy, 35, 71–94. http://dx.doi theory. Applied Psychology, 50, 337– 421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
.org/10.1177/0143831X12463170 1464-0597.00062
Conway, J. M., & Lance, C. E. (2010). What reviewers should expect from Ilies, R., Dimotakis, N., & De Pater, I. E. (2010). Psychological and
authors regarding common method bias in organizational research. Jour- physiological reactions to high workloads: Implications for well-being.
nal of Business and Psychology, 25, 325–334. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/ Personnel Psychology, 63, 407– 436. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-
s10869-010-9181-6 6570.2010.01175.x
De Cuyper, N., Bernhard-Oettel, C., Berntson, E., De Witte, H., & Alarco, Ilies, R., Schwind, K. M., Wagner, D. T., Johnson, M. D., DeRue, D. S.,
B. (2008). Employability and employees’ well-being: Mediation by job & Ilgen, D. R. (2007). When can employees have a family life? The
insecurity. Applied Psychology, 57, 488 –509. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ effects of daily workload and affect on work-family conflict and social
j.1464-0597.2008.00332.x behaviors at home. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1368 –1379.
De Cuyper, N., Mäkikangas, A., Kinnunen, U., Mauno, S., & Witte, H. D. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.92.5.1368
(2012). Cross-lagged associations between perceived external employ- Jiang, L., & Probst, T. M. (2014). Organizational communication: A buffer
ability, job insecurity, and exhaustion: Testing gain and loss spirals in times of job insecurity? Economic and Industrial Democracy, 35,
according to the conservation of resources theory. Journal of Organi- 557–579. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0143831X13489356
zational Behavior, 33, 770 –788. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/job.1800 Jiang, L., & Probst, T. M. (2017). The rich get richer and the poor get
Demerouti, E., Bakker, A. B., Nachreiner, F., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2001). poorer: Country- and state-level income inequality moderates the job
The job demands-resources model of burnout. Journal of Applied Psy- insecurity-burnout relationship. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102,
chology, 86, 499 –512. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.499 672– 681. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/apl0000179
De Witte, H. (1999). Job insecurity and psychological well-being: Review Karasek, R. A., Jr. (1979). Job demands, job decision latitude, and mental
of the literature and exploration of some unresolved issues. European strain: Implications for job redesign. Administrative Science Quarterly,
Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 8, 155–177. http://dx 24, 285–308. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2392498
.doi.org/10.1080/135943299398302 Krischer, M. M., Penney, L. M., & Hunter, E. M. (2010). Can counterpro-
De Witte, H., De Cuyper, N., Handaja, Y., Sverke, M., Näswall, K., & ductive work behaviors be productive? CWB as emotion-focused cop-
Hellgren, J. (2010). Associations between qualitative and quantitative ing. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 15, 154 –166. http://
job insecurity and well-being. A test in Belgian banks. International dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0018349
Studies of Management and Organization, 40, 40 –56. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New
Eden, D. (1990). Acute and chronic job stress, strain, and vacation relief. York, NY: Springer publishing company.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 45, 175–193. Lee, R. T., & Ashforth, B. E. (1996). A meta-analytic examination of the
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(90)90010-7 correlates of the three dimensions of job burnout. Journal of Applied
Epstein, S., & Roupenian, A. (1970). Heart rate and skin conductance Psychology, 81, 123–133. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.81.2.123
during experimentally induced anxiety: The effect of uncertainty about Mariani, M. (1999). Replace with a database: OⴱNET replaces the Dictionary
receiving a noxious stimulus. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- of Occupational Titles. Occupational Outlook Quarterly, 43, 2–9. Retrieved
chology, 16, 20 –28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0029786 from: https://www.bls.gov/careeroutlook/2001/Fall/art04.pdf
Franke, F. (2015). Is work intensification extra stress? Journal of Person- Newman, D. A. (2009). Missing data techniques and low response rates:
nel Psychology, 14, 17–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1866-5888/ The role of systematic nonresponse parameters. In C. E. Lance & R. J.
a000120 Vandenberg (Eds.), Statistical and methodological myths and urban
Gaab, J., Rohleder, N., Nater, U. M., & Ehlert, U. (2005). Psychological legends: Doctrine, verity and fable in the organizational and social
determinants of the cortisol stress response: The role of anticipatory sciences (pp. 7–36). New York, NY: Routledge.
cognitive appraisal. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 599 – 610. http://dx Newman, D. A. (2014). Missing data: Five practical guidelines. Organi-
.doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.02.001 zational Research Methods, 17, 372– 411. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
Grech, M. R., Neal, A., Yeo, G., Humphreys, M., & Smith, S. (2009). An 1094428114548590
examination of the relationship between workload and fatigue within Peacock, E. J., & Wong, P. T. P. (1996). Anticipatory stress: The relation
and across consecutive days of work: Is the relationship static or dy- of locus of control, optimism, and control appraisals to coping. Journal
namic? Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 14, 231–242. http:// of Research in Personality, 30, 204 –222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jrpe
dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014952 .1996.0014
LOOKING FORWARD 9

Podsakoff, N. P., LePine, J. A., & LePine, M. A. (2007). Differential nizational Research Methods, 13, 456 – 476. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
challenge stressor-hindrance stressor relationships with job attitudes, 1094428109351241
turnover intentions, turnover, and withdrawal behavior: A meta-analysis. Snijders, T. A. B., & Bosker, R. J. (2012). Multilevel analysis: An intro-
Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 438 – 454. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/ duction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling (2nd ed.). Thousand
0021-9010.92.2.438 Oaks, CA: Sage.
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. -Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Sonnentag, S., Kuttler, I., & Fritz, C. (2010). Job stressors, emotional
Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the exhaustion, and need for recovery: A multi-source study on the benefits
literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, of psychological detachment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 76, 355–
88, 879 –903. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879 365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2009.06.005
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1987). Dispositional optimism and Spector, P. E. (2006). Method variance in organizational research: Truth or
physical well-being: The influence of generalized outcome expectancies urban legend? Organizational Research Methods, 9, 221–232. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1177/1094428105284955
on health. Journal of Personality, 55, 169 –210. http://dx.doi.org/10
Spector, P. E., & Jex, S. M. (1998). Development of four self-report
.1111/j.1467-6494.1987.tb00434.x
measures of job stressors and strain: Interpersonal Conflict at Work
Scheier, M. F., Weintraub, J. K., & Carver, C. S. (1986). Coping with
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Scale, Organizational Constraints Scale, Quantitative Workload Inven-


stress: Divergent strategies of optimists and pessimists. Journal of Per-
tory, and Physical Symptoms Inventory. Journal of Occupational Health
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sonality and Social Psychology, 51, 1257–1264. http://dx.doi.org/10


Psychology, 3, 356 –367. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.3.4.356
.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1257 Ünal-Karagüven, M. H. (2009). Psychological impact of an economic
Schneider, B. (1987). The people make the place. Personnel Psychology, crisis: A conservation of resources approach. International Journal of
40, 437– 453. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.1987.tb00609.x Stress Management, 16, 177–194. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0016840
Shoss, M. K. (2017). Job insecurity: An integrative review and agenda for Waugh, C. E., Panage, S., Mendes, W. B., & Gotlib, I. H. (2010). Cardio-
future research. Journal of Management, 43, 1911–1939. http://dx.doi vascular and affective recovery from anticipatory threat. Biological
.org/10.1177/0149206317691574 Psychology, 84, 169 –175. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010
Shoss, M. K., Brummel, B. J., Probst, T. M., & Jiang, L. (2020). The joint .01.010
importance of secure and satisfying work: Insights from three studies.
Journal of Business and Psychology, 35, 279 –316. Received August 1, 2019
Siemsen, E., Roth, A., & Oliveira, P. (2010). Common method bias in Revision received June 19, 2020
regression models with linear, quadratic, and interaction effects. Orga- Accepted July 14, 2020 䡲

You might also like