You are on page 1of 29

International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

A review of human error in aviation maintenance


and inspection
Kara A. Latorella *, Prasad V. Prabhu 
Department of Industrial Engineering, State University of New York at Buwalo, Buwalo, NY 14260, USA
Galaxy Scientixc Corporation, 2310 Parklake Dr. Atlanta, GA 30345, USA
Received 1 June 1997

Abstract

Aviation safety depends on minimizing error in all facets of the system. While the role of #ightdeck human error has
received much emphasis, recently more attention has been directed toward reducing human error in maintenance and
inspection. Aviation maintenance and inspection tasks are part of a complex organization, where individuals perform
varied tasks in an environment with time pressures, sparse feedback, and sometimes di$cult ambient conditions. These
situational characteristics, in combination with generic human erring tendencies, result in varied forms of error. The most
severe result in accidents and loss of life. For example, failure to replace horizontal stabilizer screws on a Continental
Express aircraft resulted in in-#ight leading-edge separation and 14 fatalities. While errors resulting in accidents are most
salient, maintenance and inspection errors have other important consequences (e.g., air turn-backs, delays in aircraft
availability, gate returns, diversions to alternate airports) which impede productivity and e$ciency of airline operations,
and inconvenience the #ying public. This paper reviews current approaches to identifying, reporting, and managing
human error in aviation maintenance and inspection. As foundation for this discussion, we provide an overview of
approaches to investigating human error, and a description of aviation maintenance and inspection tasks and environ-
mental characteristics.
Relevance to industry
Following an introductory description of its tasks and environmental characteristics, this paper reviews methods and
tools for identifying, reporting, and managing human error in aviation maintenance and inspection.  2000 Elsevier
Science B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Aviation; Maintenance; Inspection; Human factors; Human error

1. Introduction
* Corresponding author. Present address: NASA Langley Re-
search Center, Crew/vehicle Integration Branch, M/S 152, Ham- In the opening remarks of the 1995 FAA Avi-
pton, VA 23681-0001, USA. Tel.: #1-757-864-2030; fax: #1- ation Safety conference, US Secretary of Trans-
757-864-7793.
E-mail address: k.a.latorella@larc.nasa.gov (K.A. Latorell). portation, Federico Pen a, challenged the airline
 Present address: The Eastman Kodak Company, 901 Elm- industry to meet the goal of zero accidents. Given
grove Rd., Rochester, NY 14653, USA. the complexity of the aviation system, this goal is

0169-8141/00/$ - see front matter  2000 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 1 6 9 - 8 1 4 1 ( 9 9 ) 0 0 0 6 3 - 3
134 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

ambitious. Trends in the airline industry and the incidence and consequences of human error in
aviation environment exacerbate the di$culty of this domain. We review current e!orts towards de-
achieving this goal. Maintenance costs, passenger tecting, reporting, and managing human errors
miles #own, and the number of aircraft have in aviation maintenance. Finally, we conclude by
all exceeded the overall growth of the aviation discussing future directions for reducing the
maintenance technician (AMT) work force (Air incidence and mitigating the e!ects of human error
Transport Association, 1994). Also, as the US in aviation maintenance and, thereby, improving
commercial aviation #eet ages, aircraft require aviation safety and e$ciency of aviation operations.
more inspection and maintenance. The concurrent
trends of increased maintenance and inspection
workload, and decreased work force seem to fore- 2. Human error
cast increasing safety issues associated with human
errors in maintenance and inspection. Understanding the role of human error in an
Fortunately, technological advances have buf- accident or incident is fundamentally di!erent from
fered, to some degree, the e!ects of these trends. simply attributing such an event to an inherently
Fail-safe systems, improved hardware, better soft- fallible human operator. Human error has been
ware design, better maintenance equipment and variously characterized as: any member of a set of
methods, and other technological advancements, human actions that exceeds some limit of accepta-
have improved safety and, in some ways, reduced bility (Swain and Guttman, 1983), any human ac-
maintenance and inspection workload. While it is tion or inaction that exceeds the tolerances de"ned
tempting to think of such technological advance- by the system with which the human interacts
ments as necessarily improvements to overall (Lorenzo, 1990), the failure to achieve an intended
safety, one must consider that innovations also outcome beyond the in#uence of random occurren-
require the humans in the system to acquire new ce (Reason, 1990), a necessary outcome to allow
skills and knowledge, and may induce additional humans to explore and understand systems (Table 1)
opportunities for human error. The focus of im- (Rasmussen, 1990; Reason, 1990), and derivative of
proving aviation maintenance and inspection has operators' social experience of responsibility and
been traditionally to improve the technology used values (Taylor, 1987). These de"nitions convey the
in these tasks. Because this focus introduced addi- multifaceted nature of human error. Principally,
tional human error concerns, more recent attempts however, they suggest the two complementary pro-
to improve aviation safety have focused on reduc- posals that: (1) human operators are organic mech-
ing inspector and repair personnel error (Reason anisms with failure rates and tolerances analogous
and Maddox, 1995). Managing human errors has to hardware/software elements of a system, (2) that
become a critical aspect of the aviation industries human error is a pejorative term for normal human
drive towards increasing the safety and reliability of behavior in often unkind environments, where only
the commercial aviation system. As evidence of this the outcome determines if this behavior is deleteri-
focus, human error was a major theme in the air- ous. These two perspectives are also re#ected in the
craft maintenance and inspection workshop held two major approaches developed to address human
during the 1995 National Aviation Safety confer- error in accident and incident analyses: human
ence (FAA, 1995). reliability assessment (HRA) and human error classi-
While the generics of human error or genotypes "cations (Kirwan, 1992a). Summarized below, HRA
(Hollnagel, 1991) are essentially constant across methods and human error classi"cations are more
work domains, the speci"cs or phenotypes (Holl- fully reviewed and contrasted by Kirwan (1992a,b).
nagel, 1991) are in#uenced by characteristics of the
task and environmental context. This paper reviews 2.1. Human reliability analysis
general approaches to the study of human error
and the characteristics of work in aviation mainten- The HRA approach is an extension of proba-
ance as a foundation for describing the nature, bilistic risk assessment (PRA). Probabilistic risk
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 135

Table 1 from either actual observation or simulation tech-


Multifaceted human error taxonomy (adapted from Rasmussen, niques. In addition to more traditional simulations
1982) (Seigel et al., 1975), simulation approaches have
Factors A!ecting Performance been developed which computationally represent
Subjective goals and intentions a dynamic model of the human operator, tasks, and
Mental load, resources external situational factors (e.g., Woods et al., 1987;
A!ective factors Cacciabue et al., 1993). Extensions of traditional
Situation Factors HRA recognize the importance of considering the
Task characteristics
Physical environment in#uence of environmental characteristics on the
Work time characteristics propensity for human errors and have included
Causes of Human Malfunction these `performance shaping factorsa (PSFs) in cal-
External events (distraction, etc.) culations of HEPs (Kirwan, 1992a) and in rich
Excessive task demand (force, time, knowledge, etc.) simulations of the environment. One disadvantage
Operator incapacitation (sickness, etc.)
Intrinsic human variability of the HRA approach, is that it requires that the
Mechanisms of Human Malfunction system exists in order to observe these error modes
Discrimination and collect data on error rates. As such, this
Input information processing method is of limited use in designing safe systems
Recall from inception. Simulation approaches provide the
Inference
Physical coordination advantage of real-time generation of (simulated)
Personnel Task human errors in response to (simulated) external
Equipment/procedure design, installation, inspection, etc. conditions and events, and thereby avail predictive
Internal Human Malfunction information by changing simulated conditions.
Detection
Identi"cation
Decision 2.2. Human error classixcations
Action
External Mode of Malfunction The second major approach to investigating hu-
Speci"ed task not performed man error is more qualitative; that is to classify
Commission of erroneous act types of human errors. It is beyond the scope of this
Commission of extraneous act
Accidentally coincidental events (sneak path) paper to review the spectrum of human error classi-
"cation systems (see Reason, 1990; Woods et al.,
1995) for a more detailed treatment of human
error. Rather, this section describes three basic
assessments identify all risks (including human er- forms that these system may take, provides repre-
ror) that a system is exposed to, describe associ- sentative examples, and concludes by emphasiz-
ations among these risks, quantify risk likelihood, ing the need for a holistic approach to classifying
and express this information in a fault tree or event human error.
tree representation. Human reliability analyses Human error classi"cation schemes have been
provide more detailed assessment of the human- described as behavioral, contextual, or conceptual
related risks inherent in systems. Human reliability in nature (Reason, 1990). Behavioral classi"cations
analyses identify human errors as the failure to describe human errors in terms of easily observed
perform an action, failure to perform an action surface features. Behavioral classi"cations partition
within the safe operating limits (e.g., time, accu- human errors on such dimensions as their formal
racy), or performance of an extraneous act which characteristics (omission, commission, extraneous)
degrades system performance. Human error prob- (e.g., Swain and Guttman, 1983), immediate conse-
abilities are then de"ned for each identi"ed error as quences (nature and extent of injury or damage),
the ratio of the number of errors occurring in a cer- observability of consequences (active/immediate vs.
tain interval, to the number of opportunities for latent/delayed) (Reason and Maddox, 1995), degree
occurrence. Human error probabilities may derive of recoverability, and responsible party. These
136 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

classi"cations provide no mapping of surface char- (Quintanilla, 1987). Reason (1990) classi"es human
acteristics to causal mechanisms. Contextual classi- errors based on social values to distinguish be-
"cations begin to address causality by associating tween, strictly, errors and violations. Errors are, as
human errors with characteristics of the envir- described above, unwitting deviations. Violations
onmental and task context. These classi"cation are deviations from operating procedures, recom-
systems are valuable because they emphasize the mended practices, rules or standards that are delib-
complex interactions among system components, erate. Reason (1990) also distinguishes between
including human operators, and generally result in violations and sabotage. Violators intend the devi-
richer data collection of circumstances surrounding ating acts, but not their potential for bad conse-
incidents and accidents. However, contextual clas- quences. Saboteurs, however, intend both the
si"cations are really correlational and not necessar- deviating act and its bad consequences. Violations
ily indicative of causal relationships. Further, these are shaped by three inter-related social factors:
correlational classi"cations cannot, alone, explain behavioral attitudes, subjective norms, and behav-
why similar environmental circumstances do not ioral control (Reason and Maddox, 1995). Oper-
deterministically produce repeatable errors (Rea- ators who commit attitudinal violations, do so
son, 1990). simply because they know they can; and conscious-
Conceptual classi"cation systems attempt to es- ly weigh perceived advantages against possible
tablish causality in terms of more fundamental, and penalties or risks. Operators who are concerned
likely predictable, characteristics of human behav- with the perceptions of subjective norms, may com-
ior. Norman's (1981) distinctions between slips and mit violations because they seek the approval of
mistakes is perhaps the simplest classi"cation others. Finally, operators may not be able to exer-
scheme in this category. Slips are failures in execut- cise the behavioral control to not commit a viola-
ing the correct intention. Mistakes result from mis- tion. Other organizational factors which contribute
taken intentions. More elaborate classi"cations to human error relate to the manner in which
typically begin with a model of human information groups of individuals interact (e.g., Reason, 1987).
processing and de"ne error types based on the These classi"cation schemes characterize various
failure modes of information processing stages or aspects of human errors. To fully address the causes
mechanisms (e.g., Reason, 1990; Rouse and Rouse, and e!ects of human error, however, a more holistic
1983). Categories of systematic error mechanisms approach is required. Rasmussen (1982) emphasizes
(e!ects of learning, interference among competing this need to place errors in a rich context. His
control structures, lack of resources, and stochastic taxonomy (Table 1) considers not only mechanisms
variability) have been identi"ed for each of three of human information processing malfunction, but
levels of cognitive control (skill, rule, knowledge) also the task, situation factors (task, physical, and
(Rasmussen and Vicente, 1989). Prabhu et al. work time characteristics), operator a!ect and in-
(1992b) organize error shaping factors by these tentions, and ultimately, the external expression of
same levels of cognitive control. Although most the error.
useful for establishing causality and for predicting
human error, conceptual classi"cations rely on the- 2.3. Responding to human error
oretical inferences rather than observable data and
are therefore more open to argument. These human Perhaps the single most important contribution
error classi"cations address the error-proneness of of human error investigation methods, both HRA
information processing mechanisms of an indi- and human error classi"cations, is to emphasize
vidual operator. that the goal of such investigations is not to at-
A smaller contingent cautions that such e!orts tribute blame. Rather, errors are traced beyond the
may drive human error research to be `individual- operator who committed it to identify predisposing
istically myopica (Quintanilla, 1987). That is, characteristics of the environment and task. Typi-
to stress the cognition of the individual to cally, accident investigations back track until
the exclusion of social and organizational factors a cause is identi"ed (Rasmussen, 1985). Because it is
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 137

usually possible to identify cases where an oper- assumptions for system design. Such design deci-
ator's failure to take compensatory actions allows sions reduce the probability of system-induced hu-
a developing failure to become expressed, these man errors, those errors that can be traced back to
failures are often attributed to human error. Such particular con"gurations of the human}machine
naive attributions of blame to the generic fallibility system (e.g., the interface design, the task design).
of human operators often halt accident investiga- However, Hollnagel (1990) suggests that system
tions prematurely, obviating the opportunity to designers often overlook the fact that human capa-
identify other causal or performance-shaping fac- city is variable and the actual variance could be
tors and develop interventions (Woods et al., 1995; larger than the expected variance in many situ-
Reason, 1990; Lorenzo, 1990). Reason (1990) sug- ations. In other words, there is a category of errors
gests that in order to break out of this `blame that result from the inherent variability of human
cyclea we must recognize that: (1) human perfor- cognition. One approach to addressing this prob-
mance is shaped by situational and environmental lem would be to reduce the requirements for perfor-
contexts, (2) simply instructing someone to not take mance until they were met by a preponderance of
an action they did not intend to take in the "rst the situations. However, this could mean that the
place is not very e!ective, (3) errors result from system would perform below capacity in the major-
multiple contributing factors, and (4) the relevant ity of the cases and the human operator might be
characteristics of situations and environments are underloaded. An alternate approach would be to
usually easier to alter than the relevant character- design the system with error tolerance. Hollnagel
istics of operators (Allen and Rankin, 1995). (1990) proposes that an error tolerant system
Of course the most obvious response to a human would:
error is to identify the causal mechanisms and alter
the system such that that error is not repeatable. E provide user with appropriate information at the
Unfortunately this requires a sophisticated error- appropriate time to minimize the opportunity
detection system, capable of identifying complex for system induced erroneous actions,
interactions, and the impractical assumption that E compensate for human perceptual dysfunction
human variability is minimal. Some would argue by providing information in redundant and sim-
that even if eliminating all human error were pos- pli"ed forms,
sible, it may not be desirable. Rasmussen (1990) E compensate for human motor (and cognitive)
argues that what we call errors are unavoidable side dysfunction by maintaining the integrity of input
e!ects of operators' exploration of the boundaries of data (through anticipation and context-depen-
acceptable performance. He contends that errors, or dent interpretation),
near errors, serve a valuable purpose in developing E contain provisions for detecting erroneous ac-
and maintaining operator expertise. Along similar tions and for instigating corrective procedures,
lines, Senders and Moray (1991) suggest that elimin- E allow easy correction and recovery of erroneous
ating the opportunity for error severely limits the actions by providing a forgiving environment.
range of possible behavior and thus inhibits trial and Similarly, Rasmussen and Vicente (1989) provide
error learning, and reduces the #exibility of human guidelines for designing system interfaces that tol-
operators. They argue that the key is to reduce the erate human error:
undesirable consequences of the error, and not ne-
cessarily the error itself. They therefore postulate E Make limits of acceptable performance visible
the concept of an error-forgiving design, rather while still reversible.
than an error-free design as a goal. E Provide feedback on the e!ects of actions to help
Hollnagel (1990) also argues that error tolerant cope with time delays.
system designs are necessary. He points out that E Make latent conditional constraints on actions
knowledge about the limitations of human capacity visible.
(e.g., with regard to perception, attention, discrim- E Make cues for actions, and represent necessary
ination, memory) is used while making reasonable preconditions for their validity.
138 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

E Supply operators with tools to perform experi- inspection and maintenance tasks (e.g., graphics
ments and test hypotheses. and procedures in work cards, reference materials,
E Integrate cues for action. defect reporting forms), but also those necessary to
E Support memory by externalizing e!ective men- coordinate inspection and maintenance activities
tal models. (e.g., shift turnover forms). The physical environment
E Present information at the most appropriate is de"ned by parameters such as temperature; noise
level for decision making. level and type; lighting level, color, and distribu-
E Present information embedded in a structure tion; and the presence of potential physical and
that serves as an externalized mental model. chemical hazards to operators. For example, pre-
E Support memory of items, acts, and data that are cautions must be exercised to ensure that personnel
not integrated into the task. are not exposed to radiation during X-ray NDT
inspections, or to excessive fumes when inspecting
Reason (1990) suggests that systems could be inside a fuel tank. The physical environment in-
designed to minimize violations by changing the cludes not only these ambient characteristics but
organizational culture and social norms, and indi- also characteristics at the workspace, such as the
vidual beliefs/values. adequacy of task-lighting provided by a #ashlight
(Reynolds et al., 1993). The organizational environ-
ment is equally important and is receiving increased
3. Aviation maintenance tasks and environments attention in aviation maintenance. The following
sections more fully characterize aviation mainten-
Aviation maintenance and inspection tasks oc- ance and inspection tasks and the surrounding or-
cur within the larger context of organizational and ganizational setting.
physical environmental factors. A system model of
aviation maintenance and inspection (Latorella 3.1. Aviation maintenance and inspection tasks
and Drury, 1992) de"nes four interacting com-
ponents in this system (operators, equipment, The typical de"nition of human error in main-
documentation, and task) and suggests that these tenance and inspection refers to the activities of the
components interact over time as well as within inspector or repair person. Drury (1996) describes
both physical and social, or organizational, envi- the functions at this level of the system as (1) plan-
ronments. In addition to considerations of the tasks ning, (2) opening/cleaning, (3) inspection, (4) repair,
performed, the system model emphasizes the inter- and (5) buy-back. Initially, a team including the
actions among operators, interactions of operators FAA, the aircraft manufacturer, and start-up oper-
with equipment and documentation used, and the ators, de"nes maintenance and inspection proced-
physical and job environment in which these tasks ures for commercial aviation airlines.
occur. Operator classi"cations include inspection Work items are de"ned by predictive models of
and repair personnel at various organizational equipment and material wear, and are informed by
levels (line operators, lead operators, foreman level prior observations, as well as incidents and acci-
operators) as well as production foremen and en- dent investigations. Airlines then de"ne actual
gineers. The equipment used in inspection and schedules in a process that must meet legal require-
maintenance tasks ranges from common tools (e.g., ments (Shepherd et al., 1991). This process requires
#ashlights, mirrors, rulers) to more elaborate considering interference between inspection and re-
equipment requiring specialized training, such as pair tasks due to required access, equipment and/or
that required for non-destructive testing/inspection personnel constraints, in an e!ort to minimize total
(NDT/NDI) (e.g., eddy-current, magnetic reson- aircraft out-of-service time. There are typically four
ance, dye-penetration techniques). The documenta- types of inspection/maintenance checks. These
tion, or more broadly, the information environment, range in the degree of inspection and maintenance
used in inspection and maintenance includes not work from the least detailed (#ight line checks
only those required and used to perform speci"c and A-checks) to the heaviest level (D-checks). The
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 139

result of this planning function results in packaging determine if they are faults or not. Faults identi"ed
maintenance and inspection items into a check, and during search are recorded for repair and buy-back
generating work cards which specify these items inspection. Table 2 provides an example of these
and procedures for accomplishing their inspec- inspection functions for both visual and eddy-cur-
tion/repair/replacement. rent inspection (Drury, 1996).
The opening/cleaning function prepares the in- The repair function also begins with a work card.
spection/repair area. Prior to inspection, the area Repair work cards specify the repair job, procedure
must be cleaned and devoid of any oil, hydraulic for repair, and note additional reference materials
#uid, or other visual interference. Typically, access required. The repair function can be decomposed
panels are removed and internal cleaning must into sub-functions similar to the inspection sub-
be performed. Cleaning and area preparation are functions: (1) initiate, (2) site access, (3) part access,
usually performed by di!erent personnel than (4) diagnosis, (5) replace/repair, (6) reset systems, (7)
inspection and repair operations. Inspection can close access (Drury, 1996). Repair begins with ac-
be performed either visually (also using tactile and cess of the appropriate work card, equipment, and
auditory cues) or using non-destructive testing parts for the repair. The repair person then locates
methods, (e.g., eddy-current, ultrasonic, magnetic the site of the repair and removes any additional
resonance, X-ray, and dye penetration) (Latia, parts to access the element requiring repair. Re-
1993), which provide an abstracted or enhanced moved items are inspected and stored. Technicians
signal for visual interpretation. Regardless of the perform diagnostic procedures speci"ed by the
method, the inspection function includes the fol- work card and determine whether to repair or
lowing sub-functions: (1) initiate, (2) access, (3) replace the target element. Once this determination
search, (4) decide, (5) respond (Drury, 1996,1994; is made, technicians may need to obtain additional
Drury et al., 1990). Inspection begins with an in- parts before actually repairing or replacing the ele-
spector obtaining a work card and any equipment ment. After the repair/replacement, the relevant
required for the job it speci"es. After obtaining this systems are reset, #uid levels are restored, and the
equipment and understanding the requirements of system adjusted to speci"cation. The repair func-
the work card, the inspector locates the inspection tion concludes by closing the access to the target
site. Inspection requires searching the target area area and making "nal adjustments. Table 3 shows
either visually or with the appropriate equipment a decomposition of the repair function (Drury,
until all items are addressed or the entire area is 1996). Repairs may be performed on the aircraft
searched. As indications appear, inspectors must itself, or as a sub-component process o!-line.

Table 2
Examples of inspection functions: Visual and eddy-current (Drury, 1996)

Function Visual inspection Eddy-current inspection

Initiate Get work card Get work card and equipment


Read and understand requirements Read and understand requirements
Calibrate eddy-current equipment
Access Locate area on aircraft Locate area on aircraft
Assume correct position for viewing Position equipment
Search View area systematically Move probe over rivet heads systematically
Stop if any indication detected Stop if any indication detected
Decide Compare indication against remembered standards Re-probe while closely watching signal on equipment
(e.g., for corrosion) monitor
Respond If indication exceeds standards, mark defect and If indication con"rmed, mark defect and create repair sheet
create repair sheet
Else, continue searching Else, continue searching
140 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

Table 3 error as the inspection and repair functions them-


Generic repair functions and tasks (Drury, 1996) selves.
At a higher level, the planning function translates
Function Tasks
organizational requirements (i.e., those imposed by
Initiate Read and understand work card regulatory agencies and manufacturers) into re-
Prepare tools and equipment quirements for airline carriers, and consequently
Collect parts and supplies translates these requirements into a schedule of
Inspect parts and supplies local activities for inspection or repair personnel.
Site Access Bring parts, supplies, tools and equipment to
work site Another function of the organizational level is to
Part Access Remove items to access parts provide quality control and assurance of the in-
Inspect/store removed items spection and maintenance processes. Quality con-
Diagnosis Follow diagnostic procedures trol in aircraft maintenance and inspection results
Determine parts to replace/repair from surveillance and auditing actions of regula-
Collect and inspect more parts and supplies
if required tory agencies (e.g., Federal Aviation Administra-
Replace/Repair Remove parts to be replaced/repaired tion (US FAA), Civil Aviation Authority (UK
Repair parts if needed CAA)), as well as quality assurance functions in the
Replace parts airline companies (Drury, 1996). Quality assurance
Reset Systems Add #uids/supplies functions include: checking engineering change or-
Adjust systems to speci"cation
Inspect adjustments ders, auditing inspection and maintenance activ-
Close Access Re"t items removed for access ities for errors, auditing components (and vendors)
Adjust items re"tted used for replacements, and examining record keep-
Remove tools, equipment, parts, and excess ing (Drury, 1996). Failures of quality assurance as
supplies well as regulatory policies/practices allow mainten-
ance and inspection human errors. These errors,
and propagating e!ects of these errors, decrease
aviation safety.
A second maintenance person, usually an inspec- In addition to these speci"c functions, general
tor, may re-inspect, or `buy-backa, a repair before organizational characteristics in#uence perfor-
the item is closed (Drury, 1996). Prior to returning mance at the individual level. Organizational fac-
an aircraft to service, all scheduled items and addi- tors (e.g., de"nition of work groups/isolation of
tional items resulting from inspection must be workers, reporting structures, payo! structures,
either certi"ed as complete or logged as deferred. and issues of trust and authority) demonstrably
Maintenance deferral is only possible in certain a!ect patterns of work in aviation maintenance
pre-de"ned situations, for items which are not operations (Taylor, 1990). More speci"cally, hu-
safety-critical (Drury, 1996). These items are treated man error in a major airline carrier's maintenance
on the next scheduled, or event-driven maintenance facility is in#uenced by characteristics such as or-
cycle. ganizational structure, people management, provis-
ion of quality tools and equipment, training and
3.2. Organizational context selection, commercial and operational pressures,
planning and scheduling, maintenance of building
The aviation maintenance and inspection system and equipment, and communications (Rogan,
includes not only the individual technicians per- 1995a).
forming the functions above, but personnel in the
organization level of the airline as well as at regula-
tory agencies, aircraft manufacturers, and compon- 4. Human error in aviation maintenance
ent vendors. The organizational context in which
aircraft maintenance and inspection occurs is The previous section brie#y outlines the func-
equally important to an understanding of human tions involved at the individual and organizational
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 141

levels of aviation maintenance and inspection. more cognitive, aspects of the inspection task (e.g.,
However, these simple descriptions do not convey making perceptual judgments) (Prabhu and Drury,
the complexity of this system. Modern aircraft and 1992).
the systems embedded in them are increasingly Given these complexities, it is not surprising that
technologically complex. New methods for inspect- human operators in this system commit errors.
ing and diagnosing these systems are increasingly Aviation maintenance and inspection errors can be
specialized. Further, inspecting and maintaining described in terms of their most immediate, observ-
commercial aviation is organizationally complex; able e!ect on aircraft equipment, ultimate e!ects on
emerging from a socio-technic process in which #ight missions (incidents/accidents), and secondary
hundreds, even thousands, of people are directly e!ects on the airline carrier industry. Further,
involved (Taylor, 1990). forms of errors in aviation maintenance and inspec-
These conditions combine to produce a work tion are de"ned as failure modes of the tasks in-
environment that predisposes the humans working volved in their functions. The incidence and forms
in this system to err. For example, given that there of aviation maintenance and inspection human er-
are 14 di!erent kinds of attachment lock mecha- rors are described in these terms below.
nisms in a narrow body aircraft seat, the chances of
overlooking a poorly locked seat are very high 4.1. Ewects of maintenance errors on aircraft
(Lutzinger, 1992). Attempts to simultaneously ac- equipment
complish the competing goals of safety, timeliness,
and pro"t result in implicit time pressures. Organ- Several studies have identi"ed the most common,
izational/economic pressures may motivate oper- immediate e!ects of human error in aviation main-
ators to violate inspection/maintenance practices. tenance. A major airline shows the distribution of
Consequences of errors are not immediately obvi- 122 maintenance errors over a period of three years
ous (Graeber and Marx, 1993). For example, in one to be: omissions (56%), incorrect installations
accident a faulty maintenance action did not have (30%), wrong parts (8%), other (6%) (Graeber and
an observable e!ect until 17 months after it occur- Marx, 1993). A three year study by the Civilian
red (NTSB, 1990). Delayed feedback dramatically Aviation Authority (CAA) found the eight most
reduces the ability of operators to learn from errors. common maintenance errors to be: incorrect instal-
Such delays also impede accident investigation be- lation of components, the "tting of wrong parts,
cause situational factors surrounding the o!ending electrical wiring discrepancies (including cross con-
`humana error are lost. In addition, because dif- nections), loose objects (tools, etc.) left in the air-
ferent types of maintenance problems present craft, inadequate lubrication, cowlings, access
themselves randomly to individual operators, it is panels and fairings not secured, fuel caps and refuel
di$cult for any one operator to identify what may panels not secured, and landing gear ground lock
be a systematic problem in an aircraft type or pins not removed before departure (UK CAA,
mechanism (cf. Inaba and Dey, 1991). Aircraft 1992, cited in Allen and Rankin, 1995). In-#ight
maintenance often spans multiple days and mul- engine shut downs on Boeing 747's in 1991 were
tiple shifts, making coordination of activities and due to the following human errors, in order of
information amongst di!erent operators over dif- occurence frequency, (Pratt and Whitney study
ferent shifts very di$cult. Quality control audits cited in Graeber and Marx, 1993):
and inspections and error reporting systems obtain
data on inspection and repair performance. How- E missing or incorrect parts,
ever they do not typically provide consistent or E incorrect installation of parts or use of worn/
timely feedback to operators on actual errors. Fur- deteriorated parts,
ther, feedback during training for inspection tends E careless installation of O-rings,
to focus on procedural aspects of the task (e.g., E B-nuts not safety wired,
setting up NDT equipment and troubleshooting E nuts tightened but not torqued or over-torqued,
rules) rather than providing feedback for other, E seals over-torqued,
142 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

E not loosening both ends of connecting tube re- and almost 20% of all accidents that occurred
placement, between 1982 and 1991. For this period, mainten-
E replacing tube assembly without "rst breaking ance and inspection factors were implicated in 47
connections between matching parts, accidents in total, these accidents resulting in 1481
E not tightening or replacing oil-tank caps, onboard fatalities (Graeber and Marx, 1993).
E not cleaning or tightening cannon plugs, Several fairly recent accidents have been at-
E dropping foreign objects into engines, tributed, at least in part, to human error in main-
E allowing water in fuel, allowing Skydrol in oil tenance and inspection operator tasks. Failure to
system. install O-ring seals on an L-1011's engines allowed
oil to leak out, eventually resulting in two of the
Data collected at a major US airline (Prabhu and
three engines ceasing operation due to oil starva-
Drury, 1992) revealed several major categories of
tion (NTSB, 1984; Strauch and Sandler, 1984). The
human errors in maintenance and inspection tasks,
pilot had shut down the third engine earlier in the
these are:
#ight in response to a low oil indication, but was
E defective component (e.g. cracked pylon, worn able to restart this engine and successfully reached
cables, #uid leakage), the airport. Maintenance on a Continental Express
E missing component (e.g., bolt-nut not secured), EMB-120 was not adequately transferred to a sec-
E wrong component (e.g., incorrect pitot static ond shift (NTSB, 1992). Forty-seven screws on the
probes installed), left horizontal stabilizer were not replaced, causing
E incorrect con"guration (e.g., valve inserted back- the leading edge to separate in #ight and resulting
wards), in 14 fatalities (Shepherd and Johnson, 1995;
E incorrect assembly sequence (e.g., incorrect se- NTSB, 1992). Failure to detect a pre-existing metal-
quence of inner cylinder spacer and lock ring lurgical defect resulted allowed a fatigue crack to
assembly), form in a critical area of a DC-10's fan disk. Com-
E functional defects (e.g., wrong tire pressure, pounding the initial error, the resulting fatigue
over-tightening nuts), crack itself was unnoticed during inspection. In
E tactile defects (e.g., seat not locking in position), #ight, this situation resulted in separation and dis-
E procedural defects (e.g., nose landing gear door charge of the rotor assembly, catastrophic failure of
not closed). the C2 engine, consequent severing of the #ight-
control hydraulic systems, and ultimately the loss
4.2. Accidents due to maintenance errors of 111 lives and many other injuries (NTSB, 1990).
In the Aloha Airlines accident (NTSB, 1989), failure
The types of maintenance errors contributing to to detect multiple site damage resulting from
accidents range from glaring omissions that are the joining cracks resulted in hull failure and a crash
direct cause, to more minor errors which combine landing. This error caused the catastrophic loss of
with other o!-normal occurrences to create these the aircraft, and only exceptional pilot performance
accidents (Graeber and Marx, 1993). Estimates of presented numerous fatalities in this accident.
the contributions of maintenance factors to the In addition, accident investigators note the con-
incidence of aviation accidents and incidents vary. tributions of organizational factors to aviation ac-
Sears (1986 cited in Graeber and Marx, 1993) states cidents. Reason and Maddox (1995) describe the
that maintenance was a contributing factor in 12% concept of an organizational accident/incident as
of the international accidents that occurred be- one in which management decisions, emerging
tween the years 1959 and 1983 (Graeber and Marx, from the corporate culture, generate latent failures
1993), and #awed maintenance practices were the that are transmitted to the workplace where they
major factor in 3% of these cases (Boeing, 1993a). create a local climate that promotes errors and
In a more recent survey, Boeing (1993b) found that violations. These latent failures occasionally break
changes in maintenance and inspection could have through engineered safety features (e.g., design,
prevented approximately 16% of the hull losses standards, procedures) to cause an accident/incident.
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 143

For example, while the Continental Express acci- 4.3. Other ewects of human errors in aviation main-
dent described above was primarily caused by a re- tenance
pair person's error (i.e., not replacing the horizontal
stabilizer screws), the accident could not have oc- Accidents due to human error forcefully under-
curred if both the air carrier's quality assurance score the potentially dire consequences of mainten-
functions and FAA's regulatory surveillance were ance errors on aviation safety. The relative rarity of
attentive. Similarly, analysis of the Sioux City acci- these accidents, however, does not imply that main-
dent (NTSB, 1990) implicated inspection and qual- tenance errors are as rare. Accidents typically result
ity control procedures during the engine overhaul from the combination of causal factors and must
process. Although correctly performed mainten- overcome several lines of defense. In addition
ance could have prevented the Aloha Airlines acci- to those that precipitated or facilitated the above
dent, this accident was attributed to both failure of accidents, maintenance errors with no direct
inspectors to detect cracks, and failure of fatigue consequences have been detected. For example, the
models to correctly anticipate crack growth and accident investigation of the China Airlines, Flight
indicate required inspection (Drury, 1996). 583 (which became unstable after leading edge slats
For every aircraft design, routine inspections, and were deployed at cruise altitude) detected that
occasionally replacement/repairs, are required at a rubber plug was left in the maintenance position
pre-de"ned intervals to prevent failures from de- for rigging the slat control system (NTSB, 1993a).
veloping (Hagemaier, 1989). Models of crack Douglas Aircraft Company engineers stated that
propagation, based on structural and material although the plug should be removed after main-
fracture mechanics, de"ne these intervals (Goran- tenance, its presence did not a!ect the operation of
son, 1989). Drury (1996) notes that this process the slat system. So maintenance errors may exist
invites misinterpretation of failures in model that, given other defense mechanisms built into the
prediction as fundamentally human errors of in- system are in place, are not consequential and
spection or repair. Accident investigation of the therefore not usually detected. It is tempting to
TWA Flight 843 that was destroyed following an dismiss such seemingly inconsequential errors as
aborted take-o! found the precipitating event to be unimportant. They are mentioned here for two
a malfunction in an angle-of-attack (AOA) sensor reasons. First, errors which seem inconsequential
in the stall warning system. Although other factors may, in other circumstances, interact with other
were involved, the investigation report emphasizes o!-normal situations to result in a `sneak-patha
that the precipitating cause was the failure of accident. Second, even if a particular erroneous
TWA's maintenance and quality assurance trend result is not damaging in e!ect, it may indicate
monitoring programs to detect the intermittent a predisposing condition in the environment, task
AOA malfunction. or operator's knowledge that may result in an error
While the self-auditing role of air carriers is un- of greater consequence.
deniably important, the role of an independent and Although accidents are the most salient and
attentive regulatory agency is critical. Only one poignant e!ects, human errors in maintenance ac-
example is necessary to underscore this point. In tivities have other important consequences. For
1990, Eastern Airlines and many of its top execu- example, maintenance errors have required air
tives were indicted by a grand jury for falsifying turn-backs, delays in aircraft availability, gate re-
maintenance records, disregarding FAA require- turns, in-#ight shutdowns, diversions to alternate
ments that repairs be examined, and creating false airports, maintenance rework, damage to main-
records to indicate that required, scheduled main- tenance equipment, and injury to maintenance
tenance had been performed (Nader and Smith, personnel. Gregory (1993) "nds that 50% of all
1994). Without an independent regulatory agency's engine-related #ight delays and cancellations are
critical review of airline and manufacturer practi- due to improper maintenance. Thirty-three percent
ces, the opportunity exists for the unscrupulous to of all military aviation equipment malfunctions re-
optimize economy at the expense of safety. sult from poor prior maintenance or improperly
144 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

applied maintenance procedures (Ru!ner, 1990). vides a classi"cation scheme for genotypes, the un-
These consequences ultimately a!ect customer satis- derlying mechanisms, of human error in aviation
faction and airline company productivity and pro"t. maintenance and inspection. He bases this classi-
The negative e!ects of human error in aviation "cation scheme on Rouse and Rouse's (1983) error
maintenance, as re#ected in accidents, incidents, and framework (Table 6).
other operational ine$ciencies, are clear. In a di!erent vein, Foyle and Dupont (1995)
identify the twelve most common causes of main-
4.4. Predicting forms of inspection and maintenance tenance personnel's `judgment interference.a The
human errors motivation for this e!ort is a quote by Jerome
Lederer, President Emeritus of the Flight Safety
Extensions of human reliability and human error Foundation, `(Maintenance) error is not the cause
classi"cation methods to the aviation maintenance of an accident. The cause is to be found in whatever
and inspection area are rare. This section presents it was that interfered with the (maintenance per-
three approaches to studying aviation mainten- son's) judgment at a critical moment, the outcome
tance and inspection errors beyond simply catalog- of which was a maintenance error (Foyle and
ing their overt consequences to equipment. Most Dupont, 1995).a Table 7 explains this judgment
similar to the HRA approach, one study employs interference in terms of a speci"c accident: on July
a fault tree analysis to investigate and quantify 11, 1991, an aircraft maintenance technician (AMT)
human error in aircraft inspection (Lock and who was aware that at least two tires on a Nation-
Strutt, 1985). Lock and Strutt (1985) develop air DC-8 had low pressure, boarded the aircraft
a #owchart to describe the inspection process and perished with 260 other persons.
(Fig. 1). Analysis of this #owchart yields six poten-
tial errors in the inspection process (Table 4), which
may co-occur in more complex error forms. These 5. Managing human error in aviation maintenance
errors are then represented in a fault tree (Fig. 2).
Lock and Strutt (1985) identify "ve PSFs relevant Great strides have been made towards Secretary
to aircraft inspection (area accessibility, general Pen a's goal of zero accidents. On a national level,
area lighting, access and visual enhancement tools, the 1995 National Safety conference (FAA, 1995)
motivation/attitude, and work method) and pro- called for additional human factors research focused
vide relative weights to indicate their importance on error detection and prevention. Speci"cally, it
for each inspection step. However, noting the di- suggested that the FAA #ight standards should es-
$culty of quantifying the probabilities needed tablish a national data base for aviation human
to complete this fault tree, the authors do not factors research, develop a maintenance error analy-
actually perform an HRA analysis of human error sis tool prototype, and develop a system for main-
probabilities. tenance personnel based on the same principles as
Drury (1991) describes human error phenotypes the Crew Resource Management (CRM) program
based on the previously described model of main- for the cockpit. This section reviews recent e!orts
tenance and inspection functions. He de"nes these towards detecting and managing human error.
human errors by decomposing inspection functions
into tasks, and identifying the failure modes of these 5.1. Detecting human errors in aviation maintenance
tasks. These error categories were re"ned through and inspection
observation of inspections, and discussions with
inspectors, supervisors, and quality control person- Detecting systemic human errors, and associated
nel (Drury et al., 1990; Drury, 1991). Table 5 performance shaping characteristics, is di$cult due
presents a sample of this error taxonomy. The to low error rates, distributed occurrence over
taxonomy above only presents errors as observed, the system, and variability of error phenotypes for
that is phenotypes of erroneous actions in aviation the same error genotype. There are two funda-
maintenance and inspection. Drury (1991) also pro- mental methods for detecting errors in aviation
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 145

Fig. 1. Inspection Model Flowchart (adapted from Lock and Strutt, 1985).

Table 4
Potential errors in the inspection process (Lock and Strutt, 1985)

Error Location in Flowchart De"nition

Scheduling (E1) Wrong execution of either of the two tasks: `identify next inspectiona or `move to locationa
Inspection (E2) Not seeing a defect when one exists
Inspection (E3) If human induced, due to either forgetting to cover area, covering area `inadequatelya or
a scheduling error
Engineering Judgment (E4) An error in deciding whether the area in which a defect is found is signi"cant or not
Maintenance Card System (E5) Arises because the work cards themselves may not be used to note defects on the hangar #oor
immediately as they are found
Noting Defect (E6) The error is noted incorrectly or not noted at all
146 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

Fig. 2. Inspection Error Fault Tree (Lock and Strutt, 1985).

maintenance. The "rst detects human errors retro- reporting systems, then, may identify human
actively, and is based on error reporting responses error contributions for a particular incident and
to system-de"ned performance deviations. In con- may identify, as a result of this incident, systemic
trast, there are other, more pro-active detection problems. These systems are therefore, reactive
methods. This section reviews error reporting and to errors inherent in the system. There are many
pro-active error identi"cation methods of detecting di!erent forms of error reporting systems.
human error in aviation maintenance and inspec- Most current error reporting systems show some
tion in the commercial aviation system. common features (Drury, 1991; Latorella and
Drury, 1992):
5.2. Reporting errors in aviation maintenance and
inspection E They are event driven } the system captures data
only if a di$culty arises or defect is found.
Error reporting systems characterize, to varying E Aircraft type and structure serve as the major
degrees, perceived causes of a negative event. Error classi"cation type for reporting.
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 147

Table 5
Sample of aircraft maintenance and inspection errors by task step (Drury, 1991)

Tasks for `Initiatea function Error(s)

1.1 Correct instructions 1.1.1 Incorrect instructions


1.1.2 Incomplete instructions
1.1.3 No instructions available
1.2 Correct equipment procured 1.2.1 Incorrect equipment
1.2.2 Equipment not procured
1.3 Inspector gets instructions 1.3.1 Fails to get instructions
1.4 Inspector reads instructions 1.4.1 Fails to read instructions
1.4.2 Partially reads instructions
1.5 Inspector understands instructions 1.5.1 Fails to understand instructions
1.5.2 Misinterprets instructions
1.5.3 Does not act on instructions
1.6 Correct equipment available 1.6.1 Correct equipment not available
1.6.2 Equipment is incomplete
1.6.3 Equipment is not working
1.7 Inspector gets equipment 1.7.1 Gets wrong equipment
1.7.2 Gets incomplete equipment
1.7.3 Gets non-working equipment
1.8 Inspector checks/calibrates equipment 1.8.1 Fails to check/calibrate
1.8.2 Checks/calibrates incorrectly

Table 6
Example of possible errors in calibrating NDI task step (Drury, 1991)

Level of Processing Possible Errors

1. Observation of system state Fails to read display correctly


2. Choice of hypothesis Instrument will not calibrate. Inspector assumes battery is low
3. Test of hypothesis Fails to use knowledge of NiCads to test
4. Choice of goal Decides to search for new battery
5. Choice of procedure Calibrates for wrong frequency
6. Execution of procedure Omits range calibration step

This example is for task 1.8.2 shown in Table 5.

E Data is further classi"ed, and its urgency deter- Second, we consider error reporting systems in the
mined, using expert assessment of error critical- most conventional sense, those residing within air-
ity. line companies and more formal regulatory report-
E Feedback is not well-formatted to be useful to ing systems.
operators.
E Error reports can result in changes to mainten- 5.2.1. Accident investigations
ance and inspection procedures. The National Transportation Safety Board
(NTSB) conducts investigations of aviation acci-
This section identi"es two forms of error report- dents in which there is any loss of life or signi"cant
ing. First, accident investigations and incident re- aircraft damage. These investigations routinely
ports are considered as a form of error reporting. consider whether documentation re#ects that
148 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

Table 7
Judgment interference in aviation maintenance personnel (Foyle and Dupont, 1995)

Judgment Interference Example

Lack of communication The AMT did not inform the pilot that tire pressures were low, even after when the pilot stated `we
got a #at tire, you "gure?a on the "nal takeo! roll
Complacency The tires had had low pressure for several days without attention
Lack of knowledge Ignorance of the manufacturer's speci"cations for tire pressure
Distraction (Any distraction to the AMT is unknown.)
Lack of teamwork The AMT and the cockpit crew were #ying together, yet did not appear to know maintenance status
of the aircraft
Fatigue AMTs did not go to sleep until at least 11 pm and were called up at 3 am. They had also been
traveling with the aircraft and working on it during down time
Lack of resources Nitrogen to in#ate the tires was not readily available
Pressure The AMT was apparently under much personal pressure to keep the aircraft #ying for job security
reasons
Lack of assertiveness The base manager, who had no authority over the AMT, told him to `forget ita when it appeared
they would be delayed 30 min if the AMT in#ated the low tires
Stress The AMT was counting on the success of this deployment to enable him to advance with the
company and to be able to settle in Canada
Lack of awareness The AMT was unaware that tire pressure was critical to the aircraftmH s safety
Norms Company procedures allowed the AMT to make an error as an acceptable practice

maintenance was performed as scheduled and was Accident investigations can also result in the
appropriate for the aircraft involved in the accident. institution of speci"c inspections/repairs/replace-
Beyond this minimal investigation of maintenance ment of components, or increase the frequency of
performance, these investigations also consider inspecting/repairing/replacing certain components.
whether inspection/maintenance personnel could For example, after identifying premature deteriora-
have identi"ed/repaired a problem that was not tion of seat cushion "re-blocking materials as
necessarily related to scheduled performance. Fur- a contributing factor in the China Eastern Airlines
ther, if an inspection/maintenance problem is im- accident, the NTSB suggested that principle main-
plicated, the investigation extends to consider tenance inspectors should inform operators of the
possible de"ciencies in airline quality control and need to `periodically inspect "re-blocking mater-
assurance programs, in the regulatory mechanism ials for wear and damage, and to replace defective
of the FAA, and in manufacturer's recommenda- materials (NTSB, 1993a).a Following the in-#ight
tions for scheduling inspection/maintenance tasks. separation of an engine and engine pylon from
These investigations result in recommendations to a Boeing-747 due to a crack in the pylon forward
the airline companies (e.g., inspection/maintenance "rewall web, Boeing issued a service bulletin, call-
practices, environmental considerations, training, ing for a detailed visual inspection of this area on
quality control and assurance practices), the FAA Boeing-747 aircraft with similar engines (NTSB,
(e.g., ensure airlines adhere to maintenance sched- 1993b). While NTSB reports thoroughly investi-
ules), and manufacturers (e.g., scheduling new or gate the causal mechanisms of accidents and pro-
increasing the frequency of inspections/repairs/re- vide useful recommendations for addressing these
placements). Investigation of the Continental acci- failures in the system, relying on accident reports
dent (NTSB, 1992), described above, resulted in to identify these mechanisms is, aside from the
recommendations at the individual operator level obvious catastrophe of any loss of life, extremely
(shift changeover), the airline level (quality assur- ine$cient. In addition, in order to perform the
ance programs), and for the FAA (ensuring that detailed analyses of accidents required, feedback at
airline quality assurance programs are sound). the operator level is diminished by its delay, and
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 149

therefore contributing situational factors, are often (1995) suggest that the aviation system would be
missed. well-served by an anonymous reporting system spe-
ci"cally for aviation inspection and maintenance
5.2.2. Anonymous incident reports technicians to report incidents during the mainten-
The NTSB conducts formal investigations of ac- ance process.
cidents. Incidents are reported to the Aviation
Safety Reporting System (ASRS), which is 5.2.3. Internal error reporting systems
maintained by NASA though an independent Airlines also maintain their own error reporting
contractor. The ASRS allows individuals to systems. Recent e!orts have attempted to overcome
anonymously report incidents and problems to three problems with prior generations of error re-
a centralized, non-regulatory repository. After porting at the airline level: (1) error reporting sys-
clarifying details in the reports, ASRS personnel tems have not been integrated within an airline, (2)
remove identifying information, allowing reporters performance shaping factors surrounding error oc-
to remain anonymous. Pilots are the primary users currences are not well-documented in error reports,
of this system and typically report incidents occur- (3) error reporting systems have not been well in-
ring during operation of the aircraft. Although tegrated across airlines.
mechanics also enter reports to the system, such Most airlines monitor incidents and accidents in
contributions are much less frequent than the con- the system very stringently. Records are kept on
tributions of pilots. Maintenance/inspection issues, such performance metrics as personnel injuries, air-
however, also arise from pilot-initiated reports. craft or equipment damage, and delays. However,
ASRS reports are searchable and are reviewed for most of the error reporting systems are used separ-
salient or recurrent problems in a periodical, `Call- ately by di!erent departments and rarely used to-
backa. Researchers can request key-word sorted gether to analyze the system as a whole (Wenner
compilations of ASRS reports. Thus, ASRS reports and Drury, 1996). Therefore, separate error report-
provide a means of identifying contributions of ing systems catalog di!erent error phenotypes. This
inspection and maintenance human error to avi- dissociation by error phenotype limits the ability to
ation incidents. recognize what may be a common error genotype.
One must exercise caution in using ASRS data. Wenner and Drury describe a situation that high-
Several factors limit interpretation of this data: (1) lights this problem: For example, if a mechanic
reports are based on one person's perspective, and drops a wrench on his foot, the incident would be
this perspective may be biased, (2) results of ASRS recorded as an &On the Job Injury'. If a mechanic
searches are not typically all-inclusive, rather they drops a wrench on an aircraft, damaging it severely,
provide a sample of reports containing search the incident would be recorded as &&Technical Op-
terms, (3) the probability of reporting is ostensibly erations Ground Damage.'' If the wrench was drop-
not equal over all types of incidents or potential ped on the aircraft, causing no damage, the incident
reporters, therefore reports contained in the ASRS would not be recorded at all. Finally, if a ground
do not necessarily represent a statistically valid operations employee drops a wrench on an aircraft,
sample of actual occurrences. It is also important to the incident would be recorded as &&Ground Opera-
note that, in exchange for reporting incidents, pilots tions Ground Damage.'' In each of these scenarios,
receive limited immunity from FAA prosecution. the error (genotype) was exactly the same, only the
Finally, although there is a mechanism for main- "nal consequences (error phenotype) di!ered, dif-
tenance personnel to report incidents, ASRS re- ferentiating the way in which each of these incidents
ports are predominantly supplied by pilots. Despite is recorded. Clearly, the use of multiple reporting
these limiting considerations, ASRS reports pro- systems that are maintained by di!erent depart-
vide useful information. They detail more examples ments, makes root error detection more di$cult.
of error phenotypes than NTSB reports, and pro- Thus incident investigation and reporting tools
vide a user's view of the factors involved, usually must be developed so that they can be applied
relatively soon after the incident. Allen and Rankin across airline systems. Wenner and Drury describe
150 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

a prototype system, called the Uni"ed Incident MEDA has "ve-categories for reporting an error
Reporting System (UIRS) that has a common occurrence:
reporting form and an outcome-speci"c form.
The common reporting form gathers data for E General data (e.g., airline, aircraft type, tail/#eet
all incidents irrespective of whether the incident C, date of incident, time of incident),
was a paperwork error, an injury, ground E Operational event data (e.g., #ight delay, gate
damage, or even had no adverse outcomes. This return, in-#ight shut down, aircraft damage),
form then directs the user to one of the outcome E Maintenance error classi"cation (e.g., improper
speci"c forms based on the hazard patterns installation, improper servicing),
developed. E Contributing factors analysis (e.g., factors related
Airline error reporting schemes typically have to information, equipment, airplane design, job,
been concerned with establishing accountability for skills, environment, organization, supervision,
an error and its consequences rather than under- communication. These include queries on cor-
standing the causal factors and the situational con- rect information, inaccessible aircraft space, new
text of the error. Analyzing data collected from an task, inadequate task knowledge, time con-
error reporting system that was designed to simply straints, environment, poor planning),
note consequences and assign accountability is usu- E Corrective actions (i.e., intervention strategy in
ally fruitless. In these cases, accident/incident in- terms of: reviewing existing procedures and pol-
vestigation usually terminates as soon as blame can icies to prevent such incidents, and identifying
be attributed to some human in the system. Sim- new corrective actions for local level.).
ilarly, error control methods derived from such an
approach are usually in the form of reprimanding In a recent "eld evaluation, MEDA appeared to
and further training the operator, and instituting an meet its objectives (Allen and Rankin, 1995). Survey
additional regulatory check for that speci"c occur- responses and evaluation of technicians' report
rence. With such simplistic error reporting schemes, forms indicated that MEDA provided a useful
then, the situational context at the point of error is standardized investigation methodology to the
lost, and with it, the opportunity to more intelli- maintenance organization. Technicians used re-
gently characterize and sensitively manage the true sponse forms in a manner consistent with MEDA's
error mechanisms. A useful error reporting system standardized investigation methodology. The
must have a general theory of the task as well as MEDA analysis identi"ed some maintenance de-
situational factors which may a!ect task perfor- "ciencies. Finally, survey data indicated that
mance. The Maintenance Error Decision Aid maintenance personnel's understanding of human
(MEDA) developed by the Boeing Customer Servi- performance issues improved after using MEDA
ces Division in cooperation with several airlines (Allen and Rankin, 1995). The "eld test also
and the FAA, is an error reporting system that tries underscored the di$culty of instituting a new tech-
to capture the causality of an incident in terms of nology and process into an organization, and
contributing factors. The objectives of MEDA are the need for human factors and process-speci"c
to (Allen and Rankin, 1995): training. Currently, MEDA is available to
customer airlines as a means of improving their
E Improve airline maintenance organizations' un- own maintenance operations and to improve
derstanding of human performance issues. communications with Boeing regarding design
E Provide line-level and organizational mainten- and manufacturing issues. MEDA promotes im-
ance personnel a standard method for analyzing provements in error reporting by more e!ectively
errors. capturing concomitant situational factors. In addi-
E Identify maintenance system de"ciencies that in- tion, by providing a common platform for error
crease exposure to error and decrease e$ciency. reporting, MEDA provides the opportunity for in-
E Provide a means of error-trend analysis for com- creased integration of error reporting among air-
mercial airline maintenance organizations. lines and with manufacturers.
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 151

Results from internal error reporting systems A human factors audit is a methodology for iden-
reach the FAA via formal channels, such as Service tifying lapses in work practices, inadequacies in the
Di$culty Reports and Voluntary Disclosures. The work environment, and human-task mismatches
FAA, then, may disseminate information to manu- that can lead to human errors. Most audits are
facturers regarding speci"c maintenance problems conducted using checklists and questionnaires. As
by issuing Advisory Circulars and Airworthiness part of a human factors audit, human factors experts
Directives. Advisory Circulars provide guidance for with domain expertise may directly observe oper-
controlling the maintenance process and are non- ators performing their jobs. These observers note the
mandatory. However, the FAA only issues Air- type, frequency, and cause of human errors. Obser-
worthiness Directives if it has conclusive proof of vations are typically recorded according to a classi-
a problem. Adherence to Airworthiness Directives "cation of failure modes guided by task analysis, and
is mandatory. In addition to these reporting sys- an understanding of relevant situational factors in
tems, manufacturers issue service newsletters and the environment. As an example, Drury's aforemen-
bulletins which contain some information on hu- tioned de"nitions of inspection and maintenance
man error incidents. tasks were the foundation for identifying failure
modes of these tasks. This classi"cation scheme pro-
5.3. Pro-active error detection methods vided a structure for observing actual technicians'
error patterns (Drury et al., 1990).
Error reporting systems are most useful for de- To conduct a human factors audit, one must "rst
veloping error management strategies to prevent de"ne: (1) how to sample (frequency and distribu-
the speci"c error addressed from reoccurring. In tion of samples), (2) what factors to measure, (3)
some cases, to the degree that situational factors how to evaluate a sample (standards, good practi-
and human error generating mechanisms are ces, and principles, for comparison), and (4) how to
captured in these error reporting schemes, these communicate results (Drury, 1997; Chervak and
systems may identify more generalizable systemic Drury, 1995). An audit must demonstrate validity,
errors. However these methods are basically react- reliability, sensitivity, and usability. When properly
ive; that is, an accident, incident, or other system- used, audits can be an important means of pro-
de"ned deviation must occur to precipitate these actively assessing error likelihood. However, audits
analyses. The safety of the aviation system would can be somewhat di$cult to conduct. They can be
be much improved if we were able to identify sys- intrusive to the normal work environment. They
temic errors, and performance shaping factors of must be performed by an individual with both
these errors, before incurring the costs associated human factors and domain expertise, usually a rare
with these precipitating events. The methods combination. It can be di$cult to obtain a large
described below have recently been employed to enough sample size for a useful audit. Finally, the
pro-actively identify error-generating situations usual trade-o!s exist between the breadth and
and characteristics of aviation maintenance and depth of analysis, and time available to conduct the
inspection operations. analysis. Drury (1997) describes implementation
considerations, and factors a!ecting the success of
5.3.1. Audits audits. Galaxy Scienti"c Corporation has de-
The Flight Standards service of the FAA per- veloped a computerized version of a human factors
forms periodic audits of airline inspection and audit for aviation maintenance and inspection,
maintenance programs. Most FAA audits are for ERNAP (Meghashyam, 1995).
regulatory purposes. These emphasize ensuring
that airlines follow prescribed inspection and main- 5.3.2. Subjective evaluations of system reliability
tenance procedures, and have appropriate quality Some error detection systems use subjective rat-
assurance programs. In addition to these formal, ing scales to determine if the task environment
regulatory audits, errors may be detected by a hu- is error-prone. This methodology assumes that a
man factors audit. system's error-proneness can be deduced by having
152 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

people who work in the environment subjectively teract, within a real system. Simulation methods
assess a set of factors (similar to the concept of o!er an alternative approach for predicting human
performance shaping factors or error shaping fac- errors, allowing more careful control/isolation
tors). While such assessments can be used e!ec- of situational and operator variables. Simulation
tively to complement reactive detection methods, implementations range from part-task simulators
implementing such systems requires caution. One in which one observes operators performing simu-
must be careful to avoid biased questions, and to lated tasks, to virtual environments which include
motivate assessors to provide factual, unprejudiced simulations of ambient conditions, to simulations
assessments. In addition, one must determine the of the system that include a simulated operator.
sample size of the data, how the resulting data will In the aviation maintenance and inspection envir-
be analyzed and interpreted, and how results will onment, part-task simulations provide the oppor-
be implemented. It is also important to provide tunity to observe operators perform visual and
proper feedback to volunteer assessors. Without eddy-current rivet inspections (e.g., Latorella et al.,
visible and e!ective feedback and actual resultant 1992). More advanced simulations have not yet
actions, such methods tend to lose credibility and been developed for aviation maintenance and in-
can become ine!ective. spection, although they have been applied to other
James Reason developed MESH (Managing En- work domains. For example, a simulation exists of
gineering Safety Health) as a pro-active subjective maintenance personnel in nuclear power plants
assessment tool for British Airways (Rogan, 1995b). (Gertman and Blackman, 1993).
MESH assumes that the system's intrinsic resis-
tance to accident-producing factors is due to the 5.4. Addressing human error control in aviation
interplay of several factors at both the local work- maintenance and inspection
place level and the organizational level. It a!ords
regular measurement of the maintenance work Once one determines that human error is a factor
force's and management's perception of the local in an accident, incident, event, or error-likely situ-
and organizational factors. (Reason and Maddox, tation, one must address how to control, or man-
1995). Randomly selected assessors periodically age, this error. Most situations can be addressed
make simple subjective ratings on certain system through a variety of interventions. Further, inter-
factors through an anonymous computer-based ventions are most e!ectively implemented when
survey. A given group of assessors operates for used in combination. Interventions for error reduc-
a limited period of time and is then replaced by tion include: selection, training, equipment design,
a new group. The MESH program accumulates job design, and aiding (Rouse, 1985). More detailed
these inputs and summarizes the factors that could lists of interventions speci"cally intended for the
contribute to accidents or incidents. Quality con- aviation maintenance and inspection environment
trol groups within the airline identify and prioritize have been classi"ed as short term and long-term
issues in the MESH pro"les. Technicians assess interventions (Shepherd et al., 1991). This section
local factors and technical management personnel reviews some of the techniques and approaches
assess organizational factors. Technicians receive that have been proposed and applied to reducing
feedback in the form of newsletters and notice the probability of human error in the aviation
boards (Rogan, 1995b). maintenance environment. This review considers
the broad categories of: (1) training, (2) job design
5.3.3. Simulation approaches and organizational considerations, (3) workspace
Direct observation, either by an expert or and ambient environment design, (4) task equip-
through subjective evaluation by individuals in ment and information design, and (5) automation.
a system, requires intrusion into the workplace. In Finally we describe an approach to identifying,
addition, interpreting these observations for causal selecting among, and justifying intervention strat-
mechanisms is often di$cult due to the number of egies for managing errors in aviation maintenance
situational and operator factors that vary, and in- and inspection.
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 153

5.4.1. Training same level of post-training knowledge as those


Training at the individual operator level con- trained in the traditional instructor-led method.
tinues to improve and take advantage of new tools Subjective evaluations indicated that technicians
and methodologies. However there are still oppor- preferred a combination of human and CBT in-
tunities for improving individual training. For struction (Johnson and Shepherd, 1993).
example, inspection training tends to focus on pro- A large portion of aircraft maintenance and in-
cedural aspects (e.g., setting up NDT equipment spection activities are accomplished by technicians
and troubleshooting rules) rather than providing working in teams. Thus, a technician has to learn to
feedback for other, more cognitive aspects (e.g., be a team member and coordinate and communic-
making perceptual judgments) (Prabhu and Drury, ate e!ectively to accomplish the team objective.
1992). Transport Canada developed a workshop While teams can enhance performance, there are
for training maintenance technicians to avoid the also many opportunities for human error in this
aforementioned `judgment interferencesa (Foyle work structure. Such functions as decision-making,
and Dupont, 1995). Based on the Transactional knowledge-sharing, and communicating goals and
Analysis model, the Dupont Model distinguishes objectives, play a crucial role in improving team
between rational (`adulta) and emotional (`childa) performance. Recent e!orts in training focus on
motivations. This workshop provides examples of enhancing teamwork in aviation maintenance and
how the rational/emotional interactions can a!ect inspection. The success of Crew Resource Manage-
a person's judgment during work, identi"es techni- ment (CRM) in improving team performance
cians' behavioral types, and emphasizes the e!ects on the commercial #ight deck, provides a model for
of stress, fatigue, and lack of communication on improving collaborative work in the inspection/
human performance (Foyle and Dupont, 1995). maintenance environment. The FAA has proposed
While this form of training has been provided to extending the CRM approach to Maintenance
#ightdeck crews for some time, it is only now being Resource Management (MRM), or Technician
extended to the maintenance environment. Resource Management (TRM), to encourage team-
In addition, e!orts have been directed toward work and e!ective problem solving in maintenance
providing aviation inspection and maintenance crews (FAA, 1991,1995). Evaluations of the few
technicians with computer-based training (CBT). MRM training programs attempted at airlines
Three years ago, the O$ce of Aviation Medicine have showed success in these e!orts (Taggert, 1990;
instituted a research and development plan to dem- Galaxy Scienti"c, 1993) in both objective per-
onstrate and evaluate the use of CBT techniques for formance measures and subjective measures of
these technicians. The prototype system provided technician attitudes (Galaxy Scienti"c, 1993).
maintenance technicians with instruction for diag- Gramopadhye et al. (1996) describe a computer-
nosing the environmental control system (ECS) of based multimedia team training tool, the Aircraft
a Boeing 767-300 (Johnson et al., 1992). In addition Maintenance Team Training software (AMTT),
to providing diagnostic instruction and practice, which includes a team skills instructional module.
the CBT system allows users access to all appropri- This module addresses the key dimensions team
ate cockpit and maintenance bay controls for the skills: communication, decision-making, interper-
ECS and access to interactive pages in the Boeing sonal relationships, and leadership. AMTT also has
fault isolation manual (FIM). The prototype system a task simulation module that allows users to apply
was distributed to most of the world's airlines, via learned team skills in a simulated aircraft mainten-
the Air Transport Association's (ATA) Main- ance situation.
tenance Training Committee, and to most FAA Endsley and Robertson (1996) analyze situation
certi"ed aviation maintenance technical schools awareness (SA) requirements for aircraft mainten-
through the Aviation Technician Education Coun- ance teams by asking operators to interpret
cil (ATEC) (Johnson and Shepherd, 1993). Results elements in the environment and describe their
from an evaluation study demonstrated that stu- spatial and temporal positions and trajectories
dents trained with the CBT system showed the (Endsley, 1988). Results suggested improvements
154 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

for technician team training to reduce error and suggest organizational changes to improve aviation
enhance performance (Endsley and Robertson, maintenance and inspection. Disciplinary actions
1996). Teams should be trained to establish shared should be uniform throughout an organization and
mental models. That is, the team should have should be structured to complement limited-
a clear understanding of what other teams know immunity policies in conjunction with incident
and do not know. Teams should be trained to reporting (Allen and Rankin, 1995). At a higher-
verbalize the decision-making process better. Tech- level of organization, the outputs of safety and
nicians must be trained to conduct better shift information programs should be shared among air-
meetings and perform as a team member. That is, lines and manufacturers (Allen and Rankin, 1995).
there should be explicit training for team leads to
convey to the team common goals, an understand- 5.4.3. Workspace and ambient environment design
ing of who is doing what, and expectations regard- Several aspects of the ambient environment af-
ing teamwork. Managers, leads, and technicians fect maintenance performance. Some maintenance
should be trained to provide better feedback (both tasks are performed in extremely cold conditions.
positive and negative) on prior attempts. Addition- At times this forces technicians to wear gloves dur-
ally, training programs should address problems ing performance of their tasks, further complicating
which cause a loss of situation awareness. One manipulation and tactile sensation. Volatile hydro-
evaluation of the CRM approach in aviation main- carbons in fuel tanks and cleaning agents at other
tenance found that such team training could also be access areas produce noxious fumes for inspectors
improved by helping technicians transition new and maintenance technicians. Recently new sol-
MRM skills to their actual working environment, vents have been identi"ed that are less noxious to
to focus directly on training technicians how to operators (Drury, 1996). Research has most fully
voice disagreements, and to plan and publicize for addressed issues of lighting adequacy and postural/
recurrent MRM training (Galaxy Scienti"c, 1993). biomechanic hazards associated with aviation
maintenance and inspection tasks.
5.4.2. Job design and organizational considerations Ninety percent of all inspection is visual inspec-
Training has great appeal since it can rapidly tion (Johnson and Shepherd, 1993). A general
reach a whole department or company. Drury methodology, has been developed in cooperation
(1996) states that training can easily be made air- with an airline partner, to recommend optimal
line-speci"c by using case studies of accidents or illumination equipment for individual inspection
errors from that airline or from similar operations tasks (Johnson and Shepherd, 1993). The resulting
in other airlines. Drury describes an e!ort to bring methodology uses task analysis data, lighting
about ergonomic/human factors changes via train- evaluations, subjective input from inspectors and
ing in aviation maintenance. This e!ort required evaluation of illumination sources to specify better
establishing and training a human factors task portable area lighting, task lighting, and ambient
force, comprised of both management and hangar illumination.
work force. A human factors expert coordinates the Maintenance tasks often require technicians to
team and acts as the advisor for human factors assume di$cult postures and to work in restricted
issues. The training program utilized the SHEL spaces. Reynolds et al. (1994) investigates the e!ects
(software, hardware, environment, and `livewarea of performance in restricted spaces on e!ort and
(humans)) model (Edwards, 1972) to organize the performance. Vertical restrictions on an inspection
human factors material. Results from this e!ort task demonstrably increase postural stress and res-
indicate several organizational factors that are piration rate variability, a measure of decreased
critical to the success of these task forces; focusing attentiveness (Reynolds et al., 1994). Sagittal re-
on issues at the right level, having a champion in strictions, however, appear to improve operator
the organization for the whole e!ort, and maintain- performance over unrestricted control conditions,
ing trust that management actions would follow indicating that some physical restriction may
recommendations. Allen and Rankin (1995) also improve task focus (Reynolds et al., 1994). This
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 155

research has developed a program for identifying ces to equipment and information. Therefore, one
maintenance tasks where postural demands exceed method for controlling these errors is to redesign
human capability while ensuring safe and reliable equipment and interfaces to information systems
performance, and for suggesting solutions to identi- (including those implemented in paper). Interface
"ed problematic tasks (Johnson and Shepherd, issues are also important in the development of new
1993). equipment. Aviation maintenance and inspection
Unfortunately, this reactive approach is the only tasks are increasingly computer based and include
real solution for improving postural problems for the use of new tools and techniques. As such, the
most of the aircraft being serviced today. However, human/machine interface to computer-based sys-
future aircraft may in#ict fewer such postural prob- tems and new equipment is increasingly important.
lems. Understanding that an aircraft design which In general, designing these interfaces would bene-
forces awkward postures, restricted access to com- "t from usability assessment and engineering
ponents, and requires frequent raising and lowering (e.g., Ravden and Johnson, 1989; Nielsen, 1993)
of equipment can lead to increased maintenance- (Table 8). Some speci"c human/machine interface
related error frequency, Boeing has developed spe- improvements to aviation inspection and main-
ci"c design criteria for the B-777 to facilitate access tenance equipment include using templates for
and maintenance (Marx, 1992). While this e!ort is interpreting and calibrating NDI signals, and im-
promising, a more general e!ort towards develop- proving the interface to work cards. Technological
ing standard guidelines for maintainability and in- improvements have also been applied to functions
spectability would be most useful. MIL-STD-1472c at higher organizational levels. For example, one
provides some guidelines for maintainability. computer-based system allows FAA inspectors
Mason (1990) describes the Bretby Maintainability to record information while auditing maintenance
Index that modi"es the SAE maintainability index operations using a stylus input device (Layton
(SAE, 1976) to include such issues as weight and Johnson, 1993).
of components, size and position of access aper- Work cards have been improved in three funda-
tures, restricted access for tools. This has been mental ways. First, the form of the work card has
developed mainly for construction machinery, been redesigned to present information in a more
however it could be extended to focus on aviation readable, and organized manner. Second, work
maintainability. cards can be improved by providing the appropri-
ate content, and in a usable form (e.g., graphically),
5.4.4. Task equipment and information design and a!ording easy access to reference materials.
Some `human errorsa in aviation maintenance Third, the physical interface to the work card
and inspection derive from poorly designed interfa- is important and must be considered. Patel and

Table 8
Usability criteria for user interface evaluation (Ravden and Johnson, 1989)

Factors Guidelines

Visual Clarity Information displayed on screen should be clear and well organized
Consistency The way the system looks/works should be consistent at all times
Informative Feedback Wherever appropriate and possible, the user should be given clear, informative feedback on where
they are in the system, what actions they have taken, whether these actions have been successful, and
what actions to be taken next
Explicitness The system structure and working should be clear to the user
Appropriate Functionality The system should meet the needs of the user when carrying out the task
Flexibility and Control The interface should be #exible in the way the information is presented to the users
Error Prevention and Control Design system to minimize the possibility of user error
User Guidance and Support Informative, easy-to-use and relevant guidance and support should be provided to help users
understand the system
156 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

colleagues (Patel et al., 1994,1993; Drury, 2000) Technicians often must refer to reference manuals
investigate these issues extensively. This research in the course of performing inspections and main-
identi"es human factors guidelines for formulating tenance work. Providing this reference information
a more technician-centered work card. In a "eld in a more accessible and usable form facilitates
evaluation, aviation inspectors preferred work performance (e.g., Inaba, 1991).
cards redesigned according to these guidelines over
original work cards (Patel et al., 1994). Drury (2000) 5.4.5. Automation
extends this research to improve the physical inter- Although some automation interventions appear
face of the work card. They implement aviation useful, development of these has typically been
inspection work cards in a computer-based hyper- technology driven, rather than human-centered
text system on a portable computer. Field eva- and requirements driven. This approach has
luations of this prototype system demonstrated a resulted in the development of automation
further improvement with this implementation. systems that are not well integrated (Drury, 1996).

Table 9
Examples of automation in aviation maintenance and inspection (Drury, 1996)

Function Automation examples

Planning E Automated stock control and parts ordering to ensure that lead times for obtaining parts
does not extend with out-of-service time.
E Optimization heuristics for packaging required items into the length of a check visit.

Opening/Cleaning E NDI devices eliminate need to open aircraft in some cases.

Inspection: initiate E Automated information presentation (e.g., Marx, 1992)


E Hypertext workcards allow access to background documentation (Lofgren and Drury,
1994).
E IMIS system in military aircraft (Johnson, 1990).

Inspection: access E Climbing robot performs eddy-current scanning of lap-splice joints (Albert et al., 1994).
Inspection: search & decision E E!ort towards automating signal processing and aiding "nal decision making (Johnson,
1989).

Inspection: response E Hypertext workcards allow integration of inspection performance with documentation of
response (Lofgren and Drury, 1994).

Repair: initiate E Aircraft Visit Management System (AVMS) at United Airlines integrates inspection and
repair activities (Goldsby, 1991).

Repair: diagnosis E Variety of AI and expert system approaches to diagnosis aiding (e.g., Husni, 1992).
E Computer-based training with intelligent tutoring for diagnosis (Johnson et al., 1992).
E On-board diagnostic system improvements (e.g., Hessburg, 1992).

Repair: repair/replace E Automation mostly limited to individual repair shop lines.


E CNC machining and robotic welding systems (Goldsby, 1991).

Repair: reset (none)


Buyback/Return to Service E Integration of inspection performance and response and maintenance actions makes
buyback easier.
E Bar codes on badges and workcards to automate job control notation (Goldsby, 1991).
E Fully electronic logbook proposed.
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 157

Table 9 presents automation approaches to equipment, are becoming more complex. As the
aviation maintenance and inspection by function commercial aviation #eet ages, and work force of
(Drury, 1996). In his review, Drury emphasizes maintenance personnel diminishes, maintenance
the importance of considering the additional workload is increasing. These pressures exacerbate
training requirements of automation interventions the likelihood of human error in aviation mainten-
and sensitively incorporating automation into ance and inspection processes. In fact, these errors
the organizational context and individuals' have various e!ects on the aviation system; from
jobs. inconsequential slips, to those which a!ect airline
e$ciency and passenger convenience, to those few
which ultimately result in an accident. In recogni-
5.4.6. Comprehensive and integrated approaches to
tion of this, the focus is now more toward under-
error management
standing the nature of human error in aviation
In addition to managing human error in aviation
maintenance and inspection, and improving
maintenance, one must manage interventions. That
methods for detecting and managing these errors.
is, how does one generate potential alternative
We review both reactive and pro-active methods of
intervention strategies, choose among these al-
error detection, and several intervention strategies
ternatives, and cost-justify these solutions to
for controlling human errors in aviation mainten-
management? Drury et al. (1996) describe a proto-
ance and inspection. Future directions in this area
type system for a more comprehensive approach to
would be to develop: (1) a maintenance incident
error reduction and management. This system be-
reporting system with limited immunity for report-
gins with the assumption that pro-active error
ing (Allen and Rankin, 1995), (2) a standardized,
monitoring and reactive error reporting are both
but rich, vocabulary/indexing scheme for charac-
essential for e!ective error control. It is based on
terizing situational and operator factors in error
the premise that it is important to identify and
reporting, (3) technologies to facilitate recognition
organize error reporting information in a manner
of hazardous patterns in situational and operator
consistent with intervention strategies. The pro-
factors, (4) aviation maintenance system simulation
posed system has "ve modules: (1) error reporting,
(cf. MAPPS in Gertman and Blackman (1993)
(2) critical incident reporting (reports of situations
and CES in Woods et al., 1987), (5) virtual
where errors/incidents almost happened but were
environmental simulations to support experimental
recovered without consequences), (3) error audit
investigations, (6) methodologies for identify-
(auditing speci"c tasks to "nd human-system mis-
ing organizational structures and job design
matches), (4) error assessment (anonymous assess-
characteristics which dampen the likelihood and
ment of the task environment by technicians and
perseverance of human errors, (7) truly human-
management), and (5) solutions database (of in-
centered, integrated task aiding, automation, and
formation from industry sources and human fac-
training.
tors experts for design changes). This prototype
system is called PERS (Pro-active Error Reduction
System) and is being developed under an FAA
Acknowledgements
research grant.
The authors are most appreciative of Dr. Colin
Drury for his useful comments on the organization
6. Conclusion and early drafts of this work, and of the reviewers
for their careful reading and helpful criticisms. The
Aviation maintenance is a complex organization "rst author gratefully acknowledges contribution
in which individuals perform varied tasks in an of "nancial support from the Industrial Engineer-
environment with time pressures, minimal feed- ing department at the University of Bu!alo,
back, and sometimes di$cult ambient conditions. through Dr. Colin Drury, for preparation of this
Aircraft, as well as inspection and maintenance work.
158 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

References Edwards, E., 1972. Man and machine: systems for safety. In: Pro-
ceedings of British Airline Pilots Association Technical Sympo-
Air Transport Association, 1994. The Annual Report of the U.S. sium. British Airline Pilots Association, London, pp. 21}36.
Scheduled Airline Industry (Brochure). Air Transport Asso- Endsley, M.R., 1988. Design and evaluation for situation aware-
ciation, Washington, DC. ness enhancements. In: Proceedings of the Human Factors
Albert, C.J., Kaufman, W.M., Siegel, M.W., 1994. Development Society 32nd Annual Meeting. Human Factors and Ergo-
of an Automated Nondestructive Inspection (ANDI) System nomics Society, Santa Monica, CA, pp. 97}101.
for Commercial Aircraft: Phase 1 Report (DOT/FAA/CT- Endsley, M.R., Robertson, M.M., 1996. Team situation aware-
94/23). National Technical Information Service, Spring"eld, ness in aviation maintenance. In: Proceedings of the Human
VA. Factors and Ergonomics Society 40th Annual Meeting. Hu-
Allen Jr., J.P., Rankin, W.L., 1995. A summary of the use and man Factors and Ergonomics Society, Santa Monica, CA,
impact of the Maintenance Error Decision Aid (MEDA) on pp. 1077}1081.
the commercial aviation industry. In: Proceedings of the Federal Aviation Administration, 1995. Zero accidents: a shared
Flight Safety Foundation International Federation of Air- responsibility. Aviation Safety Action Plan. U.S. Depart-
worthiness 48th Annual International Air Safety Seminar. ment of Transportation, Washington, DC.
Flight Safety Foundation, Arlington, VA, pp. 359}369. Federal Aviation Administration, 1991. Assessment of Mainten-
Boeing Airplane Company, 1993a. Statistical Summary of Com- ance Performance Measures and A Demonstration of CRM
mercial Jet Aircraft Accidents, Worldwide Operations, Training Evaluation Analysis Plan. Task 3 Report. Federal
1959}1992. Boeing Commercial Airplane Group, Seattle, WA. Aviation Administration, Washington, DC.
Boeing Airplane Company, 1993b. Accident Prevention Strat- Foyle, B., Dupont, G., 1995. Maintenance errors and their pre-
egies: Removing Links in the Accident Chain. Boeing Com- vention. In: Proceedings of the Flight Safety Foundation
mercial Airplane Group, Seattle, WA. International Federation of Airworthiness 48th Annual In-
Cacciabue, P.C., Carpignano, A., Vivalda, C., 1993. A dynamic ternational Air Safety Seminar. Flight Safety Foundation,
reliability technique for error assessment in man-machine Arlington, VA, pp. 353}357.
systems. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies 38, Galaxy Scienti"c Corporation, 1993. Human Factors in Avi-
403}428. ation Maintenance } Phase 2 Progress Report (NTIS
Chervak, S.G., Drury, C.G., 1995. Human factors audit program DOT/FAA/AM-93/5). National Technical Information Ser-
for maintenance. In: Human Factors in Aviation Mainten- vice, Spring"eld, VA.
ance Phase V Progress Report. Federal Aviation Adminis- Gertman, D.I., Blackman, H.S., 1993. Human Reliability and
tration, Washington, DC, pp. 93}126. Safety Analysis Data Handbook. Wiley, New York.
Drury, C.G., 2000. Relative advantages of portable computer- Goldsby, R.P., 1991. E!ects of automation in maintenance. In:
based workcards for aircraft inspection. International Jour- Proceedings of the Fifth Federal Aviation Administration
nal of Industrial Ergonomics 3(1), in press. Meeting on Human Factors Issues in Aircraft Maintenance
Drury, C.G., 1997. Chapter 49: Human factors audit. In: and Inspection. National Technical Information Service,
Salvendy, G. (Ed.), Handbook of Human Factors and Spring"eld, VA, pp. 103}123.
Ergonomics. Wiley, New York, NY. Goranson, U.G., 1989. Continuing airworthiness of aging jet
Drury, C.G., 1996. Automation in quality control and mainten- transports. In: Proceedings of the Second Annual Inter-
ance. In: Parasuraman, R., Mouloua, M. (Eds.), Automation national Conference on Aging Aircraft. Federal Aviation
and Human Performance: Theory and Applications. Law- Administration, Washington, DC, pp. 61}89.
rence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, pp. 407}426. Graeber, R.C., Marx, D.A., 1993. Reducing human error in
Drury, C.G., 1994. Function allocation in manufacturing. In: aircraft maintenance operations. In: Proceedings of the
Robertston, S.A. (Ed.), Proceedings of the Ergonomics So- Flight Safety Foundation International Federation of Air-
ciety 1994 Annual Conference. Taylor & Francis, London, worthiness 46th Annual International Air Safety Seminar.
pp. 2}16. Flight Safety Foundation, Arlington, VA, pp. 147}160.
Drury, C.G., 1991. Errors in aviation maintenance: taxonomy Gramopadhye, A., Kraus, D., Rao, P., Jebaraj, D., 1996.
and control. In: Proceedings of the Human Factors Society Application of advanced technology to team training. In:
35th Annual Meeting. Human Factors and Ergonomics So- Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 40th Annual
ciety, Santa Monica, CA, pp. 42}46. Meeting. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Santa
Drury, C.G., Prabhu, P., Gramopadhye, A., 1990. Task analysis Monica, CA, pp. 1072}1076.
of aircraft inspection activities: methods and "ndings. In: Gregory, W., 1993. Maintainability by design. In: Proceedings of
Proceedings of the Human Factors 34th Annual Meeting. the Fifth Annual Society of Automotive Engineers: Reliabil-
Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Santa Monica, ity, Maintainability, and Supportability Workshop. Society
CA, pp. 1181}1185. of Automotive Engineers, Warrendale, PA.
Drury, C.G., Prabhu, P.V., Wenner, C., Ramamurthy, M., 1996. Hagemaier, D., 1989. Nondestructive testing of aging aircraft.
Proactive error reporting system, In: Human Factors in In: Proceedings of the Second Annual International Confer-
Aviation Maintenance Phase VI Progress Report. Federal ence on Aging Aircraft. Federal Aviation Administration,
Aviation Administration, Washington, DC. Washington, DC, pp. 186}198.
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 159

Hessburg, J., 1992. Human factors considerations on the 777 aircraft Inspection tasks for o!-line experimentation. In:
on-board maintenance system design. In: Proceedings of the Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 36th Annual
Sixth Federal Aviation Administration Meeting on Human Meeting. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Santa
Factors Issues in Aircraft Maintenance and Inspection Monica, CA, pp. 92}96.
`Maintenance 2000a. National Technical Information Ser- Latorella, K., Drury, C.G. 1992. A framework for human relia-
vice, Spring"eld, VA, pp. 77}91. bility in aircraft inspection. In: Proceedings of the Seventh
Hollnagel, E., 1991. The genotype and phenotype of erroneous FAA Meeting on Human Factors Issues in Aircraft Main-
action: implications for HCI design. In: Alty, J., Weir, G. tenance and Inspection. Federal Aviation Administration,
(Eds.), Human-Computer Interaction and Complex Systems. Washington, DC.
Academic Press, London. Layton, C.F., Johnson, W.B., 1993. Job performance aids for the
Hollnagel, E. 1990. The design of error tolerant interfaces. In: #ight standard service. In: Proceedings of the Human Fac-
Proceedings of The First International Symposium on tors and Ergonomics 37th Annual Meeting. Human Factors
Ground Data Systems for Spacecraft Control. Darmstadt, and Ergonomics Society, Santa Monica, CA, pp. 26}29.
Germany, June 26}29. Lock, M.W.B., Strutt, J.E., 1985. Reliability of In-Service Inspec-
Husni, M. 1992. Advances in arti"cial intelligence for aircraft tion of Transport Aircraft Structures (Report 85013). Civil
maintenance. In: Proceedings of the Sixth Federal Aviation Aviation Authority, London.
Administration Meeting on Human Factors Issues in Aircraft Loftgren, J., Drury, C.G., 1994. Human factors advances
Maintenance and Inspection `Maintenance 2000.a National at Continental Airlines. In: Proceedings of the Eighth
Technical Information Service, Spring"eld, VA, pp. 157}171.
FAA/OAM Meeting on Human Factors in Aviation Main-
Inaba, K., 1991. Converting technical publications into mainten- tenance and Inspection `Trends and Advances in Aviation
ance performance aids. In: Proceedings of the Second An- Maintenance Operations.a National Technical Information
nual International Conference on Human Factors in Aging
Service, Spring"eld, VA, pp. 117}138.
Aircraft. Federal Aviation Administration, Washington, DC,
Lorenzo, D., 1990. A Manager's Guide to Reducing Human Er-
pp. 167}193.
rors: Improving Human Performance in the Chemical Indus-
Inaba, K., Dey, M., 1991. Programmatic Root Cause Analysis of
try. Chemical Manufacturers Associationp, Washington, DC.
Maintenance Personnel Performance Problems. U.S. Nu-
Lutzinger, R., 1992. TomorrowmH s problems as seen by mainten-
clear Regulatory Commission, Washington, DC.
ance managers. In: Proceedings of the Sixth FAA Meeting
Johnson, W.B., 1990. An integrated maintenance information
on Human Factors Issues in Aircraft Maintenance and In-
system (IMIS): an update. In: Final Report Second Federal
spection. National Technical Information Service, Spring-
Aviation Administration Meeting on Human Factors Issues
in Aircraft Maintenance and Inspection `Information Ex- "eld, VA, pp. 92}100.
change and Communications.a Federal Aviation Adminis- Marx, D., 1992. Looking towards 2000: the evolution of human
tration, Washington, DC, pp. 141}150. factors in maintenance. In: Proceedings of the Sixth FAA
Johnson, W.B., 1989. Research and development. In: Proceed- Meeting on Human Factors Issues in Aircraft Maintenance
ings of the Second Annual International Conference on and Inspection. National Technical Information Service,
Aging Aircraft. Federal Aviation Administration, Washing- Spring"eld, VA, pp. 64}76.
ton, DC, pp. 30}35. Mason, S., 1990. Improving plant and machinery maintainabil-
Johnson, W.B., Shepherd, W.T., 1993. The impact of human ity. Applied Ergonomics 3, 15}24.
factors research on commercial aircraft maintenance and Meghashyam, G., 1995. Electronic ergonomic audit system for
inspection. In: Proceedings from the Flight Safety Founda- maintenance and inspection. In: Proceedings of the Human
tion 46th Annual International Air Safety Seminar. Flight Factors and Ergonomics Society 39th Annual Meeting. Hu-
Safety Foundation, Arlington, VA, pp. 187}200. man Factors and Ergonomics Society. Santa Monica, CA,
Johnson, W.B., Norton, J.E., Utsman, L.G. 1992. New techno- pp. 1071}1813.
logy for the schoolhouse and #ightline maintenance environ- Nader, R., Smith, W.J., 1994. Collision Course. TAB Books,
ments. In: Proceedings of the Seventh FAA Conference on Blue Ridge Summit, PA.
Human Factors in Aircraft Maintenance and Inspection National Transportation Safety Board, 1984. Aircraft Accident
(NTIS PB93-146975). Federal Aviation Administration, Report, Eastern Airlines, Inc., L-1011, Miami, FL, May 5,
Washington, DC. 1983, (NTSB/AAR-84-04). National Transportation Safety
Kirwan, B., 1992a. Human error identi"cation in human relia- Board, Washington, DC.
bility assessment. Part 1: overview of approaches. Applied National Transportation Safety Board, 1989. Aircraft Accident
Ergonomics 23 (5), 299}318. Report, Aloha Airlines, Flight 243, Boeing 737-200, N73711,
Kirwan, B., 1992b. Human error identi"cation in human Maui, Hawaii, April 28, 1988, (NTSB/AAR-89-03). National
reliability assessment. Part 2: detailed comparison of Transportation Safety Board, Washington, DC.
techniques. Applied Ergonomics 23 (6), 371}381. National Transportation Safety Board, 1990. Aircraft Accident
Latia, D.C., 1993. Nondestructive Testing for Aircraft (Order Report, United Airlines, Flight 232, McDonnell Douglas,
no. EA-415). IAP, Casper, WY. DC-10, Sioux Gateway Airport, Sioux City, Iowa, July 19,
Latorella, K.A., Gramopadhye, A.K., Prabhu, P.V., Drury, C.G., 1989, (NTSB/AAR-90-06). National Transportation Safety
Smith, M.A., Shanahan, D.E., 1992. Computer-simulated Board, Washington, DC.
160 K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161

National Transportation Safety Board, 1992. Aircraft Accident Reason, J., 1990. Human Error. Cambridge University Press,
Report, Continental Express, Flight 135, Embraer 120, New York.
N33701, Eagle Lake, TX, September 11, 1991, (NTSB/AAR- Reason, J., 1987. Collective planning and its failures. In: Ras-
92-04). National Transportation Safety Board, Washington, mussen, J., Duncan, K., LePlat, J. (Eds.), New Technology
DC. and Human Error. Wiley, New York.
National Transportation Safety Board, 1993a. Aircraft Accident Reynolds, J.L., Drury, C.G., Sharit, J., Cerny, F., 1994.
Report, China Eastern Airlines, Flight 583. MD-11, B-2171, The e!ects of di!erent forms of space restriction on inspection
Shemya, Alaska, April 6, 1993, (NTSB/AAR-93-07). National performance. In: Proceedings of the Human Factors and
Transportation Safety Board, Washington, DC. Ergonomics Society 38th Annual Meeting. Human Factors
National Transportation Safety Board, 1993b. Aircraft Acci- and Ergonomics Society, Santa Monica, CA, pp. 631}635.
dent Report, Japan Airlines, Inc., Flight 46E, B-747-121, Reynolds, J.L., Drury, C.G., 1993. An evaluation of the visual
N473EV, Anchorage, Alaska, March 31, 1993, (NTSB/ environment in aircraft inspection. In: Proceedings of the
AAR-93-033). National Transportation Safety Board, Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 37th Annual Meet-
Washington, DC. ing. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Santa
Nielsen, J., 1993. Usability Engineering. Academic Press, Cam- Monica, CA, pp. 34}38.
bridge MA. Rogan, E., 1995a. MESH: Managing Engineering Safety Health.
Norman, D.A., 1981. Categorization of action slips. Psychologi- Human Factors 2, 5}6. British Airways Engineering, Lon-
cal Review 88, 1}15. don.
Patel, S., Drury, C.G., Loftgren, J., 1994. Design of work control Rogan, E., 1995b. Human factors in maintenance and engineer-
cards for aircraft inspection. Applied Ergonomics 25 (5), ing. In: Human Factors in Aviation Maintenance Phase
283}293. V Progress Report. Federal Aviation Administration, Wash-
Patel, S., Drury, C.G., Prabhu, P., 1993. Design and usability ington, DC, pp. 255}260.
evaluation of work control documentation. In: Proceedings Rouse, W.B., 1985. Optimal allocation of system development
of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 37th Annual resources and/or tolerate human error. IEEE Transactions
Meeting. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Santa on Systems, Man and Cybernetics 15 (5), 620}630.
Monica, CA, pp. 1156}1160. Rouse, W.B., Rouse, S.H., 1983. Analysis and classi"cation of
Prabhu, P., Drury, C.G., 1992. A framework for the design of the human error. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man and
aircraft inspection information environment. In: Proceedings Cybernetics 13 (4), 539}549.
of the Seventh FAA Meeting on Human Factors Issues Ru!ner, J.W., 1990. A Survey of Human Factors Methodologies
in Aircraft Maintenance and Inspection. Federal Aviation and Models for Improving the Maintainability of Emerging
Administration, Washington, DC. Army Aviation Systems. US Army Research Institute for the
Prabhu, P., Sharit, J., Drury, C., 1992b. Classi"cation of tem- Behavioral and Social Sciences, Arlington, VA.
poral errors in CIM systems: development of a framework Society of Automotive Engineers (SAE). 1976. Engineering De-
for deriving human-centered information requirements. In- sign Serviceability Guidelines } Construction and Industrial
ternational Journal of Computer Integrated Manufacturing Machinery. Society of Automotive Engineers, Warrendale,
5 (2), 68}80. PA.
Quintanilla, S.A.R., 1987. New technologies and human error: Sears, R.L., 1986. A new look at accident contributions and
social and organizational factors. In: Rasmussen, J., Duncan, the implications of operational training programs. (unpub-
K., LePlat, J. (Eds.), New Technology and Human Error. lished report). Boeing Commercial Airplane Group, Seattle,
Wiley, New York. WA. Cited in Graeber, R.C., Marx, D.A., 1993. Reducing
Rasmussen, J., 1990. The role of error in organizing behavior. human error in aircraft maintenance operations. In: Pro-
Ergonomics 33 (10/11), 1185}1199. ceedings of the Flight Safety Foundation International Fed-
Rasmussen, J., 1985. Trends in human reliability analysis. Ergo- eration of Airworthiness 46th Annual International Air
nomics 28 (8), 1185}1195. Safety Seminar. Flight Safety Foundation, Arlington, VA,
Rasmussen, J., 1982. Human errors: a taxonomy for describing pp. 147}160.
human malfunctions in indistrial installations. Journal of Seigel, A.I., Wolf, J.J., Lautman, M.R., 1975. A family of models
Occupational Accidents 4, 311}333. for measuring human reliability. In: Proceedings of the An-
Rasmussen, J., Vicente, K., 1989. Coping with human errors nual Reliability and Maintainability Symposium. Institute of
through system design: implications for ecological interface Electrical and Electronic Engineers, New York, NY, pp.
design. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies 31, 110}115.
517}534. Senders, J.W., Moray, N.P., 1991. Human Error: Cause, Predic-
Ravden, S.J., Johnson, G.I., 1989. Evaluating Usability of Hu- tion, and Reduction. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hill-
man-Computer Interfaces: A Practical Method. Halsted sdale, NJ.
Press, New York. Shepherd, W.T., Johnson, W.B., 1995. Human factors in airline
Reason, J. Maddox, M.E. 1995. Human error. In: Human maintenance and inspection, In: Proceedings of the Interna-
Factors Guide for Aviation Maintenance. U.S. Department tional Air Transport Association Maintenance Meeting. In-
of Transportation, Washington, DC (Chapter 14). ternational Air Transport Association, Montreal, Canada.
K.A. Latorella, P.V. Prabhu / International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics 26 (2000) 133}161 161

Shepherd, W.T., Johnson, W.B., Drury, C.D., Taylor, J.C., United Kingdom, Civil Aviation Authority (UK CAA). 1992.
Berninger, D., 1991. Human Factors in Aviation Mainten- Flight Safety Occurrence Document, 92/D/12, June 9, 1992.
ance, Phase 1: Progress Report: DOT/FAA/AM-91/16. Cited in Allen, J.P., Rankin, W.L., 1995. A summary of the
National Technical Information Service, Spring"eld, VA. use and impact of the maintenance error decision aid
Strauch, B., Sandler, C.E., 1984. Human factors considerations (MEDA) on the commercial aviation industry. In: Proceed-
in aviation maintenance. In: Proceedings of the Human ings of the Flight Safety Foundation International Feder-
Factors 28th Annual Meeting. Human Factors and Ergo- ation of Airworthiness 48th Annual International Air Safety
nomics Society, Santa Monica, CA, pp. 913}915. Seminar. Flight Safety Foundation, Arlington, VA, pp.
Swain, A., Guttman, H. 1983. Handbook of Human Reliability 359}369.
Analysis with Emphasis on Nuclear Poser Plan Applica- Wenner, C.L., Drury, C.G., 1996. A Uni"ed Incident Reporting
tions: Final Report (NUREG/CR-1278). United States Nu- System for Maintenance Facilities. In: Human Factors in
clear Regulatory Commission, Washington, DC. Aviation Maintenance Phase VI Progress Report. Federal
Taggert, W., 1990. Introducing CRM into maintenance training. Aviation Administration, Washington, DC.
In: Proceedings of the Third International Symposium on Woods, D., Johannesen, L., Cook, R., Sarter, N., 1995. Behind
Human Factors in Aircraft Maintenance and Inspection. Fed- Human Error: Cognitive Systems, Computers, and Hind-
eral Aviation Administration, Washington, DC, pp. 93}110. sight. Crew Systems Ergonomics Information and Analysis
Taylor, J.C., 1990. Organizational context for aircraft mainten- Center, Wright-Patterson Air Force Base, OH.
ance and inspection. In: Proceedings of the Human Factors Woods, D.D., Roth, E.M., Pople, Jr., H.E., 1987. Cognitive
34th Annual Meeting. Human Factors and Ergonomics So- Systems Engineering: An Arti"cial Intelligence Spleen for
ciety, Santa Monica, CA, pp. 1176}1180. Human Performance Assessement (NUREG-CR-4862). US
Taylor, D.H., 1987. The hermeneutics of accidents and safety. In: Nuclear Regulatory Commission, Washington, DC.
Rasmussen, J., Duncan, K., LePlat, J. (Eds.), New Techno-
logy and Human Error. Wiley, New York, NY.

You might also like