You are on page 1of 19

Personal Relationships, 14 (2007), 551–569. Printed in the United States of America.

Copyright Ó 2007 IARR. 1350-4126=07

Stress, sex, and satisfaction in marriage

GUY BODENMANN, a THOMAS LEDERMANN, a AND


THOMAS N. BRADBURY b
a
University of Fribourg, Switzerland and bUniversity of California, Los Angeles

Abstract
Using data from 198 couples, this study examines whether associations between stress occurring outside of the dyad
and key indicators of relationship functioning are mediated by stress arising within the dyad. Findings suggest that
relationship satisfaction and sexual activity are governed by hassles and problems experienced within the dyad that
are in turn related to stress arising outside the dyad. Associations between external stress and relationship function-
ing are stronger for daily hassles than for critical life events. Higher levels of daily stress predicted less sexual activ-
ity for maritally dissatisfied women and more sexual activity for maritally dissatisfied men. Self-reports of stress
covaried with self-reported indexes of satisfaction and sexuality, suggesting that contextual influences are broadly
influential in intimate relationships.

Theorists have expanded the long-standing emerge from the combination of: (a) enduring
view that interpersonal processes are a primary vulnerabilities (e.g., problematic personality
cause of marital outcomes, in recognition of traits such as neuroticism, turbulent family of
the possibility that interpersonal processes origin), (b) stressful events (e.g., major life
themselves are associated reciprocally with events, stressful circumstances, normative
the stressful events and chronic stressors to transitions), and (c) poor adaptive processes
which couples are exposed. Drawing from (e.g., inability to empathize with and support
earlier models highlighting the influence of the partner; defensive, hostile, and disengaged
minor hassles (e.g., Burr & Klein, 1994) and problem-solving skills). This perspective as-
major life events (e.g., McCubbin & Patterson, sumes that marital quality fluctuates downward
1983), these formulations aim to articulate as acute life events compromise these adaptive
how these contextual influences intersect with processes, and these fluctuations are expected
specific interactional processes and individual to be especially large when chronic stress is
difference variables to produce changes in high (Karney, Story, & Bradbury, 2005).
satisfaction and relationship stability. Karney Bodenmann’s (2000, 2005) model further
and Bradbury’s (1995; Bradbury & Karney, specifies the role of internal stress (e.g., nega-
2004) theoretical framework, derived from tive communication patterns and dyadic con-
their meta-analysis of the large literature on flicts, health problems of one partner) and
marriage, posits that distress and dissolution external stress (e.g., work stress, financial
stress, stress resulting from the family of origin
and living in impoverished neighborhoods) in
Guy Bodenmann, Institute for Family Research and Coun- marriage. This framework assumes that
seling, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland; chronic minor stresses, which originate out-
Thomas Ledermann, Institute for Family Research and side the relationship and increase the likeli-
Counseling, University of Fribourg, Fribourg, Switzerland;
Thomas N. Bradbury, Department of Psychology, Univer- hood of marital tension and conflict, are
sity of California, Los Angeles. particularly deleterious for marriage because
Correspondence should be addressed to Guy Bodenmann, they erode relationship quality slowly and
University of Fribourg, Institute for Family Research and
Counseling, Rue de Faucigny 2, CH-1700 Fribourg, often outside of conscious awareness. This
Switzerland, e-mail: joseguy.bodenmann@unifr.ch. model hypothesizes that chronic external stress

551
552 G. Bodenmann, T. Ledermann, and T. N. Bradbury

affects marital satisfaction via four mediating tionship, characterized by satisfaction with the
processes: (a) decreasing the time that partners quality and frequency of sex and by the
spend together, which in turn results in a reduc- absence of sexual dysfunction, with greater
tion of shared experiences, weakening feelings feelings of love (e.g., Hendrick & Hendrick,
of togetherness, decreasing self-disclosure, 2002), marital happiness (e.g., Brezsnyak &
and jeopardizing dyadic coping; (b) decreasing Whisman, 2004), and lower levels of marital
the quality of communication by eliciting less conflict (Metz & Epstein, 2002). Studies also
positive interaction and more negative interac- show that stress within the dyad, in the form of
tion and withdrawal; (c) increasing the risk of marital tension and conflict, covaries with
psychological and physical problems, such as lower sexual satisfaction and greater likeli-
sleep disorders, sexual dysfunction, and mood hood of sexual dysfunction (e.g., Hurlbert,
disturbances; and (d) increasing the likelihood Apt, Hurlbert, & Pierce, 2000). Surprisingly,
that problematic personality traits will be ex- Morokoff and Gillilland (1993) showed that
pressed between partners (e.g., in the form of desired frequency of sexual intercourse increa-
rigidity, anxiety, and hostility). These frame- sed with daily hassles for husbands and for
works provide the conceptual basis for the wives. Although we would not expect a posi-
present work, which tests the premise that tive association between hassles and sexual
minor and major stresses arising outside the desire, it is consistent with McCarthy’s
dyad serves as an exogenous variable that co- (2003) view that sexual activity may often
varies with internal stressors within the dyad, serve to reduce tension as couples contend
which then mediates the effects of external with stressors in everyday life or marriage.
stressors on endogenous variables, including Acute life events in the past 6 months, in
partners’ global evaluations of the marriage contrast, were unrelated to sexual functioning
and sexual intimacy. (after controlling for age) in the Morokoff and
Gillilland study, though unemployed men
experienced more difficulties in sexual perfor-
Brief review of research
mance compared to employed men.
Research addressing relationship quality and
sexual functioning is beginning to shed light
Goals of the present study
on the interplay between stress and marital
functioning. Thus, several studies show a sig- The accumulated evidence suggests that the
nificant association between higher levels of ecological niche in which couples reside is
stress and lower levels of relationship satisfac- associated with the level of satisfaction they
tion (e.g., Bodenmann, 2000, 2005; Cohan & experience and the quality of the communica-
Bradbury, 1997; Harper, Schaalje, & Sandberg, tion and physical intimacy they display. At the
2000; for a review, see Story & Bradbury, same time, a few important shortcomings of
2004); however, it seems that marital satisfac- these studies are apparent. First, most of the
tion is linked more closely to daily hassles than studies fail to differentiate between minor
to critical life events (see Williams, 1995). A forms of stress, such as daily hassles (see
5-year longitudinal study by Bodenmann and Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) and major stress,
Cina (2006) extends this work by showing that or they assess only one of these forms of stress.
daily hassles are among the most important Second, studies focusing on everyday stress
predictors of divorce. Recent studies also and daily hassles often do not distinguish
begin to outline the role that distal forms of explicitly between stress that is internal and
stress (e.g., poverty) play in expressions in external to the dyad, which is likely to inflate
warmth and hostility (Cutrona et al., 2003) correlations with marital processes and out-
and how different forms of stress can interact comes. Third, research examines the associa-
to hasten the rate at which marriages deterio- tion between stress and marriage most often in
rate (Karney et al., 2005). relation to such outcomes as marital satisfac-
Stress is also likely to affect physical inti- tion or marital communication. Relatively few
macy. Research links a satisfying sexual rela- studies examine sexuality, either by itself or in
Stress, sex, and satisfaction 553

conjunction with other marital outcomes. Hypothesis 2. We predict that actor effects
Fourth, several of these studies assess individ- (i.e., the association between the stress that
ual spouses without corresponding data from one person reports and his or her marital
their partners. As a result, studies have not functioning) will be greater in magnitude than
examined dyadic effects systematically, and parallel partner effects (i.e., the association
we know little about the reciprocal effects of between the stress that one person reports
the stress experienced by one partner on the and his or her partner’s marital functioning).
marital satisfaction and sexuality of the other. More importantly, based on findings suggest-
The present report builds on the growing ing that wives report more stress than hus-
theoretical interest in stress and marriage, bands (e.g., Bodenmann, 2000; Cohan &
and the empirical literature that supports this Bradbury, 1997) and that wives’ changes in
interest, while also addressing important limi- satisfaction appear to be more responsive to
tations in existing studies. Specifically, we dis- stress than those of husbands (e.g., Karney
tinguish between (a) stress that is external et al., 2005), we predict that the daily hassles
versus internal to the couple, (b) critical life and stress that wives experience within the
events and daily hassles, (c) actor and partner marriage will be predicted more reliably by
effects, and (d) a range of different facets of husbands’ external stress than the opposite
marital quality, including marital satisfaction, effect.
and various indexes of sexual functioning
(sexual satisfaction, activity, and dysfunction) Hypothesis 3. Although external critical
in testing the following hypotheses. life events may exert negative effects on mar-
ital quality (see Karney et al., 2005; Neff &
Hypothesis 1. As shown in Figure 1, we
Karney, 2004), we assume that negative
test the hypothesis that external stress predicts
effects of external daily hassles on marital
more hassles and higher tension within the
quality will be stronger (Bodenmann, 2005;
dyad, which in turn predicts lower relationship
Williams, 1995). This hypothesis is consistent
functioning (i.e., marital satisfaction and sex-
with Bodenmann’s contention that daily has-
ual functioning). That is, we predict that expe-
sles are particularly pernicious because they
riences of internal stress within the dyad
extract a small but persisting cost on individ-
mediate, at least partially, the relationship
uals and their relationship, often outside of
between external stress and relationship
explicit awareness.
functioning.

a3w

External stress Internal daily Endogenous E2w


a1w a2w
women stress women variables women

p1w E1w p2w


R RE1 RE2
p1m p2m
E1m

External stress Internal daily Endogenous E2m


a1m a2m
men stress men variables men

a3m

Figure 1. Actor–Partner Mediator Model with external stress as exogenous variables, internal
daily stress as mediators, and relationship functioning as endogenous variables.
554 G. Bodenmann, T. Ledermann, and T. N. Bradbury

Hypothesis 4. Following Morokoff and 2.39, p , .05; men M ¼ 35.8 (SD ¼ 17.1)
Gillilland (1993), we predict that higher levels versus M ¼ 39.4 (SD ¼ 17.9), t(77) ¼ 2.51,
of daily hassles will predict higher levels of p , .05; and reported a shorter relationship
sexual activity. As it is possible that satisfied duration, M ¼ 6.58 (SD ¼ 5.41) versus M ¼
and dissatisfied couples will vary in their 14.09 (SD ¼ 7.25), t(101) ¼ 7.50, p , .001.
capacity to manage the effects of daily stress Nonetheless, married and unmarried partici-
on their sexual interactions, we will examine pants did not significantly differ on any of
whether relationship satisfaction moderates the variables of interest, with the exception
this association. Specifically, we predict that of sexual activity (assessed with five items
higher levels of daily hassles will covary with rated on 5-point scales: 1 ¼ never; 5 ¼ very
higher levels of sexual activity of satisfied often; see below), where unmarried partici-
couples, as they are likely to possess not only pants of both genders reported a higher fre-
the interactional skills needed to discuss and quency: women M ¼ 18.1 (SD ¼ 3.5) versus
defuse daily stress but also the propensity to M ¼ 16.5 (SD ¼ 3.9), t(91) ¼ 2.47, p , .05;
engage in sexual activity when the daily has- men t(86) ¼ 3.13, p , .01.
sles in their lives subside, however temporar-
ily. Among dissatisfied couples, in contrast,
Procedure and measures
we predict that the association between daily
hassles and sexual activity will be weaker or As insufficient resources were available to con-
possibly in the opposite direction as daily duct a random-digit telephone survey of cou-
stress will not be negotiated as well and sexual ples, couples instead volunteered to participate
interaction will become less likely. in response to community-wide newspaper
advertisements placed in the German-speaking
region of Switzerland. Couples contacting the
Method
laboratory about the study were mailed a packet
of questionnaires that included separate and dis-
Participants
tinct materials for each partner, together with
Three hundred ninety-six individuals residing instructions to complete the questionnaires
in the German-speaking part of Switzerland independently and to return the forms to the
participated in the study, representing a con- institute within 2 weeks. We did not pay cou-
venience sample of 198 intact heterosexual ples for their participation as it is unusual in
couples. Among the women, 21% were 20– Switzerland to pay participants for their partic-
30 years, 54% were 31–40 years, and 25% were ipation in this kind of research. In addition to
41 years or older. Among the men, 14% were providing demographic information (age, sex,
20–30 years, 48% were 31–40 years, and 38% education, marital status, relationship duration,
were 41 or older. Although a few participants relationship satisfaction, type of residence,
ended their formal education with elementary number of children, occupation, and employ-
school (10% of the women, 7% of the men), ment status), participants completed the follow-
most earned a terminal high school degree ing measures.
(48% women, 47% of the men) or a college
or university degree (42% women, 46% men). Hassles Scale. We administered a shortened
Average relationship duration was 12.4 years and adapted version of the original Hassles
(SD ¼ 7.5; range ¼ 1.1–36.5 years), 75% of Scale (Kanner, Coyne, Schaefer, & Lazarus,
the couples (n ¼ 148) were married, and 70.4% 1981), consisting of 37 of the original 117
had children (M ¼ 1.6; SD ¼ 1.29; range ¼ items. We rephrased terms that seemed redun-
1–5). It is important to recognize that a signifi- dant (e.g., we relabeled hassles associated with
cant minority of the couples were not married. ‘‘planning meals’’ and ‘‘caring for pet’’ as has-
Comparison of married couples with not-married sles with ‘‘task sharing in household’’; we rela-
couples showed that unmarried participants beled hassles over ‘‘smoking too much’’ and
were younger, women M ¼ 33.1 (SD ¼ ‘‘use of alcohol’’ as hassles with ‘‘unhealthy
16.3) versus M ¼ 37.1 (SD ¼ 16.8), t(68) ¼ behaviors’’). Participants rated all items on
Stress, sex, and satisfaction 555

5-point scales (1 ¼ not at all stressful, 5 ¼ very riage directly in order to avoid redundancy
stressful). The items reflected a range of daily between the independent and dependent varia-
hassles (i.e., irritating, frustrating, or distressing bles studied here. Prior longitudinal studies
demands in everyday transactions) that partic- with married couples have shown that measures
ipants rated with reference to the previous of this type can yield valid data (e.g., Cohan &
month. Factor analysis of these responses Bradbury, 1997; Neff & Karney, 2004). The
yielded one factor representing stress external mean scores of this combined scale were 10.0
to the dyad (29 items, e.g., troublesome neigh- (SD ¼ 6.4, range ¼ 1–29) for women and 7.9
bors, social obligations, financial problems, (SD ¼ 5.4, range ¼ 1–35) for men, t(182) ¼
problems getting along with fellow workers, 4.34, p , .001.
customers or clients giving you a hard time,
not liking current work duties, too many inter- Partnership Questionnaire. The Partner-
ruptions, having to wait, too many things to do; ship Questionnaire (Partnerschaftsfragebogen,
internal consistency as measured by Cronbach’s or PFB; Hahlweg, 1996), a 31-item measure of
a ¼ .86) and a second representing stress marital satisfaction, consists of three sub-
internal to the dyad (8 items, e.g., problems scales: quarrelling (item examples: my partner
with your partner, overload with family respon- blames me for things that I have done in the
sibilities, time pressures in the family, demands past; my partner criticizes me in a sarcastic
of task sharing in household, different goals, way; my partner shouts at me during argu-
annoying habits of the partner; Cronbach’s a ¼ ments; a ¼ .91), affection (item examples:
.75). The mean score for external daily stress my partner makes me feel that I am physically
was 53.7 (SD ¼ 12.1, range ¼ 30–110) for attractive for him or her; my partner is affec-
women and 50.5 (SD ¼ 11.7, range ¼ 29–93) tionate toward me; a ¼ .90), and togetherness
for men, t(188) ¼ 2.80, p , .01. Mean scores (item examples: my partner shares his/her
of internal daily stress were 16.1 (SD ¼ 5.0, thoughts and feelings with me; my partner tells
range ¼ 8–39) for women and 14.4 (SD ¼ 4.3, me what he or she had experienced during the
range ¼ 8–28) for men, t(188) ¼ 4.59, p , .001. day; a ¼ .84). Items are rated on a 4-point
scale with 0 ¼ never and 3 ¼ very often. In
Life Events Questionnaire. The Life Events this study, we used only the affection and
Questionnaire (Bodenmann, 2000), based upon togetherness scales in forming an index of
the Social Readjustment Scale of Holmes and marital satisfaction (a ¼ .93). Due to medium
Rahe (1967) and the Life Experiences Survey correlations between quarrelling and internal
by Sarason, Johnson, and Siegel (1978), ass- daily stress (r ¼ .41 for women, r ¼ .48 for
esses 27 potentially stressful life events in dif- men), we excluded the subscale ‘‘quarrelling’’
ferent domains such as personal injuries (severe in order to avoid redundancy with internal
illness, handicap), experiences of loss (death of daily stress. In this study, men reported an
a loved one), work-related events (unemploy- average score of 61.6 on the PFB and women
ment, loss of work), and social conflicts (severe of 61.3, t(189) ¼ 0.32, ns, indicating nondis-
marital distress, severe social tensions with rel- tressed couples, with considerable variation
atives, friends, neighbors or colleagues at work) (for men, SD ¼ 15.4; range: 15–90; for women,
within the past 12 months. Participants indi- SD ¼ 15, range: 10–90).
cated the degree of stress caused by these life
events on a 3-point scale (1 ¼ somewhat stress- Sexual satisfaction subscale of the Marital
ful, 2 ¼ stressful, 3 ¼ very stressful). The total Satisfaction Inventory. The Marital Satisfac-
score of the scale used in this study was the tion Inventory (Snyder, 1981; German transla-
combined measure of the occurrence of critical tion by Klann, Hahlweg, & Hank, 1992) is
life events multiplied by their stress impact a 150-item true–false self-report questionnaire
(Cronbach’s a ¼ .67). In this version, we designed to assess the nature and extent of mar-
included only external major stressors (i.e., ital distress along 11 key dimensions (e.g.,
unemployment, death of a friend, changing effective communication, marital aggression,
place of domicile) that did not involve the mar- sexual dissatisfaction, agreement on finances,
556 G. Bodenmann, T. Ledermann, and T. N. Bradbury

and conflict over child rearing). In this study, measure was 22.6 (SD ¼ 13.7, range ¼ 6–73)
we used only the 19-item scale measuring for women and 14.0 (SD ¼ 9.2, range ¼ 2–77)
sexual dissatisfaction, examples, my partner for men, t(181) ¼ 7.16, p , .001.
sometimes shows too little enthusiasm for
sex; my partner has too little regard some- Symptom Check List. Derogatis (1992)
times for my sexual satisfaction: women, developed Symptom Check List (SCL–90–R),
M ¼ 8.2, SD ¼ 3.0, range ¼ 0–12; men, a 90-item instrument to assess a broad range of
M ¼ 7.7, SD ¼ 3.3, range ¼ 0–12; t(187) ¼ psychological problems and symptoms of psy-
2.69, p , .01. We recoded this scale so that chopathology. This instrument permits evalu-
higher scores indicated higher satisfaction. ation of a variety of concepts, including
The internal consistency of the scale was .82. somatization, obsessive–compulsive problems,
interpersonal sensitivity, depression, anxiety
Sexual Activity Scale. Designed for this and hostility. Users can employ three global
study, this scale assesses the typical frequency scores in research as well as clinical practice.
of sexual behaviors with five items adminis- In this study, we used the global severity index
tered on a 5-point scale (1 ¼ never, 2 ¼ rarely, to control for a general bias in the perception
3 ¼ from time to time, 4 ¼ frequently, 5 ¼ very of the participants with regard to neuroticism.
often). Behaviors assessed include petting, Participants rated this on a 5-point scale (0 ¼
partner stimulation (massages), oral sex, and not at all, 4 ¼ extremely). The reliability of
sexual intercourse. The internal consistency of this scale was a ¼ .96. The mean score was
the scale was a ¼ .86. The mean score was 49.8 (SD ¼ 40.4, range ¼ 0–233) for women
17.0 (SD ¼ 3.81, range ¼ 7–25) for women and 30.0 (SD ¼ 25.6, range ¼ 0–145) for men,
and 16.4 (SD ¼ 3.75, range ¼ 5–25) for men, t(189) ¼ 6.47, p , .001.
t(187) ¼ 2.83, p , .01.
Results
Sexual Dysfunction Scale. Following the
diagnostic categories of Diagnostic and Statis-
Bivariate correlations among measures
tical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed.
(DSM–IV; American Psychiatric Association, Intercorrelations among the study variables are
1994), men and women rated several potential shown in Table 1. Several associations are
sexual problems, indicating (a) how often they noteworthy. First, relationship satisfaction,
experienced these problems in the relationship sexual satisfaction, and sexual activity inter-
(5-point scale: never, rarely, from time to time, correlate reliably. Second, sexual dysfunction
often, very often) and (b) how much pain these appears to be a distinct variable, with low lev-
problems caused (4-point scale: no pain, mod- els of association with other sexual function-
erate pain, high pain, very high pain). Women ing variables and a nonsignificant association
rated sexual desire problems (hypoactive sex- between partners. Third, factor analysis of the
ual desire), sexual aversion problems, sexual hassles measure produced distinguishable
arousal problems, orgasmic problems, dyspar- indexes of hassles that are internal and external
eunia, and vaginismus; men rated sexual desire to the dyad. These two variables correlate
problems (hypoactive sexual desire), sexual among men and among women, as we would
aversion problems, erectile problems, orgas- expect (rs ¼ .60 and .63, respectively), yet the
mic problems, premature ejaculation, and sex- between-partner correlations are higher for
ual pain problems. In this study, we used internal hassles (r ¼ .46) than for external
a combined measure encompassing the fre- hassles (r ¼ .11), thus helping to validate the
quency of the sexual problems and the self- distinction we are drawing between them.
perceived pain. Cronbach’s alpha of this Fourth, acute life events covary reliably with
variable was less than optimal (.68 for women the external stress measure, yet these measures
and .60 for men), suggesting that these dys- share less than 20% of their variance, suggest-
functions do not represent a common underlying ing that these are not redundant measures.
construct. The average score on the combined Fifth, higher levels of marital satisfaction do
Stress, sex, and satisfaction 557

covary with lower levels of internal daily

1.00***
2.26***

.63***
2.20**
2.15*

2.16*
stress (r ¼ 2.35 for men; r ¼ 2.45 for
10

2.08

.02
.03

.00
women). This does suggest a modest degree
of confounding between these measures. In
Table 1. Intercorrelations among study variables, for women (above diagonal) and men (below diagonal) and dyads (along the diagonal)

sum, the study variables are generally per-

.73***
2.25***

.62***
2.17**
2.17**
forming as expected and further multivariate
2.14*

.07
2.01
2.02
.07
9

analyses are warranted. As relationship dura-


tion was not highly correlated with any depen-
dent variable, we did not control for it. It is,

.25***
2.29***
2.37***
2.35***
.41***
.65***
.61***
.30***

Note. SCL–90–R ¼ Symptom Check List. N ¼ 198 men and 198 women. We present correlations between the dyad members in bold along the diagonal.
however, conceivable that personal vulnera-

.00
2.07
8

bility creates a general bias with regard to par-


ticipants’ ratings of stress as well as their
evaluation of marital functioning. Intercorre-
.39***
.44***
.34***

.30***
2.20**

lations among the study variables and the


2.13*

.17*
2.03

2.11
2.11
7

SCL–90–R total score ranged between j.29j


(marital satisfaction) and j.65j (internal daily
stress) in women and between j.14j (sexual
2.23***
2.28***
2.24***
.33***
.63***

.36***
.48***

activity) and j.48j (external and internal daily


.11

.00
2.06
6

stress) in men. To control for this effect, we


partialled out overall psychological distress
(SCL–90–R total score) from all self-report
.46***
2.45***
2.49***
2.45***
.43***

.60***
.36***
.48***

data.
.09
.06
5

Statistical analyses: Investigating mediation


with dyadic data
2.44***
2.27***

.27***

.29***
.42***
.19**
2.15*

We tested hypotheses using the Actor–Partner


.03

.09
.01
4

Mediator Model (see Campbell, Simpson,


Kashy, & Fletcher, 2001; Fletcher & Thomas,
2000; Ledermann & Bodenmann, 2006),
.65***
.74***
.63***

2.27***

2.22**
2.16*
2.14*
2.14*

which allows for the analysis of mediator


2.10

2.04
3

effects in studies using dyadic data. This


model is an extended version of the widely
used Actor–Partner Interdependence Model
.64***
.56***

.63***

2.45***
2.23***
2.23***
2.30***
2.22**

(APIM) proposed by Kenny and colleagues


2.13*
2.09
2

(e.g., Kenny, 1996; Kenny & Cook, 1999),


which takes into account the interdependence
of data collected from dyadic partners. The
.64***
.68***
.73***

2.35***

2.31***

*p , .05. **p , .01. ***p , .001 (one tailed).


2.21**
2.22**

classic APIM yields an estimation of the effect


2.12*

2.15*
2.07
1

of one’s own independent variable on one’s


own dependent variable (actor effect) and on
the partner’s dependent variable (partner
10. Duration of relationship

effect). Using manifest variables, the APIM


8. SCL–90–R total score

is a saturated model with 0 df. The Actor–


6. External daily stress
4. Sexual dysfunctions
5. Internal daily stress
1. Marital satisfaction
2. Sexual satisfaction

7. Critical life events

Partner Mediator Model outlined in Figure 1


3. Sexual activity

consists of six pairwise variables (three per


partner), including two manifest exogenous
(independent) variables, two manifest mediator
Variables

variables, and two manifest endogenous


9. Age

(dependent) variables. The path models used


in this study with direct actor effects (horizontal
558 G. Bodenmann, T. Ledermann, and T. N. Bradbury

arrows) between the exogenous and endoge- significant, however, the relationship between
nous variables have 2 df; we can assume that the exogenous and endogenous variables is
direct partner effects (diagonal arrows) be- completely mediated by internal stress.
tween exogenous and endogenous variables We tested differences between parameters
are statistically irrelevant. If the path models (e.g., actor vs. partner effects) within one
with 2 df fit the data well, then this assumption model by means of model comparisons com-
is verified. The assumption of complete medi- paring the default model with a nested model
ation is—assuming good model fit—sup- assuming equal parameters. We will assume
ported if the direct effects between external that parameters are statistically different when
stress and the marital variables are not signif- the chi-square difference test is significant.
icant. Partial mediation can be inferred in the We tested differences between coefficients
association between the exogenous and endo- across different models by computing the
geneous variables if one or both of these direct 95% confidence limits using Fisher’s Z trans-
effects are significant. formation. We can say that a substantial dif-
We used the z statistic to evaluate the medi- ference exists if the confidence interval of one
ation effects between the exogenous and effect excludes the coefficient of another effect.
endogenous variables, with

^ Actor–Partner Models with external daily


^b
a
z¼ ð1Þ stress as exogenous variables
^ a^b^
r
The estimated maximum likelihood coeffi-
where a ^ denotes the estimated indirect
^b cients of the Actor–Partner Mediator Models
effect between X (exogenous variable) and Y with external daily stress as exogenous varia-
(endogenous variable) through M (mediator), bles and internal daily stress as the mediator
and r ^ a^b^ represents the estimated standard are shown in Table 2. To evaluate the fit of
error of the indirect effect. The standard error a particular structural model, we use chi-
will be estimated by Sobel’s (1982) approxi- square, the comparative fit index (CFI),
mate formula that MacKinnon and his col- root mean square error of approximation
leagues (MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, (RMSEA), and the fit criteria proposed by
West, & Sheets, 2002; MacKinnon, Warsi, & Hu and Bentler (1999), who suggest a cut-
Dwyer, 1995) recommend: off ..95 for CFI and ,.06 for RMSEA.
According to Browne and Cudeck (1993),
rffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffiffi
RMSEA values , .08 indicate an acceptable
^ a^b^
r ^2 r
¼ a ^ 2^ 1 b ^2 r^2 ð2Þ
b ^
a fit, and values , .05 indicate a good fit. As
readers can see in Table 2 (bottom), all models
where a ^ denote the estimated structural
^ and b with daily stress fit the data well. These find-
coefficients of the path X / M, and M / Y, ings, which indicate that the relations between
^ 2a^ and r
respectively, r ^ 2b^ are the estimated var- external daily stress and marital functioning
iances of a^ and b.^ are at least partially mediated by internal daily
The hypothesis holds that internal daily stress, are consistent with Hypothesis 1.
stress mediates the relation between exoge-
nous variables (represented here as external Association between external daily stress and
stress) and endogenous variables (represented internal daily stress. As shown at the top
by marital functioning variables) when (a) the left side of Table 2, actor effects relating
models show an adequate fit, (b) the direct external daily stress and internal daily stress
effects constituting a mediation effect are sig- were statistically significant, for women and
nificant, and (c) the mediation effect is sig- for men. As partner effects were not signifi-
nificant using Equations 1 and 2. When the cant and as women’s actor effect were signif-
direct actor effects between external stress icantly higher than the partner effect from men
(exogenous variable) and marital function- to women (v2Diff ¼ 9:45, p ¼ .002), we found
ing variables (endogenous variables) are not that actor effects were more important than the
Table 2. Actor and partner effects (maximum likelihood estimates), correlations, and model fits for the Actor–Partner Mediator Model with internal
daily stress as mediator and the overall distress (Symptom Check List [SCL–90–R]) as covariables
Models with external daily stress Models with acute life events
Marital Sexual Sexual Sexual Marital Sexual Sexual Sexual
Source satisfaction satisfaction activity dysfunction satisfaction satisfaction activity dysfunction
External stress / internal daily stress
Actor effects
Stress, sex, and satisfaction

Women, a 1w .38*** .38*** .38*** .38*** .25*** .25*** .25*** .25***


Men, a 1m .49*** .49*** .49*** .49*** .29*** .29*** .29*** .29***
Partner effects
Men / women, p 1w .10 .10 .10 .10 .23*** .23*** .23*** .23***
Women / men, p 1m .00 .00 .00 .00 2.08 2.08 2.08 2.08
Internal daily stress / endogenous variable
Actor effects
Women, a 2w 2.31*** 2.33*** 2.27*** .19** 2.29*** 2.31*** 2.25*** .20**
Men, a 2m 2.33*** 2.35*** 2.25*** .14* 2.22*** 2.33*** 2.18** .11
Partner effects
Men / women, p 2w 2.23*** 2.13* 2.20** .12 2.24*** 2.13* 2.21** .12
Women / men, p 2m 2.26*** 2.23*** 2.22** 2.04 2.27*** 2.23*** 2.23** 2.03
External stress / endogenous variable
Actor effects
Women, a 3w .05 .04 .06 .03 Fixed at 0 Fixed at 0 Fixed at 0 Fixed at 0
Men, a 3m .20** .04 .15* 2.08 Fixed at 0 Fixed at 0 Fixed at 0 Fixed at 0
Correlations
Between exogenous variables, R .01 .02 .01 .02 .34*** .34*** .34*** .34***
Between error terms 1, R E1 .31*** .31*** .31*** .31*** .27*** .27*** .27*** .27***
Between error terms 2, R E2 .52*** .55*** .60*** Fixed at 0 .52*** .55*** .60*** Fixed at 0
Explained variances
R 2w internal daily stress women (%) 16 16 16 16 16 16 16 16
R 2m internal daily stress men (%) 24 24 24 24 8 8 8 8
R 2w endogenous variable women (%) 19 14 14 7 18 14 14 7
559

(continued)
560 G. Bodenmann, T. Ledermann, and T. N. Bradbury

corresponding partner effects. This suggests

dysfunction

Note. Due to a poor model fit, the estimates of the model with critical life events and sexual satisfaction cannot be interpreted. The SCL–90–R total score was partialed out from all manifest
Sexual
that internal daily stress may be affected more

8.511

.130
.958
.060
by one’s own external daily stress than by the

5
partner’s external daily stress.
Models with acute life events

Association between internal daily stress


activity
Sexual

8.969

.062
.975
.079
and relationship functioning. Actor effects
were consistent in demonstrating that higher
11

levels of internal daily stress covaried with


lower levels of marital satisfaction, sexual sat-
satisfaction
Sexual

isfaction, and sexual activity, and with higher


11.004

.027
.967
.094

levels of sexual dysfunction, for men and for


21

women. With the exception of the sexual dys-


function variable, we found significant part-
satisfaction

ner effects between internal daily stress and


Marital

relationship functioning. Specifically, to the


5.169

.270
.994
.039

extent that one spouse reported more daily


16

stress and tension in the relationship, the part-


ner was more likely to report lower levels of
dysfunction

marital satisfaction, sexual satisfaction, and


Sexual

1.656

.647
1.000
.000

sexual activity. As a group, these findings


are in line with our first hypothesis that daily
1

stress and tension within the dyad, as reported


Models with external daily stress

both by one’s self and the partner, covary with


activity
Sexual

poorer relationship functioning.


0.536

.765
1.000
.000
12

Association between external daily stress and


relationship functioning. With respect to
satisfaction

direct actor effects between external daily


Sexual

.362

.009
2.035

1.000

stress and indexes of relationship func-


tioning variables, we found no significant
21

associations for women, but two significant


associations—involving marital satisfaction
satisfaction

and sexual activity—for men. These direct


Marital

0.144

1.000
.000

associations were positive, indicating that


19

2
930

men who reported higher levels of external


daily stress also reported higher levels of
marital satisfaction and sexual activity. This
Root mean square error of approximation

*p , .05. **p , .01. ***p , .001 (one tailed).

finding has to be considered in the context of


the mediator model analyses because the cor-
R 2m endogenous variable men (%)

relations between external daily stress and the


two outcome variables sexual activity and
marital satisfaction are negative (see Table 1).
Among men, these positive direct effects are
Comparative fit index
Table 2. (continued)

substantially lower than the effects between


external daily stress and internal daily stress
and between internal daily stress and the two
outcome variables marital satisfaction and
variables.

sexual activity (see Table 2). It is noteworthy


Source

that these effects may not be separate and


v2
df
p

distinct as the bivariate correlation between


Stress, sex, and satisfaction 561

marital satisfaction and sexual activity is .74 ables, to test the prediction that acute life
for women and .73 for men (see Table 1). events are less consequential than daily hassles
in predicting daily stress within the relation-
Tests of mediation. To evaluate mediation ship (Hypothesis 3). The first model analyses
effects, Equations 1 and 2 were used to test for reveal poor fits for all models with direct paths
significance. In the Actor–Partner Mediator between the exogenous and endogenous vari-
Model tested here, we can distinguish eight ables (RMSEA ranged between .081 and .132),
indirect effects (two actor–actor, two actor– except for the model with sexual dysfunctions
partner, two partner–actor, and two partner– as endogenous variables, v2(3) ¼ 2.06, p ¼
partner). In Table 3 we present the estimated .561; CFI ¼ 1.000, RMSEA ¼ .000. Because
indirect effects, standard errors, and the results the two direct effects between the exogenous
of the z statistics for the mediation effects con- and endogenous variables were small in all four
sisting of two significant direct effects. In all models, they were excluded. The model with-
four models with external daily stress as exog- out direct effects between the exogenous and
enous variables, both indirect effects involving endogenous variables counts 4 df and assumes
two actor effects were significant. There was that the relations between acute life events and
one exception, however: In the model with sex- the marital outcome variables are completely
ual dysfunction, we found no significant actor– mediated by internal daily stress. Poor fits were
actor effect for men, which was mainly due to obtained for the model that included sexual sat-
a weak actor effect from internal daily stress isfaction and thus we do not discuss this model
to sexual dysfunctions in men. Apart from the further. In contrast, we obtained good fits for
model with sexual dysfunction, both actor– the models that included marital satisfaction
partner indirect effects were substantial. The and sexual activity—with respect to RMSEA—
other four mediator effects including partner an acceptable fit resulted for the model that in-
effects between external and internal daily cluded sexual dysfunction (see Table 2, bottom).
stress were not significant in all four models
with external daily stress as exogenous variables. Association between acute life events and
Consistent with Hypothesis 1, these find- internal daily stress. As shown in Table 2,
ings suggest that couples experiencing higher in the remaining three models, all actor effects
levels of external daily stress also experience were substantially weaker than corresponding
higher levels of stress and tension within the effects between external daily stress and inter-
dyad and, in turn, lower levels of relationship nal daily stress (.25 vs. .38 for women and .29
and sexual satisfaction, sexual activity, and (to vs. .49 for men; see Table 2). Given the 95%
a moderate extent) more sexual dysfunctions. confidence limits of the actor effects between
With respect to the distinction between partial external and internal daily stress (.27 and .55 for
and complete mediation, the findings suggest women, and .40 and .68 for men), the effects
that the relations between external daily stress between critical life events and internal daily
and sexual satisfaction and between external stress were significantly lower than those
daily stress and sexual dysfunction are com- obtained between external daily stress and inter-
pletely mediated, whereas the associations nal daily stress. (This was also true for men but
between external daily stress and marital sat- not for women when using the 95% confidence
isfaction and sexual activity are partially medi- limit of the coefficients between critical events
ated by internal daily stress due to substantial and internal daily stress, which were .12 and .40
direct actor effects between the exogenous and for women and .16 and .44 for men.)
endogenous variables in men. Partner effects were also apparent, though
only in the case of acute life events reported by
men and internal daily stress reported by
Actor–Partner Models with acute life events
women. The confidence limits of .10 and .38
as exogenous variables
for the effect from men to women and 2.22
The analyses just described were repeated and .06 for the effect from women to men sup-
using acute life events as the exogenous vari- port Hypothesis 2: The association between
562 G. Bodenmann, T. Ledermann, and T. N. Bradbury

Table 3. Mediation effects for the Actor–Partner Mediator Models (APMeM) with external
stress as exogenous variables, internal daily stress as mediators, and marital functioning as
endogenous variables

p (two 95% confidence


Effect IE SE z tailed) interval
APMeM with external daily stress and marital satisfaction
Xw / Mw / Yw ¼ A / A 20.200 0.058 23.465 .001 20.31 to 20.09
Xw / Mw / Ym ¼ A / P 20.167 0.053 23.161 .002 20.27 to 20.06
Xm / Mm / Yw ¼ A / P 20.174 0.057 23.065 .002 20.29 to 20.06
Xm / Mm / Ym ¼ A / A 20.242 0.065 23.737 .000 20.37 to 20.12
APMeM with external daily stress and sexual satisfaction
Xw / Mw / Yw ¼ A / A 21.061 0.302 23.512 .000 21.65 to 20.47
Xw / Mw / Ym ¼ A / P 20.867 0.298 22.911 .004 21.45 to 20.28
Xm / Mm / Yw ¼ A / P 20.485 0.275 21.763 .078 21.02 to 0.05
Xm / Mm / Ym ¼ A / A 21.497 0.378 23.960 .000 22.24 to 20.76
APMeM with external daily stress and sexual activity
Xw / Mw / Yw ¼ A / A 21.152 0.371 23.109 .002 21.88 to 20.43
Xw / Mw / Ym ¼ A / P 20.964 0.363 22.657 .008 21.68 to 20.25
Xm / Mm / Yw ¼ A / P 20.984 0.372 22.648 .008 21.71 to 20.26
Xm / Mm / Ym ¼ A / A 21.274 0.430 22.960 .003 22.12 to 20.43
APMeM with external daily stress and sexual dysfunction
Xw / Mw / Yw ¼ A / A 2.707 1.238 2.187 .029 0.28 to 5.13
Xw / Mw / Ym ¼ A / P 20.358 0.790 20.453 .651 21.91 to 1.19
Xm / Mm / Yw ¼ A / P 1.965 1.282 1.532 .125 20.55 to 4.48
Xm / Mm / Ym ¼ A / A 1.649 1.014 1.625 .104 20.34 to 3.64
APMeM with critical life events and marital satisfaction
Xw / Mw / Yw ¼ A / A 20.007 0.002 22.743 .006 20.01 to 0.00
Xw / Mw / Ym ¼ A / P 20.006 0.002 22.602 .009 20.01 to 0.00
Xm / Mw / Yw ¼ P / A 20.007 0.003 22.537 .011 20.01 to 0.00
Xm / Mw / Ym ¼ P / P 20.007 0.003 22.424 .015 20.01 to 0.00
Xm / Mm / Yw ¼ A / P 20.007 0.003 22.550 .011 20.01 to 0.00
Xm / Mm / Ym ¼ A / A 20.007 0.003 22.425 .015 20.01 to 0.00
APMeM with critical life events and sexual activity
Xw / Mw / Yw ¼ A / A 20.038 0.015 22.506 .012 20.07 to 20.01
Xw / Mw / Ym ¼ A / P 20.036 0.016 22.301 .021 20.07 to 20.01
Xm / Mw / Yw ¼ P / A 20.040 0.017 22.330 .020 20.07 to 20.01
Xm / Mw / Ym ¼ P / P 20.038 0.017 22.163 .031 20.07 to 0.00
Xm / Mm / Yw ¼ A / P 20.042 0.018 22.326 .020 20.08 to 20.01
Xm / Mm / Ym ¼ A / A 20.037 0.018 22.059 .040 20.07 to 0.00
APMeM with critical life events and sexual dysfunctions
Xw / Mw / Yw ¼ A / A 0.103 0.048 2.150 .032 0.01 to 0.20
Xm / Mw / Yw ¼ P / A 0.109 0.053 2.038 .042 0.00 to 0.21

Note. In this table, we present mediation effects only for those models in which both direct effects were significant. IE ¼
indirect effect; w ¼ women; m ¼ men; A ¼ actor effect; P ¼ partner effect. Equation 1 was used to compute z scores;
standard error was estimated by means of Equation 2. The formula used to compute normal 95% confidence interval is
^ ^
cb61:96 r^ ^cb^ .
Stress, sex, and satisfaction 563

women’s internal daily stress and men’s acute satisfaction and between external daily stress
life events is stronger than the association and sexual activity in men. As in the first set
between men’s internal daily stress and wom- of analyses, there were no partner effects relat-
en’s acute life events. ing internal daily stress to sexual dysfunction,
As noted, Hypothesis 3 predicts that exter- for men or for women.
nal daily hassles would account for more var-
iation in internal daily stress than would acute Tests of mediation. As above, Equations 1
life events. Partially consistent with this pre- and 2 were used to test mediation effects. In all
diction, the results summarized in Table 2 three models, both actor–actor mediation
show that external daily stress accounted for effects were significant, with the exception
16% of the variation in women’s internal daily of the model with acute events, where only
stress and 24% of the variation in men’s inter- women’s actor–actor indirect effects were sub-
nal daily stress; corresponding figures for stantial (Table 3). In the model with marital
acute life events were 16% and 8%, respec- satisfaction and sexual activity, six of the eight
tively. Given that partners are more similar mediation effects were significant. Nonsignif-
in their reports of acute life events (r ¼ .39) icant mediation effects involved the effect
than their reports of external daily hassles (r ¼ from women’s life events to men’s internal
.11; see Table 1), this pattern of results is con- stress. In the model with sexual dysfunction,
sistent with the notion that acute life events are only the association between men’s critical
more likely to be shared experiences and hence events and men’s sexual dysfunction via wom-
are more likely to be recognized mutually, pro- en’s internal stress was significant.
ducing less turmoil within the dyad in turn.
Daily external hassles, in contrast, are more Marital satisfaction as a moderator of the
likely to be unshared experiences that are man- association between daily external hassles
aged at the individual level and thus may and sexual activity. In an effort to build
evoke greater tension in the partners. upon Morokoff and Gillilland’s (1993) finding
that daily hassles and sexual activity would
Association between internal daily stress and covary positively, in Hypothesis 4 we pre-
relationship functioning. Actor effects relat- dicted that this association would be moder-
ing internal daily stress to marital satisfaction, ated by marital satisfaction. We tested the
sexual activity, and sexual dysfunction were Actor–Partner Moderator Model (Campbell,
comparable in magnitude to corresponding Simpson, Kashy, & Rholes, 2001; Ledermann &
effects found in the models with external daily Bodenmann, 2006) in which men’s and wom-
stress except the associations between men’s en’s sexual activity were predicted by marital
internal stress and men’s marital satisfaction satisfaction and external hassles of both partners,
and sexual activity that appeared to be weaker together with the interaction between satisfac-
in the model with acute life events than the tion and hassles for each partner. To avoid multi-
parallel associations obtained in the model with collinearity, the predictors (external daily stress)
external daily stress (2.22 vs. 2.33, and 2.18 and moderators (marital satisfaction) were cen-
vs. 2.25). Partner effects relating internal daily tered as recommended by Aiken and West
stress to marital satisfaction were also evident (1991) among others. This Actor–Partner Mod-
for men and for women. The differences in the erator Model fit the data well (v2 ¼ 13.06, df ¼
magnitude of the effects in the models with 9, p , .16; CFI ¼ .991, RMSEA ¼ .048). Sig-
external daily stress and the models with critical nificant actor and partner effects were obtained
events derived from the different model speci- for women’s marital satisfaction (.50, p , .001;
fication (one with direct actor effects between .28, p , .001, respectively) and for men’s mar-
external stress and marital functioning having 2 ital satisfaction (.56, p , .001; .33, p , .001,
df, the other without direct actor effects respectively). Most importantly, the actor effects
between the exogenous and endogenous varia- relating the interaction of daily hassles and mar-
bles having 4 df) and the substantial direct actor ital satisfaction to sexual activity were signifi-
effects between external daily stress and marital cant for women (.10, p , .05) and for men
564 G. Bodenmann, T. Ledermann, and T. N. Bradbury

(2.13, p , .01); corresponding partner effects for women (top) and for men (bottom) show
were nonsignificant. that the moderating effects of satisfaction on
Following Cohen, Cohen, West, and Aiken the association between daily hassles and
(2003), Figure 2 presents these interactions by sexual activity take different forms for women
showing the associations between daily has- and for men. Among the most satisfied
sles and sexual activity for individuals 1 SD women, sexual activity does not appear to vary
above the mean, at the mean, and 1 SD below much as a function of daily hassles. In contrast,
the mean in marital satisfaction. The figures among women who are at or 1 SD below the

25.0

20.0
Wife's sexual activity

15.0

10.0

5.0

0.0
-2 2
Wife's external stress centered
low marital satisfaction medium marital satisfaction
high marital satisfaction

25.0
Husband's sexual activity

20.0

15.0

10.0

5.0

0.0
-2 2
Husband's external stress centered
low marital satisfaction medium marital satisfaction
high marital satisfaction

Figure 2. Association between external daily stress and sexual activity as moderated by level of
marital satisfaction, for women (top) and for men (bottom).
Note. This model was tested using two measures of external stress (i.e., external daily hassles and
acute life events), one mediator (daily stress and tension arising within the relationship), and four
variables hypothesized to reflect relationship functioning (i.e., marital satisfaction, sexual sat-
isfaction, sexual activity, and sexual dysfunction).
Stress, sex, and satisfaction 565

satisfaction mean, sexual activity tends to out reference to the contexts in which couples
decline with increasing levels of daily hassles. reside and specifically argue for distinguishing
This pattern of results is not surprising, in the between stressors that are external and internal
sense that stress does not appear to have adverse to the intimate dyad (see Bodenmann, 2000,
effects on women’s sexual activity when their 2005; Neff & Karney, 2004). Data were col-
satisfaction is high and that higher levels of lected from both partners in 198 couples in
stress appear to covary with lower levels of established relationships, and actor and partner
sexual activity when their satisfaction is lower. effects were analyzed in all models.
Among men as a group, as with women, the Results support previous findings indicat-
sexual activity of relatively satisfied men does ing that stress might play an important role
not appear to vary much as a function of their in understanding marital functioning. Three
daily hassles. For men who are at or 1 SD sets of associations support this claim. First,
below the mean in marital satisfaction, sexual whether measured as daily hassles or acute life
activity tends to increase as daily hassles events, partners reporting higher levels of
increase. Thus, the sexual activity of satisfied stress arising outside the dyad also reported
men, like that of maritally satisfied women, higher levels of stress and tension within the
appears to be relatively independent of daily dyad. Partner effects were also evident, par-
hassles. But unlike the pattern obtained for ticularly for women’s outcomes, indicating
women, higher levels of daily hassles corre- that external stress reported by men covaried
spond with higher levels of sexual activity when more strongly with wives’ experience of daily
men are at or below the sample mean in satis- relationship tension than vice versa. Second,
faction, and this association appears to be stron- spouses experiencing more stress as arising
ger among those men with lower satisfaction. within the relationship tended to have lower
In short, our most basic prediction in levels of marital satisfaction, sexual satisfac-
Hypothesis 4—that higher levels of daily has- tion, and sexual activity, and higher levels of
sles will covary with higher levels of sexual sexual dysfunction, and they tended to have
activity—was supported. In contrast, our pre- partners with lower levels of marital satisfac-
dictions about the moderating role of marital tion, sexual satisfaction, and sexual activity,
satisfaction were only partly correct: Levels of though not a higher level of sexual dysfunc-
sexual activity in the face of daily hassles were tion. Third, in the models suggesting partial
not higher among the most satisfied couples, mediation, men reporting more daily hassles
and sexual activity declined with increases in external to the relationship reported more mar-
daily hassles only for relatively distressed ital satisfaction and more sexual activity.
women but not for relatively distressed men. These two latter results were relatively
weak though reliable, and they permitted tests
of internal stress as a mediator of associations
Discussion
between either form of external stress and rela-
This study evaluated the associations among tionship outcomes. In general, results provided
stressors arising outside of marriage (in the form support for the mediational framework out-
of acute life events and daily stress), stressors lined in Figure 1 and represented by Hypoth-
arising within the relationship (in the form of esis 1. At least with the cross-sectional design
daily relationship stresses and strains), and re- used here, it seems that internal stress mediates
lationship functioning (indexed by marital the association between two forms of contex-
satisfaction and sexual variables). We tested tual variables—external daily hassles and crit-
mediational hypotheses using the Actor–Partner ical life events—and key aspects of marital
Mediator Model proposed by Ledermann and functioning such as marital satisfaction and
Bodenmann (2006), which extends the APIM sexuality. We can infer that different goals of
(e.g., Kenny, 1996) often used with dyadic data. the partners, divergent needs, and annoying
In undertaking this study, we aimed to test habits (operationalized here as stress arising
recent theoretical statements that assert that inti- within the dyad) often become harmful for
mate relationships cannot be understood with- the relationship when partners are stressed by
566 G. Bodenmann, T. Ledermann, and T. N. Bradbury

external circumstances; when external stresses because acute life events were assessed over
are reduced, these same relationship issues be- a 12-month period and were likely to be a less
come less salient to partners and their relation- salient influence on couples. Second, diary data
ship functioning can improve (Bodenmann, on the environmental challenges and interper-
2000; Neff & Karney, 2004). Of course, anal- sonal processes implicated here would help to
yses of samples in which couples are under pinpoint the ways in which stress operates on
unusually high levels of external stress might intimate relationships.
yield different results. It is possible that the Despite evidence that wives reported higher
levels of external stress studied here were not mean levels of all forms of stress in this study
sufficiently destabilizing or detrimental to the (acute life events Cohen’s d ¼ 0.32, external
marital interaction behaviors couples would daily hassles Cohen’s d ¼ 0.20, internal daily
typically use to manage stressful events and hassles Cohen’s d ¼ 0.33), husbands did not
circumstances. appear to pay a price for this: Partner effects
Three additional findings merit detailed relating wives’ internal daily stress to hus-
discussion. First, consistent with Hypothesis bands’ marital satisfaction (2.26), sexual sat-
2, the results of this study demonstrate that isfaction (2.23), and sexual activity (2.22)
various indexes of external stress are not all were comparable to parallel effects relating
associated to the same degree with relationship husbands’ internal stress to these same vari-
functioning. In particular, associations between able for wives (2.23, 2.13, and 2.20, respec-
external daily stress and tension experienced tively, for models involving external daily
within the dyad were stronger than those stress). We can speculate that wives may be
involving acute life events (cf. Bodenmann, particularly good at absorbing and not radiat-
2000; Morokoff & Gillilland, 1993; Williams, ing the stress that husbands encounter outside
1995). Why might this be? First, almost by def- the relationship and that wives are particularly
inition, partners experience the accumulation of adept at identifying moments at which support
everyday stress more frequently, and this can efforts are most needed. This possibility is
exert a greater influence on the equilibrium that consistent with recent research showing that
couples seek to establish. Second, spouses may whereas men and women are quite similar in
find it easier to attribute their internal stresses to delivering social support to one another in
salient life events than to more subtle and ordi- laboratory settings (e.g., Pasch & Bradbury,
nary daily hassles. Couples may in turn invoke 1998), women outpace men in how respon-
distinct coping efforts that will allow them to sive they are to the partner’s need for support
manage and resolve these acute events. Third, (Neff & Karney, 2005). Understanding whe-
external daily hassles may elicit less empathy ther the burden of absorbing the partner’s
and understanding from the partner, as the part- stress and providing better support than they
ner might not be aware of these hassles, their receive is detrimental for the long-term health
objectively rated intensity is typically low, and of women (e.g., in the form of depression) is
the impact of this kind of stress is likely to be beyond the scope of this study, though future
underestimated. The present findings are there- studies might benefit from investigating longer
fore in line with the transactional stress theory term health outcomes in relation to men’s
of Lazarus and Folkman (1984), which main- daily stress and women’s interpersonal res-
tains that even an event that is objectively rated ponses to this stress.
as trivial (such as missing the bus, coming late A third key finding provided support for the
to an appointment, being criticized by a supe- idea that higher levels of stress can enhance
rior) may cause a great deal of stress that can couple functioning (Hypothesis 4). Higher lev-
spill over to affect one’s work life and social els of external daily stress did predict higher
relationships. At least two design modifications levels of stress within the dyad but to a lesser
are needed to understand these associations degree daily stress also predicted higher levels
more fully. First, external daily stress, measured of satisfaction and sexual activity for men. The
as a proximal variable, may have covaried more positive association between daily stress and
highly than acute life events with internal stress sexual activity is consistent with an earlier
Stress, sex, and satisfaction 567

study by Morokoff and Gillilland (1993; the husband is higher in negative affectivity;
also see McCarthy, 2003), though here we men, however, display less positive support as
demonstrated that the nature of this association wives’ negative affectivity increases (Pasch,
varies as a function of marital satisfaction. We Bradbury, & Davila, 1997). To explore these
hypothesized that satisfied couples, by virtue possibilities in greater detail, we need further
of being more resilient to stress and more research that tracks daily stress, supportive
responsive to one another, would engage in interactions, and sexual interaction among sat-
more sexual activity to the extent that external isfied and dissatisfied men and women.
daily stresses provided them with opportuni- Among similar lines, the relatively weak
ties to interact and provide one another with pattern of results relating stress and sexual dys-
validation and compassion. This idea was not function suggests the need for further study on
supported, as levels of sexual activity were this association (cf. Bodenmann, Ledermann,
about the same in satisfied men and women, Blattner-Bolliger, & Galluzzo, 2006; Morok-
regardless of their reported levels of daily off & Gilliland, 1993). To the extent that they
stress (see Figure 2). Relatively dissatisfied are a relatively stable feature of an individuals’
spouses, on the other hand, were expected to sexual life, sexual dysfunctions may be some-
report lower levels of sexual activity when what independent of contextual influences.
stress was high than when it was low. This Interpretation of the present results must be
aspect of Hypothesis 4 was supported, though qualified by several factors. First, the cross-
only for women. For relatively dissatisfied sectional nature of this study makes causal
men, the opposite pattern was obtained: Those inferences impossible. Second, we can make
reporting higher levels of daily hassles no claims about how individuals or couples
reported higher levels of sexual activity, much might respond to across-time fluctuations in
like we anticipated for satisfied spouses. daily hassles or life events. Third, the use of
Though we must resort to speculation on this a convenience sample limits generalizability
point, this result suggests that the daily hassles to the larger population of couples. Fourth,
experienced by men who are relatively dissat- we have no independent assessment of stress,
isfied might provide opportunities for couples which leaves open the possibility that individ-
to join together against a common adversary, ual differences in personality factors are con-
and this increased contact may promote sexual tributing to the effects observed here. It is
interaction. On the basis of these moments of noteworthy, however, that intradyadic correla-
heightened interaction, partners might recog- tions were high for internal stress (.46) and
nize the inherent strengths of their relationship critical life events (.39) but rather low for
and thus enjoy higher levels of satisfaction and external daily stress (.11), suggesting that no
sexual activity. The results shown in Figure 2 general bias could be observed. Fifth, the has-
suggest that a different process operates when sles and life events that were assessed here
distressed women experience high levels of may vary dramatically between respondents;
daily hassles. Men may be at least as support- unemployment, for example, is not a uniform
ive as women, and these supportive acts may experience, and apart from the impact ratings
bring the couple closer together but not even- collected here, these variations were not well
tuate in sexual interaction. Alternatively, men measured. Other studies have profitably distin-
may not be particularly adept at providing sup- guished, for example, between chronic and
port to their distressed and stressed partner, acute stressors (e.g., Karney et al., 2005), but
opportunities for supportive exchanges may that distinction could not be pursued here
be lost and, accordingly, these wives report because duration of stress exposure was not
less sexual activity than their equally stressed assessed. Sixth, though we do not expect
counterparts who are in more satisfying rela- cultural contexts to moderate associations
tionships. An observational study of social between stress and relationship functioning,
support behaviors in couples provides support at least within Westernized societies, addi-
for this latter interpretation, in that wives dis- tional data are needed to establish the gen-
play more positive support to the extent that eralizability of the present findings. Prior
568 G. Bodenmann, T. Ledermann, and T. N. Bradbury

studies on this topic conducted with European Bodenmann, G. (2000). Stress und Coping bei Paaren
[Stress and coping in couples]. Göttingen, Germany:
and U.S. samples have yielded comparable Hogrefe.
findings (e.g., Bodenmann, 2005; Story & Bodenmann, G. (2005). Dyadic coping and its significance
Bradbury, 2004). for marital functioning. In T. Revenson, K. Kayser, &
G. Bodenmann (Eds.), Couples coping with stress: Emer-
Notwithstanding these limitations, the present ging perspectives on dyadic coping (pp. 33–50). Wash-
study links variation in key marital variables— ington, DC: American Psychological Association.
marital satisfaction and sexual functioning— Bodenmann, G., & Cina, A. (2006). Stress and coping
among stable-satisfied, stable-distressed and sepa-
with spouses’ experiences of daily hassles in rated/divorced Swiss couples: A 5-year prospective
the relationship, which in turn covary with daily longitudinal study. Journal of Divorce and Remar-
hassles and, to a lesser degree, the acute life riage, 44, 71–89.
Bodenmann, G., Ledermann, T., Blattner-Bolliger, D., &
events they encounter outside the relationship. Galluzzo, C. (2006). The associations among everyday
Evidence that higher levels of stress sometimes stress, critical life events and sexual problems. Journal
corresponded with better marital functioning of Nervous and Mental Disease, 194, 494–501.
Bodenmann, G., & Shantinath, S. D. (2004). The Couples
indicates that more complex models are needed Coping Enhancement Training (CCET): A new
to specify the mechanisms that might underlie approach to prevention of marital distress based upon
these associations and to explore their implica- stress and coping. Family Relations, 53, 477–484.
Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Preventing
tions for preventive intervention. Although marital dysfunction: Review and analysis. In F. D.
scholars often view modification of couples’ Fincham & T. N. Bradbury (Eds.), The psychology of
interactional processes as the surest route to marriage: Basic issues and applications (pp. 375–
401). New York: Guilford.
preventing adverse marital outcomes, further Bradbury, T. N., & Karney, B. R. (2004). Understanding
analysis of couples’ ecological niches may and altering the longitudinal course of marriage. Jour-
reveal that protecting the emotional climate nal of Marriage and Family, 66, 862–879.
Brezsnyak, M., & Whisman, M. A. (2004). Sexual desire
within a marriage from the harmful effects and relationship functioning: The effects of marital
of external stressors is a compelling alterna- satisfaction and power. Journal of Sex and Marital
tive (e.g., Bodenmann & Shantinath, 2004; Therapy, 30, 199–217.
Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of
Bradbury & Fincham, 1990). The most imme- assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long
diate practical implication of this study is that (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–
coping and stress management may prove to 162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Burr, W. R., & Klein, S. (Eds.). (1994). Managing family
be a useful target in intervention programs stress. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
designed to prevent and treat relationship dis- Campbell, L., Simpson, J. A., Kashy, D. A., & Fletcher, G.
tress. Enhancing traditional communication J. O. (2001). Ideal standards, the self, and flexibility of
ideals in close relationships. Personality and Social
and problem-solving skills may be insufficient Psychology Bulletin, 27, 447–462.
for strengthening the marital dyad, as our find- Campbell, L., Simpson, J. A., Kashy, D. A., & Rholes, W.
ings suggest that ineffective management of S. (2001). Attachment orientations, dependence, and
behavior in a stressful situation: An application of
daily hassles may produce relationship con- the Actor–Partner Interdependence Model. Journal
flicts and tension within the relationship. of Social and Personal Relationships, 18, 821–843.
Addressing external sources of conflict (e.g., Cohan, C. L., & Bradbury, T. N. (1997). Negative life
events, marital interaction, and the longitudinal course
see the Couples Coping Enhancement Training of newlywed marriage. Journal of Personality and
program; Bodenmann & Shantinath, 2004), has Social Psychology, 73, 114–128.
the potential to redirect partners’ change efforts Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003).
Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for
away from one another so that they focus at the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ:
least as much on the environmental circumstan- Erlbaum.
ces that are affecting them. Cutrona, C. E., Russell, D. W., Abraham, W. T., Gardner,
K. A., Melby, J. N., Bryant, C., et al. (2003). Neighbor-
hood context and financial strain as predictors of
References marital interaction and marital quality in African
American couples. Personal Relationships, 10,
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression 389–409.
testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, Derogatis, L. R. (1992). SCL–90–R, administration, scor-
CA: Sage. ing & procedures manual-II for the R(evised) version
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and and other instruments of the Psychopathology Rating
statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV). Scale Series. Townson, MD: Clinical Psychometric
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Research.
Stress, sex, and satisfaction 569

Fletcher, G. J. O., & Thomas, G. (2000). Behavior and on- research: Two extensions of the Actor–Partner Inter-
line cognition in marital interaction. Personal Rela- dependence Model]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie,
tionships, 7, 111–130. 37, 27–40.
Hahlweg, K. (1996). Fragebogen zur Partnerschaftsdiag- MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M.,
nostik (FPD) [Partnership questionnaire]. Göttingen, West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison
Germany: Hogrefe. of methods to test mediation and other interven-
Harper, J. M., Schaalje, B. G., & Sandberg, J. G. (2000). ing variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7,
Daily hassles, intimacy, and marital quality in later life 83–104.
marriages. American Journal of Family Therapy, 28, MacKinnon, D. P., Warsi, G., & Dwyer, J. H. (1995). A
1–18. simulation study of mediated effect measures. Multi-
Hendrick, S. S., & Hendrick, C. (2002). Linking romantic variate Behavioral Research, 30, 41–62.
love with sex: Development of the perceptions of love McCarthy, B. (2003). Marital sex as it ought to be. Journal
and sex scale. Journal of Social and Personal Rela- of Family Psychotherapy, 14, 1–12.
tionships, 19, 361–378. McCubbin, H. I., & Patterson, J. M. (1983). The family
Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The Social Readjust- stress process: The double ABCX model of adjust-
ment Rating Scale. Journal of Psychosomatic ment and adaptation. Marriage and Family Review,
Research, 11, 213–218. 6, 7–37.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for the fit Metz, M. E., & Epstein, N. (2002). Assessing the role of
indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional relationship conflict in sexual dysfunction. Journal of
criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Sex and Marital Therapy, 28, 139–164.
Modeling, 6, 1–55. Morokoff, P. J., & Gillilland, R. (1993). Stress, sexual
Hurlbert, D. F., Apt, C., Hurlbert, M. K., & Pierce, A. P. functioning, and marital satisfaction. Journal of Sex
(2000). Sexual compatibility and the sexual desire- Research, 30, 43–53.
motivation relation in females with hypoactive sexual Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2004). How does context
desire disorder. Behavior Modification, 24, 325–347. affect intimate relationships? Linking external stress
Kanner, A. D., Coyne, J. C., Schaefer, C., & Lazarus, R. S. and cognitive processes within marriage. Personality
(1981). Comparison of two modes of stress measure- and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 134–148.
ments: Daily hassles and uplifts versus major life Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2005). Gender differences in
events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 1–39. social support: A question of skill or responsiveness?
Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longitudinal Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88,
course of marital quality and stability: A review of 79–90.
theory, method, and research. Psychological Bulletin, Pasch, L. A., & Bradbury, T. N. (1998). Social support,
118, 3–34. conflict, and the development of marital dysfunction.
Karney, B. R., Story, L. B., & Bradbury, T. N. (2005). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66,
Marriages in context: Interactions between chronic 219–230.
and acute stress among newlyweds. In T. A. Revenson, Pasch, L. A., Bradbury, T. N., & Davila, J. (1997). Gender,
K. Kayser, & G. Bodenmann (Eds.), Couples coping negative affectivity, and observed social support
with stress: Emerging perspectives on dyadic coping behavior in marital interaction. Personal Relation-
(pp. 13–32). Washington, DC: APA. ships, 4, 361–378.
Kenny, D. A. (1996). Models of non-independence in Sarason, I. G., Johnson, J. H., & Siegel, J. M. (1978).
dyadic research. Journal of Social and Personal Rela- Assessing the impact of life changes: Development
tionships, 13, 279–294. of the life experiences survey. Journal of Consulting
Kenny, D. A., & Cook, W. (1999). Partner effects in re- and Clinical Psychology, 46, 932–946.
lationship research: Conceptual issues, analytic diffi- Snyder, D. K. (1981). Marital satisfaction inventory (MSI)
culties, and illustrations. Personal Relationship, 6, manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
433–448. Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for
Klann, N., Hahlweg, K., & Hank, G. (1992). Deutsche indirect effects in structural equation models. In S.
Validierung des ‘‘Marital Satisfaction Inventory’’ Leinhart (Ed.), Sociological methodology (pp. 290–
(MSI) von Snyder (1981). System Familie, 5, 10–21. 312). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, Story, L. B., & Bradbury, T. N. (2004). Understanding
and coping. New York: Springer. marriage and stress: Essential questions and chal-
Ledermann, T., & Bodenmann, G. (2006). Moderator- lenges. Clinical Psychology Review, 23, 1139–1162.
und Mediatoreffekte bei dyadischen Daten: Zwei Williams, L. M. (1995). Associations of stressful life
Erweiterungen des Akteur-Partner-Interdependenz- events and marital quality. Psychological Reports,
Modells [Moderator and mediator effects in dyadic 76, 1115–1122.

You might also like