You are on page 1of 8

ARCHIVAL REPORT

Three-Week Bright-Light Intervention Has Dose-


Related Effects on Threat-Related Corticolimbic
Reactivity and Functional Coupling
Patrick M. Fisher, Martin K. Madsen, Brenda Mc Mahon, Klaus K. Holst, Sofie B. Andersen,
Helle R. Laursen, Lis F. Hasholt, Hartwig R. Siebner, and Gitte M. Knudsen
Background: Bright-light intervention is reported to successfully treat depression, in particular seasonal affective disorder, but the
neural pathways and molecular mechanisms mediating its effects are unclear. An amygdala-prefrontal cortex corticolimbic circuit
regulates responses to salient environmental stimuli (e.g., threat) and may underlie these effects. Serotonin signaling modulates this
circuit and is implicated in the pathophysiology of seasonal and other affective disorders.

Methods: We evaluated the effects of a bright-light intervention protocol on threat-related corticolimbic reactivity and functional
coupling, assessed with an emotional faces functional magnetic resonance imaging paradigm at preintervention and postintervention. In
a double-blind study conducted in the winter, 30 healthy male subjects received bright-light intervention (dose range between
participants: .1–11.0 kilolux) for 30 minutes daily over a period of 3 weeks. Additionally, we considered serotonin transporter-linked
polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) genotype status as a model for differences in serotonin signaling and moderator of intervention
effects.
Results: Bright-light dose significantly negatively affected threat-related amygdala and prefrontal reactivity in a dose-dependent
manner. Conversely, amygdala-prefrontal and intraprefrontal functional coupling increased significantly in a dose-dependent manner.
Genotype status significantly moderated bright-light intervention effects on intraprefrontal functional coupling.

Conclusions: This is the first study to evaluate the effects of clinically relevant bright-light intervention on threat-related brain function.
We show that amygdala-prefrontal reactivity and communication are significantly affected by bright-light intervention, an effect partly
moderated by genotype. These novel findings support that this threat-related corticolimbic circuit is sensitive to light intervention and
may mediate the therapeutic effects of bright-light intervention.

generally critical for regulating mood and processing emotionally


Key Words: Amygdala, bright-light therapy, fMRI, 5-HTTLPR, salient information within the environment.
prefrontal cortex, serotonin Our understanding of the neurobiological mechanisms sensi-
tive to bright-light exposure is limited. A neuroimaging study in

E
xposure to bright light has been used to successfully treat healthy individuals reported that 1-minute exposure periods to
depression and other affective disorders (1,2). Most notably, blue light increased brain responses to auditory stimuli delivered
it is used to treat seasonal affective disorder (SAD), a with angry, negative prosody and functional connectivity
depressive disorder characterized by recurring, seasonally asso- between auditory cortex and the amygdala (4). Such studies of
ciated depressive symptoms during fall and winter months, as intervention effects in healthy populations can provide key
day length decreases, that abate with the spring and summer insights into the neurobiological mechanisms responsive to
months, as day length increases (3). Considering the overlap in intervention and serve as a useful benchmark for evaluating
symptoms characterizing SAD and other depressive disorders effects in clinical cohorts, but such studies are also limited by the
(e.g., anhedonia, increased negative mood, alterations in sleep obvious lack of clinical symptoms. However, clinical use of bright-
alterations, etc.) it is reasonable to hypothesize that these light therapy typically involves exposure to full-spectrum white
disorders share common neurobiological underpinnings and that light for 30 to 120 minutes daily over a period of weeks (5). Thus,
the clinical effects of bright-light therapy on SAD and related although these findings suggest that bright-light exposure affects
disorders may be through modulation of neural pathways the neural processing of negative stimuli, they do not provide
insight into the effects of bright-light exposure as it is used
clinically.
From the Center for Integrated Molecular Brain Imaging (PMF, MKM, BM, A corticolimbic circuit comprising the amygdala and medial
KKH, SBA, HRL, HRS, GMK) and Neurobiology Research Unit (PMF,
prefrontal cortex (mPFC), including anterior cingulate cortex,
MKM, BM, KKH, SBA, GMK), Copenhagen University Hospital Rigshos-
critically modulates reactivity to emotionally salient environ-
pitalet, Copenhagen O; Department of Biostatistics (KKH), University of
mental stimuli, particularly indices of threat (6,7). A role for this
Copenhagen, Copenhagen K; Danish Research Centre for Magnetic
Resonance (HRL, HRS), Copenhagen University Hospital Hvidovre,
circuit in the pathophysiology of affective disorders is supported
Hvidovre; and Department of Cellular and Molecular Medicine (LFH), by numerous neuroimaging studies reporting differences in
Copenhagen University, Copenhagen, Denmark. threat-related corticolimbic reactivity and circuit function
Address correspondence to Gitte M. Knudsen, M.D., D.M.Sc., Copenhagen between healthy and clinical cohorts, some of which normalize
University Hospital Rigshospitalet, Neurobiology Research Unit, following antidepressant treatment (8). A broad literature in
NRU 6931, Rigshospitalet, Blegdamsvej 9, Copenhagen O DK-2100, animal models and humans implicates serotonin signaling in
Denmark; E-mail: gmk@nru.dk. modulating threat-related corticolimbic circuit function (9,10). In
Received Jul 23, 2013; revised Nov 22, 2013; accepted Nov 30, 2013. one of the first descriptions of SAD and bright-light therapy,

0006-3223/$36.00 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2014;76:332–339


http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.11.031 & 2014 Society of Biological Psychiatry
P.M. Fisher et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2014;76:332–339 333

Rosenthal et al. (3) identified serotonin signaling as a candidate in consent was acquired from all participants. Inclusion criteria
its pathophysiology. Though not all studies have confirmed this, included: 1) 18 to 45 years old, 2) 18 to 30 body mass index, 3)
genetic association, imaging genetics, and neuroreceptor posi- no history of alcohol/drug abuse, 4) no history of psychiatric/
tron emission tomography studies, including from our own lab, neurological illness, 5) no excessive light exposure during study
have identified links between seasonal variation and serotonin participation and preceding autumn (e.g., sun tanning), 6) no retinal
signaling (11,12). These findings support a model wherein the pathology, 7) not taking photosensitizing medication, and 8)
serotonin system is sensitive to seasonal variation and may Seasonality Pattern Assessment Questionnaire score ⬍ 11 (18).
modulate the effects of light on mood and affect, in part by Status for the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism (i.e., LA, LG, S alleles) within
affecting threat-related corticolimbic circuit function. Despite this, the SLC6A4 gene was determined for the purpose of study inclusion.
no studies have evaluated the effects of bright-light intervention A total of 70 participants were genotyped and underwent a medical
on reactivity of this circuit or how serotonin may moderate such examination to verify meeting inclusion criteria. Of these, 16 LA/LA
effects. and 16 non-LA/LA participants (i.e., 1–2 LG or S alleles) were included
Within the current study, carried out during winter, we evaluated in the study. Due to magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)-related
the effects of bright-light intervention, at levels similar to those used artifacts, two datasets were excluded from analysis. Thus, our final
clinically, on threat-related corticolimbic circuit function within a sample size was 30 (Table 1). The study was conducted from
cohort of 30 healthy male subjects. We also considered interactive November to February, 2011 and 2012.
effects of bright-light dose with the serotonin transporter-linked
polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) polymorphism (including rs25531), Genotyping
located in the promoter region of the gene (SLC6A4) coding for the The triallelic 5-HTTLPR polymorphism, including rs25531 (i.e.,
serotonin transporter (5-HTT), as an exploratory model for differ- LA, LG, and S alleles), was determined as previously described (12).
ences in serotonin signaling based on evidence that it interacts with
seasonal variation to affect 5-HTT levels (12–14). Participants were Bright-Light Intervention Protocol
randomly assigned to receive 30 minutes of bright-light intervention Participants randomized to bright-light intervention received
(dose range: .1–11.0 kilolux [klx]) at home every morning for 3 such that 5-HTTLPR status was evenly represented across high
weeks. Participants completed an emotional faces functional mag- and low doses. From a standard distance of 50 cm, high-dose
netic resonance imaging (fMRI) paradigm preintervention and post- lamps emitted 4.9 to 7.8 klx, whereas low-dose lamps emitted .6
intervention, wherein they viewed individual angry, fearful, or to 1.9 klx. Participants and researchers were blinded to bright-
neutral faces (15). Based on previously reported effects of pharma- light dose received throughout the study. Bright-light lamps were
cologic intervention studies using antidepressants (16,17), we acquired from Smifa Healthcare (Solroed Strand, Denmark).
hypothesized that bright-light intervention would decrease threat- Bright-light lamps emitted full-spectrum white light with peaks
related amygdala reactivity and increase amygdala-prefrontal func- in the blue spectrum (Figure S1 in Supplement 1), at wavelengths
tional coupling. Additionally, we evaluated whether 5-HTTLPR status to which intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells, a key
moderated effects of bright-light intervention on corticolimbic circuit regulator of circadian phase, are sensitive (19). There were no
reactivity and functional coupling. identifiable differences between lamps. Participants received
written and oral instructions for light intervention. Participants
Methods and Materials were instructed to sit 50 cm from the lamp for 30 minutes
between 7:00 AM and 9:00 AM every morning for 3 weeks.
Participants Intervention criteria were based on recent SAD treatment guide-
Healthy male participants were recruited from the Copenhagen lines (1). Bright-light intervention around sunrise is consistent
region via online advertisements for a research protocol approved with an extension of the photoperiod and previous studies
by the Ethics Committee of Copenhagen and Frederiksberg, Den- reported that morning administration is particularly effective in
mark (H-1-2010-091; amendments: 28633,30043). Written informed relieving SAD symptoms in clinical cohorts (20,21).

Table 1. Demographic, Bright-Light Intervention, and Task Performance


Baseline Rescan Bright-Light Effect

n 30 – –
5-HTTLPR (LA/LA vs. LG or S Carrier) 14/16 – –
Age (Years) 24.3 ⫾ 3.7 – –
Education (Years) 16.1 ⫾ 1.4 – –
Intervention Length (Days) 21.2 ⫾ 2.0 (19–28) – –
Bright Light Dose (Kilolux) 3.5 ⫾ 2.9 (.1–11.0) – –
Body Mass Index (kg/m2) 23.2 ⫾ 2.8 23.0 ⫾ 2.7 p ¼ .31
Major Depression Index 5.6 ⫾ 5.1 5.1 ⫾ 4.7 p ¼ .98
Cohen0 s Perceived Stress 9.4 ⫾ 5.1 9.2 ⫾ 5.1 p ¼ .69
POMS-TMD ⫹1.7 ⫾ 16.5 ⫹1.2 ⫾ 15.0 p ¼ .48
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index 3.6 ⫾ 1.6 3.3 ⫾ 2.0 p ¼ .99
Faces Task, Reaction Time (msec) 731.4 ⫾ 79.4 753.3 ⫾ 85.2 p ¼ .76
Faces Task, Performance (% Correct) 97.5 ⫾ 1.6 97.5 ⫾ 1.7 p ¼ .79
p values reflect an ANCOVA model with light dose predicting measure at rescan, covarying for measure at baseline. Values reflect mean ⫾ SD
(minimum - maximum). Rescan BMI data available for only 23 participants.
ANCOVA, analysis of covariance; BMI, body mass index; 5-HTTLPR, serotonin transporter-linked polymorphic region; POMS-TMD, Profile of Mood States-
Total Mood Disturbance.

www.sobp.org/journal
334 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2014;76:332–339 P.M. Fisher et al.

The intervention period began following the baseline MRI scan presentations and response recordings were performed using E-
and continued for 3 weeks. To estimate light dose received, we prime (Psychological Software Tools, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania).
measured illuminance for each lamp with a luxmeter (Elma 1335;
Elma Instruments, Farum, Denmark) and measured lamp-to-eye Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Data Analysis
distance within the participant’s home environment, where light Functional images were first realigned to a subject-specific
intervention was administered (distance from lamp: mean ⫾ SD) mean functional image. The T1-weighted structural image was co-
= 71.4 cm ⫾ 30.8; median, range = 58.5 cm, 36–170). The angle registered to the functional images and segmented using VBM5
between the person and lamp is relevant for estimating light (http://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/vbm/vbm5-for-spm5/). Functional
dose. Based on participant description, we estimated this angle as images were normalized into Montreal Neurological Institute space
01 or 451 (i.e., directly facing or angled, respectively) for each using normalization parameters estimated during segmentation of
participant. From this information, we estimated illuminance the T1-weighted image. Functional images were smoothed with an
received for each participant (mean ⫾ SD = 3.5 klx ⫾ 2.9; 8 mm Gaussian kernel to minimize noise and residual difference in
median, range = 2.8 klx, .1–11.0, see Supplement 1). gyral anatomy. After preprocessing, smoothed functional images
To facilitate compliance, participants completed log books and were used within single-subject general linear models employing a
received daily mobile phone short message service reminders. canonical hemodynamic response function to estimate condition-
Participants reported average time outside during the daytime of specific and task-specific BOLD activation (i.e., beta and contrast
the intervention period (7, 16, and 7 participants reported, on images, respectively), including motion parameters and respiratory
average, less than 30 minutes outside daily, 30 to 60 minutes, and measures as covariates.
more than 60 minutes, respectively). Single-subject contrast images (i.e., weighted sum of task-
related beta images) were used in second-level random effects
Personality Measures models to determine task-specific reactivity (i.e., fearful and angry
To assess effects of bright-light intervention on affect, states vs. neutral faces). To address the issue of multiple comparisons,
measures were acquired at baseline and rescan, including Major 3dClustSim, a software program within AFNI (http://afni.nimh.nih.
Depression Inventory, Profile of Mood States, Cohen’s Perceived gov/afni) that uses a Monte Carlo simulation method was used to
Stress Scale, and Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (22–25). determine cluster extent thresholds for specific regions of interest
unlikely to have occurred by chance (α ⬍ .05). All regions of
Magnetic Resonance Imaging Data Acquisition interest were defined using the WFU Pickatlas software (http://
Participants were scanned on a 3T-Trio MRI using an eight- fmri.wfubmc.edu/software/PickAtlas), version 3.0.3 (28–30). The
channel head coil (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) as described cluster extent threshold required for our amygdala search volume,
previously (15). Blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) fMRI scans given a voxel-level threshold of p ⬍ .01, uncorrected, was k ⬎ 14
were acquired using a T2*-weighted gradient-echo spiral echo- voxels. Our mPFC region of interest was defined as Brodmann
planar imaging sequence (repetition time ¼ 2500 msec, echo time areas 24, 25, and 32 (three-dimensional dilation = 1) so as to
¼ 26 msec, flip angle ¼ 761, in-plane matrix ¼ 64  64, in-plane include mPFC regions that share anatomical connectivity with the
resolution ¼ 3  3 mm, number of slices within a whole-brain amygdala (31–33). The cluster extent threshold required for this
volume ¼ 41, slice thickness ¼ 3 mm, gap ¼ .75 mm). Image search volume was k ⬎ 249 voxels.
acquisition was optimized for signal recovery within orbital frontal
cortex by 1) tilting slice orientation from a transverse toward a Functional Coupling Analysis
coronal orientation by approximately 301; and 2) use of a We examined corticolimbic functional coupling by evaluating
preparation gradient pulse (26). Within each fMRI session, a total the correlation in the BOLD time series with one of three seed
of 312 whole-brain volumes (156 per run) were acquired. We regions: left amygdala, right amygdala, and mPFC. Furthermore,
acquired a T1-weighted, spin-echo sequence, high-resolution we evaluated the effects of bright-light intervention on functional
whole-brain three-dimensional structural magnetic resonance coupling and whether 5-HTTLPR moderated this effect. Although
scan (inversion time ¼ 800 msec, echo time ¼ 3.92 msec, coupling estimates may reflect functionally relevant correlations
repetition time ¼ 1540 msec, flip angle ¼ 91, in-plane matrix ¼ between components of neural circuits, they do not establish
256  256, in-plane resolution ¼ 1  1 mm, number of slices ¼ causality or directionality (34). We extracted the mean BOLD time
192, slice thickness ¼ 1 mm, no gap). series for each subject from each of our three seeds using MarsBaR
(http://marsbar.sourceforge.net/) (35). We defined the seeds by a 5
Emotional Faces Paradigm mm radius sphere (seed volume: 648 mm3 or 81 voxels) centered
Participants completed a gender discrimination task including on the voxel maximally responsive to our task. Functional coupling
alternating blocks of individual neutral, fearful, and angry faces, for each seed was evaluated separately. Extracted time series were
each presented in the middle of the screen (15). Faces were taken used in functional coupling analysis as has been previously
from the Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces database (27). Each described (36). Time series were mean-centered and drift-cor-
trial consisted of the stimulus presented for 1800 milliseconds and rected, and individual values exceeding three SD of the mean of
an interstimulus interval of 200 milliseconds during which time a this time series were replaced by the mean of the two adjacent
fixation cross (⫹) was present on the screen. Each block comprised values. This time series was entered as a regressor in single-subject
six trials, including three to five faces trials (mean face trials per design matrices that included as covariates task condition, motion
block ¼ 4) and one to three null trials (mean null trials per block ¼ parameters, and respiratory variables described earlier. Analyses
2) consisting of a fixation cross (⫹). Faces and null trials were yielded individual contrast images reflecting the temporal cou-
randomly mixed within each block. In total, 32 blocks of neutral pling of BOLD signal changes with a given seed.
faces were interleaved between 16 blocks of fearful and 16 blocks
of angry faces presented across two fMRI runs with a brief break Data Analysis
between runs. Neutral faces were presented twice throughout the We evaluated light dose effects as a continuous predictor using
task, whereas fearful and angry faces were presented once. Stimulus linear regression, analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), and t tests.

www.sobp.org/journal
P.M. Fisher et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2014;76:332–339 335

Mean estimates of task-related fMRI reactivity were extracted from statistically significantly negatively associated with mPFC reactivity
SPM (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) and analyzed in R, 2.15.1 at rescan, controlling for reactivity at baseline (bright-light effect:
(http://www.r-project.org/). A threshold of p ⬍ .05 (two-tailed) was mPFC [95% CI]: .21 [.36 to .058], p ¼ .009; Figure 2B). That is,
considered statistically significant. We did not observe a significant higher bright-light dose was associated with a lower mPFC response
association between age or body mass index and task reactivity to fear and angry faces compared with neutral faces at rescan.
estimates; thus, they were excluded from our models. Magnetic Next, we evaluated whether effects of bright-light intervention
resonance imaging scans were acquired at varied times through- on amygdala and mPFC reactivity were moderated by 5-HTTLPR
out the day (range: 7:00 AM to 6:00 PM). Magnetic resonance genotype status (LA/LA vs. LG or S carriers). We did not observe a
imaging scan time was not predictive of fMRI estimates and thus significant genotype by light-dose interaction effect on reactivity
were excluded from our models (Supplement 1). We evaluated measures (interaction effect: left amygdala [95% CI]: .32 [.73 to
effects of bright-light intervention using an ANCOVA model (37). .10], p ¼ .13; right amygdala: .048 [.35 to .25], p ¼ .74; mPFC:
This approach modeled a given fMRI estimate at rescan as a linear .26 [.58 to .059], p ¼ .11).
function of that measure at baseline, light dose, and other relevant
covariates (e.g., 5-HTTLPR status). Thus, the estimated association Bright-Light Intervention Increases Corticolimbic Functional
between light dose and rescan is determined while accounting for Coupling
variation at baseline. This approach is advantageous in random- Next, we evaluated the effects of bright-light intervention on
ized test-retest designs because it accounts for interindividual an estimate of communication within this corticolimbic circuit
variability in baseline response when determining effects of using functional coupling. At baseline, we observed statistically
intervention. The same approach was applied at a voxel level for significant positive functional coupling across participants
evaluating effects of intervention on functional coupling. Voxel- between each of our seeds and nearly all of our amygdala-
level functional coupling estimates at baseline and rescan were prefrontal search volume (Table S1 in Supplement 1). No brain
extracted from SPM and we applied this ANCOVA model for each regions exhibited significant negative functional coupling with
voxel using R. Results were then written back into images that any seed. Effects of bright-light dose on functional coupling were
were visualized using SPM. evaluated at a voxel level using the ANCOVA model described
above. Bright-light dose was statistically significantly positively
Results associated with functional coupling at rescan between our left
amygdala seed and mPFC, controlling for baseline coupling
Bright-Light Intervention Decreases Threat-Related (Table 2; Figure S2 in Supplement 1). Bright-light intervention
Corticolimbic Reactivity did not affect functional coupling between our right amygdala
At baseline, we observed bilateral threat-related amygdala seed and mPFC. Bright-light dose was also statistically signifi-
reactivity (i.e., angry and fear vs. neutral faces) across all cantly positively associated with functional coupling between our
participants (left amygdala: [30, 4, 18], z ¼ 4.77, k ¼ 47 mPFC seed and surrounding mPFC regions (Table 2; Figure S3 in
voxels, p ⬍ .05, corrected; right amygdala: [30, 4, 18], z ¼ 3.71, Supplement 1).
k ¼ 39 voxels, p ⬍ .05, corrected; Figure 1A). Bright-light dose was We evaluated whether 5-HTTLPR status moderated effects of
statistically significantly negatively associated with threat-related bright-light intervention on functional coupling. There were no
amygdala reactivity at rescan, controlling for reactivity at baseline statistically significant moderation effects on functional coupling
(bright-light effect: left amygdala [95% confidence interval (CI)]: with our left or right amygdala seeds. The effect of bright-light
.26 [.46 to .062], p ¼ .01; right amygdala: .22 [.36 to intervention on intraprefrontal functional coupling (i.e., mPFC
.083], p ¼ .003; Figure 1B,C). coupling with our mPFC seed) was statistically significantly
At baseline, an mPFC cluster was statistically significantly less moderated by 5-HTTLPR status such that bright-light dose was
reactive to angry and fear faces compared with neutral faces across positively associated with intraprefrontal functional coupling at
all participants (mPFC cluster: [10, 52, 0], z ¼ 4.38, k ¼ 1304 rescan in LG or S carriers, but there was no effect on LA/LA
voxels, p ⬍ .05, corrected; Figure 2A). Bright-light dose was individuals (Table 2; Figure 3).

Figure 1. Effects of bright-light intervention on amygdala reactivity. (A) Statistical parametric map highlighting amygdala response to task at baseline
across all participants (blood oxygen level-dependent contrast: angry and fear – neutral faces). Color bar represents t scores. Bright-light dose received was
negatively associated with (B) left and (C) right amygdala reactivity at rescan, accounting for baseline reactivity values. Thirty individual data points shown
in orange and blue shading represent 95% confidence limit of regression line.

www.sobp.org/journal
336 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2014;76:332–339 P.M. Fisher et al.

Discussion
Here, we present findings from the first study to evaluate the
effects of a bright-light intervention similar to clinically adminis-
tered protocols on threat-related corticolimbic reactivity and
functional coupling. Consistent with our hypothesis, we observed
a significant dose-dependent negative association between
bright-light intervention and threat-related reactivity within
amygdala and mPFC. Also consistent with our hypothesis,
bright-light intervention positively affected prefrontal functional
coupling with our left (but not right) amygdala and mPFC seeds.
Interestingly, the effect of bright-light intervention on prefrontal
functional coupling with our mPFC seed was significantly mod-
erated by 5-HTTLPR status such that intervention increased
functional coupling in LG or S carriers, whereas little effect was
observed in LA/LA individuals. Intriguingly, our finding that
5-HTTLPR status moderates the response to bright-light interven-
tion provides further evidence supporting a link between seroto-
nin signaling, bright-light intervention, and seasonality. These
findings implicate this circuit in mediating the clinical effects of
bright-light intervention and can serve as a relevant benchmark
for future studies evaluating the effects of bright-light interven-
tion on brain function as it relates to treatment.
Bright-light therapy has been used to successfully treat SAD (3)
and more recent studies have reported its effectiveness for
treating other affective disorders (2). Previous studies have
reported alterations in function of this corticolimbic circuit in
patients with affective disorders and acute bright-light exposure
affected amygdala reactivity to negative auditory stimuli (4,38,39).
Taken together with our findings, we hypothesize dysfunction of
this circuit represents a neurobiological mechanism shared across
seasonal and other affective disorders and may represent a critical
neural pathway mediating clinical responsiveness to bright-light
intervention (40). Future studies in patients with SAD are

Table 2. Bright-Light Intervention and 5-HTTLPR Effects on Corticolimbic


Functional Coupling
Figure 2. Effects of bright-light intervention on medial prefrontal
cortex (mPFC) reactivity. (A) Statistical parametric map highlighting x, y, z Z Score Cluster Size
mPFC area where functional magnetic resonance imaging response to
angry and fearful faces was less than that of neutral faces at baseline Bright-Light Intervention Effects on Functional Coupling
across all participants. Color bar represents t scores. (B) Bright-light dose Left amygdala seed
was negatively associated with mPFC reactivity (plotted as angry and Positive association – medial 4, 28, 0 4.11 938
fearful – neutral faces) at rescan. Thirty individual data points shown in
prefrontal cortex
orange and blue shading represent 95% confidence limit of
regression line. Right amygdala seed
No significant clusters – – –
mPFC seed
Positive association – medial 4, 28, 2 4.12 1594
Treatment Compliance and Effects on Behavior and Mood prefrontal cortex
We observed good participant compliance throughout the 5-HTTLPR by Bright-Light Intervention Interaction Effects on Functional
intervention period (reported days missed: mean ¼ .9, range ¼ 0–3). Coupling
Bright-light dose was not associated with significant changes in Left amygdala seed
measures of mood, depressive symptoms, and stress (Table 1). No significant clusters – – –
Participants completed the fMRI paradigm with a high degree of Right amygdala seed
accuracy that was unaffected by bright-light intervention No significant clusters – – –
(Table 1). To estimate effects of natural light exposure, we asked mPFC seed
participants to report average time outside (i.e., less than 30 Medial prefrontal cortex 12, 40, 18 3.02 394
minutes daily, 30 to 60 minutes, or more than 60 minutes). Bright- Effects of bright-light intervention were evaluated in a model that did
light dose received was not significantly different across these not include 5-HTTLPR genotype status. All coordinates are given in MNI
groups, suggesting our findings are not confounded by differ- space. Statistical threshold for mPFC search volume: p ⬍ .01 (voxel-level),
ences in natural light exposure (F2,27 ¼ .57, p ¼ .88). Additionally, cluster-extent ⬎ 249 voxels. Left and right amygdala and mPFC seeds are
5 mm spheres centered on (30, 4, 18), (30, 4, 18), and (10, 52,
natural light exposure was not significantly predictive of reactivity 0), respectively.
or functional coupling when added as a predictor in our ANCOVA 5-HTTLPR, serotonin transporter-linked polymorphic region; MNI,
model (ps ⬎ .20). Montreal Neurological Institute; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex.

www.sobp.org/journal
P.M. Fisher et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2014;76:332–339 337

coupling were increased following bright-light intervention. These


findings are consistent with a model wherein our task-related
reactivity reflects the bottom-up drive of the amygdala on this
circuit, which is dampened by bright-light intervention. By
contrast, our metric of functional coupling may more closely
relate to the top-down component of this circuit, namely
prefrontal-mediated regulation of the threat-related amygdala
reactivity, which is enhanced by bright-light intervention. These
complementary findings converge to support an effect of bright-
light intervention on this circuit that is consistent with its role
mediating responses to salient environmental stimuli. We focused
on a very well studied circuit for which there is extensive
evidence implicating it in the pathophysiology of mood and
affective disorders. Evaluating the effects of light intervention on
neural circuits (e.g., reward) or paradigms specifically probing
complementary features of this corticolimbic circuit (e.g., emotion
regulation paradigms) would benefit our understanding of the
pathways sensitive to bright-light exposure.
Serotonin signaling has been implicated in the pathophysiol-
ogy of SAD and modulating this circuit (10,11). Interestingly, a
recent study in rodents reported that direct retino-raphe projec-
tions modulate serotonergic signaling and depressive-like behav-
ioral phenotypes, providing a direct pathway through which
bright-light exposure can modulate serotonin signaling (43). In
the current study, we oversampled for individuals homozygous
for the 5-HTTLPR LA-allele, as a model for differences in serotonin
signaling and to evaluate possible effects of serotonin signaling
on bright-light intervention. Bright-light dose was positively
associated with intraprefrontal functional coupling in LG or S
carriers but not in LA/LA individuals. These findings are consistent
with a model of heightened sensitivity of LG or S carriers to
environmental input, including seasonal variation (12,44). We
have reported greater seasonal variability in 5-HTT binding in
S-allele carriers compared with LL homozygotes (12) [but see also
(45)]. Though 5-HTTLPR status did not significantly moderate
bright-light effects on task-related reactivity, effects neared
Figure 3. Serotonin transporter-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) significance for the left amygdala and mPFC (p ¼ .13 and p ¼
moderates effect of bright-light intervention on medial prefrontal cortex .11, respectively). Thus, there may be an effect that we are
(mPFC)-prefrontal functional coupling. (A) Statistical parametric map underpowered to detect with our sample size. Additionally, we
highlighting mPFC cluster, wherein the positive effect of bright-light suggest caution when interpreting our findings related to the
intervention on functional coupling with our mPFC seed was significantly 5-HTTLPR, as they, too, may be underpowered, which can increase
moderated by 5-HTTLPR genotype status (mPFC seed outlined within
inset). Color bar represents t scores. (B) Plot of bright-light intervention by
risk for spurious findings (46). This emphasizes the importance of
5-HTTLPR interaction effect showing mean functional coupling estimate additional studies probing how serotonin signaling interacts with
across 394 voxels and is intended only for visualization of interaction light intervention. Regardless, these findings provide intriguing
effect. Thirty individual data points are shown in blue or orange. Blue and evidence that the response to bright-light intervention of this
orange shading represents 95% confidence limit of regression lines for LG corticolimbic circuit may be moderated, in part, by serotonin
or S carriers (non-LA/LA) and LA/LA individuals, respectively. signaling. These associations could be more directly evaluated by
future studies pairing a bright-light intervention protocol with a
pharmacologic challenge of the serotonin system.
necessary to evaluate this hypothesis and determine whether Our current study is not without its limitations. There is no
changes in corticolimbic reactivity are associated with treatment specific clinical intervention threshold for bright-light therapy,
response. Such studies would also provide insight into a link but a 30-minute administration period is recommended for a
between bright-light effects on specific neurobiological mechanisms light dose of 10 klx (1). Thus, our study included intervention
and the subset of clinical symptoms (e.g., depressive vs. anxiety intensities at and below suggested clinical doses. Interestingly,
symptoms) that may be preferentially benefited by bright-light our findings indicate a dose-dependent association between light
therapy. intervention and effects on corticolimbic function across this
Neuroimaging studies in humans have reported an association range. Though we feel that we reasonably estimated light dose
between bidirectional amygdala-prefrontal connections and anxi- by measuring aspects of the environment in which intervention
ety (41,42), but rodent studies suggest this communication can was administered, it is important to note that maintaining
enhance and diminish responses to threatening environmental consistent light-intervention dose is more difficult than, e.g., a
cues (6). We observed that amygdala and prefrontal reactivity pharmacologic challenge using tablets. As such, we are limited in
were dose-dependently decreased by bright-light intervention. In our ability to precisely measure light dose received by each
contrast, amygdala-prefrontal and intraprefrontal functional participant. We suggest that future studies consider the use of

www.sobp.org/journal
338 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2014;76:332–339 P.M. Fisher et al.

equipment that may more precisely measure bright-light expo- 2. Terman M (2007): Evolving applications of light therapy. Sleep Med Rev
sure and other potentially relevant variables. That notwithstand- 11:497–507.
ing, a related benefit of our study design is that empirically 3. Rosenthal NE, Sack DA, Gillin JC, Lewy AJ, Goodwin FK, Davenport Y,
et al. (1984): Seasonal affective disorder. A description of the
estimating light dose as a continuous metric closely mimics real
syndrome and preliminary findings with light therapy. Arch Gen
variation that is to be expected when light intervention is Psychiatry 41:72–80.
administered in a clinical context. Unlike pharmacologic inter- 4. Vandewalle G, Schwartz S, Grandjean D, Wuillaume C, Balteau E,
vention studies, bright-light intervention lacks an easily acquired Degueldre C, et al. (2010): Spectral quality of light modulates
molecular marker for confirming treatment compliance. Despite emotional brain responses in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:
this, we encouraged compliance through daily mobile phone 19549–19554.
5. Praschak-Rieder N, Willeit M (2003): Treatment of seasonal affective
short message service reminders and the completion of log
disorders. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 5:389–398.
books. We did not observe significant effects of bright-light 6. Quirk GJ, Mueller D (2008): Neural mechanisms of extinction learning
intervention on mood or other state measures. Although this is and retrieval. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:56–72.
consistent with a previous study, it underscores a limitation of 7. Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, Lane R (2003): Neurobiology of
evaluating intervention effects in healthy individuals (47). Our emotion perception I: The neural basis of normal emotion perception.
study included only male subjects. Though this limits the general- Biol Psychiatry 54:504–514.
izability of our findings, our study design is precluded from 8. Stuhrmann A, Suslow T, Dannlowski U (2011): Facial emotion process-
ing in major depression: A systematic review of neuroimaging
confounds such as fluctuations in sex hormones. Future studies findings. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 1:10.
evaluating the effects of bright-light intervention on brain 9. Holmes A (2008): Genetic variation in cortico-amygdala serotonin
function in women would complement our current findings and function and risk for stress-related disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32:
speak to possible sex interaction effects. 1293–1314.
In summary, we show significant evidence for a dose- 10. Fisher PM, Hariri AR (2012): Linking variability in brain chemistry and
dependent effect of 3-week bright-light intervention on circuit function through multimodal human neuroimaging. Genes
Brain Behav 11:633–642.
threat-related corticolimbic reactivity and functional coupling. 11. Praschak-Rieder N, Willeit M (2012): Imaging of seasonal affective
Furthermore, the effect of bright-light intervention on intrapre- disorder and seasonality effects on serotonin and dopamine function
frontal functional coupling was moderated by 5-HTTLPR genotype in the human brain. Curr Top Behav Neurosci 11:149–167.
status, further implicating serotonin in modulating this circuit 12. Kalbitzer J, Erritzoe D, Holst KK, Nielsen FA, Marner L, Lehel S, et al.
and sensitivity to light. These findings represent an intriguing (2010): Seasonal changes in brain serotonin transporter binding in
step toward understanding neurobiological mechanisms affected short serotonin transporter linked polymorphic region-allele carriers
but not in long-allele homozygotes. Biol Psychiatry 30:1033–1039.
by bright-light intervention. This may, in turn, benefit our 13. Hu XZ, Lipsky RH, Zhu G, Akhtar LA, Taubman J, Greenberg BD, et al.
understanding of neurobiological mechanisms that bias risk (2006): Serotonin transporter promoter gain-of-function genotypes
for SAD and mediate responsiveness to bright-light interven- are linked to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Hum Genet 78:
tion as a therapeutic strategy in SAD and other affective 815–826.
disorders. 14. Lesch KP, Bengel D, Heils A, Sabol SZ, Greenberg BD, Petri S, et al. (1996):
Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin
transporter gene regulatory region. Science 274:1527–1531.
This study was funded by an unrestricted center grant to Cimbi 15. Grady CL, Siebner HR, Hornboll B, Macoveanu J, Paulson OB, Knudsen
from the Lundbeck Foundation. Support for the purchase of bright- GM (2013): Acute pharmacologically induced shifts in serotonin
light lamp materials was kindly provided by A.P. Møller and Hustru availability abolish emotion-selective responses to negative face
Chastine Mc-Kinney Møllers Fond til alemene Formaal. BM was emotions in distinct brain networks. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 23:
funded by a scholarship from Brain, Mind and Medicines. HRS was 368–378.
16. Harmer CJ, Mackay CE, Reid CB, Cowen PJ, Goodwin GM (2006):
supported by a Grant of Excellence ContAct from the Lundbeck
Antidepressant drug treatment modifies the neural processing of
Foundation (R59 A5399). PMF was funded by a grant from the nonconscious threat cues. Biol Psychiatry 59:816–820.
Danish Council for Independent Research. 17. Arce E, Simmons AN, Lovero KL, Stein MB, Paulus MP (2008):
We thank the Danish Research Centre for Magnetic Resonance Escitalopram effects on insula and amygdala BOLD activation during
for the magnetic resonance imaging facilities; Sussi Larsen, Julian emotional processing. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 196:661–672.
Macoveanu, and Pernille Iversen for assistance in data collection 18. Rosenthal NE, Genhart M, Sack DA, Skwerer RG, Wehr TA (1987):
and management; and Lone Freyr and Dorthe Givard for their Seasonal affective disorder: Relevance for treatment and research in
bulimia. In: Hudson EL, Pope HG, editors. Psychbioloy of Bulimia.
coordinated effort in maintaining blinding. We thank Anders Brusch Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, 205–228.
from Danish Fundamental Metrology and Professor Paul Michael 19. Sexton T, Buhr E, Van Gelder RN (2012): Melanopsin and mechanisms
Petersen from the Department of Photonics Engineering Diode of non-visual ocular photoreception. J Biol Chem 287:1649–1656.
Lasers and LED Systems at the Technical University of Denmark 20. Terman JS, Terman M, Lo ES, Cooper TB (2001): Circadian time of
for their help in measuring lamp characteristics. morning light administration and therapeutic response in winter
HRS declares that over the past 3 years he has received honoraria depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 58:69–75.
21. Lewy AJ, Bauer VK, Cutler NL, Sack RL, Ahmed S, Thomas KH, et al.
as reviewing editor for Neuroimage, as speaker for Biogen Idec (1998): Morning vs evening light treatment of patients with winter
Denmark A/S, and scientific Advisor for Lundbeck A/S, Valby, depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55:890–896.
Denmark. All other authors report no biomedical financial interests 22. Forsell Y (2005): The Major Depression Inventory versus Schedules for
or potential conflicts of interest. Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry in a population sample. Soc
Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 40:209–213.
Supplementary material cited in this article is available online at 23. McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman L (1992): Manual for the Profile of
Mood States. San Diego: EdITS/Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.11.031. 24. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R (1983): A global measure of
perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav 24:385–396.
1. Pail G, Huf W, Pjrek E, Winkler D, Willeit M, Praschak-Rieder N, Kasper S 25. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF III, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ (1989): The
(2011): Bright-light therapy in the treatment of mood disorders. Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric
Neuropsychobiology 64:152–162. practice and research. Psychiatry Res 28:193–213.

www.sobp.org/journal
P.M. Fisher et al. BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2014;76:332–339 339

26. Deichmann R, Gottfried JA, Hutton C, Turner R (2003): Optimized EPI 38. Mayberg HS (2003): Modulating dysfunctional limbic-cortical circuits
for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroimage 19:430–441. in depression: Towards development of brain-based algorithms for
27. Lundqvist D, Flykt A, Ohman A (1998): The Karolinska Directed diagnosis and optimised treatment. Br Med Bull 65:193–207.
Emotional Faces-KDEF [CD-ROM]. Stockholm: Department of Clinical 39. Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, Lane R (2003): Neurobiology of
Neuroscience, Psychology Section, Karolinska Institutet. emotion perception II: Implications for major psychiatric disorders. Biol
28. Lancaster JL, Woldorff MG, Parsons LM, Liotti M, Freitas CS, Rainey L, Psychiatry 54:515–528.
et al. (2000): Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain 40. Golden RN, Gaynes BN, Ekstrom RD, Hamer RM, Jacobsen FM, Suppes
mapping. Hum Brain Mapp 10:120–131. T, et al. (2005): The efficacy of light therapy in the treatment of mood
29. Lancaster JL, Summerln JL, Rainey L, Freitas CS, Fox PT (1997): The disorders: A review and meta-analysis of the evidence. Am J Psychiatry
Talairach Daemon, a database server for Talairach Atlas Labels. 162:656–662.
Neuroimage 5:S633. 41. Kim MJ, Gee DG, Loucks RA, Davis FC, Whalen PJ (2010): Anxiety
30. Maldjian JA, Laurienti PJ, Kraft RA, Burdette JH (2003): An automated dissociates dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex functional
method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based inter- connectivity with the amygdala at rest. Cereb Cortex 21:1667–1673.
rogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage 19:1233–1239. 42. Kim MJ, Whalen PJ (2009): The structural integrity of an amygdala-
31. Pandya DN, Van Hoesen GW, Mesulam MM (1981): Efferent connec- prefrontal pathway predicts trait anxiety. J Neurosci 29:11614–11618.
tions of the cingulate gyrus in the rhesus monkey. Exp Brain Res 42: 43. Ren C, Luan L, Wui-Man LB, Huang X, Yang J, Zhou Y, et al. (2013):
319–330. Direct retino-raphe projection alters serotonergic tone and affective
32. Barbas H (1995): Anatomic basis of cognitive-emotional interactions in behavior. Neuropsychopharmacology 38:1163–1175.
the primate prefrontal cortex. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 19:499–510. 44. Caspi A, Hariri AR, Holmes A, Uher R, Moffitt TE (2010): Genetic
33. Ongur D, Price JL (2000): The organization of networks within the sensitivity to the environment: The case of the serotonin transporter
orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. gene and its implications for studying complex diseases and traits. Am
Cereb Cortex 10:206–219. J Psychiatry 167:509–527.
34. Friston KJ (1994): Functional and effective connectivity in neuorimag- 45. Murthy NV, Selvaraj S, Cowen PJ, Bhagwagar Z, Riedel WJ, Peers P,
ing: A synthesis. Hum Brain Mapp 2:56–78. et al. (2010): Serotonin transporter polymorphisms (SLC6A4 insertion/
35. Brett M, Anton J, Valabregue R, Poline J (2002): Region of interest deletion and rs25531) do not affect the availability of 5-HTT to [11C]
analysis using an SPM toolbox. Neuroimage 16:S497. DASB binding in the living human brain. Neuroimage 52:50–54.
36. Fisher PM, Meltzer CC, Price JC, Coleman RL, Ziolko SK, Becker C, et al. 46. Button KS, Ioannidis JP, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ES,
(2009): Medial prefrontal cortex 5-HT2A density is correlated with Munafò MR (2013): Power failure: Why small sample size undermines
amygdala reactivity, response habituation, and functional coupling. the reliability of neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 14:365–376.
Cereb Cortex 19:2499–2507. 47. Rosenthal NE, Rotter A, Jacobsen FM, Skwerer RG (1987): No mood-
37. Vickers AJ, Altman DG (2001): Statistics notes: Analysing controlled altering effects found after treatment of normal subjects with bright
trials with baseline and follow up measurements. BMJ 323:1123–1124. light in the morning. Psychiatry Res 22:1–9.

www.sobp.org/journal

You might also like