You are on page 1of 7

International Journal of Psychophysiology 81 (2011) 152–158

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

International Journal of Psychophysiology


j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : w w w. e l s ev i e r. c o m / l o c a t e / i j p s yc h o

Performing music can induce greater modulation of emotion-related


psychophysiological responses than listening to music
Hidehiro Nakahara a,⁎, Shinichi Furuya b, 1, Tsutomu Masuko c, 2, Peter R. Francis d, Hiroshi Kinoshita e, 3
a
Morinomiya University of Medical Sciences, 1-26-16, Nankokita, Suminoe, Osaka 559-8611, Japan
b
Human Media Research Center, Kwansei Gakuin University, 2-1, Gakuen, Sanda, Hyogo 669-1337, Japan
c
Department of Music, Mukogawa Women's University, 6-46, Ikebiraki, Nishinomiya, Hyogo, 663-8558, Japan
d
Department of Exercise and Nutritional Sciences, San Diego State University, San Diego, CA 92182, USA
e
Biomechanics and Motor Control Laboratory, Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka University, 1-17, Machikaneyama, Toyonaka, Osaka 560-0043, Japan

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The present study investigated the differential effects of music-induced emotion on heart rate (HR) and its
Received 30 December 2010 variability (HRV) while playing music on the piano and listening to a recording of the same piece of music.
Received in revised form 2 May 2011 Sixteen pianists were monitored during tasks involving emotional piano performance, non-emotional piano
Accepted 5 June 2011
performance, emotional perception, and non-emotional perception. It was found that emotional induction
Available online 24 June 2011
during both perception and performance modulated HR and HRV, and that such modulations were
Keywords:
significantly greater during musical performance than during perception. The results confirmed that musical
Emotion performance was far more effective in modulating emotion-related autonomic nerve activity than musical
Music perception in musicians. The findings suggest the presence of a neural network of reward-emotion-associated
Performance autonomic nerve activity for musical performance that is independent of a neural network for musical
Perception perception.
Heart rate © 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction about 30 beats per min (bpm) greater than when the same individuals
listened to music that they perceived to be non-emotional. The
Music is universally enjoyed by both performers (instrumentalists authors also used a positron emission tomography (PET) technique to
and/or singers) and those who perceive musical performances (active monitor the activation of the central neural network associated with
or passive listeners). Changes in emotions generated by exposure to these emotional responses. Activation was observed in the ventral
music undoubtedly involve interactions with the central neural striatum, midbrain, amygdala, orbito-frontal, and medial prefrontal
network responsible for autonomic nerve functions, which can in cortex. They concluded that music recruited neural systems of reward
turn affect peripheral cardiovascular functions. A number of in- and emotion similar to those known to respond specifically to
vestigators have reported that musical perception is associated with biologically relevant stimuli, such as food and sex, and those that are
modulations in heart rate (HR) as well as its variability (HRV), blood artificially activated by drugs of abuse. Khalfa et al. (2002) monitored
pressure (BP), body temperature, perspiration, respiration and muscle skin conductance responses, a measure of sympathetic nerve activity,
tension (Allen et al., 2001; Bernardi et al., 2006, 2009; Blood and in subjects listening to musical excerpts that could induce different
Zatorre, 2001; Iwanaga et al., 2005; Khalfa et al., 2002; Ravaja and kinds of emotions (fear, happiness, sadness, and peacefulness). They
Kallinen, 2004; Sammler et al., 2007; White, 1999). For example, reported that fear and happiness produced greater sympathetic
Blood and Zatorre (2001) reported that individuals who listened to activity than sadness and peacefulness. Using a fast Fourier transform
self-selected music that consistently elicited intensely pleasant analysis of HRV, Iwanaga et al. (2005) observed that its high-
emotional responses, experienced elevated HR and respiration rate. frequency (HF) component, which reflects the activity of parasym-
The HR of subjects listening to the music with the highest valence was pathetic nerves, was decreased in subjects who listened to an
excitative type of music. Conversely, when the same subjects listened
⁎ Corresponding author. Tel.: +81 6 6616 6911, +81 6 6616 6914; fax: +81 6 6616 to a sedative type of music, the HF component increased.
6912. Bernardi et al. (2006) used autoregressive power spectrum
E-mail addresses: nakahara@morinomiya-u.ac.jp (H. Nakahara), sfuruya@umn.edu analysis to examine the HRV, ventilation, BP, and mid-cerebral artery
(S. Furuya), tomasuko@hotmail.com (T. Masuko), peterfrancis@cox.net (P.R. Francis), flow velocity of individuals listening passively to music of different
hkinoshita@moted.hss.osaka-u.ac.jp (H. Kinoshita).
1
Tel./fax: +81 079 565 786.
tempi and structures (harmony, melody, and rhythm). They reported
2
Tel./fax: +81 722 37 0067. that, when compared with baseline measurements, the ventilation,
3
Tel.: +81 6 6850 6034; fax: +81 6 6850 6030. BP, HR, the ratio of low-frequency (LF) and HF (LF/HF) increased at

0167-8760/$ – see front matter © 2011 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.06.003
H. Nakahara et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 81 (2011) 152–158 153

faster tempi and with simpler rhythmic structures. Conversely, mid- the subjects for this investigation. Their ages ranged from 22 to
cerebral artery flow and an index of baroreflex decreased under these 33 years (mean ± SD = 24.8 ± 4.9 years). Eight of the subjects were
same conditions. The authors also observed that musicians had greater professional performers, and the remaining 8 were graduate students
respiratory sensitivity to music tempo than did non-musicians. who were majoring in piano performance at the School of Music at
Subsequently, Bernardi et al. (2009) examined dynamic cardiovascular two universities in the Kansai (middle-west) region of Japan. These
and respiratory responses to various classical and operatic music latter students were aspiring to be professional pianists. All subjects
selections. Vocal and orchestral crescendos produced significant reported that within the past three years they had undergone
correlations between cardiovascular or respiratory signals and musical standard medical health checkups which included the measurement
profile, particularly skin vasoconstriction and BP, in proportion to of resting ECG, BP, chest X-ray, haemogram, and liver function, and all
crescendo. In contrast, uniform emphasis induced skin vasodilation and were classified as being “healthy”.
reduction in BP. It was also noted that the cardiovascular autonomic It was recognized that the validity of the present investigation was
variables were entrained by 10-s period rhythmic phrases. In addition, dependent upon each subject's ability to monitor his or her own
the authors observed that these phenomena occurred in all subjects emotions during performance. This was facilitated in part by the
examined, regardless of musical training, practice, or personal taste, selection of a piece of music that was not technically challenging for
even in an absence of emotional responses. The investigators concluded highly skilled and experienced performers (see below). Subsequently,
that music could be sensed and processed at a subconscious level, the procedure for subject selection was rigorous. Initially a group of 25
closely mirrored by autonomic cardiovascular responses. highly skilled pianists residing in the Kansai region was identified
In contrast to the numerous published reports of responses to from lists of participants who had appeared in recent major domestic
musical perception, a review of the literature revealed few in- and international competitions. A letter describing the general nature
vestigations of the emotion-associated autonomic cardiovascular of our proposed psycho-physiological study was sent to each member
responses of musicians during their own performances. Parr (1988) of the group in order to solicit interest in participating in the
monitored the HR, BP, cardiac output and forearm blood flow of investigation. Seventeen pianists who expressed interest were then
trained pianists while they performed four-octave, bilateral scales at interviewed telephonically, and voluntarily provided information
various tempi. HR progressively increased from 88 bpm to 120 bpm. about their piano education backgrounds and performance careers.
Other cardiac measures also increased with increasing keystroke Those who were actively performing, had more than 15 years of piano
tempo. However, due to the fact that the bilateral-scale is undoubt- training, and had no history of cardiovascular or neurological
edly not a task that would induce strong emotions in trained pianists, problems, were then given additional information about the purpose
it must be assumed that the observed changes largely reflected the of the study, and the experimental tasks to be performed during the
effect of physiological load. We have recently compared expression- experiment.
related physiological and biomechanical changes in trained pianists Approximately one month before data were collected, each of the
while they played the same musical piece with and without 17 volunteers came to the laboratory and performed the experimental
expression (Nakahara et al., 2010). Expressive and emotional music using an upright piano (U1, YAMAHA Co., Japan). In particular,
performance resulted in increased HR and sweat rate, and decreased they were asked to report whether they could successfully play the
para-sympathetic nerve activity estimated from HRV. piece in the laboratory using their usual expressive manner, and also
The apparent dearth of information about the emotion-related perform the same piece without expressing their emotions during the
autonomic and physiological responses to music performance is performance. With only a single exception, the prospective subjects
surprising. From a psycho-neurophysiological perspective, such infor- adamantly indicated that they could comply with these requests. The
mation may be potentially more valuable than information concerning individual who indicated that she was not confident that she could
musical perception, especially to performers who have professional comply was not included as a subject in the investigation. Written
and economic interests in these matters. Such information is also of informed consent was then obtained from each subject, and the Ethics
potential value for music therapists who prescribe activities involving Committee of Osaka University graduate school of medicine approved
the playing of musical instruments and singing as treatment modalities all procedures.
for their patients. Musical performance is distinctly different from
musical perception because the performer can integrate voluntary 2.2. Experimental music
sensorimotor actions that convey his or her unique artistic expression
and emotions during the performance of a musical piece. This suggests The music selected for the present study was the well-tempered
that the levels of emotion attainable with musical performance are Clavier, Vol. I, prelude No. 1 written by J. S. Bach in 18th century. This
necessarily higher than those experienced during musical perception. music was chosen for several reasons. First, in order to maintain a
Therefore, during the course of the present study we examined the constant respiratory rate it was necessary to avoid music with
effects of emotions on the autonomic functions associated with musical irregularities in tempo and/or rhythm (i.e. rubato). Baroque piano
perception and performance. Specifically, using highly trained pianists music is suitable in this respect because it is generally incompatible
as subjects, we investigated the effects of emotional input on HR and with rubato, and it encourages performers to maintain constant
HRV when listening to a J.S. Bach prelude. These parameters were also playing tempo with relatively constant loudness. Second, this piece
monitored when the same subjects performed the same musical piece can be performed predominately by hand and finger movements, and
on the piano. During both the perception and performance conditions this helps to prevent excessive body movement. Third, all of the
two contrasting conditions of emotion were examined. Under one of the subjects had in fact played it prior to participation in the investigation,
conditions subjects were requested to respond expressively or and were capable of playing it without technical concerns. This was
emotionally to the music. Under the other condition subjects were important to avoid confounding effects on autonomic activity due to
requested to inhibit emotional feeling in a voluntary manner. playing unfamiliar and/or technically demanding pieces. Fourth, vocal
and orchestral music may be suitable for eliciting stronger emotion-
2. Methods related cardiovascular responses in the listeners than solo uniform music
(Bernardi et al., 2009). However recording of the same physiological
2.1. Subjects measures in performers during naturalistic vocal and orchestral perfor-
mance is quite difficult.
Sixteen active, healthy Japanese classical pianists (12 females and In our earlier survey of valence and arousal resulting from listening
4 males), all of whom had at least 17 years of piano training, served as to different types of music, music-major students (N = 34) as well as
154 H. Nakahara et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 81 (2011) 152–158

non-music-major students (N = 29) nearly equally rated this music controlled and temperature-controlled recording studio. The exper-
with high valence (pleasant and happy: average ± SD = 8.3 ± 1.6 imental set-up consisted of a three-lead electrocardiogram (ECG, DS-
point) and with relatively low arousal (calm: 2.3 ± 1.5 point) on a 10- 2150, Fukuda Denshi Co., Japan), a sound-level meter (NL-20, Rion Co.,
point scale (Nakahara et al., 2010). Japan), a compact stereo DAT recorder (TCD-D100, Sony Co., Japan), a
stereo-sound amplifier (PMA-201SA, Denon co., Japan), a set of
2.3. Experimental tasks speakers (SS-K10ED, Sony Co., Japan), an automatic respiratory gas
analyzing system (AE-280S, Minato Med Sci. co., Japan), a position
Six experimental tasks were examined in the present study. These sensor system (C-5049, Hamamatsu photonics co., Japan), and an
were, (1) playing the experimental prelude in the performer's usual upright piano. A microphone connected to a sound-level meter was
artistic and expressive manner with instructions to voluntarily hung from the ceiling at a height of 1 m above the keyboard surface of
minimize ancillary movements of the head, trunk, and upper arms the piano. The level of sound pressure was then obtained from the
(the emotional performance task), (2) playing the same piece without sampled peak sound data. The speakers were placed on the side of the
emotions and without ancillary movements (the non-emotional piano. The gain of the sound amplifier during the listening tasks was
performance task), (3) playing the same music on the piano without adjusted for each subject to provide a similar level of sound pressure
emotion and throughout using a forte-touch (the non-emotional measured in the vicinity of the performer's head during the performance
forte-performance task), (4) listening to a recording of the musical tasks.
performance that was obtained when the same subject performed
the emotional performance task (the emotional perception task),
2.5. Procedures
(5) listening to the recording obtained while the subject performed
the non-emotional performance task (the non-emotional perception
One week before scheduled data collection, each subject came to
task), and (6) resting in a stationary sitting position (the control task).
the experimental room in the laboratory. The subject was then given
It has been recommended that segments of HR should be at least 5 min
the opportunity to play the experimental music and become
in duration for purposes of HRV analysis (Task Force, 1996). Therefore,
accustomed to the laboratory piano. They also practiced breathing
each task lasted for 5 min during which time the prelude was performed
while maintaining a constant respiration rate of 15 bpm with the
two and half times. Musical tempo for all conditions was regulated using a
guidance of a metronome during all tasks. The subjects were also
metronome and performed at 60 bpm. During the performance of the
given additional instructions, including avoidance of strenuous
perception tasks, subjects were requested to listen to the music with the
exercise and maintaining customary diet for 24 h preceding the day
same level of attention that they utilized when they were engaged in
of data collection, and to refrain from ingesting food, alcohol and
the emotional performance task or the non-emotional performance task.
caffeine for at least 2 h preceding the data collection session. Subjects
During perception tasks subjects listened to their own music performance
were also asked to practice playing the piece at home until they were
recorded prior to the experiment. This methodology was selected on the
able to play without viewing the score, and to be confident that they
basis of a pilot study using 9 pianists as subjects. During this latter
could play the piece while inhibiting their emotional involvement,
investigation we examined the differential psychological effects of
avoiding ancillary body motions, and maintaining the same constant
listening to the current experimental music recorded by the performer
respiration rate. A few days prior to the data collection session, each
him/her self and listening to the same piece played by other professional
subject visited the lab to record his/her performance of the emotional
pianists. Seven of the 9 subjects (78%) reported that listening to the other
and non-emotional tasks using a DAT recorder. Subsequently, subjects
players' performance differed clearly from their own in terms of musical
were asked to practice the listening tasks utilizing the recordings of
expression (touch, loudness, tempo etc.). These subjects stated that the
their own performances.
experimental music was an easy piece for them to play without any errors,
After arriving at the experimental room on the day of data collection,
and that they preferred to listen to their own musical performance in
Ag/AgCL disposable ECG electrodes were attached to the chest to
order to become fully involved at the emotional level, rather than
measure HR, and the subject was fitted with a mask which covered the
attempting to do so when listening to the recordings of other performers.
nose and mouth during respiratory gas analysis. Subsequently LEDs
The music selected for this experiment did not require significant
were mounted on the skin over the spinous processes of vertebra
motions of the body beyond the limited actions of the upper extremities
prominens and the fifth lumbar vertebra, and the centers of the right
necessary to perform the piece. However, subjects were requested to
coracoid process and the most superficial point on the great trochanter.
avoid non-essential movements of the body during performance tasks.
The subject was then requested to maintain a seated position in front of
Comparable restrictions on movement of the trunk, head, and upper
the piano for approximately 15 min to achieve a stable and calm
extremity during the emotional and non-emotional conditions was
cardiovascular status. The subject then performed each of the six
requested because movements of the large segments of the body
experimental tasks. Task order was randomly assigned for each of the
(mainly the trunk and head) can significantly modulate the HR
subjects. Adaptation periods of approximately 5–8 min between the
response, especially during emotional performance. Nevertheless,
tasks allowed HR to return to baseline.
increased physical load due to higher muscular tension may occur
when playing the piano with emotions. To assess this effect, we added
the third experimental condition in which subjects were asked to play 2.6. Subjective evaluation measures
the experimental music at a higher (fortissimo) tone level without
emotions, and this condition was compared with the non-emotional At the conclusion of data collection for each of the tasks the subject
performance condition. During the perception tasks, the subjects were was asked to provide a subjective rating for his or her responses to the
required to place their hands on the piano keys and assume the same experimental music using 10-point rating scales. The scales were used
body position they had adopted during the performance tasks. They to designate the levels of valence (1 = very unpleasant, and 10 = very
were also requested to keep their eyes open and to attempt to minimize pleasant) and arousal (1 = lowest arousal, and 10 = highest arousal).
motions of any portion of the body. In addition, subjects were asked to report if they were able to elicit
emotions in response to the emotional performance and perception
2.4. Apparatus tasks. They were also requested to identify the portion or measures
where the highest pleasant emotions were perceived, and finally they
The apparatus used for data collection was assembled in a were asked if they felt they were successful in inhibiting emotional
laboratory that provided a comparable environment to an acoustically arousal during each of the non-emotional tasks.
H. Nakahara et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 81 (2011) 152–158 155

2.7. Heart rate and its variability task × emotion interaction for the mean value of arousal level,
indicating a higher emotion-induced arousal level during perfor-
2.7.1. Heart rate data treatment mance than during perception. A task × emotion interaction for the
From the onset of each experimental task, ECG and sound were mean value of valence level was, however, not significant. Significant
continuously monitored and recorded using a personal computer via an main effects of task and emotion were also found for both arousal and
A/D converter at a sampling rate of 1000 Hz. Occasional ectopic beats valence levels. The highest level of pleasant feeling was commonly
such as irregularity of the heart rhythm involving extra or skipped heart reported to have been experienced during the 28th and 29th
beats were visually identified and manually replaced with interpolated measures after the onset of the music (see Fig. 1). Some reported
adjacent R–R interval values. ECG data were translated into bpm data for the highest level of pleasant feeling at the 21st and 22nd bars, and
each R–R interval. They were then re-sampled using cubic-spline some at the 33rd bar.
interpolation to obtain an equally sampled time series.
3.2. Heart rate and its variability
2.7.2. HRV analysis
A series of 256 consecutive R–R intervals were extracted for 5 min HR and HRV data were evaluated using 5-min data from the entire
HR data, re-sampled at 2 Hz, and then subjected to the autoregressive experimental period. Fig. 1 shows changes in the 10-s mean values of
method of HRV analysis. Using this method, an autoregressive spectrum HR for all subjects during 5 min for five experimental tasks. The
was calculated by fitting a 12th-order autoregressive model into the R–R emotional performance task produced constantly higher HR than the
data. The autoregressive model parameters were solved using a other tasks, while the rest and non-emotional perception tasks
forward–backward least squares method, while the autoregressive consistently produced the lowest HR values during the entire testing
spectrum was obtained from the estimated autoregressive parameters. period. It was observed that HR was already high at the beginning of
The frequency bands for LF, HF, and total frequency (TF) were 0.04– the performance tasks, but this phenomenon was not apparent for the
0.15 Hz, 0.15–0.40 Hz, and 0.01–0.4 Hz, respectively. HF/TF has been perception tasks. The findings suggested that there was an anticipa-
recognized as a parameter to assess the level of vagal modulation. LF/HF tory elevation of HR associated with playing the piano. The mean
is considered to mirror the sympathovagal balance or to reflect the values of HR for the entire 5 min, expressed as a percentage of the
sympathetic modulations (Task Force, 1996). corresponding rest value, for all subjects are given in Table 1. ANOVA
revealed a significant task × emotion interaction, and their main
2.8. Trunk movement and oxygen uptake effects, for this relative value of HR (Table 1). The phase fluctuation
was also greater during the emotional performance task than the
In order to determine the relative success with which subjects other tasks, and thus it had the largest range of HR (Fig. 1).
were able to inhibit significant movements of the body during the Table 1 also gives the results of the HRV analyses. The group means
performance tasks, the movement of the trunk in the frontal, and of HF/TF and LF/HF obtained from the autoregressive analysis are
sagittal planes, and the arm in the sagittal plane were monitored plotted in Fig. 2. There was a significant task × emotion interaction
throughout the performances. The movements of the trunk and arm effect, as well as main effects of task and emotion for HF/TF and LF/HF
were recorded using a position sensor camera (sampling freq. (Table 1).
= 150 Hz) located 1.5 m from the dorsal side and 3.5 m to the right The mean values of the sound pressure level of the piano tone were
side of the subject. The data were digitally smoothed at a cut-off virtually identical for emotional (79.2 ± 1.5 dB) and non-emotional
frequency of 10 Hz using a second-order Butterworth digital filter. (79.4 ± 1.5 dB) tasks of musical performance.
Frontal and sagittal plane angles were then generated using an inner
product method. The total angular excursion of the trunk in both 3.3. Oxygen uptake, and movements of the trunk and arm
sagittal and frontal planes, and upper arm in the sagittal plane, during
the entire playing period were then computed. The mean values of VO2 for all subjects for all experimental tasks
During all experimental tasks, oxygen uptake (VO2) was measured are shown in Table 2. The values were higher for the performance
using a gas analysis system interfaced with a laboratory personal tasks compared with the perception tasks. ANOVA revealed a
computer. The data were continuously monitored on a breath-by- significant difference only for the task (Table 2). Table 2 also shows
breath basis during the experimental period, and were continuously the mean values of total angular excursion of the trunk in the frontal
stored in the computer. The calibration of O2 and CO2 was performed (medio-lateral movement) and sagittal (fore-aft movement) planes,
with a standard gas of known concentration before each test. VO2 and the upper arm in the sagittal plane during the perception and
relative to body weight was computed for the evaluation of energy performance tasks. For all of these excursion values, ANOVA revealed
expenditure during the task period. a significant task effect, but neither the task × emotion interaction
effect nor the emotion effect was significant. The magnitudes of the
2.9. Statistical analysis differences in the mean values of energy expenditure, and movement
of the trunk and arm between the emotional and no-emotional
Depending on the purpose of the comparison, we used either a performance conditions were negligible, indicating that the effect
two-way or a one-way ANOVA with repeated measures. Independent of body movement on HR and HRV was of little or no practical
variables were task (perception versus performance), and emotion significance.
(emotional versus non-emotional). If the sphericity assumption was
violated, then Greenhouse–Geisser degrees of freedom corrections 3.4. Playing at a louder volume
were applied. Post-hoc analyses were conducted using Tukey's
procedure. Statistical significance was accepted at P b 0.05. The mean value of sound pressure measured during the entire
period for the non-emotional fortissimo performance was 83.3 ±
3. Results 1.4 dB, which was significantly higher than 79.4 ± 1.5 dB observed
during the non-emotional and regular (= less forceful) performance
3.1. Subjective measures (F1, 15 = 117.2, p b 0.001). The mean value of HR for all subjects during
the 5-min period for non-emotional forte performance was 81.7 ±
The mean values of the valence and arousal levels of all conditions 5.9 bpm, which did not significantly differ from 81.5 ± 6.8 bpm for the
for all subjects are reported in Table 1. ANOVA revealed a significant non-emotional regular performance. None of the HRV-parameter
156 H. Nakahara et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 81 (2011) 152–158

Table 1
Five-minute data for HR, HRV and subjective evaluation measures.

Task Performance Perception ANOVA F-values

Emotion Non-emotional Emotional Non-emotional Emotional Task × emotion Task Emotion

Arousal level 1.9 ± 1.0 4.0 ± 1.6 1.8 ± 0.9 2.8 ± 1.3 5.5⁎ 9.9⁎⁎ 38.8⁎⁎⁎
Valence level 4.3 ± 1.8 8.8 ± 0.9 3.1 ± 1.7 7.8 ± 1.3 0.2 13.9⁎⁎ 105.3⁎⁎⁎
HR (%) 110.6 ± 8.6 117.8 ± 9.4 103.3 ± 4.4 106.0 ± 4.8 9.5⁎⁎ 24.0⁎⁎⁎ 44.1⁎⁎⁎
HF/TF 23.4 ± 14.8 15.6 ± 9.6 33.4 ± 18.6 31.5 ± 18.0 6.5⁎ 5.1⁎ 8.5⁎
LF/HF 5.0 ± 4.0 6.8 ± 5.5 2.3 ± 1.7 2.5 ± 1.6 13.5⁎⁎ 5.7⁎ 17.5⁎⁎⁎

The values are the mean and standard deviations for all subjects.
⁎ P b 0.05.
⁎⁎ P b 0.01.
⁎⁎⁎ P b 0.001

values were significantly different during these two conditions. The (see Introduction). In the present investigation we utilized expert
mean value of VO2 was 4.8 ± 0.7 ml/min/kg for the fortissimo pianists to investigate the effect of emotion induction. Uniquely, we
performance. This also did not differ from the mean value obtained compared the emotion-induction condition with the non-emotion-
during the regular performance (see Table 2). induction condition while subjects listened to a Bach prelude
The mean values of total angular excursion of the trunk in performed on the piano and while the same subjects actually played
the frontal and sagittal planes obtained while playing at a louder the same music on the piano. It was found that HR during emotional
volume were 35.5 ± 12.8 and 34.2 ± 19.0 rad, respectively, and the musical perception was significantly higher than during non-
corresponding movements of the upper arm in the sagittal plane emotional musical perception. In addition, the HR response during
was 43.6 ± 15.0 rad. ANOVA revealed that the means for the trunk non-emotional musical perception was approximately equal to that
(F1, 15 =10.3, pb 0.01) and arm (F1, 15 =30.8 pb 0.001) in the sagittal observed during a resting condition. The indices of autonomic nerve
plane were significantly larger than those during the non-emotional activity suggested that this response was caused both by the decrease
condition (see the mean values for the non-emotional condition in in parasympathetic nerve activity and the increase in sympathetic
Table 2). nerve activity when the subjects performed an emotional task. All
subjects also reported that emotion-induction gave them a higher
4. Discussion

The results of the present investigation yielded two novel findings: A


(1) emotion induction during perception of the selected piece of
( rest% ) %HR
music as well as during performance of the same music modulated HR
and indices of autonomic nerve activity, and (2) such modulations 130
were much greater during performance than during perception.
120 Performance task
4.1. The effects of emotion induction 110
Perception task
The effect of music on emotion-related autonomic nerve activities 100
or brain activity has been monitored in the course of many studies
that have examined expressiveness or attention to musical emotions 90
non-emotional emotional

a b c d B HF / TF
94 (ratio)
60
e e
90
40
Perception task
HR ( beats / min )

86 20
Performance task
0
82 non-emotional emotional

78 C LF / HF
(ratio)

74 16

12
70
-60 0 60 120 180 240 300 8 Performance task
Time ( second ) 4 Perception task
Fig. 1. Time course of the 10-s mean value of HR during the pre-and experimental 0
non-emotional emotional
period for all experimental conditions. ○, emotional performance; ●, non-emotional
performance; □, emotional perception; ■, non-emotional perception; –, the control Emotion
task; a, a signal indicating 30 s remaining before the task onset; b, the task onset
moment; c, the end of the first round of music; d, the end of the second round of music; Fig. 2. The mean values of %HR (A), HF/TF (B) and LF/HF (C) for all subjects for each task.
e, the duration for the most frequent response of highest emotions. The vertical bar indicates the SD value.
H. Nakahara et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 81 (2011) 152–158 157

Table 2
The mean values for oxygen uptake, and trunk and arm movements.

Task Performance Perception ANOVA F-values

Emotion Non-emotional Emotional Non-emotional Emotional Task × emotion Task Emotion

VO2 (ml/min/kg) 4.7 ± 0.8 4.8 ± 0.8 4.1 ± 0.9 4.0 ± 0.5 1.1 34.7⁎⁎⁎ 0.1

Excursion (rad)
Trunk in frontal plane 34.0 ± 11.7 34.6 ± 7.4 29.2 ± 13.9 27.0 ± 8.6 1.9 17.5⁎⁎⁎ 0.3
Trunk in sagital plane 28.7 ± 15.5 29.8 ± 14.5 15.9 ± 4.4 15.3 ± 3.8 1.4 62.6⁎⁎⁎ 0.1
Arm in sagital plane 36.5 ± 12.3 40.0 ± 13.2 19.0 ± 4.9 18.8 ± 5.1 2.3 60.2⁎⁎⁎ 1.4

The values are the mean and standard deviations for all subjects.
⁎⁎⁎ P b 0.001.

level of arousal as well as valence (pleasant feeling) than non- present investigation involved more detailed psycho-physiological
emotional-induction during both performance and perception of and biomechanical analyses of piano performance with a larger
the Bach's prelude, indicating that the subjects must have received a number of highly trained pianists.
higher level of reward and positive emotions under the current One may question whether the greater increase in HR and LF/HF,
emotion-induction conditions. The present findings suggest that and decrease in HF/TF during emotional performance was due to
emotional arousal accompanied with peripheral physiological and greater movement of the trunk, arms, and hands. The subjects in this
autonomic nerve activity modulations with musical stimuli seem to study were instructed to attempt to maintain comparable restrictions
require an active effort to rouse stimuli-associated emotions. Recent on movements of the trunk, head, and upper extremity during the
brain imaging studies have provided evidence that the basal forebrain emotional and non-emotional conditions. The finding that there was
and certain brainstem nuclei, along with cortical areas involved in no emotion × task interaction effect on trunk and arm movement, as
emotional evaluation, such as the orbitofrontal and insular regions, well as on energy expenditure, appears to confirm that subjects were
may play an important role in processing of conscious intense able to effectively comply with the instructions. Therefore, it may be
emotional experience with music, which may elicit sensation of chills concluded that emotion-related body movement and its associated
(Blood and Zatorre, 2001; Koelsch et al., 2008; Menon and Levitin, physiological cost were not greater during performance than during
2005). Janata et al. (2002) reported that attentive listening to music perception. In addition, the increased level of physical activity during
requires the contribution of the superior temporal cortex, several non-emotional fortissimo performance was not accompanied by
regions in the frontal cortex, and inferior parietal cortex. These brain meaningful increases in metabolic and autonomic nerve activity.
areas are therefore most likely the location of the central neural These results suggested that muscular activity due to movements of
mechanism for the emotion-related modulation in HR and associated the trunk, arms, and hands was not a factor for the increased HR and
autonomic nerve activity during performance and perception. the HRV parameter modulation observed during emotional perfor-
The effect of active emotion induction and the corresponding mance compared to non-emotional performance. These changes are
cardiovascular changes observed in the present study were most likely more likely attributable to a higher level of emotional arousal with
initiated by a conscious level of autonomic nerve activity. However, a playing the music compared to that with listening to the music. The
recent report by Bernardi et al. (2009) has provided evidence that there data generated from the performers' subjective evaluations of the
can also be subconscious reflex autonomic responses, possibly involving attainment of emotional arousal provide some support for this
respiration and cardiovascular parameters. They demonstrated assumption; there was a higher level of pleasant feeling when playing
that music with vocal or orchestral progressive crescendos (Puccini's with emotions compared to when listening (see Table 1, task × emo-
“Turandot” and Beethoven's Ninth Symphony adagio, respectively) tion interaction) although the subjects reported that they made their
produced significant correlations between cardiovascular or respiratory best efforts to direct their emotional inflow during both performance
signals and musical profiles, particularly skin vasoconstriction and BP. and perception tasks.
These autonomic responses were more apparent than those associated Why is musical performance more effective for modulating
with music characterized by stability and uniformity (Bach's cancata), emotion-induced autonomic nerve activity than listening to a
which induced vasodilation and a reduction in BP. They also found that recording of the same self-performed music? One explanation for
some music has phrasing with similar rhythm (6 cycles/min) to the this may be that for our highly skilled and experienced subjects,
spontaneous waves in BP and other circulatory variables. The authors musical rewards or pleasurable feelings stemming from playing the
concluded that phrases at this periodicity entrains spontaneous piano exceed those that are generated from listening to recorded
cardiovascular fluctuations to the music rhythm and modifies cardio- music. As discussed above, it has been shown that a neural circuit for
vascular control. Their findings support the concept that cardiovascular rewards from the perception of pleasurable music involves the
modulation by music may not only be a result of emotions, but instead mesolimbic, prefrontal, and temporal structures (Blood and Zatorre,
may in turn influence emotions, likely in a bidirectional manner. This 2001; Koelsch et al., 2008; Menon and Levitin, 2005; Sammler et al.,
latter concept needs to be examined in a future study using musical 2007). However, there is a dearth of information about a reward
pieces that include effective use of crescendos to evoke stronger arousal circuit associated with musical performance. In a study by Parsons
responses. et al. (2005), PET was used to identify and compare the areas of the
pianist's brain activated when playing a Bach concerto and those areas
4.2. Musical performance is more effective in inducing autonomic nerve activated during playing simple scales. They found that activations of
response to emotion than listening to music the temporal, parietal, and frontal cortical areas, as well as the
thalamus, basal ganglia, and the cerebellum in the subcortical areas
The results of the present study indicate that playing the piano were more extensive while playing the concerto than they were while
induced a greater increase in HR and decrease in parasympathetic playing scales. The particular areas subserving emotions and rewards
activity during emotional performance than was observed during were, however, not appreciably active during the performance of the
emotional perception. This observation is in agreement with the concerto. A possible explanation for this observation was that subjects
findings of our preliminary investigation (Nakahara et al., 2009). The were unable to actively express their emotions due to the restricted
158 H. Nakahara et al. / International Journal of Psychophysiology 81 (2011) 152–158

and unfamiliar physical conditions of a PET experimental environ- respiration rate after each test, it was not monitored to on-line check
ment (upright, limited body movements, the face being covered by a whether the participants really adhered to the constant rate during task
plastic mask, etc.). Another possible explanation may be that the performance. Second, we utilized a playing tempo that was controlled
expressive neural network for musical playing is different from that by a metronome, rather than allowing each subject to perform at the
for musical perception. Activations observed in the posterior tempo of his or her own choice. We recognize that musicians tend to
cingulate, insula, basal ganglia, and cerebellum may form the synchronize their breathing with music during performances. There-
expressive neural network for expressive musical playing. These fore, in theory, playing at a given fixed rate might have altered their
brain areas are portions of the so-called “sensorimotor-associated” natural musical performance. In fact, none of the subjects in any way
cortical and subcortical areas, and they also have a strong connection indicated that they were concerned about the selected tempo of 60 bpm.
with the autonomic nervous centers (Spyer, 1999). Therefore, there Third, a Bach's prelude with relatively uniform emphasis was used in
can be a neural network of a reward-emotion processor for musical this study. The use of other types of music that may contain many phases
playing that has firm neural connections with the sensorimotor- of crescendos/decrescendos, and/or irregular tempi could induce
associated cortical and subcortical structures, which can then stronger arousal-related or biologically inherent cardiovascular re-
modulate cardiovascular and autonomic nerve activities, possibly sponses (Bernardi et al., 2009). We therefore acknowledge that the
independent from that for musical perception. It is also safe to assume findings from the present study provided information about a very
that this network is extraordinarily well-developed in expert pianists specific musical condition. Lastly, the number of subjects used was
as a result of their many years of training, and thus it is plausible that relatively low, which might have introduced type II errors in the
this network contributed to the higher HR and decreased parasym- statistical results.
pathetic activity observed during playing than were observed during
listening.
It can also be argued that elevated HR during emotional musical References
performance, together with high levels of HR observed at the onset of
Allen, K., Golden, L.H., Izzo Jr., J.L., Ching, M.I., Forrest, A., Niles, C.R., Niswander, P.R.,
performance, suggest involvement of a strong anticipatory cardiac Barlow, J.C., 2001. Normalization of hypertensive responses during ambulatory
facilitation. However, we did not anticipate that this would occur surgical stress by perioperative music. Psychosom. Med. 63, 487–492.
because the selected musical piece did not represent a demanding Bernardi, L., Porta, C., Sleight, P., 2006. Cardiovascular, cerebrovascular, and respiratory
changes induced by different types of music in musicians and non-musicians: the
task for our highly skilled subjects, and expressive-emotional playing importance of silence. Heart 92, 445–452.
was a routine condition for them. Nevertheless, we confirmed some Bernardi, L., Porta, C., Casucci, G., Balsamo, R., Bernardi, N.F., Fogari, R., Sleight, P., 2009.
elevation of HR about 30 s before the onset of playing, which was Dynamic interactions between musical, cardiovascular, and cerebral rhythms in
humans. Circulation 119 (25), 3171–3180.
followed by an additional marked increase immediately after a
Blood, A.J., Zatorre, R.J., 2001. Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with
“ready” call given about 10 s before the start of piano playing (see activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
Fig. 1). In the present experiment, therefore, there could also have 98, 11818–11823.
Iwanaga, M., Kobayashi, A., Kawasaki, C., 2005. Heart rate variability with repetitive
been a strong anticipatory increase in HR before the emotional
exposure to music. Biol. Psychol. 70, 61–66.
musical performance due possibly to the pianists being highly aroused Janata, P., Tillmann, B., Bharucha, J.J., 2002. Listening to polyphonic music recruits
for the emotional performance, or strongly motivated for playing a domain-general attention and working memory circuits. Cogn. Affect. Behav.
pleasurable music piece. This baseline shift of autonomic function Neurosci. 2, 121–140.
Khalfa, S., Isabelle, P., Jean-Pierre, B., Manon, R., 2002. Event-related skin conductance
might have also contributed to the higher level of HR during the initial responses to musical emotions in humans. Neurosci. Lett. 328, 145–149.
performance period. The preparatory modulation of autonomic nerve Koelsch, S., Kilches, S., Steinbeis, N., Schelinski, S., 2008. Effects of unexpected chords
activity as well as a more dynamic modulation of HR during musical and of performer's expression on brain responses and electrodermal activity. PLoS
One 3, e2631.
performance merit more detailed examination in future investiga- Menon, V., Levitin, D.J., 2005. The rewards of music listening: response and
tions. This may be accomplished with the use of a recently introduced physiological connectivity of the mesolimbic system. NeuroImage 28, 175–184.
method of bivariate-varying spectral analysis to examine dynamics of Nakahara, H., Furuya, S., Obata, S., Masuko, T., Kinoshita, H., 2009. Emotion-related
changes in heart rate and its variability during performance and perception of
the autonomic nerve activity during musical perception tasks music. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1169, 359–362 (Conference Proceedings).
(Bernardi et al., 2009). Nakahara, H., Furuya, S., Francis, P.R., Kinoshita, H., 2010. Psycho-physiological
The observation that musical performance was far more effective responses to expressive piano performance. Int. J. Psychophygiol. 75, 268–276.
Parr, S., 1988. The effects of graduated exercise at the piano on the pianist's cardiac
in modulating emotion-related neurophysiological responses than output, forearm, blood flow, heart rate, and blood pressure. Med. Probl. Perform.
musical perception should have implications for music therapy. Art. 3, 100–104.
Emotion modulation is common in the psychotherapeutic use of Parsons, L.M., Sergent, J., Hodges, D.A., Fox, P.T., 2005. The brain basis of piano performance.
Neuropsychologia 43, 199–215.
music to directly evoke emotions, to recall emotional memories, and
Ravaja, N., Kallinen, K., 2004. Emotional effects of startling background music during
to learn more flexible emotional responses. Our data indicated that reading news reports: the moderating influence of dispositional BIS and BAS
greater effects of emotion modulation would be expected as a result of sensitivities. Scand. J. Psychol. 45, 231–238.
playing an instrument, rather than listening to music. Although only Sammler, D., Grigutsch, M., Fritz, T., Koelsch, S., 2007. Music and emotion:
electrophysiological correlates of the processing of pleasant and unpleasant
trained musicians were examined in the present study, we believe music. Psychophysiology 44, 293–304.
that the potential for musical performance to evoke stronger Spyer, K.M., 1999. Central nervous control of the cardiovascular system. In: Mathias, C.J.,
emotional responses than listening to music can be extended to the Bannister, R. (Eds.), Autonomic Failure: A Textbook of Clinical Disorders of Autonomic
Nervous System, Chapter 6. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK, pp. 45–55.
general population. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of
Some limitations of the present study must be mentioned. First, we Pacing and Electrophysiology, 1996. Heart rate variability: standards of measure-
instructed the participants to control their breathing rate throughout ment, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Circulation 93, 1043–1065.
White, J.M., 1999. Effects of relaxing music on cardiac autonomic balance and anxiety
the experiment across all subjects to minimize the effect of respiration after acute myocardial infarction. Am. J. Crit. Care 8, 220–230.
on HRV. Although we recorded a respiration signal to check the average

You might also like