You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Operations Management 32 (2014) 114–126

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Journal of Operations Management


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jom

The impact of cultural differences on buyer–supplier negotiations:


An experimental study
Dina Ribbink a,∗ , Curtis M. Grimm b,1
a
Ivey Business School, Western University Canada, 1255 Western Road #3321, London, ON N6G0N1, Canada
b
Robert H. Smith School of Business, University of Maryland, 3437 Van Munching Hall, College Park, MD 20742, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: In today’s global economy, an ever-increasing number of companies are dealing with international part-
Received 21 June 2012 ners, instigating a need to understand the impact of cultural differences on business interactions. Using
Received in revised form Hall’s distinction of high- and low-context culture, this study investigates the direct and moderating
12 December 2013
effects of cultural differences in dyadic buyer–supplier negotiations. Theory is developed regarding the
Accepted 20 January 2014
impact of culture on joint profits, juxtaposing Transaction Cost Economics and the Relational View. The
Available online 31 January 2014
theory is tested with a negotiation experiment. Participants, classified by their country of origin, nego-
tiate prices and quality levels for three products. This study finds that cultural differences within the
Keywords:
Buyer–supplier relationship
negotiation dyad reduce joint profits when compared to dyads of participants with similar cultural back-
Negotiation grounds. Cultural differences also moderate the impact of trust and bargaining strategy on joint profits.
Cultural differences Overall, this study concludes that cultural differences, as encountered in day-to-day business interactions
Behavioral experiment in global supply chains, significantly impact negotiation outcomes.
Bargaining strategy © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction et al., 2010; Flynn and Saladin, 2006; Naor et al., 2010; Power et al.,
2010), there is a gap in the literature regarding theory on how
In today’s global economy, it is important for businesses to be cultural differences impact buyer–supplier relationships. Studies
aware of cultural differences, particularly in buyer–seller interac- have compared cultures in countries such as the United States and
tions. As an increasing number of firms are dealing with buyers Germany (Kaufmann and Carter, 2006) and in regions such as Asia
and suppliers abroad (Homburg et al., 2002), many are learning (Naor et al., 2010; Power et al., 2010), but have not developed
that they need to take different cultural traits into account when or tested theory with regard to how cultural differences impact
negotiating across borders. These differences impact behavior and buyer–supplier interactions. Therefore, this study addresses this
understanding of situations (Gelfand and Christakopoulou, 1999). gap in the extant theory by investigating the following research
Increased awareness of potential issues in a negotiation con- questions: Do cultural differences between a buyer and supplier
text allows firms to have more successful interactions in the long reduce the joint profit outcome of supply chain negotiations? Do
run and helps avoid misunderstandings (Brett and Okumura, 1998). cultural differences moderate the impact of trust and bargaining
Furthermore, awareness of the impact of cultural differences can strategy on joint profit outcomes?
enable firms to negotiate more effectively, resulting in greater cost More specifically, this paper contributes to the international
savings and profits. Thus, there is both a business and an academic operations management literature by juxtaposing two underpin-
need for a better understanding of the impact of cultural differ- ning theories, the Relational View and Transaction Cost Economics.
ences arising when buyers and sellers differ in nationality, or when The paper leverages these theories to develop new hypotheses
“cross-cultural dyadic sales interactions” occur (Mintu-Wimsatt regarding the impact of cultural differences in buyer–supplier rela-
and Gassenheimer, 1996, p.20). tionships. The two underpinning theories provide opposing and
Although a growing body of literature has focused on the influ- seemingly contradictory perspectives on buyer–supplier relation-
ence of globalization on supply chains (Cannon et al., 2010; Cheung ships. They can be reconciled by incorporating the presence or
absence of cultural differences. In addition, the study addresses
the recommendation of Pagell et al. (2005, p.388), who emphasize
that studies should extend beyond the question of “whether culture
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 519 661 3651; fax: +1 519 661 3485.
matters” to investigate “how culture matters”, thereby, furthering
E-mail addresses: dribbink@ivey.ca (D. Ribbink), cgrimm@rhsmith.umd.edu
(C.M. Grimm). the discussion regarding the impact of culture on buyer–supplier
1
Tel.: +1 301 405 2235. relationships.

0272-6963/$ – see front matter © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jom.2014.01.004
D. Ribbink, C.M. Grimm / Journal of Operations Management 32 (2014) 114–126 115

The new theory, in the form of hypotheses regarding the direct More specifically, the Relational View is an extension of the
and moderating effects of cultural differences on buyer–supplier Resource-Based View and argues that a firm can best develop com-
negotiation outcomes, is tested through the use of a behavioral petitive resources through relationships with other firms, such as
experiment. Building on a design by Pruitt and Lewis (1975), buyers and suppliers. RV therefore emphasizes the close, collab-
a negotiation scenario is developed that mimics a common orative relationships between buyers and suppliers as the key to
buyer–supplier environment with information asymmetry and the sustainable competitive advantage. Transaction Costs Economics,
resulting potential for one party to behave opportunistically. The on the other hand, views buyer and supplier relationships as adver-
use of dyads provides the opportunity to assess joint profits. This is sarial and emphasizes the costs and problems of such market
important because joint profits, defined as the sum of the individ- interactions. Transaction costs are the costs associated with sourc-
ual profits realized by the two parties involved in the negotiation, ing in the market as opposed to producing the good or service in
are a measure of overall supply chain performance. The pairings of house (Waldman and Jensen, 2007). Originating with the work of
buyers and suppliers are formed based on the national background Coase (1937), when a firm purchases an input for production in the
of the participants resulting in same- (from similar cultural back- marketplace, there are costs beyond those attached to the price of
grounds) and mixed-cultural (from different cultural backgrounds) the input itself. These transaction costs include the search for the
context dyads. appropriate supplier, the contract negotiation, quality control and
In sum, this study contributes to the field of operations man- the actual delivery of the product (Waldman and Jensen, 2007). This
agement in three ways. First, juxtaposing two prominent theories study focuses specifically on the buyer–supplier negotiation aspect
regarding buyer–supplier relationships, the Relational View and of market transactions, which involves a level of interdependence
Transaction Cost Economics, yields important new insights. Each between the parties (Brett, 2007).
of these theories has been used in isolation with regard to a wide Buyer–supplier negotiations are fraught with the TCE elements
range of supply chain questions. Positioning them side-by-side is a of information asymmetries, bounded rationality and the poten-
valuable exercise. Second, a foundation-based model to address the tial for opportunism. Firms never have complete information, nor
impact of culture in an international buyer–supplier relationship do they have the skills and time to attain that information. So
is developed and grounded in the Relational View and Transac- there is often asymmetric information between the parties to a
tion Cost Economics theory. The theoretic model provides testable negotiation. Managers also have cognitive limits in terms of ana-
hypotheses regarding the impact of cultural differences on sup- lyzing and processing information, which is referred to as bounded
ply chain joint profits. Third, the study tests theory on the impact rationality (Williamson, 1979, 1981). Asymmetric information and
of cultural differences in buyer–supplier dyads, a topic that has bounded rationality provide the potential for opportunism, which
not yet received attention in the international operations manage- Williamson has described as “self-interest seeking with guile”
ment literature. As suggested by Bendoly et al. (2006), the current (1979, p.234). Managers of companies may act deceptively as
study goes beyond traditional methodologies by using an exper- they negotiate contracts, for example, by withholding or distorting
imental simulation to capture behavioral aspects in the global information. They may also renege on the negotiated agreement.
buyer–supplier relationship as well as to draw inferences about This grows even more complex as supply chains now span the
causality in this context. This provides an important alternative globe, requiring businesses to conduct negotiations with both
perspective to the literature and contributes to the growing body domestic and foreign partners.
of research using experimental data as methodology (Siemsen, RV and TCE generate opposite predictions regarding the
2011). degree to which buyer–supplier relationships are collaborative or
In the following section, a brief review is provided of Trans- contentious. With regard to international buyer–supplier relation-
action Cost Economics and the Relational View in the context of ships, incorporating the notion of cultural differences can reconcile
negotiations. Based on this foundation, the model is formulated and these differences. In the absence of cultural differences, firms typ-
hypotheses relating to cultural differences are derived. In Section ically adopt the Relational View and focus on the positive aspects
3, the research methodology is explained, followed by results in of a collaborative relationship. However, in the presence of cultural
Section 4. Section 5 discusses the study’s findings, including man- differences, the involved parties are more likely to adopt an adver-
agerial implications and limitations, followed by a conclusion in the sarial relationship, as predicted by TCE. Therefore, in the context
final section. of buyer–supplier relationships, where cultural differences may or
may not be present, both theories are needed to provide a compre-
hensive grounding.
2. Theory and hypotheses
2.2. Culture, cultural differences and joint profits in negotiations
2.1. The Relational View and Transaction Cost Economics in
buyer-supplier negotiations Culture provides characteristics of identification for members
of a (social) group (Brett, 2007; Rivers and Lytle, 2007). Culture
The current study juxtaposes two overarching theories, Dyer manifests itself in shared values, beliefs, and traits, providing a set
and Singh’s (1998) Relational View (RV) and Williamson’s (1975) of rules and guidelines assisting its members to interpret people,
Transaction Cost Economics (TCE), as a foundation for development their behavior and situations (Brett and Okumura, 1998; Hofstede,
of hypotheses regarding cultural differences in buyer–supplier 1985). Cultural characteristics provide grounds for the interpreta-
relationships. RV and TCE have each been used individually as tion of actions within a context (Rivers and Lytle, 2007). Different
an explanatory theory to ground hypotheses in the context of countries and nationalities typically are associated with distinct
buyer–supplier relationships. However, each theory on its own cultures, so that cultural differences may readily arise in today’s
only provides an incomplete justification for the expected behav- global economy.
ior in international buyer–supplier negotiations. The two theories Hall’s (1976) dichotomous operationalization captures the
together provide a more powerful grounding for hypotheses essential elements of culture associated with countries or nation-
regarding buyer–supplier relationships. This is because RV and TCE alities. Hall distinguished between low- and high-context cultures.
each offer a different perspective and in combination are critical to Low-context cultures focus on direct/explicit (often verbal) com-
positioning and understanding the impact of cultural differences in munication; i.e., messages are communicated directly. In a
negotiations. negotiation context, participants often probe by asking questions to
116 D. Ribbink, C.M. Grimm / Journal of Operations Management 32 (2014) 114–126

determine the other’s goals and expectations and to understand the only perception based. Overall, their findings stress that similari-
social context of the involved parties (Brett, 2007). High-context ties help in forming more collaborative relationships which in turn
cultures, on the other hand, rely on context and communication lead to positive externalities as proposed by the Relational View.
to provide the full meaning of the message. Context variables can Correspondingly, cultural similarities are likely to result in
include, for example, “individual backgrounds, associations, values more collaborative relationships than cultural differences. In the
and position in society” (Mintu-Wimsatt and Gassenheimer, 2000, context of this study, cultural differences arise when negotiators
p.1), and “physical environment, status/power relationships, roles come from countries that differ along Hall’s distinction of low
of participants, and even the nonverbal aspects of communication” versus high context cultures. Participants from low-context cul-
(Graham et al., 1994, p.77). Although the classification is partially ture countries such as Germany and the United States (where
based on the interpretation of language, Hall’s categorization goes the cultural background fosters an emphasis on personal ben-
beyond the means of communication. High-context cultures place efits) focus primarily on their own gains (Brett and Okumura,
more emphasis on tradition and history. Change is slow and less 1998). In contrast, participants from high-context culture coun-
readily accepted than in low-context cultures. Furthermore, low- tries such as Japan and Brazil tend to stress a congruency of goals
context cultures are usually more exposed to diversity, both in and close relationships with the partner while taking their part-
terms of population groups as well as mindsets. ner’s interests into account (Gelfand and Christakopoulou, 1999,
For the present study, Hall’s operationalization of national p.263). In addition, participants from similar cultural backgrounds
culture is preferable to the Hofstede and GLOBE alternatives. are more aware of their partner’s expectations in a negotiation
Hofstede’s (1980) cultural dimensions – individualism, mas- context.
culinity, power distance, uncertainty avoidance and long-term In the presence of cultural differences, buyers and suppli-
orientation – are well represented in the academic literature (Tsui ers in a negotiation are likely to interact like actors in the TCE
et al., 2007). The GLOBE’s nine dimensions (House et al., 2004) model. Cultural differences in the buyer–supplier dyad can hence
focus on the impact of national culture on global leadership abil- play a critical role in the outcome of negotiations. Participants
ities and organizational practices (Javidan et al., 2006). However, from different cultural backgrounds pose challenges to negotia-
especially in the context of experimental negotiations, Hall’s clas- tors in an international context (Adair and Brett, 2005). Non-verbal
sification is preferable (see Brett, 2007 for an overview), as it allows communication, expectations, and context interpretations influ-
the grouping of participants into two distinct groups, which can be ence intercultural relationships and, hence, negotiations (Triandis,
contrasted. 2000). Gelfand and Christakopoulou (1999) find that different
In a supply chain setting, buyers and sellers need to engage in cultural backgrounds result in different judgment errors in negotia-
interactions in order to agree on the exchange of products, services tions; students from a collectivistic culture are better able to assess
and information. Negotiations refer to the interaction between two their partner’s priorities during a two-week online negotiation. In
or more parties with the ultimate goal of achieving a mutually the same study, US negotiators claim more value for themselves,
beneficial outcome through cooperation and resolving the often- at least initially – reflecting a greater understanding of self versus
contradictory elements of individual goals (Evans and Beltramini, the negotiating dyad (Brett and Okumura, 1998). Therefore, differ-
1987). Joint profits2 are the most common measure of negotiation ences in cultural backgrounds are expected to increase the potential
outcome, particularly in experimental research (Adler et al., 1992; for pitfalls (Brett and Okumura, 1998). Participants are required
Dwyer and Walker, 1981; Gelfand and Dyer, 2000). A dyadic rela- to find mutually beneficial outcomes during the negotiation while
tionship allows researchers to measure the monetary outcome of avoiding misunderstandings. Cross-national buyer–supplier dyads
an integrative deal. Joint profits, in this context, provide an objec- place less emphasis on close collaboration as emphasized in the
tive performance measure and are an equivalent to supply chain Relational View.
profits within a dyad. Opportunistic behavior is likely to be a more important issue
The primary theoretical contribution is to leverage these two when cultural differences are present. Transaction Cost Economics
theories, TCE and RV, to develop a model with new hypotheses states that opportunistic behavior occurs when participants in a
regarding cultural differences and buyer–supplier relationships. relationship maximize their own utility at the expense of someone
The first hypothesis discusses the impact of cultural differences else’s, often through deceptive means. However, it is only likely
on joint profits. Next, the direct impact of negotiators’ bargaining to occur in situations where opportunism is not easily detected
strategy on profits and the moderating effect of cultural differences (Waldman and Jensen, 2007). Situations of information asymme-
on this relationship are derived. Finally, trust and its relation- try and high uncertainty increase the likelihood of opportunism
ship with joint profits is discussed before providing an argument (Williamson, 1993). In intercultural negotiations, participants are
for the moderating effect of cultural differences on this relation- faced not only with the situation of negotiating a deal but also
ship. with traits, beliefs and behaviors that differ from their own (Brett,
2007). Hence, when dealing with partners from different cultural
2.3. The impact of cultural differences on joint profits backgrounds, negotiations are often more complex and uncertain
(Mintu-Wimsatt and Gassenheimer, 2000). Studies have posited
When the buyer and supplier share similar cultural back- that negotiating parties in intercultural negotiations are more likely
grounds, supply chain performance is enhanced with an outcome to not fully understand how a partner will behave in certain situa-
as predicted by the RV. Mathews et al. (1972) manipulated tions since norms differ across countries (Bagozzi et al., 2003; Brett,
participants to perceive their negotiation partner as similar or dis- 2007). Brett and Okumura (1998) find that differences in norm
similar to themselves and found that cooperation was significantly development and conflict frames have affected the degree of coop-
greater in the similar dyads. The similarities allowed for a better eration between participants, resulting in lower joint profits. This
understanding and assessment of the other negotiator and their creates an opportunity for opportunistic behavior, as one partici-
respective goals even though the similarities and differences were pant can use uncertainty to disguise and manipulate a situation to
her advantage. Expanding their line of argument to the frame of TCE
and the looming threat of greater opportunistic behavior in cultur-
2
Both the term ‘joint profits’ (i.e. Graham et al., 1994; Pruitt and Lewis, 1975) and
ally diverse dyads, this study proposes that negotiations between
‘joint gains’ (i.e. Adair and Brett, 2005; Adair, 2003) have been used in the literature partners from similar cultural backgrounds result in better joint
to describe the same concept. financial performance.
D. Ribbink, C.M. Grimm / Journal of Operations Management 32 (2014) 114–126 117

Hypothesis 1. Supply chain partners realize lower joint profits if cooperative to aggressive, differ between negotiators (Forgas,
in a bargaining dyad with differences in cultural contexts. 1998) and negotiating dyads (Gelfand et al., 2006). Bargaining
strategies oriented toward “co-operation, integration, and informa-
2.4. The impact of bargaining strategies on joint profits tion exchange” (Graham et al., 1994, p.72) can facilitate negotiation
of mutually beneficial deals. They are characterized by probing
Dyer and Singh’s (1998) Relational View posits that a firm’s com- questions to ascertain the partner’s viewpoint and perspective. A
petitive advantage is derived from collaborative interfirm linkages. positive relationship between cooperative bargaining strategies
Using Williamson’s (1985) Transaction Cost framework of mar- and joint profits has been established in the field of negotiation
ket, hierarchy or alliance, the authors propose that in relationships and strategic management (i.e., Adler et al., 1992; Graham et al.,
with some idiosyncratic investments by the parties (alliances), the 1994; Olekalns and Smith, 2003; Rubin and Brown, 1975).
relationship between two or more firms can result in a compet-
Hypothesis 2. The more cooperative the bargaining strategy, the
itive advantage as companies leverage their individual strengths
higher the resulting joint profits.
to complement each other. Relationships and interfirm linkages
of partners, depending upon each other, can create business rela- Dyer and Singh (1998) observe that a US car manufacturer
tionships that leverage these strengths. However, the ever-present established a different type of relationship with its suppliers
threat of opportunism necessitates the need to develop trust and than a Japanese manufacturer, the former being less cooperative
arrive at an agreement through bargaining and negotiation. with limited information sharing between partners. In the RV, a
During the negotiation, the often personal interactions of “country-specific institutional environment that fosters goodwill
involved parties allow both buyer and supplier to develop a trust and cooperation” (Dyer and Singh, 1998, p.673) is cited as a
relationship and gauge the other party’s commitment and trans- reason for greater success in generating positive outcomes. Hence,
parency. Lee and Cavusgil (2006) find that relational-based the understanding and acceptance of norms and conditions, by
governance mechanisms are more effective when compared to partners from the same cultural background, can translate into
formal contracts, supporting Dyer and Singh’s (1998) Relational greater profits (Adair et al., 2001; Heide and John, 1992).
View. Further, RV argues that the focus on relationships as a form Aligned with the underlying arguments of TCE, cultural dif-
of governance engages partners to a greater extent in value cre- ferences may undermine the relationship between bargaining
ation. In order to have a successful relationship and, hence, create strategies and joint profits. Since a country-specific environment
value, cooperation is key, which is operationalized in relationship provides a framework for negotiating partners, negotiation across
commitment and trust (Morgan and Hunt, 1994). Buyer–supplier national borders produces less clear boundaries. As such, greater
engagement has a positive impact on relationship performance levels of uncertainty are introduced and the threat of oppor-
(Kannan and Tan, 2006). In fact, Hoyt and Huq (2000) propose that tunism increases (Williamson, 1979). The negotiation environment
low levels of trust and cooperation within supply chain partner- is therefore more complex and the buyer and supplier act cau-
ships will result in a discontinuity of the relationships as they are tiously, focusing on avoiding opportunistic behaviors, which likely
less profitable. would increase the transaction cost. Cross-national negotiators are
The RV is also important in contextualizing the discussion also often less accommodating.
about negotiations. While negotiation studies generally empha- Several studies compare negotiations with participants from
size the rational agent by focusing on an individual’s autonomy – one country to those of another to investigate the effect of
often operationalized in experiments through the use of previously national culture on negotiations (Brett, 2000, 2007; Gelfand and
unknown partners – Barley (1991) questions the representative- Christakopoulou, 1999; Graham et al., 1994). They generally
ness of a business interaction. Consequently, Gelfand et al. (2006, find significant differences between participants from more low-
p.443) provide a detailed discussion on the impacts of “interde- context regions like the United States and Northern Europe than
pendence, cooperation and relationality” on negotiations. Their high-context ones like Asia and South America. These findings
compelling argumentation demonstrates the need to contextualize highlight that participants’ national backgrounds influence their
the negotiation by investigating the cooperation within the supply approaches and expectations. In international negotiations, these
chain as a critical element. Business negotiations and bargaining differences are only accentuated. As Forgas (1998) points out, coop-
are the essence of the buyer–supplier relationship. While differ- eration is more likely to occur when participants have similar
ent strategies are often used to describe bargaining patterns (Pruitt backgrounds, since differences are perceived as threats. Misun-
and Carnevale, 1993), a bargaining strategy is ultimately aimed at derstandings, based on cultural differences, can hinder mutually
helping each party pursue its goals. These goals may (or may not) beneficial outcomes.
be shared between the parties involved (Ness and Haugland, 2005). For example, a negotiator from a high-context culture might
The counterview is based in an adversarial relationship due offer a non-verbal signal to indicate a willingness to be cooperative
to the TCE elements of information asymmetry and opportunism and anticipate a reciprocal cooperative signal. If the negotiating
present in negotiations. During the negotiation, the level of trust, partner were from a low-context culture, that non-verbal signal
the level of information asymmetry in the relationship and the could well be missed and therefore elicit no response. The origina-
threat of opportunistic behavior all impact the choice of bargaining tor of the signal could interpret the lack of reciprocity as a rejection
strategy. According to Transaction Cost Theory, often in negotia- of the cooperative overture, and the negotiation could turn more
tions, participants are more concerned about their own outcomes combative. On the other hand, in the absence of cultural differences,
than the mutual results. As a consequence, the negotiation and the same signal, properly interpreted, could be the catalyst for a
interaction between the involved parties can be hostile and insin- highly cooperative relationship. Hence, this research proposes that
cere. In this scenario, participants are on edge and more likely to cultural differences moderate the relationship between bargaining
renege on contracts. Investments are less likely to be made and if strategy and joint profits.
so, to a lesser extent. Overall, an adversarial relationship between
buyers and suppliers negatively impacts the financial outcome Hypothesis 3. Differences in cultural contexts within a bargain-
(Williamson, 2008), resulting in lower joint profits. ing dyad negatively moderate the relationship between bargaining
Bargaining strategies aimed at generating a collaborative strategies and joint profits; in other words, the positive effect of
environment can enhance financial outcomes, consistent with more cooperative bargaining strategies on joint profits is lessened
the tenets of the RV. These strategies, which can range from when cultural differences are present.
118 D. Ribbink, C.M. Grimm / Journal of Operations Management 32 (2014) 114–126

2.5. The impact of trust on joint profits

The Relational View also informs the direct effect of trust on


joint profits. Trust, defined as the willingness to have confidence in
a partner (Moorman et al., 1992), is an important element of busi-
ness dealings in general and in negotiations specifically (Schurr and
Ozanne, 1985). According to RV, trust is essential in relationships
within networks of firms. Dyer and Singh (1998) propose that trust
can improve relational rents. Trust is essential for a constructive
dialog between involved parties (Pruitt, 1981) and is particularly
important in situations of uncertainty (Kaufmann and Carter, 2006).
Similarly, Transaction Cost Economics argues that trust coun-
ters the negative effects of information asymmetry and reduces
the threat of opportunism within a buyer–supplier relationship.
Although mutually cooperative behavior in buyer–supplier rela-
tionships is often desirable, the threat of the negative externalities
of relying on the partner can often only be mitigated through the
element of trust (Parkhe, 1993). Trust, often combined with for-
mal or informal contacts, provides the basis that fosters partners to
invest in relationship specific assets. As trust builds, it is assumed
that one’s partner is acting in good faith (in a manner that does not
hurt the trusting party), will fulfill promises and obligations, and
will generally behave as agreed upon (Cannon and Perreault, 1999;
Fig. 1. Dimensions and outcome of intercultural negotiations.
Corsten and Felde, 2005). Negotiators rely on trust to reach coop-
erative goals faster (Ueltschy et al., 2007). In addition, higher levels
of trust are found to result in longer relationships (Kaufmann and high-context cultures and low-context cultures. Negotiators from
Carter, 2006) and are a prerequisite for negotiators to move from high-context cultures place greater emphasis on trust, while their
purely distributive to mutually beneficial integrative deals (Pruitt low-context counterparts prefer a more direct form of control to
and Lewis, 1975). Thus, trust positively influences financial out- counter the threat of opportunism. Chen et al. (2002) differentiate
comes, such as joint profits, resulting from negotiations (Langerak, between in-group (within a culture) and out-group (with another
2001). culture) interactions and propose that collectivistic individuals are
more likely to behave opportunistically toward out-groups; this is
Hypothesis 4. The greater the level of trust within the bargaining partially supported by Takahashi et al. (2008). Curall and Inkpen
dyad, the higher the level of joint profits. (2002) stress a need for a better comprehension of culture in
Within the context of TCE, trust can mitigate the concern the context of trust. A good understanding of the cultural con-
for opportunistic behavior (Morgan and Hunt, 1994). Williamson text allows negotiators to better assess the risks (Brouthers and
(1979, p.241) emphasizes that personal trust “survive[s] greater Brouthers, 2000), including the threat of opportunism. In the con-
stress and display[s] greater adaptability.” According to Arrow text of opportunism, trust is argued to decrease (Morgan and Hunt,
(1973, p.24), “there is an element of trust in every transaction.” 1994). Therefore, this research posits that cultural differences mod-
Especially in the context of marketplace interactions, trust needs erate the effect the perceived level of trust has on joint profits.
to be established quickly for efficient buyer–seller interactions. Hypothesis 5. Differences in cultural contexts within a bargaining
According to the RV, trust often serves as a governing mechanism dyad negatively moderate the relationship between trust and joint
in relationships, reducing transaction costs (Dyer and Singh, 1998). profits. In other words, the positive impact of trust on joint profits
Chiles and McMackin (1996) directly integrate trust into the TCE is lessened when cultural differences are present.
framework, arguing that as trust increases, the “vulnerability to the
risk of opportunistic behavior” (p.85) decreases. Similarly, Doney The complete model is presented in Fig. 1.
and Cannon (1997) posit that trust is only possible in the con-
text of uncertainty and will decrease in the expected presence of 3. Research design and methodology
opportunism. Concern about opportunistic behavior from the dyad
partner can negatively affect the efficacy of the negotiation. At the Experiments allow researchers to study the human behav-
beginning of a relationship, it is difficult to foresee whether the ior aspect (Bendoly et al., 2006) in a dynamic business setting.
partners are likely to behave opportunistically or not (Tangpong Croson and Donohue (2002) argue that experiments allow for the
and Ro, 2009). Trust has been found to allay concerns regarding investigation of causal relationships and to test economic and
opportunism and lessens costs related to controls against oppor- operations management theories in a controlled environment. In
tunistic behaviors (Ghoshal and Moran, 1996), resulting in higher addition, they offer an opportunity to study the interdependence
levels of performance in buyer–supplier relationships (Corsten and of interpersonal interaction (Bendoly et al., 2010), providing a
Felde, 2005). direct observation of dyadic behavior in buyer–supplier relation-
It is well-established in the literature that culture and cultural ships. Finally, behavioral experiments are ideal to test theory as
differences have an impact on the development of trust. Chiles they allow researchers to establish causality (Thomas, 2011; Tokar,
and McMackin (1996) propose that a similar cultural background 2010).
translates into shared views and expectations and optimizes trust. The context of buyer–supplier relationships lends itself
Morgan and Hunt (1994) emphasize that shared values promote the well to the use of experiments (Eckerd and Bendoly, 2011;
development of trust. Therefore, in an international buyer–supplier Rungtusanatham et al., 2011; Stevens, 2011). In the psychology lit-
negotiation, it can be harder to establish trust. Interestingly, erature, Pruitt and Lewis (1975) use the sale of three commodities
Ueltschy et al. (2007) find differences in the perception of to study different negotiation approaches. In operations manage-
the importance of trust by supply chain partners representing ment literature, experimental buyer–supplier relationship studies
D. Ribbink, C.M. Grimm / Journal of Operations Management 32 (2014) 114–126 119

are limited. Carter and Stevens (2007) use an experimental design Table 1
Description of the sample.
to study supplier behavior in electronic reverse auctions, employ-
ing an automated buyer response instead of dyadic units of analysis Culture Total
[between buyer and supplier(s)] as in the present study.
Same Mixed
The current study used an experimental approach to investigate
Buyer Male 8 10.3% 12 15.4% 20 25.6%
the impact of cultural differences on dyadic buyer–supplier rela-
Female 8 10.3% 11 14.1% 19 24.4%
tionships, contributing to the existing literature on supply chain
management and international operations management with its Seller Male 12 15.4% 15 19.2% 27 34.6%
Female 4 5.1% 8 10.3% 12 15.4%
previous focus on other methodologies. Loch and Wu (2008) look at
buyer–supplier relationships using an experimental study. Dyadic Total (individual) 32 41.0% 46 59.0% 78 100.00
Total (dyad) 16 25 39
research provides unique insights that go beyond the individual
level, as teams form their own dynamics (Klein and Kozlowski,
2000). These studies find that individual perspectives on culture, the split between male and female participants for both buyer and
trust and bargaining strategy do not necessarily allow inferences seller by context-culture groups. It also shows that the current
about group- (dyadic) level behavior. Furthermore, the use of an study has two treatment groups: same context culture dyads and
experiment allows us to study the impact of cultural differences in mixed context culture dyads with 16 and 23 pairings, respectively.
a controlled environment while still mimicking a business-like set- Hair et al. (1998) recommend an ideal sample size of 20 obser-
ting. The use of an experiment is, hence, aligned with Handfield’s vations per group, but attest that as few as five observations are
(in Meredith, 2002) request that the methodology derives from the sufficient although the results may be more unstable with fewer
research question and context. Finally, Wacker (1998) emphasizes observations. Studies using similar methodology have used sam-
the need to substantiate operations management theory using var- ples sizes of 13 (Thomas et al., 2013), 19 (Kalkanci et al., 2011), and
ious methodological techniques, to which the use of experimental 15 (Benzion et al., 2008) participants per treatment cell respec-
designs is responsive. tively. Hence, the current sample size is well within the acceptable
range for behavioral experimental studies.
3.1. Sample
3.2. Experimental protocol
The sample consisted of 78 MBA students from a large US
business school. The majority of laboratory experiment studies in The data were collected using a negotiation simulation of
operations management have used undergraduate student partic- buyer–supplier dyads. This research adopted Pruitt and Lewis’
ipants (i.e., Bolton et al., 2004; Kremer et al., 2010; Loch and Wu, (1975) design with Carter and Stevens’ (2007) framing and wording
2008). The literature provides strong support for the use of students of the experiment when appropriate. Analogous to Pruitt and Lewis’
as subjects. Heisler and Gemmill (1978) compare how executives (1975), one buyer and one supplier were asked to negotiate the
and students perceive various managerial promotion issues and prices of three different products (alpha, beta, and gamma). Buyer
find that they behave similarly. Montmarquette et al. (2004, p.1388) and supplier received different price tables and the payoffs were
find that “the similarity of students’ and managers’ average net asymmetrically distributed, allowing participants to gain higher
payoffs is striking.” This suggests that, in terms of outcome, the dif- profits if they arrived at an integrative deal.4
ference between the two groups is negligible. Bendoly et al. (2006) As shown in Table 2, prices were listed as letters ranging from A
stress that well-designed experiments “test whether representa- to N and were for the specified number of units. As an addition to the
tive humans react in a predictable manner to controlled stimuli” original simulation, two levels of product quality were introduced
(p.739), hence, allowing the findings from student subjects to be (high and low). Buyer and supplier negotiated product quality along
generalizable to a broader field. with the price. In the post-negotiation survey, the supplier was
Furthermore, Stevens (2011) argues that the use of students is asked about the quality of products she actually planed to deliver.
dependent on the context of the research study. The main treatment Profits to both involved parties were determined by the delivered
variable in the current study is cultural differences in negotiating quality as opposed to the negotiated quality.
dyads and their direct and indirect impact on negotiating behavior. Two pre-tests were conducted. Initially, 22 students from an
Given that national culture and the characteristics associated with undergraduate class were asked to participate in the experiment.
them are present in both student and managers, the results are During this testing phase, the instructions were rewritten and clar-
generalizable from a student to a broader audience. ified based on comments from students. Next, a trial was run in
A key characteristic of the sample was diversity with respect an undergraduate class: 23 undergraduates and one PhD student
to nationality or country of citizenship at birth: 60.3% were born participated. This session resulted in the adjustment of the time for
in the United States with 61.5% in the buyer and 59.0% in the seller
group. For those born abroad, the majority of students (20 out of 29)
were from Asia [seven were from East Asia (China, Taiwan, Japan,
the academic performance issue the overall GPA average was gathered for all sec-
Thailand) and 13 were from South Asia (India, Pakistan)]. Three
ond year MBA students at the university from which the sample was drawn. On
were born in South America. Individual students also represented average, the GPA of international students was 0.02 points lower than their domes-
the Arabic Peninsula, Africa, Canada, the Caribbean and Europe. tic counterparts (3.49/4 versus 3.51/4); The difference between the two groups,
59.0% of all dyads were between partners of different cultural back- international and domestic students, was not statistically significant. To assess the
grounds, meaning that one partner was from a high-context culture socio-economic issue, data was gathered on program cost for the MBA, $80,366 for
resident students and $94,896 for non-resident. Thus, for both domestic and inter-
like Asia or South America while the other was from a low-context national students, the direct cost of the program is quite substantial. It follows that
culture like the United States or Northern Europe. 47 (60.3%) par- both domestic and international students would generally come from a relatively
ticipants were male and 31 (39.7%) were female.3 Table 1 displays high socio-economic status.
4
‘Distributive’ and ‘integrative’ deals are discussed in detail in Brett (2007). Dis-
tributive deals imply that there is a fixed pie of profits that are distributed across
negotiating partners. Any win by one partner results in a respective loss of the other.
3
It could be argued that students that would have left their country to become In integrative deals, through negotiation, participants can result in increases in the
educated in the U.S. might be stronger in academic performance than their domestic overall pie, resulting in win-win solutions. This concept is also referred to in the
U.S. counterparts and may also be from a higher socio-economic status. To assess literature as ‘value claiming’ versus ‘value creating’ (Olekalns and Smith, 2003).
120 D. Ribbink, C.M. Grimm / Journal of Operations Management 32 (2014) 114–126

Table 2
Price-profits for buyer and seller.

Alpha Price per 1000 units Beta Price per 1000 units Gamma Price per 1000 units

Price Profits Price Profits Price Profits

High quality Low quality High quality Low quality High quality Low quality

Seller
A $2000 $3250 A $1200 $1950 A $800 $1300
B $1750 $3000 B $1050 $1800 B $700 $1200
C $1500 $2750 C $900 $1650 C $600 $1100
D $1250 $2500 D $750 $1500 D $500 $1000
E $1000 $2250 E $600 $1350 E $400 $900
F $750 $2000 F $450 $1200 F $300 $800
G $500 $1750 G $300 $1050 G $200 $700
H $250 $1500 H $150 $900 H $100 $600
I $0 $1250 I $0 $750 I $0 $500
J −$250 $1000 J −$150 $600 J −$100 $400
K −$500 $750 K −$300 $450 K −$200 $300
L −$750 $500 L −$450 $300 L −$300 $200
M −$1000 $250 M −$600 $150 M −$400 $100
N −$1250 $0 N −$750 $0 N −$500 $0

Buyer
A $0 −$500 A $0 −$750 A $0 −$1250
B $100 −$400 B $150 −$600 B $250 −$1000
C $200 −$300 C $300 −$450 C $500 −$750
D $300 −$200 D $450 −$300 D $750 −$500
E $400 −$100 E $600 −$150 E $1000 −$250
F $500 $0 F $750 $0 F $1250 $0
G $600 $100 G $900 $150 G $1500 $250
H $700 $200 H $1050 $300 H $1750 $500
I $800 $300 I $1200 $450 I $2000 $750
J $900 $400 J $1350 $600 J $2250 $1000
K $1000 $500 K $1500 $750 K $2500 $1250
L $1100 $600 L $1650 $900 L $2750 $1500
M $1200 $700 M $1800 $1050 M $3000 $1750
N $1300 $800 N $1950 $1200 N $3250 $2000

the complete experiment from 60 min to 40 min since participants the buyer and a supplier category) were offered, analogous to a
required less time than initially expected to reach agreement. rank-order tournament (Lazear and Rosen, 1991).
Participants were unaware of their partners’ payoff scheme
and were explicitly asked not to share the information until they
3.3. Procedure finished the questionnaire and left the experiment. The subjects
spent 10 min studying the material and developing their individual
Subjects were invited to participate in a 40-min experiment bargaining strategies. Next, the dyads conducted the negotiation
that was entirely voluntary. After a short introduction, the partic- process.
ipants were paired up in teams of two (dyad). The assignments After concluding the bargaining component, each participant
were pre-arranged by the researchers using information from the filled out a questionnaire. The first piece of information col-
participant signup list. Dyads were generated based on partici- lected from the seller was the level of quality they intended to
pants’ national backgrounds, creating both mixed-context culture deliver to the buyer. This might or might not have been the
(participants representing both a high- and a low-context cultural quality level agreed upon during the negotiation. In addition, infor-
background) and same-context culture groups (both participants mation was gathered on the level of trust and the perceived
representing low-context cultures).5 Participants were randomly bargaining strategy of the partner during the negotiation. The
assigned to the role of buyer and supplier within each dyad. Role- questionnaire also requested background information about the
appropriate instruction sheets were handed out, which included participants, including their nationality at birth, age, gender, length
a price list and associated profits for each product as depicted in of time they had spent abroad as well as countries they had
Table 2. The instructions also contained information about the com- lived in.
panies represented by the participants and the number of products
they had to acquire.
Next, an administrator read out the instructions, explained the 3.4. Measures
goals of the negotiation and informed participants of the survey.
She also pointed out that they could use part or all of the informa- Next, a discussion of the variables and their operationalization
tion on the instruction sheets to devise their negotiation strategy. in the model follows. First, the dependent variable is discussed,
Participants were encouraged to ask clarifying questions. In order to followed by the hypothesized independent variables and then the
encourage competitiveness, prizes for best performances (in both controls.
Joint Profit. Joint profits, the dependent variable in this exper-
imental simulation, was used as a measure of supply chain
performance. Analogous to Pruitt and Lewis (1975), it was com-
5
The language of instruction and during the negotiation for all participants was puted as the sum of both buyer and supplier profits in a dyad
English. across all three products. With reference to Table 2, an example
D. Ribbink, C.M. Grimm / Journal of Operations Management 32 (2014) 114–126 121

can aid in understanding how joint profits are calculated. For exam- by checking that the “individual item’s coefficient is greater than
ple: the participants agreed on high quality and price B for product twice its standard error” (Chen et al., 2004, p.513). The large z-
“Alpha,” on low quality and price I for “Beta,” and on high quality and values support that individual items form the underlying factors
price G for “Gamma.” Assuming the supplier delivered high-quality and are all significant at the 1% level. The chi-square value of 23.69
products per the agreement, the joint profits totaled $4750 (Alpha: is statistically insignificant indicating that the model fits the data.
$1750 + $100; Beta: $750 + $450; Gamma: $200 + $1500). Additional goodness-of-fit indices also are within acceptable lim-
Cultural differences. Hall’s (1976) distinction between high and its (RMSEA < 0.06; RMSR < 0.08; CFI > 0.90; TLI/NNFI > 0.95), thereby
low-context culture was used to differentiate cultural groups by suggesting good model fit (Sharma, 1996).
nationality.6 The nationality of the negotiators at birth was used to
classify participants into high- or low-context culture groups. Next, 4.1. Hypothesis testing
this classification was used to assign participants into negotiating
pairs. The study at hand tested two different pairings: mixed- To test for potential non-independence of observations (Kenny
context and same (low)-context culture dyads. et al., 2006), a nonparametric chi-square test was used, showing
Trust. The perceived level of trust was measured, in the follow- the observations vary by dyad (as opposed to each observation
up questionnaire, using a five-item scale by Spake and Bishop individually). Regression analysis – with between estimation for
(2009), adapted slightly to better fit the buyer–supplier negoti- cross-sectional data – was run to analyze the experimental data
ation context. In Table 4, individual survey items are reported. (Kenny et al., 2006). The between estimator controlled for unob-
Participants were asked to rate each item on a five-point Likert served consistent effects in heterogeneity within each buyer–seller
scale ranging from ‘completely agree’ (1) to ‘completely disagree’ dyad (Rabe-Hesketh and Skrondal, 2008).
(5). The scales were reverse-coded for the remainder of this study, In the initial hypothesis, this study proposed that intercultural
facilitating interpretation in Section 4. negotiations result in lower profits. Model I in Table 5 shows that, at
Bargaining strategy. Bargaining strategy was measured in the the 5% level, joint profits are significantly influenced by the cultural
follow-up questionnaire using a scale developed by Graham et al. backgrounds of the parties involved in the negotiation, suppor-
(1994). As can be observed from the individual items in Table 4, ting hypothesis 1. Participants in mixed-context dyads generally
this measure looked at the perceived willingness to make con- agree on lower joint profits than participants with similar cultural
cessions during the negotiation. As the negotiation was ideally backgrounds.
a give-and-take, buyers and sellers could target mutually benefi- The results are mixed with regard to the effect of bargaining
cial agreements, or just follow their own agendas with a focus on strategy and trust on joint profits. Neither hypothesis 2 (the effect
self-interest irrespective of their partners’ demands. A lower score of a particular bargaining strategy on joint profits) nor hypothe-
indicated more cooperative behavior, while higher scores were an sis 4 (the effect of trust on joint profits) is supported in Model I.
indication of perceived selfishness. Again, to make interpretations However, in Model II, one observes a statistically significant effect
more intuitive, the scales were reverse-coded for the data analysis. of a more collaborative bargaining strategy on joint profits, but in
Controls. In the post-experiment survey, additional information the opposite of the hypothesized direction. Hypothesis 2 is there-
for each individual participant was collected, such as their age and fore not supported. In Model II, increased levels of trust result in
gender. In addition, data were gathered about their nationality and greater joint profits in same context-culture dyads. This is aligned
country at birth, their parents’ birth countries, time spent abroad with the proposed hypothesis and, hence, provides partial support
and other countries where they had resided. for hypothesis 4.
Turning next to the two moderating hypotheses, Model II shows
4. Results support for a positive moderating effect of cultural differences on
the relationship between bargaining strategy and joint profits (H3).
An overview of correlations between the independent variables However, this effect is counter to the proposed negative moder-
is provided in Table 3. Generally, the correlations are low. Some ating effect of cultural differences as hypothesized in H3. Hence,
correlation is present between the factors of trust and bargaining hypothesis 3 is not supported. Finally, Model II shows that cultural
strategy (0.482), but it is still below the accepted cutoff value of 0.6 differences have a moderating effect on the relationship between
(Hair et al., 1998). trust and joint profits, consistent with H5. Therefore, hypothesis 5
The factors were tested for reliability, validity and unidimen- is supported.
sionality. To assess reliability, the average correlation between The controls of gender and age do not have a significant effect
items in a scale was used. The coefficient alpha values are reported on the results when included in the model. Similarly, an additional
in Table 4. As this study used existing scales, the suggested cutoff model was run including a control variable measuring the percent-
value is 0.70 (Hair et al., 1998); it was exceeded by both factors. age of lifetime spent outside of the United States to counter the
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used on all items to effect of cultural assimilation due to living in the United States
assess fit with the latent variables. Items with low standardized for an extended period of time. There is no statistically significant
loadings (less than 0.35) and cross-loadings were discarded (one effect.
item for bargaining strategy). Confirmatory factor analysis was
chosen as preexisting scales were used. CFA is a stricter mea- 5. Discussion
sure to assess construct validity and unidimensionality (Gerbing
and Anderson, 1988). For the analysis, standardized loadings were This study uses RV and TCE to develop hypotheses and tests
used, implying that the loadings of one item within the construct theory regarding the impact of cultural differences, both direct and
were fixed to a value of 1.0. Item loadings and fit indices for the indirect, on supply chain performance in buyer–supplier relation-
CFA analysis are shown in Table 4. Convergent validity was tested ships. The results provide both significant and interesting findings.

5.1. Theoretical implications


6
As discussed earlier, in an experiment, the use of categorical variables is prefer-
able over a discrete or continuous variable (i.e. Hofstede or GLOBE dimensions)
Juxtaposing the contrasting theories of RV and TCE to develop
in order to create groups. Previous research in international negotiations provides a model, empirical support is found for the hypotheses regarding
ample support (see Brett, 2007). the impact of cultural differences on supply chain relationships.
122 D. Ribbink, C.M. Grimm / Journal of Operations Management 32 (2014) 114–126

Table 3
Correlation table.

I II III IV V

Trust (I)a 1
Coop. bargaining strategy (II)a 0.482 1
Cultural differences (III) 0.112 0.065 1
Gender (IV) −0.009 −0.090 −0.038 1
Age (V) −0.006 0.079 −0.0771 0.129 1
Time abroad as a percentage of lifetime (VI) 0.018 −0.042 −0.234 0.072 −0.024
a
Trust and cooperative bargaining strategy are reverse coded.

Table 4
Assessment of reliability and construct validity (parameter estimates, error terms and z-values).

Factors and items Estimated loadings Standardized loadings Std. error for std. ldg. z-values

Trust (coefficient alpha = 0.91)


Corporation can be trusted completely.a 0.859 1.000 0.032 26.88
Corporation can be counted on to do what is right.a 0.890 1.027 0.026 34.19
Corporation is a company that I have great confidence in.a 0.957 1.071 0.013 76.07
Corporation can be relied upon.a 0.965 1.005 0.012 83.66
I am willing to deal with a corporation again 0.694 0.622 0.061 11.46
Cooperative bargaining strategy (coefficient alpha = 0.71)
Rate a Corporation’s bargaining strategies on the following scales
Accommodating to exploitive 0.617 1.000 0.089 6.89
Honest to deceptive 0.799 1.330 0.078 10.29
Unbiased to biased 0.591 0.958 0.098 6.05
Goodness-of-fit indices
Chi-square 23.69 (p > chi2 = 0.208)
RMSEA 0.056
RMSR 0.042
CFI 0.990
TLI 0.986

All z-values are significant at p < 0.01 level.


a
Refers to either Buyer or Seller depending on participant’s role in the negotiation.

While RV states that a firm’s competitive advantage can be achieved key arguments presented in support of the first hypothesis. The
through leveraging interfirm relationships, TCE warns of the detri- presence of cultural differences facilitates misunderstandings and
mental effects that might arise out of bounded rationality and the errors in judgment during interactions between the participants.
threat of opportunistic behavior. Although both theories have been Cultural differences within the dyad can result in false readings
used individually in the supply chain literature to explain the rela- of the partner’s behavioral signals. Negotiators in these dyads are
tionship between buyers and suppliers, juxtaposing both theories faced with greater complexity, which increases the level of uncer-
contributes to new insights in the context of international business tainty in the relationship and, hence, increases the potential for
negotiations. The results of this research provide support for both missing signs of opportunistic behavior. On the other hand, cultural
theories, depending critically on whether or not cultural differences similarities allow negotiators to better judge the other’s behaviors
are present. and expectations. Consequently, there is less need for elaborate
Using the Transaction Cost Theory lens, it is argued that cul- explanations, resulting in a more direct exchange of information.
tural differences reduce the realized profits for firms. TCE clearly The familiarity in behavior also provides support in assessing the
outlines that greater levels of uncertainty translate into a greater potential to behave opportunistically and allows partners to take
risk of opportunistic behavior. The results provide support for the countermeasures to avoid unfavorable situations and outcomes.

Table 5
Between group estimation regression model of joint profits per dyad as DV (std. error in parenthesis).

Independent variable Joint profits

Model I Model II

Cultural differences (CD) −513.212 (250.7)


*
−553.229* (236.4) H1
Cooperative bargaining Strategy (CBS) −30.059 (474.0) −2394.104* (1044.1) H2
CBS* CD 2864.176* (1153.5) H3
Trust 29.771 (247.0) 1353.538* (605.3) H4
Trust* CD −1541.889* (653.1) H5
Gender −46.982 (306.5) −164.807 (293.1)
Age −29.619 (56.3) −34.080 (53.6)
Time in US as percentage of lifetime 0.515 (6.9) −2.110 (6.5)
Constant 5900.789** (1665.1) 6263.239** (1590.4)

Observations 78 78
R2 (between) 0.134 0.283
R2 (overall) 0.126 0.175
Number of dyads (Grouping variable) 39 39
*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.
D. Ribbink, C.M. Grimm / Journal of Operations Management 32 (2014) 114–126 123

Fig. 2. Interaction plot of cultural differences on trust and joint profits. Fig. 3. Interaction plot of cultural differences on bargaining strategy and joint pro-
fits.

It may also be the case that negotiators have more difficulty


establishing supply chain collaboration, as envisioned by the Rela- strategy leads to lower joint profits as expected. Similarly, in this
tional View, when negotiating with someone representing another context, a partner more insistent on discussing greater details of
cultural background. There are three potential explanations for the deal might be perceived as more self-interested or even hos-
this. First, the partners in buyer–seller relationships may be reluc- tile toward the partner’s needs. However, when there is cultural
tant to provide the required additional information to someone harmony within the dyad, a more combative bargaining strategy
they perceive as different. This, in turn, translates into lower pro- may ultimately be beneficial. When partners approach a negoti-
fits. Second, participants in business interactions might not want ation with a less accommodating bargaining strategy, this may
to threaten their counterpart, in the presence of cultural differ- prompt participants to discuss alternative options in more detail.
ences, by being a tough bargainer. The underlying assumption, in As is typical in an actual negotiation, the relationships between
this second setting, is that the partner representing a different cul- prices, quality and joint profits are complex and more significant
tural background does not understand the full implications of the attention to alternatives can open up the opportunity to discover
negotiation banter as well as someone from the same cultural back- superior joint profits. Participants sharing the same culture can
ground. Or third, someone from the same cultural background is better withstand the tension arising from more combative bargain-
likely to understand the negotiation dance with its expected intri- ing strategies and achieve a better outcome. Thus, in dyads with
cacies better than someone with a different background (Gelfand participants from similar cultural background, a cooperative bar-
and Christakopoulou, 1999). The uncertainties arising from nego- gaining strategy can cause complacency and satisficing (Gelfand
tiating in mixed-context culture dyads can lead to inconsistent et al., 2006) and not yield the full potential of the negotiation.
approaches, translating into lost opportunities and missed profits. This finding may imply that perceived hostile bargaining strate-
In either of these cases, cultural differences between participants, gies are not always detrimental to a relationship, but can provide
originating from different backgrounds, result in lower supply the stimulus for mutually beneficial negotiations in the appropriate
chain performance. setting.
An interesting finding is that trust has a positive and significant The research contributes broadly to supply chain management
impact on joint profits in dyads with similar cultural backgrounds theories of international buyer–supplier interactions. Past stud-
but not in dyads with cultural differences. Fig. 2 illustrates this ies establish that supplier relationship management (Krause et al.,
relationship. Contrary to previous studies, the results in Model 2007) and the level of inter-organizational communications (Carr
I (Table 5) show that the direct effect of trust on joint profits and Pearson, 1999; Claycomb and Franwick, 2004; Paulraj et al.,
is insignificant. However, the results hold for participants from 2008; Prahinski and Benton, 2004) impact a firm’s financial per-
same-cultural context dyads. In Model II (Table 5), the interaction formance, but have not provided theory regarding the role of
effect of cultural differences and trust is statistically significant. cultural differences in international interactions. In this context,
Trust, therefore, positively impacts joint profits in same-cultural Lovejoy (2010), who develops theory in supply chain negotiations,
contexts dyads, but does not in dyads with participants from dif- expressly emphasizes the need for more research in experimental
ferent cultural backgrounds. An explanation for this finding is that bargaining to advance both theory and practice. This study used
when partners have a similar cultural background, trust enables two overarching theories – each well established individually – to
negotiators to move from distributive to integrative deals through jointly leverage their opposing arguments in the context of inter-
increasing levels of information exchange. However, in the pres- national buyer supplier relationships. Juxtaposing both the RV and
ence of cultural differences, this relationship between trust and TCE provides powerful grounding regarding the impact of cultural
joint profits no longer holds. Consistent with TCE theory, greater differences on joint profits.
levels of uncertainty, due to cultural differences, appear to elimi-
nate the positive effect of trust on joint profits. In an international 5.2. Managerial implications
setting, negotiators may need to rely on mechanisms other than
trust to control for the potential of opportunistic behavior. Global supply chains are increasingly prevalent in today’s
It is also interesting to observe that the absence of cultural economy with managers seeking out – often through face-to-face
differences in the dyad affects the relationship between bargain- negotiation – the most cost effective, high quality products sup-
ing strategy and joint profits. Fig. 3 illustrates this relationship. pliers can provide. As a consequence, participants with different
In dyads with cultural differences, an uncooperative bargaining nationalities and oftentimes cultural differences are commonplace
124 D. Ribbink, C.M. Grimm / Journal of Operations Management 32 (2014) 114–126

in buyer–supplier interactions. The United States and China are variables which could impact the outcome. As such, the study is by
important supply chain trading partners and provide a good exam- no means all-encompassing. By using RV and TCE to develop theory,
ple of mixed-cultural context backgrounds of high (China) and low this study focuses on the factors of cultural difference, bargaining
(US) context cultures. Hence, it is important for companies dealing strategy and trust in a single exchange without prior business inter-
in such environments to be aware of the implications of cultural action of buyer and supplier. The findings of this study support the
differences. In an international context, misunderstandings are theoretic argument that cultural differences significantly impact
more likely to occur, and it can be harder to attain mutually the outcome of negotiations and that experiences and inferences
beneficial outcomes. Therefore, training should be required for from a same-cultural context are not easily transferred to an inter-
managers to prepare them for the additional challenges arising national setting. However, additional factors such as the frequency
from intercultural negotiations. Managers need to be very cul- of exchange, relationship history and commitment could impact
turally adapt in order to negotiate across national cultures. In the theory and empirical results. Therefore, future research could
conclusion, managers and negotiators need to be aware of the examine the confluence of cultural differences in combination with
implications of cultural differences and may need to adjust their such factors and develop further theory in this area.
negotiation tactics when moving into an international context. An
appropriate approach will improve the overall financial perfor- 6. Conclusion
mance of the involved parties and, hence, should be in alignment
with the objectives of the company. This study provided both a theoretical and empirical con-
tribution to the timely topic of how cultural differences affect
5.3. Limitations and future research buyer–supplier negotiations. Using Transaction Costs Economics
and the Relational View as lenses, theory was developed regarding
This study makes an important contribution, but is not without the direct and moderating effects of cultural differences on joint
limitations. Although a behavioral experiment is the most appro- profits. The model was tested with experimental data.
priate methodology for this study, there are drawbacks to using This study found that cultural differences directly influenced
a laboratory environment to test theory. One issue is the use of supply chain joint profit levels. In addition, cultural differences
MBA students rather than professionals in the field. As documented served as a moderator reducing the strength of the relationship
in the method section, there is strong justification for using stu- between trust and joint profits. Finally, the results also showed that
dents as subjects although they are generally less experienced than cultural differences moderate the effect of cooperative bargaining
professionals at international negotiations and the context of pur- strategies on joint profits. Future research should extend and test
chasing (Mantel et al., 2006). As Morris and Fu (2001) argue, some theory on this important topic.
managers in intercultural negotiations are able to better adapt to
another culture (just as some people grow up bilingual), so experi- Acknowledgments
ence might mitigate and facilitate the relationship. A comparison of
experienced versus inexperienced negotiators (in their approach to The authors thank David Cantor and John Gray as well as the
a global supply chain negotiation setting) would be a good exten- editors and reviewers of the Journal of Operations Management for
sion of this study. their helpful comments on earlier versions of this paper.
Another limitation of the experimental approach is that the par-
ticipants in the dyads were all part of an MBA program, and likely
References
had previous knowledge of their partner. In order to mitigate this
concern, the assignment of participants into dyads was random- Adair, W.L., 2003. Integrative sequences and negotiation outcome in same- and
ized. However, in a business environment, industry peers are also mixed-culture negotiations. International Journal of Conflict Management 14
likely to have some familiarity with each other (Gelfand et al., 2006). (3/4), 273–296.
Adair, W.L., Brett, J.M., 2005. The negotiation dance: time, culture, and behavioral
In addition, it can be argued that familiarity should counter the sequences in negotiations. Organization Science 16 (1), 33–51.
effect of cultural differences as participants will have developed Adair, W.L., Okumura, T., Brett, J.M., 2001. Negotiation behavior when cultures col-
a base level of trust by belonging to the same group. Given the lide: the United States and Japan. Journal of Applied Psychology 86 (3), 371–385.
Adler, N.J., Brahm, R., Graham, J.L., 1992. Strategy implementation: a comparison of
fact that this research finds strong results supporting the impact face-to-face negotiations in the People’s Republic of China and the United States.
of cultural differences on international buyer–supplier negotia- Strategic Management Journal 13 (6), 449–466.
tions, this shortcoming further strengthens this study’s arguments. Arrow, K.J., 1973. Information and Economic Behavior. Federation of Swedish Indus-
tries, Stockholm.
Future research could investigate the role of prior familiarity within Bagozzi, R.P., Verbeke, W., Gavino, J.C.J., 2003. Culture moderates the self-regulation
the dyad as a moderator of the cultural differences – joint profits of shame and its effects on performance: the case of salespersons in the
relationship. Netherlands and the Philippines. Journal of Applied Psychology 88 (2), 219–233.
Barley, S.R., 1991. Contextualizing conflict: notes on the anthropology of dis-
Culture as an individual construct is another potential limita-
putes and negotiations. In: Bazerman, M.H., Lewicki, R.J., Sheppard, B.H. (Eds.),
tion. While culture is generally thought of as societal attribute, Research in Negotiation in Organizations, vol. 3. JAI Press, Greenwich, CT.
characteristics of a society reflect individual perceptions. While Bendoly, E., Croson, R., Goncalves, P., Schultz, K.L., 2010. Bodies of knowledge for
research in behavioral operations. Production & Operations Management 19 (4),
there can clearly be individual differences within members of a
434–452.
society, on average, individuals from each context are likely to Bendoly, E., Donohue, K., Schultz, K.L., 2006. Behavior in operations management:
behave as predicted (Brett, 2007). Consequently, in negotiations assessing recent findings and revisiting old assumptions. Journal of Operations
with people from different countries, differences are more likely to Management 24 (6), 737–752.
Benzion, U., Cohen, Y., Peled, R., Shavit, T., 2008. Decision-making and the newsven-
be observed between participants than within dyads comprised of dor problem: an experimental study. Journal of the Operational Research Society
participants from the same country. In a future study, it would be 59 (9), 1281–1287.
interesting to observe individual-level scores within the context of Bolton, G.E., Katok, E., Ockenfels, A., 2004. How effective are electronic reputa-
tion mechanisms? An experimental investigation. Management Science 50 (11),
a societal group and assess the influence on each level. In addition, 1587–1602.
that study could also examine how individual aspects of trust and Brett, J.M., 2000. Culture and negotiations. International Journal of Psychology 35
bargaining behavior interact with national culture. (2), 97–104.
Brett, J.M., 2007. Negotiating Globally: How to Negotiate Deals, Resolve Disputes,
The study is also limited by breaking new ground regarding and Make Decisions Across Cultural Boundaries, 2nd ed. Wiley, New York.
the impact of cultural differences in buyer–supplier negotiations Brett, J.M., Okumura, T., 1998. Inter- and intracultural negotiation: U.S. and Japanese
and, in so doing, could not examine all the potentially interesting negotiators. Academy of Management Journal 41 (5), 495–510.
D. Ribbink, C.M. Grimm / Journal of Operations Management 32 (2014) 114–126 125

Brouthers, K.D., Brouthers, L.E., 2000. Acquisition or greenfield start-up? Institu- Hoyt, J., Huq, F., 2000. From arms-length to collaborative relationships in the supply
tional, cultural and transaction cost influences. Strategic Management Journal chain – an evolutionary process. International Journal of Physical Distribution &
21 (1), 89–97. Logistics Management 30 (9), 750–764.
Cannon, J.P., Doney, P.M., Mullen, M.R., Petersen, K.J., 2010. Building long-term ori- Javidan, M., Dorfman, P.W., de Luque, M.S., House, R.J., 2006. In the eye of the
entation in buyer–supplier relationships: the moderating role of culture. Journal beholder: cross cultural lessons in leadership from project GLOBE. Academy of
of Operations Management 28 (6), 506–521. Management Perspectives 20 (1), 67–90.
Cannon, J.P., Perreault, W.D.J., 1999. Buyer–seller relationships in business markets. Kalkanci, B., Chen, K.-Y., Erhun, F., 2011. Contract complexity and performance under
Journal of Marketing Research 36 (4), 439–460. asymmetric demand information: an experimental evaluation. Management
Carr, A.S., Pearson, J.N., 1999. Strategically managed buyer–seller relationships and Science 57 (4), 689–704.
performance outcomes. Journal of Operations Management 17 (5), 497–519. Kannan, V.R., Tan, K.-C., 2006. Buyer–supplier relationships: the impact of supplier
Carter, C.R., Stevens, C.K., 2007. Electronic reverse auction configuration and its selection and buyer–supplier engagement on relationship and firm perfor-
impact on buyer price and supplier perceptions of opportunism: a laboratory mance. International Journal of Physical Distribution & Logistics Management
experiment. Journal of Operations Management 25 (5), 1035–1054. 36 (10), 755–775.
Chen, C.C., Peng, M.W., Saparito, P.A., 2002. Individualism, collectivism, and oppor- Kaufmann, L., Carter, C.R., 2006. International supply relationships and non-financial
tunism: a cultural perspective on transaction cost economics. Journal of performance: a comparison of U. S. and German practices. Journal of Operations
Management 28 (4), 567–583. Management 24 (5), 653–675.
Chen, I.J., Paulraj, A., Lado, A.A., 2004. Strategic purchasing, supply management, and Kenny, D.A., Kashy, D.A., Cook, W.L., 2006. Dyadic Data Analysis. The Guilford Press,
firm performance. Journal of Operations Management 22 (5), 505–523. New York.
Cheung, M.-S., Myers, M.B., Mentzer, J.T., 2010. Does relationship learning lead to Klein, K.J., Kozlowski, S.W.J., 2000. From micro to meso: critical steps in conceptu-
relationship value? A cross-national supply chain investigation. Journal of Oper- alizing and conducting multilevel research. Organizational Research Methods 3
ations Management 28 (6), 472–487. (3), 211–236.
Chiles, T.H., McMackin, J.F., 1996. Integrating variable risk preferences, trust, and Krause, D.R., Handfield, R.B., Tyler, B.B., 2007. The relationships between sup-
transaction cost economics. Academy of Management Review 21 (1), 73–99. plier development, commitment, social capital accumulation and performance
Claycomb, C., Franwick, G.I., 2004. A contingency perspective of communication, improvement. Journal of Operations Management 25 (2), 528–545.
conflict resolution and buyer search effort in buyer–supplier relationships. Jour- Kremer, M., Minner, S., van Wassenhove, L.N., 2010. Do random errors explain
nal of Supply Chain Management 40 (1), 18–34. newsvendor behavior? Manufacturing & Service Operations Management 12
Coase, R.H., 1937. The nature of the firm. Economica 4 (16), 386–405. (4), 673–681.
Corsten, D., Felde, J., 2005. Exploring the performance effects of key-supplier collab- Langerak, F., 2001. Effects of market orientation on the behaviors of salespersons
oration. International Journal of Physical Distribution and Logistics Management and purchasers, channel relationship, and performance of manufacturers. Inter-
35 (6), 445–461. national Journal of Research in Marketing 18 (3), 22–234.
Croson, R., Donohue, K., 2002. Experimental economics and supply chain manage- Lazear, E.P., Rosen, S., 1991. Rank-order tournaments as optimum labor contracts.
ment. Interfaces 32 (5), 74–82. Journal of Political Economy 89 (5), 841–864.
Curall, S.C., Inkpen, A.C., 2002. A multilevel approach to trust in joint ventures. Lee, Y., Cavusgil, S.T., 2006. Enhancing alliance performance: the effects of
Journal of International Business Studies 33 (3), 479–495. contractual-based versus relational-based governance. Journal of Business
Doney, P.M., Cannon, J.P., 1997. An examination of the nature of trust in buyer–seller Research 59 (8), 896–905.
relationships. Journal of Marketing 61 (2), 35–51. Loch, C.H., Wu, Y., 2008. Social preferences and supply chain performance: an exper-
Dwyer, F.R., Walker, O.C.J., 1981. Bargaining in an asymmetrical power structure. imental study. Management Science 54 (11), 1835–1849.
Journal of Marketing 45 (1), 104–115. Lovejoy, W.S., 2010. Bargaining chains. Management Science 56 (12), 2282–2301.
Dyer, J.H., Singh, H., 1998. The relational view: cooperative strategy and sources Mantel, S.P., Tatikonda, M.V., Liao, Y., 2006. A behavioral study of supply managers
of interorganizational competitive advantage. The Academy of Management decision-making: factors influencing make versus buy evaluation. Journal of
Review 23 (4), 660–679. Operations Management 24 (6), 822–838.
Eckerd, S., Bendoly, E., 2011. Introduction to the discussion forum on using Mathews, H.L., Wilson, D.T., Monoky, J.F.J., 1972. Bargaining behavior in a
experiments in supply chain management research. Journal of Supply Chain buyer–seller dyad. Journal of Marketing Research 9 (1), 103–105.
Management 47 (3), 3–4. Meredith, J.R., 2002. 20th anniversary of JOM: an editorial retrospective and prospec-
Evans, K.R., Beltramini, R.F., 1987. A Theoretical model of consumer negotiated pri- tive. Journal of Operations Management 20 (1), 1–18.
cing: an orientation perspective. Journal of Marketing 51 (2), 58–73. Mintu-Wimsatt, A., Gassenheimer, J.B., 1996. Negotiation differences between two
Flynn, B.B., Saladin, B., 2006. Relevance of Baldridge constructs in an international diverse cultures. European Journal of Marketing 30 (4), 20–39.
context: a study of national culture. Journal of Operations Management 9 (2), Mintu-Wimsatt, A., Gassenheimer, J.B., 2000. The moderating effects of cultural
250–284. context in buyer–seller negotiations. Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Man-
Forgas, J.P., 1998. On feeling good and getting your way: mood effects on negotiator agement 20 (1), 1–9.
cognition and bargaining strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Montmarquette, C., Rulliere, J.-L., Villeval, M.-C., Zeiliger, R., 2004. Redesigning teams
74 (3), 565–577. and incentives in a merger: an experiment with managers and students. Man-
Gelfand, M.J., Christakopoulou, S., 1999. Culture and negotiator cognition: judgment agement Science 50 (10), 1379–1389.
accuracy and negotiation processes in individualistic and collectivistic cultures. Moorman, C., Zaltman, G., Deshpande, R., 1992. Relationships between providers
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 79 (3), 248–269. and users of market research: the dynamics of trust within and between orga-
Gelfand, M.J., Dyer, N., 2000. A cultural perspective on negotiation: progress, pitfalls, nizations. Journal of Marketing Research 24 (3), 314–328.
and prospects. Applied Psychology: An Internal Review 49 (1), 62–99. Morgan, R.M., Hunt, S.D., 1994. The commitment-trust theory of relationship mar-
Gelfand, M.J., Major, V.S., Raver, J.L., Nishii, L.H., O’Brian, K., 2006. Negotiating keting. Journal of Marketing 58 (3), 20–38.
relationally: the dynamics of the relational self in negotiations. Academy of Morris, M.W., Fu, H.-Y., 2001. How does culture influence conflict resolution? A
Management Review 31 (2), 427–451. dynamic constructivist analysis. Social Cognition 19 (3), 324–349.
Gerbing, D.W., Anderson, J.C., 1988. An updated paradigm for scale develop- Naor, M., Linderman, K., Schroeder, R., 2010. The globalization of operations in East-
ment incorporating unidimensionality and its assessment. Journal of Marketing ern and Western countries: unpacking the relationship between national and
Research 25 (2), 186–192. organizational culture and its impact on manufacturing performance. Journal of
Ghoshal, S., Moran, P., 1996. Bad for practice: a critique of the transaction cost theory. Operations Management 28 (3), 194–205.
Academy of Management Review 21 (1), 13–47. Ness, H., Haugland, S.A., 2005. The evolution of governance mechanisms and nego-
Graham, J.L., Mintu, A.T., Rodgers, W., 1994. Explorations of negotiation behaviors in tiation strategies in fixed-duration interfirm relationships. Journal of Business
ten foreign cultures using a model developed in the United States. Management Research 58 (9), 1226–1239.
Science 40 (1), 72–95. Olekalns, M., Smith, P.L., 2003. Social motives in negotiation: the relationships
Hair, J.F.J., Anderson, R.E., Tatham, R.L., Black, W.C., 1998. Multivariate Data Analysis, between dyad composition, negotiation processes and outcomes. International
5th ed. Prentice-Hall, Inc., Upper Saddle River, NJ. Journal of Conflict Management 14 (3/4), 233–254.
Hall, E.T., 1976. Beyond Culture. Doubleday, Garden City, NY. Pagell, M., Katz, J.P., Sheu, C., 2005. The importance of national culture in opera-
Heide, J.B., John, G., 1992. Do norms matter in marketing relationships? Journal of tions management research. International Journal of Operations & Production
Marketing 56 (2), 32–44. Management 25 (4), 371–394.
Heisler, W.J., Gemmill, G.R., 1978. Executive and MBA student views of corporate Parkhe, A., 1993. Strategic alliance structuring: a game theoretic and transaction
promotion practices: a structural comparison. Academy of Management Journal cost examination of Interfirm Cooperation. Academy of Management Journal 36
21 (4), 731–737. (4), 794–829.
Hofstede, G., 1980. Culture and organizations. International Studies of Management Paulraj, A., Lado, A.A., Chen, I.J., 2008. Inter-organizational communication as a rela-
& Organization 10 (4), 15–41. tional competency: Antecedents and performance outcomes in collaborative
Hofstede, G., 1985. The interaction between national and organizational value sys- buyer–supplier relationships. Journal of Operations Management 26 (1), 45–64.
tems. Journal of Management Studies 22 (4), 347–357. Power, D., Schoenherr, T., Samson, D., 2010. The cultural characteristic of indi-
Homburg, C., Krohmer, H., Cannon, J.P., Kiedaisch, I., 2002. Customer satisfaction in vidualism/collectivism: a comparative study of implications for investment in
transnational buyer–supplier relationships. Journal of International Marketing operations between emerging Asian and industrialized Western countries. Jour-
10 (4), 1–29. nal of Operations Management 28 (3), 206–222.
House, R.J., Hanges, P.J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P.W., Gupta, V., 2004. Culture, Lead- Prahinski, C., Benton, W.C., 2004. Supplier evaluations: communication strategies
ership, and Organizations – The GLOBE Study of 62 Societies. Sage Publications, to improve supplier performance. Journal of Operations Management 22 (1),
Inc., Thousand Oaks, CA. 39–62.
126 D. Ribbink, C.M. Grimm / Journal of Operations Management 32 (2014) 114–126

Pruitt, D.G., 1981. Negotiation Behavior. Academic Press, New York. Thomas, R.W., 2011. When student samples make sense in logistics research. Journal
Pruitt, D.G., Carnevale, P.J., 1993. Negotiation in Social Conflict. Open University of Business Logistics 32 (3), 287–290.
Press, Buckingham. Thomas, S.P., Thomas, R.W., Manrodt, K.B., Rutner, S.M., 2013. An experimental test of
Pruitt, D.G., Lewis, S.A., 1975. Development of integrative solution in bilateral nego- negotiation strategy effects on knowledge sharing intentions in buyer–supplier
tiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 32 (4), 363–392. relationships. Journal of Supply Chain Management 49 (2), 96–113.
Rabe-Hesketh, S., Skrondal, A., 2008. Multilevel and Longitudinal Modeling Using Tokar, T., 2010. Behavioral research in logistics and supply chain management. Inter-
Stata, 2nd ed. Stata Press, College Station, TX. national Journal of Logistics Management 21 (1), 89–103.
Rivers, C., Lytle, A.L., 2007. Lying, cheating foreigners!! Negotiation ethics across Triandis, H.C., 2000. Culture and Conflict. International Journal of Psychology 35 (2),
cultures. International Negotiation 12 (1), 1–28. 145–152.
Rubin, J.Z., Brown, B.R., 1975. The Social Psychology of Bargaining and Negotiation. Tsui, A.S., Nifadkar, S.S., Ou, A.Y., 2007. Cross-national, cross-cultural organizational
Academic Press, New York, NY. behavior research: advances, gaps, and recommendations. Journal of Manage-
Rungtusanatham, M., Wallin, C., Eckerd, S., 2011. The vignette in a scenario- ment 33 (3), 426–478.
based role-playing experiment. Journal of Supply Chain Management 47 (3), Ueltschy, L.C., Ueltschy, M.L., Fachinelli, A.C., 2007. The impact of culture on the
9–16. generation of trust in global supply chain relationships. Marketing Management
Schurr, P.H., Ozanne, J.L., 1985. Influence on exchange processes: buyers’ precon- Journal 17 (1), 15–26.
ceptions of a seller’s trustworthiness and bargaining toughness. Journal of Wacker, J.G., 1998. A definition of theory: research guidelines for different theory-
Consumer Research 11 (4), 939–953. building research methods in operations management. Journal of Operations
Sharma, S., 1996. Applied Multivariate Techniques. Wiley, New York, NY. Management 16, 361–385.
Siemsen, E., 2011. The usefulness of behavioral laboratory experiments in supply Waldman, D.E., Jensen, E.J., 2007. Industrial Organization: Theory and Practice, 3rd
chain management. Journal of Supply Chain Management 47 (3.). ed. Pearson Education, Inc., Boston, MA.
Spake, D.F., Bishop, J.S.J., 2009. The impact of perceived closeness on the differing Williamson, O.E., 1975. Market and Hierarchies: Analysis and Antitrust Implications.
roles of satisfaction, trust, commitment, and comfort on intention to remain The Free Press, New York.
with a physician. Health Marketing Quarterly 26 (1), 1–15. Williamson, O.E., 1979. Transaction-cost economics: the governance of contractual
Stevens, C.K., 2011. Questions to consider when selecting student samples. Journal relations. Journal of Law and Economics 22 (2), 233–261.
of Supply Chain Management 47 (3), 19–21. Williamson, O.E., 1981. The modern corporation: origins, evolution, attributes. Jour-
Takahashi, C., Yamagishi, T., Liu, J.H., Wang, F., Lin, Y., Yu, S., 2008. The intercultural nal of Economic Literature 19 (4), 1537–1568.
trust paradigm: studying joint cultural interaction and social exchange in real Williamson, O.E., 1985. The Economic Institutions of Capitalism: Firms, Markets,
time over the Internet. International Journal of Intercultural Relations 32 (3), Relational Contracting. Free Press, New York.
215–228. Williamson, O.E., 1993. Calculativeness, trust, and economic organization. Journal
Tangpong, C., Ro, Y.K., 2009. The role of agent negotiation behaviors of Law and Economics 36 (1), 453–486.
in buyer–supplier relationships. Journal of Managerial Issues 21 (1), Williamson, O.E., 2008. Outsourcing: transaction cost economics and supply chain
58–79. management. Journal of Supply Chain Management 44 (2), 5–16.

You might also like