You are on page 1of 1

GLOBALISATION AND VALUE MISMATCH 3

success of the second-generation Chinese businessmen differences can be more or less accurate with correspond-
(who possess cultural resources from both Confucianism ing differences in social adjustment. As found in the
and Western values) is attributable to their learning from special issue (Glazer, Simonovich, Roach, & Carmona,
and integration of different cultures, although they also 2018), international students overestimated the extent to
experience value mismatch and conflicts with both their which Americans endorse idiocentric values (i.e., power,
parent culture and the Western culture (Mei & Lv, 2019). hedonism, stimulation, and self-direction values) and
Although value mismatch occurs among different cul- underestimated the extent to which Americans endorse
tures, multicultural and polycultural experience can pro- allocentric values (i.e., universalism, benevolence, secu-
mote cultural competence and innovation. A vast body rity, conformity, and tradition values). As can be expected,
of literature demonstrates that people exposed to mul- acculturation stress is associated with overestimation of
ticultural and polycultural environments (e.g., via over- the predominant values, and socio-cultural adjustment is
seas learning, travelling, business, immigration, or just positively related to perceived value congruence (Glazer
cultural symbols) have relatively higher cultural compe- et al., 2018).
tence in contextual knowledge and cognitive flexibility, As for contextual factors, social environments that
for example, better performance in creative tasks such as serve as buffer or accelerator of the acculturation stress
fairy tale writing and generation of unconventional ideas may impact acculturative process and adaptation. For
(Leung & Chiu, 2010), or a deeper level of cognitive pro- example, assimilationist environments create psycholog-
cessing and creativity on the unusual uses test (Cheng, ical tensions and intergroup separation, while supportive
Leung, & Wu, 2011). Similarly, when urban Chinese are multicultural environments foster positive adjustment and
exposed to cultural mixing, as happens in the context of engagement in intergroup relationships (Plaut, Thomas, &
globalisation, they are more sensitive to both Chinese val- Goren, 2009; Schwartz et al., 2014).
ues (e.g., filial piety, modesty) and Western values (e.g.,
individualism, freedom), relative to their rural peers who
live in a monocultural environment (Chen & Chiu, 2010). THE BIDIRECTIONALITY OF CULTURAL VALUE
MISMATCH

IDENTIFYING WHO REAPS THE BENEFITS, As proposed in the Special Issue, cultural values are adap-
WHO PAYS THE COSTS OF CULTURAL VALUE tations that have co-evolved with a population’s ecologi-
MISMATCH cal niche, resulting in two distinct components—cultural
values and their corresponding ecological niche (Wu
All cultures and individuals, both the predominant as well et al., 2018). As a consequence of the trans-planetary
as the disadvantaged, or the WEIRD (i.e., Western, edu- flow of commodities—including people—in the global-
cated, industrialised, rich, and democratic) as well as the ising era, values and beliefs, and even including cogni-
non-WEIRD, are in a growing and integrative process, tive skills and reasoning, may come decoupled from their
rather than functioning as discreet (either matched or mis- co-evolved niches. In this vein, the decoupling between
matched) fixed entities that result in either conflict or these attributes and the ecological niches can be observed
assimilation (Dweck & Yeager, 2019). In this vein, per- among those who hold collectivist values but live in mod-
sonal orientation towards acculturation, accuracy of per- ern/urban societies, as well as those who are individual-
ceiving differences between host and home culture, and istic but living in traditional/rural communities (see also:
social contexts, in addition to individual characteristics, Todd et al., 2012).
play critical roles in shaping acculturative stress or/and Migrants may be considered a case of cultural values
adaptive growth (Ward & Geeraert, 2016). This article that take a walk and end up in mismatching ecological
does not contain any studies with human participants or niches. It should be noted that there are two directions
animals performed by any of the authors. regarding migration: The prevalent trend is that people
Regarding personal orientation, it has been well migrate from poorer countries to wealthier ones and from
established that people orienting themselves to inte- relatively homogenous rural areas to more diverse urban
gration (defined as being engaged in both heritage environments (Greenfield, 2009), which are under the
culture and in the larger society) are better adapted sway of WEIRD values, such as independence or individ-
than those oriented to one or the other culture (by way ualism in general (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010).
of assimilation or separation) (Sam & Berry, 2010). However, the other direction is also possible, namely
However, acculturation categories are often oversimpli- that individuals who are exposed to (or socialised with)
fied, in that, along with urbanisation and globalisation, the WEIRD values go back to live in the non-WEIRD
only one direction of acculturation is assumed, with indi- environment, because of family, business, governance,
viduals shifting towards the values and behaviours of the and sometime colonisation, which also creates mismatch
predominant environment, in order to avoid or overcome and even conflicts between the WEIRD values and the
value mismatch. However, perceptions of cultural value non-WEIRD environment (Li, 2014; Li & Liu, 2018).

© 2019 International Union of Psychological Science

You might also like