You are on page 1of 20

Psychological Bulletin Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1991, Vol. L10, No. 1,109-128 0033-2909/91/i3.00

What Is Beautiful Is Good, But. . .: A Meta-Anatytic Review of Research


on the Physical Attractiveness Stereotype

Alice H. Eagjy Richard D. Ashmore


Purdue University Rutgers—The State University of New Jersey

Mona G. Makhijani Laura C. Longo


Purdue University Rutgers—The State University of New Jersey

This review demonstrates that the physical attractiveness stereotype established by studies of per-
son perception is not as strong or general as suggested by the often-used summary phrase what is
beautiful is good. Although subjects in these studies ascribed more favorable personality traits and
more successful life outcomes to attractive than unattractive targets, the average magnitude of this
beauty-is-good effect was moderate, and the strength of the effect varied considerably from study to
study Consistent with our implicit personality theory framework, a substantial portion of this
variation was explained by the specific content of the inferences that subjects were asked to make:
The differences in subjects* perception of attractive and unattractive targets were largest for indexes
of social competence; intermediate for potency, adjustment, and intellectual competence; and near
zero for integrity and concern for others. The strength of the physical attractiveness stereotype also
varied as a function of other attributes of the studies, including the presence of individuating
information.

One of the most widely cited conclusions from research on ality traits as well as to life outcomes such as marital happiness
physical attractiveness is summarized by Dion, Berscheid, and and career success. Indeed, subjects ascribed more favorable
Walster's (1972) claim that, in people's perceptions of others, personality traits and more successful life outcomes to the pic-
"what is beautiful is good" (p. 285). This statement linking tured individuals to the extent that they were physically attrac-
beauty and goodness suggests the existence of a stereotype tive.
whereby physically attractive individuals are believed to possess The experimental paradigm introduced by Dion and her col-
a wide variety of positive personal qualities. In this article, we leagues (1972) has guided many subsequent investigators, and
integrate the available research on the physical attractiveness there is, thus, a relatively large body of research on the attractive-
stereotype to determine the extent to which the statement that ness stereotype. Most narrative reviewers of this literature have
what is beautiful is good provides an accurate summary of peo- agreed that the beauty-is-good stereotype is a strong and gen-
ple's inferences from cues that convey physical attractiveness. eral phenomenon (Adams, 1982; Alley & Hildebrandt, 1988;
In the classic study on the physical attractiveness stereotype, Berscheid, 1981; Berscheid & Walster, 1974; Dion, 1981,1986;
Dion and her associates (1972) had subjects rate facial photo- Hatfield &Sprecher, 1986; Patzer, 1985). For example, Hatfield
graphs that had been selected on the basis of judges' agreement and Sprecher wrote that "people believe good-looking people
that the pictured individuals were low, medium, or high in phys- possess almost all the virtues known to humankind" (p. xix).
ical attractiveness. Subjects' ratings pertained to various person- Most secondary sources, especially introductory social psychol-
ogy textbooks, have likewise treated the beauty-is-good stereo-
type as a homogeneous, potent, and firmly established phenom-
This research was supported by National Science Foundation enon (e.g, see Baron & Byrne, 1987, p. 197; Deaux & Wrights-
Grants BNS-8616149 to Richard EX Ashmore and Frances K. Del man, 1988, p. 250). In this article, we report the first use of
Boca, Co-Principal Investigators, and BNS-S605256 to Alice H. Eagl>; meta-analytic procedures to combine quantitatively the exist-
Principal Investigator. A preliminary report of this research was re- ing studies to examine the strength and generality of the attrac-
ported at the annual meeting of the American Psychological Society, tiveness stereotype.
June 1989. A table showing the effect sizes and study characteristics for
Although the general consensus in social psychology is that
each study included in the meta-analysis is available from Alice H.
an attractive target leads perceivers to make strong inferences of
Eagly or Richard EX Ashmore.
personality goodness, reviewers have noted exceptions to this
We thank Thomas Alley, Ellen Berscheid, Linda Jackson, and three
phenomenon. Dion (1981,1986) suggested that the beauty-is-
anonymous reviewers for their comments on a draft of the article.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to good effect is strongest for measures of social competence and
Alice H. Eagly, Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue Univer- interpersonal ease, and Bassili (1981) reached a similar conclu-
sity, West Lafayette, Indiana 47907-1364, or Richard D. Ashmore, De- sion, arguing that the core of the physical attractiveness stereo-
partment of Psychology, Tillett Hall, Kilmer Campus, Rutgers Univer- type is social vitality or extroversion. Also, beginning with
sity, New Brunswick, New Jersey 08903. Dermer and Thiel (1975), reviewers noted a dark side to the

109
110 EAGLY, ASHMORE, MAKHIJANI, AND LONGO

stereotype. Cash and Janda (1984, p. 52) labeled this aspect the Therefore, the stereotype question can be regarded as more
"what is beautiful is self-centered" stereotype on the basis of basic to social psychological analysis than the questions of
their observation that attractive people may be thought to be treatment and assessed characteristics. If the physical attractive-
vain and egotistical. What is needed at this point is a formal and ness stereotype is not at least moderately strong and robust, it is
general scheme for understanding these qualifications and ex- unlikely to underlie the effects of physical attractiveness on
ceptions. As a step in this direction, we conceptualize the pro- treatment and people's characteristics.
cess underlying the attractiveness stereotype in terms of the Our decision to focus on the physical attractiveness stereo-
person-perception construct of implicit personality theory (see type has two important implications—one methodological and
also Adams, 1982; McArthur, 1982). We then frame predictions one theoretical. Methodologically, we targeted our meta-analy-
about variation in the beauty-is-good effect in terms of this sis to include only studies that assess the physical attractiveness
conceptual approach. These hypotheses are tested by means of stereotype in a relatively pure form, without the constraints of
a quantitative review of the accumulated research. Before pre- particular role relationships such as those that regulate social
senting the details of this review, we situate our discussion of the interaction between teachers and students or therapists and
beauty-is-good stereotype within the total body of research and patients. To examine stereotyping in a relatively role-free envi-
theory on physical attractiveness. ronment, we included in our review only those studies in which
subjects inferred the attributes of people to whom they were not
linked by particular social roles. Subjects' inferences from phys-
Social Psychology of Beauty
ical appearance in role-regulated environments should be con-
A large amount of research has accumulated on physical at- sidered in future reviews that are conceptualized to take the
tractiveness (see Cash's, 1981, bibliography of almost 500 nonre- specifics of role relationships into account. In terms of theoreti-
dundant entries). Because this research area is multidisciplin- cal implications, our focus on the physical attractiveness stereo-
ary, the studies are methodologically diverse and pertain to a type led us to formulate our review in terms of existing psycho-
variety of specific hypotheses. However, among studies formu- logical research on impression formation and stereotyping. We
lated at a social psychological level of analysis, most research now turn to this formulation.
has addressed three broad questions: (a) Are attractive people
perceived differently than unattractive people? (b) Are attractive
Physical Attractiveness Stereotype and Implicit
people treated differently than unattractive people? (c) Do at-
Personality Theory
tractive people have different characteristics (i£., personality
traits, skills, behavioral tendencies) than unattractive people? Ashmore and Del Boca (1979; see also Ashmore, 1981; Ash-
The first research area, the perception of beautiful people, more, Del Boca, & Wohlers, 1986) argued that stereotypes can
concerns stereotyping on the basis of physical appearance. The be fruitfully conceptualized in terms of the concept of implicit
second area, which concerns differential treatment as a func- personality theory (D. J. Schneider, 1973). By treating stereo-
tion of physical attractiveness, examines looks-based discrimi- types as part of ordinary social cognition (see also Hamilton,
nation, a phenomenon that depends on factors in addition to 1981), Ashmore and his co-workers placed stereotypes within
stereotypes about physical attractiveness (e.g., the social roles the larger set of knowledge structures that individuals use to
governing interaction). The third type of research focuses not make sense of other people's behavior. More specifically, im-
on people's reactions to targets, but on the characteristics of plicit personality theories are hypothetical cognitive structures
attractive and unattractive individuals, as measured by various whose primary components are personal attributes (e.g., person-
methods of psychological assessment (e.g., personality tests, ality traits) and inferential relations that specify the degree to
self-reports of behavior, observations of behavior). The findings which these attributes covary. The link between such implicit
of this third type of research have often been interpreted in theories and stereotypes about group members becomes appar-
terms of self-fulfilling prophecies (e.g, Snyder, Tanke, & Ber- ent when we regard group membership as one of the personal
scheid, 1977) or a kernel of truth that underlies the attractiveness attributes inferentially associated with other attributes in such
stereotype (e.g., Alley & Hildebrandt, 1988). an implicit theory (Ashmore, 1981). In fact, the inferential rela-
Our review concerns only the first area of social psychologi- tions between group membership and other personal attributes
cal research on attractiveness: the perception of attractive and can be regarded as synonymous with the stereotype itself. Thus,
unattractive individuals. There are several reasons for this translating the usual definition of stereotype as a set of beliefs
focus. One reason is practical: A thorough review of all three about the characteristics of group members into the language of
aspects of physical attractiveness research would require many implicit personality theory, Ashmore and Del Boca (1979) de-
years to complete and would produce an outcome too large for fined stereotype as "a structured set of inferential relations that
presentation in an article format. A second reason is strategic: link a social category with personal attributes" (p. 225).
Meta-analyses are best targeted to research literatures that are The utility of this conceptualization has been demonstrated
not defined extremely broadly and that have at least a moderate for gender stereotypes. Using the implicit personality theory
amount of methodological coherence (see Mullen, 1989). A approach, Ashmore and his colleagues identified the inferential
third reason is analytic: The beauty-is-good stereotype is gener- relations linking the social categories of male and female to
ally regarded as providing the major mechanism underlying various dimensions of personality perception (see Ashmore,
differential treatment based on attractiveness and the develop- 1981). Specifically, the distinction between male and female
ment of differential assessed characteristics, at least insofar as was closely associated with the personality dimension of
these characteristics are mediated by self-fulfilling prophecies. strong-weak (or potency). Also men were thought to possess
REVIEW OF PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS STEREOTYPE 111

positive intellectual qualities such as scientific and negative so- tellectual competence, concern for others, integrity, adjustment
cial attributes such as sternness. In contrast, women were asso- (i£, psychological stability), and potency.
ciated with negative intellectual qualities such as naivete and The core of the social competence category is those interper-
positive social attributes such as helpfulness. sonal skills and traits concerned with sociability (e.g., extraver-
From this same perspective, research on the beauty-is-good sion) as well as the outcomes of such skills and inclinations (e.g.,
stereotype can be viewed as examining the inferential relations popularity). Social competence, as here construed, corresponds
between physical attractiveness and personal attributes. The closely with Bassili's (1981, p. 242) "social vitality" and Dion's
social categories of attractive and unattractive people should (1986, p. 8) "social competence and interpersonal ease." The
thus be associated in individuals' cognitions with various di- intellectual competence category centers on intellectual and
mensions of personality. This approach contrasts with those task-relevant ability (e.g, intelligent) and the results of such abil-
analyses of the physical attractiveness stereotype that take into ity and motivation (eg, career success); this category also en-
account only inferences from attractiveness to the general fa- compasses a hard and rational mental style (e.g., practical). Con-

vorability of personality. This presumed inference from attrac- cern for others, like social competence, implies interpersonal
tiveness to evaluation is precisely described by the statement skills. The emphasis, however, is not on sociability and popular-
ity, but concern for others' welfare and the associated quality of
that beauty is good. This one-dimensional evaluative model
selflessness. The positive side of this category involves being
treats the different trait and life-outcome dependent variables
emotionally supportive as well as sensitive and generous to
used in attractiveness research as if they indexed a single, ab-
others. The negative side of this category, which includes traits
stract good-bad continuum. Although the notion of a content-
such as vain and egotistical, is the dark side of the attractiveness
free evaluative dimension has much conceptual and empirical
stereotype acknowledged by some reviewers (eg., Adams, 1982;
value (e.g., Osgood, Suci, & Tannenbaum, 1957), research on
Cash & Janda, 1984). The crucial issue for defining the integrity
implicit personality theory has demonstrated that the evalua-
category is honesty. This quality can take the form of interper-
tive dimension can be partitioned by perceivers into content-
sonal actions such as telling the truth (vs. being a liar) or abiding
specific types of evaluative meaning. Support for this partition-
by laws and norms (vs. breaking them). The adjustment cate-
ing of evaluative meaning exists in Rosenberg and his col-
gory, which is a slight enlargement of Rosenberg's (1977) psycho-
leagues' research using multidimensional scaling (Kim &
logical stability category, includes not only normal (vs. abnor-
Rosenberg, 1980; Rosenberg, 1977; Rosenberg, Nelson, & Vive-
mal) psychological functioning, but also other indicators of pos-
kananthan, 1968; Rosenberg & Sedlak, 1972). According to this
itive adult adjustment, such as self-esteem and good health.
research, one particularly important distinction between types
Potency resembles Osgood and colleagues' (1957) potency or
of evaluative meaning, at least for young adults, is between so-
strong versus weak factor. In interpersonal terms, potency im-
cial and intellectual competence (Rosenberg, 1977; Rosenberg et
plies dominance over other people (Ashmore, 1981; White,
al, 1968). In addition, on the basis of his own and others' re-
1980).
search, Rosenberg identified the content-specific evaluative cat-
egories of maturity, concern for others, integrity, psychological
Sources of the Physical Attractiveness Stereotype
stability, and physical attractiveness. He also designated potency
as an important category that is sometimes relatively indepen- Understanding how these types of evaluative meanings
dent of evaluation (Kim & Rosenberg, 1980; Rosenberg et al, might he mentally associated with physical attractiveness re-
1968; see also Ashmore, 1981). In view of Rosenberg's inclusion quires consideration of how people learn their implicit theories
of physical attractiveness among these content-specific evalua- of personality including their beliefs about what is associated
tive categories, analyzing the beauty-is-good stereotype within with good looks. Two general types of inputs seem most impor-
the framework of implicit personality theory is especially ap- tant: (a) direct observations of attractive and unattractive people
propriate. in oneTs social environment and (b) exposure to cultural repre-
This conceptual framework suggests that part of the varia- sentations of attractive and unattractive people.1 With regard to
tion in the impact of physical attractiveness on subjects' infer- direct observation, perceivers no doubt observe that better look-
ences about personality and life outcomes may be explained by ing people receive more favorable reactions from others; for
the specific content of these inferences (see also Bull & Rumsey, example, attractive people are more popular with peers and
1988, pp. 296-297). In particular, physical attractiveness may receive preferential treatment from others (see reviews by
be more strongly linked to some dimensions of implicit person- Adams, 1982; Dion, 1981,1986; Feingold, 1990b; Patzer, 1985;
ality theories than to other dimensions. To test this possibility, but see Bull & Rumsey, 1988; Langlois, 1986). Perceivers may
we developed a content-analysis scheme for classifying the de- assume that these favorable reactions are elicited, not merely by
pendent variables used in studies of the physical attractiveness
stereotype into the components identified in research on im-
1
plicit personality theory. This scheme takes Rosenbergls (1977) In addition, physical attractiveness may be aesthetically pleasing
and therefore may elicit positive affect that leads perceivers to infer,
evaluative categories as its starting point. Although Rosenberg's
perhaps to account for the affect, that attractive people have favorable
maturity category could not be treated as a separate class be-
characteristics. Although there is little evidence that the sight of physi-
cause it was.seldom represented in research on the physical
cally attractive people induces positive affect, researchers who manipu-
attractiveness stereotype, dependent variables corresponding late affect by presenting photographic slides commonly include images
to Rosenberg's other types were common enough to include of physically attractive people among the stimuli that induce positive
them as categories in the meta-analysis: social competence, in- affect (e.g., Larsen, Diener, & Cropanzano, 1987).
112 EAGLY, ASHMORE, MAKHUANI, AND LONGO

people's good looks, but by other positive aspects of people's perceivers may infer on the basis of attractive people's pre-
behavior and personality. sumed social competence that they have other favorable traits.
Depending in part on the impact that popularity and favor- For example, given that popularity is valued, physical attractive-
able treatment have on personality and behavior, perceivers ness may be viewed as a valued commodity and thereby as a
may observe that physical attractiveness is correlated with source of interpersonal power. As a consequence, perceivers
various positive traits. Consistent with Feingold's (1990b) re- may associate beauty with interpersonal potency. In addition,
view, research on the true covariation between physical attrac- perceivers may assume that good looks are not only a source of
tiveness and other personal attributes suggests that good looks power but also major ingredients of happiness, self-esteem, and
covary with social skills, social adjustment, and an absence of the other aspects of adjustment. Yet, all these second-order in-
shyness and social anxiety (e.g., Adams, 1977; Adams & Ware- ferences that people make from attractiveness may not lead to
ham, no date; Goldman & Lewis, 1977; Lerner & Lerner, 1977; positive impressions of beautiful targets. For example, concern-
Pilkonis, 1977). Aside from this relation between physical at- ing integrity, people might infer that attractive individuals are
tractiveness and social competence, evidence for true covaria- exposed to more opportunities for interpersonal cheating (e.g,
tion between good looks and personal attributes is much less married people having affairs; see Brigham, 1980). Also per-
convincing, although there is some suggestion of a weak posi- ceivers may assume that the popularity of attractive people
tive correlation with adjustment (see Cash, 1985) and potency makes it unnecessary for them to develop nurturant qualities or
(Adams, 1977; Chaiken, 1979,1986). interpersonal sensitivity to maintain their friendships and other
Turning to cultural messages about appearance, we suggest relationships.
that American culture associates beauty with good things and In conclusion, our reasoning suggests a number of principles
ugliness with bad things. In children's television and books, the that might underlie the content and structure of the physical
wicked witch and evil giant are ugly and the heroic prince and attractiveness stereotype. Most important, good looks should
virtuous princess are attractive. In advertising, attractive mod- strongly imply social competence because of (a) the perception
els appear in positive settings (e.g, in happy crowds, as the ob- that attractive individuals elicit positive reactions from others,
ject of admiration) and with valued possessions (e.g- fancy cars, (b) the perception of true covariation between attractiveness
fashionable clothes). In addition, as shown by Downs and and social competence, and (c) the media portrayal of attractive-
Harrison's (1985) content analysis of the verbal content of tele- ness as critical to heterosexual popularity and social attention.
vision commercials, statements extolling the virtues of attrac- In addition, good looks may imply a variety of other positive
tive appearance are quite common. \et in these cultural produc- qualities based on the inferential linkages we have noted and
tions, beauty may be associated more often with some positive possibly based to some extent on observations of true (or media-
qualities than others. Although appropriate content analyses of depicted) covariation between attractiveness and other attri-
media content are lacking, we suspect that the media most butes. This analysis, therefore, does support the notion of a
often link good looks with sociability and popularity, especially general mental association between beauty and nonphysical
with the opposite sex. We doubt that the media consistently goodness. However, as our analysis has also implied, the ten-
associate physical attractiveness with integrity, concern for dency for good looks to be associated with positive qualities
others, adjustment, or potency. Cultural portrayals of individ- may not be very strong overall and may vary considerably in
uals outstanding in these qualities are not clearly distinguished strength, depending on the particular dimension of personality
by positive appearance, nor are beautiful people necessarily assessed.
exemplars of such qualities.
Despite the possibility that the media may portray attractive
Predictions for Meta-Analysis
people as successful in their careers, we doubt that physical
attractiveness is strongly linked with intellectual competence in Predictions about specific content of physical attractiveness
the popular culture. People depicted as outstanding in intellect stereotype. Our primary predictions follow from the implicit
or accomplishment are not uniformly good-looking, nor are personality theory framework as well as from the additional
good-looking people necessarily portrayed as smart and ac- assumptions that we introduced about (a) social attention and
complished (e.g, those labeled as dumb blondes). A weaker im- rewards received by beautiful people, (b) genuine covariation
pact of attractiveness on intellectual competence than on social between physical attractiveness and various attributes, (c) me-
competence was supported by Ashmore, Tumia, and Schreier's dia content, and (d) perceivers' inferential linkages. These pre-
(1980) variation of the classic Asch (1946) experiment on the dictions concern the differential effects that physical attractive-
effect of the adjectives warm or cold on impression formation. ness has on perceivers' inferences about the personality traits
Ashmore and colleagues substituted good-looking for warm and that represent the various dimensions of their implicit theories
unattractive for cold, and they found that this attractiveness of personality. Although we predict that attractive targets
manipulation strongly influenced the ascription of social com- should be assigned more positive attributes than unattractive
petence to the stimulus person but not the ascription of intellec- targets, we hypothesize that this tendency will vary consider-
tual competence. ably as a function of type of evaluative meaning. Specifically for
In understanding the content of the physical attractiveness the reasons we have indicated, information about physical at-
stereotype, we also need to take into account the inferences by tractiveness should have its strongest impact on inferences
which perceivers may associate stereotypic attributes with other about social competence. Bassili (1981) and Dion (1981,1986)
personal attributes. In particular, given the strong link we have would probably agree with this prediction. Because of the likely
demonstrated between attractiveness and social competence, spillover that we described from social competence to potency
REVIEW OF PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS STEREOTYPE 113

and adjustment, physical attractiveness should affect inferences ity traits should be weakened. This prediction has been repeat-
to these dimensions, albeit somewhat more weakly than infer- edly confirmed in research showing that perceivers' inferences
ences to social competence. from sex to personality or behavior are reduced or even re-
According to the reasoning we have presented, manipula- versed by giving subjects other information about targets (e.g,
tions of physical attractiveness should have a relatively small Deaux & Lewis, 1984; Del Boca & Ashmore, 1980; Eagry &
effect on inferences about intellectual competence and an even Steffen, 1984; Locksley, Borgida, Brekke, & Hepburn, 1980;
weaker impact on integrity and concern for others. The impact Locksley, Hepburn, & Ortiz, 1982).
of attractiveness on concern for others may be especially weak The sex of the target is an item of information that subjects in
given that this category includes traits such as vanity and ego- physical attractiveness experiments almost always possess. This
tism. As we have already noted, reviewers have implicated such fact raises the question of whether the attractiveness stereotype
traits in the dark side of the physical attractiveness stereotype may be stronger for female targets than for male targets. Such a
(Adams, 1982; Cash & Janda, 1984; Dermer & Thiel, 1975).2 trend would be consistent with reviewers' conclusions that at-
These predictions suggest that the specific content of the tractiveness is more central to the gender role of women than of
dependent variables used in studies of the physical attractive- men as well as to women's personal identity, self-esteem, and
ness stereotype contributes to observed differences in the interpersonal outcomes (see Bar-Tal & Saxe, 1976; Rodin, Sil-
strength of the stereotype. Studies that happened to include berstein, & Striegel-Moore, 1985). Moreover, the media more
many dependent variables assessing perceptions of social com- often portray women than men as decorative and sexual objects
petence should show a stronger impact of physical attractive- (see Courtney & Whipple, 1983; Roberts, 1982). In addition, in
ness than studies containing higher proportions of dependent television commercials explicit verbal statements linking
variables selected from the other dimensions of personality. beauty and good outcomes are more commonly paired with
The content of dependent variables should also account for vari- female actors than with male actors (Downs & Harrison, 1985),
ations within studies in the strength of subjects' inferences from and viewers of televised advertisements regard the women who
physical appearance to aspects of targets' personalities. are depicted as more concerned with their looks than the men
Other predictions. Because implicit theories of personality are (Scheibe, 1979). Finally, Jackson, Sullivan, and Rostker
comprise much more than beliefs about attractiveness, our (1988) found that women, compared with men, rated physical
framework raises the issue of whether information that subjects attractiveness as more important to them and reported engag-
receive about attributes other than physical attractiveness may ing in more behaviors to improve their looks. Such findings
also account for inconsistencies in the outcomes of studies. In suggest that physical attractiveness may be more important in
particular, receiving information that conveys attributes of the perceiving women than in perceiving men.
target other than physical attractiveness may moderate infer- Whether physical attractiveness is more important to per-
ences from physical attractiveness to personality traits. ceivers of one sex than the other is not entirely clear. In view of
In general, the beauty-is-good effect should become smaller Feingold's (1990a) meta-analysis showing that men place greater
as the amount of other information about targets increases. This value on physical attractiveness than women do, especially
prediction is consistent, first of all, with Bull and Rumsey's when romantic attraction is considered, ft is possible to argue
(1988) survey of existing research. It is also congruent with gen- that physical attractiveness could be a more powerful dimen-
eral models describing how people combine items of informa- sion in men's than women's implicit theories of personality.
tion into an overall judgment. Specifically, averaging models However, the research we review on the physical attractiveness
(see N. H. Anderson, 1974,1981) postulate that the weight or stereotype was not carried out in the context of romantic rela-
importance of an item of information as a determinant of a tionships. Moreover, the question addressed in research on the
judgment becomes smaller as a perceiver takes more items of physical attractiveness stereotype pertains to the meaning in-
information into account. Such combinatorial models predict ferred from attractiveness cues, not the importance of attractive-
that the impact of physical attractiveness on judgments of per- ness. Men and women may differ in the importance they ac-
sonality lessens as more other information is received. cord to attractiveness even though they infer similar meaning
In analyzing the probable effect of giving subjects additional (e.g., social competence) from attractiveness cues. Particularly
information about the target, we must also consider the infor- in view of evidence that the content of gender stereotypes
mativeness or diagnosticity of physical attractiveness compared differs little for male and female subjects (e.g., Deaux & Lewis,
with other information that subjects may receive about targets. 1983,1984; Eagly & Steffen, 1984), we suspect that the physical
In experiments on the physical attractiveness stereotype, sub- attractiveness stereotype is widely shared in American culture
jects sometimes receive information about targets' personal his- and is therefore little affected by variation in subject character-
tory and personality, and, in videotaped or live presentations of istics, including subject sex. To the extent that some studies on
targets, subjects receive extensive information about targets' the physical attractiveness stereotype used male subjects and
personal style. This other information may very commonly be
more diagnostic than physical attractiveness in relation to the 2
One reason we do not predict a negative relation between attractive-
traits and behaviors that subjects were asked to predict. This
ness and concern for others is that the findings cited by some narrative
argument is analogous to one made in relation to gender stereo-
reviewers to provide evidence for the dark side of the physical attractive-
types, namely, that information other than sex may be more ness stereotype may not be representative of the total domain of find-
strongly linked than sex to the various dimensions of personal- ings on egotism and vanity. In addition, our concern for others class of
ity that comprise perceivers' implicit theories. When perceivers attributes is somewhat broader than egotism and vanity because ft
receive such information, their inferences from sex to personal- includes the positive facets of concern for others and selflessness.
114 EAGLY, ASHMORE, MAKHIJANI, AND LONGO

others used female subjects, our meta-analytic method allows tive (i.e.. one pole could be identified as more positive than the other).
us to approach this issue empirically by examining subjects' sex Consistent with our focus on partitioning evaluative meaning into its
as a predictor of their inferences about targets' personalities. components, we excluded studies if their dependent measures were not
clearly evaluative. For example, we eliminated studies that examined
only ratings of masculinity and femininity because they concern a
Method different hypothesis, what is beautiful is sex-typed (e.g. Cash & Janda,
1984), rather than what is beautiful is good. Similarly, we did not con-
Sample of Studies sider ascriptions of attitudes or group membership to targets (e.g, be-
ing a "feminist"; Jacobson & Koch, 1978) or causal attributions for
Computer-based information searches were conducted using the fol- targets' success and failure (e.g.. Turkat & Dawson, 1976). We also ex-
lowing keywords: physical attractiveness, unattractive, beautiful, ugly, cluded studies if their dependent measures assessed subjects' personal
ugliness, facial feature, and physical appearance. These keywords were reactions to targets (eg., ratings of whether the subject would like to
searched in the following data bases: PsycINFO (Psychological Ab- have the target as a date; Touhey, 1979) rather than the ascription of
stract^, 1967 to 1987; ERIC (Educational Resources Information evaluative characteristics to targets. Because the meta-analysis did not
Center), 1966 to 1987; Dissertation Abstracts Online, 1861 to 1987; ABI/
concern the treatment of attractive and unattractive targets, we also
INFORM (a worldwide business and management data base), 1971 to
excluded studies using only behavioral measures (e.g., shocking a tar-
1987; Sociological Abstracts, 1963 to 1987; Mental Health Abstracts,
get; Miller, Gillen, Schenker, & Radlove, 1974). In addition, we elimi-
1969 to 1987; MEDLINE (Medical Subject Headings), 1966 to 1987;and
nated the following types of dependent variables reported in studies
AARP (American Association of Retired Persons), 1985 to 1987. The
accepted for the sample: (a) ratings of targets' productions (eg., essays;
Social SciSearch data base was also searched to locate articles that
Bull & Stevens, 1979), (b) ratings of targets' effectiveness in a specific
cited Dion and associates (1972) or Dermer and Thiel (1975). Each
occupational role (eg., counselor; Cash & Kehr, 1978), and (c) ratings of
search ended with the latest information available in the summer of
targets' behavior (e.g., audibility of voice; Jackman, 1979). These elimi-
1987.
nations reflect our interest in the personality stereotype rather than
We also consulted Cash's (1981) bibliography, which contains almost
perceptions of people's products or role enactments. For studies report-
500 abstracts obtained through computer-assisted searches using the
ing more than one dependent variable, the meta-analysis included only
keyword physical attractiveness. Finally, we searched through the refer-
those measures that met these criteria for allowable dependent mea-
ence lists of all located studies and carefully read numerous review
sures (eg., behavioral measures were excluded). .
articles (e.g., BerscheidA Walster, 1974), books (eg., Patzer, 1985), and
4. Consistent with our focus on the physical attractiveness stereo-
chapters of books (e.g., Adams, 1982).
type, we included studies only if, following the precedent of Dion and
Selecting studies that focus on physical attractiveness stereotype.
co-workers' (1972) seminal experiment, subjects judged personality
These search procedures yielded over 600 possibly relevant reports.
traits and life outcomes of target persons with whom they had no prior
Given our focus on the physical attractiveness stereotype (and not on
role relationship. Because the review is not concerned with the effects
the treatment of attractive and unattractive people or their actual char-
of specialized role relationships on the impact of physical attractive-
acteristics), we initially pared down the sample to include only studies
ness, we excluded studies in which subjects and targets had an ongoing
in which subjects rated attractive and unattractive targets on evaluative
role relationship, for example, studies of students' ratings of their
attributes. The resulting sample was reduced further to include in the
final sample only studies that assessed the beauty-is-good stereotype in teachers (e.g., O'Reilly, 1987) or of therapists' ratings of their patients
situations unconstrained by particular role relationships (eg., teacher- (eg., Nordholm, 1980). We also excluded studies in which subjects role-
played a relationship with one or more targets, for example, studies in
student). To select those studies that assessed the attractiveness stereo-
type in this relatively pure form, we implemented a number of addi- which subjects were instructed to rate a counselor from the perspective
tional exclusion rules: of a client (e.g., Cash & Kehr, 1978). In addition, studies were deleted if
1. Studies were included in the sample if the general physical attrac- the target persons were described as paired by means of a role relation-
tiveness of target persons was one of its independent variables. Studies ship (eg., Hartnett & Secord's, 1983, targets were described as married
were excluded if they instead examined the impact of specific physical couples).
characteristics of target persons, such as height, weight, body type, or 5. Several other criteria excluded studies examining specialized
disfiguring facial characteristics (e.g., Dibiase & Hjelle, 1968). types of target persons. Although studies presenting targets in specific
2. Studies were included only if physical attractiveness was defined job categories (eg., counselor) were included as long as the subjects
independently of the subjects who rated the targets' characteristics. were not in an actual or hypothetical role relationship with these tar-
Attractiveness was in fact manipulated in almost all these studies. In a gets, studies were eliminated if the targets were mentally ill (eg., men-
few of the included studies, high- and low-attractiveness targets were tal hospital patients in the Martin, Friedmeyer, & Moore, 1977, study),
identified only after the subjects had participated, by attractiveness victimized (eg., rape victims in the Jacobson & Popovich, 1983, study),
ratings that were independent of subjects' own perceptions of targets' or specialized in certain other ways (eg., described as having had pre-
physical attractiveness (e.g., Barnes & Rosemhal. 1985). We excluded marital sexual intercourse in the Hocking, Walker, & Fink, 1982,
studies in which the attractiveness stereotype was defined only in study). Studies with targets of these types were excluded because physi-
terms of subjects' own perceptions of targets* physical attractiveness; cal attractiveness would probably have specialized implications in
that is, the stereotype was assessed by correlations between subjects' these contexts (e.g, it would not imply good social skills in mentally ill
own ratings of targets' physical attractiveness and their ratings of these targets; it might have atypical moral and sexual implications for rape
targets on traits and life outcomes (e.g., P. A. Schneider, Conger, & victims or unmarried persons described as sexually active).
Firth. 1986). Such studies are causally ambiguous and could illustrate 6. To confine our review to the presumably fully developed stereo-
the "what is good is beautiful" principle (Gross & Crofton, 1977) if type among late adolescents and adults, studies were included only if
subjects inferred good looks from positive personality characteristics. the subjects (and targets) were over the age of 14 years. The developmen-
3. Studies were included only if one or more of their dependent tal aspects of physical attractiveness stereotyping could well be the
variables was a rating of stimulus persons' personality traits (e.g., object of a separate meta-analysis.
warmth, intelligence, trustworthiness) or life outcomes (eg., career suc- 7. Finally, numerous studies were excluded because they lacked suf-
cess, marital success) on a dimension that could be considered evalua- ficient information for the computation of at least one effect size (eg.,
REVIEW OF PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS STEREOTYPE 115

R. Anderson &Nida, 1978). Such reports either lacked statistical detail difference was positive when attractive targets were rated more favor-
or presented statistical information in a form that does not allow the ably than unattractive targets and negative when unattractive targets
computation of effect sizes (e.g., a multivariate analysis of variance were rated more favorably. The computation of g was based on (a)
[MANOVA]; a regression with physical attractiveness included along means and standard deviations or error terms for 446 effect sizes, (b) Fs
with other predictors). and K for 110 effect sizes, (c) correlations or chi-squares for 26, and (d)
In summary, our selection criteria narrowed our sample to studies level p values (e.g, p < .05) for 14. Thus, 596 effect sizes were computed.
that examined the effects that targets' general physical attractiveness To reduce computational error, these effect sizes were calculated
has on perceivers' inferences about evaluative personal attributes in independently by Alice H. Eagly and Mona G. Makhijani. When the
settings that eliminate specialized roles and personal relationships data for a study were reported separately for male and female targets,
linking perceivers and targets. The resulting sample (see Appendix) effect sizes were calculated separately for each sex. These additional
consisted of 69 documents reporting 76 studies.3 reports, separated by sex of target, supplemented the reports combined
over sex of target, which were used in most analyses.
Variables Coded From Each Study The 596 effect sizes that we computed reported data on 104 different
individual rating scales as well as on 100 different composite measures
The following information was coded from each report: (a) date of that authors of the studies had computed by aggregating two or more
publication; (b) number of observations;' (c) number of judgments ag- ratings. To facilitate the analysis of these extremely diverse measures,
gregated into each observation underlying the study-level effect sizes;5 we aggregated them in various ways before analyzing them. To provide
(d) publication form (journal article, dissertation or unpublished docu- a single measure for each study, thus creating effect sizes that satisfy the
ment); (e) source of subject population (college undergraduates, other); independence assumption of meta-analytic statistics (Hedges & Olkin,
(f) nationality of subject population (American, Canadian); (g) sex of 1985), we computed a single effect size for each study, combining all
subjects (male, female, male and female, unknown); (h) instructional separately calculated effect sizes. This study-level effect size was calcu-
set (accuracy, i.e., of subject's judgment in relation to a presumed crite- lated using Rosenthal and Rubin's (1986) suggested formula and assum-
rion [see Dion et al., 1972], impression formation, other, none or un-
clear); (i) type of attractiveness variation (attractive and unattractive
3
targets selected, changed appearance of same target, other);' (j) evi- A list of the studies excluded by these criteria is available from
dence that attractiveness varied as intended (preselection judgments, either Alice H. Eagly or Richard D. Ashmore.
4
postexperimental ratings, both, none); (k) control for confounds be- The number of observations is the n taken into account by the
tween attractiveness and other attributes of targets (multiple targets at statistical analysis for the effects of physical attractiveness.
5
each attractiveness level, changed appearance of same target, other Study-level effect sizes were aggregated across all of the dependent
controls, no such control or unknown); (1) within versus between varia- variables (see Computation and Analysis of Effect Sizes). For example,
tion of attractiveness (within, between); (m) stimulus modality (photo- each observation might represent a subject's ratings of 4 target persons
graph, videotape, live, written description, other or more than one); (n) on 10 items, for a total of 40 judgments. To the extent that measures
part of target's body viewed (face, head and shoulders, full body, un- were based on multiple judgments, they should yield more reliable
clear, mixed or irrelevant); (o) stimuli in color (yes, no, unknown, estimates of the physical attractiveness stereotype, in the manner that
mixed or irrelevant); (p) source of stimuli (college yearbook, researcher the number of items in a test relates to the reliability of the total test
took own photographs or videotapes, other, unknown, irrelevant or (e.g, Ghiselli, 1964).
6
more than one); (q) basis of stimuli selection (means and variances of When a study's design included more than two levels of attractive-
judges' ratings, means of judges' ratings, consensual meaning of ness, we compared only the highest and lowest levels, if the report was
words, unknown or intuitive, mixed or irrelevant); (r) sex of targets sufficient to yield this comparison. This rule maximized comparabil-
(male, female, male and female); (s) race of targets (whites only, other ity of effect sizes across the studies because the typical study presented
exclusion, not restricted, unknown, mixed or irrelevant); (t) restric- targets preselected to be extremely attractive and extremely unattrac-
tions on target's appearance (eyeglasses excluded, other exclusion, tive. The following variations were coded as "other" but included as
more than one exclusion, no or unknown exclusion or appearance ma- separate categories in preliminary analyses: high versus medium (when
nipulated, mixed or irrelevant); (u) presence of individuating informa- only these conditions were present in experiment), changed verbal de-
tion along with attractiveness information (present, absent); (v) inde- scription, median split on attractiveness, and attractiveness treated as a
pendent variables other than physical attractiveness in design, regard- continuous variable.
7
less of whether they provided individuating information (yes, no); and When physical attractiveness was a between-subjects variable, this
(w) unusual features of statistical analysis (yes, no). standard deviation was computed separately within the high- and low-
The categories we listed for some of these variables were simplified attractiveness conditions and pooled. When physical attractiveness
from an initially more detailed set because (a) some of these more was a within-subjects variable, the standard deviation was the standard
detailed categories had few or no entries and (b) prediction of the effect deviation of the differences between the paired observations. These
sizes was not improved by the more detailed categories. The variables standard deviations were estimated, whenever possible, only from the
for which the initial coding was more detailed are the following: publi- portion of each study's data entering into the effect size.
cation form, instructional set, type of attractiveness variation, control Some statisticians have recommended that, when estimating stan-
for confounds, and presence of individuating information. dard deviations from the mean square error of an ANOVA, meta-ana-
These variables were independently coded by Alice H. Eagly and lysts reconstitute this error term by adding into the sum of squares
Mona G. Makhijani, with a median agreement of 95% (estimated * error all other sums of squares except that for the independent variable
[kappa] = .90); "other independent variables in design" yielded the of interest (see Glass, McGaw, & Smith, 1981; Hedges & Becker, 1986).
lowest agreement: 85% If = .39). Disagreements were resolved by dis- This strategy was not chosen for this meta-analysis because physical
attractiveness was crossed with manipulated variables that in some
experiments were quite powerful. Consequently, adding sums of
Computation and Analysis of Effect Sizes squares for manipulated variables to the sum of squares error would
have had differing impact on these error terms across the studies. In
The effect size calculated is g, the difference between the means of addition, statistical information was typically not extensive enough to
two groups, divided by the relevant standard deviation.7 The sign of the allow us to reconstitute the error terms.
116 EAGLY, ASHMORE, MAKHIJANI, AND LONGO

ing that the correlation between measures was .48. This correlation was techniques for calculating categorical models provide a between-
estimated by averaging the correlations between dependent variables classes effect (analogous to a main effect in an ANOVA) and a test of the
(a) reported in the studies or (b) derived from coefficient alphas homogeneity of the effect sizes within each class. The between-classes
reported for multiple-kern dependent variables used in the meta- effect is estimated by QB, which has an approximate chi-square distri-
analysis. bution with p—1 degrees of freedom, where pis the number of classes.
Categorization of dependent variables. To enable us to test our hy- The homogeneity of the effect sizes within each class is estimated by
pothesis that physical attractiveness has more impact on some types of <2w, which has an approximate chi-square distribution with m - \
evaluative beliefs than others, we also classified each study's variables degrees of freedom, where m is the number of effect sizes in the class.
into the following six categories: (a) social competence, including inter- The tables reporting tests of categorical models also include the mean
personal skills and traits (sociable, fun loving) and successful outcomes weighted effect size for each class, calculated with each effect size
of such skills (popular, likable); (b) intellectual competence, including weighted by the reciprocal of its variance, and an indication of whether
intellectual ability (intelligent, skillful), rational mental style (rational, this mean differed significantly from the 0.00 value that indicates ex-
scientific), high task motivation (ambitious, hard working), and suc- actly no effect of physical attractiveness.
cessful outcomes of task endeavors (receiving good grades, achieving The continuous models are least squares simple linear regressions,
career success); (c) concern for others, including social sensitivity (sensi- calculated with each effect size weighted by the reciprocal of its vari-
tive, empathic), nurturance (compassionate, generous), and lack of ego- ance. Each such model yields a test of the significance of a predictor as
tism (modest, not egotistic); (d) integrity, including honesty (trust- well as a test of model specification, which evaluates whether signifi-
worthy, honest) and respect for norms (faithfulness to spouse); (e) ad- cant systematic variation remains unexplained in the regression model
justment, including good mental health (normal, well adjusted), (Hedges, 1982b; Hedges & Olkin, 1985). The sum of squares error sta-
happiness (satisfied, happy), high self-esteem (confident, positive self- tistic, QEr which provides this test of model specification, has an ap-
regard), and well-being (maturity, good health); and (f) potency includ- proximate chi-square distribution with k - p -1 degrees of freedom,
ing power (strong, self-assertive) and dominance (dominant, acting as where k is the number of effect sizes and p is the number of predictors
leader). A composite measure reported in a study was placed into one (not including the intercept). If correctly specified models are not
of these categories only if 60% or more of its component ratings fit into achieved when implementing continuous models (or homogeneity is
the category. not achieved within the classes when implementing categorical mod-
els), the results of these analyses cannot be interpreted as confidently as
In addition, some measures were placed into one of two other catego-
they would otherwise be.
ries. The first of these categories is general evaluation, which was cho-
As a supplementary analysis, we attained homogeneity by identify-
sen for single-item measures whose evaluative meaning was completely
ing outliers among the effect sizes and sequentially removing those that
nonspecific (e.g., good) and for composite measures whose component
reduced the homogeneity statistic by the largest amount (see Hedges &
scales fit two or more of our specific categories and failed to attain the
Olkin, 1985). Using such a procedure, Hedges (1987) found for several
criterion of at least 60% fitting any one category. The second of these
meta-analyses on psychological topics that the removal of up to 20% of
additional categories is other, which was chosen for measures that nei-
the outliers in a group of heterogeneous effect sizes usually resulted in a
ther fit into any of the six specific categories nor qualified as measures
high degree of homogeneity. Inspection of the percentage of effect sizes
of general evaluation.
removed to attain homogeneity allows one to determine whether the
The measures from a study were thus placed into one or more of
effect sizes are homogeneous aside from the presence of relatively few
these eight categories. This classification was accomplished by the four
aberrant values. The mean attained after removal of such outliers may
authors' independent categorizations of the measures; three of the four
better represent the distribution of effect sizes than would the mean
authors placed 80% of the single-item dependent variables and 72% of
based on all of the effect sizes.
composite measures in the same category. Disagreements were re-
solved by discussion between Richard D. Ashmore and Laura C.
Longo. The 596 effect sizes calculated from the studies' reported find- Results
ings were aggregated into category-level effect sizes for each study for as
many of the eight categories as the study represented. These aggrega- Characteristics of the Studies
tions were calculated under the assumption that the correlation be-
Before considering the findings reported in research on the
tween measures placed in the same category was .48. Each study thus
physical attractiveness stereotype, we examined the character-
yielded, in addition to a study-level effect size, one or more of these
istics of the studies from which conclusions about this research
eight category-level effect sizes.
will be drawn. Table 1 shows many of these study characteris-
Analysis of effect sizes. The gs were converted to ds by correcting
them for bias (i.e., g's overestimate of the population effect size, which tics.
occurs especially for small samples; see Hedges, 1981; Hedges & Olkin, As shown by the central tendencies of the characteristics
1985). Then the study outcomes were combined by averaging thet/s. To listed in Table 1, studies (a) were published relatively recently, (b)
determine whether each set of ds shared a common effect size (i.e., was were published as journal articles, (c) involved a moderate num-
consistent across the studies), we calculated a homogeneity statistic, Q, ber of observations, and (d) aggregated a moderate number of
which has an approximate chi-square distribution with k — 1 degrees of judgments into each observation. The studies used subjects
freedom, where k is the number of effect sizes (Hedges, 1981; Hedges & who were college undergraduates, Americans, and both male
Olkin, 1985). and female. In addition, the studies generally (a) established an
In the absence of homogeneity, we accounted for variability in hetero- impression-formation instructional set; (b) compared percep-
geneous effect sizes by relating them to the attributes of the studies. To
tions of targets selected to be attractive and unattractive; (c)
determine the relation between these study characteristics and the
presented both preselection and postexperimental evidence
magnitude of the effect sizes, both categorical and continuous models
that attractiveness varied as intended; (d) controlled confounds
were tested (Hedges, 1982a, 1982b; Hedges & Olkin, 1985). Categorical
models, which are analogous to ANOVAs, may show that heteroge- by means of multiple targets at each attractiveness level, within
neous effect sizes are homogeneous within the subgroups established each level of target sex; (e) varied attractiveness on a between-
by dividing studies into classes based on study characteristics. The subjects basis; (f) presented targets by means of photographs or
REVIEW OF PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS STEREOTYPE 117

Table 1
Summary of Study Characteristics

Variable and class Value Variable and class Value

Median publication year 1980 Stimulus modality


Photograph or slide 60
Publication form Videotape 8
Journal article 53 Live 5
Dissertation or unpublished document 23 Written description I
Other or more than one 2
Median number of observations 108
Median number of judgments aggregated into each Part of target's body viewed
observation 12 Face 11
Head and shoulders 37
Source of subject population Full body 15
College undergraduates 70 Unclear 11
Other 6 Mixed or irrelevant 2

Nationality of subjects Stimuli in color


American 70 Yes 19
Canadian 6 No 18
Unknown 37
Sex of subjects Mixed or irrelevant 2
Male 9
Female 8 Source of stimuli
Male and female 58 College yearbook 22
Unknown 1 Researcher took own photographs or videotapes 21
Other 6
Instructional set Unknown 20
Accuracy 19 Irrelevant or more than one 7
Impression formation 38
Other 9 Basis of stimuli selection
None or unclear 10 Means and variances of judges' ratings 35
Means of judges' ratings 28
Type of attractiveness variation Consensual meaning of words 1
Attractive and unattractive targets selected 57 Unknown or intuitive 11
Changed appearance of same target 10 Mixed or irrelevant 1
Other 9
Sex of targets
Evidence that attractiveness varied as intended Male 8
Preselection judgments 19 Female 30
Postexperimental ratings 8 Male and female 38
Both 46
None 3 Race of targets
Whites only 21
Control for confounds Other exclusion 1
Multiple targets 52 Not restricted 1
Changed appearance of same target 9 Unknown 51
Other controls' 3 Mixed or irrelevant 2
No such control or unknown 12
Restrictions on target's appearance
Within versus between variation of attractiveness Eyeglasses excluded 2
Within 18 Other exclusion 12
Between 58 More than one exclusion 13
No or unknown exclusion or appearance changed 47
Mixed or irrelevant 2

Presence of individuating information


Present 48
Absent 28

Note. For categorical variables, numbers in tables represent frequencies of reports in each class.
* Includes changed written description and use of both multiple targets and changed appearance in same study

slides; (g) used head-and-shoulders poses of targets; (h) did not source of stimuli; (j) selected stimuli based on means and vari-
mention whether stimuli were presented in color or black and ances of judges' ratings or on means only; (k) used female tar-
white; (i) used college yearbook photographs, or photographs or gets only or both male and female targets; (1) did not report on
videotapes taken by the researcher, or did not mention the the race of the targets; (m) did not report on or had no restric-
118 EAGLY, ASHMORE, MAKHIJANI, AND LONGO

tions on targets' appearance or used targets whose appearance women. This stereotype was inconsistent in magnitude across
was changed; and (n) presented individuating information the studies and somewhat smaller once outlying effect sizes
along with the attractiveness information. were deleted.

Summary of Study-Level Effect Sizes Physical Attractiveness Stereotype and Type of Attributes
Ascribed to Targets
The summary of the study-level effect sizes given in Table 2
allows one to determine whether more favorable characteristics The categorical model in Table 3 tests our prediction that the
were ascribed to attractive individuals than to unattractive indi- strength of the physical attractiveness stereotype depends on
viduals based on the available reports. An overall difference in the particular dimension of meaning that was assessed. This
the stereotypic direction is shown by a positive mean effect size model produced a highly significant between-classes effect. As
that differed significantly from the 0.00 value that indicates expected, the largest mean effect size was produced for ratings
exactly no difference (i*, by a confidence interval that did not of social competence and the smallest effect sizes for concern
include 0.00). As expected, evaluations were more favorable for for others and integrity. Ratings on the remaining three specific
attractive targets than for unattractive targets, as shown by the dimensions of meaning (intellectual competence, adjustment,
weighted mean of the effect sizes8 as well as by the unweighted potency) produced effect sizes that were in between these
mean and the median. In addition, a large proportion (.91) of classes of effect sizes. Consistent with this pattern, the general
these study-level effect sizes favored the attractive targets over evaluation category produced effect sizes in the middle range of
the unattractive targets. This proportion differed significantly the means for the six specific types of meaning, as did the
from .50, the proportion expected under the null hypothesis category labeled other, which contained an extremely diverse
(/K.001). set of ratings that did not fit into the six specific categories that
As also shown in Table 2, the effect sizes were not homoge- we established. We evaluated the significance of the differences
neous, and homogeneity was not attained until a relatively large between these classes of effect sizes by means of post hoc com-
proportion of the effect sizes C33) was removed. The removal of parisons, which are represented by the subscripts attached to
outliers produced a somewhat smaller weighted mean that also the mean effect sizes given in Table 3 (see Hedges & Becker,
differed significantly from 0.00. 1986; Hedges & Olkin, 1985).'
The various indexes of the central tendency of the effect sizes
thus substantiate the existence of an attractiveness stereotype, Relations Between Other Study Attributes and the
as documented by the ascription of more favorable characteris-
Physical Attractiveness Stereotype
tics to attractive men and women than to unattractive men and
The relations between other study attributes and the physical
attractiveness stereotype were evaluated on the study-level ef-
fect sizes. The categorical models that produced significant be-
Table 2
tween-classes effects are given in Table 4. The first model indi-
Summary of Study-Level Effect Sizes
cates a significant between-classes effect for publication form;
Criterion Value journal articles produced a stronger stereotype than disserta-
tions and unpublished articles. The second model shows a sig-
Including outliers nificant effect for instructional set; the accuracy set yielded a
« 76 weaker stereotype. Given Dion's (1981,1986) logic that the accu-
Mean weighted effect size (dj- 0.58 racy set would reduce stereotyping and encourage careful think-
95%CIforrf + 0.54/0.62 ing about targets' personal characteristics, a priori compari-
Homogeneity (Q) of effect sizes comprising rf+" 450.62*
sons between the accuracy class of effect sizes and the three
Mean effect size (mean d) 0.61
95% CI for mean d 0.50/0.72 other classes seem appropriate. By such comparisons, the
Median effect size 0.57 stereotype was significantly weaker with the accuracy set than
Differences favoring attractive targets" 69/76 (.91) each of the other three sets (ps < .05).
Although the physical attractiveness stereotype related to the
Excluding outliers
type of method researchers used to vary attractiveness, post
n 51 hoc contrasts show only that the quite diverse category of other
n removed outliers 25 (.33) manipulations produced a stronger stereotype than the more
Mean weighted effect size (d+) 0.49
typical manipulation that compared stimuli preselected to be
95% CI for d+ 0.44/0.54
Homogeneity (Q) of effect sizes comprising rf+ 69.11 high or low in attractiveness ( p < .001). The type of control that
researchers established for confounds of the physical attractive-
Note. Effect sizes are positive for more favorable evaluation of attrac-
tive than unattractive targets and negative for more favorable evalua-
tion of unattractive targets. CI = confidence interval.
* The weighted means were computed by weighting each effect size
" Effect sizes were weighted by the reciprocal of the variance. b Signi-
by the reciprocal of its variance (Hedges & Olkin, 1985), a procedure
ficance indicates rejection of the hypothesis of homogeneity. ° Fre-
quencies are the number of study-level effect sizes favoring the more that gives more weight to effect sizes that are more reliably estimated.
9
attractive targets divided by the total number of effect sizes. The pro- Although our dimensions differed somewhat from those used by
portion appears in parentheses. Feingold (1990b), the ordering of the dimensions was substantially simi-
lar.
REVIEW OF PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS STEREOTYPE 119

Table3
Categorical Model for Type of Attributes Ascribed to Targets

95% CI for <k


Between-classes Mean weighted Homogeneity within
Attribute type effect (G>) n efiect size (d,+) Lower Upper each class (G^

279.97*
Social competence 35 0.68. 0.62 0.73 213.60*
Adjustment 18 0.52.J 0.45 0.60 239.59*
Potency 14 0.49^ 0.39 0.58 36.67*
Intellectual competence 38 0.46t 0.41 0.51 178.48*
Integrity 11 0.13, 0.03 0.23 80.84*
Concern for others 22 0.01, -0.06 0.09 284.65*
General evaluation 39 0.57^ 0.52 0.62 414.59*
Other 13 0.54^ 0.44 0.64 154.00*

Note. Effect sizes are positive for more favorable evaluation of attractive than unattractive targets and negative for more favorable evaluation of
unattractive targets. Differences between means that do not have a subscript in common are significant at the .05 level or beyond. CI = confidence
interval.
* Significance indicates rejection of the hypothesis of homogeneity.
*p<.001.

ness variable also produced a significant between-classes effect. The number of judgments aggregated into each observation
Yet, using post hoc contrasts, the only significant comparison was the only continuous variable that produced a significant
among the four classes showed that the stereotype was stronger model. Thus, the physical attractiveness stereotype was
when no control was used or when the nature of any controls stronger when a greater number of judgments (i£., items) were
was unknown than it was when the typical multiple-target tech- aggregated into each of the observations underlying the study-
nique was used (p < .05). In addition, consistent with the level effect sizes (b = 0.0017, tf = .11, p < .025; QE[74] =
greater control of extraneous variables and the more precise 445.38, p < .001). This analysis raises the question of whether
error term in within-subjects designs, within-subjects varia- the various dimensions of evaluative meaning shown in Table 3
tions of attractiveness produced a stronger stereotype than be- differed with respect to the number of judgments aggregated
tween-subjects variations. into each observation. Although some differences were ob-
As also shown in Table 4, several attributes of the target per- served, they showed no relation to the strength of the evaluative
sons affected the strength of the physical attractiveness stereo- stereotype (Le, to the means shown in Table 3).
type. The use of color (vs. black and white) stimuli related signifi-
cantly to the effect sizes. Post hoc comparisons showed that
smaller efiect sizes were produced by stimuli that were known Discussion
to be black and white than by stimuli that were in color (p <
Strength of the Physical Attractiveness Stereotype
.005) or for which color was unknown (p < .001). The next
model given in Table 4 failed to confirm our hypothesis that the Although the consensus among social psychologists has been
physical attractiveness stereotype is stronger for female targets that the beauty-is-good stereotype is extremely strong and ro-
than for male targets. This hypothesis was tested on the 98 bust, our implicit personality theory framework led us to ex-
effect sizes produced when studies reporting separate data for pect that the tendency for people to ascribe more favorable
male and female targets were represented by these separate personal attributes to attractive targets than to unattractive tar-
effect sizes and the other studies were represented by study-level gets would be somewhat modest in size and quite variable
effect sizes. Although the variable of target sex produced a sig- across studies. Relevant to the overall magnitude of the physical
nificant categorical model, the attractiveness stereotype was attractiveness stereotype are the several measures of efiect size
stronger for reports combined over male and female targets (i.e, central tendency reported in Table 2. These measures varied
reports that could not be partitioned by sex of target) than for from a high of 0.61 for the unweighted mean to a low of 0.49 for
reports on either male targets (p < .01) or female targets (p < the weighted mean with outliers excluded. These figures indi-
.001). Contrary to hypothesis, the stereotype did not differ for cate approximately one-half standard deviation in the direction
male and female targets. Moreover, we calculated the sex-of- of more positive personality traits and life outcomes ascribed to
target model separately within each of the dimensions shown in attractive compared with unattractive targets.
Table 3 and obtained no evidence of a stronger stereotype for Methodological features, both of our meta-analysis and of
female than male targets within any of these dimensions. Also, the research paradigms of the studies we reviewed, should be
sex of subjects did not produce a significant model. Finally, taken into account in interpreting the magnitude of the effect
consistent with our hypothesis that individuating information sizes we obtained. Among the important features of our meta-
would attenuate inferences made on the basis of physical attrac- analytic methods are the rules we followed for including studies
tiveness, the stereotype was weaker when such information was in the present sample. In particular, studies were included only
present rather than absent. if we could compute at least one effect size, and among the
120 EAGLY, ASHMORE, MAKHIJANI, AND LONGO

Table 4
Categorical Models on Study-Level Effect Sizes

95% CI for rf,+


Between-classes Mean weighted Homogeneity within
Variable and class effect (Q,) « effect size (dH) Lower Upper each class (Gw,)'

Publication form 23.98***


Journal article 53 0.64 0.60 0.68 258.01***
Dissertation or unpublished document 23 0.45 0.39 0.51 168.64***
Instructional set 9.20*
Accuracy 19 0.50 0.43 0.56 156.86***
Impression formation 38 0.60 0.54 0.65 227.00***
Other 9 0.62 0.47 0.77 21.90**
None or unclear 10 0.64 0.57 0.71 35.36***
Type of attractiveness variation 16.03***
Attractive and unattractive targets selected 57 0.54 0.50 0.58 60.30***
Changed appearance of same target 10 0.66 0.52 0.81 23.14**
Other 9 0.73 0.64 0.82 51.16***
Control for confounds 10.18**
Multiple targets 52 0.55 0.51 0.59 382.92***
Changed appearance of same target 9 0.66 0.51 0.81 23.14**
Other controls 3 0.54 0.35 0.73 4.36
No control or unknown 12 0.69 0.61 0.78 30.00**
Within versus between variation of attractiveness 13.03***
Within 18 0.66 0.60 0.72 126.87***
Between 58 0.53 0.48 0.57 310.72***

Stimuli in color" 23.15***


Yes 19 0.63 0.55 0.72 112.12***
No 18 0.42 0.35 0.50 137.89***
Unknown 37 0.63 0.58 0.68 172.99***

Sex of targets 24.23***


Male 30 0.57 0.51 0.64 154.49***
Female 52 0.50 0.45 0.56 280.24***
Both 16 0.72 0.65 0.79 52.80***

Presence of individuating information 36.63***


Present 48 0.46 0.41 0.51 172.96***
Absent 28 0.68 0.63 0.73 241.03***

Note. Effect sizes are positive for more favorable evaluation of attractive than unattractive targets and negative for more favorable evaluation of
unattractive targets. Cl = confidence interval.
"Significance indicates rejection of the hypothesis of homogeneity. b The category "mixed or irrelevant*1 (n = 2) was omitted from this analysis.
*p<.05. **p<.01. ***p<.001.

included studies, only effect sizes that we could compute en- considered an influence, our inclusion of all available unpub-
tered into our aggregations. Although some meta-analysts have lished findings diluted the impact of this bias on our aggregated
assigned the value of 0.00 to unknown effect sizes, we were effect sizes. Moreover, the mean effect size in the unpublished
reluctant to compromise the accuracy of our data in this way, studies remained substantial (see Table 4).
especially in view of the fact that our computed effect sizes were One of the methodological features of physical attractiveness
then aggregated into study-level effect sizes. Nonetheless, our studies relevant to interpreting the magnitude of our effect sizes
procedure probably increased the magnitude of our average is the degree to which extraneous variables were controlled in
effect sizes because of the tendency for minimally reported find- typical experiments. Such control reduces the standard devia-
ings to be nonsignificant and generally relatively small. tions that serve as the denominator of the effect sizes. Com-
An additional consideration in evaluating meta-analytic pared with many other areas of research in social and personal-
methods is the extent to which the magnitude of the physical ity psychology (e.g., prosocial and antisocial behavior, group
attractiveness stereotype may have been exaggerated in our re- process), research on the physical attractiveness stereotype is
view because of a publication bias in favor of significant find- relatively controlled, because carefully selected target stimuli
ings (Greenwald, 1975; Lane & Dunlap, 1978). Indeed, in our are presented to subjects in a laboratory setting that usually
data the effect sizes were larger in published than unpublished precludes social interaction between the subjects and targets. In
research (see Table 4), a typical result in meta-analyses (see addition, because dependent variables that aggregate subjects'
Glass et al., 1981). Although publication bias must therefore be responses ordinarily strengthen effects (Abelson, 1985; Ajzen,
REVIEW OF PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS STEREOTYPE 121

1988), the fact that the observations analyzed in the studies we 20% or less of effect sizes that Hedges (1987) removed to attain
reviewed typically represented a moderate number of judg- homogeneity among studies on several diverse psychological
ments should also be taken into account (see Table 1). Thus, two hypotheses. Furthermore, other recent meta-analyses on social
important methodological features of this research literature— psychological topics have achieved homogeneity by removal of
the relatively high level of experimental control and the moder- smaller proportions of effect sizes than the 33% removed in the
ate amount of aggregation of responses—favored the produc- present review (see Eagly & Johnson, 1990; Johnson & Eagly,
tion of large effect sizes. 1989). Thus, the effect sizes produced by studies on the physical
Several additional considerations are relevant to evaluating attractiveness stereotype should be viewed as quite inconsistent
the magnitude of the average ds produced by our meta-analysis in magnitude.
(see Eagly, 1987). One set of guidelines for the d metric has been
suggested by Cohen (1977), who proposed that 0.20 can be
described as small, 0.50 as medium, and 0.80 as large. Cohen
Impact of Type of Attributes on the Physical
based these standards on his impressions of effect sizes in the
behavioral sciences and maintained that phenomena repre-
Attractiveness Stereotype
sented by medium effect sizes would ordinarily be noticed in
daily life. Cohen's characterization of medium effect sizes as Congruent with our hypotheses, which built on the excep-
discriminable is consistent with the transformation of our tions and qualifications noted in some narrative reviews (e.g.,
mean weighted effect size of 0.58 into the metric of Rosenthal Adams, 1982; Alley & Hildebrandt, 1988; Bassili, 1981; Bull &
and Rubin's (1982) binomial effect-size display. By this transfor- Rumsey, 1988; Cash & Janda, 1984; Dermer & Thiel, 1975;
mation, 64% of attractive people but only 36% of unattractive Dion, 1981, 1986), there was considerable variation in the
people were perceived as having above-average personalities. strength of the beauty-is-good effect from study to study and
This difference of 28% suggests a substantial tendency, despite from measure to measure within studies. As expected, some of
the fact that a d of 0.5 8 can be interpreted as signifying that only this variation was explained by the type of dependent variable
8% of the variability in subjects' inferences about targets' per- researchers had used. The fact that physical attractiveness had
sonalities was explained by their physical attractiveness. its strongest impact on social competence supports our conten-
Another basis for interpreting the average effect sizes for the tion that the core of the physical attractiveness stereotype is
physical attractiveness stereotype is to compare them with aver- sociability, popularity, and similar attributes. Other evaluative
age effect sizes typically produced by meta-analyses on other dimensions varied considerably in how closely they were psy-
hypotheses in social and personality psychology. Eagly's (1987) chologically connected to good looks versus bad looks. Physical
overview of such effect sizes suggested that they ranged from attractiveness had little impact on integrity and concern for
roughly 0.00 to 1.20. For example, mean </s for sex differences others; potency, adjustment, and intellectual competence
in social behavior ranged from a low of 0.13 for helping behav- showed intermediate impact.
ior to a high of 1.19 for the use of pause fillers in vocal behavior. In none of our categories of evaluative meaning were attrac-
Another such comparative standard is provided by Cooper and tive people perceived less favorably than unattractive people,
Findley's (1982) computation of a mean d of 0.92 for the find- although attractive people possessed no advantage in our con-
ings cited in a sample of social psychology textbooks.10 Because cern for others class. Because the argument that the attractive-
studies cited in textbooks are likely to provide the best demon- ness stereotype has a dark side (e.g. Cash & Janda, 1984;
strations of those phenomena that social psychologists believe Dermer & Thiel, 1975) suggests that attractive people are per-
are genuine, this figure no doubt considerably overestimates the ceived to be more likely than unattractive people to possess the
strength of typical findings for these phenomena.
negative attributes of vanity and selfishness, we performed sepa-
In summary, the magnitude of the physical attractiveness rate analyses to determine whether ratings of modesty versus
stereotype in our meta-analytic findings was enhanced by (a)
vanity (« = 6) or egotism versus selflessness (n = 6)—two compo-
our exclusion of studies for which we could not calculate an
nents of our concern for others class—would show significant
effect size, (b) an apparent publication bias in favor of positive
reversals of the usual tendency to ascribe more favorable quali-
findings, and (c) experimental designs featuring a high degree of
ties to beautiful people. Indeed, both means differed signifi-
experimental control and moderate aggregation of judgments.
cantly from 0.00 in the predicted negative direction: d+ — —0.67,
In view of these considerations as well as the numeric guide-
CI = -0.51/-0.84 for modesty versus vanity; d< = -0.25, CI =
lines commonly used for evaluating the magnitude of effect
—0.08/-0.41 for egotism versus selflessness. However, the
sizes, the beauty-is-good effect should be considered moderate
mean for egotism became positive when one highly negative
in magnitude but certainly not extremely strong.
outlying effect size was removed. Thus, we conclude only that
Finally, in relation to the robustness of the beauty-is-good
attractive people are perceived more negatively than unattrac-
stereotype, it is worth commenting on the consistency of the
tive people in one respect: They are thought to be more vain
findings across the studies. The direction of the effect proved
and less modest. The present results, then, serve to clarify the
quite consistent, as shown by the fact that 91% of the study-level
dark side of the physical attractiveness stereotype. It includes
effect sizes favored the attractive targets (see Table 2). Nonethe-
less, as shown by the large value of the Q statistic (see Table 2),
the findings were quite inconsistent in magnitude. Moreover, to
10
achieve a homogeneous set of findings, 33% of the study-level This mean effect size aggregates comparisons involving (tests, F
effect sizes had to be removed. This figure compares with the tests, and correlation coefficients.
122 EAGLY, ASHMORE, MAKHUANI, AND LONGO

vanity but perhaps not the apparently similar attribute of ego- subjects merely rate photographs of targets whom they do not
tism." expect to meet.
The heterogeneity of the attractiveness stereotype across di- A second, more radical interpretation is that our hypothesis
mensions of evaluative meaning has important implications for about the greater weight of attractiveness in judgments of
interpreting research in this area. These meta-analytic findings women is incorrect. If so, how can this conclusion be reconciled
raise questions about the common practice of aggregating sub- with research suggesting that physical attractiveness is more
jects' responses into composite indexes. To take the Social De- important to the female gender role than to the male gender
sirability Index used by Dion and her collaborators (Dion et al, role and to women's self-esteem and personal identity (see sum-
1972) as an example, we placed 7 of the 14 items in this compos- mary presented early in this article)? To resolve this apparent
ite in our social competence category, 3 in potency, 3 in concern paradox, we suggest framing the effects of physical attractive-
for others, and 1 in other. Because this composite emphasized ness in terms of two stages: (a) In a first stage, perceivers infer
social competence, its distribution of items across the dimen- from appearance cues that targets have certain personality
sions of personality resulted in a relatively strong stereotype, a characteristics; and (b) in a second stage, the implications of
much stronger stereotype than would have been obtained by these inferences unfold during social interaction. In the first
choosing, for example, a large number of items representing stage, physical attractiveness may cause perceivers to draw the
integrity and concern for others. Our meta-analytic results thus same conclusions about women's and menls personalities (iff., it
suggest that researchers should select items systematically by conveys the same information about social competence and
designing indexes that represent the content-specific dimen- other attributes). However, in the second stage, conclusions
sions of subjects' implicit theories of personality. Aggregate based on attractiveness may be more consequential for women
measures consisting of items that were selected through more than men. Specifically, as social interaction proceeds, the gains
arbitrary methods produce unreliable estimates of the strength in perceived social competence enjoyed by good-looking peo-
of the physical attractiveness stereotype and obscure the subtle- ple may have more important consequences for women be-
ties of people's stereotypic thinking about looks. cause the structuring of interaction by means of gender roles
assigns women more responsibility for the maintenance of posi-
tive and friendly relationships (see Eagly, 1987). Therefore,
Impact of Other Variables on the Physical women may work harder to be perceived as socially competent,
accounting for women's greater interest in their personal ap-
Attractiveness Stereotype
pearance and the greater centrality of looks in their self-
Individuating information. The implicit personality theory concepts. Also if marriage and family relationships are more
framework guiding the present analysis also suggested that the central to women than men (see Wood, Rhodes, & Whelan,
strength of the physical attractiveness stereotype would be af- 1989), the implications of looks for heterosexual attraction may
fected by information about the targets in addition to their be more important to women than men.
physical attractiveness. As predicted, the beauty-is-good effect Within- versus between-subjects manipulation of attractive-
was weaker when subjects received individuating information ness. The finding that the attractiveness stereotype was
along with attractiveness cues. This finding, which is consistent stronger for within-subjects manipulations than for between-
with the prior research we noted early in this article, suggests subjects manipulations of appearance is amenable to two possi-
that the evaluative implications of looks may be somewhat weak ble explanations. One interpretation is that the within-subjects
in many natural settings in which perceivers have extensive in- procedure provides a more precise error term because subjects
formation about other people in addition to their physical ap- served as their own controls (see also footnote 7). A second
pearance. Looks should thus be relatively less important in per- interpretation is that the context in which stimuli are perceived
ceptions of friends, acquaintances, family members, and co- in within-subjects experiments—namelji exposure to targets of
workers than in perceptions of strangers. widely differing levels of physical attractiveness—may induce a
Sex of targets. The prediction that the attractiveness stereo- perceptual contrast effect whereby attractive targets are seen as
type is stronger for female targets than for male targets was not more attractive and unattractive targets as less attractive than
supported.12 In seeking to explain why, we offer two quite dif- they would otherwise be. Such contrast effects are well docu-
ferent lines of reasoning. One possibility is that this hypothesis mented in research on judgments of physical attractiveness (see
may be valid under circumstances not present in the research Patzer, 1985, for a summary). The more extreme judgments of
paradigms represented in this meta-analysis. In particular, the attractiveness that result from such a contrast effect could in-
greater weight of attractiveness in perceivers' judgments of duce more extreme judgments of personality.
women may occur in the context of face-to-face interaction or, Color versus black-and-white stimuli. Another finding of our
more generaUy, in situations in which perceivers are likely to
interact with targets. If women take more interest in and respon-
sibility for the interpersonal aspects of daily experience (see
" Our conclusion about egotism is somewhat tentative because five
Eagly, 1987; Eagly & Wood, in press), physical attractiveness
of the six egotism effect sizes were based on composite measures of
may have more impact on judgments of women than men in
egotism that included some attributes in addition to egotism and its
natural settings where people engage in social interaction, and synonyms.
social competence therefore has important consequences. The 12
Yet Feingold (1990b) showed that physical attractiveness led to
greater importance of attractiveness to perceptions of women stronger inferences of sexual warmth for female targets than for male
may not be revealed in the typical stereotype study, in which targets.
REVIEW OF PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS STEREOTYPE 123

meta-analysis is that the beauty-is-good effect was more pro- Limitations of This Meta-Analysis
nounced for color stimuli than for black-and-white stimuli. In-
terpretation of this finding must remain tentative because, as a Our integration of research on the physical attractiveness ste-
result of the correlational nature of meta-analytic findings, the reotype addressed only a limited number of questions, and fu-
color versus black-and-white aspect of the stimuli was con- ture meta-analyses of this research literature could well con-
founded to some extent with other features of the experiments sider several other important issues. In particular, the beauty-is-
(e.g., the studies with color stimuli encompassed all of those good hypothesis might be compared with an ugly-is-bad
with face-to-face interaction). Nonetheless, if we assume that hypothesis (Hatfield & Sprecher, 1986; Patzer, 1985). Such a
color stimuli are more vivid than black-and-white photographs, meta-analysis would compare the differences in judgments of
this finding can be interpreted in terms of the greater psycholog- high- and medium-attractiveness targets with the differences in
ical impact of vivid (vs. pallid) stimuli. Vivid information is judgments of medium- and low-attractiveness targets. We did
presumed to attract and hold people's attention because it is, for not compute effect sizes comparing medium-attractiveness tar-
example, concrete and imagery provoking, emotionally interest- gets with high- or low-attractiveness targets because the design
ing, or proximal in a sensory, temporal, or spatial way (Nisbett comparing high- and low-attractiveness targets is the prototype
& Ross, 1980). Although empirical support for the hypothesis for research on this topic. In fact, 71% of the studies in our
that vividly presented information has more impact on judg- sample did not include a middle or medium level of attractive-
ments than pallid information has been mixed, many of the ness.
failures to confirm vividness hypotheses may have been due to Another limitation of our meta-analysis is that we had no
researchers' failures to specify exactly what was made vivid by satisfactory way to gauge the strength of researchers' manipula-
their particular vividness manipulations (see Chaiken & Eagly, tions of physical attractiveness. Some of the between-study vari-
ability in findings was no doubt due to variation in the strength
1983; Taylor & Thompson, 1982). This ambiguity was for the
of the attractiveness manipulation. Most researchers used
most part not present in this research because photographs
judges' ratings to select their target stimuli, but rules for select-
were typically presented, making the physical appearance of
ing good- and bad-looking stimuli varied (e.g, some investiga-
the target vivid. Thus, the typical experimental paradigm for
tors excluded targets that were rated as extremely attractive or
research on the physical attractiveness stereotype may be espe-
unattractive, whereas others did not; see Wallston & O*Leary's,
cially well adapted to demonstrating vividness effects, and pri-
1981, discussion of this point). Although manipulation checks
mary research within this paradigm might well explore these
were often present, data were generally not reported in suffi-
effects.
cient detail to allow the computation of effect sizes representing
Number of judgments aggregated into dependent variable.
the perceived difference in the physical attractiveness of the
The finding that the beauty-is-good effect was stronger when a
high- and low-attractiveness targets. Moreover, norms of attrac-
larger number of judgments was aggregated into the dependent
tiveness probably changed over time so that the features of
variable is explicable in terms of ordinary principles of reliabil-
highly attractive faces varied somewhat from study to study.
ity and aggregation: Dependent variables show stronger rela- These variations in looks may have influenced the particular
tions to appropriate predictors to the extent that measures of inferences that perceivers made.
these variables are more reliable because they are based on The conclusions of our meta-analysis are also limited by the
aggregations of correlated responses (e.g., Ajzen, 1988). This characteristics of the existing paradigm for assessing the attrac-
relation between the strength of findings and response aggrega- tiveness stereotype. Many researchers from Dion and co-
tion has been obtained in other meta-analyses (Eagly & John- workers (1972) onward have used the same basic laboratory
son, 1990; Eagly & Steffen, 1986), and it is appropriate to rou- experimental procedure. In the interest of experimental con-
tinely track the effects of aggregation in quantitative reviews. trol, Dion and associates' paradigm presented only one type of
Other findings. The relation that the physical attractiveness stimulus in a particular way: namely, college students depicted
stereotype showed to instructional set, type of attractiveness in head-and-shoulders photographs. In this study and in most
variation, and control for confounds are of somewhat less inter- subsequent research, the targets were young adults with the
est because it is difficult to interpret these findings in any but physical appearance idiosyncracies of people who attend col-
the most tentative way. \fet the tendency for the accuracy in- lege. This stimulus domain reduced the likelihood that good-
structional set to produce a weaker stereotype than other in- looking but bad types (e.g, dumb blonde, whore/tramp, femme
structional sets is interesting and consistent with Dion and col- fatale, sleazy guy, gigolo) were represented. For example, the
leagues' (1972) logic in choosing this particular set (see Dion, prototypic femme fatale is generally depicted in the media as
1981,1986). It is also worth noting the tendency for the stereo- older than the modal college woman and identified by clothes,
type to be more pronounced in studies that apparently did not makeup, and jewelry that are unlikely to be worn by college
(vs. did) implement any control for confounds of attractiveness women posing for yearbook photos.
with other features of target stimuli. Because such studies pre- The most important limitation of the present meta-analysis
sented only two individuals, one attractive person and one unat- follows directly from our goal of evaluating the strength and
tractive person (of each sex if both sexes of targets were in- generality of the physical attractiveness stereotype. Because of
cluded), attractiveness could have been confounded with other our decision to study only the stereotype itself, we included only
aspects of targets' physical appearance. The fact that this lack of studies that assessed evaluative perceptions of targets in situa-
control was associated with a stronger beauty-is-good effect sug- tions that did not involve personal or role relationships between
gests that confounds favored researchers' hypothesis. perceiver and target. Although this emphasis allowed us to do
124 EAGLY, ASHMORE, MAKHIJANI, AND LONGO

the best possible job of documenting the overall size of the constraints imposed by social roles and by organizational
attractiveness stereotype and its variability, especially as a func- norms and hierarchies.
tion of the specific type of inference, it did not permit us to
probe the impact that the beauty-is-good stereotype has on ac-
tion. We have thus not examined the impact of attractiveness References
stereotyping in ongoing social interaction and everyday role Abelson, R. P. (1985). A variance explanation paradox: When a little is
relationships. Quantitative summaries of the large research liter- a lot. Psychological Bulletin, 97,129-133.
ature addressing such issues can now proceed given the rela- Adams, G. R. (1977). Physical attractiveness research: Toward a devel-
tively clear description that the present review provides of the opmental social psychology of beauty. Human Development, 20,
physical attractiveness stereotype itself. 217-239.
Adams, G. R. (1982). Physical attractiveness. In A. G. Miller (Ed), In
the eye of the beholder: Contemporary issues in stereotyping(pp. 253-
Conclusion 304). New York: Praeger.
Adams, G. R., & Cohen, A. S. (1974). Children's physical and interper-
This meta-anatysis indicates that narrative reviewers were
sonal characteristics that affect student-teacher interactions. Journal
correct in concluding that, in terms of perceivers' inferences of Experimental Education, 43,1-5.
from cues that convey physical attractiveness, what is beautiful Adams, G. R., & Wareham, J. (no date). Beautiful is good: Mechanism of
is good. At the same time, our findings reveal that this effect is self and others'perceptions. Unpublished manuscript, Utah State Uni-
highly variable, and that the implicit personality theory frame- versity; Logan, UT.
work successfully identifies factors that help account for this Ajzen, I. (1988). Attitudes, personality, and behavior. Chicago: Dorsey
variation. Thus, our results indicate that the beauty-is-good ef- Press.
fect depends crucially on the type of inference the perceiver is Allen, B. P. (1978). Social behavior: Facts and falsehoods about common
asked to make. Good looks induce strong inferences about so- sense, hypnotism, obedience, altruism, beauty, racism, and sexism.
Chicago: Nelson Hall.
cial competence and weaker inferences about potency, adjust-
Alley, T. R, & Hildebrandt, K. A. (1988). Determinants and conse-
ment, and intellectual competence, but have little impact on
quences of facial aesthetics. In T. R. Alley (Ed), Social and applied
beliefs about integrity and concern for others. One important aspects of perceiving faces (pp. 101-140). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
direction for future primary research is to design explicit tests Anderson, N. H. (1974). Cognitive algebra: Integration theory applied
of the effects we demonstrated of the type of attributes con- to social attribution. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental
tained in dependent measures. Such research would require the social psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 1-101). New York: Academic Press.
development of reliable and valid indexes of the content-speci- Anderson, N. H. (1981). Integration theory applied to cognitive re-
fic dimensions of personality represented in people's implicit sponses and attitudes. In R. E. Petty, T. M. Ostrom, & T. C. Brock
theories. Indeed, some researchers have already taken steps in (Eds.), Cognitive responses in persuasion (pp. 361-397). Hillsdale,
this direction. Specifically, Bassili (1981), who also proposed NJ: Erlbaum.
Anderson, R., & Nida, S. A. (1978). Effect of physical attractiveness on
that the attractiveness stereotype is not as general as implied by
opposite- and same-sex evaluations. Journal of Personality, 46,401-
the phrase beauty is good (see discussion early in this article),
413.
used factor analysis to demonstrate that Dion and colleagues' Asch, S. E. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. Journal of Ab-
(1972) Social Desirability Index contains several content- normal and Social Psychology, 41, 258-290.
specific types of evaluative meaning. He also offered a more Ashmore, R. D. (1981). Sex stereotypes and implicit personality theory.
general instrument for measuring dimensions of personality In D. L. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive processes in stereotyping and inter-
that are affected by positive versus negative appearance. group behavior (pp. 37-81). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Other parts of the social psychological research literature on Ashmore, R. D., & Del Boca, F. K. (1979). Sex stereotypes and implicit
attractiveness also provide fruitful domains for quantitative re- personality theory: Toward a cognitive-social psychological concep-
viewing. The most pressing next step is to integrate the results tualization. Sex Roles, 5, 219-248.
Ashmore, R. D., Del Boca, E K., & Wohlers, A. J. (1986). Genderstereo-
of the large number of studies that have examined the effects of
types. In R. D Ashmore & F. K. Del Boca (Eds.), The social psychol-
physical appearance on how people are treated (e.g, in educa-
ogy of female-male relations: A critical analysis of central concepts
tional settings, Adams & Cohen, 1974; in judicial proceedings, (pp. 69-119). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
Stewart, 1980). Our finding that the strength of the beauty-is- Ashmore, R. D, Tumia, M, & Schreier, P. (1980). Forming impressions
good stereotype depends on the type of inference perceivers of personality: A reanalysis of the central trait effect through multidi-
make yields some predictions about treatment effects. For ex- mensional scaling. Unpublished manuscript, Rutgers University,
ample, targets' physical attractiveness should have greater im- New Brunswick, NJ.
pact on how people treat them in the general domain of social Barnes, M. L., & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Interpersonal effects of experi-
competence (eg., peer popularity achieved) than intellectual menter attractiveness, attire, and gender. Journal of Personality and
competence (e.g., school grades received), integrity (e.g, sen- Social Psychology, 48, 435-446.
Baron, R. A, & Byrne, D. (1987). Social psychology: Understanding
tences assigned for legal violations), or concern for others (e.g.,
human interaction (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
opportunities provided for prosocial behavior). In addition, by
Bar-Tal, D, & Saxe, L. (1976). Physical attractiveness and its relation-
extending our logic about the dampening effect of individuat- ship to sex-role stereotyping. Sex Roles, 2,123-133.
ing information, we expect a weakening of the overall effect of Bassili, J. N. (1981). The attractiveness stereotype: Goodness or glam-
beauty as one moves from stereotype studies to research assess- our? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 2, 235-252.
ing behavior in natural settings and even more reduction when Berscheid, E. (1981). A review of the psychological effects of physical
these natural settings require that people interact within the attractiveness. In G. W Lucker, K. A. Ribbens, & J. A. McNamara
REVIEW OF PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS STEREOTYPE 125

(Eds), Psychological aspects of facial form (pp. 1-23). Ann Arbor, Downs, A. C, & Harrison, S. K. (1985). Embarrassing age spots or just
MI: Center for Human Growth. plain ugly? Physical attractiveness stereotyping as an instrument of
Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1974). Physical attractiveness. In L. sexism on American television commercials. Sex Roles, 13,9-19.
Berkowitz (Ed), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 7, Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior: A social-role
pp. 157-215). New York: Academic Press. interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Brigham, J. C. (1980). Limiting conditions of the "physical attractive- Eagly, A. H., & Johnson, B. T. (1990). Gender and leadership style: A
ness stereotype": Attributions about divorce. Journal of Research in meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 233-256.
Personality, 14, 365-375. Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the
Bull, R., & Rumsey, N. (1988). The social psychology of facial appear- distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personal-
ance. New York: Springer-Verlag. ity and Social Psychology, 46, 735-754.
Bull, R, & Stevens, J. (1979). The effects of attractiveness of writer and Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V J. (1986). Gender and aggressive behavior: A
penmenship on essay grades. Journal of Occupational Psychology. meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psycho-
52, 53-59. logical Bulletin, 100, 309-330.
Cash, T. F. (1981). Physical attractiveness: An annotated bibliography Eagty, A. H., & Wood, W (in press). Explaining sex differences in social
of theory and research in the behavioral sciences. Psychological Doc- behavior: A meta-analytic perspective. Personality and Social Psy-
uments, 11, 83. (Ms. No. 2370) chology Bulletin.
Cash, T. F. (1985). Physical appearance and mental health. In J. A. Feingold, A. (1990a). Gender differences in effects of physical attrac-
Graham & A. M. Kligman (Eds.), The psychology of cosmetic treat- tiveness on romantic attraction: A comparison across five research
ments (pp. 196-216). New York: Praeger. paradigms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 981-
Cash, T. F, & Janda, L. H. (1984, December). The eye of the beholder. 993.
Psychology Today, pp. 46-52. Feingold, A. (1990b). Good-looking people are not what we think. Un-
Cash, T. F, & Kehr, J. (1978). Influence of nonprofessional counselors' published manuscript.
physical attractiveness and sex on perceptions of counselor behavior. Ghiselli, E. E. (1964). Theory of psychological measurement. New York:
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 25, 336-342. McGraw-Hill.
Chaiken, S. (1979). Communicator physical attractiveness and persua- Glass, G. V, McGaw, B., & Smith, M. L. (1981). Meta-anafysis in social
sion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37,1387-1397. research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Chaiken, S. (1986). Physical appearance and social influence. In C. P.
Goldman, W, & Lewis, P. (1977). Beautiful is good: Evidence that the
Herman, M. P. Zanna, & E. T. Higgins (Eds), Physical appearance,
physically attractive are more socially skillful. Journal of Experimen-
stigma, and social behavior: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 3, pp. 143-
tal Social Psychology, 13,125-130.
177). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Greenwald, A. G. (1975). Consequences of prejudice against the null
Chaiken, S., & Eagly, A. H. (1983). Communication modality as a deter-
hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 82,1-20.
minant of persuasion: The role of communicator salience. Journal of
Gross, A. E, & Crofton, C. (1977). What is good is beautiful. Socio-
Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 241-256.
metry, 40, 85-90.
Cohen, S. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences
Hamilton, D. L. (Ed). (1981). Cognitive processes in stereotyping and
(rev. ed). New York: Academic Press.
intergroup behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cooper, H. M., & Findley, M. (1982). Expected effect sizes: Estimates
Hartnett, J. I, & Secord, G. (1983). Physical attraction and its effects on
for statistical power analysis in social psychology. Personality and
the perception of extra-marital affairs. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
Social Psychology Bulletin, 8,168-173.
56, 310.
Courtney, A. E., & Whipple, T. W (1983). Sex stereotyping in advertis-
ing. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Hatfield, E., & Sprecher, S. (1986). Mirror, mirror: The importance of
looks in everyday life. Albany: State University of New York Press.
Deaux, K., & Lewis, L. L. (1983). Components of gender stereotypes.
Psychological Documents, 13. 25. (Ms. No. 2583) Hedges, L. V(1981). Distribution theory for Glass's estimator of effect
Deaux, K., & Lewis, L. L. (1984). Structure of gender stereotypes: size and related estimators. Journal of Educational Statistics, 6,107-
Interrelationships among components and gender label. Journal of 128.
Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 991-1004. Hedges, L. V (1982a). Fitting categorical models to effect sizes from a
Deaux, K., & Wrightsman, L. S. (1988). Social psychology. Pacific series of experiments. Journal of Educational Statistics, 7,119-137.
Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Hedges, L. V (1982b). Fitting continuous models to effect size data.
Del Boca, F. K., & Ashmore, R. D. (1980). Sex stereotypes through the Journal of Educational Statistics, 7,245-270.
life cycle. In L. Wheeler (Ed), Review of personality and social psy- Hedges, L. V (1987). How hard is hard science, how soft is soft science?
chology (Vol. 1, pp. 163-192). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. The empirical cumulativeness of research. American Psychologist,
Dernier, M., & Thiel, D. L. (1975). When beauty may fail. Journal of 42, 443-455.
Personality and Social Psychology, SI, 1168-1176. Hedges, L. V, & Becker, B. J. (1986). Statistical methods in the meta-
Dibiase, W J., &Hjelle, L. A. (1968). Body-image stereotypes and body- analysis of research on gender differences. In J. Hyde & M. C. Linn
type preferences among male college students. Perceptual and Motor (Eds), The psychology of gender: Advances through meta-anafysis
Skills, 27,1143-1146. (pp. 14-50). Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Dion, K. K. (1981). Physical attractiveness, sex roles and heterosexual Hedges, L. V, & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-anafysis.
attraction. In M. Cook (Ed), The bases of human sexual attraction Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
(pp. 3-22). London: Academic Press. Hocking, J. E, Walker, B. A., & Fink, E. L. (1982). Physical attractive-
Dion, K. K. (1986). Stereotyping based on physical attractiveness: Is- ness and judgments of morality following an "immoral" act. Psycho-
sues and conceptual perspectives. In C. R Herman, M. P. Zanna, & logical Reports, 51,111-116.
E. T. Higgins (Eds), Physical appearance, stigmaand social behavior: Jackman, D. M. (1979). Effects of physical attractiveness and sex on the
The Ontario Symposium (Vol. 3, pp. 7-21). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. perception of assertiveness, aggressiveness, and submissiveness.
Dion, K. K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is Dissertations Abstracts International, 39, 5648B. (University Micro-
good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285-290. films No. 79-10272)
126 EAGLY, ASHMORE, MAKHIJANI, AND LONGO

Jackson, L. A, Sullivan, L. A., & Rostker, R. (1988). Gender, gender Pilkonis, P. A. (1977). The behavioral consequences of shyness. Journal
role, and body image. Sex Roles, 19,429-443. of Personality, 45, 596-611.
Jacobson, M. B., & Koch, W (1978). Attributed reasons for support of Roberts, E. J. (1982). Television and sexual learning in children. In D.
the feminist movement as a function of attractiveness. Sex Roles, 4, Pearl, L. Bouthilet, & J. Lazar (Eds), Television and behavior: Ten
169-174. years of scientific progress and implications for the eighties (Vol. 2, pp.
Jacobson, M. B, & Popovich, P. M. (1983). Victim attractiveness and 209-223). Washington, DC: US. Government Printing Office.
perceptions of responsibility in an ambiguous rape case. Psychology Rodin, J, Silberstein, L., & Striegel-Moore, R. (1985). Women and
ofWomen Quarterly, 8,100-104. weight: A normative discontent. In T. B. Sonderegger & R. A.
Johnson, B. T, & Eagly, A. H. (1989). The effects of involvement on Dienstbier (Eds), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1984 (Vol. 33,
persuasion: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 290-314. pp. 267-307). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
Kim, M. R, & Rosenberg, S. (1980). Comparison of two structural Rosenberg, S. (1977). New approaches to the analysis of personal con-
models of implicit personality theory. Journal of Personality and So- structs in person perception. In A. L. Land & J. K. Cole (Eds.),
cial Psychology, 38, 375-389. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1976 (Vol. 24, pp. 179-242).
Lane, D. M., & Dunlap, W P. (1978). Estimating effect size: Bias result- Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
ing from the significance criterion in editorial decisions. British Rosenberg, S., Nelson, C, & Vivekananthan, P. S. (1968). A multidi-
Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 31,107-112. mensional approach to the structure of personality impressions.
Langlois, J. H. (1986). From the eye of the beholder to behavioral real- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 283-294.
ity: Development of social behaviors and social relations as a func- Rosenberg, S., & Sedlak, A. (1972). Structural representations of im-
tion of physical attractiveness. In C. P. Herman, M. P. Zanna, & E. T. plicit personality theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experi-
Higgins (Eds.), Physical appearance, stigma, and social behavior:The mental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 235-297). New York: Academic
Ontario Symposium (Vol. 3, pp. 23-51). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Press.
Larsen. R. J., Diener, E., &Cropanzano, R. S. (1987). Cognitive opera- Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1982). A simple, general purpose display
tions associated with individual differences in affect intensity. Jour- of magnhudeof experimental effect. Journal of Educational Psychol-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 767-774. ogy, 74,166-169.
Lerner, R., & Lerner, J. (1977). Effects of age, sex and physical attrac- Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1986). Mela-analytic procedures for
tiveness on child-peer relations, academic performance, and elemen- combining studies with multiple effect sizes. Psychological Bulletin,
tary school adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 13,585-590. 99, 400-406.
Locksley, A, Borgida, E., Brekke, N., & Hepburn, C. (1980). Sex stereo- Scheibe, C. (1979). Sex roles in TV commercials. Journal of Advertising
types and social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Research, 19, 23-27.
ogy, 39.S21-S31. Schneider, D. J. (1973). Implicit personality theory: A review. Psycholog-
Locksley, A, Hepburn, C, & Ortiz, Y (1982). Social stereotypes and ical Bulletin, 79, 294-309.
judgments of individuals: An instance of the base-rate fallacy. Jour- Schneider, P. A, Conger, A, & Firth, E. A. (1986, April). Sex as a
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 23-42. moderator of the beauty-good relationship. Paper presented at the
Martin, P. 1, Friedmeyer, M. H, & Moore, J. E. (1977). Pretty patient/ meeting of the American Association for Counseling and Develop-
healthy patient? A study of physical attractiveness and psychopathol- ment, Los Angeles.
ogy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 33,990-994. Snyder, M., Tanke, E. D., & Berscheid, E. (1977). Social perception and
McArthur, L. Z. (1982). Judging a book by its cover: A cognitive analy- interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereo-
sis of the relationship between physical appearance and stereotyp- types. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 656-666.
ing. In A. H. Hastorf & A. M. Isen (Eds.), Cognitive social psychology Stewart, J. E. (1980). Defendant's attractiveness as a factor in the out-
(pp. 149-211). New York: Elsevier. come of criminal trials: An observational study. Journal of Applied
Miller, A. G., Gillen, B., Schenker, C., & Radlove, S. (1974). The predic- Social Psychology, 10, 348-361.
tion and perception of obedience to authority. Journal of Personality, Taylor, S. E., & Thompson, S. C. (1982). Stalking the elusive "vividness"
42, 23-42. effect. Psychological Review, 89,155-181.
Mullen, B. (1989). Advanced BASIC meta-analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erl- Touhey, J. C. (1979). Sex-role stereotyping and individual differences in
baum. liking for the physically attractive. Social Psychology Quarterly, 42,
Nisbett, R. E.,& Ross. L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies and short- 285-289.
comings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Turkat, D., & Dawson, J. (1976). Attributions of responsibility for a
Nordholm, L. A. (1980). Beautiful patients are good patients: Evidence chance event as a function of sex and physical attractiveness of target
for the physical attractiveness stereotype in first impressions of pa- individual. Psychological Reports, 39, 275-279.
tients. Social Science and Medicine, 14A, 81-83. Wallston, B. S, & O"Leary, Y E. (1981). Sex makes a difference: Differ-
O'Reilly, M. T. (1987). Physical attractiveness of professors in dental ential perceptionsof women and men. In L. Wheeler (Ed),/tevfewo/
education: Its relationship to students' ratings of teaching effective- personality and social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 9-41). Beverly Hills,
ness (Doctoral dissertation, University of Pittsburgh, 1986). Disser- CA: Sage.
tation Abstracts International, 47, 433A. White, G. M. (1980). Conceptual universal in interpersonal language.
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J, & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measure- American Anthropologist, 82, 759-781.
ment of meaning. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. Wood, W, Rhodes, N., & Whelan, M. (1989). Sex differences in positive
Patzer, G. L. (1985). The physical attractiveness phenomena. New York: well-being: A meta-analytic review of the effects associated with
Plenum Press. marital status. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 249-264.
REVIEW OF PHYSICAL ATTRACTIVENESS STEREOTYPE 127

Appendix

References for Studies in Meta-Analysis

Adams, G. R., & Huston, T. L. (1975). Social perception of middle-aged Gallucci, N. T, & Meyer, R. G. (1984). People can be too perfect:
persons varying in physical attractiveness. Developmental Psychol- Effects of subjects' and targets' attractiveness on interpersonal at-
ogy, 11. 657-658. traction. Psychological Reports, 55, 351-360.
Alicke, M. n, Smith, R. H., & Klotz, M. L. (1986). Judgments of physi- Gillen, B. (1981). Physical attractiveness: A determinant of two types
cal attractiveness: The role of faces and bodies. Personality and So- of goodness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7.277-281.
cial Psychology Bulletin, 12, 381-389. Gillen, B. (1975). Physical attractiveness and sex as determinants of
Anderson, S. M., & Bern, S. L. (1981). Sex typing and androgyny in trait attributions. Dissertation Abstracts International, 36,1945B.
dyadic interaction: Individual differences in responsiveness to physi- (University Microfilms No. 75-21,693)
cal attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, Gillen, B., & Sherman, R. C. (1980). Physical attractiveness and sex as
74-86. determinants of trait attributions. Multivariate Behavioral Research,
Armstrong, D P. (1979). The facial attractiveness riddle (Doctoral dis- 15, 423^37.
sertation, Southern Illinois University at Carbondale, 1978). Disser- Green, C. E, Cunningham, 1, & Yanico, B. J. (1986). Effects of coun-
tation Abstracts International, 39, 5127B. selor and subject race and counselor physical attractiveness on im-
Barnes, M. L, & Rosenthal, R. (1985). Interpersonal effects of experi- pressions and expectations of a female counselor. Journal of Counsel-
menter attractiveness, attire, and gender. Journal of Personality and ing Psychology, 33, 349-352.
Social Psychology, 48, 435-446. Guise, B. J., Pollans, C. H., & Turkat, I. D. (1982). Effects of physical
Barocas, R., & Karoly, P. (1972). Effects of physical appearance on attractiveness on perception of social skill. Perceptual and Motor
social responsiveness. Psychological Reports, 31, 495-500. Skills, 54,1039-1042.
Bassili, J. N. (1981). The attractiveness stereotype: Goodness or glam- Hailey, B. J. (1976). Selective attention as a factor in the maintenance of
our? Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 2, 235-252. physical attractiveness and sex role stereotypes (Doctoral disserta-
Benassi, M. A. (1981). The effects of order of presentation and physical tion, University of Georgia, 1975). Dissertation Abstracts Interna-
attractiveness on attributions of ability (Doctoral dissertation, De tional, 36, 4227B.
Paul University). Dissertation Abstracts International, 42, 2596B. Havassy, V J. (1981). Reactions to professional women as a function of
Black, H. K. (1974). Physical attractiveness and similarity of attitude in attractiveness and level of competence (Doctoral dissertation, Cali-
interpersonal attraction. Psychological Reports, 35, 403-406. fornia School of Professional Psychology). Dissertation Abstracts In-
Boor, M., & Zeis, E R. (1975). Effect of physical attractiveness on 1Q ternational, 42, 2130B.
estimation: A failure to extend results of prior research. Psychologi- Heilman, M. E.,&Stopeck, M. H. (1985). Attractiveness and corporate
cal Documents, 5, 234. (Ms. No. 929). success: Different causal attributions for males and females. Journal
Byrne, D, London, O., & Reeves, K. (1968). The effects of physical of Applied Psychology, 70, 379-388.
attractiveness, sex, and attitude similarity on interpersonal attrac- Hicks, R. A., Pellegrini, R. I, & Tomlinson, N. (1978). Attributions of
tion. Journal of Personality, 36,259-271. female college students to male photographs as a function of attrac-
Carter, J. A. (1978). Impressions of counselors as a function of coun- tiveness and pupil size. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47,1265-1266.
selor physical attractiveness. Journal ofCou,:'eling Psychology, 25, Hill, M. K., & Lando, H. A. (1976). Physical attractiveness and sex-role
28-34. stereotypes in impression formation. Perceptual and Motor Skills,
Cash, T. E, Begley, P I, McCown, D. A, & Weise, B. C. (1975). When 43,1251-1255.
counselors are heard but not seen: Initial impact of physical attrac- Jackson, L. A. (1983). The perception of androgyny and physical attrac-
tiveness. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 22, 273-279. tiveness: Two is better than one. Personality and Social Psychology
Cash, T. E, Kehr, J. A., Poryson, J., & Freeman, V (1977). Role of physi- Bulletin, 9, 405-413.
cal attractiveness in peer attribution of psychological disturbance. Janda, L. H., O'Grady, K. E., & Barnhart, S. A. (1981). Effects of sexual
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 45, 987-993. attitudes and physical attractiveness on person perception of men
Covington, J. K. (1981). The communicator's physical attractiveness and women. Sex Roles, 7,189-199.
and credibility as determinants of the effectiveness of a speech (Doc- Joseph, W B. (1977). Effect of communicator physical attractiveness
toral dissertation, Louisiana State University). Dissertation Ab- and expertness on opinion change and information processing. Dis-
stracts International, 42, 437A. sertation Abstracts International, 38, 5017A. (University Microfilms
Cunningham, M. R. (1986). Measuring the physical in physical attrac- No. 77-31,899)
tiveness: Quasi-experiments on the sociobiology of female facial Kaplan, R. M. (1978). Is beauty talent? Sex interaction in the attractive-
beauty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 925-935. ness halo effect. Sex Roles, 4,195-204.
Dermer, M. (1974). When beauty fails (Doctoral dissertation, Univer- Kleinke, C. L, & Kahn, M. L. (1980). Perceptions of self-disclosers:
sity of Minnesota, 1973). Dissertation Abstracts International, 34, Effects of sex and physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality, 48,
4402A. 190-205.
Dermer, M, & Thiel, D. L. (1975). When beauty may fail. Journal of Kleinke, C. L., Staneski, R. A, & Pipp, S. L. (1975). Effects of gaze,
Personality and Social Psychology, J/,1168-1176. distance, and attractiveness on males' first impressions of females.
Dion, K.., Berscheid, E, & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Representative Research in Social Psychology, 6, 7-12.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285-290. Kollar, M. M. (1973). The beautiful is rotten phenomenon: A negative
Dushenko, T. W, Perry, R. P., Schilling, J., &Smolarski, S. (1978). Gener- stereotype of physical attractiveness. Dissertation Abstracts Interna-
ality of the physical attractiveness stereotype forage and sex. Journal tional, 34, 4632B. (University Microfilms No. 74-07,769)
of Social Psychology, 105, 303-304. Landy, D., & Sigall, H. (1974). Beauty is talent: Task evaluation as a
Gallucci, N. T. (1984). Effects of men's physical attractiveness on inter- functionof the performer's physical attractiveness. Journal of Person-
personal attraction. Psychological Reports, 55, 935-938. ality and Social Psychology, 29, 299-304.
128 EAGLY, ASHMORE, MAKHIJANI, AND LONGO

La Voie, J. C, & Adams, G. R. (1978). Physical and interpersonal attrac- Saner, S. I. (1983). Beyond the halo effect: Physical attractiveness and
tiveness of the model and imitation in adults. Journal of Social Psy- impression formation as a function of cue availability (Doctoral dis-
chology, 106,191-202. sertation, University of Connecticut). Dissertation Abstracts Interna-
Lee, L. J., Adams, G. R, & Dobson, W R. (1984). Male and female tional, 44,1253B.
attributions and social influence behavior towards a physically at- Sanders, C. H, & Borowy, T. D. (1983). Physical attractiveness: Its influ-
tractive female. Journal of Psychology, 117,97-103. ence on the perceptions of counselors (Report No. CG-017-587).
Lewis, K. N, & Walsh, W B. (1978). Physical attractiveness: Its impact Knoxville: University of Tennessee. (ERIC Document Reproduc-
on the perception of a female counselor. Journal of Counseling Psy- tion Service No. ED 246-345)
chology, 25, 210-216. Saxe, L. (1976). Perceiver characteristics as determinants of judgments
Longtain, M. J. (1981). Status generalization and performance expec- of physically attractive and unattractive others (Doctoral disserta-
tation effects of sex role stereotypes, physical attractiveness, and tion, University of Pittsburgh, 1975). Dissertation Abstracts Interna-
expertise. Dissertation Abstracts International, 42, 3034B. (Univer- tional, 37, 2577B.
sity Microfilms No. 81-28,655) Small-Weil, S. B. (1981). The relationship between gender, physical
Lucker, G. W (1977). Physical attractiveness, individual differences attractiveness and occupational prestige (Doctoral dissertation,
and personality stereotyping (Doctoral dissertation, University of Tufts University, 1980). Dissertation Abstracts International, 41,
Texas at Austin, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts International, 37, 3613B.
4219B. Snyder, M, &Rothbart, M. (1971). Communicator attractiveness and
Maddux, J. E, & Rogers, R. W (1980). Effects of source expertness, opinion change. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 3, 377-
physical attractiveness, and supporting arguments on persuasion: A 387.
case of brains over beauty. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Solomon, S., & Saxe, L. (1977). What is intelligent, as well as attractive,
ogy, 39, 235-244. is good. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 3, 670-673.
Miller, A. G. (1970). Role of physical attractiveness in impression for- Stretch, R. H, & Figley, C. R. (1980). Beauty and the boast: Predictors
mation. Psychonomic Science, 19, 241-243. of interpersonal attraction in a dating experiment. Psychology: A
Mims, P. R, Hartnett, J. J., & Nay, W R. (1975). Interpersonal attrac- Quarterly Journal of Human Behavior, 17, 35-43.
tion and help volunteering as a function of physical attractiveness. Tanke, E. D. (1982). Dimensions of the physical attractiveness stereo-
Journal of Psychology, 89,125-131. type: A factor/analytic study. Journal of Psychology, 110, 63-74.
Molberg, A. N. (1977). The effects of interpersonal distance, physical Thornton, B., & Linnstaedter, L. (1980). The influence of physical
attractiveness, and sex on impression formation (Doctoral disserta- attractiveness and sex-role congruence on interpersonal attraction.
tion, University of Minnesota, 1976). Dissertation Abstracts Interna- Representative Research in Social Psychology, 11, 55-63.
tional, 37, 6408B. Webster, M., & Driskell, J. E. (1983). Beauty as status. American Jour-
Moore, J. S., Graziano, W G., & Miller, M. G. (1987). Physical attractive- nal of Sociology, 89,140-165.
ness, sex role orientation, and the evaluation of adults and children. Widgery, R. N. (1974). Sex of receiver and physical attractiveness of
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 95-102. source as determinants of initial credibility perception. Western
Nielsen, J. P., & Kernaleguen, A. (1976). Influence of clothing and Speech, 38,13-17.
physical attractiveness in person perception. Perceptual and Motor Widgery, R. N., & Webster, B. (1969). The effects of physical attractive-
Skills, 42, 775-780. ness upon perceived initial credibility. Michigan Speech Journal, 4,
Osterkamp, M. B. (1980). Impressions of older persons in initial inter- 9-19.
actions (Doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas, 1979). Disser- Wiener, S., Saxe, L, & Bar-Tal, D. (1975). The effects of physical attrac-
tation Abstracts International, 41,849A. tiveness on attributions of causality for success and failure (Report
Patzer, G. L. (1983). Source credibility as a function of communicator No. CG-010-288). Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh. (ERIC
physical attractiveness. Journal of Business Research, 11, 229-241. Document Reproduction Service No. ED 117-588)
Pistilli.J. A. (1976). Stereotyped perceptionsof women asafunctionof Wilson, M., Crocker, J., Brown, C. E., Johnson, D, Liotta, R, & Konat,
menstrual cycle phase and physical attractiveness (Doctoral disser- J. (1985). The attractive executive: Effects of sex of business asso-
tation, Kent State University, 1975). Dissertation Abstracts Interna- ciates on attributions of competence and social skills. Basic and Ap-
tional, 36, 4760B. plied Social Psychology, 6, 13-23.
Reeves, R. A. (1982). The effects of source physical attractiveness, simi-
larity, and message type on persuasion: When beauty is not enough Received May 17,1989
(Doctoral dissertation, University of Georgia). Dissertation Ab- Revision received November 20,1989
stracts International, 43, 2044B. Accepted November 6,1990 •

You might also like