You are on page 1of 9

Behavior and Social Issues

https://doi.org/10.1007/s42822-019-0001-y
REGULAR ARTICLE

Tutorial: Selection of Cultures and the Role


of Recurrent Contingencies and Interlocking Behavioral
Contingencies

Kalliu Carvalho Couto 1

# Association for Behavior Analysis International 2019

Abstract
The interplay between a population’s traits and environmental events feeds evolution
by natural selection. These traits may vary from innate predispositions to acquire
specific behavioral topographies to the ability to learn through local environmental
changes (operant behavior). Thus, learning capability is historically inseparable from
evolution by natural selection. In humans, besides inheriting behavioral predispositions
and the ability to acquire novel behavioral repertoires, learning is dependent on social
learning of culturally transmitted practices. Cultural practices are selected by environ-
mental events as a function of their adaptive value to the group and also enhance
members’ adaptive capability. Within a verbal community, individuals cooperate and
coordinate their behavior, producing environmental changes that would not be possible
otherwise. Those cooperative and coordinated responses are under the control of the
verbal community’s sets of contingencies (i.e., culture), which also evolve over time. In
this paper, the coevolutionary processes involving natural selection, selection of oper-
ant behavior, and selection of cultures (environmental settings) will be discussed within
a behavior-analytic perspective. A special focus will be given to interlocking behavioral
contingencies (IBCs) involved in maintaining and transmitting practices (controlling
IBCs) and in the verbal community’s joint efforts (execution IBCs).

Keywords Selection of cultures . Controlling and execution IBCs . Metacontingency

The behavior-analytic conceptual framework, experimental studies, and applied field


are grounded in the selectionist approach, which was first extensively used by Darwin
to explain the evolution of species (Dawkins, 1989; Ruse, 2012). In the Darwinian
view, species’ most adaptive traits are selected from variation within the species

* Kalliu Carvalho Couto


coutokalliu@gmail.com

1
Oslo Metropolitan University, Oslo, Norway
Behavior and Social Issues

population by environmental variables and transmitted through generations. These


traits vary from sensitivity to physical characteristics of the environment (e.g., the
ability to perceive a specific acoustic frequency) to innate predispositions for acquiring
specific behavioral topographies. The capability of learning new behavioral repertories
(operant behavior) during ontogenesis also has adaptive value and is part of the
phylogenetic history of many species, including humans. Thus, selection of human
behavior during ontogenesis is historically inseparable from evolution by natural
selection. Whereas the behavior-analytic community readily accepts the influence of
phylogeny on behavioral acquisition, the emphasis in research has been on how
environmental variables select organisms’ behavioral repertoire during ontogenesis.
Humans inherit behavioral predispositions for specific behavioral topographies (e.g.,
the ability to communicate through vocal sounds) and the capability of incrementally
acquiring new behavioral repertoires during ontogenesis. The incremental learning of
novel behavioral repertoires frequently occurs in social settings. Behavioral patterns
shared by a verbal community (i.e., cultural practices) are selected by environmental
events due to their adaptive value to the group and also serve to enhance members’
adaptive capability (Skinner, 1981). In their turn, cultural practices vary across geo-
graphically separated populations as functions of local environmental characteristics
and within- and between-group competition. Such cumulative behavioral and cultural
lineages are historically stored, from storytelling to law and literature, as sets of
environmental settings (Couto & Sandaker, 2016). Environmental settings within a
verbal community are sets of conditional relations specifying and providing conse-
quences to classes of responses emitted in certain contexts. Thus, these conditional
relations are selected within verbal communities, connecting the behavior of individ-
uals to the joint group effort. For that connection to occur, controlling contingencies
must be selected over time and are informally and formally established and transmitted.
As described by Skinner (1953), group control of individual behavior happens when
people have interacted for sufficient time and different classes of behavior are charac-
terized as Bgood^ and Bbad^ or Bright^ and Bwrong,^ and reinforced and punished
accordingly. Members will be incentivized or punished to act in ways that increase the
fitness of the group, even at a cost to their own fitness. The present paper discusses how
sets of contingencies and interlocking behavioral contingencies (IBCs) may become
recurrent within cultural-level selection. First, social behavior, cooperation, and coor-
dination are described as the basis for groups as units of selection. Next, the method by
which within-group sets of contingencies and IBCs are selected as environmental
settings, which provide controlling variables to group members and shape them toward
engaging in the verbal community joint effort, will be discussed.

Social Behavior

A functional analysis of social behavior will consider how frequently and with what
magnitude individuals are antecedents to or consequences for each other. Thus, the
learning principles in social behavior are not different from those at work when
interacting with a nonsocial environment. Social behavior is different because of its
complexity rather than its nature (Schmitt, 1998). Although the learning mechanisms in
social behavior may not differ from those found in other forms of operant behavior, the
Behavior and Social Issues

contingencies of survival involved in natural selection have equipped each species with
a different potential for using social information.
Social insects may be among the most efficient species when it comes to using social
information in favor of group fitness. Bees use sophisticated group decision-making to
precisely localize the best potential nest sites within several kilometers around their
colony (Seeley & Buhrman, 1999). Bees cooperate in such a coordinated fashion that
they are often described as superorganisms (E. O. Wilson, 2000). The coordination of
labor in insect colonies makes it possible for different specialized groups to work on
different activities simultaneously (Robinson, 1992), similar to organ systems within an
organism. Despite their learning limitations, bees are also capable of acquiring new
behavioral topographies through operant conditioning (e.g., string pulling to feed) and
of transmitting practices to the colony through social learning (Alem et al., 2016).
Vertebrates are incredibly flexible learners and often capable of transmitting complex
skills across generations by social learning. However, they are limited in how many
individuals can coordinate their responses in a common effort. Their capability of
behaving cohesively rarely exceeds 200 individuals (Moffett, 2013) and relies on
individuals being able to recognize every member of the group. The only known
exceptions are naked mole rats and humans (Moffett, 2012). Through natural selection,
humans acquired both the ability to learn and socially transmit complex behavior and
the capability of coordinating their behavior as a superorganism.

Cooperation, Coordinated Responses, and Metacontingencies

Within groups, natural selection favors competition among individuals, and genes and
their expressive traits are selected. A between-group competition will favor cooperation
among individuals to outcompete other groups from the same or different species. As
stated by D. S. Wilson and Wilson (2007), BSelfishness beats altruism within groups.
Altruistic groups beat selfish groups^ (p. 345). Cultural practices within a given group
may (a) emerge by the simple cumulative effects of members’ behavior (i.e.,
macrobehavior; Glenn, 2004), (b) emerge by their cooperative effort, or (c) require
coordination of responses. Smoking behavior, for example, has probable short-term
consequences for individuals, such as a pleasurable feeling, and it may have long-term
consequences, such as lung cancer. The cumulative effect of smoking as a practice of
many individuals produces consequences at the societal level, such as increased health
care costs and reduced air quality. Other practices require cooperation among the agents
involved. For example, keeping the streets of a neighborhood clean depends on the
cooperative behavior of most of its residents. The behavior of each resident is main-
tained by the cumulative effect of their effort (e.g., a clean neighborhood) and by
supportive contingencies (e.g., social reinforcers). However, those cooperative re-
sponses do not depend on the coordination of behavior among residents of the
neighborhood. Individuals can work at their own pace.
Within coordinated practices, members must behave in relation to each other to
achieve the common goal. Coordinated responses will increase the availability of
resources for the group and may or may not produce immediate reinforcing conse-
quences for individuals. In a volleyball game, for example, members of the team have
to pass the ball at least one time before throwing it back to the other side of the court.
Behavior and Social Issues

The behavior of each player serves as antecedent or consequent stimuli to the behavior
of the others, and throwing effectiveness is the product of this coordination. The
ultimate instance selected by scoring points is the last throw, but as the last throw is
also dependent on the patterns of coordinated responses of multiple players, the
coordination may also be selected.
When considering interactions among organisms as a unit of selection on its own,
we arrive at a phenomenon described by Skinner as a social episode. A social episode
occurs when two or more persons coordinate their behavior to produce an environ-
mental change that would not be possible for persons working alone (Skinner, 1953).
Coordinated behavior, as defined by Keller and Schoenfeld (1950), involves the
behavior of two or more organisms taking place in some specified order, resulting in
the production or removal of environmental consequences. The frequency and type of
cooperative and coordinated responses within a group will increase or decrease as a
function of their effect on a selecting environment (SE), similar to response classes in
operant behavior. The metacontingency concept emerges (Glenn, 1986, 1988; Glenn
& Malott, 2004; Malott & Glenn, 2006) as a conceptual tool (Todorov, 2006) that
describes a functional relation between coordinated responses in the form of IBCs and
the product of this joint effort (aggregate product; AP) with an SE (Glenn et al., 2016).
The first two terms of the metacontingency (IBCs and AP) can be defined as a
culturant. The third term of the metacontingency (SE) is an environmental event that
influences the probability of culturant occurrences in the future. Figure 1 depicts a
metacontingency where the behavior of each individual serves as antecedent or
consequent stimuli to other individuals (IBCs), producing an AP that would not be
possible by the simple sum of individuals’ behavior. Events occurring within an SE
influence the probability of future occurrences of IBCs and APs (i.e., the culturant).
Individuals engage or leave the metacontingency; however, the culturant is still a
function of its functional relation with the SE and is recognizable over time (i.e.,
cultural lineage).

Fig. 1 Schematic representation of a metacontingency. Together, IBCs (1) and their AP (2) can be analyzed as
a culturant (3), which is shaped by its interaction with an SE (4). Members of a metacontingency may be
substituted over generations (5); however, a culturant is still a function of its interaction with the SE
Behavior and Social Issues

Selection of Cultures: Controlling and Execution IBCs

In a classic example of a metacontingency presented by Glenn and Malott (2004),


employees of a restaurant coordinate their behavior in IBCs, producing an AP (i.e.,
meals and service) that interacts with an SE (i.e., customers). The IBCs and AP within
the restaurant will vary and be selected as a function of their effects on the SE,
including competition with other restaurants. In their turn, the recurrent sets of IBCs
will function as conditional relations necessary for maintaining the behavior of
employees involved in service and meal quality and also provide stimulus control to
new and current members. Couto and Sandaker (2016) suggested that selection of
conditional relations (i.e., environmental settings) within groups can be described as
selection of cultures, whereas the control that those conditional relations exercise on the
behavior of the members can be described as cultural selection. Whereas the selection
of cultures occurs at the cultural level, cultural selection may take place at the
behavioral level.
Due to their adaptive value, specific patterns of IBCs will become recurrent among
populations, and they may have at least two functions: They may be directly involved
in (a) the production of APs and (b) the selection and maintenance of new and current
members’ behavior. Going back to Glenn and Malott’s (2004) restaurant example, the
SE may influence the frequency of sets of IBCs directly related to the improvement of
meals and service quality (AP) and sets of IBCs involved in providing within-group
stimulus control to new and current employees’ behavior. This operational differenti-
ation is defined here as execution IBCs (eIBCs), for sets of IBCs related to the
improvement of APs, and controlling IBCs (cIBCs), for those related to within-group
stimulus control.
Selection of cultures as the selection of conditional relations may be exemplified
with a metaphor: gene and phenotype selection in natural selection. Within a species,
the genetic code is a set of rules encoding instructions used for organisms’ general
functions. However, it is the phenotype’s characteristics, not genes, that are in contact
with the SE. In a metacontingency, the APs are in contact with an SE, and the sets of
IBCs carry the instructional code by which the group practices are maintained (cIBCs)
and their function is executed (eIBCs). For example, cIBCs may include control over
free riders, support contingencies, and transfer mechanisms of practices, and eIBCs,
related to the production of APs, include adoption of innovative strategies and group
decision-making processes (see Fig. 2).
When analyzing the selection of cultures from this perspective, the distinction
between eIBCs and cIBCs is made by function rather than by topography. For example,
IBCs directly involved in the improvement or maintenance of an AP (eIBCs) can
function as cIBCs when they provide stimuli control to a member of a group who is
learning (e.g., by observation) the practice of the group. In the same way, cIBCs may
also influence the maintenance and improvement of APs and therefore can be analyzed
as eIBCs depending on the angle at which the analysis takes place.
A similar account of functional differences within metacontingencies was offered by
Glenn (1986) when describing ceremonial and technological contingencies and
metacontingencies. Glenn (1986) describes ceremonial contingencies as socially medi-
ated contingencies that maintain the behavior of individuals through social conse-
quences, often related to authority or social status. Glenn writes, BCeremonial control
Behavior and Social Issues

Fig. 2 A metacontingency where IBCs can function as cIBCs (1), providing stimulus control to members of
the verbal community, and eIBCs, when they are directly related to the production of APs (2). Both c- and
eIBCs are part of the cultural system, which interacts with an SE (3), including competition between groups

is exemplified by ‘Do it because I say so’^ (1986, p. 3). The behavior under ceremonial
control is not necessarily in direct contact with the SE. Similar to cIBCs, the conditional
relations described by Glenn (1986) as ceremonial provide contingencies to members
of a verbal community. However, cIBCs are a generic description of functional
relations, as they describe the reinforcing effects over members of a verbal community
rather than the type of control. Technological metacontingencies involve IBCs that are
selected by their usefulness and that are constantly shaped by direct contact with the
AP’s effect on the SE; eIBCs may be an operational account of IBCs directly related to
the production of APs.
Todorov (2013) proposes the concepts of conservative and transformative
metacontingencies. Conservative metacontingencies require stringent APs—that is,
there is little room for variation. Conversely, transformative metacontingencies require
originality in the AP. Todorov provides an example of a transformative
metacontingency by describing a group of researchers who work together to produce
a paper (AP). The production of a paper is under the control of a transformative
metacontingency, as the AP is always original. From the perspective taken here,
conservative and transformative metacontingencies vary in their frequency and type
of cIBCs and eIBCs. For example, researchers working on the production of a paper
will behave under the control of both the sets of rules (e.g., research literature; cIBCs)
of their verbal community and the direct interactions that contribute to the final, unique,
and original paper (eIBCs). To achieve the role of a researcher, each individual who
contributed to the paper has, perhaps for many years, engaged in cooperative behavior
under the influence of cIBCs more often than eIBCs. Still, their participation when
engaging in cIBCs indirectly contributes to the production of the AP’s execution. Thus,
Behavior and Social Issues

researchers will engage in cIBCs and eIBCs depending on their roles, expertise, and the
nature of the paper.

Controlling IBCs and Execution IBCs in Context

The distributions of e- and cIBCs vary according to the group’s context and its function.
In for-profit organizations, eIBCs are usually stated in their strategic plan, vision, and
mission statement, and cIBCs are found in their organizational structure, internal rules,
and training programs (e.g., Sandaker, 2009). The extent to which individuals can
benefit from self-serving behavior may also influence the frequency of cIBCs required
for that group to function optimally. One example is groups within common pool
resources (CPRs) contexts. In a CPRs situation, it may be advantageous for each
member to explore economic opportunities that achieve maximum individual benefit;
however, the cumulative effect of many individuals behaving in this manner may be
disadvantageous to the collectivity. Economists describe this phenomenon as the
tragedy of the commons (Hardin, 1968). A well-known example of the tragedy of the
commons is a finite area of land with limited resources shared by multiple farmers. It
might seem advantageous for each farmer to exploit the land resources for maximum
individual benefit. However, if all farmers decide to do so, the resources of the finite
area of land will be depleted, thereby prejudicing all farmers at once. It was believed by
many economists that groups within CPRs environments needed external management
to avoid the tragedy of the commons. Ostrom (1990) found that groups that work well
without top-down management interference had developed internal control mecha-
nisms to overcome the tragedy of the commons. Thus, a CPRs context, where
individuals are tempted to benefit from short-term reinforcers, may require different
distributions of cIBCs from those of a company in which an individual does not have
access to reinforcers by working alone.
Laws can be analyzed as sets of written IBCs created to control individuals’
behavior within a verbal community (Todorov, 2005). Thus, laws can be interpreted
as written descriptions of cIBCs. Sets of cIBCs may describe existent practices or may
be arbitrarily created as an instrument of change. Several studies have investigated the
occurrence of contingencies and metacontingencies in written laws (e.g., de Carvalho
and Todorov, 2016; Todorov, Moreira, Prudêncio, & Pereira, 2004). Controlling
agencies such as government, religion, and economy have formally established sets
of controlling contingencies for sets of behavior within a verbal community (Skinner,
1953). Schools are another example of controlling agencies that make strong use of
cIBCs. Schools, as environmental settings, provide the stimulus control that shapes
students’ repertoires, preparing them for the challenges of adulthood. Evidence-based
interventions within schools are used to support teachers in classroom management by
clearly specify controlling contingencies and IBCs in which students should engage
(Bowman-Perrott, Burke, Zaini, Zhang, & Vannest, 2016; Simonsen, Fairbanks,
Briesch, Myers, & Sugai, 2008). At the societal level, controlling agencies have the
function of sets of cIBCs as contingencies involved in providing stimulus control to
members of that verbal community. At the level of controlling agencies, the same sets
of IBCs may be considered eIBCs, as they are involved with the AP of that specific
controlling agency—namely, the quality of teaching in a particular school.
Behavior and Social Issues

Final Remarks

When participating in a group’s joint effort, humans are capable of both coordinating
their responses to achieve the collective group goal and transmitting complex interac-
tion patterns over generations. Those patterns of interactions evolve, shaping the
behavior of new group members. As currently described, metacontingencies (e.g.,
Glenn et al., 2016) focus on the selection of coordinated responses that enable groups
to achieve shared goals. The inclusion of the selective mechanisms, by which sets of
contingencies and IBCs are selected and transmitted, adds the second aspect of human
cultures: how environmental settings evolve. The differentiation between both process-
es is pointed out by Couto and Sandaker (2016) when describing cultural selection and
selection of cultures. The present paper expands on Couto and Sandaker’s interpreta-
tion, identifying two possible recurrent sets of IBCs within metacontingencies (cIBCs
and C-IBCs).

Acknowledgements The author would like to thank Ingunn Sandaker, Lucas de Carvalho, and Gunnar Ree
for their support and feedback during the writing process. Tara Grant, the two anonymous reviewers, and the
editor also gave valuable comments when revising this manuscript. The present text was adapted from the
introduction of the author’s PhD thesis.

References

Alem, S., Perry, C. J., Zhu, X., Loukola, O. J., Ingraham, T., Søvik, E., & Chittka, L. (2016). Associative
mechanisms allow for social learning and cultural transmission of string pulling in an insect. PLoS
Biology, 14(10), e1002564. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1002564.
Bowman-Perrott, L., Burke, M. D., Zaini, S., Zhang, N., & Vannest, K. (2016). Promoting positive behavior
using the good behavior game: A meta-analysis of single-case research. Journal of Positive Behavior
Interventions, 18(3), 180–190. https://doi.org/10.1177/1098300715592355.
Couto, K. C., & Sandaker, I. (2016). Natural, behavioral and cultural selection-analysis: An integrative
approach. Behavior and Social Issues, 25, 54–60. https://doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v25i0.6891.
Dawkins, R. (1989). The selfish gene (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
de Carvalho, I. C., & Todorov, J. C. (2016). Metacontingências e produtos agregados na lei de diretrizes e
bases da educação: primeiro o objetivo, depois como chegar lá [Metacontingencies and aggregated
product in the law and guidelines and basis for education: first the objective, then how to get there].
Revista Brasileira de Análise do Comportamento, 12(2), 75–85. https://doi.org/10.18542/rebac.v12
i2.4400.
Glenn, S. S. (1986). Metacontingencies in Walden two. Behavior Analysis and Social Action, 5, 2–8.
Glenn, S. S. (1988). Contingencies and metacontingencies: Toward a synthesis of behavior analysis and
cultural materialism. The Behavior Analyst, 11(2), 161–179. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03392470.
Glenn, S. S. (2004). Individual behavior, culture, and social change. The Behavior Analyst, 27(2), 133–151.
https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03393175.
Glenn, S. S., & Malott, M. E. (2004). Complexity and selection: Implications for organizational change.
Behavior and Social Issues, 13(2), 89–106. https://doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v13i2.378.
Glenn, S. S., Malott, M. E., Andery, M. A. P. A., Benvenuti, M., Houmanfar, R. A., Sandaker, I., et al. (2016).
Toward consistent terminology in a behaviorist approach to cultural analysis. Behavior and Social Issues,
25, 11–27. https://doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v25i0.6634.
Hardin, G. (1968). The tragedy of the commons. Science, 162, 1243–1248. https://doi.org/10.1126
/science.162.3859.1243.
Keller, F. S., & Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). Principles of psychology: A systematic text in the science of
behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Behavior and Social Issues

Malott, M. E., & Glenn, S. S. (2006). Targets of intervention in cultural and behavioral change. Behavior and
Social Issues, 15(1), 31–56. https://doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v15i1.344.
Moffett, M. W. (2012). Supercolonies of billions in an invasive ant: What is a society? Behavioral Ecology,
23(5), 925–933. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ars043.
Moffett, M. W. (2013). Human identity and the evolution of societies. Human Nature, 24(3), 219–267.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9170-3.
Ostrom, E. (1990). Governing the commons: The evolution of institutions for collective action. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
Robinson, G. E. (1992). Regulation of division of labor in insect societies. Annual Review of Entomology,
37(1), 637–665. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ento.37.1.637.
Ruse, M. (2012). The philosophy of human evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Sandaker, I. (2009). A selectionist perspective on systemic and behavioral change in organizations. Journal of
Organizational Behavior Management, 29(3–4), 276–293. https://doi.org/10.1080/01608060903092128.
Schmitt, D. R. (1998). Social behavior. In K. A. Lattal & M. Perone (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in
human operant behavior. Boston: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-1947-2_15.
Seeley, T. D., & Buhrman, S. C. (1999). Group decision making in swarms of honey bees. Behavioral Ecology
and Sociobiology, 45(1), 19–31. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002650050536.
Simonsen, B., Fairbanks, S., Briesch, A., Myers, D., & Sugai, G. (2008). Evidence-based practices in
classroom management: Considerations for research to practice. Education and Treatment of Children,
31(1), 351–380. https://doi.org/10.1353/etc.0.0007.
Skinner, B. F. (1951). How to teach animals. Scientific American, 185(2), 26–29. https://doi.org/10.1038
/scientificamerican1251-26.
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.
Skinner, B. F. (1981). Selection by consequences. Science, 213(4507), 501–504. https://doi.org/10.1126
/science.7244649.
Todorov, J. C. (2005). Laws and the complex control of behavior. Behavior and Social Issues, 14(2), 86–91.
https://doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v14i2.360.
Todorov, J. C. (2006). The metacontingency as a conceptual tool. Behavior and Social Issues, 15(1), 92–94.
https://doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v15i1.347.
Todorov, J. C. (2013). Conservation and transformation of cultural practices through contingencies and
metacontingencies. Behavior and Social Issues, 22, 64–73. https://doi.org/10.5210/bsi.v22i0.4812.
Todorov, J. C., Moreira, M., Prudêncio, M. R. A., & Pereira, G. C. C. (2004). O Estatuto da Criança e do
Adolescente como metacontingência. In M. Z. S. Brandão, F. C. S. Conte, F. S. Brandão, Y. K.
Ingberman, V. L. M. Silva, & S. M. Oliani (Eds.), Sobre comportamento e cognição: Contingências e
metacontingências, contextos sócio-verbais e o comportamento do terapeuta Vol. 13 (pp. 44–51). Santo
André: ESETec.
Wilson, D. S., & Wilson, E. O. (2007). Rethinking the theoretical foundation of sociobiology. Quarterly
Review of Biology, 82(4), 327–348. https://doi.org/10.1086/522809.
Wilson, E. O. (2000). Sociobiology. The new synthesis (25th anniversary ed.). Cambridge: Harvard University
Press.

You might also like