You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/283534949

Growth in language and literacy skills among children with disabilities in


inclusive early reading first classrooms

Article  in  Topics in Early Childhood Special Education · January 2013


DOI: 10.1177/0271121413477498

CITATIONS READS
18 607

3 authors, including:

Katherine Green Nicole Patton Terry


University of West Georgia Georgia State University
22 PUBLICATIONS   63 CITATIONS    31 PUBLICATIONS   364 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Parent-Child Interactive Early Communication Study. View project

iCARE: Instruction of Children At Risk or with Exceptionalities. A Head Start Partnership. View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Katherine Green on 07 November 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


477498
in Early Childhood Special EducationGreen et al.
© Hammill Institute on Disabilities 2011

Reprints and permission: http://www.


TEC33410.1177/0271121413477498Topics

sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Article
Topics in Early Childhood Special Education

Progress in Language and Literacy Skills


2014, Vol. 33(4) 249­–259
© Hammill Institute on Disabilities 2013
Reprints and permissions:

Among Children With Disabilities in sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav


DOI: 10.1177/0271121413477498
tecse.sagepub.com
Inclusive Early Reading First Classrooms

Katherine B. Green, MEd1, Nicole Patton Terry, PhD1,


and Peggy A. Gallagher, PhD1

Abstract
Quality literacy instruction in preschool can be critical to the future academic success for all children, but may be even
more so for children with disabilities. The purpose of this study was to examine progress in emergent literacy skills of
young children with disabilities, compared with their typical peers, in an inclusive preschool setting. Participants in this study
included 77 prekindergarteners with disabilities and 77 children with no identified disabilities who were matched based on
age, teacher, and school. Children were enrolled in inclusive Early Reading First prekindergarten classrooms. Results suggest
that although children with disabilities made significant gains mirroring the progress of their typical peers, as a group, they
did not catch up to the achievement of their typical peers. Children with disabilities showed the greatest progress in Print
Awareness and Recognizing Uppercase Letters. Implications for future instruction and research are outlined.

Keywords
emergent literacy, inclusion, disabilities, preschool

Learning to read is one of the most important skills for chil- Fortunately, researchers have found that emergent literacy
dren in our society. Preschoolers who exhibit well-developed skills can be taught to and learned by young children with
emergent literacy skills typically have better success in disabilities (e.g., Adams, Foorman, Lundberg, & Beeler,
all academic areas from elementary through high school 1998; Blachman, Ball, Black, & Tangle, 2000; Wagner,
(Lonigan, Burgess, & Anthony, 2009). Conversely, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1994). Furthermore, additional stud-
researchers have found that children who lack appropriate ies show that preschool children with disabilities demon-
early literacy skills are more likely to have difficulty acquir- strate growth in emergent literacy skills when they are given
ing reading skills, read less, and receive less practice a structured literacy-rich environment (Katims, 1994). The
than proficient readers (Allington, 1984; Snow, Burns, & current investigation focuses on the language and literacy
Griffin, 1998). Subsequently, children with difficulties in outcomes of children with disabilities in such an environ-
emergent literacy skills may fall even further behind their ment: Early Reading First (ERF) classrooms.
peers as they progress in school (Allington, 1984; Snow et As part of the No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, ERF
al., 1998). While receiving quality literacy instruction in programs focus on enhancing children’s early language and
preschool is critical to the future academic success for all literacy skills through curriculum adoption and/or enhance-
children, it is especially so for children who enter school ment, classroom modification, and teacher professional
developmentally behind their peers. With estimations that development (e.g., coaching, workshops, and classroom
more than one in three children experience difficulty learn- support). The purpose of ERF was to prevent later reading
ing to read (Adams, 1990; Shaywitz, Escobar, Shaywitz, difficulties by providing young children, particularly
Fletcher, & Makuch, 1992), it is important that emergent from lower socioeconomic backgrounds, with high-quality
literacy skills be specifically taught in the preschool
classroom. 1
Georgia State University, Atlanta, USA
Many children with disabilities struggle to acquire emer-
gent literacy skills that are associated with later literacy Corresponding Author:
Katherine B. Green, Department of Educational Psychology and Special
achievement such as oral vocabulary, phonological aware- Education, Georgia State University, P.O. Box 3979, Atlanta, GA 30302-
ness, and print and alphabet knowledge (Sulzby & Teale, 3979, USA.
1991; Teale & Sulzby, 1986; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998). Email: kgreen16@gsu.edu

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com by Katherine Green on November 6, 2015


250 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 33(4)

language and literacy-rich environments to prepare them and systematic instruction in homogeneous small groups
for school success. (Gersten et al., 2008; Gunn, Biglan, Smolkowski, & Ary,
2000; Jenkins, Peyton, Sanders, & Vadasy, 2004; Vadasy,
Sanders, & Peyton, 2005; Vaughn et al., 2006), as well as
Critical Emergent Language and progress monitoring of emergent literacy skills at least one
Literacy Skills and Instruction time per month (Mathes et al., 2005; McMaster, Fuchs,
Emergent literacy skills are predictive of later reading Fuchs, & Compton, 2005; Vaughn et al., 2006). Students
success and therefore should be emphasized in preschool who do not make adequate progress in Tiers 1 or 2 may
instruction (Chatterji, 2006; National Early Literacy Panel require more intensive, individualized daily instruction, as
[NELP], 2008; Scarborough, 2001). In a recent meta- found in Tier 3 interventions (Gillon, 2000; McMaster et al.,
analysis of studies on emergent literacy skills and instruc- 2005). The movement between tiers is based on academic
tion, NELP (2008) found that the skills with the strongest progress throughout the school year. The children receiving
predictive relationship with later literacy outcomes included instruction in Tiers 2 or 3 may or may not have an individu-
alphabet knowledge (letter names and sounds), phonologi- alized education plan (IEP; Greenwood et al., 2011).
cal awareness (ability to think about and manipulate sounds High-quality emergent literacy instruction is of impor-
in words), print concepts (knowledge of forms and use of tance to young children in the prevention of later eligibility
print), and oral language (ability to use and comprehend of special education, as little or no exposure to early literacy
language in communicative contexts). Moreover, the panel experiences places children at risk of later challenges in lan-
found that specific instructional strategies and approaches guage and literacy (Chard & Kameenui, 2000; Shonkoff &
can promote the development of these skills, including Phillips, 2000; Zill & Resnick, 2006). Furthermore, chil-
code-focused interventions (designed to teach skills related dren who attend preschool are placed at less risk of later
to understanding the alphabetic principle) and language- special education identification (Belfield, 2005), as well as
enhancement interventions (designed to improve expres- for learning disabilities (Conyers, Reynolds, & Ou, 2003)
sive and receptive oral language skills) in preschool and than those who do not attend preschool. Yet, with the
kindergarten programs. Specifically, phonological aware- expansion of inclusive placements within early childhood
ness instruction had the most significant and largest effect settings (Odom et al., 2004), it is important to examine how
size (0.82) of all code-focused interventions to later literacy these placements effect the academic skills of children with
skills, supporting the importance of phonological aware- disabilities and their typically developing peers.
ness in the early childhood setting. Overall, the findings
from the NELP report highlight not only the critical early
language and literacy skills that are likely to support future Early Literacy Achievement Among
reading success but also the wide variety of instructional Preschoolers With Disabilities in
approaches that can be taken during the preschool years to Inclusive Settings
promote growth in young children with and without dis-
abilities. Many children with a variety of disabilities may experience
Instructional strategies for early childhood classrooms challenges with learning emergent literacy skills. Children
can be discussed in terms of Response to Intervention with language impairments, particularly those with delays
(RTI). RTI is a multitiered prevention pyramid model in vocabulary, comprehension, syntax, and phonological
designed to detect, prevent, and address academic chal- awareness, are more likely to experience difficulty with
lenges of children. The primary or universal tier of RTI sup- conventional (e.g., decoding, oral reading fluency, reading
ports the academic needs of all children in the classroom. comprehension, writing, and spelling) and emergent liter-
Instruction in the universal tier may also be referred to as acy skills (e.g., oral language, print and letter knowledge,
evidence-based reading instruction (Vaughn & Fuchs, and phonological processing; Bishop & Adams, 1990;
2006) or high-quality reading instruction (Division for Catts, 1997; NELP, 2008; Scarborough, 1990; Wagner &
Learning Disabilities, 2007). In early primary years, univer- Torgesen, 1987; Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998). Children
sal strategies may include whole-class instruction with the with cognitive delays (e.g., Down syndrome), autism, and
critical emergent literacy skills (i.e., phonological aware- other developmental disabilities often have language
ness, alphabet knowledge, print concepts, and oral lan- impairments that are characteristic of their disability. For
guage), as well as differentiated early literacy instruction instance, children with autism may have difficulty with
such as varying the time, content, level of support, and scaf- spontaneous language, pragmatics, delayed grammar usage,
folding (Connor et al., 2009). Within the RTI model, Tier 2 oral language skills, and vocabulary development skills
interventions are designed for students who exhibit chal- (American Psychiatric Association, 2000; V. Smith, Mirenda,
lenges or weak progress with regular classroom instruction. & Zaidman-Zait, 2007; Wilkinson, 1998). Children with
Evidence-based Tier 2 strategies include intense, explicit, Down syndrome typically have stronger expressive

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com by Katherine Green on November 6, 2015


Green et al. 251

language skills than receptive skills, and have particular emergent literacy progress in children with disabilities and
challenges with phonology (e.g., phonological processes typically developing peers within inclusive settings. This
and poorer speech intelligibility) and syntax (e.g., delays in study will add to the literature base by examining progress
morphology and complex utterances; Martin, Klusek, in emergent literacy skills for all children in inclusive pre-
Estigarribia, & Roberts, 2009). school classrooms and to help determine whether a high-
Inclusion within early childhood settings is a primary quality language and literacy-based classroom can assist in
placement for many children with a variety of disabilities reducing the achievement gap between children with dis-
(Odom, Buysse, & Soukakou, 2011). Inclusion not only abilities and their typically developing peers. The purpose
refers to the placement of children with disabilities in the of this study was to examine progress in emergent literacy
same class as typically developing peers but also includes skills of young children with disabilities, compared with
the socialization and shared learning environments with typ- their typically developing peers, in an inclusive ERF pre-
ically developing peers (Division for Early Childhood/ school setting. The following research questions were posed:
National Association for the Education of Young Children,
2009). Within an inclusive environment, children with dis- Research Question 1: How much progress did children
abilities should be provided with the supports and services with disabilities experience in oral vocabulary, pho-
as necessary (Rafferty, Piscitelli, & Boettcher, 2003; Winter, nological awareness, and alphabet and print knowl-
1999), as well as equal opportunities within the same class- edge during the prekindergarten year, compared with
room (Odom et al., 1996; Peck, Odom, & Bricker, 1993). their typically developing peers?
Several studies have shown that children with and with- Research Question 2: Did the achievement gap
out disabilities benefit socially in inclusive settings (e.g., between children with disabilities and typical peers
Buysse & Bailey, 1993; Buysse, Goldman, & Skinner, narrow in oral vocabulary, phonological awareness,
2002; Cross, Traub, Hutter-Pishgahi, & Shelton, 2004; and alphabet and print knowledge over the course of
Gallagher & Lambert, 2006; Guralnick & Groom, 1988; the prekindergarten year?
Jenkins, Speltz, & Odom, 1985). However, fewer studies
have explored how children with disabilities progress in
specific emergent literacy skills within inclusive preschool Method
settings. In a study examining language development and Participants
social competence in inclusive and segregated settings of
children with disabilities, Rafferty et al. (2003) found no The sample was pooled from a larger evaluation database of
differential impact on the effect sizes on language and 652 children who participated in ERF classrooms. The data
social competences between inclusive and segregated set- were collected over 2 academic year periods. Of these 652
tings for preschoolers with mild to moderate disabilities. children, 77 had IEPs, spoke English as their first language
Holahan and Costenbader (2000) found that preschoolers (M age = 50 months, SD = 6.14), and exhibited adequate
functioning at a higher level of social and emotional skills speech and language skills to perform the assessment tasks
performed better on developmental outcomes in inclusive without adaptations. This is the population we chose to
settings as opposed to segregated settings. In an examina- study. These 77 children with disabilities were matched to
tion of changes in language development for preschoolers 77 children with no identified disabilities who also spoke
with autism and typically developing peers in segregated English as their first language (M age = 51 months, SD =
and integrated settings, Harris, Handleman, Kristoff, Bass, 5.1). The sample consisted of 37% female and 63% male
and Gordon (1990) found that all children, typically devel- participants. All children in the sample attended the ERF
oping and those with autism, benefitted from a quality lan- prekindergarten program 1 entire school year. Children
guage enriched inclusive preschool in the area of language were matched based on the school or child care site they
development. The instructional approaches consisted of attended, then the classroom, next their age in months, and
whole group, small group, and individualized instruction in finally their gender. There were no instances where matches
the inclusive environment. were not able to be narrowed using this hierarchy.
All children were enrolled in inclusive prekindergarten
classrooms that were participating in ERF. The 38 class-
Purpose of Study rooms were located in public elementary schools and pri-
One reason many children with disabilities may be placed vate child care sites in a large, urban area in the southeastern
in inclusive preschool classrooms is to improve their aca- United States. Inclusionary criteria for classrooms included
demic outcomes. Therefore, it is important to not only an ELLCO Pre-K (M. W. Smith & Dickinson, 2002)
examine whether children with disabilities show significant Literacy Environment Checklist average score of “basic”
progress in social skills but also to examine early academic (M = 3) for the Language and Literacy Environment items.
areas, as well. There is a paucity of research regarding Because these participating sites received ERF funding, all

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com by Katherine Green on November 6, 2015


252 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 33(4)

sites served children who lived in poverty, as indicated by Procedures


the percentage of children participating in federal free and
reduced lunch programs. Importantly, all of the children in this study attended ERF
The children with disabilities were receiving special classrooms during their prekindergarten year. Classroom
education services for a variety of disabilities commonly teachers who participated in ERF were provided with pro-
diagnosed by the preschool years, including developmen- fessional development to enhance children’s language, lit-
tal delays, autism, pervasive developmental disorders–not eracy, and cognitive skills. The teachers in this ERF
otherwise specified, speech and language impairments, program participated in approximately 50 hr of profes-
cognitive impairments, and Down syndrome. Although sional development on teaching language and literacy skills
each disability presents specific challenges, children in in the preschool classroom. They also had coaches and
this sample were all functioning at social, cognitive, weekly on-site support in their school.
behavioral, and linguistic levels to the extent their IEP The PPVT-3 was given to all children in the fall and
teams had recommended they participate in language and spring of their prekindergarten school year, per regulations
literacy instruction in the general education classroom of the ERF program. In addition, although not required,
with typical peers. In addition, only data from children some participating classrooms used the PALS-PreK to cap-
who were able to complete the tasks according to stan- ture additional information regarding the children’s early
dardized administrative format were included in the language and literacy skills. When this information was
analyses. available, it was used in the analyses for this study.
Assessments were administered at the child’s school in
standardized format according to test manuals by trained
Measures examiners. Data were analyzed to determine progress in the
Oral receptive vocabulary. Vocabulary was assessed using language and literacy skills of both groups of children.
the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Third Edition
(PPVT-3; Dunn & Dunn, 1997). The PPVT-3 is a nation-
ally normed assessment and is appropriate for ages 2 Results
through adulthood. On this assessment, children are pre- To answer the research questions, two types of primary
sented with an array of four pictures of common objects analyses were performed. First, pre–post repeated measures
and actions. Children are asked to identify a target picture ANOVAs were conducted to examine progress within the
by pointing (e.g., “point to a faucet”). Internal reliability groups and differences between the two groups on the
on the PPVT-3 is .92 to .98 (median = .95; PPVT-3; Dunn PPVT-3 and PALS-PreK, as well as any interaction effects.
& Dunn, 1997). The PPVT-3 and each of the PALS-PreK subtests were
analyzed separately. Gain scores were computed on all
Emergent literacy achievement. A variety of early literacy measures to examine how scores changed over the prekin-
skills were measured with the Phonological Awareness Lit- dergarten year and to determine whether the achievement
eracy Screening Prekindergarten (PALS-PreK; Invernizzi, gap between children with disabilities and typically devel-
Sullivan, Meier, & Swank, 2004). This battery of assess- oping peers was changed between the groups (see Table 1).
ments was chosen because it can be administered individu-
ally, and because many of the emergent literacy skills can
be assessed quickly and reliably. This study included the Progress in Early Language
following subtests of the PALS-PreK: (a) Alphabet and Literacy Skills
Knowledge—Children are asked to name uppercase let- Between-group results. Using repeated measures ANOVAs,
ters, children who name 16 or more correctly are asked to the data were analyzed to determine the differences in prog-
name the lowercase letters, and children who name nine or ress on language and literacy skills from fall to spring
more lowercase letters are asked to say the sounds that the between the typically developing peers and children with
letters make; (b) Beginning Sound Awareness—Children disabilities. As expected, all children exhibited progress
are presented with pictures and are asked to produce the during the prekindergarten year. Furthermore, typically
first sound for the words that begin with /s/, /m/, and /b/; developing children performed significantly better than
(c) Print and Word Awareness—Children are asked to children with disabilities on the PPVT-3, η2 = .177,
identify various text components while the examiner reads F(1, 142) = 30.483, p < .001, as well as on all subtests of the
a nursery rhyme printed in a book; and (d) Rhyme Awareness— PALS-PreK: Uppercase Letter Recognition, η2 = .060,
Children are presented with four pictures and are asked to F(1, 70) = 4.47, p < .05, Print Awareness, η2 = .171,
identify the two pictures that rhyme. Reliability ranges F(1, 84) = 17.269, p < .001, Beginning Sounds, η2 = .086,
from .75 to .93 for the four subtests (Invernizzi et al., F(1, 86) = 8.092, p < .05, and Rhyme Awareness, η2 = .167,
2004). F(1, 80) = 16.089, p < .001.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com by Katherine Green on November 6, 2015


Green et al. 253

Table 1.  Mean Group Performance and Gain Scores.


Children with disabilities N Fall scores (SD) Spring scores (SD) Percentage gained
PPVT-3 77 88.60 (21.12) 90.53 (17.9) 2.18
Uppercase letter recognition 36 13.69 (10.83) 18.36 (9.53) 34.11
Beginning sounds 44 4.09 (4.08) 5.20 (4.32) 27.14
Print awareness 43 3.79 (3.11) 5.95 (3.27) 56.99
Rhyme awareness 41 4.85 (3.04) 6.15 (3.40) 26.80
Typical peers
 PPVT-3 77 104.17 (16.28) 105.64 (15.52) 1.41
  Uppercase letter recognition 36 17.86 (8.69) 22.56 (5.79) 26.32
  Beginning sounds 44 5.68 (3.72) 7.91 (3.25) 39.26
  Print awareness 43 6.37 (2.74) 8.02 (1.86) 25.90
  Rhyme awareness 41 6.71 (2.70) 8.63 (1.85) 28.61
Note. PPVT-3 = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Third Edition. Scores on the PPVT-3 are in standard scores and PALS-PreK are in raw scores. Percentage
gained is the percent the students improved on the measure from fall to spring.

Figure 1.  Mean growth in standard scores on the PPVT-3 from fall to spring by group.
Note. PPVT-3 = Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Third Edition.

Within-group results. The data were also analyzed to examine Achievement gap between children with disabilities and typically
how each group progressed on the language and literacy developing peers. Given that children from both groups
tasks from fall to spring. On the PPVT-3, neither group showed significant progress on specific language and liter-
showed significant progress in standard scores from fall to acy tasks from fall to spring, we next computed gain scores
spring. However, unlike the PPVT-3, significant within- to see how the children’s skills changed over 1 academic
group differences were observed for both groups of chil- school year, and whether the achievement gap between the
dren on several subtests of the PALS-PreK. All children groups changed as well. There were no interactive effects;
performed significantly better from fall to spring on Upper- thus, there were no significant achievement gap changes for
case Letter Recognition, η2 = .387, F(1, 70) = 44.278, any measure for either group nor were there any signifi-
p < .001, Print Awareness, η2 = .456, F(1, 84) = 70.312, cant effects on within-subjects. On the PPVT-3, children in
p < .001, Beginning Sounds, η2 = .232, F(1, 86) = 25.938, both groups gained slightly less than two standard score
p < .001, and Rhyme Awareness, η2 = .262, F(1, 80) = 28.366, points by the spring (see Figure 1). Although significant
p < .001. Although both groups showed progress on all within-group differences were not found in mean standard
assessments, the effect sizes between pre- and post-assessments scores, there was a slight change from fall to spring narrow-
were considered small (Cohen, 1988). ing the performance gap between the groups by .46 points.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com by Katherine Green on November 6, 2015


254 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 33(4)

Figure 2.  Mean Growth in PALS-PreK Print and Word Awareness subtest from fall to spring.
Note. PALS-PreK = Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening Prekindergarten.

Figure 3.  Mean growth in PALS-PreK uppercase alphabet recognition subtest from fall to spring.
Note. PALS-PreK = Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening Prekindergarten.

Interestingly, a different pattern was found on the Discussion


PALS-PreK. Although children in both groups made prog- This study had two goals. First, we examined progress in
ress from fall to spring on each subtest, the performance emergent literacy skills in children with disabilities in
gap between typically developing children and children inclusive language and literacy-enriched prekindergarten
with disabilities narrowed on the Print Awareness subtest, classrooms as compared with their typically developing
specifically by .51 raw score points (see Figure 2). The classmates. Second, we explored whether the achievement
gap widened on the Uppercase Letter Recognition, Rhyme gap was narrowed between the children with disabilities
Awareness, and Beginning Sounds subtests by .03, .62, and children without identified disabilities. Overall, the
and 1.12 raw score points, respectively (see Table 1; see results suggested that children with disabilities made sig-
Figures 3–5). nificant progress in emergent literacy skills, mirroring the

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com by Katherine Green on November 6, 2015


Green et al. 255

Figure 4.  Mean growth in PALS-PreK Rhyme Awareness subtest from fall to spring.
Note. PALS-PreK = Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening Prekindergarten.

Figure 5.  Mean growth in PALS-PreK beginning sounds subtest from fall to spring.
Note. PALS-PreK = Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening Prekindergarten.

gains of their typically developing peers. Yet as a group, higher posttest scores than their matched peers. However,
the children with disabilities did not catch up with their as a group, the children with disabilities started further behind,
typically developing peers on any language and literacy task. and the progress patterns did not allow them to catch up to
Importantly, the typically developing peers and the group the achievement of their typically developing peers.
of children with disabilities varied in their individual Not surprisingly, typically developing children outper-
scores. Some participants with disabilities had higher formed the children with disabilities throughout the prekin-
pretest scores than did the typically developing peers. dergarten year. However, children with disabilities made
Furthermore, some children with disabilities exhibited similar gains in receptive vocabulary as the typically

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com by Katherine Green on November 6, 2015


256 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 33(4)

developing peers. Given the nature of standard scores, it is classrooms. The NELP (2008) report stated that alphabet
difficult to show progress on this measure over 1 school knowledge, phonological awareness, print concepts, and
year. Nevertheless, it is encouraging to find that children oral language have a strong predictive relationship with
with disabilities not only maintained their vocabulary skills later literacy outcomes. As children with disabilities experi-
but also showed some progress over the prekindergarten ence difficulties when learning these emergent literacy
year while participating in whole-classroom language and skills (Sulzby & Teale, 1991; Teale & Sulzby, 1986;
literacy instruction. Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998), and given the increase of
On the PALS-PreK, children with disabilities made the inclusive environments (Odom et al., 2011), this study adds
least amount of progress on the phonological awareness to the research on academic benefits of children with dis-
tasks, as opposed to their typically developing peers. The abilities in inclusive literacy-rich environments.
children with disabilities never caught up with typical peers Although positive, the results should be viewed with
on these tasks. Children with disabilities experienced the caution. There are limitations to this study. Because this
most amount of gain with the print awareness and recogniz- study involved analyses from a much larger data set, infor-
ing uppercase letters tasks. For the typically developing mation was missing regarding the exact nature of special
peers, the least amount of gain was in print awareness; how- education services the children with disabilities received,
ever, even on this task, the gains made by children with the specific special education eligibility category of each
disabilities did not allow them to surpass or match the post- individual child and information on individual ethnicities,
test score of their typical peers. and socioeconomic levels. This is a limitation that limits the
With regard to the achievement gap between the children interpretation and generalization of the results. Another
with disabilities at the beginning of the prekindergarten limitation is that not all of the children in this study were
year, there were no significant changes for any of the tasks, given the PALS-PreK assessment, as noted in Table 1.
as indicated by the lack of interaction effects within the Information regarding the missing pieces of descriptive
analysis of groups and measures. This finding was qualified data would be beneficial for future research. Nevertheless,
by an examination of the means. Children with disabilities this study is an important step in characterizing how chil-
progressed from the instruction they received, but only nar- dren with disabilities fare in language and literacy inclusive
rowed the achievement gap in oral expressive language and classrooms. It is also important to acknowledge that the
print awareness, and the gap actually widened on the pho- children in this study were participating in ERF classrooms,
nological awareness measures. where they were exposed to high-quality language and lit-
These findings can be interpreted as support that chil- eracy instruction and resources from trained teachers and
dren with disabilities may experience significant gains in coaches who supported that instruction. Certainly, this is
orthographic skills (e.g., alphabet recognition and print not the case in all prekindergarten classrooms. It is not clear
concepts) in inclusive settings. Meanwhile, phonological whether such positive results would have been found in
awareness instruction may require more explicit instruc- classrooms without these kinds of supports in place.
tion. Certainly, there is empirical evidence that suggests Implications for the classroom from this study include
that young children who struggle with phonological aware- the need to explicitly address phonological awareness
ness can benefit from explicit, small group, intensive skills to children with disabilities. These findings may be
instruction during the preschool years (Elbaum, Vaughn, interpreted in terms of RTI. For example, the results indi-
Hughes, & Moody, 1999; Phillips, Clancy-Menchetti, & cated that children with disabilities benefited from partici-
Lonigan, 2008; Pullen & Justice, 2003). Explicit instruction pating in high-quality language and literacy instruction in
includes the teaching of the most efficient and effective inclusive environments, such as would be seen in a Tier 1 or
method possible (Carnine, Silbert, Kame’enui, & Tarver, universal instruction. Yet, some students, such as the lower
2004). Within this instructional approach, the teacher leads achieving students may require additional instruction in
the instruction, determines the instructional goals and pace, phonological awareness skills possibly in small group set-
chooses the appropriate materials, and provides immediate tings, such as a Tier 2 intervention. It is possible that had the
corrective feedback to the student. Tasks may be broken lower achieving students received explicit, small group, or
down into smaller skills and are sequenced to allow for stu- individualized instruction commonly found in Tiers 2 or 3
dent mastery of prerequisite skills before moving on to of RTI, the achievement gap between typically developing
more challenging skills (Joyce, Weil, & Calhoon, 2000). peers and children with disabilities may have narrowed.
As with other research on this topic (e.g., Cross et al., Examples of current RTI frameworks and resources for pre-
2004; Harris et al., 1990; Holahan & Costenbader, 2000; kindergarten include Center for Response to Intervention
Rafferty et al., 2003), the findings from this study suggest in Early Childhood (CRTIEC, 2012; www.crtiec.org/),
that children with disabilities benefit in language and emer- Recognition and Response (Coleman, Buysse, & Neitzel,
gent literacy skills from participating in high-quality lan- 2006), and Exemplary Model of Early Reading Growth and
guage and literacy instruction in inclusive prekindergarten Excellence (EMERGE; Gettinger & Stoiber, 2008).

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com by Katherine Green on November 6, 2015


Green et al. 257

It is also important to note that the skills the children Funding


with disabilities had the most challenge with auditory-based
and more abstract concepts (e.g., beginning sounds and The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support
rhyming awareness). The skills they performed best in were for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This
orthographic and more concrete concepts (e.g., print and study was supported in part by the Developing Readers Early and
word awareness and letter recognition). Being aware of this Mightily (DREAM) and Reinforce, Educate, and Develop Early
information, teachers may need to have a greater instruc- Readers Successfully (READERS). Early Reading First Program
tional focus on teaching phoneme and rhyming awareness grants were awarded to the United Way of Greater Atlanta (Grant
tasks. Implications for future research areas include the Nos. S359BO50040 and S359BO60041).
increased need to study emergent literacy skills in children
with disabilities, particularly in inclusive environments, and References
examine what types of Tier 2 and 3 interventions might Adams, M. J. (1990). Learning to read: Thinking and learning
affect the academic gap for children with disabilities. about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
In sum, the findings in this study guide us toward future Adams, M. J., Foorman, B. R., Lundberg, I., & Beeler, T. (1998).
research that examines the academic implications of inclusive The elusive phoneme: Why phonemic awareness is so impor-
environments for young children with disabilities. Children tant and how to help children develop it. American Educator,
with disabilities are increasingly placed in inclusive prekin- 22, 18–29.
dergarten environments. Furthermore, many researchers have Allington, R. L. (1984). Content, coverage, and contextual reading
suggested that inclusion is socially beneficial for children in reading groups. Journal of Reading Behavior, 16, 85–86.
with and without disabilities (Buysse & Bailey, 1993; Buysse American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statis-
et al., 2002; Cross et al., 2004; Gallagher & Lambert, 2006; tical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washing-
Guralnick & Groom, 1988; Jenkins et al., 1985). However, ton, DC: Author.
there is a paucity of literature on the academic progress of Belfield, C. R. (2005). The cost savings to special education
children with disabilities in inclusive environments. This from preschooling in Pennsylvania. Harrisburg: Pennsylva-
investigation adds to the literature on academic skills of typi- nia Department of Education, Pennsylvania Build Initiative.
cal children and children with disabilities on inclusive literacy- Retrieved from http://www.portal.state.pa.us/portal/server.pt?
rich environments. This study was an attempt to answer open=18&;objID=381892&mode=2
questions about how all prekindergarten children progress Bishop, D. V. M., & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of
when participating in literacy-enriched environments, and the relationship between specific language impairment, pho-
indeed they did show progress. However, their growth is not nological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child
without complexity, as some skills may be best approached in Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 1027–1050.
other instructional contexts, specifically those related to pho- Blachman, B. A., Ball, E. W., Black, R., & Tangle, D. M. (2000).
nological awareness instruction, as the achievement gap Road to the code: A phonological awareness program for
between children with and without disabilities widened for young children. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brooks.
these skills. Overall, we view these findings as supportive of Buysse, V., & Bailey, D. B. (1993). Behavioral and developmental
inclusive language and literacy instruction during the prekin- outcomes in young children with disabilities in integrated and
dergarten year, and as a positive step toward understanding segregated settings: A review of comparative studies. Journal
the conditions under which children with disabilities can ben- of Special Education, 26, 434–461.
efit academically from participating in these classrooms. Buysse, V., Goldman, B. D., & Skinner, M. (2002). Setting effects
on friendship formation among young children with and with-
Acknowledgments out disabilities. Exceptional Children, 68, 503–517.
We would like to thank the Smart Start/Education staff at United Carnine, D. W., Silbert, J., Kame’enui, E. J., & Tarver, S. G.
Way Greater Atlanta, particularly Katrina D. Mitchell, for their (2004). Direct instruction reading (4th ed.). Upper Saddle
assistance with this project. We especially thank the children and River, NJ: Pearson.
families who participated in this project, without whom this Catts, H. W. (1997). The early identification of language-based
research would not have been possible. reading disabilities. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services
in Schools, 28, 86–87.
Authors’ Note Center for Response to Intervention in Early Childhood. (2012).
The opinions expressed in this article are authors’ and do not Available from http://www.crtiec.org/
represent views of the funding agencies. Chard, D. J., & Kameenui, E. J. (2000). Struggling first-grade
readers: The frequency and progress of their reading. Journal
Declaration of Conflicting Interests of Special Education, 34, 28–38.
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with Chatterji, M. (2006). Reading achievement gaps, correlates, and
respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this moderators of early reading achievement: Evidence from the
article. Early Childhood Longitudinal Study (ECLS) kindergarten to

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com by Katherine Green on November 6, 2015


258 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 33(4)

first grade sample. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, Greenwood, C. R., Bradfield, T., Kaminski, R., Linas, M., Carta, J. J.,
489–507. & Nylander, D. (2011). The response to intervention (RTI)
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sci- approach in early childhood. Focus on Exceptional Children,
ences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 43, 1–21.
Coleman, M. R., Buysse, V., & Neitzel, J. (2006). Recognition Gunn, B., Biglan, A., Smolkowski, K., & Ary, D. (2000). The effi-
and response: An early intervening system for young children cacy of supplemental instruction in decoding skills for His-
at risk for learning disabilities (Full report). Chapel Hill: The panic and non-Hispanic students in early elementary school.
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, FPG Child Devel- Journal of Special Education, 34, 90–103.
opment Institute. Guralnick, M. J., & Groom, J. M. (1988). Peer interactions in
Connor, C. M., Piasta, S. B., Fishman, B., Glasney, S., Schatschneider, C., mainstreamed and specialized classrooms: A comparative
Crowe, E., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). Individualizing student analysis. Exceptional Children, 54, 415–425.
instruction precisely: Effects of child by instruction interac- Harris, S. L., Handleman, J. S., Kristoff, B., Bass, L., & Gordon, R.
tions on first graders’ literacy development. Child Develop- (1990). Changes in language development among autistic and
ment, 80, 77–100. peer children in segregated and integrated preschool settings.
Conyers, L. M., Reynolds, A. J., & Ou, S. (2003). The effect of Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 23–31.
early childhood intervention and subsequent special education Holahan, A., & Costenbader, V. (2000). A comparison of develop-
services: Findings from the Chicago child-parent centers. Edu- mental gains for preschool children with disabilities in inclu-
cational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 25, 75–95. sive and self-contained classrooms. Topics in Early Childhood
Cross, A. G., Traub, E. K., Hutter-Pishgahi, L., & Shelton, G. Special Education, 20, 224–235.
(2004). Elements of successful inclusion for children with sig- Invernizzi, M., Sullivan, A., Meier, J., & Swank, L. (2004). Pho-
nificant disabilities. Topics in Early Childhood Special Educa- nological Awareness Literacy Screening PreK (PALS-PreK).
tion, 24, 169–183. Charlottesville: University of Virginia.
Division for Early Childhood/National Association for the Educa- Jenkins, J. R., Peyton, J. A., Sanders, E. A., & Vadasy, P. F.
tion of Young Children. (2009). Early childhood inclusion: (2004). Effects of reading decodable texts in supplemental
A joint position statement of the division for early childhood first-grade tutoring. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 53–85.
(DEC) and the national association for the education of young Jenkins, J. R., Speltz, M. L., & Odom, S. L. (1985). Integrating
children (NAEYC). Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina, normal and handicapped preschoolers: Effects on child devel-
FPG Child Development Institute. opment and social interaction. Exceptional Children, 52, 7–17.
Division for Learning Disabilities. (2007). Thinking about Joyce, B., Weil, M., & Calhoon, E. (2000). Models of teaching
response to intervention and learning disabilities: A teacher’s (6th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
guide. Arlington, VA: Author. Katims, D. S. (1994). Emergence of literacy in preschool children
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary with disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 17, 58–69.
Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Lonigan, C., Burgess, S., & Anthony, J. (2009). Development of
Elbaum, B., Vaughn, S., Hughes, M., & Moody, S. W. (1999). emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool chil-
Grouping practices and reading outcomes for students with dren: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study.
disabilities. Exceptional Children, 65, 399–415. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596–613.
Gallagher, P. A., & Lambert, R. G. (2006). Classroom quality, Martin, G. E., Klusek, J., Estigarribia, B., & Roberts, J. E. (2009).
concentration of children with special needs, and child out- Language characteristics of individuals with Down syndrome.
comes in Head Start. Exceptional Children, 73, 31–52. Topics in Language Disorders, 29, 112–132.
Gersten, R., Compton, D., Connor, C. M., Dimino, J., Santoro, L., Mathes, P. G., Denton, C., Fletcher, J., Anthony, J., Francis, D.,
Linan-Thompson, S., & Tilly, W. D. (2008). Assisting students & Schatschneider, C. (2005). The effects of theoretically
struggling with reading: Response to Intervention and multi- different instruction and student characteristics on the skills
tier intervention for reading in the primary grades. A practice of struggling readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 40,
guide (NCEE 2009-4045). Washington, DC: National Center 148–182.
for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of McMaster, K. L., Fuchs, D., Fuchs, L. S., & Compton, D. L.
Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education. Retrieved (2005). Responding to nonresponders: An experimental field
from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/publications/practiceguides/ trial of identification and intervention methods. Exceptional
Gettinger, M., & Stoiber, K. (2008). Applying a response- Children, 71, 445–463.
to-intervention model for early literacy development in National Early Literacy Panel. (2008). Developing early literacy:
low-income children. Topics in Early Childhood Special Report of the National Early Literacy Panel. Washington, DC:
Education, 27, 198–213. National Institute for Literacy.
Gillon, G. T. (2000). The efficacy of phonological awareness inter- Odom, S. L., Buysse, V., & Soukakou, E. (2011). Inclusion for
vention for children with spoken language impairment. Lan- young children with disabilities: A quarter century of research
guage, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 31, 126–141. perspectives. Journal of Early Intervention, 33, 344–356.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com by Katherine Green on November 6, 2015


Green et al. 259

Odom, S. L., Peck, C. A., Hanson, M., Beckman, P. J., Lieber, J., Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Prevent-
Brown, W. H., & Schwartz, I. S. (1996). Inclusion at the pre- ing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC:
school level: An ecological systems analysis. Social Policy National Academy Press.
Report: Society for Research in Child Development, 10,18–30. Sulzby, E., & Teale, W. H. (1991). The development of the young
Odom, S. L., Vitztum, J., Wolery, R., Lieber, J., Sandall, S., child and the emergence of literacy. In J. Flood, D. Lapp,
Hanson, M. J., & Horn, E. (2004). Preschool inclusion in the J. R. Squire, & J. M. Jensen (Eds.), Handbook of research on
United States: A review of research from an ecological sys- teaching the English language arts (pp. 300–313). Mahwah,
tems perspective. Journal of Research in Special Educational NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Needs, 4, 17–49. Teale, W. H., & Sulzby, E. (1986). Emergent literacy: Writing and
Peck, C. A., Odom, S. L., & Bricker, D. (1993). Integrating young reading. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
children with disabilities into community programs: Ecologi- Vadasy, P. F., Sanders, E. A., & Peyton, J. A. (2005). Relative
cal perspectives on research and implementation. Baltimore, effectiveness of reading practice or word-level instruction in
MD: Paul H. Brookes. supplemental tutoring: How text matters. Journal of Learning
Phillips, B. M., Clancy-Menchetti, J., & Lonigan, C. J. (2008). Disabilities, 38, 364–380.
Successful phonological awareness instruction with preschool Vaughn, S., & Fuchs, L. S. (2006). A response to competing
children: Lessons from the classroom. Topics in Early Child- views: A dialogue on response to intervention. Assessment for
hood Special Education, 28, 3–17. Effective Intervention, 32, 58–61.
Pullen, P. C., & Justice, L. M. (2003). Enhancing phonological Vaughn, S., Mathes, P., Linan-Thompson, S., Cirino, P., Carlson, C.,
awareness, print awareness, and oral language skills in pre- Pollard-Durodola, S., & Francis, D. (2006). Effectiveness of
school children. Intervention in School & Clinic, 39, 87–98. an English intervention for first-grade English language learn-
Rafferty, Y., Piscitelli, V., & Boettcher, C. (2003). The impact of ers at risk for reading problems. Elementary School Journal,
inclusion on language development and social competence among 107, 153–180.
preschoolers with disabilities. Exceptional Children, 69, 467–479. Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonologi-
Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic cal processing and its casual role in the acquisition of reading
children. Child Development, 61, 1728–1743. skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.
Scarborough, H. S. (2001). Connecting early language and liter- Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Devel-
acy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and prac- opment of reading-related phonological processing abilities:
tice. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent vari-
of early literacy research (Vol. 1, pp. 97–110). New York, able longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30,
NY: Guilford. 73–87.
Shaywitz, S. E., Escobar, M. D., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and
& Makuch, R. (1992). Evidence that dyslexia may represent emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848–872.
the lower tail of the lower distribution of reading ability. Wilkinson, K. M. (1998). Profiles of language and communication
New England Journal of Medicine, 326, 145–150. skills in autism. Mental Retardation and Developmental Dis-
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (2000). From neurons to neigh- abilities Research Reviews, 4, 73–79.
borhoods: The science of early childhood development. Winter, S. (1999). The early childhood inclusion model: A pro-
Washington, DC: National Academy Press. gram for all children. Olney, MD: Association for Childhood
Smith, M. W., & Dickinson, D. K. (2002). User’s guide to the Education International.
early language and literacy classroom observation toolkit. Zill, N., & Resnick, G. (2006). Emergent literacy of low-income
Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. children in Head Start: Relationships with child and family
Smith, V., Mirenda, P., & Zaidman-Zait, A. (2007). Predictors of characteristics, program factors, and classroom quality. In
expressive vocabulary growth in children with autism. Journal D. K. Dickenson & S. B. Neuman (Eds.), Handbook of early
of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 149–160. literacy research (Vol. 2, pp. 347–371). New York, NY: Guilford.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com by Katherine Green on November 6, 2015

View publication stats

You might also like