You are on page 1of 14

Topics in Early Childhood Special

Education http://tec.sagepub.com/

A Program for Improving Toddler Communication Through Parent Coaching


Heather W. Moore, Erin E. Barton and Maria Chironis
Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 2014 33: 212 originally published online 14 August 2013
DOI: 10.1177/0271121413497520

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://tec.sagepub.com/content/33/4/212

Published by:
Hammill Institute on Disabilities

and

http://www.sagepublications.com

Additional services and information for Topics in Early Childhood Special Education can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://tec.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://tec.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

>> Version of Record - Jan 2, 2014

OnlineFirst Version of Record - Aug 14, 2013

What is This?

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014


497520
research-article2013
TECXXX10.1177/0271121413497520Topics in Early Childhood Special EducationMoore et al.

Topical Article
Topics in Early Childhood Special Education

A Program for Improving Toddler


2014, Vol. 33(4) 212­–224
© Hammill Institute on Disabilities 2013
Reprints and permissions:
Communication Through Parent sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/0271121413497520

Coaching tecse.sagepub.com

Heather W. Moore, PhD, CCC-SLP1, Erin E. Barton, PhD, BCBA-D2,


and Maria Chironis, MS1

Abstract
The purpose of this manuscript was to describe a community-based program, Language and Play Everyday (LAPE), aimed
at evaluating effective practices for enhancing parents’ capacity to increase their toddlers’ communication skills. LAPE was
a parent education program focused on coaching parents to embed naturalistic language-enhancing strategies within daily
routines. Participants included eight families of toddlers with expressive communication delays ranging in age from 22 to
36 months. LAPE was delivered using group and individual sessions. After participating in the program, parents increased
their responsivity and use of other language-enhancing behaviors. Social validity measures indicated that parents were
satisfied with procedures, goals, and outcomes of the project. Moreover, children improved their expressive language
skills. Implications for future research and application are discussed.

Keywords
infants and toddlers, speech and language delays, parent coaching

Language development is a primary task of the first few Children with language delays are more likely to experi-
years of life. Delays in language often are primary indica- ence literacy delays (van der Schuit, Peeters, Segers, van
tions of developmental delays and are likely to impact Balkom, & Verhoeven, 2009) and social, emotional, or
development across all domains. While there is variation in behavioral problems (van Daal, Verhoeven, & van Balkom,
the rate and timing of language development, the sequence 2007). The effects of language impairments are cumulative
of language milestones is generally predictable among chil- and might impact later school success if not addressed
dren within cultures. Most typically developing toddlers early.
acquire a core vocabulary of functional words by their 2nd
year of life (Crais, 1990). However, some toddlers exhibit
delayed vocabulary acquisition associated with identified
Family-Centered Approach
disorders (e.g., Down syndrome) or diagnosed develop- A family-centered approach is central to the general phi-
mental delays (American Speech-Language-Hearing losophy and framework of EI (McWilliam, 2010, 2012;
Association, 2005). In addition to children with known dis- Powell & Dunlap, 2010). Research consistently supports EI
orders and delays, 15% of toddlers develop language at a approaches that focus on enhancing parent and caregiver
slower pace without any concomitant delays in other devel- capacity to meet the needs of infants and toddlers (Bruder &
opmental domains (Desmarais, Sylvestre, Meyer, Bairati, & Dunst, 2000). For example, research indicated that increased
Rouleau, 2008). While a subset of these toddlers will catch parent responsivity is associated with enhanced child lan-
up to typically developing children by the preschool years, guage skills (Dunst & Trivette, 2009a) and improved devel-
some will have persistent delays (Paul, 2000; Rescorla, opmental outcomes (Baggett et al., 2010). Several recent
2002). Recent federal data indicated that the number of
infants and toddlers with delays served under Part C Early 1
University of Oregon, Eugene, USA
Intervention (EI) increased from just under 190,000 (1.6% 2
University of Colorado Denver, USA
of all infants and toddlers) in 1998 to over 320,000 (2.2%)
Corresponding Author:
in 2007 (U.S. Department of Education, 2012) and sug- Heather W. Moore, Communication Disorders and Sciences Program,
gested that 8% to 12% of preschool children have language 5284 University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403-5284, USA.
delays (U.S. Preventive Services Task Force, 2006). E-mail: hbmoore@uoregon.edu

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014


Moore et al. 213

Table 1.  LAPE Evidence-Based Family Coaching Practices.

Evidence-based Practices LAPE example


1.  Individualized approach to family coaching The individual home sessions were structured according to the family’s needs. Parents
chose the strategies they wanted to practice.
2. Focus on daily routines with the family’s The individual home sessions were scheduled during routines the family identified. The
materials in natural settings coaches supported the parents in using the strategies during daily routines.
3. Focus on supporting family participation Initial group sessions focused on early language and play development and how parent
and building family capacity and interactions can influence development. Group sessions also allowed parents to
competence support each other’s efforts at home. Improvements in caregiver confidence were
measured over time.
4. Identify functional child outcomes a At the beginning of the training we asked parents to identify a functional goal for their
discussion about the family’s priorities child. To meet this goal, parents identified strategies they wanted to work on and
specific words they wanted to help their children say each week.
5. Works directly with the caregiver and During individual sessions, targeted strategies were reviewed and parents practiced
uses effective coaching strategies the strategies with their children while their coach observed. If needed, coaches
modeled strategies. After each practice activity, parents reflected on their use of the
strategies and ways they could incorporate them into daily routines. Then coaches
provided specific feedback, support, and suggestions.
6. Focus on caregiver responsivity and Parents were taught to identify times when their children were initiating
caregiver–child interactions communication. Then they were taught take conversational turns with their children
during play and daily interactions.
7. Use of videos for demonstration and Parent–child interactions were recorded prior to intervention. After the responsive
reflection strategies were introduced, parents watched the videos and rated their use of each
strategy. Then the coaches reviewed and discussed the videos again with the parents.
8. Identify opportunities to practice across At the end of each group and individual training, parents identified strategies they will
the day and in between visits use and target words they will teach in the coming week.
9.  Systematic progress monitoring Parents completed a daily log to rate their use of each strategy and a weekly log to
note their child’s progress on targeted vocabulary.

Note. LAPE = Language and Play Everyday.


Source. Evidence-based Practices are adapted from Dunst and Kassow (2007), McWilliam (2010), Powell and Dunlap (2010), Rush and Shelden (2011).

reviews and meta-analyses support EI approaches that focus 2010). A family-centered approach means practitioners
on enhancing parents’ responsivity and capacity to support focus on increasing the quality of parent−child interactions
their children’s development. Dunst and his colleagues within daily routines and activities, which is likely to
identified specific essential characteristics of parent respon- enhance family quality of life (McWilliam, 2010). Several
sivity related to improved child developmental outcomes. family coaching practices have been identified based on
Effective parents responded contingently, based on the family-centered practices and adult learning principles
child’s focus or intent, and in mutually reinforcing ways (Powell & Dunlap, 2010; Rush & Shelden, 2011; Sandall,
(Dunst & Trivette, 2009a; Trivette, Dunst, & Hamby, Hemmeter, Smith, & McLean, 2005). These coaching prac-
2010). Moreover, approaches that promoted full participa- tices (see Table 1) emphasize the families’ strengths and
tion of families in assessment and intervention were related values, identify natural learning opportunities within daily
to improved child outcomes (Dunst, Trivette, & Hamby, routines and activities, and use evidence-based adult learn-
2006, 2007, 2008a). ing strategies to build parents’ capacity to enhance their
For infants and toddlers, responsive interactions with children’s development (Campbell & Sawyer, 2007).
parents during daily routines and activities provide essential
opportunities for learning and promoting development. In
Parent Implemented Naturalistic
fact, the influence of parents on child development is criti-
cal; parents are the most significant facilitators of learning Language Interventions
for infants and toddlers (Powell & Dunlap, 2010). Thus, EI Early language development is largely impacted by every-
should focus on family coaching (i.e., supporting their abil- day conversations between parents and their children (Hart
ity to enhance their child’s development) rather than pro- & Risley, 1995; Kaiser & Roberts, 2011; Roberts & Kaiser,
viding direct service to the child. This focus on the family 2011). Language development is likely directly related to
ensures the child has multiple learning opportunities during the quality and quantity of the language input received,
meaningful routines and activities (Jung, 2003; McWilliam, which occurs within the context of social interactions and is

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014


214 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 33(4)

individually determined. Parent education programs that Stahmer & Mandell, 2007). Additional research is needed
focus on supporting child language development can be to examine strategies for improving parent education curri-
highly effective (Kaiser & Roberts, 2011). Naturalistic lan- cula that can be cost-effective and efficiently implemented
guage interventions focus on teaching parents to embed by practitioners within a variety of community settings.
language-enhancing strategies into daily routines and play. The purpose of this manuscript is to describe the devel-
Common characteristics of naturalistic teaching strategies opment of a parent-education program, Language and Play
include: using direct and natural reinforcement, focusing on Everyday (LAPE). The first and second authors developed
functional skills, using a variety of materials, and following LAPE to address a community need for an efficient and
the child’s interests and lead (McGee, Morrier, & Daly, cost-effective, community-based parent-education pro-
1999). The use of these strategies allow for authentic learn- gram, focused on increasing parents’ use of naturalistic
ing experiences and greater generalization of language language-enhancing strategies and improving communica-
skills (Kashinath, Woods, & Goldstein, 2006). Research tion skills in their toddlers’ with expressive communication
indicates that naturalistic teaching strategies are feasible— delays. The community need paralleled national research on
meaning parents can implement them with high fidelity, Part C EI services. That is, research has shown that the ser-
and effective—meaning they are functionally related to vices families receive through Part C were fewer than rec-
improvements in child language development (Roberts & ommended, and not all eligible children and families were
Kaiser, 2011). being served (Dunst & Trivette, 2009b; Hebbeler et al.,
Evidence-based naturalistic teaching approaches include 2012; Hume, Bellini, & Pratt, 2005). For example, the
enhanced milieu teaching (EMT; Hancock & Kaiser, 2006) National Early Intervention Longitudinal Study (NEILS;
and responsive education-prelinguistic milieu teaching Hebbeler et al., 2007) found families received an average of
(RE-PMT; Warren et al., 2006). EMT is a hybrid approach 1.5 hours of Part C services per week, families missed about
that incorporates environmental arrangement, responsive one fourth of their scheduled visits, and the amount of fund-
interaction, and milieu strategies to increase expressive and ing per child has decreased by an estimated 50% over the
pragmatic language in children with emerging language last decade (Hebbeler et al., 2012). This report concluded
skills. Several studies suggest that parent-implemented that overall, families received relatively few face-to-face
EMT is an effective and efficient intervention for late- services. Moreover, for 44% of the families, the services
talking toddlers (Kaiser, Hancock, & Nietfeld, 2000; focused only on the child, rather than the family, which is
Kashinath et al., 2006; Roberts & Kaiser, 2011). In addi- unlikely to have a larger impact on the child’s development.
tion, parents consistently learned the strategies and reported LAPE was developed to address this community need by
feeling confident in their ability to use EMT following using a cost-effective, group format to teach parents to: (a)
training (Peterson, Carta, & Greenwood, 2005). RE-PMT identify and respond to their children’s communication
uses the same intervention strategies as EMT but focuses on attempts, (b) provide opportunities for their toddlers to
teaching prelinguistic language skills (i.e., intentional, non- communicate, and (c) use developmentally appropriate lan-
verbal communication). The research on RE-PMT indicates guage. Specific strategies and supports were identified
it also is an effective, parent-implemented intervention for based on the individual needs of each child and family. The
children (Fey et al., 2006; Yoder & Warren, 2002). components of the program, evaluation procedures, pro-
Research also supports implementing these approaches gram outcomes, and implications for future research and
with parents during individual and group trainings. Group practice are described.
training, with individualized supports, allows information
to be shared in a quick and concise format, provides space
and time for shared parent support, and might be less costly Description of Program
(Carter et al., 2011; Kaiser, Hemmeter, Ostrosky, Alpert, &
Hancock, 1995). However, the most popular group training
Participants
programs (e.g., Hanen Centre, 2011) are costly for interven- Thirteen parents and their toddlers with expressive com-
tionists, not well suited for current Part C systems, and munication delays participated in LAPE across three ses-
might not be effective for all families because they are not sions. However, only eight parent−child dyads attended all
easily individualized (Carter et al., 2011). Furthermore, sessions and complete all assessment; thus, only these
although there are empirically supported intervention eight were included in these analyses. Five additional
approaches for enhancing parents’ capacity to promote their parent−child dyads participated in LAPE sessions, but did
child’s language development, there is a significant research not complete all assessments or missed sessions. Parents
to practice gap. Recent research indicates evidence-based were recruited from a local EI agency and local early child-
practices are not being implemented in EI, and the amounts hood programs (seven mothers and one father participated).
of services families receive are fewer than recommended LAPE was a voluntary program. Six of the eight children
(Dunst & Trivette, 2009b; Hebbeler, Spiker, & Kahn, 2012; were receiving additional EI services (see Table 2). Parents

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014


Moore et al. 215

Table 2.  Child Demographic Characteristics.

PLS-IVb VABS-IIc
Dyad Disability statusa Chronological age (months) Race of child Other community services AC EC DLS SS Motor
1 DD 31 White/Caucasian EI toddler group 85 74 83 87 114
2 DD 31 Multiracial EI toddler group 77 69 98 89 72
3 SELD 25 White/Caucasian No other services 128 83 117 89 90
4 DD 25 White/Caucasian EI toddler group 81 79 105 95 111
5 SD 32 White/Caucasian SLP services 81 77 95 93 93
6d SD 24 White/Caucasian SLP services 81 (67) 96 (79) 77 82 90
7e SELD 28 White/Caucasian No other services 78 73 87 78 88
8 DD 32 White/Caucasian EI toddler group 50 50 73 78 79
a
DD = developmental disability; SELD = specific expressive language delay; SD = structural disability (e.g., cleft lip/palate). bPLS-IV = Preschool Language
Scale (4th ed.); AC = Auditory Comprehension subtest; EC = Expressive Communication subtest. cVABS-II = Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (2nd
ed.); DLS = Daily Living Skills domain; SS = Socialization domain; Motor = Motor Skills domain. dThis child was 7 days away from turning 24 months at
the time of testing. The PLS-IV scores in parentheses are after rounding up his age to 24 months. eOnly father–child dyad.

varied in age (i.e., ages ranged from 29 to 41 years), educa- embed a 6-week break after the first individual session.
tion (i.e., two reported “some college,” one reported “com- Moreover, they replaced the final group session with an
munity college degree,” four reported “Bachelor’s degree,” individual session. Thus, for cohort 3, the total session
and one reported “master’s degree”), and income level length was 15 weeks. This was intended to promote mainte-
(i.e., four reported an income level between nance of parent use of LAPE strategies and individualize
“US$20,000-US$50,000” and four reported an income the final session to the needs of the dyad. Although there
level between “US$50,000-US$80,000”). Inclusion crite- were variations in the format, the curriculum was identical
ria for children were (a) chronological age between 24 and across cohorts.
36 months, (b) verbal vocabulary between 10 and 50 words
as measured by the MacArthur-Bates Communicative
Interventionists
Development Inventory (Words and Sentences Form;
CDI-II; Fenson et al., 2007), and (c) had parents who The first author, a speech-language pathologist, led all
attended all sessions and completed all assessments. group sessions and graduate students in Early Intervention/
Early Childhood Special Education (EI/ECSE) and
Communication Disorders Sciences (CDS) were the pri-
Training Structure and Settings mary interventionists. They received practicum course
The LAPE program consisted of group and individual ses- credit for participating. Across the three cohorts, a total of
sions across three cohorts of parents and toddlers. The 12 graduate students participated in LAPE. Each parent was
cohorts were conducted sequentially over 1 year. Two or paired with a student who served as his or her “coach” for
three parent−child dyads were included in these analyses the duration of LAPE. The students also administered the
from each cohort (Cohort 1 included three of four initial assessment tests, participated in the group sessions,
parent−child dyads, Cohort 2 included two of five and led individual sessions. The first and second author, an
parent−child dyads, and Cohort 3 included three of four early childhood special educator, taught the students the
parent−child dyads). The group sessions occurred in a treatment protocol, oversaw their individual session plans,
classroom at a community school and the individual ses- and closely supervised them during all assessment and
sions occurred in the families’ homes. Individual sessions coaching activities.
were 1 hr and group sessions ranged from 90 min to 2 hr
based on group preferences (i.e., Cohorts 1 and 3 requested
Parent Strategies
2-hr sessions and Cohort 2 requested 90-min sessions). For
the first two cohorts, the group and individual sessions were LAPE followed a family coaching approach to teach par-
staggered across 7 weeks (i.e., two group sessions, one indi- ents to use naturalistic language teaching strategies (see
vidual session, two group sessions, one individual session, Tables 1 and 3). LAPE strategies were chosen because they
and a final group session). Because the parents in Cohorts 1 were empirically supported, easily embedded into daily
and 2 did not maintain use of LAPE strategies at follow-up, routines, and appropriate given the needs of toddlers.
the authors decided to stagger Cohort 3’s sessions and Moreover, several studies have demonstrated parents can

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014


216 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 33(4)

Table 3.  LAPE Strategies and Dependent Variables.

LAPE strategy/description Corresponding dependent variable


Wait and respond. Wait for your child to communicate and then Responsivity (RE) = Number of times the adult responded
respond accordingly. to child’s words or actions / Number of spoken adult turns
Talk in short, simple sentences. When talking directly to your Target Level Talk (TLT) = Number of utterances spoken
child, use sentences your child would be likely to say. by the adult within 1–3 words of child’s identified MLU /
Number of spoken adult utterances
Answer instead of asking. Avoid asking yes/no and “test” Questions (Q) = Number of adult yes/no questions + test
questions (e.g., What color is this? How many do you have?). questions / Number of spoken adult utterances
Repeat and add. When your child says a word or sentence, repeat Expansions (EXP) = Number of times the adult expanded
what s/he said and add a word. child’s utterance by adding a new and related word,
phoneme, or grammatical marker.
Do what your child does and talk about it. Imitate your child’s Verbal Mapping (VM) = Number of times the adult imitated
actions, and say what s/he would say. child’s nonverbal action and added a word or sentence.
Say what your child would say. Respond to your child’s Incidental Teaching (IT) = Number of times the adult
spontaneous requests/comments by saying the word/words your responded to the child’s communication attempt by saying a
child might say in a way that encourages/prompts her/him to word the child might say and Model (M) = Number of times
imitate you. the adult modeled the name of an object or action with the
intention that the child would imitate it.
Give your child a choice. Say the words for the items as you Forced Choice (FC) = number of times the adult verbally
provide choices, such as “do you want milk or juice?” gave a choice between 2 or more objects.
Environmental Arrangement. Give your child more These were not coded, but were designed to create
opportunities to communicate by setting up situations where your opportunities for communicative interactions and promote
child wants/needs to (e.g., Ask for more of something, Seek your the child’s initiations.
assistance, Comment on something new or silly).

Note. Due to low frequency of occurrence, the following related variables were combined into one variable, titled other language-enhancing behaviors
(O-LEBs) = EXP + VM + IT + FC + M / Number of total spoken adult utterances. LAPE = Language and Play Everyday.

implement these strategies with training and support toddler goals included: “[Child] will use 25 new words by
(see Roberts & Kaiser, 2011, for a meta-analysis of this the end of LAPE.” Examples of parent goals were “I will
research). learn the best strategies to use,” and “I will get tips to help
my child talk.”
Subsequent group sessions started with a group discus-
Parent Training Components sion about progress. Each parent reported on her toddler’s
LAPE consisted of three major components: group parent progress and how she was utilizing previously taught strate-
support sessions, individual sessions in homes, and practice gies at home (not included in the second session). The first
in-between sessions. This was intentionally designed to author encouraged parents to reflect on, comment, and sup-
give parents opportunities to interact with other parents of port each other’s progress and offer suggestions as needed.
toddlers with communication delays; deliver content and Then parents were separated and met individually with their
model strategies in a supportive, cost-effective manner; dis- coaches to more specifically discuss and document progress
cuss opportunities to practice the strategies during daily at home. After the individual coaching activity, the first
routines in-between sessions; and provide individualized, author provided an overview of new strategies (see Table 4).
routine-based coaching. The following sections describe Parents also met individually with coaches at the end of
the LAPE program components. every session to plan their use of the strategies at home
(new and previously taught).
Group support sessions.  Table 4 outlines the content of the
parent support sessions. The goals of the first session were Individual sessions.  Each family also received individual ses-
to: (a) allow the parents to meet and learn more about each sions in their home 2 to 3 times during their participation in
other, (b) introduce basic communication and play concepts LAPE (see Table 4). The coaches used the family coaching
(e.g., early language and play milestones), and (c) help par- strategies described in Table 1. During the first home ses-
ents identify their toddler’s current communication skills. sion, parents and coaches watched the parent−toddler video
Parents also worked individual with their coaches to iden- sample collected prior to intervention and rated the parent’s
tify goals for their toddlers and themselves. For example, use of each of the LAPE strategies already introduced in the

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014


Moore et al. 217

Table 4.  LAPE Parent Group Session Content.

Sessiona New content Method Review from previous session Method


1 Introductions Group Group activity  
Overview / Schedule
  Review CDI-II and Individual review with coach  
sound repertoire
results
  Language and play Group discussion  
milestones
  Identify how your child Individual activity with group  
communicates discussion
  Identify parent and Individual discussion with coach  
child goals
2 Defining and identifying Group discussion then individual Discuss home play observations Group discussion
home routines (play, discussion with coach)
social, and daily)
  Responsive strategies Group discussion, video Confirm parent and child goals Individual discussion with
examples, role-play coach
3 Teaching new Group discussion, individual Discuss child progress, Group discussion and then
vocabulary through planning with coach homework, and use of individual discussion with
responsive strategies responsive strategies at home coach
4 Environmental Group discussion, modeling with Discuss child progress, Group discussion and then
arrangement classroom examples, individual homework, and use of individual discussion with
strategies discussion and planning with responsive strategies at home coach
coach
5 Modeling Group discussion, modeling, role- Discuss child progress, Group discussion and then
play, individual discussion with homework, and use of individual discussion with
coaches responsive strategies, coach
6 Individual conferences to review child and parent progress environmental arrangement
and make recommendationsb strategies at home

Note. LAPE = Language and Play Everyday.


a
Fidelity was measured using group session checklists for each session. bCovered in final individual session for Cohort 3.

group sessions. Based on this discussion, the parent chose two strategies (previously learned in group sessions) to
two strategies to focus on during the remainder of the visit focus on during the visit, practiced the strategies, reflected,
(e.g., say what your child would say, target level talk). The and received feedback (as described above). Before the end
coach and parent discussed the strategies and the coach of the visit, the coach and parent set up a plan for practicing
modeled use of the strategies. Then, the parent initiated a all LAPE strategies during the family’s daily routines
routine (e.g., mealtime, diapering) or a play activity (e.g., at home.
book reading, playing with toys) with their toddler and
practiced the strategies for 5 to 10 min. Following the activ- In-between session practice.  As previously mentioned, at the
ity, the coach encouraged the parent to self-reflect on her or end of each group and individual session, the parents identi-
his use of the targeted strategies. For example, the coach fied when and how they planned to practice the LAPE strat-
might have asked the parent to talk about what went well egies at home and wrote their ideas on a weekly LAPE
during the routine and what was difficult. Then, the coach strategy log. After the first home coaching session, parents
provided specific, performance-based feedback (e.g., were asked to practice the strategies 15 min per day during
“When you used just two words to say “hat on” he imitated routine and play activities with their children and rate their
you!” or “Wow. You were really responsive when he progress on their weekly logs.
walked into the playroom. You followed and resumed your
play there.” Or “When you waited for her to initiate pulling
Evaluation Procedures
on her pants, she said ‘help’!”). At the beginning of each
subsequent visit, the coach and parent had a general discus- Toddler standardized measures.  To fully describe the partici-
sion about the child’s language progress and his or her use pants before intervention, two assessments were adminis-
of the LAPE strategies at home. The parent then identified tered with each toddler (see Table 2). The Preschool

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014


218 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 33(4)

Language Scale 4th edition (PLS-4; Zimmerman, Steiner, independently coded by a second coder to calculate interob-
& Pond, 2002) was administered to measure the child’s server agreement (IOA). IOA was calculated using the
receptive and expressive language. The Vineland Adaptive point-by-point method (Kennedy, 2005). IOA averaged
Behavior Scales (VABS-II; Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Balla, 90.7% (82.0%–96.6%) across dyads, sessions, behaviors,
2005) was completed through parent interview and was and time.
used to measure the child’s daily living, social, and motor
skills. In addition, the parent-completed CDI-2 measured Fidelity.  The authors measured implementation fidelity and
the child’s verbal vocabulary and was used as part of the intervention fidelity. Implementation fidelity refers to the
inclusion criteria and as an outcome measure. procedures used to support the parents’ use of LAPE strate-
gies; intervention fidelity refers to the parents’ use of indi-
Play and routine observations.  Toddler and parent progress vidual strategies with their toddlers (Dunst et al., 2008b).
was measured using summative and formative measures. Implementation fidelity was collected in three ways. First, a
Prior to the start of LAPE each parent−child dyad partici- checklist was created for the group sessions outlining the
pated in two videotaped samples in their homes (collected planned components of the sessions (e.g., “discuss parent
on the same day). During a 10-min play sample, parents and child progress at home,” “review the strategy Repeat
played with their toddlers using a standard set of toys pro- and Add”). Students (who were not presenting or coaching)
vided by the authors. The toys included dolls, cups, plates, used the checklist designed for LAPE group sessions to
books, blocks, and games (e.g., Mr. Potato HeadTM). In a measure implementation fidelity of every group session.
10-min routine sample, parents interacted with their chil- This ensured the group training components were similar
dren during a parent-identified routine. Parents selected across cohorts. The number of planned components for
mealtime, diapering, dressing, or getting ready for bed. group sessions ranged from 5 to 12. The group session
During pre-LAPE observations, parents were instructed to implementation fidelity averaged 93.6% (range = 78%–
interact with their children using any strategies they nor- 100%) across sessions and cohorts. Second, the first or sec-
mally use to encourage their children to communicate. The ond author completed fidelity checklists at all individual
same play and routine activities were videotaped again sessions. These outlined the planned components of indi-
immediately after LAPE (i.e., post) and at a 3-month vidual sessions to ensure the coaches conducted these ses-
follow-up. During these sessions, parents were instructed to sions as planned and that they were similar across dyads.
use LAPE strategies. Five graduate students transcribed and The number of planned components for individual sessions
coded all videos after establishing 90% interobserver agree- ranged from 9 to 12. The individual sessions implementa-
ment (IOA) on nonstudy videos. Four parent variables were tion fidelity averaged 90.42% (range = 77%–100%) across
coded: (a) responsivity (RE), (b) target level talk (TLT), (c) sessions, dyads, and cohorts. Occasionally a parent would
questions (Q), and (d) other language-enhancing behaviors want to spend extra time working on a specific strategy or a
(O-LEBs). These are described in Table 3. Two child vari- routine was cut short, and the coach would decide to skip or
ables were calculated: (a) mean length of utterance (MLU; spend less time on a component. Thus, the implementation
mean number of words per child utterance) and (b) initia- fidelity indicated coaches completed most of the planned
tions (see Table 6). components, but were flexible and responsive to parent and
toddler needs (McWilliam, 2012). Third, attendance was
Progress monitoring. Parents monitored their use of the recorded at all group sessions. Two parents missed one
LAPE strategies and progress toward self-selected goals. group session each, and missed content was reviewed dur-
As described previously, the parents completed a respon- ing a make-up session. Intervention fidelity was measured
sivity log every week. If a parent did not bring a completed using a weekly review of the parent-completed responsivity
log with them to a group or individual session, the coach log. All eight parents reported consistently using the LAPE
asked the parent to complete it before the group session strategies at home.
began. During group and individual sessions, the parents
reviewed their progress as reflected on their log, and dis- Social validity.  Social validity was measured in two ways.
cussed with their coaches the progress toward their self- First, parent satisfaction with the group trainings was mea-
selected goals. Following intervention, parents also sured after each group session with an anonymous ques-
completed the LAPE Goal Questionnaire, which asked tionnaire. These ratings were consistently high. Second, at
them to indicate whether LAPE helped them meet their the end of LAPE parents rated their satisfaction with the
goals for themselves and their children. goals, procedures, and outcomes with an anonymous social
validity questionnaire. The overall mean for the nine items
Interobserver agreement. Twenty-eight (58%) randomly on the questionnaire was 5.32 (i.e., the scale ranged from
selected play and routine session videos (i.e., including at 1 = strongly disagree to 6 = strongly agree), which signi-
least 20% of videos across dyads, sessions, time) were fied strong social validity. Parents indicated LAPE was

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014


Moore et al. 219

Table 5.  Parent Outcomes by Variable.

Pre Post Follow-up


Dyad Behavior Play Routine Play Routine Play Routine
1 RE 49.4 29.4 69.0 64.5 62.2 55.4
  TLT 64.4 72.6 93.9 91.9 94.0 89.0
  Q 35.6 22.1 13.7 12.6 17.9 23.3
  O-LEBs 4.0 2.1 24.4 24.3 16.3 7.5
2 RE 39.4 39.2 67.0 56.0 53.8 57.0
  TLT 63.6 65.1 72.0 73.0 93.3 89.9
  Q 15.5 16.8 7.0 10.7 8.9 13.5
  O-LEBs 8.6 8.7 15.9 8.7 24.4 10.1
3 RE 53.2 43.3 70.8 53.4 63.2 50.9
  TLT 55.3 33.8 95.4 78.1 84.6 66.7
  Q 39.4 36.6 5.8 8.2 3.9 17.5
  O-LEBs 10.1 7.0 27.6 30.1 5.1 14.0
4 RE 50.3 55.8 77.7 43.0 57.4 48.5
  TLT 62.0 60.0 95.4 80.5 89.8 77.2
  Q 27.7 26.2 8.3 17.0 20.5 15.0
  O-LEBs 22.9 6.9 52.8 13.6 6.3 9.5
5 RE 58.9 53.4 63.8 58.8 62.7 65.4
  TLT 66.9 70.5 95.7 79.9 78.4 64.4
  Q 43.0 31.6 10.6 6.9 16.8 28.2
  O-LEBs 5.0 6.0 16.5 12.6 13.5 12.2
6 RE 42.6 62.0 69.5 77.7 77.3 63.4
  TLT 71.3 61.8 86.8 77.7 93.5 78.0
  Q 22.1 32.0 3.5 22.3 4.7 13.4
  O-LEBs 6.6 13.4 21.1 8.5 8.4 3.7
7 RE 51.9 31.5 82.0 39.1 76.9 42.2
  TLT 38.4 35.2 86.4 44.5 88.7 54.4
  Q 45.0 16.7 15.3 4.7 13.0 17.7
  O-LEBs 17.2 1.8 24.6 11.4 28.7 10.9
8 RE 28.8 29.7 44.0 36.0 57.0 35.5
  TLT 95.0 96.2 98.4 97.5 97.6 91.5
  Q 35.7 18.4 20.8 12.2 26.2 17.0
  O-LEBs 4.0 5.0 29.6 16.3 6.2 10.3
Group M (SD) 46.9 (9.4) 42.8 (12.8) 68.0 (11.3) 53.6 (14.0) 63.8 (8.8) 52.2 (10.2)
  64.6 (15.8) 61.9 (20.3) 90.5 (8.6) 77.9 (15.7) 90.0 (6.1) 76.4 (13.6)
  33.0 (10.4) 25.1 (7.7) 10.6 (5.7) 11.8 (5.7) 14.9 (7.8) 18.2 (5.1)
  9.8 (6.9) 6.4 (3.7) 26.6 (11.7) 15.7 (7.7) 13.6 (9.0) 9.8 (3.1)

Note. RE = Responsivity: Percentage of spoken adult turns; TLT = target level talk: Percentage of total adult utterances; Q = question asking: Percent-
age of total adult utterances; O-LEBs = other language-enhancing behaviors: Percentage of total adult utterances.

effective for increasing their children’s communication conducted by cohort. However, there were no differences
skills across daily routines (M = 5.14) and during play inter- identified across cohorts. Thus, given the small number of
actions (M = 5.21). Moreover, they reported using LAPE dyads, outcomes were analyzed individually and are pre-
strategies outside of the home (M = 4.64) and stated that sented as such in the subsequent sections (see Tables 5
they would recommend the LAPE program to other parents and 6).
with toddlers with communication delays (M = 5.86).
Parent Outcomes
Program Outcomes Responsivity (RE). Parents demonstrated variability in RE
Multiple measures were used to evaluate parent and child during pre-LAPE observations, and seven parents increased
outcomes from LAPE. Initially, outcome analyses were their RE following intervention. In general, parents were

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014


220 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 33(4)

Table 6.  Child Outcomes by Variable.

MLUd in play Percent child initiationse


CDIb-II—Number of
  words Pre Post F Pre Post F
Disability
Dyad statusa Pre Post Fc Play Rout Play Rout Play Rout Play Rout Play Rout Play Rout
1 DD 47 101 205 1.0 1.0 1.3 1.4 1.4 1.3 21.8 48.4 46.1 56.6 38.5 32.7
2 DD 12 35 56 1.0 1.0 1.2 1.0 1.1 1.1 1.3 3.3 5.2 3.5 22.7 10.2
3 SELD 39 139 541 1.0 1.0 1.3 1.4 3.6 1.9 20.9 14.3 41.9 38.2 73.9 31.8
4 DD 16 79 208 1.0 1.1 1.1 1.1 1.2 1.2 1.9 4.7 10.1 6.6 16.7 1158
5 SD 40 127 208 1.1 1.0 1.3 1.4 1.4 1.2 8.6 6.2 19.6 37.6 20.7 17.5
6 SD 11 205 501 1.0 1.0 1.1 1.1 1.9 2.4 13.0 11.3 20.4 18.3 48.4 0.0
7 SELD 14 306 515 0.0 1.0 1.3 1.2 1.2 1.7 0.0 5.0 33.3 13.0 31.5 10.6
8 DD 19 42 52 1.3 1.2 1.0 1.1 1.8 1.5 12.5 1.8 39.1 41.5 55.1 46.3
M 24.8 129.3 285 0.9 1.0 1.2 1.2 1.7 1.5 10.0 11.9 27.0 26.9 38.4 20.1
SD 14.7 90.2 203.6 0.4 0.1 0.2 0.2 0.8 0.5 8.6 15.3 15.3 19.1 19.7 15.4
a
Disability status: DD = developmental disability, SELD = specific expressive language delay, SD = structural disability. bCDI = MacArthur−Bates Com-
municative Development Inventory–2nd Edition (words and sentences form). cF = follow-up. dMLU = mean length of utterance (Number of words in
intelligible utterances divided by total intelligible utterances). ePercent Child Initiations = percentage of child turns that are initiated by the child (i.e.,
intelligible and unintelligible utterances and nonverbal communicative acts initiated by the child divided by total number of child turns).

more RE during play than in routines. However, at 3-month Results of the LAPE Goals Questionnaire indicated that out
follow-up only two parents continued to improve RE; five of 27 LAPE goals written by the parents, 25 were met, and
did not maintain postintervention levels. 2 were not addressed because they were speech production
goals.
Target Level Talk (TLT). Parents demonstrated variability
during pre-LAPE observations in their TLT (i.e., the per-
Child Outcomes
centage of utterances within their child’s MLU). All eight
parents improved their use of TLT in play and routine activ- All children increased their vocabulary following LAPE and
ities following LAPE, and parents consistently used more at follow-up, as reported by parents on the CDI-II. As
TLT during play. Overall, parents maintained their use of expected, progress varied by disability group. Children with
TLT at 3-month follow-up. Specific Expressive Language Delays (n = 2) gained the
greatest number of words (M = 502 words, range = 501–502);
Questions (Q).  Parents demonstrated a high percentage of Q children with Developmental Delays (n = 4) gained the fewest
pre-LAPE. All eight parents decreased their use of Q fol- number of words (M = 130 words, range = 33–194). Likewise,
lowing intervention and used fewer Q during play than in all children increased their mean number of words per utter-
routines. However, at follow-up, none of the eight parents ance (MLU) postintervention and at follow-up. Overall, chil-
maintained these decreases during routines. Four of the dren started at a mean of 0.9 during play and 1.0 during
eight parents maintained the decreases during play. routines and by follow-up were at 1.7 for play and 1.5 for
routines. The average MLU for typically developing toddlers
Other Language-Enhancing Behaviors (O-LEB).  Parents dem- (age 18–36 months) is 1.8 to 3.1 (Paul & Norbury, 2011). The
onstrated variability during pre-LAPE observations in their children varied in their use of communicative initiations prior
use of O-LEB. All participants increased their use of O-LEB to the start of LAPE with a larger range of initiation in the
following intervention; the greatest increase occurred dur- routine samples than the play samples. All children increased
ing play. However, none of the eight parents maintained their use of initiations post-LAPE. However, at follow-up,
these increases during routines. Three parents maintained children increased their use of initiations during play but not
their increased use of O-LEB during play. routines.

Parent Self-Progress Monitoring Discussion


As mentioned previously, all parents reported consistently This project was designed to evaluate the effectiveness of a
using the LAPE strategies at home on a weekly log. community-based parent education program: LAPE. The

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014


Moore et al. 221

intervention was designed based on recommended family completing the routine (e.g., getting the child dressed),
coaching practices and incorporated empirically supported rather than the child’s communication skills, whereas dur-
language-enhancing strategies (e.g., environmental arrange- ing play parents can focus directly on teaching new com-
ment, responsive interaction strategies). The effectiveness munication skills. Parents might need more support and
of LAPE was evaluated with multiple quantitative mea- guidance on how to embed communication strategies into
sures. Three notable findings were identified. First, the routines while still attending to the task at hand.
results from LAPE are promising. The parents increased
their use of language-enhancing strategies (i.e., RE, TLT,
Limitations and Future Research
modeling) and decreased their use of question asking
(which might limit child language growth) after participat- Although multiple outcome measures were utilized, causal
ing in LAPE. Moreover, parents were satisfied with LAPE relations between intervention and child outcomes could
and indicated that LAPE met most of the goals they wrote not be deduced. As discussed previously, LAPE was devel-
for themselves and their children. Although child progress oped to address a community need, so no control partici-
varied substantially according to disability group, all chil- pants were identified. Future research should experimentally
dren increased their vocabulary, MLU, and initiations. examine the efficiency and effectiveness of the LAPE
While causal relations cannot be determined based on the model as it compares with “business as usual” EI across
small sample size, lack of controls, and the developmental disability groups (e.g., using a randomized wait-list control
nature of child language, the outcomes are notable due to design). Moreover, there is a dearth of information on pater-
the relatively small dosage of intervention (i.e., 4–5 group nal use of language-enhancing strategies. Although only
sessions, 2–3 individual sessions; totaling 9.5 to 12 hr over one father participated in this study, the authors anecdotally
7 or 15 weeks) as compared with other similar parent- noted differences between the mothers’ and the father’s
training interventions, such as Hanen—It Takes Two to interactions with their children. As fathers take a more
Talk (i.e., 8 group sessions, 3 individual video feedback active parenting role and participate more in therapeutic
sessions, totaling 30 hr; Girolametto & Weitzman, 2006) interventions with their children, research should examine
and EMT (approximately 30 individual sessions contingent their unique interaction and communication styles.
on parents meeting criterion, totaling 10 hr; Hancock & Parents in LAPE did not maintain their use of language-
Kaiser, 2006). The current evaluation supports existing enhancing strategies at 3-month follow-up. Future studies
research documenting that a hybrid approach (group train- should examine practices that promote maintenance, includ-
ing with individual support) might be effective for teaching ing monthly parent support sessions, web-based supports,
parents language-enhancing strategies in a short period of or individual follow-up and coaching. In fact, previous
time (Carter et al., 2011; Kaiser et al., 1995). research indicates parent education group trainings are only
Second, with the exception of TLT, six parents did not effective when supplemented with intensive, individual
maintain their use of LAPE strategies at the 3-month supports (Carter et al., 2011; Kaiser et al., 1995). However,
follow-up. Although RE, Q, and O-LEBs did not return to few cost-effective, early language intervention approaches
pre-LAPE levels, there was a clear decrease in use over exist that are feasible and useful for interventionists work-
time. Two out of three parents maintained their high levels ing within Part C systems, and the few that do are not effec-
of RE at 3-month follow-up in the third cohort, which had a tive for all families (Carter et al., 2011) or are not flexible
longer treatment duration (same dosage), but Q and O-LEB enough to be implemented by a variety of EIs in authentic
did not maintain. Previous studies also have documented settings. For example, future research might examine the
that use of language-enhancing strategies does not maintain use of phone or web-based support as a supplement to
over time without extensive follow-up coaching and sup- follow-up coaching to promote parents’ maintenance of
port (Peterson et al., 2005). skills learned. Future studies should continue to examine
Third, parents consistently used more LAPE strategies feasible, effective curricula that can be implemented within
during play than routines. Play and routines were discussed the confines and limitations of the Part C service delivery
at length during the group and individual sessions and prac- systems.
ticed during individual sessions. After analyzing data from
the first two cohorts, the authors decided to change the for-
Implications for Practice
mat of the daily responsivity log to encourage more practice
during routines and incorporated more routine examples LAPE is an example of a parent education program devel-
during group and individual sessions. However, there were oped through recommended practice guidelines and current
still no substantial differences between the cohorts. These research. LAPE was designed as a supplemental “first level
differences might indicate that it is more difficult to embed of response” to provide brief, intensive help to parents of
language-enhancing strategies into routines, perhaps toddlers exhibiting communication delays. The following
because the primary focus of the routine for the parent is five recommendations are based on LAPE outcomes.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014


222 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 33(4)

First, group training might be effective for giving par- interventionists should teach responsive and environmental
ents information in a short period of time. Group-based pro- arrangement strategies first (Hancock & Kaiser, 2006).
grams can be cost-effective and offer the support and
guidance of peer-parents, which might increase engage-
Conclusion
ment and participation. This has been shown to be true for
parent education programs focused on reducing children’s The purpose of this project was to develop a parent educa-
challenging behaviors (McIntyre & Phaneuf, 2008). Groups tion model focused on enhancing parent responsivity and
should be kept small (5–6 families) and allow ample time improving the language skills of toddlers with expressive
for parents to support each other, share progress, and dis- communication delays. LAPE incorporated family coach-
cuss implementation of the strategies. In LAPE, the authors ing and language-enhancing strategies using group and
anecdotally noted that parents frequently supported each individual sessions in a relatively brief period of time (i.e.,
other during group sessions. 7–15 weeks). After participating in LAPE, all parents
Second, despite the advantages of group-based parent increased their use of language-enhancing strategies and
education programs, groups alone are not effective (Kaiser decreased their use of question asking. However, only two
et al., 1995); individual support is necessary to support fam- parents maintained these increases after 3 months. Social
ilies. In LAPE, the purpose of the individual sessions was to validity measures indicated parents were satisfied with pro-
help the parents identify which specific strategies they used cedures, goals, and outcomes of the project. This suggests
well, which new strategies they wanted to start implement- LAPE is a promising parent education program, though fur-
ing, and how to implement them in their homes. Moreover, ther refinements are needed.
parent−child observations were important for making data-
based decisions. Thus, individual sessions should be based Declaration of Conflicting Interests
on each family’s unique needs. The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with
Third, effective family coaching practices exist and respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this
should be implemented (Powell & Dunlap, 2010). During article.
home visits, early interventionists should build relation-
ships with families, model strategies, provide multiple Funding
opportunities for practice, and give performance-based
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support
feedback that builds on the parent’s self-reflection. Video
for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This
recordings should be used for demonstration, self-reflection, project was funded in part by a grant from the University of
and performance-based feedback. Oregon College of Education Hope Baney Fund.
Fourth, parents should be taught to embed language-
enhancing strategies during daily play and routine activities.
References
During group and individual sessions, early interventionists
should talk with parents about how they typically engage American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2005). Roles
with their children and then individualize examples accord- and responsibilities of Speech-Language Pathologists service
ingly. Not all parents have the time or desire to play in tradi- persons with mental retardation/developmental disabilities
(guidelines). Available from www.asha.org/policy
tional ways (e.g., sitting on the floor with toys) with their
Baggett, K., Davis, B., Feil, E., Sheeber, L., Landry, S., Carta, J.,
children everyday. Instead early interventionists should dis- & Leve, C. (2010). Technologies for expanding the reach of
cuss what they are already doing (e.g., playing and talking in evidence-based interventions: Preliminary results for promot-
the car, at bath time) and which daily activities might be ing social-emotional development in early childhood. Topics
most conducive to teaching new communication skills, in Early Childhood Special Education, 29, 226–238.
acknowledging that some home activities are more hectic Bruder, M. B., & Dunst, C. J. (2000). Expanding learning oppor-
and task-directed than others. Once activities are identified, tunities for infants and toddlers in natural environments: A
early interventionists should help parents identify specific chance to re-conceptualize early intervention. Zero to Three,
opportunities during those activities to embed the strategies 20, 34–36.
and provide modeling and time for parents to practice during Campbell, P. H., & Sawyer, L. B. (2007). Supporting learning
coaching sessions (McWilliam, 2010). opportunities in natural settings through participation-based
services. Journal of Early Intervention, 29, 287–305.
Fifth, coaching should be responsive to the family’s
Carter, A. S., Messinger, D. S., Stone, W. L., Celimli, S.,
changing needs and the child’s language development. Nahmias, A. S., & Yoder, P. (2011). A randomized con-
Early interventionists should teach milieu strategies (e.g., trolled trial of Hanen’s “More Than Words” in toddlers with
model) if responsive and environmental arrangement strate- early autism symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and
gies are not producing significant outcomes for the child. Psychiatry, 52, 741–752.
For some parents, milieu strategies might promote more Crais, E. R. (1990). World knowledge to word knowledge. Topics
directive and fewer responsive behaviors. Thus, early in Language Disorders, 10, 45–62.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014


Moore et al. 223

Desmarais, C., Sylvestre, A., Meyer, F., Bairati, I., & Rouleau, N. Hebbeler, K., Spiker, D., & Kahn, L. (2012). Individuals with
(2008). Systematic review of the literature on characteristics Disabilities Education Act’s early childhood programs:
of late-talking toddlers. International Journal of Language Powerful vision and pesky details. Topics in Early Childhood
and Communication Disorders, 43, 361–389. Special Education, 31, 199–207.
Dunst, C. J., & Kassow, D. Z. (2007). Characteristics of inter- Hume, K., Bellini, S., & Pratt, C. (2005). The usage and perceived
ventions promoting parental sensitivity to child behavior. outcomes of early intervention and early childhood programs
Winterberry Research Syntheses, 1, 13. for young children with autism spectrum disorder. Topics in
Dunst, C. J., & Trivette, C. M. (2009a). Capacity-building family- Early Childhood Special Education, 25, 195–207.
systems intervention practices. Journal of Family Social Jung, L. A. (2003). More is better: Maximizing natural learning
Work, 12, 119–143. opportunities. Young Exceptional Children, 6, 21–26.
Dunst, C. J., & Trivette, C. M. (2009b). Research evidence to Kaiser, A. P., Hancock, T., & Nietfeld, J. P. (2000). The effects of
inform and evaluate early childhood intervention practices. parent-implemented enhanced milieu teaching on the social
Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 29, 40–52. communication of children who have autism. Early Education
Dunst, C. J., Trivette, C. M., & Hamby, D. W. (2006). Family sup- and Development, 11, 423–446.
port program quality and parent, family and child benefits. Kaiser, A. P., Hemmeter, M. L., Ostrosky, M. M., Alpert, C. L.,
Asheville, NC: Winterberry Press. & Hancock, T. B. (1995). The effects of group training and
Dunst, C. J., Trivette, C. M., & Hamby, D. W. (2007). Meta- individual feedback on parent use of milieu teaching. Journal
analysis of family-centered helpgiving practices research. of Childhood Communication Disorders, 16, 39–48.
Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Kaiser, A. P., & Roberts, M. Y. (2011). Advances in early com-
Reviews, 13, 370–378. munication and language intervention. Journal of Early
Dunst, C. J., Trivette, C. M., & Hamby, D. W. (2008a). Research Intervention, 33, 298–309.
synthesis and meta-analysis of studies of family-centered Kashinath, S., Woods, J., & Goldstein, H. (2006). Enhancing
practices. Asheville, NC: Winterberry Press. generalized teaching strategy use in daily routines by parents
Dunst, C. J., Trivette, C. M., McInerney, M., Hollan-Coviello, R., of children with autism. Journal of Speech, Language, and
Masiello, T., Helsel, F., & Robyak, A. (2008b). Measuring Hearing Research, 49, 466–485.
training and practice fidelity in capacity building scaling-up Kennedy, C. H. (2005). Single case designs for educational
initiatives. Asheville, NC: Winterberry Press. research. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, McGee, G. G., Morrier, M. J., & Daly, T. (1999). An inciden-
J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates communicative tal teaching approach to early intervention for toddlers with
development inventories: User’s guide and technical manual autism. Journal of the Association for Persons With Severe
(2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. Handicaps, 24, 133–146.
Fey, M. E., Warren, S. F., Brady, N. C., Finestack, L. H., McIntyre, L. L., & Phaneuf, L. K. (2008). A three-tier model
Brendin-Oja, S. L., Fairchild, M., & Yoder, P. J. (2006). of parent education in early childhood. Topics in Early
Early effects of responsivity education/prelinguistic Childhood Special Education, 27, 214–222.
milieuteaching for children with developmental delays and McWilliam, R. A. (2010). Routines based intervention: Supporting
their parents. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing young children and their families. Baltimore, MD: Paul H.
Research, 49, 526–547. Brookes.
Girolametto, L., & Weitzman, E. (2006). It takes two to talk— McWilliam, R. A. (2012). Implementing and preparing for home
The Hanen Program for parents. In R. J. McCauley, & visits. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 31,
M. E. Fey (Eds.), Treatment of language disorders in chil- 224–231.
dren (pp. 77–103). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. Paul, R. (2000). Predicting outcomes of early expressive language
Hancock, T. B., & Kaiser, A. P. (2006). Enhanced milieu teaching. delay: Ethical implications. In D. V. Bishop, & L. B. Leonard
In R. J. McCauley, & M. E. Fey (Eds.), Treatment of lan- (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: Causes
guage disorders in children (pp. 203–236). Baltimore, MD: characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp. 195–209).
Paul H. Brookes. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
The Hanen Centre. (2011). It Takes Two to Talk® certification Paul, R., & Norbury, C. (2011). Language disorders: From
workshop schedule. Retrieved from http://www.hanen.org/ infancy through adolescence. Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier
professional-development/Workshop-Schedule/ Health Sciences.
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the Peterson, P., Carta, J., & Greenwood, C. (2005). Teaching
everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, enhanced milieu language teaching skills to parents in mul-
MD: Paul H. Brookes. tiple risk families. Journal of Early Intervention, 27, 94–109.
Hebbeler, K., Spiker, D., Bailey, D., Scarborough, A., Mallik, S., Powell, D., & Dunlap, G. (2010). Family-focused interventions
Simeonsson, R., & Nelson, L. (2007). Early intervention for promoting social-emotional development in infants and
for infants and toddlers with disabilities and their families: toddlers with or at risk for disabilities. Roadmap to Effective
Participants, services, and outcomes. Final Report of the Intervention Practices #5. Tampa: University of South
National Early Intervention Longitudinal Study (NEILS). Florida, Technical Assistance Center on Social Emotional
Menlo Park, CA: SRI International. Intervention for Young Children.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014


224 Topics in Early Childhood Special Education 33(4)

Rescorla, L. (2002). Language and reading outcomes to age 9 Disabilities Act, 2009. Retrieved from http://www2.ed.gov/
in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language, and about/reports/annual/osep/2009/parts-b-c/index.html
Hearing Research, 45, 360–371. U.S. Preventive Services Task Force. (2006). Screening for speech
Roberts, M., & Kaiser, A. (2011). The effectiveness of parent- and language delay in preschool children: Recommendation
implemented language interventions: A meta-analysis. American statement. Pediatrics, 117, 497–501. Retrieved from http://
Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20, 180–199. www.aafp.org/afp/2006/1015/p1373.html
Rush, D. D., & Shelden, M. L. (2011). The early childhood coach- van Daal, J., Verhoeven, L., & van Balkom, H. (2007). Behaviour
ing handbook. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing. problems in children with language impairment. Journal of
Sandall, S., Hemmeter, M. L., Smith, B., & McLean, M. (2005). Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 1139–1147.
DEC recommended practices: A comprehensive guide for van der Schuit, M., Peeters, M., Segers, E., van Balkom, H., &
practical application in early intervention/ early childhood Verhoeven, L. (2009). Home literacy environment of pre-
special education. Missoula, MT: DEC. school children with intellectual disabilities. Journal of
Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland Intellectual Disability Research, 53, 1024–1037.
Adaptive Behavior Scales: 2nd ed. (Vineland II), Survey inter- Warren, S. F., Bredin-Oja, S. L., Fairchild, M., Finestack, L. H.,
view form/caregiver rating form. Livonia, MN: Pearson. Fey, M. E., & Brady, N. C. (2006). Responsivity education-
Stahmer, A. C., & Mandell, D. (2007). State infant/toddler pro- prelinguistic milieu teaching. In R. J. McCauley, & M. E. Fey
gram policies for eligibility and services provision for young (Eds.), Treatment of language disorders in children (pp. 47–75).
children with autism. Administration and Policy in Mental Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Health and Mental Health Services Research, 34, 29–37. Yoder, P. J., & Warren, S. F. (2002). Effects of prelinguistic milieu
Trivette, C. M., Dunst, C. J., & Hamby, D. W. (2010). Influences teaching and parent responsivity education on dyads involv-
of family systems intervention practices on parent-child inter- ing children with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Speech,
actions and child development. Topics in Early Childhood Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 1158–1174.
Special Education, 30, 3–19. Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G., & Pond, R. E. (2002).
U.S. Department of Education. (2012). 31st annual report to Preschool Language Scale (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX:
congress on the implementation of the Individuals With Harcourt Assessment.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at Universidad de Murcia on January 10, 2014

You might also like