You are on page 1of 20

Yong-Kyu Kim

Editor

Handbook of Behavior
Genetics

123
Editor
Yong-Kyu Kim
University of Georgia
Athens, GA
USA
yongkyu@arches.uga.edu

ISBN 978-0-387-76726-0 e-ISBN 978-0-387-76727-7


DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-76727-7

Library of Congress Control Number: 2008941695

c Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009


All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written permission of the
publisher (Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, 233 Spring Street, New York, NY 10013, USA), except for brief
excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and
retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter
developed is forbidden.
The use in this publication of trade names, trademarks, service marks, and similar terms, even if they are not identified
as such, is not to be taken as an expression of opinion as to whether or not they are subject to proprietary rights.
While the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of going to press, neither
the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legal responsibility for any errors or omissions that may
be made. The publisher makes no warranty, express or implied, with respect to the material contained herein.

Printed on acid-free paper

springer.com
Chapter 30

Genetic Analysis of Conduct Disorder and Antisocial Behavior

Soo Hyun Rhee and Irwin D. Waldman

Introduction regard for, and violation of, the rights of others that begins
in childhood or early adolescence and continues into adult-
In this chapter, we examine the evidence for genetic influ- hood” (p. 645). A diagnosis of ASPD requires a history of
ences on conduct disorder and antisocial behavior. First, CD before the age of 15 and three or more of the following
we present results from a meta-analysis of twin and adop- criteria: failure to conform to social norms with respect to
tion studies estimating the relative magnitude of genetic lawful behaviors (i.e., as indicated by repeatedly performing
and environmental influences on antisocial behavior (Rhee acts that are grounds for arrest), deceitfulness, impulsivity,
& Waldman, 2002). Second, we discuss recent studies irritability and aggressiveness, reckless disregard for safety,
that have examined several interesting issues in the eti- consistent irresponsibility, and lack of remorse. Conduct dis-
ology of antisocial behavior, including genotype × envi- order, a criterion for the diagnosis of ASPD, is described
ronment interactions, co-occurrence with other psychiatric by the DSM-IV as “a repetitive and persistent pattern of
disorders, the etiology of psychopathy, and the etiology behavior in which the basic rights of others or major age-
of adolescent-limited versus life-course-persistent antisocial appropriate societal norms or rules are violated” (p. 90,
behavior. Third, we review association studies examining the APA, 1994). It usually occurs in childhood or early ado-
influence of specific candidate genes on antisocial behav- lescence and is manifested as aggression toward people and
ior and linkage studies conducting genome-wide screens for animals, destruction of property, deceitfulness or theft, and
quantitative trait loci (QTLs) influencing antisocial behavior. serious violations of rules. Second, studies examining crim-
In this review, we focused on phenotypes directly related inality (an unlawful act that leads to arrest, conviction, or
to conduct disorder or antisocial behavior. Studies were incarceration) and delinquency (unlawful acts committed as
included if they clearly examined antisocial personality dis- a juvenile) were included. Third, we included studies exam-
order, conduct disorder, criminality, or aggression; if there ining aggression, which is usually studied as a personal-
was empirical evidence suggesting that the measure of anti- ity characteristic and assessed with measures such as the
social behavior used in the study successfully discriminated Adjective Checklist (Gough & Heilbrun, 1972). Fourth, stud-
an antisocial group from a control group; or if there was ies examining an omnibus operationalization that includes
empirical evidence suggesting that the measure used in the aggression and delinquency items, such as the externalizing
study is significantly related to a more established opera- scale from the Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach & Edel-
tionalization of antisocial behavior. brock, 1983), were included.
Studies examining four operationalizations of antisocial
behavior were included. First, studies examining psychiatric
diagnoses of antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) and con- Current Issues and Research
duct disorder (CD) were included. The Diagnostic and Statis-
tical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV; A Meta-analysis of Twin and Adoption Studies
American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994), describes Examining Antisocial Behavior
the essential features of ASPD as “a pervasive pattern of dis-

More than a hundred twin and adoption studies of antiso-


S.H. Rhee (B) cial behavior have been published. Nonetheless, it is diffi-
Institute for Behavioral Genetics, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO cult to draw clear conclusions regarding the magnitude of
80309, USA genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behav-
e-mail: Soo.Rhee@colorado.edu ior given the current literature. The main reason for this

Y.-K. Kim (ed.), Handbook of Behavior Genetics, 455


DOI 10.1007/978-0-387-76727-7 30,  c Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009
456 S.H. Rhee and I.D. Waldman

difficulty is the considerable heterogeneity of the results in and Moffitt (1993) have suggested that antisocial individuals
this area of research, with published heritability estimates can be divided into a smaller group whose antisocial behav-
(i.e., the magnitude of genetic influences) ranging from very ior is persistent throughout the life course and caused pre-
low (e.g., 0.00; Plomin, Foch, & Rowe, 1981) to very high dominantly by genetic influences and a larger group whose
(e.g., 0.71; Slutske et al., 1997). It is important to remember antisocial behavior is limited to adolescence and caused pre-
that there is not a fixed, absolute heritability for antisocial dominantly by environmental influences. If their hypothesis
behavior. The heritability estimate describes the magnitude is correct, the magnitude of genetic influences on antisocial
of genetic influences in a particular population at a particular behavior should be lower in adolescence than in childhood or
time (Plomin, DeFries, McClearn, & McGuffin, 2001), and adulthood. We also compared the results of twin and adop-
it is possible that the heterogeneity in the results of behavior tion studies. Twin and adoption studies have unique assump-
genetic studies of antisocial behavior may reflect real, sub- tions or biases that can make interpretations of their results
stantive differences as well as those due to methodological difficult. Comparing the results of twin and adoption studies
variations across studies. In the literature, various hypotheses can help determine whether the results of behavior genetic
have been proposed to explain these heterogeneous results studies have been influenced by these unique assumptions or
across studies, including differences in the age of the sample biases. To the degree that the results of twin and adoption
(e.g., Cloninger & Gottesman, 1987), the age of onset of anti- studies are similar, it is more likely that the results reflect
social behavior (e.g., Moffitt, 1993), and the measurement of the true magnitude of genetic and environmental influences.
antisocial behavior (e.g., Plomin, Nitz, & Rowe, 1990). One cannot rule out the possibility, however, that the results
We conducted a meta-analysis of twin and adoption stud- of twin and adoption studies are similar because they share
ies in order to provide a clearer and more comprehen- similar biases to some extent that influence their results in
sive picture of the magnitude of genetic and environmental the same direction.
influences on antisocial behavior. Given previous hypothe- Two types of adoption studies were included in the present
ses proposed to explain the heterogeneity in the results, we meta-analysis: (1) parent–offspring adoption studies (i.e.,
examined the possible moderating effects of three study char- comparing the correlation between adoptees and their adop-
acteristics (i.e., the operationalization of antisocial behavior, tive parents with the correlation between adoptees and their
assessment method, and zygosity determination method) and biological parents) and (2) sibling adoption studies (i.e.,
two participant characteristics (i.e., the age and sex of the comparing the correlation between adoptive siblings with
participants) on the magnitude of genetic and environmental the correlation between biological siblings). When interpret-
influences on antisocial behavior. ing the results of parent–offspring data, it is important to
We tested the operationalization of antisocial behavior consider the possibility that the correlations between the
as a possible moderator given the evidence that antiso- parents and the offspring may be reduced by the age dif-
cial personality disorder, conduct disorder, criminality, and ference between the two generations and that the magnitude
aggression are distinct but related constructs (e.g., Robins & of familial (i.e., genetic and shared environmental) influ-
Regier, 1991). We examined four levels of operationaliza- ences may be underestimated. Genetic influences on a trait
tion, which included diagnosis (ASPD or CD), criminality, may differ from one generation to another because the genes
aggression, and antisocial behavior (an omnibus operational- affecting the same trait may differ in their expression across
ization that included aggression and delinquency items). We age due to genotype–environment interaction. For exam-
tested assessment method and zygosity determination as ple, genetic influences in the younger generation may be
moderators because of evidence suggesting that these are increased because of environmental facilitation of antisocial
potential methodological confounders (e.g., Plomin, 1981; behavior, e.g., via secular increases in substance use and
McCartney, Harris, & Bernieri, 1990). We compared five less-stringent parenting practices (e.g., Lykken, 1997). Also,
assessment methods, including self-report, report by oth- there may be cohort-specific shared environmental influences
ers (i.e., parent and teacher report), official records, objec- other than the cultural transmission from parents to offspring.
tive measures, and reactions to aggressive material as well Therefore, each type of adoption study was compared to the
as three zygosity determination methods, including blood twin studies separately.
grouping, questionnaires, and a combination of blood group- One hundred forty-one twin and adoption studies of anti-
ing and questionnaires. Sex was examined given the con- social behavior were identified by examining the PsycInfo
sistent evidence that antisocial behavior is more prevalent and Medline databases and contacting authors of unpub-
in males than females (e.g., Hyde, 1984; Wilson & Herrn- lished manuscripts identified by abstracts of the Behavior
stein, 1985). Age was examined because of the potential to Genetics Association meetings and searching the Disserta-
test an interesting hypothesis regarding the development of tion Abstracts and ERIC databases. After excluding stud-
antisocial behavior by comparing studies that included chil- ies that were unsuitable given inadequate construct validity,
dren, adolescents, and adults. DiLalla and Gottesman (1989) inability to calculate effect sizes given lack of information,
30 Genetic Analysis of Conduct Disorder and Antisocial Behavior 457

and simultaneous assessment of related disorders, such as genetic influences was estimated in addition to the magnitude
alcoholism, 96 studies remained. Non-independence among of shared environmental influences, the best fitting model
these studies was addressed by either choosing the effect was the ACDE model. Based on this analysis, there were
size from the largest sample when non-independent sam- moderate additive genetic (a2 = 0.32), non-additive genetic
ples had differing sample sizes or by averaging the effect (d2 = 0.09), shared environmental (c2 = 0.16), and non-
sizes across the samples when non-independent samples shared environmental (e2 = 0.43) influences on antisocial
had the same sample size. After addressing the problem of behavior.
non-independence, 10 independent adoption samples and 42 Operationalization was a significant moderator of the
independent twin samples remained. The meta-analysis was magnitude of genetic and environmental influences. This
conducted on these 52 independent samples. The studies means that there was a statistically significant difference
included in the meta-analysis are marked with an asterisk (∗ ) between a model that constrained the parameter estimates
in the References section. to be the same across the different levels of the moderator
Effect sizes were determined in one of three ways. First, and a model that allowed the parameter estimates to dif-
some adoption and twin studies used a continuous variable fer across the different levels of the moderator. The ACE
to measure antisocial behavior and reported either Pearson model was the best fitting model for diagnosis (a2 = 0.44,
product moment or intraclass correlations, which were the c2 = 0.11, e2 = 0.45), aggression (a2 = 0.44, c2 = 0.06,
effect sizes used from these studies in the meta-analysis. Sec- e2 = 0.50), and antisocial behavior (a2 = 0.47, c2 = 0.22,
ond, a dichotomous variable was used, and concordances, e2 = 0.31), whereas the ADE model was the best fitting
percentages, or a contingency table (including the number model for criminality (a2 = 0.33, d2 = 0.42, e2 = 0.25).
of twin pairs with both twins affected, one twin affected, and Within the operationalization of diagnosis, the a2 estimate
neither twin affected) was reported. The information from the was higher in studies examining CD (a2 = 0.50, c2 = 0.11,
concordances or percentages was transformed into a contin- e2 = 0.39), whereas the e2 estimate was higher in studies
gency table, which was then used to estimate the tetrachoric examining ASPD (a2 = 0.36, c2 = 0.10, e2 = 0.54).
correlation (i.e., the correlation between the latent contin- Assessment method also was a significant moderator, with
uous variables that are assumed to underlie the observed the ACE model fitting best for self-report (a2 = 0.39, c2 =
dichotomous variables). For these studies, the tetrachoric 0.06, e2 = 0.55) and report by others (a2 = 0.53, c2 = 0.22,
correlation was the effect size used in the meta-analysis. e2 = 0.25), whereas the AE model fit best for reaction to
Third, we were able to directly estimate the tetrachoric cor- aggressive stimuli (a2 = 0.52, e2 = 0.48). All of the stud-
relation from the raw data for some studies because we had ies using the assessment method of records were also studies
access to the data (e.g., Slutske et al., 1997; Waldman, Levy, examining criminality, for which the ADE model fits best
& Hay, 1995). (a2 = 0.33, d2 = 0.42, e2 = 0.25).
Alternative models positing that antisocial behavior is Age was a significant moderator, with the ACE model fit-
affected by additive genetic influences (A), shared environ- ting best for children (a2 = 0.46, c2 = 0.20, e2 = 0.34),
mental influences (C), non-additive genetic influences (D), adolescents (a2 = 0.43, c2 = 0.16, e2 = 0.41), and adults
and nonshared environmental influences (E) were tested. The (a2 = 0.41, c2 = 0.09, e2 = 0.50). The magnitude of famil-
ACE model, the AE model, the CE model, and the ADE ial influences (a2 and c2 ) decreased with age, whereas the
model were compared. Given that the ACDE model can be magnitude of non-familial influences (e2 ) increased with age.
tested only when both twin and adoption studies are included Operationalization, assessment method, and age were fre-
in the analysis, it was only possible to estimate c2 and d2 quently confounded in the studies included in the meta-
simultaneously when analyzing all of the data included in analysis. For example, parent report was more frequently
the meta-analysis. For other analyses (i.e., the comparison used in studies examining antisocial behavior than in stud-
between twin and adoption studies and the tests of modera- ies examining diagnosis or aggression, as well as in studies
tors), both twin and adoption studies were not always avail- examining children than in studies examining adolescents or
able across different types of studies. Therefore, we were adults. The results of analyses examining age as a signifi-
limited to comparing the ACE, AE, CE, and ADE models cant moderator were not consistent with DiLalla and Gottes-
for analyses other than those that included all data included man’s hypothesis that the magnitude of genetic influences
in the meta-analysis. should be higher in childhood and adulthood than in ado-
lescence. In contrast, the magnitude of genetic influence was
lower in both adolescence and adulthood than in childhood.
Overview of the Results Nonetheless, the presence of confounding among the moder-
ators must be considered, implying that the higher heritabil-
When all available data from both twin and adoption studies ity for childhood may actually reflect the higher heritability
were analyzed together and the magnitude of non-additive for parent report.
458 S.H. Rhee and I.D. Waldman

Zygosity determination method was a significant moder- the small number of studies addressing them in the liter-
ator, such that the ADE model was the best fitting model ature. Below, studies examining genotype × environment
for studies using blood grouping (a2 = 0.14, d2 = 0.33, interaction, co-occurrence with other psychiatric disorders,
e2 = 0.53), whereas the ACE model was the best fitting the etiology of psychopathy, and the etiology of adolescent-
model for studies using the questionnaire method (a2 = 0.43, limited versus life-course-persistent antisocial behavior are
c2 = 0.27, e2 = 0.30) and a combination of the two meth- reviewed.
ods (a2 = 0.39, c2 = 0.11, e2 = 0.50). These parameter
estimates are difficult to interpret, given that studies using
the most stringent method of zygosity determination (i.e., Genotype × Environment Interaction
blood grouping) and the least stringent method of zygosity
determination (i.e., questionnaire) yielded higher estimates In addition to estimating the main effects of genes and envi-
of genetic influences (broad h2 = 0.43–0.47) than studies ronments on various forms of antisocial behavior, researchers
using a combination of the two methods (broad h2 = 0.39). also have examined whether genes and the environment inter-
Although sex was a significant moderator when data from all act to influence antisocial behavior using both the adoption
studies were analyzed, there were no statistically significant design and the twin design, which have differing advantages
sex differences in studies that included both sexes (males: and disadvantages. The adoption study is the ideal method
a2 = 0.43, c2 = 0.19, e2 = 0.38; females: a2 = 0.41, for testing genotype–environment interactions because the
c2 = 0.20, e2 = 0.39). genetic and environmental influences on a trait are disen-
Parent–offspring adoption studies found a lower magni- tangled and can be measured distinctly. Unfortunately, the
tude of familial influences on antisocial behavior (i.e., lower power to test the genotype × environment interaction term
a2 and c2 and higher e2 ) than the twin and sibling adoption may be reduced in adoption studies of antisocial behavior
studies. There are several possible reasons for this result. because of range restriction in the variables used to indi-
First, the age difference between the children and their par- cate the environmental and/or genetic influences on antiso-
ents may lead to lower correlations, given that there may be cial behavior. McClelland & Judd (1993) demonstrated that
age- or cohort-specific genetic and/or environmental influ- restricting the range of the predictor variables will reduce the
ences. This age difference is absent in the twin studies residual variance of the product of the two predictors and, in
and smaller in the sibling adoption studies, thus supporting turn, the statistical power to detect an interaction. The prob-
this explanation. Second, because of the practical obstacles lem with range restriction is especially a concern in adoption
involved in conducting an adoption study, in several stud- studies of antisocial behavior because the chance of adoptees
ies different operationalizations and methods of assessment being placed in adoptive homes with criminal or antisocial
were used in the adoptees and their parents (e.g., criminality adoptive parents is very low. Therefore, the statistical diffi-
via official records in the parents and aggression via self- culties of detecting interactions should be considered in inter-
report in the adoptees). preting adoption studies examining gene–environment inter-
There was no statistically significant difference between actions. In contrast, genotype–environment interactions are
the results of twin studies and the sibling adoption studies. more difficult to test in twin studies because the genetic and
This result should be interpreted cautiously considering the environmental influences on a trait are likely to be correlated.
fact that 42 independent twin samples were compared with On the other hand, range restriction is less of a problem in
only 3 independent sibling adoption samples. Although the twin studies, where the samples are more representative of
power to detect a statistically significant difference between the general population.
the two types of studies may have been limited by the small Data from several adoption studies (Cadoret, Cain, &
number of sibling adoption studies, the parameter estimates Crowe, 1983; Cloninger, Sigvardsson, Bohman, & von Knor-
for the twin studies (a2 = 0.45, c2 = 0.12, e2 = 0.43) and ring, 1982; Mednick, Gabrielli, & Hutchings, 1983) show
the sibling adoption studies (a2 = 0.48, c2 = 0.13, e2 = evidence of genotype–environment interaction for antisocial
0.39) were very similar. behavior. Mednick et al. (1983) conducted a cross-fostering
analysis of Danish adoptees. Among adoptees who had a
criminal background in both their biological and adoptive
parents, 24.5% of them became criminal themselves. This is
Additional Issues in the Etiology of Antisocial
in comparison to 20% of adoptees who have a criminal back-
Behavior ground only in their biological parents, 14.7% of adoptees
who have a criminal background only in their adoptive par-
Several interesting issues in the etiology of antisocial behav- ents, and 13.5% of adoptees with no criminal background.
ior were beyond the scope of the meta-analysis, or we were Cloninger et al. (1982) found similar results for petty crim-
unable to conduct a quantitative review of these issues given inality in Swedish adoptees when they considered both bio-
30 Genetic Analysis of Conduct Disorder and Antisocial Behavior 459

logical variables (i.e., criminal biological parents) and envi- from the lowest (3.5%) to the highest (69.6%) genetic risk
ronmental variables (i.e., negative rearing experiences and group. That is, genetic risk had less effect on conduct prob-
adoptive placement). Among adoptees with both biologi- lems in the non-maltreated group (i.e., the more benign envi-
cal and environmental risks, 40% were criminal. This is in ronment) than in the maltreated group (i.e., the less benign
comparison to 12.1% of those with only biological risk fac- environment).
tors, 6.7% of those with only environmental risk factors, and The Jaffee et al. (2005) study differs from the Rowe
2.9% of those with neither biological nor environmental risk et al. (1999) and Button et al. (2005) studies in several
factors. Also, in a sample of adoptees from Iowa, Cadoret ways, including the specific environmental variable exam-
et al. (1983) found that when both genetic and environmen- ined and the analytical method used. It is possible that
tal risk factors were present, they accounted for a greater the effects of the interaction between genes and family
number of antisocial behaviors than an additive combination warmth/dysfunction on antisocial behavior are different from
of the two kinds of risk factors acting independently. The those of the interaction between genes and physical mal-
genotype–environment interactions were not statistically sig- treatment. Additional studies examining genotype × specific
nificant in Cloninger et al. (1982) or Mednick et al. (1983), environmental influence interactions on antisocial behavior
most likely given reduced power to test an interaction in the are needed.
presence of range restriction.
More recently, several twin studies have examined the
interaction between genes and specific environmental influ- Co-occurrence with Other Psychiatric Disorders
ences. Rowe, Almeida, and Jacobson (1999) examined the
interaction between genetic influences and family warmth Antisocial behavior co-occurs with several other psy-
on aggression and reported that heritability increased and chiatric disorders such as major depressive disorders,
the magnitude of shared environmental influences decreased attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and drug
with greater family warmth. Rowe et al. suggested the dependence. In recent years, many researchers have exam-
possibility that greater genetic influences are required for ined the degree to which the covariance between antisocial
the expression of aggression in more benign environments, behavior and other psychiatric disorders is due to common
whereas social norms and peer models may lead to the genetic or environmental influences.
expression of aggression in individuals without genetic pre- There is evidence of significant co-occurrence between
disposition in more adverse environments. A recent study conduct disorder (CD) and ADHD, which occur together in
by Button, Scourfield, Martin, Purcell, and McGuffin (2005) 30–50% of the cases in both epidemiological and clinical
supports Rowe et al.’s findings. They examined the interac- samples (Biederman, Newcorn, & Sprich, 1991). The results
tion between genes and family dysfunction and concluded of several multivariate behavior genetic studies examining
that the heritability of antisocial behavior decreased and the co-occurrence between ADHD and CD are largely con-
the magnitude of both shared environmental and nonshared sistent, with most studies (Dick, Viken, Kaprio, Pulkkinen, &
environmental influences increased as family dysfunction Rose, 2005; Nadder, Rutter, Silberg, Maes, & Eaves, 2002;
increased. Nadder, Silberg, Eaves, Maes, & Meyer, 1998; Silberg
Jaffee et al. (2005) examined the effect of the interac- et al., 1996; Thapar, Harrington, & McGuffin, 2001; Wald-
tion between genetic influences and physical maltreatment man, Rhee, & Levy, 2001) reporting a substantial overlap
on conduct problems in a different way. They divided their between the genetic influences on ADHD and the genetic
twin sample into four groups based on levels of genetic risk influences on CD. An exception was Burt, Krueger, McGue,
as a function of their co-twin’s conduct disorder status and and Iacono’s (2001) examination of ADHD, oppositional
the pair’s zygosity (i.e., lowest risk – MZ co-twin with no defiant disorder (ODD), and CD, in which a single shared
CD diagnosis; low risk – DZ co-twin with no CD diagno- environmental factor was the largest contributor to the
sis; high risk – DZ co-twin with CD diagnosis; highest risk covariation among ADHD, ODD, and CD.
– MZ co-twin with CD diagnosis) and absence/presence of Researchers also have noted the high rate of co-
maltreatment. The maltreated group with the highest genetic occurrence between CD and MDD, with reported rates of
risk had the highest probability of a CD diagnosis. There was co-occurrence between depression and CD/ODD ranging
a significant interaction between genetic risk and maltreat- from 21 to 83% (Angold & Costello, 1993). There are three
ment, but the conclusions are different from those reached published studies examining the etiology of the occurrence
by Rowe et al. (1999) and Button et al. (2005). In the non- between CD and MDD or the broader constructs of exter-
maltreated group, there was a 44.2% increase in the probabil- nalizing and internalizing behavior/disorders, and the results
ity of a CD diagnosis from the lowest (1.9%) to the highest of these studies are conflicting. Gjone and Stevenson (1997)
(46.1%) genetic risk group. In the maltreated group, there examined the covariance between parent reports of inter-
was a 66.1% increase in the probability of a CD diagnosis nalizing and externalizing behavior problems in adolescent
460 S.H. Rhee and I.D. Waldman

twins and concluded that although there are both common tricity, and dishonesty. Personality characteristics are empha-
genetic and shared environmental influences on the covari- sized in this condition, rather than overt antisocial acts.
ance between internalizing and externalizing behavior, there Researchers hypothesize that psychopathic individuals, who
is more consistent evidence for common shared environ- represent a subset of individuals diagnosed with CD or
mental influences. O’Connor, McGuire, Reiss, Hetherington, ASPD, may be those who are most likely to engage in life-
and Plomin (1998) examined the covariation between anti- course-persistent antisocial behavior (Viding, Blair, Mof-
social behavior and depressive symptoms assessed using a fitt, & Plomin, 2005) and instrumental aggression (Blair,
composite of parent report, self-report, and observers’ report Peschardt, Budhani, Mitchell, & Pine, 2006), both of which
in adolescents and found that there were moderate genetic, are particularly heritable.
shared environmental, and nonshared environmental influ- Recently, Waldman and Rhee (2006) presented the results
ences on the covariation between the two symptom domains. of a meta-analysis of the small number of behavior genetic
In Kendler, Prescott, Myers, and Neale’s (2003) examina- studies examining psychopathy. These included the stud-
tion of self-reported internalizing and externalizing disorders ies reviewed in the earlier Rhee & Waldman (2002) meta-
in adults, the authors found that genetic factors influencing analysis (Brandon & Rose, 1995; DiLalla, Carey, Gottes-
externalizing disorders had little influence on internalizing man, & Bouchard, 1996; Gottesman, 1963, 1965; Loehlin
disorders, and genetic factors influencing internalizing disor- & Nichols, 1976; Loehlin, Willerman, & Horn, 1987; Taylor,
ders had little influence on externalizing disorders. (They did Iacono, and McGue, 2000; Torgersen, Skre, Onstad, Edvard-
not find a clear distinction for shared environmental influ- sen, & Kringlen, 1993) as well more recent results from
ences and nonshared environmental influences on external- Taylor, Loney, Bobadilla, Iacono, and McGue (2003) and
izing and internalizing disorders.) Several methodological Blonigen, Carlson, Krueger, and Patrick (2003). In contrast
differences among these studies may explain their differing to the aforementioned overall results for antisocial behavior
conclusions, including the specific constructs examined, the (a2 = 0.32, d2 = 0.09, c2 = 0.16, e2 = 0.43), there is little
assessment methods used, and the age of the participants. evidence for shared environmental influences on psychopa-
Conduct disorder also commonly co-occurs with sub- thy, and the AE model fits best (h2 = 0.49, e2 = 0.51).
stance use and substance use disorders. For example, Reebye, The results from an interesting study by Viding
Moretti, and Lessard (1995) reported that 52% of adoles- et al. (2005) were not included in the meta-analysis because it
cent inpatients with CD also had a substance use disor- examined the magnitude of genetic and environmental influ-
der. Researchers have concluded that there are genetic influ- ences on extreme status on psychopathy (whereas the other
ences on the co-occurrence between antisocial behavior and studies examined the etiology of psychopathy in the general
substance use/substance use disorders across a range of population). Viding et al.’s study is also the first examina-
substances. Silberg, Rutter, D’Onofrio, and Eaves (2003) tion of the etiology of psychopathy in children. They exam-
found significant common genetic influences on smoking ined a sample of 7-year-old twin pairs in the general popu-
and conduct disturbance. Slutske et al. (1998) reported that lation and then selected probands based on extremely high
genetic influences accounted for most of the covariance scores on antisocial behavior and callous-unemotional traits.
(with nonshared environmental influences accounting for the Their results are similar to those from the Waldman and
remainder) between CD and alcohol dependence. Miles, van Rhee meta-analysis. Extreme antisocial behavior accompa-
den Bree, and Pickens (2002) found evidence of moder- nied by extreme callous-unemotional traits was highly heri-
ate common genetic influences and low common nonshared table (h2 g = 0.81) with no evidence of shared environmen-
environmental influences on CD and marijuana use. Grove tal influences, but extreme antisocial behavior unaccompa-
et al. (1990) found significant genetic correlations between nied by extreme callous-unemotional traits was less heritable
illegal drug problems and both childhood and adult antisocial (h2 g = 0.30) and affected by shared environmental influ-
behavior. Recently, Button et al. (2006) reported that there ences (c2 g = 0.34).
were moderate genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared Recently, Larsson, Andershed, and Lichtenstein (2006)
environmental influences on the covariance between CD and published a twin study examining the etiology of psychopa-
polysubstance dependence vulnerability. These studies are thy in adolescents. Twins aged 16–17 years were assessed via
consistent in suggesting that common genetic influences play the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory (YPI). Their results
a significant role in the substantial co-occurrence of conduct are also consistent with those of previous studies examining
problems and substance use and/or dependence. the etiology of psychopathy, as they found moderate genetic
and nonshared environmental influences and little evidence
of shared environmental influences on all three YPI dimen-
The Etiology of Psychopathy sions (grandiose/manipulative – a2 = 0.51, c2 = 0.03,
e2 = 0.46; callous/unemotional – a2 = 0.43, c2 = 0.00,
Psychopathy is a personality-based construct characterized e2 = 0.57; impulsive/irresponsible – a2 = 0.56, c2 = 0.00,
by superficial charm, lack of empathy, lack of guilt, egocen- e2 = 0.44). A common pathway model (where the covari-
30 Genetic Analysis of Conduct Disorder and Antisocial Behavior 461

ance among the three YPI dimensions is represented by an influences, whereas juvenile antisocial traits were largely
intermediate latent variable) fits the data well, and the her- influenced by shared environmental influences. Taylor,
itability of the latent psychopathic personality factor was Iacono, and McGue (2000) examined antisocial behavior
slightly higher (h2 = 0.63, c2 = 0.00, e2 = 0.37) than those in the co-twins of probands with early-onset and late-onset
of the three YPI dimensions. delinquency. Monozygotic co-twins of probands with early-
Another recent paper by Blonigen, Hicks, Krueger, onset delinquency were more antisocial than dizygotic co-
Patrick, and Iacono (2005) examined the co-occurrence twins of probands with early-onset delinquency, but the
between psychopathic traits and internalizing and externaliz- monozygotic and dizygotic co-twins of probands with late-
ing psychopathology. They found evidence of genetic influ- onset delinquency had similar levels of antisocial behav-
ences on two distinct psychopathic traits, fearless dominance ior. Finally, Jacobson, Neale, Prescott, and Kendler (2001)
(which is characterized by social potency, stress immu- reported that adolescent antisocial behavior had a lower
nity, and fearlessness) and impulsive antisociality (which heritability and a greater magnitude of shared environmen-
is characterized by negative emotionality and low behav- tal influences in their subsample of youth whose antisocial
ioral constraint). Genetic influences on fearless dominance behavior did not persist into adulthood than in their subsam-
were negatively correlated with those on internalizing ple of youth whose antisocial behavior did persist into adult-
psychopathology, whereas genetic influences on impulsive hood.
antisociality were positively correlated with those on exter-
nalizing psychopathology.
Examination of the Influence of Specific Genes
and QTLs on Antisocial Behavior
Genetic Influences on Adolescent-Limited Versus
Life-Course-Persistent Antisocial Behavior Given the evidence of moderate genetic influences on con-
duct disorder and antisocial behavior, many researchers have
In a seminal review paper in 1993, Moffitt hypothesized investigated the relations of specific genes with antisocial
that there are two developmental types of antisocial behavior behavior in linkage and association studies. The number of
that have different etiologies. Life-course-persistent antiso- association and linkage studies examining antisocial behav-
cial behavior begins in early childhood, continues throughout ior is far fewer than those examining disorders or phenotypes
the child’s lifetime, and is posited to show stronger genetic that are often comorbid with antisocial behavior, such as
influences and greater deficits in neuropsychological func- ADHD, alcoholism, and suicidality. Thus, many researchers
tioning. In contrast, adolescent-limited antisocial behavior is examining these overlapping phenotypes also have examined
posited to be more affected by environmental influences such the evidence for association of specific genes with antiso-
as antisocial peer models. Therefore, Moffitt hypothesizes cial behavior in clinical samples with ADHD (e.g., Thapar
that the magnitude of genetic influences should be higher for et al., 2005), alcoholism and drug abuse (e.g., Soyka, Preuss,
antisocial behavior occurring in childhood or adulthood than Koller, Zill, & Bondy, 2004), suicidality (e.g., Zalsman
in adolescence. et al., 2001), and schizophrenia (e.g., Strous et al., 2003). In
The results of the meta-analysis do not support this the present review, we focused on studies of antisocial behav-
hypothesis, as the magnitude of genetic influences on antiso- ior in the general population, clinical samples with antisocial
cial behavior was lower in both adolescence and adulthood behavior, or general psychiatric samples, given the possibil-
than in childhood. However, it is possible that this result is ity that some of the conflicting results and failures to replicate
due to the presence of confounding between age and assess- across studies may be due to sample differences.
ment method (i.e., most studies that examined the heritabil- Broadly speaking, there are two general strategies for
ity of antisocial behavior in childhood used parent reports, identifying quantitative trait loci (QTLs) that contribute to
and most studies that examined the heritability of antisocial the etiology of a disorder or trait, namely linkage and asso-
behavior in adolescence and adulthood used self-reports). ciation. The purpose of a linkage study is to test whether the
Given that studies that use parent reports have yielded higher putative risk-inducing gene is at a particular chromosomal
heritabilities than studies that use self-reports, the higher her- locus, whereas the purpose of an association study is to test
itability for antisocial behavior in childhood may thus reflect whether a particular allele of a candidate gene is associated
the higher heritability for parent report. with the disorder or trait.
Several studies that have examined adolescent-limited and In genome scans, evidence for linkage between a disorder
life-course-persistent antisocial behavior in the same sam- or trait and evenly spaced DNA markers distributed across
ple using the same assessment method have reported results the entire genome is evaluated. Evidence for linkage between
supporting Moffitt’s hypothesis. Lyons et al. (1995) con- any of these DNA markers and the trait or disorder of interest
trasted the etiology of adult and juvenile antisocial traits and implicates a broad segment of the genome that may contain
reported that adult antisocial traits had significant genetic hundreds of genes, and lack of evidence for linkage can, in
462 S.H. Rhee and I.D. Waldman

some cases, be used to exclude genomic segments. Subse- dopaminergic and serotonergic neurotransmitter pathways
quent fine-grained linkage analyses can then use a new set of may be conjectured based on several lines of converging evi-
more tightly grouped markers within the implicated genomic dence suggesting a role for these neurotransmitter systems
region to locate the functional mutation. Thus, genome scans in the etiology and pathophysiology of these traits and their
may be thought of as exploratory searches for putative genes relevant disorders.
that contribute to the etiology of a disorder. The fact that
major genes have been found for many medical diseases via
genome scans without a priori knowledge of those genes’ Association Studies
function or etiological significance is a testament to the use-
fulness of this method. Unfortunately, the power of linkage Dopamine transmission. There is significant co-occurrence
analyses in genome scans is typically quite low, making it between CD and ADHD, with 30–50% of cases having
very difficult, if not impossible, to detect QTLs that account both disorders in both epidemiological and clinical sam-
for less than ∼15% of the variance in a disorder. There- ples (Biederman et al., 1991). Also, several twin studies
fore, the promise for linkage-based genome scans of complex examining the co-occurrence between the two disorders sug-
traits remains largely unknown. gest that there are genetic influences common to both (Dick
In contrast to linkage, association has the statistical power et al., 2005; Nadder et al., 1998, 2002; Silberg et al., 1996;
to detect QTLs that account for a relatively small percent- Waldman et al., 2001). Therefore, specific genes associated
age of the variance in a disorder. A disadvantage of asso- with ADHD also may influence CD.
ciation is that a QTL cannot be detected unless the marker There are several lines of evidence suggesting that the
being examined is very close to the QTL. Therefore, many dopamine transporter gene is associated with ADHD. The
more DNA markers would be needed to conduct a genome dopamine transporter is the site of action of stimulant med-
scan using association, which has been made possible by ications effective in the treatment of ADHD (Winsberg &
recent advances in array-based genotyping technologies. Comings, 1999), and dopamine transporter levels are approx-
Until recently, researchers have used association solely to imately 70% higher in the brains of individuals with ADHD
conduct candidate gene studies. than those of controls (Dougherty et al., 1999). “Knock-
In many ways, candidate gene studies are polar opposites out” mice with a deletion of both copies of the dopamine
of genome scans. In contrast to the exploratory nature of transporter are hyperactive (Giros, Jaber, Jones, Wightman,
genome scans, well-conducted candidate gene studies repre- & Caron, 1996) and in humans, a VNTR polymorphism in
sent a targeted test of the role of specific genes in the etiology the 3 untranslated region of the dopamine transporter gene
of a disorder as the location, function, and etiological rele- has shown association with ADHD in previous studies, with
vance of candidate genes are most often known or strongly the more common 10-repeat allele being the risk allele (e.g.,
hypothesized a priori. Thus, an advantage of well-conducted Cook et al., 1995; Faraone et al., 2005; Mill et al., 2005;
candidate gene studies in comparison with genome scans is Waldman et al., 1998).
that positive findings are easily interpretable because one Given such evidence, two studies have examined the
already knows the gene’s location, function, and etiological evidence of association between this polymorphism of the
relevance, even if the specific polymorphism(s) chosen for dopamine transporter gene and externalizing behavior in
study in the candidate gene is not functional and the func- children. Jorm, Prior, Sanson, Smart, Zhang, and East-
tional mutation(s) in the candidate gene is as yet uniden- eal (2001) examined the association between the DAT1 gene
tified. There are also disadvantages to the candidate gene and externalizing behavior problems and associated temper-
approach given that only previously identified genes can be ament traits in a longitudinal study of children from age
studied. Thus, one cannot find genes that one has not looked 3 to 16 in the general population. They examined whether
for or have yet to be discovered, and because there are rela- children with the 10-repeat/10-repeat, 10-repeat/non 10-
tively few strong candidate genes for psychiatric disorders, repeat, and non 10-repeat/non 10-repeat genotypes differed
the same genes are examined as candidates for almost all in the mean level of behavior problems but found no evi-
psychiatric disorders, regardless of how disparate the disor- dence of association between the DAT1 gene and any of the
ders may be in terms of their symptomatology or conjectured phenotypes examined, including parent reports of hostile-
pathophysiology. aggressive behavior, conduct disorder, socialized aggression,
In well-designed studies, however, knowledge regarding or oppositional behavior, at any age.
the biology of the disorder is used to select genes based on Young et al. (2002) examined the association between
the known or hypothesized involvement of their gene product the same VNTR polymorphism of the DAT1 gene and par-
in the etiology of the trait or disorder (i.e., its pathophysio- ent reports of externalizing behavior at ages 4, 7, and 9.
logical function and etiological relevance). With respect to A community sample of children from twin and adoption
antisocial behavior, genes underlying various aspects of the studies was examined, using a sibling-based association
30 Genetic Analysis of Conduct Disorder and Antisocial Behavior 463

method (Fulker, Cherny, Sham, & Hewitt, 1999) that sepa- examination. Retz et al. divided the subjects into a vio-
rates the allelic effect into between-family effects (that may lent and nonviolent group and found that the S allele was
include possible population stratification effects) and within- significantly over-represented in the violent group. More
family effects (where population stratification is controlled recently, Sakai et al. (2006) found that the S allele is a risk
by the inclusion of siblings). In contrast to evidence from allele for conduct disorder with aggression in a case–control
ADHD studies suggesting that the 10-repeat allele is the risk- study comparing conduct-disordered adolescents and non-
inducing allele, Young et al. found that the 9-repeat was the conduct-disordered controls as well as in a transmission dis-
risk-inducing allele for externalizing behavior at ages 4 and equilibrium test examining conduct-disordered adolescents
7, but not at age 9. Young et al. cite that the less frequent and their parents. Beitchman et al. (2006) reported similar
9-repeat allele has been shown to be the risk-inducing allele results in a study examining three alleles of the 5-HTTLPR
for other phenotypes correlated with externalizing behavior, polymorphism (i.e., S, Lg, and La; La is “high transcrib-
such as alcohol dependence (Sander et al., 1997; Schmidt, ing”; Lg is “low transcribing”, similar to S); they found
Harms, Kuhn, Rommelspacher, & Sander, 1998) and cocaine that the “low-expressing” genotypes of the 5-HTTLPR poly-
intoxication (Gelernter, Kranzler, Satel, & Rao, 1994). morphism (S/S, Lg/S, Lg/Lg) are associated with childhood
Serotonin transmission. Several lines of evidence suggest aggression in a case–control study.
that genes influencing serotonergic neurotransmission In an adoption study sample, Cadoret et al. (2003) found a
may be associated with conduct disorder and antisocial complex pattern of association between the serotonin trans-
behavior. Low cerebrospinal fluid concentrations of porter gene and externalizing behavior (i.e., aggressivity,
5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid, the major metabolite of conduct disorder, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-
serotonin, are found in aggressive subjects (e.g., Kruesi der). In the presence of antisocial biological parents, the
et al., 1990; Lidberg, Tuck, Asberg, Scalia-Tomba, & L allele was associated with increased externalizing behav-
Bertilsson, 1985). Also, pharmacological challenge trials ior, whereas in the presence of alcoholism in the biological
suggest an association between low serotonin functioning parents, the S allele was associated with increased external-
and violence or aggression. For example, aggression is izing behavior. The authors also found a significant genotype
associated with blunted prolactin (PRL) responses to by gender interaction, with the S allele being associated with
the treatment of 5-HT releaser, d-fenfluramine (Coccaro, higher externalizing behavior in males and the S allele being
Kavoussi, Cooper, & Hauger, 1997; Coccaro et al., 1987). associated with lower externalizing behavior in females.
Given this evidence, researchers have examined the Haberstick, Smolen, and Hewitt (2006) also reported mixed
association between antisocial behavior and several genes findings. Family-based association tests were conducted in a
involved in serotonergic transmission, including the twin sample, examining parent and teacher reports of aggres-
serotonin transporter gene, the HTR1B gene, and the sive behavior at ages 7, 9, 10, 11, and 12, with teacher reports
tryptophan hydroxylase gene. also available at age 8. A statistically significant result was
The serotonin transporter is known to influence seroton- obtained only for teacher reports of aggressive behavior at
ergic transmission and several researchers have examined a age 9, with the S allele being the risk allele.
polymorphism in the promoter area with two allelic vari- Given research suggesting that genes involved in sero-
ants that differ in transcriptional activity (5-HTTLPR). The tonin functioning may be candidate genes for aggression and
S or short allele of the polymorphism has lower transcrip- the evidence that 5-HT1B knockout mice show increased
tional activity (Heils et al., 1996) and is associated with a aggression (Saudou et al., 1994), New et al. (2001) examined
blunted response in fenfluramine-induced prolactin release the association between a common polymorphism caused by
pharmacological challenges (e.g., Reist, Mazzanti, Vu, Tran, a silent G to C substitution in the HTR1B gene and self-
& Goldman, 2001), whereas serotonin transporter availabil- reported impulsive aggression in a sample of subjects with
ity is higher in individuals with the L or long allele than in one or more personality disorders but found no evidence of
those with the S allele (Heinz et al., 2000). association.
Baca-Garcia et al. (2004) examined the association Tryptophan hydroxylase (TPH) is the rate-limiting
between the serotonin transporter gene promoter linked enzyme in the serotonin pathway in the conversion of trypto-
region and impulsivity and aggressive behavior among sui- phan into 5-hydroxytryptophan, the direct precursor of 5-HT.
cide attempters and a control group of blood donors and Researchers have examined two common SNPs in intron 7
found no difference among the SS, SL, and LL geno- of the TPH gene, A218C and A779C, that are in strong link-
types in the level of impulsivity or history of aggressive age disequilibrium. The genotypes of these two SNPs were
behavior in either group. In contrast, Retz, Retz-Junginger, found to be identical in nearly 100% of the sample in one
Supprian, Thome, and Rösler (2004) found a significant asso- Japanese study (Kunugi et al., 1999), with the 218A and
ciation between the serotonin transporter gene and violent the 779A being linked and the 218C and 779C alleles being
behavior in a sample of adult males referred for a forensic linked.
464 S.H. Rhee and I.D. Waldman

The results of studies examining the association between have examined a functional 30-bp variable number of tan-
the TPH gene and antisocial behavior are conflicting. Two dem repeat polymorphism in the promoter region of the
studies suggest that the A allele of the A218C/A779C poly- MAOA gene, for example, in which alleles 2 and 3 (i.e.,
morphism is associated with increased antisocial behavior. 3.5 or 4 repeats) have shown greater transcriptional activ-
Manuck et al. (1999) examined association of the A218C ity than alleles 1 and 4 (i.e., 3 or 5 repeats; Sabol, Hu, &
polymorphism of the TPH gene with aggression and anger- Hamer, 1998).
related traits and found that the A allele was associated Manuck, Flory, Ferrell, Mann, and Muldoon (2000) exam-
with higher aggression, a tendency to experience unpro- ined the association between the VNTR polymorphism of
voked anger, and a tendency to express anger outwardly. the MAOA gene and aggression and impulsivity in a male
The A allele was also associated with attenuated peak community sample. In contrast to the results expected based
prolactin response to fenfluramine, although this relation- on the findings from the Dutch kindred study (Brunner, Nel-
ship was only found in men. Hennig, Reuter, Netter, Burk, son, et al., 1993; Brunner, Nelen, et al., 1993) and the trans-
and Landt (2005) examined the association between the genic mouse study (Cases et al., 1995), individuals with the 1
TPH A779C polymorphism and aggression in a community and 4 alleles (i.e., with lower transcriptional activity) scored
sample. They examined two different types of aggression lower on the composite measure of aggression and impul-
assessed by the Buss–Durkee hostility inventory, neurotic sivity than individuals with the 2 and 3 alleles (i.e., with
hostility and aggressive hostility, and found that the A allele higher transcriptional activity). Also, individuals in the 1/4
of the A779C polymorphism of the TPH gene was associated allele group also showed more pronounced central nervous
with aggressive hostility but not neurotic hostility. In con- system serotonergic responsivity than individuals in the 2/3
trast to Hennig et al. and Manuck et al., Staner et al. (2002) allele group in a neuropsychopharmacologic challenge (i.e.,
found that the C allele of the A218C polymorphism was prolactin response to fenfluramine hydrochloride).
associated with a higher level of impulsive aggression in Caspi et al. (2002) examined the interaction between
a sample of nonpsychotic, impulsive inpatients. Similarly, the MAOA gene and maltreatment early in life, given evi-
New et al. (1998) examined the association between the dence that maltreatment in early life alters norepinephrine,
A218C polymorphism and impulsive aggression in a sam- serotonin, and dopamine systems (e.g., Bremner & Vermet-
ple of patients with personality disorders and found that ten, 2001). They hypothesized that maltreated children with
the C allele was associated with higher levels of impulsive a genotype that confers high levels of MAOA expression
aggression. would be less likely to develop antisocial behavior, and
Staner et al. (2002) suggest that the differences in sam- tested this hypothesis in a longitudinal community sample
ple composition may account for the discrepancies in these of male adolescents. The main effect of MAOA on antiso-
results, as the association with the C allele was found in cial behavior was not significant, although the main effect
psychiatric samples (New et al., 1998; Staner et al., 2002) of maltreatment was significant. The effect of maltreatment
and not in community samples (Manuck et al., 1999; Staner on antisocial behavior was much stronger in males with the
et al., 2002). Hennig et al. (2005) suggest that differences genotype conferring low MAOA activity than in males with
in the conclusions may be due to differences in the type of the genotype conferring high MAOA activity. This was a sig-
aggression being examined, as they found evidence for asso- nificant finding, as it is one of the first demonstrations of an
ciation of the A allele with aggressive hostility, but not with interaction between a measured gene and a measured envi-
neurotic hostility, which was more similar to the measure of ronmental influence on a behavioral trait in humans. Caspi et
impulsive aggression examined in New et al. (1998). al.’s results are not necessarily in contrast to those of Manuck
MAOA. Monoamine oxidase A is a degradative enzyme et al., given that among individuals with no maltreatment
that catalyzes deamination of serotonin, norepinephrine, and experience, those with the genotype conferring low MAOA
dopamine. Two lines of evidence suggest that the gene activity had lower antisocial behavior than those with the
coding for MAOA may be related to antisocial behavior. genotype conferring high MAOA activity.
First, deletion of the gene encoding MAOA led to height- Since the publication of Caspi et al.’s results (2002),
ened aggression in transgenic mice (Cases et al. 1995). Sec- several researchers have attempted to replicate the MAOA-
ond, a rare MAOA point mutation resulting in no MAOA by-maltreatment interaction on antisocial behavior, with
enzyme was associated with mild mental retardation and varying results. Huang et al. (2004) found a significant inter-
impulsive aggression in males in a large Dutch kindred action in a psychiatric sample, reporting that the high-activity
(Brunner, Nelson, et al., 1993; Brunner, Nelson, Breakefield, MAOA allele was associated with lower impulsivity in those
Ropers, & van Oost, 1993). Although this mutation is rare reporting early childhood abuse. However, this finding did
and unlikely to predict aggressive behavior in the general not extend to measures of aggression or hostility, and simi-
population, other common allelic variations of the MAOA lar results were not found in females. In males, Huang et al.
gene may be associated with aggression. Several researchers found that childhood abuse was more common in individuals
30 Genetic Analysis of Conduct Disorder and Antisocial Behavior 465

with the low-activity MAOA allele, suggesting evidence of Recently, Kim-Cohen et al. (2006) reported replication of
evocative gene–environment correlation. the Caspi et al. (2002) findings in children assessed at age 5
Foley et al. (2004) examined the interaction between and 7. As in Caspi et al., maltreatment had a stronger effect
the MAOA gene and adverse childhood environment on antisocial behavior in boys with the low MAOA activ-
(i.e., parental neglect, exposure to interparental violence, ity genotype than in boys with the high-activity genotype.
and inconsistent parental discipline) on conduct disor- In addition, Kim-Cohen conducted a meta-analysis of five
der in a community twin sample. They replicated Caspi studies examining the maltreatment by MAOA interaction on
et al.’s (2002) findings, reporting that the low MAOA activ- antisocial behavior (Caspi et al., 2002; Foley et al., 2004;
ity allele was a risk factor for conduct disorder only in Haberstick et al., 2005; Kim-Cohen et al., 2006; Nilsson
the presence of adverse childhood environment. Most of et al., 2005) and found support for the original Caspi et al.
the power to detect this interaction came from the individ- finding.
uals with extremely adverse childhood environment. Foley
et al. (2004) also examined the evidence for evocative
gene–environment correlation, but in contrast to Huang
et al. (2004), found that the low MAOA activity allele did not Linkage Studies
predict exposure to childhood adversity. Nilsson et al. (2005)
found support for the initial Caspi et al. (2002) finding There are no linkage studies of conduct disorder examining a
in a study examining the interaction between MAOA and general population sample or a clinical sample being treated
a broader construct of psychosocial risk (i.e., dwelling in only for conduct problems. However, two recent linkage-
multi-family housing and experiences with violent victim- based genome-wide screens have been conducted in sam-
ization). They reported a stronger effect of psychosocial risk ples ascertained for substance use problems to search for loci
on antisocial behavior in boys with the low-activity MAOA influencing conduct disorder.
genotype than in boys with the high-activity MAOA geno- Dick et al. (2004) published the first genome-wide link-
type. Widom and Brzustowicz (2006) attempted to replicate age scan of conduct disorder in an adult sample collected
the Caspi et al. (2002) finding in participants in a prospective as part of the Collaborative Study on Genetics of Alco-
cohort design study. The interaction between MAOA geno- holism (COGA). They examined retrospectively reported
type and abuse/neglect was replicated in Caucasians but not childhood conduct disorder and conduct disorder symptoms
in non-white participants. in a sample of alcoholic probands and their first-degree rel-
Haberstick et al. (2005) also examined the interaction atives. They conducted nonparametric, multipoint linkage
between MAOA and maltreatment on antisocial behavior analyses using affected sibling pairs, reporting lod scores
in a community sample. Despite having adequate power, equal to or greater than 1.5 for CD diagnosis at chromo-
they were not able to replicate Caspi et al.’s (2002) find- some 19p13 at 35 cM (lod score = 2.14), chromosome 2p11
ing. However, they found a nonsignificant trend in the at 136 cM (lod score = 1.65), chromosome 12q13 at 78
predicted direction, with individuals with the high-activity cM (lod score = 1.79), and chromosome 3q12 at 134 cM
MAOA allele having lower antisocial behavior in the mal- (lod score = 1.60). For CD symptoms as a quantitative trait,
treated group and individuals with the low-activity MAOA suggestive evidence for linkage was found at chromosome
allele having a lower level of antisocial behavior in the non- 1q32 at 34 cM (lod score = 2.17) and at chromosome 19q12
maltreated group. Huizinga et al. (2006) examined the inter- at 46 cM (lod score = 2.1).
action between MAOA and self-reported physical abuse and Stallings et al. (2005) examined evidence for linkage for
violent victimization in participants in the National Youth conduct disorder symptoms in a clinical sample being treated
Survey Family Study. Maltreatment by a parent was signifi- for comorbid substance use disorders and antisocial behav-
cantly related to adolescent and adult antisocial and violence- ioral problems. Using DeFries–Fulker linkage analysis, a
related behavioral problems, but the main effect of MAOA regression-based sibling pair linkage method, they found
and the MAOA-by-maltreatment interaction was not sig- two peaks yielding lod scores greater than 1 for conduct
nificant. Similar results were found for violent victimiza- disorder symptoms, one on chromosome 9q34 at 162 cM
tion. Young et al. (2006) examined the interaction between (lod score = 1.76) and the other on chromosome 17q12 at
MAOA and maltreatment in a group of adolescents being 54 cM (lod score = 1.26). Unfortunately, there are no over-
treated for significant conduct and substance use problems. laps between the suggestive linkage peaks reported in Dick
They were unable to replicate Caspi et al.’s (2002) find- et al. (2004) and Stallings et al. (2005). Although Stallings
ings, reporting that there were no significant differences et al. (2005) found a suggestive peak on chromosome 3q24-
in the relationship between maltreatment and conduct dis- 3q25 at 173 cM with a lod score of 1.63 for substance depen-
order between the low- and high-activity MAOA geno- dence vulnerability, a similar peak was not found for conduct
types. disorder symptoms.
466 S.H. Rhee and I.D. Waldman

Future Directions little consensus in the findings of these studies. There are
several possible reasons for conflicting results and failures
to replicate, including small sample sizes, differences in the
Twin and Adoption Studies
types of samples employed, differences in the range of phe-
notypes examined, differences in analytical approaches, and
The results of Rhee & Waldman’s (2002) meta-analysis sug- the possibility of population stratification. Future directions
gest several future directions for twin and adoption studies for association studies include the need to increase our under-
of antisocial behavior. First, there may be meaningful dis- standing regarding how the various operationalizations of
tinctions in the operationalizations of antisocial behavior that antisocial behavior are related to each other and apply this
need further examination, such as violent versus nonviolent knowledge to the search for specific genes influencing antiso-
crime, criminality versus delinquency, relational versus overt cial behavior. For example, a highly heritable common latent
aggression, and instrumental versus reactive aggression. Sec- phenotype or endophenotype for antisocial behavior may be
ond, multivariate analyses examining the extent to which a better phenotype than conduct disorder symptoms, which
the different operationalizations of antisocial behavior have generally show moderate shared environmental influences.
common or specific genetic and environmental influences The differences across studies suggest a need for increased
are needed. An example of such an analysis is Cloninger collaboration among researchers investigating the genetics
& Gottesman’s (1987) finding that there is little genetic of antisocial behavior. Such collaboration among researchers
overlap between violent and nonviolent crime. Such results examining attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder has been
will be very important to consider in the search for spe- productive (e.g., Kent, 2004). There are many possible can-
cific genes influencing antisocial behavior. Related to this didate genes for antisocial behavior that have yet to be exam-
goal is the clarification of the relation between antisocial ined, and technological advances will allow more intensive,
behavior and possible endophenotypes. Third, the results of systematic studies involving dense mapping or sequencing
the meta-analysis show that there is significant confounding of specific candidate genes and whole genome association
between assessment method and operationalization and age studies.
(e.g., criminality always being assessed using official records Knowledge regarding the neuro-cognitive dysfunctions
and childhood externalizing behavior always being assessed related to antisocial behavior from neuropsychological and
using parent report), suggesting that the assessment methods functional imaging studies has been increasing. For exam-
used in future behavior genetic studies of antisocial behavior ple, Blair et al. (2006) recently reviewed the evidence for
should be diversified given the possibility that results may two neural systems implicated in psychopathy, one involving
reflect the assessment method rather than the operationaliza- the amygdala and the other involving the orbital/ventrolateral
tion of antisocial behavior being assessed. One such example frontal cortex. As the evidence regarding specific genetic
is Arseneault et al.’s (2003) finding that there were strong effects on antisocial behavior becomes clearer, the applica-
genetic influences on antisocial behavior when ratings agreed tion of imaging genomics (i.e., combining neuroimaging and
across mother, teacher, observer, and self-report. Fourth, the genetic analysis to examine genes’ effects on brain informa-
power to test genotype × environment interactions may be tion processing) in the etiology of antisocial behavior will
limited by range restriction in adoption studies. Therefore, become an important future direction.
future behavior genetic studies should consider alternative The first evidence of a specific gene × measured environ-
research design strategies, such as over-sampling extreme ment interaction was found for antisocial behavior 4 years
observations (McClelland & Judd, 1993). For example, such ago (Caspi et al., 2002), resulting in considerable interest
studies may over-sample children with a low genetic pre- in measured gene × environment interactions. Given the
disposition to antisocial behavior who are reared in envi- large number of possible candidate genes and environmental
ronments that predispose them to antisocial behavior. Fifth, risk factors for antisocial behavior, the possibility of type I
studies examining specific shared and nonshared environ- errors and spurious findings should be considered carefully.
mental influences while controlling for genetic influences are Recently, Moffitt, Caspi, and Rutter (2005) have encouraged
needed. careful measured gene × environment interaction hypothesis
testing and have emphasized the importance of specifying a
priori theoretically plausible triads of a gene, an environmen-
tal pathogen, and a behavioral phenotype. They described
Association and Linkage Studies seven strategic steps needed to organize future hypothesis-
driven studies of measured gene × environment interaction,
In comparison to other comorbid disorders such as ADHD which include (1) consulting the quantitative behavioral-
and alcoholism, there are few association and linkage studies genetic literature, (2) identifying a candidate environmen-
of conduct disorder and antisocial behavior. So far, there is tal pathogen for the disorder in question, (3) optimizing
30 Genetic Analysis of Conduct Disorder and Antisocial Behavior 467

environmental risk measurement, (4) identifying candidate ∗ Brandon, K, & Rose, R. J. (1995). A multivariate twin family study of
susceptibility genes, (5) testing for an interaction, (6) eval- the genetic and environmental structure of personality, beliefs, and
alcohol use. Behavior Genetics, 25, 257.
uating whether a measured gene × environment interaction
Bremner, J. D., & Vermetten, E. (2001). Stress and development: behav-
extends beyond the initially hypothesized triad of gene, envi- ioral and biological consequences. Development and Psychopathol-
ronmental pathogen, and disorder, and (7) replication and ogy, 13, 473–489.
meta-analysis. Brunner, H. G., Nelson, M., Breakefield, X. O., Ropers, H. H., & van
Oost, B. A. (1993). Abnormal behavior associated with a point
mutation in the structural gene for monoamine oxidase. Science,
Acknowledgment This work was supported in part by NIDA DA- 262, 578–580.
13956 and NIMH MH-01818. Brunner, H. G., Nelen, M. R., van Zandvoort, P., Abeling, N. G., van
Gennip, A. H., Wolters, E. C., et al. (1993). X-linked borderline
mental retardation with prominent behavioral disturbance: pheno-
Note: The asterisk denotes a study that was considered for type, genetic localization, and evidence for disturbed monoamine
inclusion in the meta-analysis. metabolism. American Journal of Human Genetics, 52, 1032–1039.
Burt, S. A., Krueger, R. F., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2001).
Sources of covariation among attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor-
der, oppositional defiant disorder, and conduct disorder: The impor-
tance of shared environment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110,
References 516–525.
Button, T. M. M., Hewitt, J. K., Rhee, S. H., Young, S. E., Corley, R.
Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1983). Manual for the Child P., & Stallings, M. C. (2006). Examination of the causes of covari-
Behavior Checklist and Revised Behavior Profile. Burlington, VT: ation between conduct disorder symptoms and vulnerability to drug
University of Vermont. dependence. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 38–45.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical Button, T. M. M., Scourfield, J., Martin, N., Purcell, S., & McGuffin, P.
manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American (2005). Family dysfunction interacts with genes in the causation of
Psychiatric Association. antisocial symptoms. Behavior Genetics, 35, 115–120.
Angold, A, & Costello, E. J. (1993). Depressive comorbidity in children Cadoret, R. J., Cain, C. A., & Crowe, R. R. (1983). Evidence for gene-
and adolescents: Empirical, theoretical and methodological issues. environment interaction in the development of adolescent antisocial
American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1779–1791. behavior. Behavior Genetics, 13, 301–310.
Arseneault, L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Rijsdijk, F. V., Jaf- Cadoret, R. J., Langbehn, D., Caspers, K., Troughton, E. P., Yucuis, R.,
fee, S. R., et al. (2003). Strong genetic effects on cross-situational Sandhu, H. K., et al. (2003). Associations of the serotonin trans-
antisocial behaviour among 5-year-old children according to moth- porter promoter polymorphism with aggressivity, attention deficit,
ers, teachers, examiner-observers, and twins’ self-reports. Journal and conduct disorder in an adoptee population. Comprehensive Psy-
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 832–848. chiatry, 44, 88–101.
∗ Cadoret, R. J., Troughton, E., O’Gorman, T. W., & Heywood, E.
Baca-Garcia, E., Vaquero, C., Diaz-Sastre, C., Garcı́a-Resa, E., Saiz-
Ruiz, J., Fernández-Piqueras, J., et al. (2004). Lack of association (1986). An adoption study of genetic and environmental factors in
between the serotonin transporter promoter gene polymorphism and drug abuse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 1131–1136.
∗ Cadoret, R. J., Yates, W. R., Troughton, E., Woodworth, G., & Stewart,
impulsivity or aggressive behavior among suicide attempters and
healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Research, 126, 99–106. M. A. (1995). Adoption study demonstrating two genetic pathways
∗ Baker, L., Mack, W., Moffitt, T., & Mednick, S. (1989). Sex differences to drug abuse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 42–52.
∗ Cadoret, R. J., Yates, W. R., Troughton, E., Woodworth, G., & Stewrat,
in property crime in a Danish adoption cohort. Behavior Genetics,
19, 355–370. M. A. (1996). An adoption study of drug abuse/dependency in
Beitchman, J. H., Baldassarra, L., Mik, H., De Luca, V., King, N., females. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 37, 88–94.
∗ Carey, G. (1992). Twin imitation for antisocial behavior: Implications
Bender, D., et al. (2006). Serotonin transporter polymorphisms and
persistent, pervasive childhood aggression. American Journal of for genetic and family environment research. Journal of Abnormal
Psychiatry, 163, 1103–1105. Psychology, 101, 18–25.
Biederman, J., Newcorn, J., & Sprich, S. (1991). Comorbidity of atten- Cases, O., Seif, I., Grimsby, J., Gaspar, P., Chen, K., Pournin, S., et al.
tion deficit hyperactivity disorder with conduct, depressive, anxiety, (1995). Aggressive behavior and altered amounts of brain serotonin
and other disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 564–577. and norepinephrine in mice lacking MAOA. Science, 268, 1763–
Blair, R. J. R., Peschardt, K. S., Budhani, S., Mitchell, D. G. V., & Pine, 1766.
D. S. (2006). The development of psychopathy. Journal of Child Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W.,
Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 262–275. et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated
∗ Blanchard, J. M., Vernon, P. A., & Harris, J. A. (1995). A behavior children. Science, 297, 851–854.
∗ Cates, D. S., Houston, B. K., Vavak, C. R., Crawford, M. H., & Uttley,
genetic investigation of multiple dimensions of aggression. Behav-
ior Genetics, 25, 256. M. (1993). Heritability of hostility-related emotions, attitudes, and
Blonigen, D. M., Carlson, S. R., Krueger, R. F., & Patrick, C. J. (2003). behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 16, 237–256.
∗ Centerwall, B. S, & Robinette, C. D. (1989). Twin concordance for
A twin study of self-reported psychopathic personality traits. Per-
sonality and Individual Differences, 35, 179–197. dishonorable discharge from the military: With a review of genetics
Blonigen, D. M., Hicks, B. M., Krueger, R. F., Patrick, C. J., & of antisocial behavior. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 30, 442–446.
Iacono, W. G. (2005). Psychopathic personality traits: Heritabil- Cloninger, C. R, & Gottesman, I. I. (1987). Genetic and environmen-
ity and genetic overlap with internalizing and externalizing psy- tal factors in antisocial behavior disorders. In S. A. Mednick, T. E.
chopathology. Psychological Medicine, 35, 637–648. Moffitt, & S. A. Stack (Eds.), The causes of crime: New biological
∗ Bohman, M. (1978). Some genetic aspects of alcoholism and crimi- approaches (pp. 92–109). New York: Cambridge University Press.
nality: A population of adoptees. Archives of General Psychiatry, Cloninger, C. R., Sigvardsson, S., Bohman, M., & von Knorring, A. -L.
35, 269–276. (1982). Predisposition to petty criminality in Swedish adoptees. II.
468 S.H. Rhee and I.D. Waldman

Cross-fostering analysis of gene-environment interaction. Archives Foley, D. L., Eaves, L. J., Wormley, B., Silberg, J. L., Maes, H. H.,
of General Psychiatry, 39, 1242–1247. Kuhn, J., et al. (2004). Childhood adversity, monoamine oxidase A
Coccaro, E. F., Kavoussi, R. J., Cooper, T. B., & Hauger, R. L. (1997). genotype, and risk for conduct disorder. Archives of General Psychi-
Central serotonin activity and aggression: inverse relationship with atry, 61, 738–744.
prolactin response to D-fenfluramine, but not CSF 5-HIAA concen- Fulker, D. W., Cherny, S. S., Sham, P. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (1999). Com-
tration, in human subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154, bined linkage and association sib-pair analysis for quantitative traits.
1430–1435. American Journal of Human Genetics, 64, 259–267.
Coccaro, E. F., Siever, L. J., Klar, H., Rubenstein, K., Benjamin, Gelernter, J., Kranzler, H. R., Satel, S., & Rao, P. A. (1994).
E., & Davis, K. L. (1987). Diminished prolactin responses to Genetic association between dopamine transporter protein alle-
repeated fenfluramine challenge in man. Psychiatry Research, 22, les and cocaine-induced paranoid. Neuropsychopharmacology, 11,
257–259. 195–200.
∗ Coid, B., Lewis, S. W., & Reveley, A. M. (1993). A twin study of ∗ Ghodsian-Carpey, J., & Baker, L. A. (1987). Genetic and environmen-

psychosis and criminality. British Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 87–92. tal influences on aggression in 4- to 7-year-old twins. Aggressive
Cook, E. H., Stein, M. A., Krasowski, M. D., Cox, N. J., Olkon, D. M., Behavior, 13, 173–186.
Kieffer, J. E., et al. (1995). Association of attention-deficit disor- Giros, B., Jaber, M., Jones, S. R., Wightman, R. M., & Caron,
der and the dopamine transporter gene. American Journal of Human M. G. (1996). Hyperlocomotion and indifference to cocaine and
Genetics, 56, 993–998. amphetamine in mice lacking the dopamine transporter. Nature, 379,
∗ Cunningham, L., Cadoret, R. J., Loftus, R., & Edwards, J. E. (1975). 606–612.
Studies of adoptees from psychiatrically disturbed biological par- Gjone, H, & Stevenson, J. (1997). The association between internaliz-
ents: Psychiatric conditions in childhood and adolescence. British ing and externalizing behavior in childhood and early adolescence:
Journal of Psychiatry, 126, 534–549. Genetic or environmental common influences? Journal of Abnormal
∗ Deater-Deckard, K., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., & Plomin, R. Child Psychology, 25, 277–286.
(1997). Dimensions and disorders of adolescent adjustment: A ∗ Gottesman, I. I. (1963). Heritability of personality: A demonstration.

quantitative genetic analysis of unselected samples and selected Psychological Monographs, 77(9, Whole No. 572).
extremes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 515–525. ∗ Gottesman, I. I. (1965). Personality and natural selection. In S. G. Van-

Dick, D. M., Li, T.-K., Edenberg, H. J., Hesselbrock, V., Kramer, J., denberg (Ed.), Methods and goals in human behavior genetics (pp.
Kuperman, S., et al. (2004). A genome-wide screen for genes influ- 63–80). New York: Academic Press.
encing conduct disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 9, 81–86. Gough, H. G, & Heilbrun, A. B. (1972). The adjective checklist manual.
Dick, D. M., Viken, R. J., Kaprio, J., Pulkkinen, L., & Rose, R. J. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
(2005). Understanding the covariation among childhood external- Grove, W. M., Eckert, E. D., Heston, L., Bouchard, T. J., Segal, N.,
izing symptoms: Genetic and environmental influences on conduct & Lykken, D. T. (1990). Heritability of substance abuse and antiso-
disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and oppositional cial behavior: A study of monozygotic twins reared apart. Biological
defiant disorder symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Psychiatry, 27, 1293–1304.
33, 219–229. ∗ Gustavsson, J. P., Pedersen, N. L., Åsberg, M., & Schalling, D. (1996).
∗ DiLalla, D. L., Carey, G., Gottesman, I. I., & Bouchard, T. J. (1996). Exploration into the sources of individual differences in aggresion-
Heritability of MMPI personality indicators of psychopathology in , hostility-, and anger-related (AHA) personality traits. Personality
twins reared apart. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 491–499. and Individual Differences, 21, 1067–1071.
DiLalla, L. F, & Gottesman, I. I. (1989). Heterogeneity of causes for Haberstick, B. C., Lessem, J. M., Hopfer, C. J., Smolen, A., Ehringer,
delinquency and criminality: Lifespan perspectives. Development M. A., Timberlake, D., et al. (2005). Monoamine oxidase A
and Psychopathology, 1, 339–349. (MAOA) and antisocial behaviors in the presence of childhood and
Dougherty, D., Bonab, A. A., Spencer, T. J., Rauch, S. L., Madras, adolescent maltreatment. American Journal of Medical Genetics,
B. K., & Fischman, A. J. (1999). Dopamine transporter density in 135, 59–64.
patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet, 354, Haberstick, B. C., Smolen, A., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Family-based
2132–2133. association test of the 5HTTLPR and aggressive behavior in a
∗ Eaves, L. J., Silberg, J. L., Meyer, J. M., Maes, H. H., Simonoff, E., general population sample of children. Biological Psychiatry, 59,
Pickles, A., et al. (1997). Genetics and developmental psychopathol- 836–843.
ogy: 2. The main effects of genes and environment on behav- Heils, A., Teufel, A., Petri, S., Stöber, G., Riederer, P., Bengel, D., et
ioral problems in the Virginia Twin Study of Adolescent Behav- al. (1996). Allelic variation of human serotonin transporter gene
ioral Development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, expression. Journal of Neurochemistry, 66, 2621–2624.
965–980. Heinz, A., Jones, D. W., Mazzanti, C., Goldman, D., Ragan, P., Hom-
∗ Edelbrock, C., Rende, R., Plomin, R., & Thompson, L. A. (1995). A mer, D., et al. (2000). A relationship between serotonin transporter
twin study of competence and problem behavior in childhood and genotype and in vivo protein expression and alcohol neurotoxicity.
early adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, Biological Psychiatry, 47, 643–649.
775–785. Hennig, J., Reuter, M., Netter, P., Burk, C., & Landt, O. (2005). Two
∗ Eley, T. C., Lichtenstein, P., & Stevenson, J. (1999). Sex differ- types of aggression are differentially related to serotonergic activity
ences in the etiology of aggressive and nonaggressive antisocial and the A779C TPH polymorphism. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119,
behavior: Results from two twin studies. Child Development, 70, 16–25.
155–168. Huang, Y., Cate, S. P., Battistuzi, C., Oquendo, M. A., Brent, D.,
Faraone, S. V., Perlis, R. H., Doyle, A. E., Smoller, J. W., Goral- & Mann, J. J. (2004). An association between a functional poly-
nick, J. J., Holmgren, M. A., et al. (2005). Molecular genetics of morphism in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter, impulsive
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57, traits and early abuse experiences. Neuropsychopharmacology, 29,
1313–1323. 1498–1505.
∗ Finkel, D, & McGue, M. (1997). Sex differences and nonadditiv- Huizinga, D., Haberstick, B. C., Smolen, A., Menard, S., Young, S.
ity in heritability of the multidimensional personality question- E., Corley, R. P., et al. (2006). Childhood maltreatment, subsequent
naire scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, antisocial behavior, and the role of monoamine oxidase A genotype.
929–938. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 677–683.
30 Genetic Analysis of Conduct Disorder and Antisocial Behavior 469

Hyde, J. S. (1984). How large are gender differences in aggression? A gene may be associated with variability in aggression, impulsiv-
A developmental meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 20, ity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity. Psychiatry
722–736. Research, 95, 9–23.
Jacobson, K. C., Neale, M. C., Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2001). McCartney, K., Harris, M. J., & Bernieri, F. (1990). Growing up and
Behavioral genetic confirmation of a life-course perspective on anti- growing apart: A developmental meta-analysis of twin studies. Psy-
social behavior: Can we believe the results? Behavior Genetics, 31, chological Bulletin, 107, 226–237.
456. McClelland, G. H, & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of
Jaffee, S. R., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Dodge, K. A., Rutter, M., Taylor, detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychological Bulletin,
A., et al. (2005). Nature×nurture: Genetic vulnerabilities interact 114, 376–390.
with physical maltreatment to promote conduct problems. Develop- ∗ McGue, M., Sharma, A., & Benson, P. (1996). The effect of common

ment and Psychopathology, 17, 67–84. rearing on adolescent adjustment: Evidence from a U.S. adoption
Jorm, A. F., Prior, M., Sanson, A., Smart, D., Zhang, Y., & Easteal, S. cohort. Developmental Psychology, 32, 604–613.
(2001). Association of a polymorphism of the dopamine transporter Mednick, S. A., Gabrielli, W. F, & Hutchings, B. (1983). Genetic influ-
gene with externalizing behavior problems and associated temper- ences in criminal behavior: Evidence from an adoption cohort. In K.
ament traits: A longitudinal study from infancy to the mid-teens. T. Van Dusen & S. A. Mednick (Eds.), Prospective studies of crime
American Journal of Medical Genetics, 105, 346–350. and delinquency (pp. 39–56). Boston, MA: Kluwer-Nijhof.
Kendler, K. S., Prescott, C. A., Myers, J., & Neale, M. C. (2003). The ∗ Meininger, J. C., Hayman, L. L., Coates, P. M, & Gallagher, P. (1988).

structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for common psy- Genetics or environment? Type A behavior and cardiovascular risk
chiatric and substance use disorders in men and women. Archives of factors in twin children. Nursing Research, 37, 341–346.
General Psychiatry, 60, 929–937. Miles, D. R., van den Bree, M. B., & Pickens, R. W. (2002). Sex differ-
Kent, L. (2004). Recent advances in the genetics of attention ences in shared genetic and environmental influences between con-
deficit hyperactivity disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 6, duct disorder symptoms and marijuana use in adolescents. American
143–148. Journal of Medical Genetics, 114, 159–168.
Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Williams, B., Newcombe, R., Mill, J., Xu, X., Ronald, A., Curran, S., Price, T., Knight, J., et al.
Craig, I. W., et al. (2006). MAOA, maltreatment, and gene- (2005). Quantitative trait locus analysis of candidate gene alleles
environment interaction predicting children’s mental health: New associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in
evidence and a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 903–913. five genes: DRD4, DAT1, DRD5, SNAP-25, and 5HT1B. American
Kruesi, M. J., Rapoport, J. L., Hamburger, S., Hibbs, E., Potter, W., Z., Journal of Medical Genetics, 133, 68–73.
Lenane, M., et al. (1990). Cerebrospinal fluid monoamine metabo- Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent
lites, aggression, and impulsivity in disruptive behavior disorders antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological
of children and adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, Review, 100, 674–701.
989–994. Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Rutter, M. (2005). Strategy for investigating
Kunugi, H., Ishida, S., Kato, T., Sakai, S., Tatsumi, M., Hirose, T., et interactions between measured genes and measured environments.
al. (1999). No evidence for an association of polymorphisms of the Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 473–481.
tryptophan hydroxylase gene with affective disorders or attempted Nadder, T. S., Rutter, M., Silberg, J. L., Maes, H. H., & Eaves, L. J.
suicide among Japanese patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, (2002). Genetic effects on the variation and covariation of attention
156, 774–776. deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional-defiant dis-
Larsson, H., Andershed, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2006). A genetic fac- order/conduct disorder (Odd/CD) symptomatologies across infor-
tor explains most of the variation in the psychopathic personality. mant and occasion of measurement. Psychological Medicine, 32,
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115, 221–230. 39–53.
Lidberg, L., Tuck, J. R., Asberg, M., Scalia-Tomba, G. P., & Bertilsson, Nadder, T. S., Silberg, J. L., Eaves, L. J., Maes, H. H., & Meyer, J. M.
L. (1985). Homicide, suicide and CSF 5-HIAA. Acta Psychiatrica (1998). Genetic effects on ADHD symptomatology in 7- to 13-year-
Scandinavica, 71, 230–236. old twins: Results from a telephone survey. Behavior Genetics, 28,
∗ Livesley, W. J., Jang, K. L., Jackson, D. N., & Vernon, P. A. (1993). 83–99.
Genetic and environmental contributions to dimensions of personal- ∗ Nathawat, S. S, & Puri, P. (1995). A comparative study of MZ and DZ

ity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 1826–1831. twins on level I and level II mental abilities and personality. Journal
∗ Loehlin, J. C., & Nichols, R. C. (1976). Heredity, environment, and of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology, 21, 87–92.
personality. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press. New, A. S., Gelernter, J., Goodman, M., Mitropoulou, V., Koenigsberg,
∗ Loehlin, J. C., Willerman, L., & Horn, J. M. (1987). Personality resem- H., Silverman, J., et al. (2001). Suicide, impulsive aggression, and
blance in adoptive families: A 10-year follow-up. Journal of Person- HTR1B genotype. Biological Psychiatry, 50, 62–65.
ality and Social Psychology, 53, 961–969. New, A. S., Gelertner, J., Yovell, Y., Trestman, R. L., Nielsen, D. A., Sil-
Lykken, D. T. (1997). Incompetent parenting: its causes and cures. Child vermann, J., et al. (1998). Tryptophan hydroxylase genotype is asso-
Psychiatry and Human Development, 27, 129–137. ciated with impulsive-aggression measures. A preliminary report.
∗ Lyons, M. J., True, W. R., Eisen, S. A., Goldberg, J., Meyer, J. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 81, 13–17.
M., Faraone, S. V., et al. (1995). Differential heritability of adult Nilsson, K. W., Sjöberg, R. L., Damberg, M., Leppert, J., Öhrvik, J.,
and juvenile antisocial traits. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, Alm, P. O., et al. (2005). Role of monoamine oxidase A genotype
906–915. and psychosocial factors in male adolescent criminal activity. Bio-
∗ Lytton, H., Watts, D., & Dunn, B. E. (1988). Stability of genetic deter- logical Psychiatry, 59, 121–127.
mination from age 2 to age 9: A longitudinal twin study. Social ∗ O’Connor, T. G., McGuire, S., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., &

Biology, 35, 62–73. Plomin, R. (1998). Co-occurrence of depressive symptoms and anti-
Manuck, S. B., Flory, J. D., Ferrell, R. E., Dent, K. M., Mann, J. J., social behavior in adolescence: A common genetic liability. Journal
& Muldoon, M. F. (1999). Aggression and anger-related traits asso- of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 27–37.
ciated with a polymorphism of the tryptophan hydroxylase gene. ∗ Owen, D., & Sines, J. O. (1970). Heritability of personality in children.

Biological Psychiatry, 45, 603–614. Behavior Genetics, 1, 235–248.


Manuck, S. B., Flory, J. D., Ferrell, R. E., Mann, J. J., & Muldoon, M. Plomin, R. (1981). Heredity and temperament: A comparison of
F. (2000). A regulatory polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase- twin data for self-report questionnaires, parental ratings, and
470 S.H. Rhee and I.D. Waldman

objectively assessed behavior. In L. Gedda, P. Parisi, & W. E. Nance Silberg, J., Rutter, M., D’Onofrio, B., & Eaves, L. (2003). Genetic and
(Eds.), Twin research 3: Intelligence, personality, and development environmental risk factors in adolescent substance use. Journal of
(pp. 269–278). New York: Alan R. Liss, Inc. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 664–676.
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., McClearn, G. E., & McGuffin, P. (2001). Silberg, J., Rutter, M., Meyer, J., Maes, H., Hewitt, J., Simonoff, E., et
Behavioral genetics (4th ed.). New York: Worth Publishers. al. (1996). Genetic and environmental influences on the covariation
∗ Plomin, R., Foch, T. T., & Rowe, D. C. (1981). Bobo clown aggres- between hyperactivity and conduct disturbance in juvenile twins.
sion in childhood: Environment, not genes. Journal of Research in Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 803–816.
Personality, 15, 331–342. ∗ Slutske, W. S., Heath, A. C., Dinwiddie, S. H., Madden, P. A. F.,

Plomin, R., Nitz, K., & Rowe, D. C. (1990). Behavioral genetics and Bucholz, K. K., Dunne, M. P., et al. (1997). Modeling genetic and
aggressive behavior in childhood. In M. Lewis & S. M. Miller environmental influences in the etiology of conduct disorder: A
(Eds.), Handbook of developmental psychopathology (pp. 119–133). study of 2,682 adult twin pairs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
New York: Plenum Press. 106, 266–279.
∗ Rahe, R. H., Hervig, L., & Rosenman, R. H. (1978). Heritability of Slutske, W. S., Heath, A. C., Dinwiddie, S. H., Madden, P. A., Bucholz,
type A behavior. Psychosomatic Medicine, 40, 478–486. K. K., Dunne, M. P., et al. (1998). Common genetic risk factors for
Reebye, P., Moretti, M. M., & Lessard, J. C. (1995). Conduct disorder conduct disorder and alcohol dependence. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
and substance use disorders: Comorbidity in a clinical sample of chology, 107, 363–374.
preadolescents and adolescents. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 40, Soyka, M., Preuss, U. W., Koller, G., Zill, P., & Bondy, B. (2004). Asso-
313–319. ciation of 5-HT1B receptor gene and antisocial behavior in alco-
Reist, C., Mazzanti, C., Vu, R., Tran, D., & Goldman, D. (2001). Sero- holism. Journal of Neural Transmission, 111, 101–109.
tonin transporter promoter polymorphism is associated with attenu- Stallings, M. C., Corley, R. P., Dennehey, B., Hewitt, J. K., Krauter, K.
ated prolactin response to fenfluramine. American Journal of Medi- S., Lessem, J. M., et al. (2005). A genome-wide search for quantita-
cal Genetics, 105, 363–368. tive trait loci influencing antisocial drug dependence in adolescence.
Retz, W., Retz-Junginger, P., Supprian, T., Thome, J., & Rösler, M. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 1042–1051.
(2004). Association of serotonin transporter promoter gene poly- Staner, L., Uyanik, G., Correa, H., Tremeau, F., Monreal, J., Crocq, M.
morphism with violence: Relation with personality disorders, impul- -A., et al. (2002). A dimensional impulsive-aggressive phenotype is
sivity, and childhood ADHD psychopathology. Behavioral Sciences associated with the A218C polymorphism of the tryptophan hydrox-
and the Law, 22, 415–425. ylase gene: A pilot study in well-characterized impulsive inpatients.
Rhee, S. H, & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and environmental influ- American Journal of Medical Genetics, 114, 553–557.
ences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption ∗ Stevenson, J, & Graham, P. (1988). Behavioral deviance in 13-year-old

studies. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 490–529. twins: An item analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child
Robins, L. N, & Regier, D. A. (1991). Psychiatric disorders in America. and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 791–797.
New York: The Free Press. Strous, R. D., Nolan, K. A., Lapidus, R., Diaz, L., Saito, T., &
∗ Rowe, D. C. (1983). Biometrical genetic models of self-reported delin- Lachman, H. M. (2003). Aggressive behavior in schizophrenia is
quent behavior: A twin study. Behavior Genetics, 13, 473–489. associated with the low enzyme activity COMT polymorphism:
Rowe, D. C., Almeida, D. M., & Jacobson, K. C. (1999). School context A replication study. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 120,
and genetic influences on aggression in adolescence. Psychological 29–34.
Science, 10, 277–280. Taylor, J., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2000). Evidence for a genetic
∗ Rushton, J. P., Fulker, D. W., Neale, M. C., Nias, D. K. B, & Eysenck, etiology of early-onset delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
H. J. (1986). Altruism and aggression: The heritability of individ- ogy, 109, 634–643.
ual differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, Taylor, J., Loney, B. R., Bobadilla, L., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M.
1192–1198. (2003). Genetic and environmental influences on psychopathy trait
Sabol, S. Z., Hu, S., & Hamer, D. (1998). A functional polymorphism dimensions in a community sample of male twins. Journal of Abnor-
in the monoamine oxidase A gene promoter. Human Genetics, 103, mal Child Psychology, 31, 633–645.
273–279. ∗ Taylor, J., McGue, M., Iacono, W. G., & Lykken, D. T. (2000). A

Sakai, J. T., Young, S. E., Stallings, M. C., Timberlake, D., Smolen, A., behavioral genetic analysis of the relationship between the social-
Stetler, G. L., et al. (2006). Case-control and within-family tests for ization scale and self-reported delinquency. Journal of Personality,
an association between conduct disorder and 5HTTLPR. American 68, 29–50.
Journal of Medical Genetics, 141, 825–832. ∗ Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Wilcox, K., Segal, N.,

Sander, T., Harms, H., Podschus, J., Finckh, U., Nickel, B., Rolfs, A., & Rich, S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart
et al. (1997). Alellic association of a dopamine transporter gene and together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
polymorphism in alcohol dependence with withdrawal seizures or 1031–1039.
delirium. Biological Psychiatry, 41, 299–304. Thapar, A., Harrington, R., & McGuffin, P. (2001). Examining the
Saudou, F., Amara, D. A., Dierich, A., LeMeur, M., Ramboz, S., Segu, comorbidity of ADHD-related behaviours and conduct problems
L., et al. (1994). Enhanced aggressive behavior in mice lacking 5- using a twin study design. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179,
HT1B receptor. Science, 265, 1875–1878. 224–229.
∗ Scarr, S. (1966). Genetic factors in activity motivation. Child Develop- Thapar, A., Langley, K., Fowler, T., Rice, F., Turic, D., Whittinger,
ment, 37, 663–673. N., et al. (2005). Catechol o-methyltransferase gene variant and
Schmidt, L. G., Harms, H., Kuhn, S., Rommelspacher, H., & Sander, T. birth weight predict early-onset antisocial behavior in children with
(1998). Modification of alcohol withdrawal by the A9 allele of the attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychi-
dopamine transporter gene. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, atry, 62, 1275–1278.
474–478. ∗ Thapar, A., & McGuffin, P. (1996). A twin study of antisocial
∗ Schmitz, S., Fulker, D. W., & Mrazek, D. A. (1995). Problem behavior and neurotic symptoms in childhood. Psychological Medicine, 26,
in early and middle childhood: An initial behavior genetic analysis. 1111–1118.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 1443–1458. ∗ Torgersen, S. Skre, I., Onstad, S., Edvardsen, J., & Kringlen, E.
∗ Seelig, K. J., & Brandon, K. O. (1997). Rater differences in gene- (1993). The psychometric-genetic structure of DSM-III-R per-
environment contributions to adolescent problem behavior. Behav- sonality disorder criteria. Journal of Personality Disorders, 7,
ior Genetics, 27, 605. 196–213.
30 Genetic Analysis of Conduct Disorder and Antisocial Behavior 471

∗ van den Oord, E. J. C. G., Boomsma, D. I., & Verhulst, F. C. (1994). ∗ Willcutt,E. G., Shyu, V., Green, P., & Pennington, B. F. (1995, April).
A study of problem behaviors in 10- to 15-year-old biologically A twin study of the comorbidity of the disruptive behavior disorders
related and unrelated international adoptees. Behavior Genetics, 24, of childhood. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society
193–205. for Research in Child Development. Indianapolis, IN.
∗ van den Oord, E. J. C. G., Verhulst, F. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (1996). A Wilson, J. Q., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1985). Crime and human nature. New
genetic study of maternal and paternal ratings of problem behaviors York: Simon and Schuster.
in 3-year-old twins. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 349–357. ∗ Wilson, G. D., Rust, J., & Kasriel, J. (1977). Genetic and family ori-

Viding, E., Blair, R. J. R., Moffitt, T. E., & Plomin, R. (2005). Evidence gins of humor preferences: A twin study. Psychological Reports, 41,
for substantial genetic risk for psychopathy in 7-year-olds. Journal 659–660.
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 592–597. Winsberg, B. G, & Comings, D. E. (1999). Association of the dopamine
∗ Waldman, I. D., Levy, F., & Hay, D. A. (1995). Multivariate genetic transporter gene (DAT1) with poor methylphenidate response. Jour-
analyses of the overlap among DSM-III-R disruptive behavior dis- nal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
order symptoms. Behavior Genetics, 25, 293–294. 38, 1474–1477.
∗ Waldman, I. D., Pickens, R. W., & Svikis, D. S. (1989). Sex differ- Young, S. E., Smolen, A., Corley, R. P., Krauter, K. S., DeFries,
ences in genetic and environmental components of childhood con- J. C., Crowley, T. J., et al. (2002). Dopamine transporter
duct problems. Behavior Genetics, 19, 779–780. polymorphism associated with externalizing behavior problems
Waldman, I. D, & Rhee, S. H. (2006). Genetic and environmental influ- in children. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 114,
ences on psychopathy and antisocial behavior. In C. Patrick (Ed.), 144–149.
Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 205–228). New York: The Guilford Young, S. E., Smolen, A., Hewitt, J. K., Haberstick, B. C., Stallings, M.
Press. C., Corley, R. P., et al. (2006). Interaction between MAO-A geno-
Waldman, I. D., Rhee, S. H., & Levy, F. (2001). Causes of overlap type and maltreatment in the risk for conduct disorder: Failure to
among symptoms of ADHD, oppositional defiant disorder, and con- confirm in adolescent patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163,
duct disorder. In F. Levy & D. A. Hay (Eds.), Attention, genes, and 1019–1025.
ADHD (pp. 115–138). New York: Brunner-Routledge. ∗ Young, S. E., Stallings, M. C., Corley, R. P., Hewitt, J.

Waldman, I. D., Rowe, D. C., Abramowitz, A., Kozel, S. T., Mohr, J. K., & Fulker, D. W. (1997). Sibling resemblance for con-
H., Sherman, S. L., et al. (1998). Association and linkage of the duct disorder and attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder in
dopamine transporter gene and attention-deficit hyperactivity dis- selected, adoptive, and control families. Behavior Genetics,
order in children: heterogeneity owing to diagnostic subtype and 27, 612.
severity. American Journal of Human Genetics, 63, 1767–1776. Zalsman, G., Frisch, A., Bromberg, M., Gelernter, J., Michaelovsky,
Widom, C. S, & Brzustowicz, L. M. (2006). MAOA and the “cycle E., Campino, A., et al. (2001). Family-based association study of
of violence”: childhood abuse and neglect, MAOA genotype, and serotonin transporter promoter in suicidal adolescents: no associa-
risk for violent and antisocial behavior. Biological Psychiatry, 60, tion with suicidality but possible role in violence traits. American
684–689. Journal of Medical Genetics, 105, 239–245.

You might also like