You are on page 1of 31

Learning to Love:

Milestones and Mechanisms


Everett Waters
Kiyomi Kondo-Ikemura
German Posada
State University of New York at Stony Brook
John E. Richters
Laboratory for Developmental Psychology
National Institute of Mental Health

The central questions in any developmental analysis theory.


are "What develops?", "What is the course of devel-
Paradigms and Perspectives
opment?", and "What are the mechanisms of
change?" Highlighting seemingly purposeful and Differences among paradigms in personality re-
context sensitive secure base behavior that organizes search are nowhere more evident than in competing
attachment relationships, Bowlby (1958) defin ed at- perspectives on the development of attachment rela-
tachment as an emotional bond that ties the child to tionships. Freud (e.g. 1949) viewed the infant's tie
one or a few figures across time and space. He de- to its mother in terms of drive reduction and emerg-
scribed its development in terms of emerging prefer- ing mental structures that channel and transform in-
ence for one or a few figures, the onset of secure stinctual drives. Social learning theorists (e.g. Mac-
base behavior, and a change in the representation of coby & Masters, 1972) and behaviorists (e.g. Ge-
attachment figures during the childhood transition wirtz, 1972) saw only discrete behaviors, displayed
from sensorimotor to representational thought. differentially toward the mother, and maintained by
her attention and responsiveness. To date, neither
Here we re-examine Bowlby's developmental
of these views has produced powerful assessment
outline with an eye toward providing finer detail, in-
tools or sustained productive research programs.
corporating traditional mechanisms of learning into
attachment theory, and placing greater emphasis on John Bowlby's (1958, 1960, 1969) ethological/
the parent's contribution to the organization of at- control systems theory of attachment was founded
tachment behavior throughout childhood. We also upon important new insights into what develops.
emphasize the role of self awareness, self- Instead of working to reconcile traditional perspec-
observation, and self-consistency in attachment de- tives, Bowlby offered a new paradigm that compre-
velopment and in ties between attachment and so- hended both affective and behavioral facets of at-
cialization outcomes. Although most questions re- tachment. His approach also made sense of previ-
garding attachment and the self are unresolved (e.g. ously inexplicable fears in infancy and of attach-
Connell & Wellborn, this volume; Sroufe, in press), ment behavior's sensitivity to infant state and the
the processes of self-observation and "informal infer- state of the environment. The assessment para-
ence" implicated here in developing attachment rela- digms and research programs that have prospered
tionships are also central to cognitive theories of the under the rubric of Bowlby's "ethological theory"
self (e.g. Epstein, 1973, 1980, this volume). This attest to the validity of his insights into what devel-
common thread provides important hints about the ops.
role of attachment in the development of one's self
In Bowlby's view, attachment is a tie that binds

Reprinted from: M. Gunner & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.) The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol 23,
Self processes in development). 1991, Vol. 23, pp. 217-255. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
LEARNING TO LOVE

individuals together over time and space. A person pirical data, and trying to translate Bowlby's theory
comes to use another as a secure base from which into valid and economical measurement proce-
to explore and as a haven of safety. This "secure dures. Are patterns of secure base behavior stable?
base phenomenon" is regulated by a behavior con- Are they related to socialization or only to behav-
trol system that emerges during the first year of life ior in relationships? Does temperament offer an
and influences the organization of affect, cogni- alternative to the control system interpretation of
tion, and behavior in attachment relationships secure base behavior? This is the essence of con-
across the lifespan. According to Bowlby, the at- struct validation. Although definition and descrip-
tachment control system is analogous to control tion must precede explanation, they do not replace
systems that regulate complex adaptive behavior it. Eventually, we have to explain the correlations
patterns in other species. Both biological biases in that helped us define the parameters of our con-
human learning abilities and social experience struct.
guide its development. Bowlby's emphasis on the
secure base phenomenon has led to powerful at- From Drives to Control Systems
tachment measures and sustained bountiful re- Freud's Contribution
search programs. As we shall see, it also suggests
We need only review Freud's last work, Abriss der
a great deal about the role of close social relation-
psychoanalyse (1949; An outline of psychoanaly-
ships in the origin and development of the self.
sis), to realize that developmental psychologists
Bowlby summarized the developmental course are still working from the agenda he set. In this
of attachment behavior and the underlying control brief work he touches upon personality, the self,
system in terms of four stages: (1) undiscriminat- gender roles, social competence, emotion, proso-
ing social responsiveness, (2) focused responsive- cial and antisocial behavior, social cognition, and
ness to one or a few figures, (3) the emergence of moral judgment, and upon processes involving re-
secure base behavior, and (4) the transformation of inforcement, punishment, imitation, memory and
secure base behavior into a goal-corrected partner- information processing skills, family interaction,
ship with the primary caregiver. This description and parenting.
has organized attachment research for over 20
Freud's descriptive insights about human at-
years. Early research concentrated on infant-
tachment include the following:
mother interaction and the development of focus-
sed responsiveness. In the mid-1970's, theory and 1. An individual's attachment to another cannot be
research concentrated on individual differences in equated with the amount of overt behavior to-
secure base behavior. More recently, attention has ward that person or with the amount or duration
turned to attachment beyond the secure base period of protest that follows separation.
(e.g. Ricks, 1986; Main & Kaplan, 1989; Parkes &
Stevenson-Hinde, 1985). 2. Loss of a loved one is always painful and is a
major challenge to an individual's adaptive re-
After two decades of theoretical and descrip- sources.
tive work, we know a great deal about what devel-
ops and about the developmental course of early 3. Attachment is never given up voluntarily or
attachment relationships. The long deferred ques- completely.
tion remains "What are the mechanisms of 4. Grief and mourning are processes rather than be-
change?" Indeed, the most the pressing issue in havior and they serve an adaptive function for
contemporary attachment theory is to describe the individual who experiences a significant
complete causal pathways to explain well- loss.
replicated correlations between early care and sub-
sequent patterns of secure base behavior, and be- 5. The process of grieving is not concluded when
tween secure base behavior in infancy and subse- the crying stops.
quent behavior with parents and siblings, social 6. Human infants lead an exceedingly complex
competence, self esteem, and behavior problems. cognitive and emotional life.
Correlational data played a critical role in the 7. Early attachment relationships are prototypes of
initial phases of attachment research when we were later love relationships.
asking how best to define the construct, checking
the broad outlines of attachment theory against em- It is important to distinguish Freud's genuine in-
sights about human attachments from the psycho-

2
WATERS ET AL.

dynamic metaphors and models used to express fants' reactions to separation and loss are more
them and knit them together. Although psycho- than mere cries. They reflect the same grief and
dynamic formulations have almost no explanatory mourning process experienced by adults. The simi-
role in contemporary theory and research, Freud's larity of infant separation responses to phases of
descriptive insights remain at the heart of contem- adult grief and mourning was subsequently docu-
porary attachment theory. Having set so much of mented in several striking films by Bowlby's col-
our current agenda, Freud, like Piaget, will be long leagues, Joyce and James Robertson.
remembered for his descriptive insights, even if
cognitive and brain sciences eventually replace the An ethological control-systems analysis of at-
motivational theory that tied them together. Genu- tachment motivation. Bowlby's goal in developing
ine descriptive insights remain, even after explana- an ethological/control systems view of attachment
tory devices and methods tied to particular mo- was to replace Freud's drive reduction model of
ments in the history of science are replaced by motivation with one that was better grounded in
more adequate ones. As in the work of so many contemporary biological theory and research.
grand theorists, there is genius merely in defining Many telling criticisms leveled at psychoanalytic
what the question should be and in recognizing theory focussed on Freud's motivational models.
what the answers might be like. Bowlby recognized that only an alternative moti-
vational model could preserve Freud's genuine in-
Bowlby's Contributions sights about emotional bonds in infancy and adult-
If we were to organize John Bowlby's many contri- hood. Control-systems theory allowed Bowlby to
butions to psychiatry, psychoanalysis, and the be- emphasize the seemingly motivated and purposeful
havioral sciences into just a few categories, we organization of infant attachment behavior without
might include (1) his role in preserving Freud's in- attributing to the infant sophisticated cognitive
sights about attachment, (2) his own contributions abilities or intentions.
to attachment theory, (3) the role he has played in In brief, Bowlby proposed that human infants'
translating attachment theory into practice, and (4) behavior toward their primary caregivers is under
the role he, working in tandem with Mary Ains- the control of an attachment behavioral control
worth, played in the training and development of system. He described this control system as a neu-
scholars who have advanced attachment theory and rally based feedback system that integrated several
research during the last 20 years. For our present functions:
purposes, Bowbly's role in preserving Freud's in-
sights about attachment and his own contributions 1. defining a set goal that the system uses as a cri-
to attachment theory are of primary importance. terion for activation of adaptive behaviors. In
the case of attachment, Bowlby defined the
Beginning in the early 1960's, the mainstream goal as a degree of proximity or access to the
in developmental psychology shifted from grand caregiver. This set goal can be modified in the
theory toward methodological rigor and empiri- short term in response to contextual factors and,
cism. Bowlby recognized that a genuine paradigm in the long term, in response to experience with
clash was in progress and that psychoanalytic in- a particular caregiver.
sights might be discarded wholesale. In a series of
early papers (Bowlby 1958, 1960, 1962) he identi- 2. collating information about the infant's previous
fied key attachment-related insights in psychoana- experience with the caregiver, the infant's state,
lytic theory, noted that they were not inextricably the caregiver's location and activities, interest-
tied to Freud's mental energy and drive reduction ing objects and events in the environment, spe-
models, and preserved them by providing an alter- cial cues to danger (e.g. looming objects, dark-
native motivational model based on ethological ness, novelty.)
and control systems theories of the day. Were it 3. comparing information about the current state of
not for Bowlby's timely intercession, the past 20 the infant, caregiver, and environment with the
years might have been spent rediscovering rather criterion defined by the set goal.
than building upon Freud's insights. In addition to
preserving important psychoanalytic insights about 4. activating behavior patterns that correct devia-
attachment, Bowlby contributed insights of his tions from the set goal and maintain the infant
own. First, he emphasized that the infant mother within the bounds defined by the set goal.
relationship is a genuine attachment not merely an Critical behaviors here include crying, ap-
infantile precursor. He also emphasized that in- proach, following, clinging, and exploration.

3
LEARNING TO LOVE

Descriptively, the control system is said to main- viduals, as well as illness, hunger, or exhaustion.
tain a balance between attachment behavior In this respect, many aspects of contemporary
(proximity seeking) and exploratory behavior. As infant behavior can only be understood in terms
with any control system, the key parameter of indi- of the environment to which they are adapted.
vidual differences is not the quantity of any par-
ticular behavioral output but the efficiency and 7. Because the attachment control system requires
success with which the behavioral system main- experience in order to become operational, dif-
tains the infant within the parameters defined by ferences in early experience can lead to different
the set goal. operating characteristics in different individuals.
These tend to be stable over time and are impor-
Bowlby cited a wide range of examples from tant contributors to individual differences in ad-
ethology to establish that control system models justment and personality. They may change in
were respectable and powerful concepts in behav- response to experience in significant relation-
ioral biology. He also discussed at length evidence ships or experience in psychotherapy.
that natural selection could account for the pres-
ence of such control systems in animal nervous Bowlby's Developmental Model
systems. Bowlby (1969, ch. 14) described four phases in the
The following postulates outline the logic of development of infant-mother attachment and
Bowlby's analysis: mentioned mechanisms that might underlie devel-
opments within and across phases. The model is
1. Humans and their ancestors were under consid- summarized in Figure 1.
erable pressure from predators in the environ-
ment in which key characteristics of our species Phase 1. The first phase in Bowlby's model is
evolved. a brief period of undiscriminating responsiveness.
His analysis is much like Piaget's in that he de-
2. Maintaining a degree of proximity or access to scribes innate behavior patterns as the foundations
adults reduces the likelihood of depredation. upon which later organized behavior develops and
3. Species specific behavior patterns and learning proposes that interaction with the environment is
abilities have genetic substrates whose represen- critical. The notion that attachment arises from in-
tation in a population is influenced by the effects teraction rather than emerging fully formed is im-
of the behavior pattern or learning ability on re- portant both in demystifying the processes in-
productive success. volved and in accounting for the adaptedness of at-
tachment behavior.
4. As a result of the selective advantages conferred
by certain behavior patterns and learning abili- The mechanisms Bowlby mentions include re-
ties, an attachment control system is part of our flex patterns of grasping, crying, sucking, and
primate evolutionary endowment. other neonatal adaptations that clearly serve a vari-
ety of non-attachment related functions. The com-
5. The organization of the attachment behavioral mon element they share is in increasing the time
system is encoded in the underlying plan of the the mother spends with the infant. Woodson,
human nervous system and becomes readily Shepherd, and Chamberline (1981; see also,
available when perceptual and motor systems Woodson, 1983) demonstrated a remarkable rela-
mature, if the infant experiences patterns of care tionship between infant crying, maternal holding,
and living circumstances that are not entirely infant body temperature, and bilirubin metabolism
foreign to our species. Bowlby uses the term that clearly illustrates the multiple functions of
"average expectable environment." these behaviors as well as the interplay between
6. The attachment behavioral system is sensitive to behavior and physiology that must have played a
a variety of prepotent environmental and infant role in their evolution. Mechanisms that might be
state variables that would have been associated more narrowly adapted to serve the development
with increased risk of depredation in the envi- of attachment have received less attention.
ronment in which key human characteristics In brief, Bowlby views attachment as arising
evolved ("the environment of evolutionary from interaction, but not from interaction alone.
adaptedness"). These include darkness, separa- From the beginning, attachment involves the inter-
tion from adults, and unfamiliar settings or indi- play of experience and species specific biases in
learning abilities. Surprisingly, very little has

4
WATERS ET AL.

The Development of Attachment tributing goals or other cognitive guidance that


Bowlby's Four Phase Model might simply have replaced one bit of magic with
another. Or was it simply one bit of magic replac-
Phase Age (months) ing another?
1. Non-focussed orienting (0 - 3) This is the only point at which evolutionary
and signaling theory plays a critical role in Bowlby's theory. The
argument is this: Attachment arises from interac-
2. Focus on one or more figure(s) (3 - 6) tion between an infant with certain biases in its
learning abilities and an average expectable envi-
3. Secure base behavior (6 - 24) ronment (i.e. responsive mother). The biases in in-
fant learning abilities, taken with the expectable
4. Goal-corrected partnership (24 - 30+) environment, essentially guarantee that the attach-
ment behavioral system will be put together ac-
Figure 1. The development of attachment: cording to the species specific pattern.
Bowlby's four phase model. Bowlby proposed that these biases evolved by
natural selection. This was a perfectly reasonable
been said about what these biases might be. hypothesis, if it could be supported by evidence
that specific biases in learning abilities can indeed
Phase 2. The second phase in Bowlby's model evolve. Research on imprinting in precocial birds
describes a period of differential responsiveness offered clear and well-studied examples of species
and focusing on one or a few figures. Bowlby specific biases in socially significant learning abili-
mentions an in-built bias to orient toward certain ties that can plausibly be attributed to evolution.
classes of stimuli, exposure learning, and an in- The point here is not that imprinting is a mecha-
built bias to approach that which is familiar as po- nism involved in human infant attachment. Im-
tentially relevant mechanisms underlying this proc- printing is cited merely as an example of socially
ess. Interaction with a parent providing species significant learning that had been shaped by evolu-
typical patterns of care is also critical. tion. Any example of socially significant biases in
On the face of it, it would seem difficult to learning abilities would have done as well. In ret-
disentangle biases in infant learning abilities, the rospect, it is unfortunate that, in addition to illus-
structure of parental behavior, and infant cognitive trating biases in a species learning abilities, loco-
processes play in the tendency to focus on one or a motor imprinting also resembles following in hu-
few figures. Bowlby does stipulate, for example, man infant attachment. This has led to mis-
that the emergence of attachment depends on the interpretations of Bowlby's argument and to misin-
infant's encountering patterns of care that are co- terpretations of the role ethology plays in his work.
adapted to the attachment behavioral system. He The reference to evolution at this point in
refers to this as the "average expectable environ- Bowlby's theory serves a very specific role -- that
ment," the caretaking environment that is taken for is, to tie the biases in learning abilities that under-
granted in the design of the attachment behavioral lie the development of a behavioral control system
system. Underlying processes and limits on an in- to a specific mechanism. Citing a well understood
fant's ability to establish concurrent focal relation- mechanism that is neither drive related nor tied to
ships with more than one figure have not been ex- prevailing contingencies of reinforcement sets
amined in detail. Bowlby's theory apart from those that preceded it.
Phase 3. The centerpiece in Bowlby's model Beyond this, there is nothing inherently evolution-
is, of course, his description of the infant's ten- ary, ethological, or biological about Bowlby's view
dency to maintain proximity to a focal figure of attachment. While the designation "ethological
through locomotion and signaling, which Ains- attachment theory" commemorates the influence of
worth subsequently designated the secure base ethological theory and research on Bowlby's early
phenomenon. This brilliant descriptive insight thinking, it also leads to misapprehensions.
concerning the nature of the child's tie to its parent Note also that the theory does not depend on
allowed Bowlby to highlight the apparently pur- identifying predation or any other specific factor as
poseful organization of infant attachment behavior the selective pressure that led to the biases in our
without invoking drive concepts and without at-

5
LEARNING TO LOVE

learning abilities. The key is that attachment be- part for different outcomes among attached infants.
havior is expected to enhance an individual's re- That is, the maternal behaviors that best predict
productive success and the success of its offspring. differences in attachment outcomes were viewed
Predation is but one factor influencing this. At- as more likely than others to play critical causal
tachment behavior may also have helped maintain roles in the normative development of attachment.
supervision and thus reduce the likelihood of acci- In effect, this correlational strategy assumes that
dents and injuries unrelated to predation. More- information about differences among individuals
over, its contributions to repro-ductive success can support (causal/developmental) inferences
may have changed during the course of primate about changes within individuals over time. This
evolution, ultimately contributing more as a pre- assumption also underlies many popular designs in
cursor to the capacity for adult bonds and parental causal analysis and structural equation modeling.
care than as an anti-predator strategy in infancy. In retrospect, we might criticize this strategy
This has been a source of naive and fruitless specu- on several counts. Evidence that a particular ma-
lation that is more likely to discredit attachment ternal behavior is correlated with attachment out-
theory than to add depth or clarity. There is every comes is necessary but not sufficient evidence that
reason to be interested in the evolution of attach- it plays a causal role as attachment develops. We
ment behavior, but the issue is not central to must also show that the maternal behavior pre-
Bowlby's analysis of relationships between early cedes the attachment behavior and specify a plausi-
attachment and either later personality or later rela- ble causal mechanism. Research designs that
tionships. measure maternal behavior early and outcome
As the theory stands today, it seems more ap- variables later, assessing neither the outcome vari-
propriate to refer to it as the control systems, or able in the first phase nor the influence of maternal
even the affective/cognitive control systems per- behavior at the final phase of the study, are not de-
spective. If this were more widely appreciated, cisive on this point. From an evolutionary per-
there might be fewer misapprehensions regarding spective, we should note that traits critical to sur-
the "biological" orientation of attachment theory; vival tend to be relatively uniform within a species
moreover, attachment theorists might feel more and not particularly amenable to analysis in terms
comfortable demurring when asked about the of individual differences. Insofar as attachment
"adaptive significance" of specific attachment pat- behavior evolved to reduce depredation, we could
terns. argue that its development would be highly canal-
ized and primarily dependant upon maternal be-
Bowlby's analysis of the attachment control haviors in which there is little diversity. In this
system clearly offers much more than one bit of light, the correlates of diversity would remain in-
magic to replace another. Indeed, if Bowlby fi- teresting but might not prove critical to the onset of
nessed anything at all, it is not the role of biases in attachment behavior.
learning abilities, but rather the relationship be-
tween maternal care and the emergence of secure More importantly, Ainsworth's longitudinal/
base behavior. Both Bowlby and Ainsworth ini- correlational strategy reflects what might be called
tially worked toward a normative theory of attach- "the developmental bias", that is the tendency to
ment--that is, the emphasis was on the typical in- look for the causes of behavior in the relatively re-
fant rather than on individual differences among mote past rather than in contemporaneous influ-
infants. The individual diff- erences orientation, ences. Ainsworth's descriptions of maternal sensi-
which would have been a long term goal in any tivity, cooperation vs interference are descriptive
event, entered the work not as a substantive inter- insights of the first order. Sensitive and responsive
est but, rather, as a methodological strategy. Ains- care are clearly the most consistent and significant
worth was inter- ested in identifying patterns of interactive behavior correlates of later attachment
maternal behavior that explained the emergence of outcomes. That the correlations between maternal
secure base behavior. The obvious strategy would sensitivity and secure base behavior are rarely
have been to identify maternal behaviors that dis- greater than .40 is easily accounted for in terms of
tinguish between infants who do and do not be- methodological and measurement issues. What is
come attached. The practical problem is that es- missing is a detailed explanation of how these par-
sentially all home-reared infants become attached. ticular patterns of maternal behavior would lead to
the emergence of secure base behavior. Even a de-
Because attachment arises from interaction, scriptive analysis, something similar to Piaget's de-
different histories of interaction should account in scriptions of stage transitions in sensorimotor de-

6
WATERS ET AL.

velopment, would be extremely useful. As it is, from sensorimotor to representational thought, de-
there seems to be a bit of magic here. Once recog- tailed so eloquently in Piaget's work, has a parallel
nized, however, the need to identify plausible in the development of attachment and establishes
causal mechanisms relating maternal behavior to early attachment as similar to, and even a proto-
secure base behavior is quite manageable. type of, later love relationships.
Phase 4. Bowlby describes the fourth phase in Clearly, the four-phase model has served at-
attachment, which begins sometime after the sec- tachment theory very well. Important insights have
ond year, as the phase of "goal corrected partner- been preserved. The view that attachment arises
ship." Very little is said about this phase, except from interaction is well understood and empirical
that the infant is increasingly able and willing to evidence has favored the secure base phenomenon
take the mother's immediate goals and activities as the better of several competing descriptions of
into account when the attachment behavioral sys- attachment behavior. It should be noted, however,
tem is active. In a word, attachment behavior be- that we have not yet demanded a great deal of the
comes somewhat less peremptory under ordinary theory. As we move from the descriptive phase of
circumstances. Bowlby's description of this phase research into a more formal mode of hypothesis
is clearly influenced by Piaget's description of testing, weaknesses in the four-phase model be-
changes at the end of the sensorimotor period of come increasingly apparent.
cognitive development. The only descriptive data
we have on this stage are Marvin's (1977) disserta- First of all, Bowlby's developmental descrip-
tion, which shows relationships between attach- tion abstracts attachment from the context of re-
ment security, cooperation/self-control tasks when lated behavioral and cognitive developments. This
the mother is too busy to respond, and tolerance of was useful when the attachment construct was less
separation. familiar; today we gain more by placing the secure
base phenomenon in a broader context. Second,
In principle, this should be a critical part of although control systems theorists have mentioned
Bowlby's developmental model: As the last phase a number of mechanisms relevant to the develop-
in the sequence, it is the one that must interface ment of attachment, they have not sys-tematically
with "mature" attachment patterns. Developing followed the influence of specific mechanisms
this interface is one of the critical tasks of attach- across the full course of attachment development.
ment theory. Until it is accomplished, the Bowlby/ Moreover, they have overlooked some mecha-
Ainsworth perspective will remain vulnerable to nisms, placed too much emphasis on very early in-
the criticism that it is a theory of infant attachment, fluences, and placed relatively little emphasis on
a theory of adult attachment, and a great deal in be- concurrent influences and traditional learning
tween left to the imagination. mechanisms. Perhaps most importantly, at-
Advantages and Limitations tachment theorists in the Bowlby/Ainsworth tradi-
tion have placed little emphasis on the secure base
In the early years, Bowlby's outline of develop- figure's role in organizing and providing coherence
ment from interaction to goal-corrected partnership and consistency to early secure base behavior. The
played an important definitional role in attachment observational/ethological underpinnings of the out-
theory. It clearly expressed his view that attach- line have not been updated since Ainsworth's early
ment can have biological underpinnings, without home studies in Baltimore. It is our impression
springing fully-formed into the infant's behavioral that the current description underestimates how
repertoire or operating without environmental in- long it takes for secure base behavior to become
put. His view remains that attachment arises from consolidated and efficient and suggests that its on-
interaction-- biology conditions but does not deter- set is more discrete than it really is.
mine the outcome.
While highlighting that onset of secure base is
Bowlby's four phase model also serves as a not the final stage in attachment development, the
framework within which to present his key de- goal-corrected partnership concept doesn't capture
scriptive insight - the control system analysis of in- later development of attachment very well.
fant attachment behavior. It also formalizes im- Among other things, it seems easier to describe
portant insights about changes in attachment at the how the child's interests and goals diverge from the
end of infancy. First, attachment does not decline parents' than to identify common goals that are at-
along with separation protest. Second, the trend tachment-related and could serve as the basis for

7
LEARNING TO LOVE

such a partnership. Thus, Triver's (1972, 1974, strata.


1985) analyses of competing parent and child in- This description is more detailed than
terests and the biology of parent-offspring conflict Bowlby's. It casts a broader descriptive net and
seem to provide a more powerful evol-utionary emphasizes that attachment related development
perspective on developmental changes after in- continues throughout childhood and beyond. It
fancy. Problems inherent in the goal-corrected also suggests that secure base behavior (and thus
partnership concept may, in part, account for the any underlying control system) takes much longer
fact that it receives less attention in the second and to become organized and is much more dependent
third volumes of Bowlby's attachment series and on supportive parental behavior than Bowlby sug-
has not been the starting point for recent advances gested. Although these departures from Bowl-by's
in attachment theory and assessment. Main & original outline complicate the picture somewhat,
Kaplan (1989), for example, develop their theory they pave the way toward incorporating traditional
of adult working models from the secure base con- learning mechanisms into attachment theory. This
cept rather than the goal-corrected partnership. is a critical integration, one that psychoanalytic
Fortunately, the genuine insights at the core of theory never achieved. Our pre-sentation also
attachment theory can be preserved in the context highlights the role of cognitive/reflective processes
of alternative developmental descriptions. The un- in developmental change. Particular note is paid to
dertaking here is clearly evolutionary rather than processes of self-observation and to a process we
revolutionary, but it is directly relevant to the term "informal inference." As a step toward un-
theme of this year's Minnesota Symposium. As we derstanding rela-tionships between attachment and
will see, an alternative view of developmental the self, we also consider the extent to which early
changes in attachment brings with it the prospect attachment relationships provide important infor-
of closer coordination with self-related constructs. mation during the formative stages of what Epstein
(1973) has called one's theory of one's self.
It is in the nature of developmental research
that the question "What develops?" recurs at differ- A Revised Developmental Analysis
ent levels of analysis. At each turn, the course of
Phase 1: From interaction to familiarity and pref-
development is described in more detail and ques-
erence. The initial phase in our revised develop-
tions about mechanisms of change come into
mental analysis corresponds to the first phase in
sharper focus. Thus, research inspired by
Bowlby's analysis. In the course of interaction and
Bowlby's four-stage model has brought us to the
routine care, the infant acquires at least sensorimo-
threshold of new, finer-grained descriptions, better
tor familiarity with one or a few primary caregiv-
understanding of ties between attachment and de-
ers. The first sensorimotor anti-cipations can be
velopmental change in other domains, and a more
described as "islands of pre-dictability" organized
comprehensive view of mechanisms underlying
around interaction and caregiving routines. In the
developmental changes in attachment behavior.
first weeks, these "islands of predictability" are too
We propose recasting Bowlby's four stage- closely tied to behavior to be described as
model in terms of the following developmental "expectations," But they are the foundations upon
phases: (1) early dyadic interaction, (2) emergence which the infant builds expectations and expands
of the self-other distinction, (3) onset of secure its temporal horizons. From predictability grows
base behavior in infancy, (4) consolidation of se- preference.
cure base behavior in early childhood, (5) emer-
One of Bowlby's most important insights was
gence of a positive orientation toward parental so-
placing the origins of attachment this early in in-
cialization goals and internalization of family val-
fancy, long before secure base behavior emerges.
ues in early childhood, and (6) a period in which a
Correlations between early care and later secure
partnership of sorts develops around the task of
base behavior (e.g. Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, &
maintaining communication and supervision once
Wall, 1978) provide evidence for this insight.
the child begins to be independent. The last of
Nonetheless, they do not allow us to choose be-
these phases recon-ceptualizes Bowlby's notion of
tween alternative causal models. Attachment theo-
the goal-corrected partnership, which in this analy-
rists have often interpreted these correlations as
sis is placed in middle childhood and tied to so-
evidence that early care has a direct causal influ-
cialization practices in specific cultures and social
ence on later behavior. Such influences are not

8
WATERS ET AL.

unimaginable: Direct effects of early experience on learn musical patterns so easily and have strong
later behavior are well documented in ethological and memorable affective responses to them. Con-
literature. In many instances, the critical environ- sider too how readily we learn to enjoy throwing
mental input is available only for a brief time and things in the air. These and a host of other biases
the effects may not be evident until maturation. in our learning abilities are so distinctive that in the
Although compelling, in several respects etho- aggregate they distinguish our species from any
logical data do not parallel early maternal care and other as well as any suite of physical of physiologi-
secure base behavior in human infants. First, al- cal traits.
though the examples Bowlby cited are often com- With strong assertions about our learning
plex, the behaviors are typically more stereotyped abilities at the core of Bowlby's attachment theory,
than secure base behavior in human infants. In ad- it is somewhat of a mystery that the task of uncov-
dition, human infants interact with primary care- ering and cataloguing attachment-related biases in
givers almost continuously throughout infancy and human infant learning abilities has received so lit-
early childhood. Consequently, the correlational tle attention. Here, for Bowlby's admirers and crit-
data are equally consistent with the hypothesis that ics alike, is the prospect of a strong and dangerous
early interaction predicts later interaction, and only test that goes to the foundations of the theory.
the later having direct effects on secure base be- What could be plainer than predicting that human
havior. To decide this matter, we must first spec- infants are endowed with an array of learning bi-
ify in detail what develops and then test hypothe- ases that map so completely and so redundantly
ses about proposed mechanisms of change. Figure into the predictable caregiving environment as to
2 presents the first three phases of our revised de- guarantee the emergence of preference and eventu-
velopmental outline. Mechanisms are presented on ally attachment in virtually every case ?
the left and products on the right.
Phase 2: Mother as the intersection of sensori-
Mechanisms relevant to the development of motor schemes. The second phase in our proposed
familiarity, predictability, and preference in early analysis begins with the coordination of sensori-
infancy might include traditional mechanisms of motor schemes. In The Origins of Intelligence,
learning, the type of contiguity learning often men- Piaget described how an infant comes to recognize
tioned by Robert Cairns (e.g. 1972), less well particular objects as occasions for practicing par-
known mechanisms that underlie species identifi- ticular action patterns. As these action patterns be-
cation and preferences in mammals and birds (e.g. come increasingly inter-coordinated, objects be-
Roy, 1980), and perhaps species-specific biases in come more discriminable and eventually become
the infant's learning abilities. The last of these is identified as objects distinct from the actions the
central to Bowlby's theory as currently formulated. infant can perform on them.
Among these biases, affective response to contin-
gency seems to be a biologically prepared response An attentive adult is, of course, the opportu-
in human infants. However, it is not necessarily nity par excellence for sensorimotor expression,
adapted specifically to the development of attach- and none is encountered more often, at closer
ment relationships. Other biases in our learning range, and in more modes than the primary care-
abilities, such as the fact that affective contrast giver. According to Piaget, objects are recognized
does not interfere with bonding at this age, may first as suckables, lookables, and graspables, and
have evolved specifically to accommodate attach- then as suckable-lookables, lookable-graspables,
ment formation. Unfortunately, the range of learn- etc. How much more vivid then must be the in-
ing biases in play during attachment formation has fant's view of a caregiver who is at once nutritively
never been catalogued or even examined in detail. and non-nutritively suckable, graspable, lookable,
listenable, and all of this in every combination?
Far from being the general purpose learning Suppose the caregiver is good at identifying infant
machines envisioned in traditional learning theo- signals, selecting responses, and delivering them in
ries, humans are peculiar learners indeed. Con- a timely manner (i.e., she is, in Ainsworth's terms,
sider how easily we learn about sounds at the be- "sensitive"). Suppose further that she coordinates
ginnings and ends of words, that we remember her behavior with the infant's ongoing behavior in
both the beginning and the end of a word list better a way that supports sensorimotor coordinations
than the middle, that we learn better if practice is and establishes bridging ties from one moments ac-
spaced rather than concentrated in time, that we tivities to the next (i.e., again in Ainsworth's terms,

9
LEARNING TO LOVE

The Development of Attachment:


Mechanisms and Milestones

Interaction Familiarity
Predictability
Preference

Interaction among Self - Other distinction


sensori-motor Mother as permanent object
schemes

Reflection of "mother " Self as object ("I am …")


schema

Figure 2. Precursors of attachment: Mechanisms and milestones in early infancy.

she is "cooperative" rather than interfering). In all her own behavior in coordination with or anticipa-
likelihood, such a partner will be more uniquely tion of the infant's ongoing behavior. The care-
defined than inanimate objects and sooner recog- giver provides much of the organization that is
nized as existing independent of the infant's behav- eventually consolidated in the infant's sensorimo-
ior (e.g. Bell, 1970; Kaye, 1982; Winnicott, 1965). tor schemata and in the first conceptual catagories.
Here, in the context of early interaction, previously This is the sense in which Winnicott (1965) ob-
acquired familiarity, and preference, is the first served that "there is no such thing as an infant."
true external object. Note especially that, from the infant's point of
Perhaps there is nothing specific to attachment view, the experience is the same as if he had ar-
in all this. Our point is not that we should mark ranged it all himself -- that is, attributing a major
the onset of attachment here at 3-5 months-of-age. organizing role to the caregiver does not diminish
We should not; nonetheless, there are compelling the significance of ongoing interactions to the
reasons to present developing attachment relation- child. This is not to concede that the development
ships and emergence of the self-other distinction in of attachment is trivially under stimulus control, as
a common developmental outline. First, each of proposed by traditional learning theorists. As we
the learning biases mentioned in the previous sec- see shortly, the primary caregiver provides impor-
tion is in full play here. Second, the infant has to tant elements of structure throughout the develop-
recognize the caregiver as an object existing in its ment of the first attachment relationship; indeed,
own right before the concept of attachment can she probably structures the infant's secure base be-
make any sense at all. Third, and more impor- havior to a far greater degree and for a much
tantly, we can see here the first example of a proc- longer time than attachment theory currently envi-
ess that is influential throughout infancy and early sions.
childhood. The caregiver modifies the environ- Phase 3: Reflection of the mother scheme: Self
ment in order to serve as a matrix upon which the as object. As outlined earlier, the mother is the
organization of the infant's behavior can play out. aliment (input) to an entire suite of sensori-
She does this both incidentally, as a consequence affective schemata. As these schemata become
of entailing so many behavioral possibilities for the well practiced and intercoordinated, she becomes
infant, and intentionally, by arranging objects and better and better defined, until she is recognized as

10
WATERS ET AL.

an object in her own right. This is a significant desire to complete basic descriptive research be-
step beyond the ability to recognize and discrimi- fore analyzing processes and mechanisms of
nate that developed in Phase 2. Of course, to the change in detail. The proper relationship between
sensorimotor infant there are no differences among description and explanation is, of course, a matter
objects upon which the same acts can be per- of strategy, one that sharply divides research pro-
formed. The mother, however, is virtually unique grams into construct- vs operationist-oriented ap-
in the range and complexity of intersections be- proaches. One limitation of traditional learning
tween her caretaking and play behavior and the in- approaches has been the tendency to define attach-
fant's schemata. ment in certain terms in order that could be ex-
Thus, just as the primary caregiver is defined plained by a standard litany of mechanisms. In
as the intersection of the things she does toward contrast, Bowlby and his construct-oriented col-
the infant, there comes a moment in which the in- leagues view attachment as a biological phenome-
fant recognizes itself as the intersection of the non that had to be discovered and described on its
schemes she directs at him -- that is, "She is the own terms before we can know what needs to be
common element in a vast array of sensori- explained.
affective contexts and experiences." Then by a Phase 4: Initiation of secure base behavior.
process of "informal inference," for the first time, As indicated in Bowlby's model and confirmed in
"I am the common element that ties her behavior research employing the Ainsworth Strange Situa-
together across these contexts and behaviors." tion, secure base behavior is evident in most in-
Through a process perhaps akin to perceptual fants by 12-months. In the proposed analysis,
learning, the infant notices itself in the reflection of however, the boundaries of the secure base phase
the experiences that define its mother as an inde- are considerably wider than in the traditional
pendent object. In subsequent months, this I is model and the emergence and consolidation of se-
mapped into a physical self-representation and cure base behavior are placed in separate phases.
eventually becomes evident in mirror self-
recognition tests. Piaget might have described this Informal observations suggest that the rudi-
as a process of cognitive reorganization. Such ex- ments of secure base behavior emerge within a few
planations leavened his theory with an element of days or weeks of the first free crawling. It seems
magic-- about which it was just a trifle rude to ask. likely, therefore, that important cognitive sub-
In contrast, we propose that the tendency to ob- strates of secure base behavior are available and in
serve one's own behavior is a biological given, eas- some preliminary way even organized, before the
ily verified by empirical research. Thus, reference onset of locomotion. These substrates might in-
to informal inference has considerably more ex- clude, among other things, knowledge of the spa-
planatory potential than Piaget's appeals to cogni- tial layout of the home, expectations about
tive reorganization. mother's typical behavior and her responsiveness
to infant signals, and at least some ideas about us-
If such mechanisms are critical to developing ing her as a resource when interacting with inter-
attachment relationships, why are they left to cog- esting toys and objects.
nitive/perceptual psychologists? Why haven't they
attracted attention as key issues for attachment re- Granting this, a problem remains. In a word,
search? They are obviously relevant to the task of the notion that attachment arises from interaction
cataloguing species-specific learning abilities that doesn't explain exactly how sensitive care early in
support developing attachment relationships. A infancy could affect secure base behavior six
variety of factors seem to have contributed to this. months to a year later. Pressed to account for the
It is at least partly an unintended consequence of relationship between maternal sensitivity and the
elegant simplifications employed when Bowlby in- behavioral details of secure base behavior, attach-
troduced his theory. Foremost among these de- ment theorists traditionally cite biases in the in-
vices is the four phase model outlined above. The fant's learning abilities. Given certain biases in the
model clarifies a new paradigm by abstracting infant's learning abilities, sensitive early care is
milestones in attachment behavior from the com- said to initiate or catalyze the process by which
plex developmental context in which they are em- components of the attachment control system fall
bedded. into place. This is an interesting hypothesis with
implications for behavioral development in gen-
Attention to mechanisms was also delayed by eral, and in view of the ethological literature it is

11
LEARNING TO LOVE

not altogether implausible. It will not become priming; indeed, it requires little explanation at all.
compelling, however, until we have detailed re- As in other domains, new skills are applied to fa-
search on biases in infant learning abilities and an miliar problems as soon as they are learned (i.e.,
empirically based catalogue of control system without explicit instruction related to each poten-
components. tial application). When locomotion enters the in-
The search for mechanisms that can have di- fant's repertoire, it is promptly applied to the al-
rect effects from early infancy into later infancy ready familiar problem of balancing exploration
and toddlerhood should not be left to specialists in and hedonic state. Biases in the infant's learning
learning theory or to comparative psychologists. It abilities may have to be invoked to explain 1) the
should be placed at the top of the attachment speed and complexity of prelocomotor learning, 2)
agenda. We should not, however, construe this the infant's ability to monitor mother's access and
search as a critical test of Bowlby's attachment the- responsiveness consistently, and 3) the selection
ory. Correlations between early care and later be- and coordination of exploratory, proximity seek-
havior are clearly replicable, as are relationships ing, and contact related behaviors. These biases
between attachment status in infancy and later may account for the fact that eliciting and termi-
adaptive behavior in many domains. The critical nating conditions of attachment and exploratory
issues is to explain them. In this context, it matters behavior are so similar across cultures. The impor-
not whether early care affects later behavior di- tant task for attachment theorists is to detail these
rectly or merely predicts later care, which then biases rather than merely alluding to them.
proves to be the proximal cause. Although theo- Although extensive prelocomotor experience
rists in the Bowlby/Ainsworth tradition (e.g. with organized patterns of maternal care may fa-
Sroufe, Egeland, & Kreutzer, in press) have fa- cilitate the onset of secure base behavior, some of
vored the former, any explanation combining bi- our recent work suggests that explicit instruction
ases in infant learning abilities, structure provided plays a critical role in perfecting it -- that is, secure
by the primary caregiver, and traditional learning base behavior is not merely learned, it is taught.
mechanisms could be equally consistent with Once we expect this, it is easy to confirm both in
Bowlby's control systems model. The significance humans and non-human primates. Figure 4 shows
of these mechanisms is highlighted in Figure 3, a mother macaque teaching her infant to return to
which continues our revised model through her. Our home observations of human infants sug-
Bowlby's secure base stage. gests that similar behavior is common in our spe-
Just as play and caregiving interactions can cies as well. This should be recognized in attach-
help organize sensorimotor schemes and provide ment theory, and the learning mechanisms in-
information relevant to secure base behavior prior volved should be examined in detail.
to the onset of locomotion, care and interaction Maternal influences on the organization of in-
with a sensitive caregiver continue to provide and fant secure base behavior are not limited to explicit
organize information related to secure base behav- instruction and prompting. In a recent study of 24
ior throughout infancy. In a sense then, early se- female Japanese macaques and their infants at the
cure base behavior is closely tied to dyadic spatial South Texas Primate Observatory, Waters &
patterns seen in early care. The infant knows the Kondo-Ikemura (submitted for publication) exam-
general outline of the "secure base strategy (or ined the maternal behavior correlates of individual
game)" before the onset of locomotion. Distance, differences in infants' ability to use the mother as a
accessibility, and caregiver responsiveness provide secure base. Our goal was to determine whether
information that modifies affective state. Interac- there are strong links between infant attachment
tion, proximity, and contact can reestablish he- behavior and concurrent maternal behavior in free-
donic equilibrium. The infant learns that the key ranging animals and to identify the domains in
to the puzzle is to use the mother. which these are most evident. To assess infant at-
It matters little whether the distance between tachment security, we developed a 90-item Q-set
infant and caregiver is managed by coordinating similar to the Attachment Q-set used in our re-
infant communication with maternal locomotion or search on human infants. A Q-sort definition of
by the infant's own locomotion. This being the the hypothetical infant macaque "most able to use
case, the rapid onset of secure base behavior is not its mother as a secure base" was used to assign
surprising, nor is it strong evidence of biological

12
WATERS ET AL.

The Development of Attachment:


Mechanisms and Milestones

Explicit instruction Secure base behavior emerges


Mother monitoring
and retrieving

Distress tolerance Separation protest


Knowledge of: declines
environment
mother behavior
own abilities

Practice Secure base behavior


Operant learning consolidated
Improved locomotion
More knowledge

Figure 3. Attachment and secure base behavior: Mechanisms and milestones in infancy and early
childhood.

each infant a score on attachment security. Each very strong correlations between infant security
infant was observed for approximately two hours and concurrent maternal behavior in support of se-
and then described by sorting the Q-sort items in cure base behavior. Interestingly, the strongest
terms of how characteristic they were of the infant concurrent correlates of infant secure base behav-
in question. Items that described the infant well ior are related to the quality of maternal supervi-
received higher scores (maximum = 9). Items that sion.
were less characteristic or the opposite of the in-
fant in question were assigned lower scores Infants who were most able to use their moth-
(minimum = 1). The correlation between the array ers as a secure base had mothers who supervised
of scores describing the infant and the array of them closely and consistently, were persistent in
scores describing the hypothetical most secure in- caregiving, and were willing and powerful enough
fant served as the infant's score on attachment se- to defend them if they came into conflict with ju-
curity. veniles or other adults. Face-to-face interaction is
not a primary mode of infant-mother interaction in
Maternal behavior was observed using a 90- this species, and there was little evidence that this
item maternal behavior Q-set that focused on so- or other behavior analogous to "sensitivity" in hu-
cial interaction with other adults, caregiving, pro- mans was related to the infants' secure base behav-
tection, interaction, supervision, communi-cation, ior. Indeed, by standards applied to human moth-
and other behaviors that mapped onto the items in ers, even the mothers of the more secure infants
the infant secure base Q-set. Two-hour observa- could be remarkably insensitive and intrusive.
tions of maternal behavior were obtained on a
schedule insuring that maternal and infant Q-sorts The salience of supervision and consistency in
were not obtained during the same time interval for these data is attributable in part to the fact that
any dyad. these infant macaques were constantly at risk of
serious injury from other macaques; nonetheless,
As summarized in Table 1, there are indeed the data clearly illustrate the fact that an infant's

13
LEARNING TO LOVE

Figure 4. The leaving game. Mother leaves, then pauses and calls or makes partial return. Infant
goes to mother. Mother repeats leaving sequence. Five second sequence drawn from film. From
Hinde and Simpson (1975, p. 52).

use of its mother as a secure base depends on her tion of the basis phenomenon as it was over
behaving as one. If she is predictable, consistent, mechanisms. The control systems model has
powerful, and available, she provides a matrix or clearly proved to be the better description. The
template around which the infant can organize ex- task now is to explicate the roles of traditional
ploration and effective attachment behavior. It is learning mechanisms in the development of secure
unnecessary to attribute all the structure of the se- base behavior. The understandable concern among
cure base phenomenon to the attachment behav-ior attachment theorists is that admitting a role for tra-
control system; indeed it is most reasonable to ditional learning mechanisms may reopen old de-
imagine that the components of such a behavioral bates with learning theorists. This should not deter
control system depend upon a matrix of maternal us. Although every operational definition of at-
behavior in order to become inter-coordinated and tachment proposed by traditional learning theorists
begin to act as a system (Waters, 1981). Although has proven utterly sterile, the mechanisms detailed
mechanisms that might tie early care to later secure in traditional learning theory may play important
base behavior deserve attention in theory and re- roles that learning theorists never anticipated. In a
search, mechanisms that relate infant behavior to word, learning may yet prove more interesting and
concurrent maternal behavior deserve special at- useful than Learning Theory.
tention, if only because they have been overlooked
for so long. The notion that mother's behavior plays a sig-
nificant role in organizing and maintaining attach-
Early competition between attachment theory ment behavior has an important implication for
and learning theory was as much over the descrip- longitudinal research. We need to pay more atten-

14
WATERS ET AL.

tion to the possibility that consistency in parental solidate the important observation that decline in
behavior and supervision can explain predictive/ separation protest does not imply decline in the in-
correlational results in attachment research. This fant-caregiver attachment. On the contrary, as
is why we developed a measure of mother mon- cognitive development and experience are causing
key's behavior and why we are developing a paral- separation protest to decline, the same mechanisms
lel one for human mother's behavior. are contributing to the consolidation of secure base
behavior, as described in the next phase. Changing
In brief, the fact that Infant Behavior A pre-
perceptions of separation have obvious implica-
dicts Child Behavior B is likely to reflect the fact
tions for attachment behavior, but they reflect little
that parenting is consistent. As discussed earlier,
about changes in the underlying control system.
this could be the case for early security and later
socialization, where mechanisms that would give Phase 6: Consolidating secure base behavior.
direct effects of security on socialization are hard In Bowlby's model, secure base behavior emerges
to imagine. by age one-year, begins to decline along with sepa-
ration protest at around age two, and gives way to
Phase 5: The decline of separation protest.
what he calls the "goal-corrected part-nership,"
The fifth phase in the proposed model covers the
around the beginning of the third year. This is a
period during which separation protest declines.
point at which our own naturalistic observations
Most research places this between 12 and 30
begin to diverge somewhat from Bowlby's descrip-
months-of-age, though for this presentation the
tion. Despite early signs of secure base behavior
timing is not critical and we are not implying that
along with the onset of locomotion and the rather
any phase begins and ends discretely. Several fac-
reliable separation related responses that can be
tors contribute to the decline of separation protest.
elicited in the laboratory, secure base behavior in
Specifically, the infant becomes: naturalistic settings does not seem very well organ-
ized or very consistent in 12-month-olds. Tradi-
1. More able to tolerate distress without becoming
tional descriptions underestimate the time it takes
disorganized;
to consolidate this complex behavior; moreover,
2. More able to predict caregiver behavior and to our observations do not point strongly to the emer-
monitor caregiver location and behavior; gence of a meaningful goal-corrected partnership
at either the end of infancy or during early child-
3. More able to regulate and re-establish contact on hood.
his own (i.e., gains a degree of control that
makes situations less novel and thus less threat- The secure base phenomenon has received
ening); surprisingly little attention in naturalistic research
on human infants. Ainsworth's (Ainsworth et al.
4. More experienced with various types of environ- 1978) descriptions of secure base behavior in the
ments and can devote less effort and attention fourth quarter of the first year in her Baltimore
to the features of the situation per se and more
study and Anderson's (1972) observations in public
attention to monitoring and forethought in a parks stand almost alone and are rarely cited. This
wider range of circumstances; is more than a curiosity or an oversight; it reflects
5. More experienced with coping in various envi- several difficulties associated with control system
ronments and thus has a better estimate of his models. Patterns of behavior are inherently more
ability to cope under various circumstances (a difficult to define and describe than are discrete
degree of "self knowledge") and is less likely to behaviors. In addition, the performance of a con-
initiate retreat to mother early in encounters trol system cannot be equated with the quantity of
with novel situations. behavioral output. For proper assessment, we must
focus instead on the success and efficiency with
Note that the infant's reflecting on past behav- which the control system maintains itself within
ior does not have to be explained. As above, self- specified set goals (see, Waters, 1981; Waters &
observation is a fundamental fact of adaptation in Deane, 1985).
humans. Note also that the tendency to observe,
represent, and reflect on one's own behavior is cen- A behavioral control system's performance is
tral to Epstein's self theory (1972, and this vol- not inherently beyond quantitative analysis, but it
ume). presents measurement problems much more diffi-
cult than those associated with rates and frequen-
This phase is included in our outline to con-

15
LEARNING TO LOVE

Table 1
Correlations Between Maternal Behavior and Infant Security Scores
Among Japanese Macaquesa
Q-Set Item Pearson Correlation
Does not allow other monkeys to hold infant. (R)b .64
Occupied in caretaking, to the exclusion of other activities. .64
Keeps infant closer for some time after unusual event has ceased. .55
Carries infant when moving from place to place (i.e., Doesn't just walk off). .54
Devotes more time to infant than to older siblings. .53
Does not quickly become bored with caretaking. (R) .52
Accepts or tolerates infant using mother's tail or body during play. .50
Alert to subtle changes in the environment. .50
Does not hesitate to punish infant in appropriate circumstances. .47
Rarely changes attitude toward infant. (R) .46
Continues caretaking behavior even if infant wiggles or gets annoyed. (R) .45
Monitors infant's location and activities consistently. .44
Retrieves infant or drives adults away if infant approaches .44
(Esp. adult males or dominant females)
Allows infant to leave in unfamiliar settings. .42
Recognizes infant signals of fear, etc. .41
Allows infant to play with novel objects. (R) .40

Note. All values significant at p < .05 or less.


a
For additional items related to maternal rank see Waters & Kondo-Ikemura (submitted).
b
(R) = Wording of original Q-set item is reversed in this table (as is the sign of the correlation coefficient) for
clearer presentation.

cies of specific behaviors. Thus, the Ainsworth et lems that arise from the control systems
al. (1978) summarized infants' ability to use the
mother as a secure base in terms of highly subjec- conceptualization of infant attachment relation-
tive ratings, and Anderson et al. found the phe- ships. Among these are the following:
nomenon rather elusive when they moved beyond 1. Researchers from other methodological tradi-
discrete frequency counts and timing intervals. tions have found it difficult to understand the
Our efforts to address these measurement problems control systems view of the attachment con-
warrant a brief description. struct and to fully appreciate all it implies
During the past few years, we have tried to re- about the range of relevant behavior and the
solve some of the difficult methodological prob- responsiveness of secure base behavior to con-
text.

16
WATERS ET AL.

2. It is very expensive to collect detailed behav- for the methodology we were trying to develop, it
ioral data on a phenomenon as complex as the could probably be ignored, put off until later, or
secure base phenomenon by conventional time scaled away. Soon, however, the same result ap-
sampling methods, with the consequence that peared in a larger set of parent reports we were
we use small samples, rarely replicate studies, collecting on different children, and later in other
and often under-design projects to keep down parent report data and in Q-sort data from other
the number of subjects. laboratories. Across laboratories and studies, the
3. It is difficult to evaluate the attachment behav- mean security score for 12-month-olds has ranged
ioral system if the starting point is rate or fre- from .2 - .3. The mean score in typical samples of
quency data obtained through conventional 3-4 year olds ranges from .4 -.7!
observational methods. This problem arises in Rather than interpret these results as evidence that
part from the fact that conventional methods older children are more attached, we take them lit-
have only a very limited ability to take contex- erally: Correspondence between children's be-
tual information into account within manage- havior and the pattern of behavior that defines the
able observational coding systems. secure base phenomenon increases with age. That
4. Definitions of constructs such as attachment se- is, they become much better at using the mother as
curity have been difficult to formalize, which a base from which to explore as they get older.
makes them difficult to communicate, evalu- This is a very striking result, yet it is quite under-
ate, and improve. standable in light of the preceding discussion.

5. It has been difficult to establish discriminant Relevant mechanisms are likely to include
validity of attachment constructs and to ad- practice, operant learning, and improved locomo-
dress alternative interpretations proposed at tion. The infant also becomes increasingly famil-
the end of longitudinal studies. iar with and confident about its abilities and limita-
tions, caregiver behavior, and facets of increas-
Much of our work has involved developing and ingly diverse environments. There is nothing mys-
validating an economical, behaviorally specific Q- terious in this. Every element can be examined in
sort methodology for assessing secure base behav- detail. The more fully we appreciate that attach-
ior. For the most part, these problems are proving ment behavior is learned, even taught, the more
much more manageable with the Attachment Q- evident it becomes that it must take time. Accord-
sort methodology we have de-veloped. The ingly, the 12-month Ainsworth Strange Situation
method has proved quite easy to learn; moreover, probably assesses nascent rather than mature se-
students, parents, and even researchers from other cure base behavior. Viewed in this light, it is
theoretical perspectives have consistently reported amazing that 12-month Strange Situation data are
strong "ah-ha" experiences and a sense of what at- at all correlated with patterns of care in early in-
tachment theory is all about as soon as they have fancy; yet the pattern of results is clear and replic-
spent some time with the Q-set items. able.
Both observer and mother reports are reliable Of course, correlations between early maternal
with only a few hours of observation. Q-sort data sensitivity and later attachment security do not ex-
on one- and two-year-olds map quite well into plain attachment security. They are new facts in
Strange Situation data on attachment security (e.g., search of an explanation, as are correlations be-
Vaughn & Waters, submitted). Table 2 sum- tween secure base behavior and later de-
marizes some of the Q-set items that distinguish velopmental outcomes. Our Q-sort research high-
significantly between secure and anxiously at- lights the need for additional descriptive/
tached one-year-olds. observational data on secure base behavior at vari-
We encountered one of our most important re- ous ages. The Attachment Q-sort is an economical
sults almost as soon as we began using the Attach- method of surveying a wide range of behavior in
ment Q-set. In the first informal comparison of Q- advance of surgically precise time sampling and
sort security scores between small samples of one- sequential analysis of specific behavior patterns.
and three-year-olds, it was evident that the mean Phase 7: Identification. If our goal were to
score was much higher in the older group. This cover only the time period spanned by Bowlby's
struck us as more of a nuisance than anything else. four-phase model, we could conclude our presenta-
After all, unless it entailed some sort of problem

17
LEARNING TO LOVE

Table 2
Attachment Q-Set Home Observations of One-Year-Olds:
Differences Between Infants Classified Secure and Insecure
in the Strange Situation

Q-Sort Item-title (Item number)a Item Means t (56)


Secure Insecure
b
Enjoys playful physical contact with mother (64R) 8.09 7.27 1.86+
(.90) (2.11)
Does NOT expect mother to be unresponsive (54R) 7.78 6.40 3.24***
(1.12) (1.92)
Affectively responsive and expressive (25) 7.70 6.58 2.08*
(1.51) (2.40)
Prefers to be comforted by mother (35) 7.55 6.67 2.15*
(1.21) (1.76)
Looks to mother for reassurance when wary (31) 7.54 6.75 1.99+
(1.00) (1.56)
Person oriented rather than object oriented. (65R) 7.08 6.02 2.93**
(1.07) (1.60)
Easily comforted by mother (4) 7.03 6.10 2.02*
(1.51) (2.02)
Laughs easily with mother (87R) 6.98 6.12 1.78+
(1.37) (2.17)
Affective sharing occurs during play (77) 6.83 5.73 2.23*
(1.84) (1.89)
Predominant mood is happy (3) 6.67 5.35 2.09*
(2.24) (2.60)
Acts to maintain social interaction (40) 6.64 5.17 2.79**
(1.94) (2.06)
Imitates mother's behavior (88) 6.61 5.67 2.28*
(1.53) (1.59)
Easily distracted from distress (22) 6.52 5.52 1.93+
(1.93) (1.90)
Transition from explor. to prox. is smooth (52) 6.36 5.23 2.89***
(1.34) (1.63)
Gross motor control is smooth and coordinated (46R) 6.33 5.46 1.76+
(1.65) (2.02)
Does NOT lack self-confidence (48R) 6.20 5.42 2.09*
(1.41) (1.43)

18
WATERS ET AL.

Table 2 (Continued

Done NOT become angry with mother easily. (82R) 6.03 5.06 1.93+
(1.72) (2.05)
Stays closer to mother in unfamiliar settings (72) 5.77 5.15 1.69+
(1.44) (1.29)
Transition from prox. to explor. is smooth (68R) 5.66 4.85 1.77+
(1.73) (1.74)
Prefers tasks that are difficult (6R) 5.20 4.58 1.89+
(1.30) (1.19)
NOT Demanding when initiating activities 5.03 3.71 2.52*
with mom (74R) (1.69) (2.18)
Explores objects thoroughly (19) 5.00 4.17 1.82+
(1.75) (1.69)
Adapts active play to avoid hurting mother (84R) 4.83 4.23 2.04*
(.91) (1.25)
Careful with toys (27) 4.78 3.83 2.31*
(1.15) (1.83)
Cries to prevent separation (29) 4.27 3.25 2.16*
(2.10) (1.47)
Remains fearful of moving toys or animals (1) 3.87 3.19 2.93***
(.84) (.93)
Note. *** = p < .005, ** = p < .01, * = p < .05, + = p < .10

a Item numbers from the 100-item Q-set are in parentheses.


b "R" beside an item number indicates that the item wording from the 100-item Q-set has been reversed to
clarify tabular presentation and the raw scores have been reflected (i.e. value in table is 10 - mean score.)
Signs of the t-values are correspondingly reversed.
c Values in parentheses are standard deviations.

tion with the consolidation of secure base behavior task of explaining widely cited empirical relation-
in the second or third year, leaving out the goal ships between patterns of secure base behavior and
corrected partnership concept altogether. In doing later developmental outcomes. As earlier, correla-
so, however, we would overlook the important tions are not explanations; rather, they are facts in
transition from sensorimotor to cognitive represen- search of explanations. Detailing mechanisms that
tation of attachment relationships that Bowlby account for the predictive validity of attachment
tried unsuccessfully to capture in terms of the goal data broadens the foundations of attachment theory
corrected partnership. As Main & Kaplan (1989) and clarifies the place of attachment in develop-
have recently demonstrated, this move to this level ment. Accordingly, let us at least briefly turn to
of representation is the key to linking developmen- sequelae of secure base behavior that carry attach-
tal theories of attachment with perspectives on at- ment relationships from sensorimotor to represen-
tachment among adults. tational thought and from preference to values.
Concluding our analysis with consolidation of Correlations between infant attachment secu-
secure base behavior would also leave open the rity and later socialization outcomes are among the
most widely cited findings in the attachment litera-

19
LEARNING TO LOVE

ture. At the same time, they present attachment nal study of study of attachment, identification,
theory with one of its most difficult challenges. and socialization in 3.5 - 7-year-old boys at SUNY
Specifically, it is not obvious how a child's confi- Stony Brook. In brief, maternal reports of attach-
dence in its mother's availability and responsive- ment security were obtained at age 3.5 years, using
ness (i.e. secure attachment) could keep it from our Attachment Q-set. We subs-equently obtained
putting a rock through the schoolhouse window. parent reports regarding the child's orientation to-
(Were it not contrary to the empirical data, one ward socialization practices at age 5, using an 81
could just as easily imagine that confidence in item Likert format survey. Typical correlations
Mom or Dad saving him would increase the likeli- between attachment security and subsequent scores
hood of the child throwing the rock.) on identification items are presented in Table 3.
Obviously, few parents would approve of their In brief, we described two facets of identi-
children vandalizing the neighborhood school; fication: encapsulation during infancy and commit-
nonetheless, given opportunity and ammunition, ment during early and middle childhood. During
some children would transgress and others would infancy the child is typically enmeshed in secure
not. Given comparable socialization pressure base relationships with both parents. As already
against vandalism, why the diverse outcomes? d , the parents provide much of the matrix upon
Part of the explanation turns on the fact that so- which the child organizes its behavior. In a sense,
cialization pressures on children are not entirely the family is a behavioral/affective economy from
comparable across families. Individual differences which the child derives considerable benefits in the
in temperament, IQ, and other traits might contrib- form of nurturance and support for exploration and
ute as well. Most importantly, literature on behav- cognitive development. Figure 5 extends our de-
ior problems and delinquency suggests that chil- velopmental outline through middle childhood. It
dren exposed to entirely appropriate socialization captures the transition to representational thought
practices and conventional norms of good behavior emphasized in Bowlby's coal corrected partnership
differ in their orientation toward these norms. In stage and also emphasizes the foundations of so-
short, some children don't care as much as others. cialization that are established at this age.
Some don't care at all. They take their lumps and
do what they want. During infancy and toddlerhood, the home-
reared child is largely insulated from rule systems
Hypotheses relating early attachment relation- that differ dramatically from or offer alternatives to
ships to later orientation toward socialization were the family's. Parents are in a position to state the
central to Freud's theory of psychosocial develop- rules of the game and to shape the child's initial
ment and were extensively investigated in early so- conceptualization of itself, of them, and of the
cial learning research. Unfortunately, empirical world beyond the family. When sensitive, consis-
confirmation was beyond the concepts and meth- tent, facilitative caretakers make the most of this
odologies of the day, and these hypotheses have re- situation, a predictable outcome is the development
ceived little attention in recent theory or research. of secure attachment, self-confidence, and their
They deserve renewed attention in light of concurrent correlates.
Bowlby's attachment theory and new methods of
assessing attachment related constructs. To this Although the child does not yet make an inten-
end, Richters & Waters (in press) redefined the tra- tional commitment to the family's norms of good
ditional concept of identification in terms consis- behavior, he or she is behaviorally committed to
tent with current views of cognitive-social learning the family system long before these norms are
and empirical research on development in infancy even explained or imposed as rules. Participating
and early childhood. Although their formulation is in attachment relationships allows the child to
neither psychodynamic nor focussed on gender and maintain organized behavior and to maximize a
explicit imitation, the term identification was re- wide range of benefits over time. Accordingly, the
tained to acknowledge the origins of the attach- child enmeshes itself in the family system to what-
ment-socialization hypothesis in psychoanalytic ever extent parental behavior supports. Herein lie
and social learning theory. the seeds of prosocial motivation.
Preliminary empirical support for a link be- During middle childhood, the rules of the
tween attachment and a child's orientation toward game become increasingly complex. Parents begin
socialization is emerging in an ongoing longitudi- to expect more consistent conformity to family

20
WATERS ET AL.

rules, and the range of rules and contingencies ex- cialization as an inherently coercive process. It as-
pands at whatever rate parents estimate cognitive signs reinforcement and punishment important
development allows. If, up to this point, confor- roles in shaping the behavioral commitment upon
mity had simply been a matter of contingency which such informal inferences are predicated.
management, we might expect it to extinguish rap- But beyond this, contingency management is sim-
idly as the child moves out from under the parents' ply another source of information available from
constant direct supervision; however this is not the the environment and from self-observation. These
typical outcome. In most respects, children retain are the cognitive and motivational mechanisms un-
significant elements of the family's behavioral derlying social and observational learning in early
norms and values in the face of novel alternatives, childhood.
competing input, and opportunities to observe
other children operating under different sets of Our conceptualization differs considerably
contingencies. from the traditional view of identification. It does
not specifically involve gender or focus on the
This is not to suggest that contingency man- same sex parent. The focus is on identification
agement is irrelevant to socialization or that early with family norms rather than with one parent or
socialization inoculates a child against every unto- the other. Our concept is also much less focused
ward influence encountered in the peer culture; on literal imitation than is traditional social learn-
rather, a variety of factors related to the economics ing theory. It most closely approximates Rotter's
of family living, the encapsulated context of early notion of individual differences in the reinforce-
socialization, and the behavioral commitment en- ment value of stimuli. Secure attachment makes
tailed in early attachments have the predictable the child value the parent more, want to avoid pa-
outcome of rendering a child receptive to parental rental censure, and over-look models that are dis-
socialization practices. Of course, even if the cordant with family norms. As summarized in
child's behavior is not integrated with that of one Figure 6, identification provides a missing mecha-
or a few caregivers as described in the preceding nism necessary to explain the correlations between
phases, he or she can be controlled by contingency attachment and specific socialization outcomes.
management, until independence renders this im- Attachment is correlated with social-ization out-
practical. Control, however, will remain extrinsic comes because of its influence on the child's orien-
and the child's orientation toward norms will re- tation toward socialization. This, in turn, interacts
main one of conformity rather than commitment. with childrearing practices to yield differential so-
We employ the term identifica-tion to summa- cialization outcomes.
rize (though not to explain) the child's investment
in the family system and readiness to accept newly If a child cares about parental socialization
articulated demands. As long as the child's social goals and if the parents' socialization practices are
world is mainly within the family, identification sound, then effective and enduring outcomes are
can be explained as an informal inference from expected. If, on the other hand, attachment prob-
participation in parental attachments and the be- lems have resulted in an indifferent attitude toward
havioral affective economy of the family. When socialization pressures, then even appropriate prac-
parents first "reveal" that a particular behavior or tices will only be effective in the short term. In ad-
attitude is part of the family's system, the child can dition, even secure attachment and a positive ori-
reasonably (if informally) infer that "If that's the entation toward parental socialization goals can be
system, then that's for me." Insofar as the child is followed by significant socialization failures if par-
already behaviorally committed to parental attach- ents' socialization practices are inconsistent, un-
ments and to the family, he or she is biased toward clear, or in other respects inappropriate. That is,
accepting the new behavior or attitude without ex- attachment does not explain socialization out-
plicit training or persuasion. As Epstein sug-gests, comes; it moderates them through the interaction
anything less would challenge postulates of the of identification with child rearing practices.
child's self definition or self theory and engender Viewed in this light, it is ironic that attach-
negative emotion. Indeed, the child should find ment theory and social learning theory continue to
that conformity and advocacy of newly defined be viewed (and practiced) as competing perspec-
norms confirms central self-theory pos-tulates and, tives. After all, the paradigm clash between the
thus, engenders positive emotion. control system view and learning theory was pre-
cipitated by disagreements about the level of com-
This is a positive alternative to views of so- plexity and organization at which attachment

21
LEARNING TO LOVE

Table 3
Attachment Security at 3.5 Years-Old and Identification
Q-Set Items at 4 - 5 Years-Old (N = 81 males)

Identification Q-Set Item Pearson Correlation

When he realizes he is doing something wrong, he tries to undo it. .47


Readily accepts (parent's) suggestions or advice. .42
Does not persist in begging for things after (parent) tells him "No!" (R) .42
Stops doing things (parent) has punished him for.(R) .41
Tells (parent) what family rules are (as if checking to see if he has them right. .41
Points (to parent) out ways he and (parent) are alike. .40
Does not refuse to obey (parent) by saying "No!" (R) .38
Does not become angry when (parent) has to interrupt something he likes to do. .37
Doesn't hit, throw toys, or yell at (parent) when he is angry. (R) .37
Learns family rules quickly; doesn't have to be told twice. .37
Becomes sad rather than angry when (parent) has to punish him. .36
Embarrassed when (parent) catches or punishes him for misbehaving; .35
offers to make amends.
Asks before doing something that might be unsafe or not allowed.. .35

Note. (R) = Wording of original Q-set item is reversed in this table (as is the sign of the
correlation coefficient) for clearer presentation.

should be defined. No one disputed the impor- obviously relevant to a comprehensive explanation
tance of the causal mechanisms detailed in the lit- of the development and developmental signifi-
erature on human learning, and the definitional is- cance of attachment relationships. In fact, a so-
sue has been resolved in favor of the control sys- phisticated theory of social learning and social-
tem/secure base conceptualization. Although tradi- (including self-) cognition is essential to the suc-
tional learning theorists never defined attachment cess of Bowlby's effort to demystify and preserve
in very useful terms, the well studied mechanisms Freud's insights about human attachment.
of operant and social learning are not the exclusive Phase 8: A Supervision Partnership. A key
property of one theoretical perspective. They are function of attachment in infancy and early child-

22
WATERS ET AL.

The Development of Attachment:


Mechanisms and Milestones

Learn and infer family norms


Compliance demands
Operant learning and imitation Identification
Informal inference from existing
behavioral commitment

Parents trying to monitor


and supervise Partnership in
Informal inference about the maintaining
advantages of participating supervision
in family system

Figure 5. Attachment, identification, and the supervision partnership: Mechanisms and milestones.

hood is to foster the development of independence. imity seeking represent reorganization within and
The transition from sensorimotor to representa- among behavioral systems, not attenuation of the
tional thought brings major changes in proximity infant-mother relationship. This is a crucial in-
seeking, contact maintaining, and communication sight. Without it, we might overlook the organiz-
between parent and offspring. The secure base ac- ing role that parent behavior serves throughout
quires a cognitive representation and, as it be- childhood, adolescence, and early adulthood in
comes portable, exploratory and social excursions many cultures and families. We would also find
can become increasingly extended and extensive. ourselves puzzled by grief and mourning at the
Peremptory proximity and contact seeking in re- loss of a parent. Thus, although we questioned the
sponse to moderate uncertainty, discomfort, or view that secure base behavior in the sensorimotor
stress is replaced by communication and direct period develops into a goal corrected partnership in
coping. early childhood, we conclude our developmental
Rather than competing with emerging behav- outline by returning to the partnership concept, not
ioral systems such as peer affiliation, adult bond- as a species' charac-teristic way to manage attach-
ing, and parenthood, the attachment behavioral ment relationships in childhood and adolescence
system operates in coordination with them; only but, rather, as a strategy employed in some fami-
when direct coping fails, or in the face of extreme lies, in some cultures.
threats, does it reassert the priority among behav- Beginning in middle childhood, a child can
ioral systems that it enjoyed in infancy and early enter into a wide range of contracts with adults.
childhood. These arise within and across families. Within
These changes occasion difficult transitions. families, many key socialization practices entail
With the goal-corrected partnership, Bowlby em- exchange agreements between parent and child (e.
phasized that age-related changes in overt prox- g. rewards for good behavior.) Often, the need to

23
LEARNING TO LOVE

care for younger children, share transportation, and ing an infant as a secure base, however, the par-
divide household duties lead to agreements about ent's intention to supervise an older child is not
division of labor and reciprocity. Across families, sufficient. The parent can only supervise and serve
an adult may agree to extend privileges contingent as a secure base if he or she is kept aware of the
upon a child's good behavior, offer instruction or child's excursions and plans, both when the plan is
pay in exchange for the child's assistance, or super- first formulated and later when plans and play sites
vise and assist the child on the expectation that the change significantly during the day. This is a re-
child's parents will reciprocate. sponsibility that only the child can fulfill. Parents,
Unlike early attachment relationships, in of course, have a similar responsibility to keep the
which infant and adult share a common goals such child up to date if they venture forth while the
as maintaining proximity, advancing the infant's child is away at play. The supervision partnership
cognitive competence, and affect regulation, these has to include arrangements for a mode of commu-
relationships are optional and essentially eco- nication, a set of contingency plans, or alternative
nomic; they allow child and adult to meet their dif- supervision when the parent is away. Parents may
fering needs and goals by exchanging material or also want to limit the child's activities to places
behavioral assets of comparable worth. They as- where supervision by other adults is close at hand.
sume neither attachment between the parties nor It becomes the child's responsibility to operate
skills specifically derived from early attachments. within these limits or to negotiate exceptions in ad-
Most social arrangements between parents and vance.
children at this age are probably of this nature. The items listed in Table 4 suggest the types
One exception, however, is a supervision partner- of child behavior that might be used to assess the
ship designed to span the transition from family child's participation in such a partnership.
socialization to independence in a particular fam-
ily, community, or culture. Obviously both the parent's initiative and con-
sistency and the child's willingness to participate
During infancy and early childhood, parents are critical to this kind of partnership. It is
largely control the level of supervision over the unlikely that such a partnership could arise solely
child's behavior. At least, the balance of responsi- as a result of early sensitive care or a secure attach-
bility falls to the parent. In cultures such as our ment in infancy. Yet the parent's role in the part-
own, however, 6 - 16 year-old children spend a nership is analogous to sensitive care early, and the
considerable amount of time away from the par- quality of early attachment may influence the
ents' immediate supervision; yet may parents (and child's willingness and desire to participate.
children) undertake to maintain some level of con-
sistent supervision. The child's time with the peer Early secure base experience may also provide
group or in other activities is viewed as continuous an enduring understanding of what it is to relate to
with exploration and training begun earlier, not as a person over space and time. In view of the limits
detachment or independence. This entails a com- egocentrism places on social cognition and coop-
mon goal not found in the commercial relation- eration at this age, this could be an important asset.
ships described earlier. Specifically, within this ar- Where supervision partnerships are seen, they
rangement child and parent share the goal of main- validate the insight that Bowlby conveyed in his
taining a degree of supervision and contact when references to the goal-corrected partnership; at-
the child is away from direct parental supervision tachment does not simply decline as the child's ho-
for long periods. rizons expand. Other attachment-related be-
In a sense, a supervision partnership is an ex- havioral strategies that become necessary or possi-
tension of the parent's role as a matrix for the ble only beyond early childhood may make this
child's behavior and as a secure base; but now the point as well. Our capacity for abstraction, predic-
balance of responsibility between child and parent tion, and communication enables the attachment
is more evenly balanced than in infancy. The par- behavioral system to continue serving as an impor-
ent must want to maintain supervision and avail- tant resource as other behavioral systems mature,
ability during this transitional period and insure even into adulthood. For exam-ple, in our society,
that the child cooperates. Desire to do this may even adult children are expected to maintain im-
differ markedly across cultures and com-munities, plicitly agreed upon modes and schedules for
and from family to family. Unlike the task of serv- "keeping in touch" with parents, who have been
known to view lapses with alarm or anger. More-

24
WATERS ET AL.

Figure 6. Links between attachment and socialization outcomes: Identification and the consistency
of parental behavior as mediating mechanisms.

over, as parents come to depend more and more on tures or only in the fullness of relationships late in
their adult children, the children often adopt a life.
similar view of their parents' habits of "keeping in
touch." Conclusion
Research on the secure base analogues in child- Our discussion of milestones and mechanisms in
hood, adolescence, and adulthood has hardly be- attachment, identity, and identification is firmly
gun. We mention the supervisory partnership to rooted in Bowlby's theory and in his efforts to pre-
see what can be done with Bowlby's partnership serve the best conceptual and descriptive insights
notion and to emphasize that the secure base con- from the psychoanalytic tradition. Our analysis is
cept is powerful enough to support a comprehen- evolutionary rather than revolutionary and suggests
sive perspective on attachment relationships. A that a major integration of perspectives is at hand.
complete description of mechanisms and develop- Attachment theory and research have a great
mental milestones in attachment has to reach well deal to gain from detailing the roles that active par-
beyond infancy and early childhood. Although ent behavior and traditional learning mechanisms
most adolescents and young adults may already play in developing attachment relationships. Cog-
have learned to love, most still have a great deal to nition and inference are also important mecha-
learn about how to love. Thus Bowlby's emphasis nisms overlooked by traditional learning para-
on attachment in infancy and early childhood is en- digms. We have described the self-other distinc-
tirely consistent with the view that relationship ex- tion as a developmental milestone that emerges
perience and commitment in adolescence and from sensorimotor understanding of the mother as
adulthood can influence cognitive models bearing an object. The mechanism is "informal inference,"
on the conduct of adult attachment relationships a process that probably plays a role in a wide range
and parenting. There may even be modes of at- of developmental changes. The sense of security
tachment that are encountered only in some cul-

25
LEARNING TO LOVE

that arises within a well-functioning pattern of se- Ainsworth, M. (1972). The development of infant-
cure base behavior also arises initially as an mother attachment. In B. Caldwell & H. Ric-
"informal inference" about parental availability ciutti (Eds.), Review of child development re-
and responsiveness. Cognitive/emo-tional com- search (Vol. 3). Chicago: University of Chi-
mitment and identification arise from behavioral cago Press.
commitment and meshing by the same process of
"informal inference." Such effects on identification Ainsworth, M., Bell, S., & Stayton, D. (1971). In-
and socialization implicate attachment relation- dividual differences in strange situation be-
ships in a wide range of sociali-zation outcomes. havior of one-year-olds. In H. R. Schaffer
As a consequence of self-observation and informal (Ed.), The origins of human social relations.
inference, the child takes its own behavior and atti- London: Academic Press.
tudes as empirical evidence about itself and for- Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S.
malizes these as postulates of a self theory. A l - (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ:
though we have dis-cussed specific mechanisms in Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
association with specific developmental mile-
stones, it should be understood that most of them Anderson J. W. (1972). Attachment behaviour out
operate throughout development. The same must of doors. In N. Blurton-Jones (Ed.), Ethologi-
be said of parental influence on attachment behav- cal studies of child behavior. (pp. 199-215).
ior. Secure base behavior is not simply elicited by Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pp.
early care, parental behavior provides a matrix that 199-215.
continues to help organize it far beyond infancy. Bell, S. (1970). The development of the concept
The combination of multiple mechanisms acting of the object as related to infant-mother at-
con-currently and continuing parental support pro- tachment. Child Development, 41, 291-311.
vide an element of redundancy and consistency
that accounts in part for the virtual inevitability of Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child's tie to
attachment within what Bowlby called our envi- its mother. International Journal of Psycho-
ronment of evolutionary adaptedness. Analysis, 39, 350-373.
Neither the evolution of the self theory nor the Bowlby, J. (1960). Grief and mourning in infancy
ability to establish and maintain love relationships and early childhood. Psychoanalytic Study of
reaches a decisive conclusion in childhood or even the Child, 15, 9-52.
in adolescence. Even if early experience proves Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: (Vol. 1),
critical for learning to love, learning how to love Attachment. New York: Basic Books.
takes a very long time. Some of the mechanisms
involved are specific to attachment processes; oth- Cairns, R. (1972). Attachment and dependency: A
ers are nonspecific and contribute to develop in psychobiological and social learning synthe-
other domains as well. Thus, describing either at- sis. In J. Gewirtz (Ed.) Attachment and de-
tachment or the self theory as antecedent to the pendency. Washington, D.C.: Winston.
other oversimplifies a complex sequence of devel-
Cairns, R. (1979). The origins and plasticity of in-
opmental interactions. Throughout develop-ment,
terchanges. San Francisco: Freeman.
attachment relationships directly and indi-rectly
generate a vast array of self-relevant data. Even if Epstein, S. (1973). The self-concept revisited: A
attachment relationships within the family prove to theory of a theory. American Psychologist,
be the royal road to supportive adult-adult relation- 28, 404-416.
ships and caring parental bonds, this is only one of
Epstein, S. (1980). Self-concept: A review and the
many roads that must be traveled on the way to a
proposal of an integrated theory of personal-
well-defined, well-functioning self theory. What
ity. In E. Staub (Ed.), Personality: Basic is-
we can say for sure is that along this road we find
sues and current research. Englewood Cliffs,
many important clues.
NJ: Prentice-Hall.
REFERENCES Freud, S. (1949/1953). Abriss der psychoanalyse
(An outline of psychoanalysis). Frankfurt am
Ainsworth, M. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant Main: Fischer Bucherei.
care and the growth of love. Baltimore: Johns
Hopkins University Press.

26
WATERS ET AL.

Table 4
Supervision Partnership From 6-12 Years:
Preliminary Q-set Items

1. Returns home at a predictable time.


2. Starts playing somewhere or with some group and ends up elsewhere in
other activities or with other children without returning home.
3. Comforts parent when upset.
4. Seeks parent when injured.
5. Informs parent of injuries.
6. Screams at parent in anger. (-)
7. Can help parent supervise younger siblings.
8. Helps parent by keeping certain areas of the home or certain sets of toys
or clothes tidy.
9. Open with parent about social worries when they occur.
10. Asks parent for information about physical maturation.
11. Reports brushes with danger and near accidents to parent.
12. Accepts restrictions related to safety.
13. Willing to stay within reach of parent's supervision. (informs parent, agrees
to report change of activities, follows time restrictions, etc.)
14. Enjoys having parent scratch back or talk before going to bed.
15. Seems confident at home with babysitters at night.
16. Stays angry at parent for a long time. (-)
18. Is cold and indifferent to parent. (-)
19. Shows an interest in parent's work/chores.
20. Uses home as a main base for play activities.
21. Feels like parents are always in the way. (-)
22. Likes to go places with the parent.
(Prefers to go off on own activities)
23. Wants to be left alone when in a bad mood. (-)
24. Cuddly when tired.
25. Proud of parent.

27
LEARNING TO LOVE

Table 6 (continued)
26. Interested in parent approval when achieving something new.
27. Confides in parent when upset with or put upon by others.
28. Gets angry when disciplined. (-)
29. Rarely spends as much as an hour at a time helping parent. (-)
30. Glad to get something for parent even if parent could get it just as easily.
31. Thinks that childcare is parent's absolute top priority.
32. Pulls away if parent is affectionate. (-)
33. Says things to hurt parent. (-)
34. Coercive: Bribes (-)
35. Coercive: Threatens to misbehave. (-)
36. Coercive: Threatens to hurt self / get hurt doing risky things. (-)
37. Often goes off for over two hours without telling parent. (-)
38. Vague or evasive about where or with whom he is playing. (-)
39. Requires close supervision. (-)
40. Comes to parent for help when in trouble.
41. Accepts that parent is right when disciplined.
42. Fear of doing something is reduced if parent says it is safe or ok.
43. Lies to parent. (-)
44. Takes whatever parent offers - but not really appreciative. (-)
45. Accepts that cooperation or service to parent is reasonable pay back for
help parent has provided in the past.
46. Expects parent knows answers to most questions.
47. Willing to profit from parent's experience in matters of risk and safely.
48. Willing to profit from parent's experience in other areas.
49. Resents restrictions imposed so that parent can get own work done. (-)
50. Tells parent about funny experiences or observations.
51. Jokes with parent.
52. Hides bad experiences from parent for fear of being blamed. (-)
53. Hides mistakes and accidents from parent for fear of being blamed. (-)
54. Is clinging and immature with parent. (-)
55. Reminds parent or retells stories of good times had with parent.

28
WATERS ET AL.

Gewirtz, J. (1972). Attachment and dependence Robertson, J., & Robertson, J. (1967). Film: Young
and a distinction in terms of stimulus control. children in brief separation. No. 1: Kate aged
In J. Gewirtz (Ed.), Attachment and depend- 2 years 5 months, in foster care for 27 days.
ency. Washington: Winston. (16 mm). New York: New York University
Hinde, R. A., & Simpson, M. (1975). Qualities of Film Library.
infant-mother relationships in rhesus mon- Rotter, J. (1954). Social learning and clinical psy-
keys. In Parent-Infant Interaction. CIBA chology. Englewood-Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
Foundation Symposium, No. 33. Hall.
Kaye, K. (1982). The mental and social life of ba- Roy, M. A. (Ed.) (1980). Species identity and at-
bies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. tachment: A phylogenetic evaluation. N.Y.:
Maccoby, E., & Masters, J. (1970). Attachment Garland STPM Press.
and dependency. In P. Mussen (Ed.) Carmi- Sroufe, L.A. (in press). An organizational per-
chael's manual of child psychology. New spective on the self. In D. Cicchetti & M.
York: Wiley. Beeghley (Eds.), The self in transition : In-
Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (in press). Interview fancy to childhood. Chicago: University of
based adult attachment classifications: Related Chicago Press.
to infant-mother and infant-father attachment. Sroufe, L.A., Egeland, B., & Kreutzer, T. (in
Developmental Psychology. press). The fate of early experience following
Marvin, R. (1977). An ethological-cognitive developmental change: Longitudinal ap-
model for the attenuation of mother- child proaches to individual adaptation in child-
attachment behavior. In T. Alloway, L. hood.
Krames, & P. Pliner (Eds.), Advances in the Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual
study of communication and affect (Vol. 3). selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selec-
The development of social attachment. New tion and the descent of man 1871-1971 (pp.
York: Plenum. 136 179). Chicago: Aldine.
Parkes, C., & Stevenson-Hinde, J.( Eds.) (1982). Trivers, R. (1974). Parent-offspring conflict.
The place of attachment in human behavior. American Zoologist, 14, 249-264.
New York: Basic Books.
Trivers, R. (1985). Social evolution. Menlo Park,
Piaget, J. (1936/1952). The origins of intelligence CA: Benjamin/Cummings.
in children. New York: Norton.
Valenzuela, M., & Waters, E. (in preparation,
Piaget, J. (1937/1962). Play, dreams and imitation 1989). Use of the attachment Q-set to validate
in childhood. New York: Norton. the Strange Situation in a low SES Chilean
Richters, J., & Waters, E. (in press). Attachment population.
and socialization: The positive side of social Vaughn, B., & Waters, E. (submitted). Attachment
influence. In S. Feinman & M. Lewis (Eds.), behavior at home and in the laboratory: Q-
Social influences and behavior. New York: Sort observations and Strange Situation classi-
Plenum. fications of one-year-olds.
Ricks, M. (1985). The social transmission of pa- Waters, E. (1981). Traits, behavioral systems, and
rental behavior: Attachment across genera- relationships: Three models of infant-mother
tions. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), attachment. In K. Immelman, G. Barlow, L.
Growing points in attachment theory and re- Petrinovitch, & M. Main, (Eds.), Behavioral
search. Monographs of the Society for Re- Development: The Bielefeld Interdisciplinary
search in Child Development, 50 (Serial No. Project (pp. 621-650). Cambridge, England:
209), 211-227. Cambridge University Press.
Robertson, J. (1952). Film: A two-year-old goes to Waters, E., & Deane, K. (1985). Defining and as-
hospital. (16mm). New York: New York sessing individual differences in attachment
University Film Library. relationships: Q-methodology and the organi-

29
LEARNING TO LOVE

zation of behavior in infancy and early child-


hood. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.),
Growing points in attachment theory and re-
search. Monographs of the Society for Re-
search in Child Development, 50 (Serial No.
209), 41-65.
Waters, E., & Kondo-Ikemura, K. (submitted).
Maternal behavior and infant security 1n Old
World monkeys: Conceptual issues and a
methodological bridge between human and
non-human primate research.
Winnicott, D. (1965). The maturational processes
and the facilitating environment. London:
Hogarth Press.
Woodson, R. (1983). Newborn behavior and the
transition to extrauterine life. Infant Behav-
ior and Development, 6, 139-144.
Woodson, R., Reader, J. S., & Chamberline G.
(1981). Blood pH and crying in the new-
born infant. Infant Behavior and Develop-
ment, 4, 41-45.

30
WATERS ET AL.

31

You might also like