You are on page 1of 11

Ecological Psychology

Some Recent and Prospective Developments

ALLAN W. WICKER Claremont Graduate School

ABSTRACT: Building on the pioneering work of prelude to .the discussion, I briefly sketch several
Barker and Wright, ecological psychologists have major contributions of Barker and his colleagues
demonstrated the relevance of behavior-setting theory on which the present work builds.
and research to issues in organizational, community,
social, and environmental psychology. They have used
a broad range of research strategies and have shown Early Work in Ecological Psychology
increased interest in applied problems. Recent and
BEHAVIOR SETTINGS
prospective developments in ecological psychology in-
clude streamlined descriptive surveys of community
The basic environmental unit in ecological psy-
and organization behavior settings; the study of. inter-
chology is the behavior setting. It may be helpful
setting linkages and of the life cycles of settings;
applications of manning theory and research on stress to think of behavior settings as miniature social
to work settings, particularly to settings that serve the systems whose basic function is to carry out set-
public; extensions of manning theory by drawing on ting programs. For example, a public lecture is a
the literature on small-group performance and job behavior setting. Its program includes the chair-
enrichment; and the development of a technology for person's introducing the speaker, the speaker's
.improving human environments. delivering his or her talk, and the audience's sit-
ting quietly (or laughing and applauding, or what-
Over the past three decades, ecological psychology ever the case may be). To facilitate the pro-
has grown from a seminal idea into a recognized gram, behaviors of people and arrangements of
branch of our discipline.1 Largely through the physical objects within the boundaries of the set-
efforts of Roger Barker, Herbert Wright, and a ting are highly coordinated. The members of the
few close colleagues (e.g., Barker, 1965, 1968;
Barker & Associates, 1978; Barker & Gump, 1964;
Barker & Schoggen, 1973; Barker & Wright,
19SS; Wright, 1967), many psychologists have This article is an adaptation and extension of an in-
vited address presented at the meeting of the Western
come to appreciate the importance of (a) study- Psychological Association, San Francisco, April 21, 1978.
ing human behavior and experience in their natural I am indebted to Joseph McGrath and Daniel Stokols
contexts and (b) analyzing human environments for helpful comments on an earlier draft of the manu-
script.
at a level more molar than that of the stimulus. Requests for reprints should be sent to Allan W.
Many social scientists have viewed ecological Wicker, Faculty in Psychology, Claremont Graduate School,
psychology as an isolated and somewhat esoteric Claremont, California 91711.
1
Readers should be aware that ecological psychology
area of study, with its own terminology, methods, and environmental psychology are not simply different
and concepts. However valid such a view may labels for the same subarea of psychology. The concepts,
have been in the past, it is no longer appropriate. theories, and findings of ecological psychology summarized
in this article represent one of several bodies of research
Contemporary developments in ecological psy- generally considered within the larger domain of en-
chology suggest close conceptual ties with other vironmental psychology. Among the other concerns of
areas of psychology and use a broad range of environmental psychology are human spatial behavior,
territoriality, privacy, crowding, environmental percep-
research methods (including laboratory experi- tion, and ecologically responsible behavior. The scope of
ments)/ They also show a concern with direct, environmental psychology can be seen in recent over-
practical applications of research. I want to dis- views of the field (e.g., Craik, 1973, 1977; Proshansky &
Altman, 1979; Stokols, 1978), textbooks (e.g., Bell, Fisher,
cuss some of these recent developments and to sug- & Loomis, 1978), and books of readings (e.g., Proshan-
gest promising areas for future research. As a sky, Ittelson, & Rivlin, 1976; Stokols, 1977).

Vol. 34, No. 9, 7SS-76S AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • SEPTEMBER 1979 • 755


Copyright 1979 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
0003-066X/79/3409-0755$00.75
audience stop conversing when the chairperson merous other facets of the communities, including
approaches the lectern. All of the seats in the their child-rearing systems, degree of local con-
lecture hall have been placed facing the front so trol over public settings, quality of life for the
that those in attendance can see and hear the elderly, and leadership patterns, were also induced
speaker. The boundaries of a setting are both by Barker and Schoggen (1973) from such be-
physical (the walls of the lecture hall) and tem- havior-setting surveys.
poral (the scheduled hour and day of the lecture).
Barker views behavior settings as self-regulating THE THEORY OF UNDERMANNING
systems whose inhabitants will take the steps
necessary to preserve their programs. For ex- From their earliest observations in Midwest,
ample, if some essential component of a setting Barker and his colleagues were struck by the ex-
(whether a person or an inanimate object) is miss- tensive participation of residents—including ado-
ing, occupants of the setting will seek it out and lescents and children—in community behavior set-
bring it in. And if a component isn't functioning tings. The participation was so extensive, in fact,
properly (that is, in accordance with the program that citizens were thinly spread over numerous,
of the setting), it will be either corrected or re- diverse settings. They appeared to be overcom-
moved. For example, if there were no chairs in mitted. Barker's attempt to explain the claim of
a lecture hall, some would be carried in or some the town's settings on residents' time and efforts
other adjustment would be made. If a member led to his theory of undermanning 2 (Barker, 1960,
of the audience began to talk so loudly that others 1968; Barker & Schoggen, 1973). The theory
could not hear, he or she would probably be asked builds on the idea that behavior settings counter-
to quiet down or to leave. act threats to their programs and that one fre-
Much of the empirical work on behavior settings quent specific threat is the lack of enough people
conducted by Barker and his colleagues has been to carry out a setting program in a smooth and
devoted to identifying and describing the publicly proper manner.
available behavior settings in two small towns: Os- Settings may be undermanned (that is, have too
kaloosa, Kansas (population 800, called Midwest) few active participants to carry out the setting's
and Leyburn, England (population 1,300, called program properly) or optimally manned (that is,
Yoredale). Literally hundreds of behavior settings have the right number of people to handle the
occurred in each town during the one-year survey setting's program). An example of an under-
periods. Among the settings identified were shoe manned setting would be a crowded restaurant
repair shops, Bible study classes, chess club meet- with fewer waitresses than needed. (Below, I
ings, banks, court sessions, and high school bas- discuss overmanned settings, and the fact that the
ketball practices. Each setting in the survey was staff and clientele of a setting generally have
described in numerous ways, including how long complementary manning levels.)
it lasted, the age and sex of the participants, According to Barker, any undermanned setting
and the extent to which the setting program in- generates forces that act on its occupants to keep
volved such processes as thinking, talking, and the setting functioning. The occupants sense that
showing emotion (Barker & Schoggen, 1973; the setting's program is threatened, and they re-
Barker & Wright, 1955). spond by acting more vigorously, by taking up a
These community surveys revealed much about wider range of tasks, and by prodding others to
life in the two towns. To illustrate, survey data do the same. Participants in undermanned set-
documented restrictions on females' participation tings have to deal with tasks that are more im-
in the. public settings of both Midwest and Yore- portant to the setting than do people in optimally
dale and confirmed some of the biases to which manned settings. If a person's contribution to an
feminists have objected. Females spent less time undermanned setting is inadequate, he or she re-
in the public settings of their communities than ceives corrective inputs from others present but
did males, yet females were overrepresented in set-
tings controlled by churches and schools and in
settings where talking and expressing emotion were 2
The term manning theory is not intended to connote
prominent activities. They were underrepresented the masculine gender; it applies to both sexes. I use
the term here to keep terminology consistent with that
in business and governmental settings and in set- used in Barker's work. Staffing theory is perhaps a better
tings where physical activity was prominent. Nu- term.

756 • AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • SEPTEMBER 1979


is not usually rejected. Only as a last resort is behavior settings of a community are awesome.
a poor performer asked to leave an undermanned Barker's survey procedures required 13 data
setting. These events lead participants in under- sheets, approximately 230 data entries, for each
manned settings to feel that they serve in impor- behavior setting identified—and there were more
tant and valued roles, that is, to feel more in- than 800 settings in Midwest in a single year.
volved. Each descriptive dimension was applied to each
The first attempt to study the theory of man- setting. The processing of these data was further
ning was conducted in several Kansas high schools complicated by the fact that most of the descrip-
(Barker & Gump, 1964). The research indicated tors were not linked to any explicit theoretical
that students from small schools, whose behavior framework. Most of the survey findings were
settings were presumably undermanned, had many left to stand alone or were used for simple com-
more positions of responsibility in voluntary school parisons, such as contrasts between Midwest and
activities such as football games and band concerts Yoredale (Barker & Schoggen, 1973). /
than did students from a larger school, whose set- Ecological psychologists are currently sensitive
tings were presumably optimally manned. More- to the problem of accumulating excessive amounts
over, students fr,om the small schools—even mar- of data with no theoretical underpinnings. Sur-
ginal students—reported more pressures from peers veys of behavior settings have been streamlined
to participate and more feelings of involvement in several ways. In some instances survey periods
than did students from the large school. These have been shortened from one year to a few
and related differences associated with organization months or weeks. The physical areas surveyed
size have generally been confirmed by subsequent have been confined to selected portions of a com-
research in schools (Baird, 1969; Wicker, 1968, munity or an institution. Moreover, practical and
1969a; Willems, 1967) and in churches (Wicker, theoretical criteria have guided the selection of
1969b; Wicker & Kauma, 1974; Wicker & Mehler, descriptors to be applied to settings. To illus-
1971). trate, LeCompte and Willems surveyed only the
treatment sector of a large rehabilitation hospital.
They described each setting in terms of such prac-
Extensions of the Early Work tical concerns as the types of treatment activities
-\ and the amounts of time spent in them by various
DESCRIPTION OF BEHAVIOR SETTINGS
staff groups (Le Compte, 1972a, 1972b; LeCompte
Barker has argued the value of descriptive studies & Willems, 1970; Willems, 1976).
of human environments and behaviors primarily oh Representative surveys of entire communities
scientific grounds: the need for ecologically valid can be conducted more efficiently with representa-
base-rate data. Records that describe conditions tive samples than with exhaustive ones. Price
existing in people's everyday lives can, for ex- and Blashfield (197S) shewed that many of Bark-
ample, allow researchers to make comparisons of er's descriptors could be reduced to a smaller
psychologically relevant events in different cul- number of dimensions. Their factor analysts of
tures and in different historical periods. They Barker's data on a small Kansas town reduced 43
can provide clues to the processes of acculturation descriptors to nine interpretable factors that ac-
and personality formation and can be helpful in counted for 62% of the variance. They also did a
formulating theories that would not be developed cluster analysis on the same data, which yielded
from laboratory experiments (Barker, 1965; several clusters of behavior settings that had simi-
Barker & Associates, 1978; Barker & Wright lar descriptive profiles. Future researchers might
1949).8 use the smaller number of descriptors Price and
Aside from the Barker group's own work, how- Blashfield identified, and they might sample set-
ever, very few surveys of behavior settings have tings from the clusters rather than include the
been conducted on entire communities. Perhaps entire population of settings in a community.
psychologists have not been convinced of the sci- It seems clear that descriptive studies of the
entific benefits of such endeavors. Or perhaps behavior settings of communities and organizations
potential researchers have lacked the requisite
resources (knowledge, skills, time, money) for the
work, The levels of time and effort required to 8
Lazarus (Note 1) recently expressed a similar view-
identify, and particularly to describe, all of the point in the context! of stress and coping research.

AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST * SEPTEMBER 1979 • 757


can serve both scientific and practical purposes. • Shared clienteles: Which settings serve the
In the future, surveys of behavior settings may be same clients, and how extensive is the overlap?
more application oriented. Their potential uses • Economic dependencies: Which settings depend
include assessing the social impact of governmental on other settings for their financial resources?
agencies or other types of institutions on a com- • Flow of information: Which settings are linked
munity, analyzing organizations intensively, and by formal and/or informal channels of communi-
evaluating designed environments. Examples of cation to other settings?
such uses are available in the literature. The • Flow of goods and services: Which settings
behavioral opportunities provided by the churches provide goods or services to other settings?
of Midwest and Yoredale and by the County Ag- (Miller's, 1972, analysis of the information-pro-
ricultural Extension Service in Midwest have been cessing and matter-energy-processing subsystems
reported (Barker, Barker, & Ragle, 1967; Ragle, in organizations might be a helpful source for re-
Barker, & Johnson, 1967). As noted above, Le- searchers interested in the last two types of links.)
Compte and Willems have analyzed the treatment The relationships of behavior settings to the
environments of a rehabilitation hospital (Le- formal organizations of which they are a part
Compte, 1972(a, 1972b; LeCompte & Willems, should also be examined. Supersystems generally
1970; Willems, 1976), and Bechtel (1977) has constrain the behavior variance of their component
described an evaluation of a public housing project. systems (Katz & Kahn, 1978, p. 63). Aspects of
(These and other uses have been further discussed larger systems that may constrain behavior set-
by Wicker, 1979.) tings include control systems; management-defined
I would like to suggest two additional ap- missions; resource limitations, such as lack of
proaches to describing community and organiza- capital or technology; and division of labor, that
tional settings that could have both scientific and is, the assignments of organizational tasks to dif-
practical value. According to the ecological view- ferent branches and departments (cf. Blau, 1974).
point, behavior settings are components of larger, To give but one example, as a result of a corpo-
more complex social systems such as communities rate board's decision to discontinue one product
and institutions. But the various ways in which line and introduce another, some behavior settings
behavior settings are linked to one another and at a much lower organizational level will be al-
to such supersystems remain largely unexplored. tered, some will be eliminated entirely, and some
In their descriptive surveys, Barker and his col- new ones will be created.
leagues have usually considered behavior settings A second, related way that ecological psycholo-
as firmly bounded, discrete units, and they have gists could contribute to the understanding of
attended primarily to the internal characteristics communities and institutions is by studing the life
of settings. An exception is the classification of cycles of important behavior settings within them
behavior settings according to the authority sys- —the circumstances that bring settings into be-
tems (schools, churches, governments, business, or ing, the ways settings adapt to changing external
voluntary associations) that control them (Barker, conditions, and the factors that contribute to their
1968; Barker & Schoggen, 1973). eventual demise.4
Oth6r kinds of connections among community Unfortunately, existing methods for surveying
and organization behavior settings should also be behavior settings are not appropriate for studying
examined. Here are some links that researchers the development of individual settings. The stan-
might consider: dard surveys are cross-sectional; that is, they
• Interpersonal links: To what extent are occu- identify all of the settings that occur within spe-
cified time-and-space boundaries without consid-
pants (leaders, workers, clients) of a given setting
ering how long the settings have existed or whether
acquainted with occupants of other settings? (See
they are in a transitional stage. A variety of
Sarason, Carroll, Maton, Cohen, & Lorentz, 1977, other techniques, including some from related so-
for an introduction to the literature on social net- cial science disciplines, will be required to under-
works.) stand the life cycles of behavior settings. Such
• Shared leadership: Which settings share lead-
ers, and how extensive is the overlap? (See Bind-
ing, 1970, for an analysis of shared leadership in *Stokols (1978, in press) also recently suggested this
Midwest's behavior settings.) line of inquiry.

758 • AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • SEPTEMBER 1979


procedures as content analysis, participant ob- be traced by interviewing local government offi-
servation, in-depth interviewing, and historical cials, realtors, developers, and other entrepreneurs.
and archival analysis might be used, either alone Important data on the origins of some mature
or in combination with more traditional survey behavior settings may be obtained from such
and systematic observational methods (cf. Bou- governmental and organizational records as appli-
chard, 1976; Schatzman & Strauss, 1973; Camp- cations for building permits, environmental im-
bell, Note 2). pact reports, requests for zone changes, and min-
Such methods may be applied to a variety of utes of hearings and meetings. Local newspapers
provocative issues. For example, how are be- and organizational memos might be content ana-
havior settings "born"? Are there periods when lyzed. Retrospective reports could be obtained
settings are especially vulnerable? Is there any from persons who played important roles in the
way to detect "old age" or "senility" in a behavior creation of settings. An analysis of local com-
setting? Do setting programs become so elabo- munity politics will undoubtedly be essential for
rated over time that the setting's regulatory mech- an adequate understanding of the birth and growth
anisms respond slowly or not at all? And how is of some behavior settings.
the fate of behavior settings influenced by such New concepts and methods may also be required
factors as new technologies, changes in the econ- to understand some of the important ways in
omy, unsatisfied wants and needs of people in which behavior settings adapt to changing ex-
the system, changing laws and social customs, and ternal conditions. An early conceptual problem
rapid shifts in clientele? will be to develop a working definition of the
An idea of how research might proceed can be meaningful environment of behavior settings. Re-
gotten from considering the question of how be- sidual definitions, such as "external factors im-
havior settings come into being. The earliest pinging on the setting," are not useful, for they
stages in the birth of some behavior settings may provide no guidelines for measurement. An ade-
be the unfilled needs or wants of one or a few quate working definition must take into account
members of an organization or a community. The links among behavior settings as well as relation-
people who are unsatisfied, or others who see their ships between settings and their supersystems.
need, may first conceive of and then act to create The links that affect the fate of behavior settings
a fulfilling setting. For example, citizens of a must be identified from among the much larger
small town may see the need for local medical number of links that undoubtedly exist. It may
care and seek to establish a clinic, or parents who also be useful to distinguish varying degrees of
believe their children should have music training dependencies among settings. For example, a de-
may attempt to have music added to the curricu- cision to close a factory would have immediate
lum of local schools. Information on such nascent and final consequences for the work behavior set-
behavior settings might be obtained from surveys tings in the plant. A tavern across the street
of community residents and from interviews with from the factory might be seriously but less im-
community leaders. (See Sarason, 1972, and Sara- mediately affected. Community businesses and
son, Zitnay, & Grossman, 1971, for a discussion voluntary organizations might also experience im-
of how one group planned a service institution.) portant but less drastic consequences.
Not all behavior settings arise from grass-roots Further elaboration of these ideas and the de-
needs, however. Settings may also be created by velopment of new methods and new combinations
actions of persons or agencies outside a community of methods for studying them may provide us
or organization. To illustrate, real estate specula- with tools for better understanding communities
and organizations as dynamic systems.
tors may build and promote shopping centers,
motel chains may locate facilities near freeway
exits to accommodate travelers rather than the EXTENSIONS OF MANNING THEORY

local populace, legislative committees may schedule Several years ago, Joseph McGrath and I sug-
hearings at convenient field locations. The con- gested some amendments to Barker's theory of
ception of such externally imposed behavior set- undermanning (Wicker, McGrath, & Armstrong,
tings might usefully be studied by participant ob- 1972). We proposed that level of manning be
servers in boardrooms and administrative offices. regarded as a continuum with three critical re-
Subsequent development of these settings could gions—undermanning, adequate (or optimal) man-

AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • SEPTEMBER 1979 • 7S9


ning, and overmanning—and we specified how tween manning levels and involvement with a
each level should be denned. The conceptual group task (Petty & Wicker, 1974; Wicker, Kir-
separation of overmanning from adequate or op- meyer, Hanson, & Alexander, 1976; Hanson &
timal manning was a distinction not clearly made Wicker, Note 3). (People in overmanned condi-
by Barker. We also suggested that manning tions report less involvement than people in un-
levels should be determined for individual be- dermanned conditions.) It also appears from our
havior settings in organizations, and that if level work that manning levels mediate some effects
of manning is to be calculated for entire organiza- previously attributed to differences in group size.
tions, it should be based on summations of data That is, we find differences in experiences that
from individual settings. (In the early research are supposed to result from differences in group
on schools and churches [e.g., Barker & Gump, size under conditions where size is constant but
1964; Wicker, 1969b; Willems, 1967], organiza- manning levels differ. Another provocative finding
tion manning levels were inferred from two sta- from these experiments is that manning levels
tistics: number of members and number of be- have carry-over effects on people's judgments
havior settings in an organization.) We further about the number of people required for a task.
suggested that manning should be calculated sepa- For example, groups of research participants who
rately for (a) people who have responsible posi- were shorthanded oil a group task judged a sub-
tions in a setting (workers or staff) and (b) those sequent task to need fewer people than did groups
who are merely members, clients, or onlookers. of participants who had more than enough help
For example, a crowded physician's office would be on the prior task. It appears that people's adap-
undermanned at the staff level and overmanned at tation levels (Helson, 1964) for personnel require-
the patient level. ments shift as a function of their exposure to dif-
The example of a physician's office also illus- ferent levels of manning. Perhaps this finding
trates my more recent interest in certain behavior reflects the pervasiveness of Parkinson's law: Work
settings in service institutions: settings in which expands to fit the time (and personnel) available
entrepreneurs or paid employees (staff) provide for its completion (Parkinson, 19S7).
a service to customers, patients, or clients. Such We have also applied the extended manning
settings can be under-, adequately, or overstaffed, theory to behavior settings in service institutions,
and they can be under-, adequately, or overpopu- specifically to work stations of Yosemite National
lated with clients. Their staffing levels and client Park rangers (Wicker, 1979; Wicker & Kirmeyer,
population levels tend to be in a complementary 1976). In some analyses we used the number
relation: A setting overpopulated with clients is, of people entering the park on a given day as our
by definition, understaffed; an underpopulated set- index of staff load (and presumably of manning
ting often is overstaffed. level). We correlated this (objective) load mea-
My work on service settings has been influenced sure with rangers' perceptions of their work loads,
by the literature on stress and sensory overload, their strategies for coping with departures from
as well as by manning theory. I have assumed adequate manning, and their job involvement on
that departures from adequate staffing and ade- specific work days. In general, heavier work loads
quate client population can be stressful for both were associated with more coping strategies being
types of setting occupants, and I have used Mc- considered and enacted and also with greater feel-
Grath's model of social psychological stress (Mc- ings of challenge and being needed on the job.
Grath, 1976) to analyze their coping strategies. However, the positive relationship between work
The literature on sensory overload (e.g., Milgram, load and quality of work experience declined over
1970) was the source of predictions about such the course of the summer. By the end of the
residual consequences of chronic understaffing as season, days when work loads were heavy were
staff members' tendencies to become cynical I about characterized more by feelings of physical and
their work and their tendencies to apply negative emotional fatigue than by job involvement.
stereotypes to clients (Wicker, 1979; Wicker & These and other findings, as well as 6ur work-
Kirmeyer, 1976). ing model of service behavior settings (Wicker,
Several colleagues and I have conducted labora- 1979; Wicker & Kirmeyer, 1976), suggest that
tory experiments on these recent conceptual ex- staff members may suffer negative residual conse-
tensions of manning theory. These studies have quences from having to cope with inadequate
confirmed the expected inverse relationship be- staffing levels. Promising research along these

760 • AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • SEPTEMBER 1979


lines has recently been undertaken by ecological (1) settings for which both tasks and matching are
and social psychologists. Kirmeyer (1978) found specified (many behavior settings in industry
that the spacing of client demands over the work would fall ift this category), (2) settings for which
day—that is, the patterning of busy and slack tasks are specified but matching is not (certain
periods—is a better predictor of the quality of behavior settings under the aegis of national vol-
staff members' work experiences than is the total untary organizations such as the Red Cross and
number of demands. Longer term consequences of some churches would be included here), and (3)
understaffirtg and other stressful conditions ex- settings for which neither tasks nor person-to-task
perienced by human-service providers are being matches are specified (activities of small busi-
explored in Maslach's program of research on nesses and local voluntary organizations are ex-
burnout, the emotional disengagement of staff from amples, cf. Steiner, 1972, chap. 3). (More refined
clients and their problems (Maslach, 19 7 6, Note 4). conceptions of the division of labor and matching
Further extensions of manning theory might in behavior settings might take into account de-
draw on research in two other areas of psycho- grees of specification rather than using dichoto-
logical inquiry: small-group performance and job mies.)
enrichment. Most behavior-setting programs are Steiner's discussion of group composition calls
carried out by small, task-oriented groups of peo- attention to a class of variables that has been
ple in face-to-face interaction. But with few neglected by ecological psychologists: the personal
exceptions (e.g., McGrath, 1976; Petty & Wicker, characteristics of individuals. The knowledge j
1974; Steiner, 1972; Weick, 1970; Wicker et al., skills, and social and personal dispositions of
1976), the ecological and small-group literature occupants of behavior settings affect how well,
have not been linked. The potential benefits of and in some cases whether, settings function. The
association are substantial, and they would flow particular occupant characteristics that are im-
both ways. The longitudinal study of people car- portant may, of coqrse, vary from one setting to
rying out behavior-setting programs and coping another. Settings also vary in the degree to
with such threats as understaffing can be used to which it is desirable for occupants to be similar
supplement, if not to supplant, the current domi- on relevant characteristics. The division of labor
nant pattern of investigations using closed, ade- in a particular setting might, for example, make
quately manned, ad hoc groups of college students it desirable for workers to have diverse technical
working for part of an hour on intellectual prob- abilities yet to be similar in sociability.
lems assigned by researchers. The above derivations from the literature on
Below, I sketch a few points of convergence group performance are similar to points that can
between the developing concerns of ecological psy- be drawn from the literature on job enrichment.
chology and the literature on group performance. Many job-enrichment programs attempt to reduce
The examples are drawn from Steiner (1972); the division of labor in work settings so that work-
further examples could be developed from other ers use a wider variety of skills to perform a
aspects of Steiner's work and from such sources larger portion of the whole task, that is, the be-
as Hackman (1976), Hackman and Morris havior-setting program. And several researchers
(1975), and McGrath and Altman (1966). have noted the importance of individual differences
The first point of convergence draws on Stein- in workers' reactions to enlarged jobs (e.g., Hack-
er's discussion of the ways groups organize to per- man, 1977; Hackman & Oldham, 1975; Wanous,
form divisible tasks. The programs of behavior 1977).
settings are, to varying degrees, the products of These considerations bear on manning theory.
organizing processes of setting occupants. The The severity of consequences of undermanning
occupants must determine whether they are free and overmanning of workers should be directly
to divide the setting program into distinct tasks related to the degree to which tasks, and also
(job assignments) or whether the tasks to be done matching, are specified. If jobs or assignments
have already been specified or constrained in im- are rigidly specified, the absence of a key worker
portant ways. The occupants must also deter- can incapacitate a behavior setting. But in set-
mine whether they are allowed to match (assign) tings where tasks and matching are unspecified,
persons to tasks or whether the matching has been overmanning can be dealt with (at least to some
specified. Behavior settings can be placed in three extent) by dividing the program into smaller
categories based on these two organizing factors: pieces and giving more people something to do

AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • SEPTEMBER 1979 • 761


(cf. Wicker et al., 1976); undermanning (at least crease the satisfaction and well-being of the people
within some range) can be handled by combining (both staff and clients) who occupy them.
jobs. In fact, a reduction in the division of labor Our first attempts at behavior-setting technology
of behavior settings may be necessary if the hy- were designed as experiments in which we sought
pothesized psychological consequences of under- as much control as possible over extraneous events.
manning are to occur. For example, workers may We wanted to learn whether the specific interven-
feel involved and challenged when undermanning tions we made could account for certain changes
leads to restructured jobs, but they may not have in the experiences and behaviors of setting occu-
these experiences when undermanning merely re- pants. One such project was the introduction of
sults in more work to be done in the usual man- a queuing device (an arrangement of posts con-
ner (cf. Hackman & Oldham, 1975). nected by chains) to channel passengers onto buses
The quality of workers' experiences in behavior at a heavily used bus stop in Yosemite National
settings may also depend on their various sensitivi- Park (Wicker, 1979). The device was designed
ties to departures from adequate manning. Some to make the boarding process safer and more
workers may find moderate or brief levels of un- equitable. Another project sought to reduce per-
derstaffing to be stimulating and challenging, but ceived waiting time and stress among walk-in pa-
others may find the same condition aversive. Pre- tients at a hospital emergency facility. Signs
liminary research suggests that workers' reactions were posted to inform patients of the facility's
to overstaffing may be related to individual differ- priority system for processing patients (e.g., am-
ences in tolerance for waiting (Wicker et al., bulance arrivals were given, first priority), infor-
1976). The distribution of such sensitivities mation was posted to apprise patients of their
among setting occupants may affect how well the place in line, and silent movies were shown to
setting program is carried out. provide an entertaining distraction (Vieth, Note
Other implications can be drawn for the study 5). (Note that steps taken to improve setting
of the life cycles of behavior settings. When a functioning need not exploit staff or clients; these
setting is created, the decisions made regarding steps are designed to aid both staff and clients
its division of labor and method of matching per- in executing the program of the setting.)
sons to tasks presumably reflect current conditions In both experiments our interventions produced
(e.g., organization rules, union contracts, available some desirable outcomes. Passengers boarded
space and equipment, abilities of available per- faster and more equitably. Patients perceived
sonnel). When conditions change, are correspond- their waiting times to be shorter. But in retro-
ing adjustments made in the division of labor and spect, the experimental model may not be the
in personnel assignments, or is there a progressive best one for behavior-setting technologists to use.
drift toward ineffectiveness? The life expectancy Behavior settings are not static, and the require-
of a behavior setting may depend on the degree to ment that conditions be held constant throughout
which its occupants monitor changing conditions the course of an experiment cannot be met in
and on the degree to which they are free to adjust most settings. Moreover, rarely does the re-
work tasks and assignments. searcher/technologist know enough about a setting
to plan an intervention that is worth being con-
tinued without change throughout the course of
A Future Direction for Ecological an experiment. Very often something will happen
Psychology duripg an experiment to moderate the effect of
the treatment or to make the pretreatment period
Several of the trends discussed above depart from
incomparable with the post treatment period. For
Barker's description of the ecological psychologist
example, clerks may complain about conditions of
as a scientist who seeks to understand human
reduced lighting required for showing movies, and
behavior and environments by unobtrusively ob- the administration may decide that the lights must
serving events that he or she has not constrained in be returned to pretreatment levels.
any significant way (Barker, 1965, 1968). In this A more suitable approach for behavior-setting
section I propose an even more radical departure technologists is a "fine-tuning" procedure that re-
—that ecological psychologists develop a technol- sembles the formative evaluation-research model
ogy for deliberately intervening in behavior set- (Campbell, Note 2 ) . According to this model,
tings to improve setting functioning and to in- new social programs are continuously monitored

762 • AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • SEPTEMBER 1979


from their inception, and when it is obvious that obviously don't work can be dropped after a rea-
certain aspects of a program are unworkable or sonable trial period. Faltering adjustments can
ineffective, they are promptly revised or aban- be modified. When warranted, apparently success-
doned. Campbell noted that "many errors in ful innovations can be more rigorously evaluated
planning are visible to the naked eye" and do not by experimental procedures.
have to be confirmed by full-blown quantitative Some aspects of this fine-tuning approach are
experimentation. currently being applied by Donald Lemmex at a
Innovations in behavior settings could be treated venereal disease clinic located in a county health
in a similar fashion, as I discuss below. Before facility. He initially assessed1 the setting by ob-
any intervention is made, however, the recipient serving its operation, interviewing the staff, and
behavior setting must be carefully analyzed. It is seeking to learn the concerns and anxieties of
in this assessment phase that behavior-setting patients visiting the clinic. Among the modifica-
technologists are likely to make their most dis- tions he is introducing are partitions to screen
tinctive contributions. They will bring to the patients in the waiting room from the view of
task a sensitivity to both the structural and the passersby in the hall, signs that make it clear to
dynamic features of settings, as well as a respect people entering the setting that they are entering
for the intricate interdependence among the hu- the VD clinic, rearrangement of the furniture in
man and nonhuman setting components. Their the waiting room to accommodate couples and
tools will allow them quickly to establish the children, and a change in receptionist procedures
boundaries of the environments they are dealing (from asking patients "What are you here for?"
with and to focus on the heart of these environ- to asking '"Are you here for a VD test?"). It
ments: behavior-setting programs. They will also seems likely that some of these innovations will
understand that behavior settings afford a variety require further adjustments, and some may be
of satisfactions and dissatisfactions to setting occu- abandoned altogether, depending on how they seem
pants and that the perspectives of all types of to work during the period they are continuously
participants should be assessed. monitored.
In this assessment stage, behavior-setting tech- The main benefit from behavior-setting tech-
nologists would observe the setting in operation nology is likely to be improved conditions for
during its busy, normal, and slack periods; ob- people who occupy the settings where the tech-
tain the perspective of both staff and clients on nology is applied. But another kind of payoff is
the setting—its functions, any dysfunctions they possible. If an effective technology for improv-
see, and the satisfactions and dissatisfactions the ing a diversity of behavior settings does develop,
setting provides; and where possible, spend some it could provide insights that would advance
time actually participating in the various staff theory in ecological psychology (cf. Bass, 1974).
and client roles in the setting. The technologists These comments on ecological psychology's re-
would also scrutinize the required behaviors, physi- cent past and its near future reflect the percep-
cal facilities, and their arrangements, standard tions of only one participant. Other prescriptions
operating procedures, and workers' abilities, in for the field are available (e.g., Barker & Associ-
order to identify any instances of poor fit among ates, 1978, chap. 19; Stokols, 1978; Willems, 1974,
these aspects of the setting. (Some guidelines for 1977). However, I believe that most ecological
analyzing behavior settings, their programs, and psychologists recognize the value of developing
certain other setting features are given in Wicker, closer links with related branches of psychology
1979.) Additional inquiries should establish what
features must remain as they are.8
5
This broad initial assessment should suggest Behavior-setting techniques might also be combined
with the methodologies of environmental assessment as-
ways to improve the setting's functioning and the sociated with other paradigms of environmental research
outcomes for setting occupants. Only changes (e.g., environmental cognition, environmental attitudes,
that could feasibly be incorporated into the setting operant approaches, assessment of social climate). Such
combinations might prove more powerful than the con-
program on an enduring basis should be introduced cepts and methods associated with a single paradigm.
—behavior settings should not be whimsically Environmental interventions have been proposed and
tinkered with. All innovations should be consid- described by researchers from a variety of backgrounds
(e.g., Cone & Hayes, 1977; Craik & Zube, 1976; Hoiahan,
ered provisional and should be continuously moni- 1978; Ittelson, Pfoshansky, & Rivlin, 1970; Leff, 1978;
tored. Elements of an intervention package that Sommer, 1969, 1974; Steele, 1973).

AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • SEPTEMBER 1979 • 763


and with other social science disciplines. And they Binding, F. R. S. Behavior setting leadership in an Amer-
would surely agree that the benefits of closer as- ican community (Doctoral dissertation, University of
Kansas, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International,
sociation would flow in both directions. 1970, 31, 3034A. (University Microfilms No. 70-25,
304)
Blau, P. M. On the nature of organizations. New York:
REFERENCE NOTES Wiley, 1974.
Bouchard, T. J., Jr. Field research methods: Interview-
1. Lazarus, R. S. The stress and coping paradigm. In C. ing, questionnaires, participant observation, systematic
Eisdorfer, A. Kleinman, & D. Cohen (Chairs), The observation, unobtrusive measures. In M. D. Dunnette
critical evolution of behavioral paradigms for psychi- (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psy-
atric science. Conference sponsored by the University chology. Chicago: Rand McNally, 1976.
of Washington, Seattle; held in Gleneden Beach, Ore- Cone, J. D., & Hayes, S. C. Applied behavioral analysis
gon, November 1978. and the solution of environmental problems. In I. Alt-
2. Campbell, D. T. Qualitative knowing in action re- man & J. Wohlwill (Eds.), Human behavior and en-
search. Paper presented at the meeting of the American vironment: Advances in theory and research (Vol. 2).
Psychological Association, New Orleans, September 1974. New York: Plenum Press, 1977.
3. Hanson, L., & Wicker, A. W. Effects of overmanning Craik, K. H. Environmental psychology. In P. H. Mus-
on group experience and task performance. Paper pre- sen & M. R. Rosenzweig (Eds.), Annual review of
sented at the meeting of the Western Psychological psychology (Vol. 24). Palo Alto, Calif.: Annual Re-
Association, Anaheim, California, April 1973. views, 1973.
Craik, K. H. Multiple scientific paradigms in environ-
4. Maslach, C. Personal communication, May 1978. mental psychology. International Journal of Psychol-
5. Vieth, R. J. Experimental innovations in the waiting ogy, 1977,12, 147-157.
room of a hospital emergency facility. Unpublished Craik, K. H., & Zube, E. H. (Eds.). Perceiving environ-
master's thesis, Claremont Graduate School, 1977. mental quality: Research and applications. New York:
Plenum Press, 1976.
Hackman, J. R. Group influences on individuals. In
REFERENCES M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and
organizational psychology. Chicago: Rand McNally,
Baird, L. L. Big school, small school: A critical ex- 1976.
amination of the hypothesis. Journal of Educational Hackman, J. R. Designing work for individuals and
Psychology, 1969, 60, 253-260. for groups. In J. R. Hackman, E. E. Lawler, & L. W.
Barker, R. G. Ecology and motivation. In M. R. Jones Porter (Eds.), Perspectives on behavior in organizations.
(Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 8). New York: McGraw-Hill, 1977.
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1960. Hackman, J. R., & Morris, C. G. Group tasks, group
Barker, R. G. Explorations in ecological psychology. interaction process, and group performance effective-
American Psychologist, 1965, 20, 1-14. ness: A review and proposed integration. In L. Berk-
Barker, R. G. Ecological psychology: Concepts and meth- owitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychol-
ods for studying the environment of human behavior. ogy (Vol. 8). New York: Academic Press, 1975.
Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1968. Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. Development of the
Barker, R. G., & Associates. Habitats, environments, and Job Diagnostic Survey. Journal of Applied Psychology,
human behavior: Studies in ecological psychology and 1975, 60, 159-170.
eco-behavioral science from the Midwest Psychological .Helson, H. Adaptation-level theory. New York: Harper
Field Station, 1047-1972. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, '& Row, 1964.
1978.
Barker, R. G., Barker, L. S., & Ragle, D. D. M. The Holahan, C. J. Environment and behavior: A dynamic
churches of Midwest, Kansas, and Yoredale, York- perspective. New York: Plenum Press, 1978.
shire: Their contributions to the environments of the Ittelson, W. H., Proshansky, H. M., & Rivlin, L. G. The
towns. In W. J. Gore & L. C. Hodapp (Eds.), Change environmental psychology of the psychiatric ward. In
in the small community. New York: Friendship Press, H. M. Proshansky, W. H. Ittelson, & L. G. Rivlin
1967. (Eds.), Environmental psychology: Man and his physi-
Barker, R. G., & Gump, P. V. (Eds.). Big school, small cal setting. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston,
school: High school size and student behavior. Stan- 1970.
ford, Calif.: Stanford University Press, 1964. Katz, D., & Kahn, R. L. The social psychology of or-
Barker, R. G., & Schoggen, P. Qualities of community ganizations (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley, 1978.
life. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1973. Kirmeyer, S. L. Effects of work overload and under-
Barker, R. G., & Wright, H. F. Psychological ecology staffing on rangers in Yosemite National Park. Unpub-
and the problem of psychosocial development. Child lished doctoral dissertation, Claremont Graduate School,
Development, 1949, 20, 131-143. 1978.
Barker, R. G., & Wright, H. F. Midwest and its children: LeCompte, W. F. Behavior settings: The structure of
The psychological ecology of an American town. New the treatment environment. In W. J. Mitchell (Ed.),
York: Harper & Row, 1955. (Reprinted 1971 by Environmental design: Research and practice. Pro-
Archon Books, Hamden, Conn.) ceedings of the EDRA 3/AR8 Conference. Los An-
Bass, B. M. The substance and the shadow. American geles: University of California, 1972. (a)
Psychologist, 1974, 29, 870-886. LeCompte, W. F. The taxonomy of a treatment environ-
Bechtel, R. B. Enclosing behavior. Stroudsburg, Pa.: ment. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilita-
Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross, 1977. tion, 1972,53, 109-114. (b)
Bell, P. A., Fisher, J. D., & Loomis, R. J. Environmental LeCompte, W. F., & Willems, E. P. Ecological analysis
psychology. Philadelphia, Pa.: Saunders, 1978. of a hospital: Location dependencies in the behavior

764 • AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • SEPTEMBER 1979


of staff and patients. In J. Archea & C. Eastman ogy (Vol. 29). Palo Alto, Calif.: Annual Reviews,
(Eds.), EDRA 2: Proceedings of the Second Annual 1978.
Environmental Design Research Association Conference. Stokols, D. The use of intrapersonal and contextual
Pittsburgh, Pa.: Carnegie-Mellon University, 1970. theories in social psychology. In M. J. Saks & R. F.
Leff, H. L. Experience, environment, and human poten- Kidd (Eds.), Advances in applied social psychology:
tials. New York: Oxford University Press, 1978. Vol. 1. Conceptual and practical problems. Hillsdale,
Maslach, C. Burned-out. Human Behavior, September, N.J.: Erlbaum, in press.
1976, pp. 16-20; 22. Wanous, J. P, Who wants job enrichment? In J. R.
McGrath, J. E. Stress and behavior in organizations. Hackman, E. E. Lawler, & L. W. Porter (Eds.), Per*
In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and spectives on behavior in organizations. New York-
organizational psychology. Chicago: Rand McNally, McGraw-Hill, 1977.
1976. Weick, K. The "ess" in stress: Some conceptual and
McGrath, J. E., & Altman, I. Small group research. methodological problems. In J. E. McGrath (Ed.),
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1966. Social and psychological factors in stress. New York:
Milgram, S. The experience of living in cities. Science, Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1970.
1970, 167, 1461-1468. Wicker, A. W. Undermanning, performances, and stu-
Miller, J. G. Living systems: The organization. Be- dents' subjective experiences in behavior settings of
havioral Science, 1972,17, 1-182. large and small high schools. Journal of Personality
Parkinson, C. N. Parkinson's law and other studies in and Social Psychology, 1968, 10, 255-261.
administration. Boston: Hoiighton Mifflin, 19S7. Wicker, A. W. Cognitive complexity, school size, and
Petty, R.' M., & Wicker, A. W. Degree of manning and participation in school behavior settings: A test of
degree of success of a group as determinants of mem- the frequency of interaction hypothesis. Journal of
bers' subjective experiences and their acceptance of a Educational Psychology, 1969, 60, 200-203. (a)
new group member. JSAS Catalog of Selected Docu- Wicker, A. W. Size of church membership and members'
ments -in Psychology, 1974, 4, 43. (Ms. No. 616) support of church behavior settings. Journal of Per-
Price, R. H., & Blashfield, R. K. Explorations in the sonality and Social Psychology, 1969,- 13, 278-288. (b)
taxonomy of behavior settings: Analysis of dimensions Wicker, A. W. An introduction to ecological psychology.
and classifications of settings. American Journal of Monterey, Calif.: Brooks/Cole, 1979.
Community Psychology, 1915, 3, 335-351. Wicker, A. W., & Kauma, C. Effects o^ a merger of a
Proshansky, H. M., & Altman, I. Overview of the field. small and a large organization on members' behaviors
In W. P. White (Ed.), Resources in environment and and experiences. Journal of Applied Psychology, 1974,
behavior. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological 59, 24-30.
Association, 1979. Wicker, A. W., & Kirmeyer, S. L. From church to
Proshansky, H. M., Ittelson, W. H., & Rivlin, L. G. En- laboratory to national park. In S. Wapner, S. B.
vironmental psychology: People and their physical set- Cohen, & B. Kaplan (Eds.), Experiencing the environ-
tings (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart & Win- ment. New York: Plenum Press, 1976.
ston, 1976. Wicker, A. W., Kirmeyer, S. L., Hanson, L., & Alexander,
Ragle, D. D. M., Barker, R. G., & Johnson, A. Mea- D. Effects of manning levels on subjective experi-
suring extension's impact. Journal of Cooperative Ex-
ences, performance, and verbal interaction in groups.
tension, 1967,5(3), 178-186.
Organizational Behavior and Human. Performance, 1976,
Sarason, S. B. The creation of settings and the future 17, 251-274.
societies. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1972.
Wicker, A. W., McGrath, J. E., & Armstrong, G. E.
Sarason, S. B., Carroll, C. F., Maton, K., Cohen, S., &
Organization size and behavior setting capacity as de-
Lorentz, E. Human services and resource networks.
San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1977. terminants of member participation. Behavioral Sci-
ence, 1972,17, 499-513.
Sarason, S..B., Zitnay, G., & Grossman,^1. K, The crea-
tion of a community setting. Syracuse, N.Y.: Syra- Wicker, A, W., & Mehler, A. Assimilation of new mem-
cuse University, Division of Special Education and Re- bers in a large and a small church. Journal of Applied
habilitation and the Center on Human Policy, 1971. Psychology, 1971, 55, 151-156.
Schatzman, L., & Strauss, A. L. Field research: Strategies Willems, E. P, Sense of obligation to high school activi-
for a natural sociology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Pren- ties as related to school size and marginality. of stu-
tice-Hall, 1973. dent. Child Development, 1967,, 38, 1247-1260.
Sommer, R. Personal space: The behavioral basis of de- Willems, E. P. Behavioral technology and behavioral
sign. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, 1969. ecology. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1974,
Sommer, R. Tight spaces: Hard architecture and how 7, 151-165.
to humanize it. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, Willems, E. P. Behavioral ecology, health status, and
1974. health care: Applications to .the rehabilitation setting.
Steele, F. I. Physical settings and organization develop- In I. Altman & J, F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Human behavior
ment. Reading, Mass.: Addison-Wesley, 1973. and environment: Advances in theory and research
Steiner, I. D. Group process and productivity. New (Vol. 1). New York: Plenum Press, 1976.
York: Academic Press, 1972. Willems, E. P. Steps toward an ecobehavioral technology.
Stokols, D. (Ed.). Perspectives on environment and be- In A. Rogers-Warren & S. F. Warren (Eds.), Ecologi-
havior: Theory, research, and applications. New York: cal perspectives in behavior analysis. Baltimore, Md,:
Plenum Press, 1977. University Park Press, 1977.
Stokols, D. Environmental psychology. In L. W. Porter Wright, H, F. Recording and analyzing child behavior.
& M. R. Rosenzweig (Eds.), Annual review of psychol- New York: Harper & Row, 1967. .

AMERICAN PSYCHOLOGIST • SEPTEMBER 1979 • 765

You might also like