You are on page 1of 19

Psychological Bulletin Copyright 2007 by the American Psychological Association

2007, Vol. 133, No. 1, 46 – 64 0033-2909/07/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.46

Comprehending Envy
Richard H. Smith and Sung Hee Kim
University of Kentucky

The authors reviewed the psychological research on envy. The authors examined definitional challenges
associated with studying envy, such as the important distinction between envy proper (which contains
hostile feelings) and benign envy (which is free of hostile feelings). The authors concluded that envy is
reasonably defined as an unpleasant, often painful emotion characterized by feelings of inferiority,
hostility, and resentment caused by an awareness of a desired attribute enjoyed by another person or
group of persons. The authors examined questions such as why people envy, why envy contains hostile
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

feelings, and why it has a tendency to transmute itself. Finally, the authors considered the role of envy
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

in helping understand other research domains and discussed ways in which people cope with the emotion.

Keywords: envy, social comparison, social emotions

Envy, the unpleasant emotion that can arise when we compare conservative philosopher, struck a similar chord with an especially
unfavorably with others, is a common experience for most people vigorous denunciation of envy, which she claimed dominated the
regardless of culture (e.g., Foster, 1972; Schoeck, 1969; Teitel- latter part of the 20th century. In this “age of envy,” the impulse to
baum, 1976; Walcot, 1978). One reason that envy is important to begrudge the advantages and heroic strength of the few led to
understand is that it appears to be a hostile emotion that often “hatred of the good for being good” (Rand, 1971, p. 130). Envy is
prompts aggressive behaviors. Its antagonistic nature is exempli- also a frequent theme of philosophers who focus on the motivation
fied by the many publicized crimes (e.g., Schoeck, 1969; Thern- for people’s desire for social equality (e.g., de la Mora, 1987;
strom, 1998) and intergroup conflicts (e.g., Beck, 1999; Glick, Nozick, 1974; Rawls, 1971), and it is an inscrutable quandary in
2002) attributed to it; the countless literary tales of assassina- fictional attempts by novelists to imagine utopian societies free of
tion, murder, and sabotage provoked by it (e.g., de la Mora, discord caused by inequalities (e.g., Hartley, 1960; Lowry, 1993).
1987; Schoeck, 1969); its generative role in many Biblical The sociologist Schoeck (1969) laid out an especially sweeping set
events ranging from Lucifer’s evil nature, to Cain’s slaying of of claims about the far-reaching role of envy at all levels of
Abel, to Christ’s crucifixion (e.g., Aquaro, 2004); its presence
society. He argued that envy is the foundational explanation for
in two of the 10 commandments in the Old Testament; and by
pan-cultural norms that serve to maintain social stability, although
psychoanalytic claims that it is a destructive, life-denying in-
this process often leads to the unfortunate stifling of creativity and
stinct characterized by rage (e.g., Klein, 1957). Envy is also
to various personal vices. More recently, the Christian philosopher
characterized by its link with an assortment of pernicious ten-
Aquaro (2004) made the case for envy being the core emotion
dencies, such as a willingness to sacrifice one’s own outcomes
in order to simply diminish the envied person’s relative advantage driving most sinful behaviors and thus creating the need for the 10
(e.g., Berke, 1988; Parks, Rumble, & Posey, 2002; Thernstrom, commandments to combat these sins.
1998; Zizzo & Oswald, 2001), a desire to destroy good things if Remarkably, despite the many plausible claims for the powerful
the alternative is that others have them (e.g., Klein, 1957; Scheler, influence of envy in everyday social interactions and for its role in
1915/1961; Schimmel, 1993), or a feeling of malicious joy when shaping societal norms, psychological research on envy is only in
the envied person suffers (R. H. Smith et al., 1996) or when an its early stages. But there are a number of helpful studies on envy
envied group fails (Leach, Spears, Branscombe, & Doosje, 2003), that have emerged. In this article, we review this empirical liter-
even if the suffering is undeserved (Brigham, Kelso, Jackson, & ature. In doing so, we discuss a set of definitional and conceptual
Smith, 1997). challenges to understanding envy in the context of the many
Claims for the importance of understanding envy go beyond its scholarly claims that have been made about the emotion, and we
being a hostile emotion. Nietzsche (1887/1967) argued that envy is also outline a way of thinking about envy that aims to capture its
the prime cause for the egalitarian morality inherent in Christianity core features. We then describe an example of an area of research
in which the strong are brought down by the weak using principles in social psychology, prejudice and intergroup relations, in which
of morality sanctified by religion. Rand (1971), the ultra- envy has been shown to play an important role in advancing
understanding. We also describe another area of research, psycho-
social predictors of mental and physical health, as an example of
an area in which there is no existing research on envy but which
Richard H. Smith and Sung Hee Kim, Department of Psychology,
University of Kentucky.
would profit from efforts to incorporate this factor. Finally, we
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Richard discuss the important problem of how people can cope with envy
H. Smith, Department of Psychology, Kastle Hall, University of Kentucky, and suggest how understanding this problem has implications for
Lexington, KY 40506. E-mail: Rhsmit00@uky.edu coping with negative emotions in general.
46
COMPREHENDING ENVY 47

Defining Envy (Parrott & Smith, 1993; R. H. Smith, Kim, & Parrott, 1988). It is
hard to imagine envy without such a longing. But it is probably
Definitions of envy emphasize that it is an unpleasant, often unwise to equate longing with envy, however necessary it may
painful emotion characterized by feelings of inferiority, hostility, seem to be for envy to arise. People certainly use the term envy in
and resentment produced by an awareness of another person or this sense alone, but as with admiration, its character seems harm-
group of persons who enjoy a desired possession (object, social less and free of feelings of inferiority or hostility. Unlike envy
position, attribute, or quality of being; e.g., Parrott, 1991; Parrott & proper, longing appears to focus on the thing itself that one would
Smith, 1993). These blended features of inferiority, hostility, and like to possess, rather than on the person possessing it. It seems to
resentment generally persist in most definitions, although as Par- entail an appreciation for what is desired, decoupled from any
rott (1991) noted, envy also has more controversial contours than obvious antagonistic implications following from the fact that
other emotions such as anger or sadness. Envy is also considered another person currently enjoys it. Thus, it is claimed that people
as one of a group of related emotions (e.g., envy, shame, jealousy, use this form of envy in a free and open sense (e.g., Foster, 1972;
relative deprivation, and indignation) characterized by negative Heider, 1958; Silver & Sabini, 1978) without worry that others
affective reactions to the superior fortunes of others (e.g., Heider, will misconstrue their feelings as hostile. Envy proper, however, as
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

1958; Ortony, Clore, & Collins, 1988).


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

most scholarly opinions emphasize (e.g., Foster, 1972; Heider,


Once one moves from generally characterizing envy to estab- 1958; Schoeck, 1969; Silver & Sabini, 1978) and research supports
lishing its distinctive and consistent features, a number of defini- (Heikkinen, Latvala, & Isola, 2003), is often kept secret. Some
tional challenges rise up to confound the task. Some of these are claim it is the very last emotion that people will admit to feeling
semantic, and others stem from confusing associations with other (e.g., Schoeck, 1969; Silver & Sabini, 1978). Longing may have
emotions. In the first section of this article, we address these two negative connotations, especially if it becomes inordinate and
challenges. grasping, thus turning into greed as Schimmel (1993) argued. But
though some scholars argue that greed can produce ugly, some-
Envy Proper and Benign Envy times hostile behaviors (e.g., Ashwin, 2005; Waska, 2004), it does
Scholars are quick to point out that people use the word envy in not appear to involve a desire to destroy the thing that is desired.
at least two partially contradictory senses (e.g., Foster, 1972; Neu,
1980; Rawls, 1971; Silver & Sabini, 1978). One sense, “envy Envy and Jealousy
proper,” is the meaning found in dictionary definitions and is the
main focus of scholarship on envy. The other meaning, referred to Another definitional challenge concerns how envy differs from
as “benign envy” (Rawls, 1971) or “nonmalicious envy” (Parrott, jealousy (Foster, 1972; Guerrero & Andersen, 1998; Neu, 1980;
1991), is different from envy proper in at least one core aspect: Parrott & Smith, 1993; Russell, 1930; Salovey & Rodin, 1986;
being free of hostile meaning. When people use the word envy in Schoeck, 1969; Silver & Sabini, 1978; R. H. Smith et al., 1988).
this latter sense, they can actually be using it to mean an emotion Although envy is often confused with jealousy, research shows
closer to admiration than to envy (e.g., Foster, 1972; Silver & that these two emotions are actually quite different (e.g., Parrott &
Sabini, 1978). Smith, 1993; R. H. Smith et al., 1988). Envy typically involves two
Perhaps it is useful to grant a type of envy that is essentially free people and occurs when one lacks something enjoyed by another.
of hostile feelings (e.g., Parrott, 1991). Envy undoubtedly requires The target of envy may be a person or a group of persons, but the
that the envying person recognize something of value in the envied focus of envy is that one lacks something compared with a specific
person (though, as a coping response, devaluation may be the target, whether it be a target individual or target group. Jealousy
result, as we outline more fully later), and this recognition would typically involves three people and occurs when one fears losing
seem to inspire at least a modicum of admiring goodwill. This type someone to another person. Envy and jealousy result from differ-
of envy may be more common than envy proper and may often ent situations, generate distinct appraisals, and produce distinctive
bring about constructive, emulative actions rather than ill will and emotional experiences.
its possible destructive consequences. Yet, the acceptance of a Semantic confusion. Research comparing envy and jealousy
benign form of envy may obscure the nature of envy. The absence isolates three reasons why they can appear more similar than they
of hostile feelings in benign envy may render the emotion funda- actually are. First, people often use the terms envy and jealousy
mentally different from envy proper both in terms of the felt interchangeably (Salovey & Rodin, 1986), and this naturally en-
experience and in terms of its likely consequences. Also, it is courages the view that they are more or less equivalent emotions.
important to emphasize that benign envy, if it is to be called envy, But this overlapping usage is asymmetric. In a study by R. H.
is not the emotion that is typically the focus of scholarly attention, Smith et al. (1988), participants wrote short descriptions of situa-
however much it may be the understood use in many instances of tions in which they felt either strong envy or strong (romantic)
everyday language and however much it may be an important jealousy. Coders rated each description for whether it conformed
response to be studied in its own right. In our view, benign envy to standard definitions of envy or jealousy. Envy accounts corre-
is envy sanitized (Ashwin, 2005) and lacks a core ingredient of the sponded to the standard definition of envy in almost all cases.
emotion, namely some form of ill will. Jealousy, by contrast, evoked envy and jealousy descriptions
equally. Thus, the linguistic ambiguity of the term jealousy can
contribute to a false sense that envy and jealousy are equivalent
Envy and Longing
emotions.
Studies examining the content of envy episodes show that it The co-occurrence of envy and jealousy. A second reason for
involves a longing for or a coveting of what another person has envy and jealousy appearing more similar than they are is that
48 SMITH AND KIM

these emotions often co-occur (Parrott & Smith, 1993). A rival can participant differences in elevation and scatter. This adjustment
be threatening exactly because he or she has enviable qualities, was achieved by subtracting each participant’s mean rating from
which may then increase jealousy as well (DeSteno & Salovey, each of his or her individual ratings and then dividing this by the
1996). Participants in DeSteno and Salovey’s research were pre- standard deviation of the participant’s ratings. When these adjusted
sented with a series of rivals for their partners’ attentions. Descrip- scores were used, the pattern of difference between jealousy and
tions of the rivals varied in ways that would be expected to create envy was consistent with predictions for qualitative differences.
more or less envy by virtue of the relevance of the characteristics Those affective components more salient in envy than in jealousy
for the participants’ self-definition. Greater jealousy was reported were as follows: longing for what another has, feeling inferior,
when the domain of a rival’s achievements was also the one in harboring resentment and ill will, and feeling that the ill will is
which envy would be expected. DeSteno and Salovey measured wrong or unsanctioned. Those components more salient in jeal-
jealousy rather than envy, which could only be inferred from the ousy than in envy were as follows: fear of loss, distrust, righteous
manipulation of the relevance of the characteristics for the partic- anger over betrayal, and uncertainty about the circumstances.
ipants’ self-definition. These results confirmed qualitative differences between the expe-
Parrott and Smith (1993) also demonstrated this tendency for riences of envy and jealousy and indicated that the higher intensity
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

envy and jealousy to co-occur and included measures of both envy of most cases of jealousy compared with envy can mask these
and jealousy as well. Participants wrote accounts of situations in differences.
which they experienced either strong envy or strong (romantic)
jealousy. Coding of the accounts for the presence of both emotions Envy and Resentment
revealed that 59% of the jealousy accounts included envy. Again,
this makes sense as romantic rivals often become rivals because A final definitional challenge in understanding envy involves its
they may have enviable attributes, thus attracting the attention of particularly complex associations with resentment, another emo-
one’s partner. Also, the very fact that they have the attention of tion often triggered by a social comparison with someone or some
one’s partner might produce envy in itself. It is interesting that group of persons enjoying an advantage. It is often claimed that
only 11.5% of the envy accounts also involved a threatened rela- when we envy, we feel that the envied person does not quite
tionship. A threat to a relationship may frequently evoke a focus on deserve his or her advantage (e.g., Heider, 1958; Scheler, 1915/
someone who seems superior as well (hence, envy), but a focus on 1961; R. H. Smith, 1991) or at least that our disadvantage is
someone who seems superior typically should not evoke a threat to undeserved (Ben-Ze’ev, 2000). Heider (1958) argued that envy
a relationship. often contains a sense of injustice because it will typically occur
In a second study, Parrott and Smith (1993) showed that despite between people who are similar in terms of background, class, and
the frequent co-occurrence of envy and jealousy, it is nonetheless the like. Psychological “balance forces” require that similar people
possible to isolate distinctive affective components. They com- should have similar outcomes, a principle that Heider called an
posed hypothetical scenarios designed to evoke either envy or “ought” force. Heider reasoned that the envious person often feels
jealousy independently. Participants rated these scenarios in terms a sense of injustice because the envied person’s advantage violates
of how the protagonist was likely to feel using affect terms what “ought to be.”
distinctively characteristic of either envy (e.g., inferiority, longing, But envy is not the same as resentment proper, strictly speaking,
and resentment) or jealousy (e.g., anger, fear of rejection or loss, as philosophers such as Rawls (1971) argued. Generally, if the
distrust, and anxiety). One situation involved a college freshman advantage is unfair, especially in terms of objectively derived and
trying out for a varsity tennis team. In the high-envy versions, the agreed on standards, the full-blown emotions of resentment proper
rival made the team, whereas the protagonist did not; these fates and indignation rather than envy should result, unalloyed with
were reversed in the low-envy versions. In the high-jealousy envy, as others have also argued (e.g., D’Arms, 2002; Neu, 1980;
versions, the protagonist saw the rival flirting with the protago- R. H. Smith, 1991; Walker & Smith, 2002) or have tried to
nist’s boyfriend or girlfriend. In the low-jealousy versions, the demonstrate empirically (R. H. Smith, Parrott, Ozer, & Moniz,
protagonist observed a similar scene but the identity of the person 1994). Indignation and feelings of resentment proper arise, by
with whom the rival flirted was unspecified. Jealousy-related feel- definition, from unfair treatment. Invidious resentment occurs
ings were strongly affected by the identity of the rival’s romantic when the advantage is painful but fair by such objective standards
partner, whereas envy-related feelings were strongly affected by (R. H. Smith, 1991; R. H. Smith et al., 1994). To the extent that
the superiority of the rival. envy contains a sense of injustice, it is argued to be qualitatively
Jealousy and its greater intensity. A third reason why envy different from the kind that produces indignation and resentment in
and jealousy can seem similar follows from a feature that often their pristine forms (R. H. Smith, 1991; R. H. Smith et al., 1994).
makes them distinct, namely that jealousy is typically more intense It is subjectively derived. In many instances of envy, additional
than envy, as a series of studies by Salovey and Rodin (1986) evidence of unfairness is probably nurtured quickly and more so
demonstrate. Important qualitative differences between the felt over time so that the feelings seem more legitimate, as suggested
experience of two emotions can be masked by this intensity. by R. H. Smith (1991, 2004). But, as Heider (1958) asserted, the
Parrott and Smith (1993) addressed this issue by examining the envying person is probably aware that his or her sense of injustice
relative salience of different affective components within each is far from fully legitimate. For one thing, as Heider (1958) also
emotion in addition to comparing raw scores. Analysis of raw argued, people are usually taught to rejoice in other people’s
scores showed that jealousy was indeed more intense than envy on successes. In a sense, envy violates social conventions that usually
almost all affective components. But, a second analysis used require supportive rather than competitive, begrudging reactions to
participants’ ratings after they were adjusted for between- another person’s success. In most cultures, envy is considered a sin
COMPREHENDING ENVY 49

and thus shameful (e.g., Foster, 1972; Heider, 1958; Schoeck, R. H. Smith et al. (1994) speculated that one reason for subjec-
1969; Silver & Sabini, 1978). tive injustice beliefs being part of envy is that many of the
Some psychologists (e.g., Feather & Sherman, 2002) and philos- invidious advantages enjoyed by others are unfair, at least in the
ophers (e.g., Rawls, 1971) argue strongly against blending resentment sense that the envying person cannot be blamed for his or her
with envy. They claim that to the extent that justice-related feelings inferiority. Attributes such as intelligence, physical attractiveness,
arise resulting from unflattering social comparisons, these feelings and musical ability can seem arbitrarily bestowed, and envying
are, ipso facto, resentment, not envy. Furthermore, the co-occurrence people can feel unfairly handicapped by how they stand on the
of resentment and envy may be due solely to the tendency for envying distributions of such attributes. However, societal norms disallow
people to rationalize the cause of their envy. To the extent that the claiming an injustice because of these handicaps; in fact, these
offending superiority in another person can be perceived as unfair, attributes appear to contribute to many socially agreed upon stan-
one’s envy is transformed into resentment proper even if an outside dards of merit. Yet, from the subjective point of view of people in
observer might label the feeling as mere envy. the grip of envy, they can feel unfairly treated by life and thus
Other scholars argue that envy emerges from a basic desire for resentful even though this resentment seems far from legitimate
equal treatment present from a very early age. Bertrand Russell
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

and thus far from righteous indignation or resentment proper. Few


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(1930) claimed that children deeply resent “the very slightest people wish to be labeled envious; therefore, envious resentment is
appearance of favoring one child at the expense of another” and, as likely to be privately held and subjectively valid at best, giving it
a result, “distributive justice, absolute, rigid and unvarying, must
a different quality from feelings of resentment and indignation that
be observed by any one who has children to deal with” (p. 82). He
enjoy social validation.
found little difference between children and adults, fundamentally, in
The question of whether envy should be partly characterized by
their reactions to advantages enjoyed by others. A recent Freudian
a sense of injustice is an especially difficult one not easily resolved
interpretation of envy by Forrester (1997) takes up this claim and
by either argument or empirical focus. We shall suggest more fully
suggests that the “call for justice and equality is founded on the
below that the question may be best explored by understanding the
transformation of envy” (p. 20). Children, in their desire for equal
treatment with their siblings, insist that such treatment is just and experience of envy as a process that can take several paths, one
fair. In a sense, envy creates the desire for justice; without envy, being in the direction of furthering a sense of injustice already
“there would be no desire for justice” (Forrester, 1997, p. 20). present in some form when envy first arises. There may come a point
In any event, a sense of injustice seems prevalent enough in in this process in which a sense of injustice so dominates the emo-
experiences of envy that this sense usually makes its way into tional experience that the initial envy is quite transformed into resent-
definitions of the emotion. Even if its initial presence could be ment proper and the emotion label of envy no longer makes complete
shown empirically to be the result of a quick defensive maneuver sense. Nonetheless, even this kind of justice feeling, inspired by envy
triggered by envy, to the extent that it happens typically in envy, in its incipience, might still be distinguished from outright indig-
one can argue that a sense of injustice is a core feature of the nation, never tainted by any question of its legitimacy.
experience of envy.
The only direct empirical attempt to address this difficult issue
can be found in a study by R. H. Smith et al. (1994). These authors
Summary of Definitional Issues
argue that justice concerns of some sort may need to be part of
On the surface, envy seems easy to define. However, in every-
envy because without such concerns the hostile aspect of envy is
day use, the term envy is often confused with its more benign
absent. Feelings of inferiority alone, without a subjective sense of
forms, which are closer to admiration and longing. In our view, it
injustice, may lead only to a self-focus on one’s inferiority, the
is crucial to recognize that envy, by proper definition and scholarly
affective outcomes being depressive rather than hostile. Partici-
tradition, contains hostile feelings that can lead to hostile actions.
pants wrote accounts of experiences in which they felt strong envy
Envy is also often confused with jealousy, because of its semantic
and then completed measures assessing whether the invidious
overlap in the use of the term jealousy, the tendency for both
advantage was objectively (e.g., “Anyone would agree that the
envied person’s advantage was unfairly obtained.”) or subjectively emotions to co-occur, and the typically higher intensity of jeal-
unfair (e.g., “It seemed unfair that the person I envied started out ousy. But envy involves cases in which another person has what
in life with certain advantages over me.”), whether it produced a we want but cannot have, whereas jealousy involves the threat of
sense of inferiority (e.g., “The discrepancy between the person I losing someone to a rival. Finally, envy has especially complex
envied and me was due to my own inferior qualities.”), and the associations with resentment, as many definitions incorporate
degree to which they felt hostile toward the envied person and some sense of injustice within the initial experience of envy and
depressed because of this person’s advantage. Inferiority strongly certainly as a common means of coping with the emotion. Good
predicted depressive feelings but not hostility, which indicated that arguments can be made that separating envy from a sense of
feeling inferior, by itself, is insufficient for the full range of affects injustice in the full-blown sense but ridding resentment from
associated with envy. Beliefs about objective injustice predicted definitions of envy seem to miss an important ingredient. In sum,
hostility but not depressive feelings, which indicated that objective how should we define envy? In terms of an overall characterization
unfairness should create hostility but should have no obvious link of how envy is experienced, envy is an unpleasant and often
with feeling inferior (and therefore feeling depressed). Subjective painful blend of feelings characterized by inferiority, hostility, and
injustice beliefs predicted both depressive feeling and hostility, resentment caused by a comparison with a person or group of
which indicated that this kind of sense of injustice may be impor- persons who possess something we desire. This seems a reasonable
tant in bringing about these defining features of envy. working definition.
50 SMITH AND KIM

Important Questions Concerning Envy news, they had rated their perceptions of similarity to each person
in their unit, including the person who was ultimately promoted.
Why Do We Envy? Perceptions of similarity with this promotee predicted the degree
Envy is an unpleasant emotion, but why is it so? Before pro- of envy felt toward her.
ceeding with an analysis of other aspects of envy, it is worth Self-relevance of the comparison domain. Sharing
stepping back and considering this basic question. Why is it that comparison-related similarities with the advantaged person is im-
another person’s advantage can make people feel this painful, portant for envy to arise, but research also shows that the domain
socially abhorrent feeling? of comparison in which the envied person enjoys an advantage
Perhaps the most telling answer to why we envy is that the should be self-relevant (e.g., Salovey & Rodin, 1984, 1991; Silver
advantages enjoyed by other people often have potent conse- & Sabini, 1978; Tesser, 1991). A core part of one’s self-worth
quences for the self, as a long tradition of empirical work shows must be linked to doing well on the domain of comparison. Unless
(e.g., Buunk & Gibbons, 1997; Festinger, 1954; Mussweiler, 2003; doing well matters, it is unlikely for a social comparison to create
R. H. Smith, Diener, & Wedell, 1989; Suls & Miller, 1977; Suls & an emotion of any kind, as emotions in general arise because they
are linked to a person’s important goals, a point that most emotion
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Wheeler, 2000). Relative standing usually contributes much to


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

deciding who gains the prized things in life. Social comparisons researchers stress (e.g., Lazarus, 1991; Ortony, Clore, & Collins,
also help form the foundations for inferences about the self (Fest- 1988). As many theorists claim (e.g., James, 1890/1950; Pyszc-
inger, 1954). They also contribute to ability assessments—for zynski, Greenberg, Solomon, Arndt, & Schmiel, 2004; Tesser,
example, superior relative performance indicates success (and high 1991), most people are motivated to maintain a positive self-
ability) and inferior performance indicates failure (and low ability; concept. Furthermore, most people’s self-worth will be invested in
e.g., Kelley, 1967). Because of the usually potent consequence of doing well in certain areas more than in others (e.g., James,
social comparisons for tangible outcomes and for self-evaluation, 1890/1950; Tesser, 1991). In the Schaubroeck and Lam (2004)
noticing another person’s relative advantage, logically, should lead study just noted, one likely reason why participants felt envious
to some sort of negative feeling (R. H. Smith, 2000). Although toward the promotee was that being promoted was important and
motivational variables and various self-serving construals of social self-relevant to them.
comparison information may blunt the perceived effects of social A study by Salovey and Rodin (1984) provides strong evidence
comparisons (e.g., Collins, 1996; Lockwood & Kunda, 1997; for the importance of both similarity and self-relevance in envy.
Wood & Wilson, 2003), many experiments provide empirical Participants received feedback on a career aptitude test that sug-
demonstration of how important this information can be in influ- gested that their career prospects in their preferred field were either
encing people’s emotions (e.g., see Suls & Wheeler, 2000, for outstanding or poor. They were then given the career prospects of
recent reviews). Envy is perhaps the most important marker for another student who had done better or worse than they had on
when these social comparisons reflect poorly on the self in ways either the same career domain or a different (less self-relevant)
that personally matter. career domain. Envy occurred only when participants, having
received negative feedback, compared themselves with the student
who had done better on a career domain relevant to them.
Whom Do We Envy?
Similarity. Schoeck’s (1969) and Foster’s (1972) reviews of The Question of Perceived Control
the anthropological literature on envy make a strong case for its
universal nature. But envy is hardly the inevitable response to We envy similar others who otherwise enjoy an advantage in an
unflattering social comparisons (e.g., Collins, 1996; Lockwood & area linked to our self-worth. But there is another important feature
Kunda, 1997). Scholarly claims (e.g., Aristotle, 322 BC/1941; of the comparison situation to consider. It is usually claimed that
Heider, 1958) as well as empirical findings (e.g., Parrott, 1991; people feeling envy must believe that the desired attribute is
Salovey & Rodin, 1984; Salovey & Rothman, 1991; Schaubroeck beyond their power to obtain (e.g., Elster, 1998; Neu, 1980;
& Lam, 2004; Tesser, 1991) suggest that we envy people who are Scheler, 1915/1961; R. H. Smith, 1991; Vecchio, 1997). Empirical
similar to ourselves, save for their advantage on the desired do- evidence for the importance of perceived control in envy can be
main. As Aristotle phrased the point, when it comes to envy it is inferred from the more general research on social comparisons.
usually “potter against potter.” This is generally true of how social Participants in a study by Testa and Major (1990), after learning
comparisons operate (e.g., Festinger, 1954). We seek out, attend that they had done poorly on a task, were exposed to a superior
to, and are affected by social comparisons with people who share performing comparison person. Half of the participants were told
comparison-related attributes, such as gender, age, and social class that they could improve their performance (high control), and the
(Goethals & Darley, 1977), as a number of studies show (e.g., other half were told that they could not improve (low control).
Gastorf & Suls, 1978). Studies also show that similarity testing Participants in the low-control conditions showed the highest
appears to be the default strategy when we first make social depressive and hostile reactions to the superior performing com-
comparisons (Mussweiler, 2003). Without such similarities, social parison person. Given that envy is characterized by a mixture of
comparisons can seem irrelevant, and our reactions may be indif- depressive and hostile feelings, these results suggest that the par-
ferent and detached. ticipants would also have reported envy.
Schaubroeck and Lam’s (2004) recent field study shows the In terms of self-evaluation, a set of studies by Lockwood and
importance of similarity in predicting envy in an effective way. Kunda (1997) shows that perception of control also predicted
Participants were female bank employees in small work units who whether a comparison with a superior person would negatively
had been passed over for promotion. Months before receiving this affect self-views. In one study, 1st-year undergraduate participants
COMPREHENDING ENVY 51

were exposed to information about another student who was doing issue of control affects emotional reactions to another’s advantage.
very well on a self-relevant domain. This student was either a Even when the outcomes enjoyed by the advantaged person seem
1st-year student as well or was a 4th-year student. Exposure to the undeserved, an extreme sense of low control may lead to depres-
1st-year student tended to be deflating, whereas exposure to the sive, helpless feelings. An extreme sense of high control may lead
4th-year student was self-enhancing. Analyses of open-ended re- to indignation, anger, and corrective action. Conditions leading to
sponses showed clearly that the 4th-year student was often inspir- envy may inhabit a territory in between these two extremes where
ing to 1st-year participants, as this person gave them a sense of a sense of control is low but where the desired outcome can be
what their own possibilities might be if they took similar emulous imagined and where the deservingness judgments are subjective
actions. However, comparisons with the 1st-year student seemed rather than objective.
to highlight what the participants had not done and tended to be In sum, the role of perceived control in envy is complex. One of
debilitating rather than inspiring. An additional study measured the reasons why envy is painful may be that similarity with the
participants’ beliefs about whether their own abilities could in- envied person creates a sense of possibility together with a real-
crease over time. Participants with malleable, optimistic beliefs ization that this possibility will be frustrated. There is a sense that
about their abilities found exposure to the similar but more suc- the outcomes or attributes enjoyed by the advantaged person could
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

cessful fellow student to be more self-enhancing than did those be the things that one should also have by virtue of this similarity.
with fixed beliefs. These studies focused on self-evaluation, and, In this sense, there is an expectation of that it could, even should,
because envious feelings are directly linked to self-evaluations, we happen. On the other hand, the facts suggest otherwise when
can suppose that invidious feelings would parallel the pattern considered with realism; what seems possible is actually unlikely.
found for self-evaluations. This coming together of both expectation and frustration may help
Similarity and its complex role in perception of control. Al- explain why unflattering comparisons with far superior others are
though the research on reactions to unflattering social comparisons not thought to create envy. People who are vastly superior to us
appears to confirm that low perceived control should be an im- seem in a different category, and this dissimilarity may quell any
portant factor in envy, the issue is complex. If it is true that we are sense of expectation.
more likely to envy those who are similar in background charac-
teristics, would not this similarity suggest the capacity to obtain the The Hostile Nature of Envy
desired attribute as well? Similarity should lead to a higher sense
of control. Elster (1998) suggested a resolution to this puzzle by Schoeck (1969) detailed many examples of crimes in which
claiming that the envying person must be able to imagine the perpetrators, victims, witnesses, or investigators attributed a crime
possibility of enjoying the desired attribute. As he put it, envy to envy or made statements consistent with an envy motive. These
“presupposes that I can tell myself a plausible story in which I crimes range from a case of arson that led to the deaths of a group
ended up with the envied possession” (p.169), which is why of exceptionally talented Ph.D. students at Cornell, to the slashing
“princes may envy kings and starlets envy stars, but most people of the tires of a group of private cars in an incident in Germany,
envy neither, or, only weakly” (p.169). Elster emphasized that the and to the false accusations of crime directed at the physically
envying person’s imagining the possibility is more abstract than attractive before World War I. We also alluded earlier to the many
real. It is a frustrated desire. In Elster’s words, the envying person other historical or literary instances of hostile behavior claimed to
believes it “could have been me” rather than it “will be me.” be at least partly caused by envy. But what is the empirical
Elster’s analysis parallels what would be predicted by research on evidence for the hostile nature of envy?
the simulation heuristic (e.g., Kahneman & Tversky, 1982). In The perception of hostility in other people’s envy. Silver and
general, emotional reactions are more intense if participants can Sabini (1978) considered the question of hostility in envy by
more easily imagine alternatives to a particular emotion-inducing examining the conditions in which people perceive envy in others.
situation than if they cannot. Participants watched videotaped interactions between two stu-
Relative deprivation and perceived control. Research on rel- dents. One of these students learns that he has been admitted to
ative deprivation (e.g., Crosby, 1976; Folger, 1987; H. J. Smith & Yale for graduate school, and a friend of this student is seen
Kessler, 2004) is also instructive with regard to the role of per- reacting to this news by making a comment to a third person. In the
ceived control in envy. A precondition for relative deprivation is condition considered prototypical of envy, the friend has failed to
that another person’s advantage be perceived as undeserved, and get into graduate school himself or has only gotten into a poor
so the feelings associated with relative deprivation largely entail quality school. Furthermore, his comment is inappropriately neg-
varieties of resentment. As already noted, envy has complex con- ative (e.g., “. . . did you see the way he went on about it? Think he
nections with a sense of injustice, and, therefore, factors associated was the only person that ever got into graduate school . . .”).
with relative deprivation have potential relevance for understand- Almost all participants attributed the friend’s inappropriately de-
ing envy as well. One of the important factors predicting relative rogatory comments to envy. Variations in this core condition
deprivation concerns the feasibility of obtaining the outcomes reduced attributions of envy: (a) if the friend had been admitted to
enjoyed by the advantaged person. It is interesting that some initial an equally prestigious graduate school, such as Harvard (eliminat-
research (Crosby, 1976) suggests that relative deprivation is more ing the necessary condition of an advantage); (b) if the advantaged
likely if the desired outcome is feasible to obtain; hence, there is student in fact acted in an arrogant, boastful way (lessening the
high perceived control. However, later research suggests it is the inappropriateness of negative comment); (c) if the comment was a
lack of feasibility that predicts relative deprivation (Folger, 1987; depressive acknowledgement of lowered worth (highlighting feel-
Walker, Wong, & Kretzschmar, 2002). The inconsistency in these ings of inferiority without accompanying hostility); and (d) if the
findings may suggest the general difficulty of capturing how the comment appropriately focused on how much the student deserved
52 SMITH AND KIM

his success (eliminating any sense of ill will). Silver and Sabini the self that participants were willing to incur in order to burn the
concluded that most people believe that envy is a hostile feeling outcomes of the advantaged other.
(implied by the derogatory comment) linked to the envying per- Envious hostility in the workplace or in group settings. The
son’s painful experience of another person’s advantage. Whereas workplace is a competitive, often hierarchical domain in which
the nonenvious response might range from admiration to depres- envious hostility may often play an important role (Vecchio, 2000,
sion, the tell-tale sign of envy is hostility, even in the absence of 2005). A clear empirical example of this is the study by Schaubro-
boasting on the part of the advantaged person. From the nonen- eck and Lam (2004) cited earlier. Envy mediated the dislike of
vying person’s perspective, the hostility seems uncalled for but fellow employees who had the advantage of being promoted. This
still understandable when envy is its root cause. By contrast, when study is particularly convincing with regard to envy because its
boasting is present, then the envy attribution is unnecessary; the measure of envy was well-informed by theoretical perspectives on
hostility implied by the derogatory remark seems appropriate, envy and was differentiated from other similar concerns, such as
legitimate, and envy free. objective feelings of injustice. Research in workplace settings by
Silver and Sabini’s (1978) study does not include a measure of Vecchio (2005) also provides supportive evidence. Participants’
invidious hostility, and one might question whether the negative invidious concerns were negatively correlated with self-esteem
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

remark in the envy condition is a true indicator of hostility, but and job satisfaction and positively correlated with Machiavellian-
their results are instructive. The average person appears to believe ism and propensity to quit, outcomes that may also have been
that a defining ingredient of envy is some form of inappropriate, caused by envy-related hostility.
arguably hostile reaction caused by another person’s advantage. It Group settings in general can also be breeding grounds for
is interesting that, as Parrott (1991) asserted, this belief does not envious hostility. Duffy and Shaw (2000), using small work
necessarily entail that the envying person recognizes that his or her groups, showed longitudinally that self-reports of invidious con-
hostile reaction is inspired by envy, an issue that we address in cerns were negatively related to group performance. They also
more detail later. The envying person might label his or her feeling found that, through an increase in social loafing and a decrease in
as indignation and might see arrogance in the advantaged other, group potency and cohesion, these concerns were indirectly related
whereas the nonenvious person sees appropriate confidence. Pos- to absenteeism and group satisfaction. Envious hostility may have
sibly, it does not require that the envied person do anything to precipitated some of these outcomes.
deserve the hostile reaction either. In fact, Silver and Sabini’s Envy and schadenfreude. A number of studies also suggest the
(1978) results suggest that, from an observer’s perspective, the less hostile nature of envy by showing that envy predisposes a person
the apparent hostility seems deserved, the more envy will seem the to feel pleasure, schadenfreude, when a misfortune befalls the
cause. Most important, in terms of understanding the nature of envied person (Brigham et al., 1997; R. H. Smith et al., 1996). In
envy, some sort of negative reaction, reasonably described as the R. H. Smith et al. (1996) study, participants viewed a video-
hostile, is a signature feature of envy. taped interview of a student who was intending to apply to medical
Hostility and narrowing relative differences at own expense. school. Details about his academic achievements and activities that
Recent findings in behavioral economics (e.g., Sanfey, Rilling, emerged in the interview were manipulated to suggest someone
Aronson, Nystrom, & Cohen, 2003; Zizzo & Oswald, 2001) typify with either enviable superiority or average qualities. At the end of
another way of characterizing the hostile nature of envy. As noted the tape, an epilogue informed participants that the student had
earlier, scholars often claim that envy is destructive in nature, so been arrested for stealing amphetamines from a lab where he was
much so that people feeling envy would just as soon have the working, and, as a result, he had to delay plans for medical school.
desired advantage destroyed (or the person or persons enjoying the Envy created by the manipulation of invidious superiority (mea-
advantage) if they themselves are denied it. Furthermore, people sured while the tape was paused toward the end of the interview)
feeling envy appear willing to compromise their own outcomes if mediated schadenfreude (measured after the epilogue). A second
this means that the advantaged person will suffer. This suggests study by Brigham et al. (1997) indicates that the link between envy
that relative outcomes matter more than absolute levels when envy and schadenfreude is especially robust. In the R. H. Smith et al.
is involved. Zizzo and Oswald’s (2001) research is especially (1996) study, the misfortune was “deserved,” and therefore one
consistent with such claims. Participants in groups of 4 were could argue that envy produces only malicious emotions such as
initially given nearly equal amounts of money and then played a schadenfreude when the advantaged person contributes to his or
computerized game against each other. During the game, 2 of the her own misfortune. However, in the Brigham et al. (1997) study,
participants received an extra amount of money, giving them an which also used the videotaped interview procedure, envy medi-
advantage over the others. At the end of the game, each participant ated schadenfreude even when the advantaged person was not to
was allowed a chance to “burn” some of the earning of the other blame for this misfortune and thus had suffered undeservedly.
participants. But, this chance came at a price. For each dollar These studies linking envy and schadenfreude capture some of
(U. S.) they burned, they had to pay between 2 cents and 25 cents. the distinctive features of envious hostility. Schadenfreude, espe-
Disadvantaged participants tended to burn more of the earnings of cially in response to an undeserved misfortune, clearly suggests an
the advantaged participants, even when the costs of burning in- underlying hostility on the part of the person feeling envy. Al-
creased. The precise motivation behind this tendency to “burn the though there may be circumstances in which people express it
rich” appeared to be a blend of both envy and a concern for openly, schadenfreude is a socially undesirable emotion. Social
fairness. It is interesting that even advantaged participants burned norms and the average person’s internalized values would seem to
the outcomes of the others, but they did so indiscriminately. work against both the private feeling of schadenfreude and cer-
Arguably, envy partly spurred the disadvantaged participants’ be- tainly its public expression, at least when the feeling is inspired
havior. Its particular, hostile quality was indicated by the price to simply by another person’s advantage and particularly when the
COMPREHENDING ENVY 53

misfortune is undeserved. In this regard, envious hostility appears Frustration, injustice, and hostility. The frustration inherent in
to resist being subdued despite its abhorrent nature, suggesting its invidious comparisons may also contribute to hostility in envy.
intractable influence and power. From the perspective of the en- Research traditions linking frustration to aggression go back to the
vying person, events that reduce or, better yet, fully remove the monograph by Dollard, Miller, Doob, Mowrer, and Sears, (1939)
envied person’s advantage should serve that part of the self that and find support in some recent studies as well (Berkowitz, 1989,
fears the consequences of inferiority and wishes to enjoy the fruits 1990; Dill & Anderson, 1995). As envy is argued to arise from a
of superiority. frustrated desire for an attribute enjoyed by another person, such
In sum, there are a variety of studies suggesting that envy is a frustration might therefore lead to hostility and aggression.
hostile emotion. Whether it emerges as dislike, as various negative Empirical work shows that undeserved frustration is more likely
outcomes in the workplace or in group settings, as the willingness to produce aggression than deserved frustration (e.g., Kulik &
to give up one’s own highest outcome so that another person’s Brown, 1979; Pastore, 1952; Rule, Dyck, & Nesdale, 1978). If a
advantage can be lessened, or as pleasure when an envied person sense of injustice characterizes the envious response, then anger
suffers a misfortune, even if it is an undeserved one, the hostile and hostility should be all the more likely as well (e.g., R. Brown,
nature of envy manifests itself. 1985; R. H. Smith et al., 1994). Unjust treatment is a sure trigger
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

for anger (e.g., R. Brown, 1985). Many claim that it is the proto-
Why is Envy a Hostile Emotion? typical cause for revenge, for example (e.g., Kim & Smith, 1993;
Kim, Smith, & Brigham, 1998). Recent work in neuroeconomics
It is understandable that another’s superiority might be discon- by Sanfey et al. (2003) using the ultimatum game paradigm is
certing given the likely consequences of inferiority that we out- consistent with this linking of unfair treatment with emotion. In
lined above. But why should another’s invidious advantage create this game, 2 participants split a sum of money; 1 player has the
hostility as well? Why not adapt quickly to one’s inferiority and power to propose the split and the other can either accept or reject
capitulate to this fact? Also, as we emphasize, social norms usually it. It is a useful paradigm for examining social emotions having
dictate that we express pleasure at another’s advantage; doing social comparison origins, because judgments about whether the
otherwise is sinful in most cultures (e.g., Aquaro, 2004; Emmons proposal is fair (in a relative sense) seem to govern participants’
& McCullough, 2004; Schimmel, 1993; Silver & Sabini, 1978). reactions as much as opportunities for gaining money in an abso-
Adaptive reaction to low ranking. Submissive reactions to lute sense. The rational solution is for the proposer to offer the
another’s superiority make evolutionary sense, as failing to act smallest possible share and for the responder to accept it, because
submissively to others who possess superiority can result in being any sum is more than what the responder possessed going into the
harmed (e.g., Allan & Gilbert, 2002; Buss, 1999; Campos, Barrett, experiment. But actual solutions tend to be closer to sharing the
Lamb, Goldsmith, & Sternberg, 1983). But, it also makes sense money equally; otherwise, the unfairness of a less equal offer leads
that disadvantaged people should be on the constant lookout for to rejection. Participants in Sanfey et al.’s study responded to fair
opportunities for self-assertion, a point that Silver and Sabini and unfair proposals. Functional magnetic resonance image scan-
(1978) emphasized. Envy may serve as a kind of call to action, and ning during these responses showed that unfair offers elicited
its hostile nature may make the impulse more resolute. Not only heightened activity in brain areas related to emotion compared
may hostility provide motivation, but it may also give one’s with fair offers, showing that unfairness, defined by being disad-
motivation a focus, as emotion researchers such as Plutchik (2002) vantaged compared with another person, is a clear source of
have argued. A touch of invidious anger and resentment may break emotion.
the envying person free of a prevailing submissive frame of mind, We also argued earlier that invidious resentment is likely to be
override worry about the possible social reprisal, and help focus subjective in kind. The envying person may believe that the
energy on the source of the problem. advantage enjoyed by the envied person is unfair, but the basis of
Beck (1999) argued that hostility may also be a natural, reflex- this belief is unlikely to provide the means to claim unfairness to
ive response to perceived inferiority. Given that people appear to others openly. An existential grievance may often drive this sub-
have a strong and probably adaptive desire to maintain a positive jective sense of injustice. As Parrott (1991) noted, “One’s place in
self-evaluation (e.g., Beach & Tesser, 2000; Silver & Sabini, the world, one’s lot in life, is not quite what one wants, and it all
1978), any unflattering social comparisons undermining this goal seems the luck of the draw” (p. 14). We cited the study by R. H.
and the resulting emotional sting may prompt a natural lashing out. Smith et al. (1994), which demonstrated that such subjective sense
Frank (1999) made the argument that it should be adaptive to be of injustice predicted not only depressive reactions but also hos-
oriented toward bettering one’s condition. This enhances the like- tility. Thus, it appears that the sense of injustice that we argue is
lihood of one’s having a competitive advantage over others. But part of envy may provide another factor that may help explain its
the algorithm “do the best you can” has a problem. It is unclear hostile nature.
when you can relax and feel that you have done enough. By Envy and shame. Another possible explanation for the hostile
contrast, the algorithm “do better than your nearest competitor” nature of envy follows from the connection between envy and
solves this problem in an efficient way. You have done enough shame. Shame is the “painful feeling of having lost the respect of
when you have done better than a particular person. The adaptive others because of the improper behavior, incompetence, etc., of
goal should not be to better yourself but rather to be better than oneself” (Webster’s New World Dictionary of the American Lan-
your competitor. Once this is achieved, further effort is unneces- guage, 1982, p. 1308). Shame appears similar to envy in the sense
sary, except the effort to ensure our continued high status. Possi- that it also involves a sense of inferiority (e.g., Cheung, Gilbert, &
bly, the discontent that is part of envy is the emotional recognition Irons, 2004; Gilbert, 1998; Kaufman, 1989). However, envy is
of inferiority; the hostility is the goad for action. caused by an actual unflattering social comparison, whereas shame
54 SMITH AND KIM

may be caused by feelings of inferiority in a more generalized In sum, the hostile nature of envy may seem puzzling at first.
sense. Shame may also be different from envy in that it involves a However, there are reasons to expect an unflattering social com-
more constant focus on a defective, inferior aspect of the self (e.g., parison to produce more than depressive feelings. Invidious hos-
Lewis, 1971; Tangney, 1995; Tangney & Dearing, 2002), whereas tility can be understood as a self-assertive, immediate response to
envy, arguably, has a dual focus on both inferiority and hostility inferiority, a natural result of frustration (especially perceived
directed at the advantaged person (R. H. Smith, 2000). unfair frustration, however subjectively derived), and a likely
One might predict that persistent focus on inferiority would product of how people cope with the shame associated with their
create largely depressive reactions in which anger is directed envy-causing inferiority and with the additional shame linked to
inward. The existing evidence cited earlier on the inferiority com- their shame.
ponent of envy would suggest so (R. H. Smith et al., 1994). Also,
part of the shame response appears to be of this kind. However, the Private Awareness and Public Acknowledgement of Envy
course that shame takes seems to lead outward as well. Research
suggests that people feeling shame will tend to lash out at others Despite envy’s capacity to cause discontent and despite its
assumed presence in many human interactions, most scholars
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(e.g., Tangney & Dearing, 2002; Tangney & Salovey, 1999), the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ultimate result being what Scheff and Retzinger (1991) character- claim that people deny feeling it. People’s tendencies to misreport
ized as a “shame–rage” spiral. or be mistaken about their emotions are a general feature of many
If shame is a typical response to having a devalued self, then it emotions (Platman, Plutchik, & Weinstein, 1971; Plutchik, 2002;
follows that any case of envy, which by definition involves a sense Watson, 2000), but the nature of envy may amplify such tenden-
of inferiority, can create shame as well. By this logic, a component cies. Many scholars claim that people not only avoid admitting the
of envious hostility is shame based. What may make envy the best feeling to others but that they are also loathe to acknowledge the
label for the emotion is when the cause of the sense of inferiority feeling in private as well (e.g., Foster, 1972; Schoeck, 1969; Silver
results from an explicit social comparison. & Sabini, 1978). These presumed tendencies may largely be be-
A second way that shame may help explain envious hostility cause envy is so painful and self-threatening (e.g., Foster, 1972)
stems from the socially undesirable nature of envy. We have and because societal norms reinforce its repugnant nature (e.g.,
Silver & Sabini, 1978).
emphasized that envy is typically regarded with extreme disap-
Competitive axis of envy. Foster (1972) argued that there are at
proval (e.g., Foster, 1972; Heider, 1958; Parrott & Smith, 1993;
least two distinct axes along which envious reactions can fall. The
Schoeck, 1969). As N. H. Anderson (1968) found, out of 555
competitive axis is more or less out in the open and is governed by
personality-trait words, envious ranked 425th in terms of likeabil-
certain rules through which people can affect their own envy and
ity. This means that when we feel envy we should tend to be
the envy of others. At least in Western culture, advertising takes
ashamed of it, potentially aggravating feelings of inferiority that
advantage of envious feelings to encourage people to “keep up
much more. We may even feel ashamed of our shame. Thus, one
with Joneses” and, through buying various products, literally to
possible outcome resulting from the blending of envy and shame
become one of the Joneses. People arrange their status among
should be a more acute sense of inferiority together with the
others by doing various socially acceptable things, such as con-
enactment of processes making hostility feelings more likely.
suming products in conspicuous ways (Veblen, 1989/1994). Also,
The interconnections between envy and shame and the hostile societies find ways to minimize the possibility that envy felt along
aspects of both emotions are highlighted in Montaldi’s (1999) this competitive axis would cause winning and losing to lead to
analysis of envy. In contrast to theoretical views of envy that argue disruptive behavior. Foster concluded that this type of envy is
for justice feelings in the emotion, Montaldi argued that hostile largely benign in nature and will be readily confessed.
feelings in envy can sometimes result from the combination of Fear axis of envy. Foster (1972) claimed, along with Schoeck
feeling inferior and feeling responsible for one’s inferiority. The (1969), that the second axis, the fear axis, is largely hidden from
failure to match the envied person’s advantage is placed at one’s view and usually emerges in symbolic forms. Because of the acute
own doorstep, giving a damning quality to one’s inferiority. “Merit threat to the self implied by envy and the abhorrent hostility that
envy” is the label Montaldi used for envy of this kind. Such comes with the emotion, Foster argued that people operate on the
deserved inferiority might be expected to create a depressive focus basis of the following set of fears and resulting concerns: (a) fear
on one’s defective self, but Montaldi suggested that in most cases of being envied for the advantages one enjoys and concern over
this is a too threatening an outcome. Envious hostility arises as a shielding oneself from the hostile actions that envy can cause, (b)
defense against the withering implications of blameworthy inferi- fear of being accused of envying others and thus being seen as
ority. It is shameful to be inferior especially if you are partly to believing oneself inferior and having hostility toward others, and
blame, it is shameful to feel hostile toward another person simply (c) fear of recognizing one’s own actual envy and admitting the
because of his or her deserved advantage, and, finally, it is shame- implications of this feeling. According to Foster, these latter two
ful to be a person suffused with shame. It is a demoralizing fears lead people to deceive others about envy they recognize in
mixture. Derogating a rival (usually on moral dimensions that lend themselves and to deceive themselves about their own envy
themselves to biased perception) then serves as a defense against through rationalizations and exculpatory psychological strategies.
the threat to the self as negative feelings about the self become Foster (1972) made the case that many social behaviors, which
projected onto the advantaged person. This possibility is consistent may seem unrelated to envy on the surface, can be attributed to
with research in other domains showing that the self-image threats these envy-related fears. One example Foster gave of symbolic
lead people to denigrate others as a means to restore a favorable behavior is particularly illuminating. He claimed that in Western
self-image (e.g., Fein & Spencer, 1997). European society, the principal way that people express envy is
COMPREHENDING ENVY 55

through its opposite, namely, through compliments. This claim is have good things, derogation beliefs that advantaged people usu-
almost shocking until one considers the possibility that “most ally lack moral character, nondesert beliefs that fortunate people
societies discourage compliments and praise, because they recog- are undeserving of their good fortune, causal-delusion beliefs that
nize them for what they often are–aggressive behavior” (p. 172). advantaged people cause others to be deprived, imagined improve-
The very fact that people can become uncomfortable when they ment beliefs that life would be improved by having other people’s
receive a compliment implies that they are wary of the envy that advantages, and pessimism beliefs that good fortune is unlikely to
may partly motivate it. Despite their surface friendliness and be obtained. Although this scale is still in its developmental stage,
warmth, compliments can be warning signs that the person making preliminary evidence supports the idea that it taps aspects of envy
the compliment would very much like to take away what we enjoy. not captured by an envy scale having high face validity. The DES
Foster emphasized that this does not mean that every time a person (R. H. Smith et al., 1999) was positively correlated with three of
gives a compliment, it carries the opposite unconscious aggressive the envy schemas: leveling, improvement, and pessimism. Dero-
meaning. Furthermore, there is no research that confirms Foster’s gation and nondesert, two features clearly linked with definitional
claim. But it is worth entertaining the possibility that compliments features of envy, were unrelated to the DES. Thus, being willing to
commonly represent behaviors “stemming from envy at a deep admit to envy does not necessarily mean that one will admit to, or
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

psychological level” (Foster, 1972, p. 173). Clearly, the possible be aware of, all the feelings that envy might actually entail. People
symbolic character of envy-inspired behavior should make it dif- feeling envy wish to avoid concluding that their envy comes with
ficult to recognize where envy resides and how it manifests itself. hostility and resentment, which may help explain why they can
Implications for measuring envy: Direct and indirect measures report envy in the first place. Montaldi’s research suggests that
of dispositional envy. The tendency for people to avoid admitting assessing envy schemas may be an important way of tapping
their envy to others and even to themselves presents challenges for envious feelings that evade awareness or self-report.
researchers who try to study the emotion. For example, one ap- Research on envy and schadenfreude. Researchers studying
proach to studying envy has taken a dispositional perspective that envy cannot ignore the problems of awareness and accurate re-
typically involves participants giving retrospective assessments of porting. A case in point is the research on envy and schadenfreude
their tendencies to feel envy and associated feelings. A recently described earlier. Although two studies have found that envy
developed dispositional envy scale (DES; R. H. Smith, Parrott, creates the conditions for schadenfreude (Brigham et al., 1997;
Diener, Hoyle, & Kim, 1999) consists of eight items, four of which R. H. Smith et al., 1996), two other studies fail to support this link
ask respondents to indicate the degree and frequency of their (Feather & Sherman, 2002; Hareli & Weiner, 2002). The support-
experiences of envy. It is certainly possible that many respondents ing studies differ from the nonsupporting studies in three arguably
who complete the scale tend to underreport their feelings because critical ways. First, the supporting studies created actual envy
of the socially undesirable nature of the term envy. This problem using target persons who were perceived as real by participants,
may have been partially addressed by additional items designed to whereas the nonsupporting studies asked participants to imagine
assess reactions tied to envious reactions without containing the their reactions to hypothetical situations. Second, the supporting
word envy (e.g., “It somehow does not seem fair some people seem studies manipulated and measured envy using a cover story that
to have all the talent”). Another measure of enviousness developed successfully convinced participants that the focus of the research
by Gold (1996) uses a number of items containing familiar idioms was on issues unrelated to envy. The measures of envy were placed
that were argued to encourage truthful responses. A facet of the among filler items that probably served to distract participants
scale measures spitefulness over another person’s enjoyment of a from the actual focus as well. Finally, the supporting studies used
desired attribute. Instead of asking whether participants felt spite- multiple items to measure envy that covered the range of affects
ful, an item reads, “It makes me feel good to rain on someone’s theoretically associated with envy (such as feelings of inferiority,
parade.” Both the DES and the measure developed by Gold have hostility, and invidious resentment), whereas the nonsupporting
proven reliable and are correlated with other measures in ways that studies used items that probably tapped benign aspects of envy.
suggest their construct validity. In the case of the DES, it also Given the nature of envy and the problems of awareness and social
predicted envious reactions to target persons beyond measures of desirability, it is unlikely that the nonsupporting studies either
self-esteem and neuroticism. Nonetheless, both scales probably manipulated envy or measured it effectively. Even the supporting
fail to capture all aspects of envy, especially those aspects asso- studies are probably tapping a fraction of the envious affect that
ciated with its more self-threatening features. may actually be present in many participants, either because of
The inherent weakness found in a more or less direct measure of these participants’ lack of awareness of their envy or their prefer-
dispositional envy has prompted empirical efforts by Montaldi ence to let their envy go undetected.
(1999) to measure envy in a much less direct way. In his unpub- Possible insights using a psychoanalytic perspective. Grap-
lished Survey of Values scale, Montaldi avoided items having any pling with the implications of people’s lack of awareness of their
clear face validity as a self-report measure of envy. The goal was own envy raises complexities that may suggest the usefulness of
to use items that tap envy by assessing beliefs or schemas, which taking into account psychoanalytic perspectives. Psychoanalytic
reflect envious feelings indirectly. The items covered an assort- traditions (e.g., Etchegoyen & Nemas, 2003; Klein, 1957;
ment of domains (e.g., grades, intelligence, money, physical at- Laverde-Rubio, 2004) are often associated with forms of envy
tractiveness) and focused on a number of what Montaldi suggested focused on the penis, womb, and breast (Klein, 1957), and many
are envy-related schemas. These schemas entailed zero-sum be- contemporary psychologists have probably found such entry points
liefs that the world is structured such that only some can have the into understanding envy difficult places to start. Also, the testabil-
good things in life, leveling beliefs that the world would be a better ity of psychoanalytic ideas presents hard challenges (e.g., Clarke,
place if no one could have good things rather than if only a few 2003). Nonetheless, the literature on this approach is vast and still
56 SMITH AND KIM

evolving (e.g., Ashwin, 2005; Etchegoyen & Nemas, 2003), and, Some people will probably recognize their envy in the tradi-
given its emphasis on unconscious processes, a psychoanalytic tional sense of how it is defined (that is, envy proper rather than
perspective may be worth considering. For example, the contem- benign envy). They will realize that their envy is the basis for their
porary psychoanalytic researchers Etchegoyen and Nemas (2003) hostile feelings, and they will sense that their private, subjective
have suggested that envy can involve an unconscious projective sense of injustice and resentment is probably a weak basis for this
identification with the envied person, who represents the image of ill will and for their begrudging the envied person’s advantage.
whom the envying person would want to be in the ideal sense. But, Although this acknowledgement of envy and its causes is threat-
because feelings of inferiority partly motivate such identification, ening, positive and constructive responses are possible. People
this idealization is blended with resentment and derogation. Iden- feeling envy might select other domains to link with their self
tification works because people feeling envy can praise the ideal- worth, as Tesser’s (1991) self-evaluation maintenance theory pre-
ized envied object (deflecting the attribution of envy) and yet find dicts, and begin to feel appreciation for the initially envied at-
room to be critical as well (thus appeasing their envy). This tribute and admiration toward the person or group of persons
process, which is presumed to operate unconsciously, although enjoying the attribute. Eventually, they may feel grateful for their
own advantages on other domains, and enhanced subjective well-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

difficult to capture empirically, might indeed help characterize the


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

full range of ways that envy can operate. It is worth noting that being might result.
Foster’s (1972) claim that compliments are often symbolic exam- Efforts to cope may also lead to a chronic focus on their
ples of unacknowledged envy fits the psychoanalytically guided inferiority, which might exacerbate shame and ultimately lead to
perspective. “Against whom is that eulogy directed?” (Unamuno, depression. However, as would be expected by evidence for de-
1917/1996, p. 103, cited by Foster, 1972, p. 173) says the envious fensive processes in other domains (e.g., Aronson, 1992; Harmon-
protagonist in one of Unamuno’s classic Spanish stories, when he Jones, 2000; Miller & Ross, 1975; Montaldi, 1999; Paulhus,
hears someone speak well of another. Possibly, one can sometimes Fridhandler, & Hayes, 1997; Pyszczynski, Greenberg, & Solomon,
detect undercurrents of envy-inspired hostility in high praise. 2005; Tesser, 2000), defensive reactions are probably more com-
mon. If scholarly opinions are correct, people feeling envy will
tend to find ways to justify their hostility, such as by making
The Transmutational Nature of Envy downward comparisons (Gibbons & Gerrard, 1991; Wills, 1981),
especially on moral domains (Montaldi, 1999), thus rendering the
Because envy usually reflects painful inferiority and an inap- advantaged person or persons undeserving of their advantage by
propriate hostility aimed at another person, we agree with schol- virtue of their perceived moral failings. This downward compari-
arly views that it is also likely that people suppress its public son process may also be an alternative path from shame in some
expression and even deny the feeling to themselves. As Farber cases and may contribute to a shame–rage spiral, a path consistent
(1966) argued, these features probably give envy a protean (Far- with Scheff and Retzinger’s (1991) analysis mentioned earlier.
ber, 1966, p. 36) character that should make it exceedingly difficult In general, defensive responses might be expected to be the rule,
to locate and follow its course. Indeed, for the greater part of leading to an almost immediate transmuting of the feeling as soon
the time it may be “. . . suppressed, preempted, or transmuted to as it arises. In this “transmutation zone,” people feeling envy are
some other emotion” (Elster, 1998, p. 165). Its “talent for dis- likely to nurture and feed the initial subjective sense of injustice
guise” (Farber, 1966, p. 36) may trick the observer as well as and find ways to perceive the envied as undeserving of their
“. . . the envious one himself, whose rational powers may lend advantages because of their moral failings. The label of envy for
almost unholy assistance to the need for self-deception” (Farber, their feelings might be avoided because this undermines the legit-
1966, p. 36). imacy of their envy-based hostility. Over time, if a focus on the
Arguments for the transmuting and protean character of envy undeserved advantage of the envied dominates their thinking
suggest that it is an emotion that is best understood as an episode rather than their own contribution to the situation, people feeling
“unfolding in time” (Parrott, 1991, p. 12). People feel envy when envy might be able to convince themselves that they have an
they notice an advantage enjoyed by another person or group of increasingly legitimate cause for feeling hostile, although they may
persons. This advantage creates envy because this person or group still be wary of publicizing their feelings. This seemingly legiti-
of persons is similar in most respects except for the advantage mate but largely private grievance should tend to give free license
itself, because the advantage is on a domain of high self-relevance, for envious people to engage in a variety of indirectly hostile
and because the advantage seems unobtainable. This coming to- behaviors (e.g., negative gossip and backbiting). As the brute fact
gether of similarity, high self-relevance, and low control creates a of inferiority lingers in consciousness despite feelings of private
set of likely cognitive– emotional appraisals and reactions (e.g., grievance, however, a whole set of negative outcomes may ensue.
recognition of inferiority, frustration over the low likelihood of These might include an array of subrosa actions designed to
achieving a desired goal, a subjective sense of injustice, and undermine and sabotage the advantaged person’s position, actions
adaptive self-assertion) that then produces the blend of inferiority, only slightly tainted in any conscious way by their invidious roots
hostile, and resentful feelings often given the label of envy. These and largely colored by a cynical, hateful outlook on life.
blended feelings are, arguably, the first pangs of envy. But, these Alternately, people experiencing this transmutational process
incipient feelings start a process that can take different paths as the might find additional ways to focus on both the moral baseness of
envying person copes with the threatening nature of the emotion. the target of their envy and on the seemingly unfair process
The outcome of this process is likely to evolve quickly into through the advantage came about. This process might begin to tip
displayed emotions and felt emotions that can be described by the transmutational process toward indignation and resentment
labels quite different from envy. proper together with the possibility of convincing others of the
COMPREHENDING ENVY 57

validity of their sense of injustice. And, finally, if people then find Silver, & Smith, 2004) emphasizes that intergroup emotions arise
a way to gain a degree of increased control, the end state will be in situations in which group members psychologically identify
righteous indignation and full-blown resentment and the open with an ingroup in the context of events that bear on the relative
aggression that this state of affairs can grant. well-being of the group. Intergroup emotions are thought to reg-
ulate behavior toward outgroups. As with the other recent models,
Applications and Future Directions the theory assumes that emotions will be particularly good predic-
tors of action tendencies toward outgroups. Identification with the
Existing research on envy has begun to isolate its main features group is key because this identification links group outcomes to
and its consequences for the envying person and for the envied. the self, which then makes any intergroup event (such as a com-
However, many questions about envy remain to be addressed. parison of superiority or inferiority) something that might give rise
Speculations about its capacity for transmutation have little con- to emotions. Although research testing this theory has yet to
firming evidence. Also, the role of envy in various phenomena, examine envy, the work that has been done shows that intergroup
such as the behaviors that Foster (1972) claimed are symbolic of emotions such as anger and fear mediate appraisals of strength
envy, is largely untested. Many of the claims for envy’s role in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(i.e., implied superiority or inferiority) and a desire to take action


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

aggression beg for more extensive examination, although the chal- against the outgroup (anger) or to move away from the outgroup
lenges inherent in doing so are substantial. For the remainder of (fear; Mackie et al., 2000). The model lends itself to incorporating
this article, we describe two examples of research domains in variables such as status differences. Presumably, envy would be all
which envy is either beginning to play a role in theoretical and the more acute in cases of status inferiority when the individual
empirical advances or might be expected to do so. We offer these identifies with the group. Some sort of hostile action directed at the
as examples for how a better understanding of envy and the use of outgroup would be predicted.
this understanding to examine other areas have the potential to Research on intergroup emotions theory suggests a powerful
yield great dividends. We also review efforts aimed at understand- role for intergroup emotions in prejudice. In fact, another study
ing the ways people cope with envy. (E. R. Smith, Miller, & Mackie, 2002) showed that the direct
effects of either positive or negative emotion on prejudice were
Envious Prejudice and Intergroup Relations more powerful than the effects of stereotypes, which were mini-
mal. These results are consistent with the claims of early theorists
Early theoretical and empirical work on stereotyping and prej-
such as Allport (1954) and the contemporary approach of Alex-
udice tended to examine emotional reactions to outgroups in
ander, Brewer, and Livingston (2005) that stereotypes often
general terms (e.g., Cottrell & Neuberg, 2005). Although research
emerge as products of the emotions that the outgroup elicits.
reflected the great varieties of stereotype content (e.g., Asians are
Because of the threatening nature of envy, it is exactly the kind
“hardworking” and “clannish”), people’s emotional reactions to
of emotion that should lead to defensively inspired construals of
outgroups relied on terms such as dislike or like (e.g., Cottrell &
the advantaged outgroup that might serve to reduce this threat.
Neuberg, 2005). More recent research reveals a marked change in
Displays of confidence by envied outgroup members would be
approach. Several groups of researchers have outlined and begun
seen as arrogance, frugality would be seen as stinginess, and the
testing models that suggest a set of specific, rather than general,
emotional reactions to outgroups that reflects the variety found in gathering together of outgroup members would be labeled clan-
stereotype content (Alexander, Brewer, & Livingston, 2005; Esses, nishness. Also, one would predict that envious prejudice will tend
Haddock, & Zanna, 1993; Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, 2002; to be disguised or successfully transmuted into a more socially
Mackie, Devos, & Smith, 2000). Furthermore, these qualitatively palatable and more readily justifiable emotion, such as indignation.
distinct reactions seem to capture well the range of theoretically Hostile actions caused by such prejudice might actually be more
important evaluations people should have about outgroups and extreme than if caused by an emotion less threatening to the self.
suggest the functional value of these reactions (e.g., Cottrell & Ironically, by being extreme, these actions may serve to justify
Neuberg, 2005). themselves and deflect an awareness of the disreputable origins of
Envious reactions are important explicit or implicit ingredients the actions in envy.
in these new models. For example, Fiske et al. (2002) provided Glick (2002) outlines an example of how envy can be applied to
evidence for the role of two factors that they argue play a partic- understanding historical examples of anti-Semitism exhibited in
ularly important role in intergroup relations: status and competi- Nazi Germany. According to Glick’s analysis, many Germans
tion. When outgroups possess stereotypical high status or compe- blamed Jews for the frustrating set of conditions in Germany
tence but are not perceived to be in competition with one’s own following World War I and hated them in part because they were
ingroup, then members of such outgroups produce emotions such perceived as the cause of these conditions. Glick argued that
as respect and admiration. By contrast, when high status outgroups stereotypes about Jews provided the means for attributing blame.
are perceived to be in competition with one’s own group, then On the one hand, because of their apparent disproportionate influ-
envy is a common result. The implications of these distinct emo- ence in various areas of German life, Jews were perceived to have
tions are profound. Whereas respect and admiration should pro- considerable power. On the other hand, they were perceived to
duce benevolent reactions, envy should produce antagonism and have a set of inferior traits (such as deceitfulness and cunning),
begrudging attitudes toward the outgroup’s high status. suggesting that they had taken advantage of, and would continue to
Construing prejudicial attitudes in terms of emotions such as take advantage of, their influence in ways detrimental to Germany.
envy has the potential to elucidate more precisely the nature of This combination of both perceived power and inferior morally
intergroup reactions. Intergroup emotions theory (e.g., Mackie, defective traits created the ingredients for envy as well as the
58 SMITH AND KIM

means to justify hatred toward Jews in such a way that the envy did unhappiness (e.g., Schimmel, 1993). Part of the reason for this
not need to be acknowledged. claim is that envy implies that one’s principal standard for deter-
Glick (2002) argued that this “envious prejudice” explanation mining self-worth is relative (e.g., Russell, 1930; Sullivan, 1956).
has a number of advantages over other explanations. For example, This seems a likely road to unhappiness because, for most people,
the traditional scapegoating hypothesis would suggest that hatred one can argue that there will always be others who compare better.
of Jews would have resulted in the German people’s venting their Scholars contend that another downside of envy is that the desired
frustrations on an innocent but weak target. The focus on a weak attributes themselves may increasingly seem unworthy of one’s
target would result from a displacement process as aggression desire as they remain beyond one’s reach over time and may even
against the actual, more powerful causal agent might bring about become a source of destructive contempt (e.g., Scheler, 1915/
punishment. This displacement might also flow from internal 1961; Schimmel, 1993; Schoeck, 1969). Those qualities in others
conflict in the Freudian sense as well. Frustration originating from that could actually provide pleasure if appreciated for their intrin-
built-up inhibited aggressive and sexual impulses might cause sic value, for example, become sources of pain and targets of
one’s own negative traits, or negative traits in general, to be destruction. Thus, envy is claimed to have an enveloping, corro-
projected on the weak target. One of the points that Glick made to sive character that sours one’s view of life, a kind of “poison
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

counter this kind of explanation is that Jews were hardly power- spreading throughout the body” (Schimmel, 1993, p. 60).
less, and, clearly, were not the weakest target that the Nazis could Evidence suggests that envy may indeed be linked with a host of
have selected. Also, why would Jews continue to be the target of negative mental health outcomes (R. H. Smith et al., 1999). Dis-
hatred even after Germany had regained its military and economic positional envy, as measured by a scale (DES) described earlier,
power? Glick’s model suggests that frustration can lead to scape- predicted invidious reactions to advantaged targets and did so
goating when the target is more plausibly linked with a perceived beyond other individual difference measures of self-esteem, neu-
cause of the frustration. The German’s particular stereotypical roticism, depression, and hostility. This measure was also corre-
views of Jews fostered a belief that they were both powerful (and lated with many indices of well-being. It was negatively correlated
therefore envied) and also ill intentioned (and therefore a threat with self-esteem and various measures of life satisfaction and
that needed to be dealt with). The plausibility of the Jews as a positively correlated with depression, neuroticism, hostility, and
cause for Germany’s woes, together with processes such as self- resentment. The enviousness scale by Gold (1996), also described
protective motivations that envy inspires to avoid inference of earlier, was positively correlated with measures of inferiority feel-
ingroup inferiority and to rationalize invidious hostility, allowed ings, trait anger, irritability, as well as measures of depression,
the Nazis to target Jews with especially vigorous zeal. Further- anxiety, phobic anxiety, somatization, and obsessive compulsive-
more, the mixture of both powerful and negative stereotyping ness, suggesting that enviousness is characterized by general mal-
made it more likely that the envious grounding for this hatred adjustment. Additional research using the DES showed that it was
could go unacknowledged. Yet, because the motivation was invid- negatively correlated with dispositional gratitude (McCullough,
ious, it could lead to actions that were destructive both to Jews and Emmons, & Tsang, 2002; McCullough, Tsang, & Emmons, 2004),
the Germans themselves. adding evidence to the claims that envy makes it less likely that the
Glick’s (2002) analysis highlights the rich potential for exam- good things about oneself and one’s circumstances will be appre-
ining intergroup relations in terms of conditions likely to produce ciated. The tendency to feel grateful, in contrast to dispositional
envious prejudice. The Nazis’ treatment of Jews has features that envy, appears to have wide-ranging positive implications for sub-
make it unique, and yet one can predict that broadly similar jective well-being (e.g., Emmons & McCullough, 2003; Watkins,
conditions exist in other countries that put certain groups at risk for 2004), suggesting that it is no small matter if envy works against
envious prejudice (Glick, 2002). Also, envy is likely to help this tendency.
explain many examples of international conflict, such as the ill will Envy and physical health. The harmful effects of envy may
felt between poorer and richer nations. Unrest between groups extend to physical health. There appears to be a social gradient
within nations may also have envy lurking as a causal force, as effect such that people having lower status across the spectrum
Schimmel (1993) argued. One can guess about the role of envy in (e.g., Krieger, Williams, & Moss, 1997) tend to experience more
various examples of communist takeovers in Russia, China, North stress (e.g., Stansfeld, North, White, & Marmot, 1995) and worse
Korea, and Cambodia where there were mass killings of people health (e.g., Adler et al., 1994; N. B. Anderson & Armstead, 1995;
who had held any sort of prior power, whether they were the Gonzalez, Rodriguez, & Calero, 1998; Illesy & Baker, 1991). Low
wealthy or the educated. It is also possible that some examples of status can reduce people’s ability to control and cope with chronic
global Islamic terrorist activities are partly due to envy of Western stressors, which then takes it toll on physical health through a
power and influence, which then becomes transmuted into justified variety of possible processes (e.g., Dohrenwend, 1973; Langer &
resentment (Zakaria, 2001). The recent models of prejudice focus- Michael, 1963; McLeod & Kessler, 1990; Stansfeld, Head, &
ing on specific stereotype contents and the specific emotions they Marmot, 1998), some having to do with various negative
evoke herald a new generative phase in research on stereotyping cognitive– emotional reactions to low status of the same type also
and prejudice, and fully understanding the role of envy is likely to linked with unhappiness (e.g., Barefoot et al., 1991; Marmot,
be an important aspect of this research. 2004; Matthews, 1989; Ranchor, Bouma, & Sanderman, 1996;
Taylor, Repetti, & Seeman, 1997). Thus far, this research has
focused on negative cognitive– emotional reactions such as depres-
Envy and Mental and Physical Health
sion, anxiety, anger and hostility, and hopelessness and their
Envy and mental health. Many scholars over the centuries and harmful association with poor health. Arguably, a significant share
across cultures claim that envy has a special capacity to create of the negative cognitive– emotional reactions to low status also
COMPREHENDING ENVY 59

involves envy, as envy implicates social comparison processes There is another way of thinking about social support that may
more directly than most other reactions. Inferior status over which have potent implications for the influence of envy on both mental
one has low control, a main ingredient underlying the social and physical health. T. W. Smith et al. (2004) noted that giving
gradient effect on health, is a root cause of envy as well. Further- social support may be just as important in terms of health, if not
more, because of envy’s conceptual links with other negative more important, than receiving it. In a recent study, the beneficial
cognitive– emotional reactions (i.e., depression, anger, and hostil- health effects of receiving social support were absent when the
ity), it may provide a partial explanation for these particular effects of giving social support were controlled for statistically
reactions and suggest why they seem to cluster together in pre- (S. L. Brown, Nesse, Vinokur, & Smith, 2003). An envious frame
dicting poor health (e.g., Raynor, Pogue-Geile, Kamarck, McGaf- of mind is unlikely to generate genuine beneficence toward others,
fery, & Manuck, 2002). especially toward those who are envied. Envy probably inspires
It is important to stress that envy contains a number of the taking away rather than giving. In sum, the envious person can be
cognitive– emotional elements also thought to help explain the link expected to generate greater hostility from others and to feel more
between low status and poor physical health. As we have noted, hostility toward them through a transactional process that is far
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

people who are envious typically report depressive, unhappy feel- from healthy.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

ings (R. H. Smith et al., 1999) and hostility. These negative Another point about the possible relationship of envy to poor
emotional states, both separately and in combination (see Gallo & health is suggested by the research linking positive emotions with
Matthews, 2003; T. W. Smith, Glazer, Ruiz, & Gallo, 2004, and enhanced physical health. Just as there are health costs associated
Suls & Bunde, 2005, for reviews), appear to set in motion pro- with negative emotions and cognitions, there appear health bene-
cesses that compromise health. It is possible that envious people fits associated positive emotions (e.g., Fredrickson, 2004). To the
suffer because their hostile attitudes and behaviors make it less extent that people are feeling envy, they will not be feeling the
likely that they will receive the benefits of social support, for kind of positive emotions that have been shown to build stable
example (e.g., Cohen & Wills, 1985; O’Neil & Emery, 2002; personal and social resources important for good health, for ex-
T. W. Smith, Pope, Sanders, Allred, & O’Keefe, 1988). Hostile ample (e.g., McCraty & Doc, 2004; Watkins, 2004). Also, envy
people who come to their hostility through an envious world view may be incompatible with religious and spiritual worldviews of the
may be especially unpleasant companions. As envious people are kind that appear linked with enhanced mental and physical health
more likely to see other people’s advantages as undeserved (R. H. (e.g., Emmons & McCullough, 2004; Schimmel, 1993).
Smith et al., 1994), this attitude is liable to show itself through It is important to highlight that, although the link between
inappropriate derogation of the envied person’s achievements, as hostility and other negative emotions and poor health outcomes is
Silver and Sabini’s (1978) study on perceptions of envy shows, solid (e.g., T. W. Smith et al., 2004), the evidence on mediational
and through unappealing backbiting, as Wert and Salovey’s (2004) processes is less strong (e.g., Gallo & Matthews, 2003). No re-
analysis of gossip suggests. As just noted, envious people are search has thus far examined envy as a possible mediator of the
probably less likely to appreciate qualities in others that might link between status and health, for example. In addition, no re-
cause delight in the nonenvious; rather, contempt and ill will may search has examined the possible role of envy in mediating other
be the typical and repellent reaction to these qualities. If the health effects. Little can be claimed for sure about the role of envy
envious feel delight, it may more typically come in the form of as a more distal cause of poor health, as might be the case with
schadenfreude when advantaged people suffer, as we noted earlier. individuals who are envious by disposition. However, the existing
Such proclivities should make for fewer friendships and more research is certainly consistent with arguments suggesting the role
antagonistic interactions, which may then become even more neg- of envy in health outcomes.
ative over time. With regard to hostility, T. W. Smith et al. (2004) argued for an
Just as people avoid depressed individuals (e.g., Coyne, 1976), interpersonal perspective that takes into account variables such as
envious people, whose underlying hostility may leak through their dominance–submissiveness and hostility–friendliness in predicting
attempts to disguise their emotions, may be similarly avoided. the precise character of interactions and isolating the particular
What is more, those actually possessing the envied advantages reactions that have health implications. Tapping individual emo-
have cause to fear the actual effects of this hostility, as Foster’s tions such as envy will be an important part of a full understanding
(1972) analysis suggests. Also, help offered to the envious person of this kind of perspective. Once again, if envy is associated with
may actually go unappreciated. The offer of help implies that the a particular health outcome, then this means that status issues are
envious person needs help, perhaps accentuating the contrast be- likely to be playing a role, that the interpersonal situation is
tween the superiority of the offering person and the inferiority of creating a sense of inferiority, and that this inferiority is creating
the one in apparent need. Considerable research on the “threat to hostility. We noted earlier the research suggesting that subjective
self-esteem” model of reactions to aid (e.g., Fisher, Nadler, & feelings of injustice were linked with both hostility and depressive
Whitcher, 1982) shows the potentially double-edged nature of feelings. There is considerable research suggesting the association
help; for the envious, this may be especially true. Also, gratitude between hostility and depression (Raynor et al., 2002) and a
from the envious is an unlikely result of being helped, as the number of suggestions for why they might be associated (e.g.,
research showing a negative correlation between dispositional Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979; T. W. Smith & Anderson,
envy and dispositional gratitude would indicate (McCullough et 1986; Watson & Pennebaker, 1989). Especially in situations in
al., 2002; McCullough et al., 2004). Whereas gratitude probably which there are variations in social status, understanding the nature
breeds more help, social support should flow away from the of envy with regards to justice feelings presents another way of
envious and ungrateful. considering why hostility and depression covary.
60 SMITH AND KIM

Explicating these processes is a great research challenge, as need to be combined with other measures. Given the likelihood
many people do not recognize their own envy, and many people that many people will deny feelings envy in the first place or will
who do recognize it will hide it. Furthermore, because envy be unaware of their envy, it is unclear that they would accurately
appears to easily transmute itself, as an object of study it presents report how they cope with the emotion. The combining of tradi-
itself as a moving target. But there is reason to expect that the tional self- and peer reports of envy and envy-coping strategies,
successful incorporating of the emotion of envy into health models together with less direct approaches as suggested by Montaldi
should lead to a significant increment in our understanding of how (1999), may be a useful research strategy. Tapping coping reac-
psychological processes can relate to health. tions to envy-provoking situations using daily and momentary
assessment techniques might be used in conjunction with retro-
Coping with Envy spective reports. This is because it is unclear whether people can
accurately recall their specific thoughts, emotions, and behaviors
It is also important to further an understanding of envy because that they may have used for coping at an earlier time, as recent
doing so may lead to better ways to help people cope with envy. research has shown (e.g., Ptacek, Smith, Espe, & Raffety, 1994;
Salovey and Rodin (1988) reported that a mail-in survey on Stone et al., 1998). In the case of coping with envy, the mismatch
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

experience of both envy and jealousy completed by readers of between retrospective reports and momentary measures may pro-
Psychology Today generated twice as many responses compared vide insight into the coping process itself, revealing the path that
with most other surveys done by the magazine. Most of the defensive processes take. Coping is a complex process regardless
respondents recounted efforts to cope with the emotions, and the of domain (see Folkman & Moskowitz, 2004, for a recent review),
majority of these efforts were unsuccessful. and understanding how people cope with envy may tax the limits
Types of strategies for coping with envy. Salovey and Rodin of available research methods. And yet, succeeding in the chal-
(1988) followed up their survey by examining what strategies lenge may yield considerable dividends for understanding coping
people use to cope with envy and jealousy and to assess which processes in the particular case of envy and across other domains
seemed most effective in doing so. Participants completed a ques- in which people are confronted with strong negative emotions.
tionnaire assessing their emotional reactions to envy-provoking The research we have reviewed on the mental and physical
situations in a number of domains and the frequency with which health implications of envy should make it clear that investigating
they used three distinctive coping strategies (self-reliance, self- coping strategies for envy is an important research challenge. The
bolstering, and selective ignoring). A number of important, sug- problems presented by envy are everywhere: for the average
gestive findings emerged. Self-reliance, which included items tap- person encountering frequent unflattering social comparisons
ping emotional control (e.g., “refrain from feeling angry”), (R. H. Smith, 2000), for the psychotherapist (Salovey & Rodin,
perseverance (“don’t give up”), and goal commitment (e.g., “be- 1988) who must often help people in the throes of envy, for the
come committed to the goal”), and to a lesser extent, selective manager contending with the envy-based friction inherent in com-
ignoring (“decide it isn’t so important”) were associated with petitive workplaces (Vecchio, 1997), and for the powerful world
reduced envy. Self-bolstering (e.g., “think about my good quali- leader who must take into account envy emanating from weaker
ties”) was unrelated to reduced envy; however, for those partici- countries, to name just a few examples. But the existing guidelines
pants already experiencing envy, it was associated with less de- for helping people with their envy-based problems seem rudimen-
pression and, along with self-reliance, with less anger, both affects tary. For example, if a manager can promote only a single em-
associated with the experience of envy. Salovey and Rodin (1988) ployee in a small work group, as was the case in Schaubroeck and
interpreted these findings to mean that the more effective strategies Lam’s (2004) study, what would be the best way to announce and
for reducing initial envy appear to be stimulus focused rather than enact the promotion? Are there strategies that would reliably
self-focused. forestall envy or mitigate its presence among those not promoted?
Salovey and Rodin (1988) speculated that the person who has Vecchio’s (1997) analysis of workplace envy and jealousy spec-
accumulated repeated envying-inducing situations, despite at- ulates that praise and recognition would be effective unless they
tempts to dismiss their relevance for the self, may begin to expe- were seen as manipulative. They might also help give the unpro-
rience generalized feelings of sadness and anger along with vari- moted employees “a greater sense of inclusivity in unit activities”
ous self-deprecating thoughts. Self-bolstering may be an effective so as to avoid their feeling “left out” (p. 555). But these and other
strategy for moderating these self-deprecating thoughts and muting possibilities are untested. The next generation of research on envy
these negative affective reactions, as suggested by the research on has many issues to examine.
the buffering effects of having multiple valued aspects of the self
on the depressive effects of specific failure (Linville, 1987; Ro- Summary and Conclusions
thermund & Meiniger, 2004).
Challenges associated with studying coping strategies. More The empirical study of envy is only in its beginning stages.
research is needed to follow-up on the Salovey and Rodin (1988) However, the existing research largely supports centuries of schol-
study. There may be other strategies, depending on the character- arly attempts to characterize its main features and to suggest its
istics of the particular invidious encounter, that may need to be broad and important consequences for the interior life of the
considered (see Vecchio, 1997, for speculations about coping person feeling envy and for interactions with others at the inter-
strategies relevant for the workplace, for example). The findings personal and intergroup level. Envy occurs when a desired advan-
for different coping styles need to be replicated using prospective tage enjoyed by another person or group of persons causes a
methods in which coping styles can be shown to predict effective person to feel a painful blend of inferiority, hostility, and resent-
coping. Also, self-report methods of assessing coping strategies ment. Envy is likely to arise when the advantaged person has
COMPREHENDING ENVY 61

similar comparison-relevant characteristics, when the domain of Beck, A., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, B. (1979). Cognitive therapy
comparison is important to the self, and when the prospects for of depression. New York: Guilford Press.
obtaining the advantage seem unlikely despite this similarity. Ben-Ze’ev, A. (2000). The subtlety of emotions. Cambridge, MA: MIT
Although envy occurs when the advantage is fair by socially Press.
proscribed standards, from the subjective private perspective of the Berke, J. (1988). The tyranny of malice: Exploring the dark side of
character and culture. New York: Summit Books.
envying person, the advantage is likely to be perceived as unfair,
Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and
thus giving envy a resentful character. Because envy contains reformulation. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59 –73.
self-threatening feelings of inferiority and hostility and is socially Berkowitz, L. (1990). On the formation and regulation of anger and
repugnant, it is claimed that people tend not to acknowledge aggression: A cognitive-neoassociationistic analysis. American Psychol-
feeling it, both publicly and privately. Furthermore, it may be that ogist, 45, 494 –503.
people will often cope with their fears about the implications of Brigham, N. L., Kelso, K. A., Jackson, M. A., & Smith, R. H. (1997). The
their envy by transmuting it into emotions more palatable to the roles of invidious comparisons and deservingness in sympathy and
self and to others. schadenfreude. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 19, 363–380.
Although envy appears powerfully linked with an assortment of Brown, R. (1985). Social psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Free Press.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Brown, S. L., Nesse, R. M., Vinokur, A. D., & Smith, D. M. (2003).


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

human ills, it is hard to locate in full view and to measure in a


complete sense, probably because of its repugnant and threatening Providing social support may be more beneficial than receiving it:
Results from a prospective study of mortality. Psychological Science,
nature. Much more research needs to be done on envy so that the
14, 320 –327.
many untested assumptions about the emotion can be examined
Buss, D. M. (1999). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of the mind.
empirically. Nonetheless, existing research already suggests that it Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
indeed plays an important role in numerous outcomes, from prejudice Buunk, B. P., & Gibbons, F. X. (Eds.). (1997). Health, coping, and
to schadenfreude to personal unhappiness. Also, it may possibly play well-being: Perspectives from social comparison theory. Mahwah, NJ:
a significant role in physical health. A case can already be made that Erlbaum.
a vast, varied array of outcomes will be fully understood only when Campos, J. J., Barrett, K. C., Lamb, M. E., Goldsmith, H. H., & Sternberg,
the role of envy is incorporated into explanations. C. (1983). Socioemotional development. In P. H. Mussen (Ed.), Hand-
book of child psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 783–915). New York: Wiley.
References Cheung, M. S. P., Gilbert, P., & Irons, C. (2004). An exploration of shame,
social rank, and rumination in relation to depression. Personality and
Adler, N. E., Boyce, T., Chesney, M. A., Cohen, S., Folkman, S., Kahn,
Individual Differences, 36, 1143–1153.
R. L., et al. (1994). Socioeconomic status and health: The challenge of
Clarke, S. (2003). Psychoanalytic sociology and the interpretation of emo-
the gradient. American Psychologist, 49, 15–24.
tion. Journal for the Theory of Social Behavior, 33, 145–164.
Alexander, M. G., Brewer, M. B., & Livingston, R. W. (2005). Putting
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering
stereotype content in context: Image theory and interethnic stereotypes.
hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310 –357.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 781–794.
Allan, S., & Gilbert, P. (2002). Anger and anger expression in relation to Collins, R. (1996). For better or worse: The impact of upward social
perceptions of social rank, entrapment and depressive symptoms. Per- comparison on self-evaluations. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 51– 69.
sonality and Individual Differences, 32, 551–556. Cottrell, C. A., & Neuberg, S. T. (2005). Different emotional reactions to
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison- different groups: A sociofunctional threat-based approach to “preju-
Wesley. dice.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 770 –789.
Anderson, N. B., & Armstead, C. A. (1995). Toward understanding the Coyne, J. C. (1976). Depression and the response of others. Journal of
association of socioeconomic status and health: A new challenge for the Abnormal Psychology, 85, 186 –193.
biopsychosocial approach. Psychosomatic Medicine, 57, 213–225. Crosby, F. (1976). A model of egoistic relative deprivation. Psychological
Anderson, N. H. (1968). Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. Review, 83, 85–113.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 272–279. D’Arms, J. (2002). Envy. Retrieved October 11, 2005, from Stanford
Aquaro, G. R. A. (2004). Death by envy: The evil eye and envy in the University, Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford Encyclopedia of Phi-
Christian tradition. Lincoln, NE: Universe. losophy Web site: http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/envy/
Aristotle. (1941). Rhetoric. In R. McKeaon (Ed.), The basic works of de la Mora, G. R. (1987). Egalitarian envy: The political foundations of
Aristotle. New York: Random House. (Original work published in 322 social justice. New York: Paragon House Publishers.
BC) DeSteno, D. A., & Salovey, P. (1996). Jealousy and the characteristics of
Aronson, E. (1992). The return of the repressed: Dissonance theory makes one’s rival: A self-evaluation maintenance perspective. Personality and
a comeback. Psychological Inquiry, 3, 303–311. Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 920 –932.
Ashwin, M. (2005). Cronos and his children: Envy and reparation. Un- Dill, J. C., & Anderson, C. A. (1995). Effects of frustration justification on
published manuscript. Retrieved March 5, 2005, from http://human- hostile aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 21, 359 –369.
nature.com/ashwin/index.html Dohrenwend, B. S. (1973). Social status and stressful life events. Journal
Barefoot, J. C., Peterson, B. L., Dahlstrom, W. G., Siegler, I. C., Anderson, of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 225–235.
N. B., & Williams, B., Jr. (1991). Hostility patterns and health impli- Dollard, J., Miller, N. E., Doob, L. W., Mowrer, O. H., & Sears, R. R.
cations: Correlates of Cook–Medley Hostility Scale scores in a national (1939). Frustration and aggression. New Haven, CT: Yale University
survey. Health Psychology, 10, 18 –24. Press.
Beach, S. R. H., & Tesser, A. (2000). Self-evaluation maintenance and Duffy, M. K., & Shaw, J. D. (2000). The Salieri syndrome: Consequences
evolution: Some speculative notes. In J. M. Suls & L. Wheeler (Eds.), on envy in groups. Small Group Research, 31, 3–23.
Handbook of social comparison: Theory and research (pp. 123–140). Elster, J. (1998). Alchemies of the mind: Rationality and the emotions.
New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.
Beck, A. (1999). Prisoners of hate: The cognitive basis of anger, hostility, Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus
and violence. New York: HarperCollins. burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-
62 SMITH AND KIM

being in daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, between socioeconomic status and ischemic heart disease in cohort and
377–389. case-control studies: 1960 –1993. International Journal of Epidemiol-
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (Eds.). (2004). The psychology of ogy, 27, 350 –358.
gratitude. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. Guerrero, L. K., & Anderson, P. A. (1998). The dark side of jealousy and
Esses, V. M., Haddock, G., & Zanna, M. P. (1993). Value, stereotypes, and envy: Desire, delusion, desperation, and destructive communication. In
emotions as determinants of intergroup attitudes. In D. L. Hamilton & B. H. Spitzberg & W. R. Cupach (Eds.), The dark side of close rela-
D. M. Mackie (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactive pro- tionships (pp. 33–70). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
cesses in group perception (pp. 137–166). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Hareli, S., & Weiner, B. (2002). Dislike and envy as antecedents of
Etchegoyen, R. H., & Nemas, C. R. (2003). Salieri’s dilemma: A counter- pleasure at another’s misfortune. Motivation and Emotion, 26, 257–277.
point between envy and appreciation. International Journal of Psycho- Harmon-Jones, E. (2000). A cognitive dissonance theory perspective on the
analysis, 84, 1–14. role of emotion in the maintenance and change of beliefs and attitudes. In
Farber, L. (1966). Ways of the will. New York: Basic Books. A. Manstead & N. Frijda (Eds.), Emotions and belief: How feelings influ-
Feather, N. T., & Sherman, R. (2002). Envy, resentment, schadenfreude, ence thoughts (pp. 185–211). New York: Cambridge University Press.
and sympathy: Reactions to deserved and undeserved achievement and Hartley, L. P. (1960). Facial justice. London: Hamish Hamilton.
subsequent failure. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New York:
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

953–961. John Wiley.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Fein, S., & Spencer, S. J. (1997). Prejudice as self-image maintenance: Heikkinen, E., Latvada, E., & Isola, A. (2003). Envy in a nurse education
Affirming the self through derogating others. Journal of Personality and community. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 40, 259 –268.
Social Psychology, 73, 31– 44. Illesy, R., & Baker, D. (1991). Contextual variations in the meaning of
Festinger, L. A. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human health inequality. Social Science & Medicine, 32, 359 –365.
Relations, 7, 117–140. James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover Pub-
Fisher, J. D., Nadler, A., & Whitcher, A. S. (1982). Recipients to aid. lications. (Original work published in 1890)
Psychological Bulletin, 91, 27–54. Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). The psychology of preferences.
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often Scientific American, 246, 160 –173.
mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow Kaufman, G. (1989). The psychology of shame: Theory and treatment of
from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and shame-based syndromes. New York: Springer.
Social Psychology, 82, 878 –902. Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In D. Levine
Folger, R. (1987). Reformulating the preconditions of resentment: A ref- (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 192–238). Lincoln, NE:
erent cognitions model. In W. T. Smith & J. C. Masters (Eds.), Social University of Nebraska Press.
comparison, social justice, and relative deprivation: Theoretical, empir- Kim, S. H., & Smith, R. H. (1993). Revenge and conflict escalation.
ical, and policy perspectives (pp. 183–215). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Negotiation Journal, 9, 37– 43.
Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2004). Coping: Pitfalls and promise. Kim, S. H., Smith, R. H., & Brigham, N. L. (1998). Effects of power
Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 745–774. imbalance and the presence of third parties on reactions to harm:
Forrester, J. (1997). Dispatches for the Freud wars. Cambridge, MA: Upward and downward revenge. Personality and Social Psychology
Harvard University Press. Bulletin, 24, 353–361.
Foster, G. (1972). The anatomy of envy. Current Anthropology, 13, 165– Klein, M. (1957). Envy and gratitude. London: Tavistock Institute.
202. Krieger, N., Williams, D. R., & Moss, N. E. (1997). Measuring social class
Frank, R. H. (1999). Luxury Fever. New York: Free Press. in U.S. Public Health Research: Concepts, methodologies, and guide-
Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Gratitude, like other positive emotions, broadens lines. Annual Review of Public Health, 18, 341–378.
and builds. In R. A. Emmons & M. E. McCullough (Eds.), The psychol- Kulik, J. A., & Brown, R. (1979). Frustration, attribution of blame, and
ogy of gratitude (pp. 145–166). New York: Oxford University Press. aggression. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 15, 183–194.
Gallo, L. C., & Matthews, K. A. (2003). Understanding the association Langer, T. S., & Michael, S. T. (1963). Life stress and mental health. New
between socioeconomic status and physical health: Do negative emo- York: Free Press.
tions play a role? Psychological Bulletin, 129, 10 –51. Laverde-Rubio, E. (2004). Envy: One or many? International Journal of
Gastorf, J. W., & Suls, J. (1978). Performance evaluation via social Psychoanalysis, 85, 401– 418.
comparison: Performance similarity versus related-attribute similarity. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford Uni-
Social Psychology, 41, 297–305. versity Press.
Gibbons, F. X., & Gerrard, M. (1991). Downward comparison and coping Leach, C. W., Spears, R., Branscombe, N. R., & Doosje, B. (2003).
with threat. In J. M. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social comparison: Malicious pleasure: Schadenfreude at the suffering of another group.
Contemporary theory and research (pp. 317–346). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 932–943.
Gilbert, P. (1998). What is shame? Some core issues and controversies. In Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York: International
P. Gilbert & B. Andrews (Eds.), Shame: Interpersonal behavior, psy- Universities Press.
chopathology, and culture (pp. 3–38). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford Linville, P. L. (1987). Self-complexity as a cognitive buffer against stress-
University Press. related illness and depression. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Glick, P. (2002). Sacrificial lambs dressed in wolves clothing: Envious chology, 53, 663– 676.
prejudice, ideology, and the scapegoating of Jews. In L. S. Newman & Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the
R. Erber (Eds.), What social psychology can tell us about the Holocaust impact of role models on the self. Journal of Personality and Social
(pp. 113–142). Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press. Psychology, 73, 91–103.
Goethals, G. R., & Darley, J. M. (1977). Social comparison theory: An Lowry, L. (1993). The giver. New York: Houghton Mifflin/Walter Lor-
attributional approach. In J. M. Suls & R. L. Miller (Eds.), Social raine Books.
comparison processes (pp. 259 –278). Washington, DC: Hemisphere. Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions:
Gold, B. T. (1996). Enviousness and its relationship to maladjustment and Explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context. Journal
psychopathology. Personality and Individual Differences, 21, 311–321. of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 602– 616.
Gonzalez, M. A., Rodriguez, A. F., & Calero, J. R. (1998). Relationship Mackie, D. M., Silver, L., & Smith, E. R. (2004). Intergroup emotions:
COMPREHENDING ENVY 63

Emotions as an intergroup phenomenon. In L. Z. Tiedens & C. W. Leach Laham (Eds.), Social motivation: Conscious and unconscious processes
(Eds.), The social life of emotions (pp. 227–245). Cambridge, United (pp. 40 –54). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Kingdom: Cambridge University Press. Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Arndt, J., & Schimel, J.
Marmot, M. (2004). The status syndrome. New York: Times Books. (2004). Why do people need self-esteem? A theoretical and empirical
Matthews, K. A. (1989). Are sociodemographic variables markers for review. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 435– 468.
psychological determinants of health? Health Psychology, 8, 641– 648. Ranchor, A. V., Bouma, J., & Sanderman, R. (1996). Vulnerability and
McCraty, R., & Doc, C. (2004). The grateful heart: The psychophysiology of social class: Differential patterns of personality and social support over
appreciation. In R. A. Emmons & M. E. McCullough (Eds.), The psychol- the social classes. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 229 –237.
ogy of gratitude (pp. 230 –255). New York: Oxford University Press. Rand, A. (1971). Return of the primitive: The anti-industrial revolution.
McCullough, M. E., Emmons, R. A., & Tsang, J. A. (2002). The grateful New York: Meridian.
disposition: A conceptual and empirical topography. Journal of Person- Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
ality and Social Psychology, 82, 112–127. Press.
McCullough, M. E., Tsang, J. A., & Emmons, R. A. (2004). Gratitude in Raynor, D. A., Pogue-Geile, M. F., Kamarck, T. W., McGaffery, J. M., &
intermediate affective terrain: Links of grateful moods to individual Manuck, S. B. (2002). Covariation of psychosocial characteristics asso-
ciated with cardiovascular disease: Genetic and environmental influ-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

differences and daily emotional experience. Journal of Personality and


ences. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 191–203.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Social Psychology, 86, 295–309.


McLeod, J. D., & Kessler, R. C. (1990). Socioeconomic status differences Rothermund, K., & Meiniger, C. (2004). Stress-buffering effects of self-
in vulnerability to undesirable life events. Journal of Health and Social complexity: Reduced affective spillover or self-regulatory processes?
Behavior, 31, 162–172. Self and Identity, 3, 263–281.
Miller, D. T., & Ross, M. (1975). Self-serving biases in the attribution of Rule, B. G., Dyck, R., & Nesdale, A. R. (1978). Arbitrariness of frustra-
causality: Fact of fiction? Psychological Bulletin, 82, 513–523. tion: Inhibition or instigation effects on aggressions. European Journal
Montaldi, D. F. (1999). Dispositional envy: Envy types and schemas. of Social Psychology, 8, 237–244.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Texas A&M University. Russell, B. (1930). The conquest of happiness. New York: Liveright.
Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment: Mech- Salovey, P., & Rodin, J. (1984). Some antecedents and consequences of
social-comparison jealousy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
anisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 110, 472– 489.
ogy, 47, 780 –792.
Neu, J. (1980). Jealous thoughts. In R. Rorty (Ed.), Explaining emotions
Salovey, P., & Rodin, J. (1986). The differentiation of social-comparison
(pp. 425– 463). Berkeley: University of California Press.
jealousy and romantic jealousy. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Nietzsche, F. (1967). On the genealogy of morals (W. Kaufmann & R. J.
chology, 50, 1100 –1112.
Hollingdale, Trans.). New York: Random House. (Original work pub-
Salovey, P., & Rodin, J. (1988). Coping with envy and jealousy. Journal
lished in 1887)
of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7, 15–33.
Nozick, R. (1974). Anarchy, state, and utopia. New York: Basic Books.
Salovey, P., & Rodin, J. (1991). Provoking jealousy and envy: Domain
O’Neil, J. N., & Emery, C. F. (2002). Psychosocial vulnerability, hostility, and
relevance and self-esteem threat. Journal of Social and Clinical Psy-
family history of coronary heart disease among male and female college
chology, 10, 395– 413.
students. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 9, 17–36.
Salovey, P., & Rothman, A. J. (1991). Envy and jealousy: Self and society.
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of
In P. Salovey (Ed.), The psychology of jealousy and envy (pp. 271–286).
emotions. New York: Cambridge.
New York: Guilford Press.
Parks, C. D., Rumble, A. C., & Posey, D. C. (2002). The effects of envy
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D.
on reciprocation in a social dilemma. Personality and Social Psychology
(2003, June 13). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the
Bulletin, 28, 509 –520. Ultimatum Game. Science, 300, 1755–1758.
Parrott, W. G. (1991). The emotional experiences of envy and jealousy. In Schaubroeck, J., & Lam, S. K. (2004). Comparing lots before and after:
P. Salovey (Ed.), The psychology of jealousy and envy (pp. 3–30). New Promotion rejectees’ invidious reactions to promotees. Organizational
York: Guilford. Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 94, 33– 47.
Parrott, W. G., & Smith, R. H. (1993). Distinguishing the experiences of Scheff, T. J., & Retzinger, S. M. (1991). Emotions and violence. Toronto,
envy and jealousy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, Canada: Lexington Books.
906 –920. Scheler, M. (1961). Ressentiment (L. A. Coser, Ed., W. W. Holdhein,
Pastore, N. (1952). The role of arbitrariness in the frustration–aggression Trans.). Glencoe, IL: Free Press. (Original work published in 1915).
hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 728 –731. Schimmel, S. (1993). Seven Deadly Sins. New York: Bantam Doubleday.
Paulhus, D. L., Fridhandler, B., & Hayes, S. (1997). Psychological defense: Schoeck, H. (1969). Envy: A theory of social behavior. New York: Har-
Contemporary theory and research. In J. A. Johnson & R. Hogan (Eds.), court, Brace, and World.
Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 543–579). San Diego, CA: Silver, M., & Sabini, J. (1978). The perception of envy. Social Psychology
Academic Press. Quarterly, 41, 105–117.
Platman, S. R., Plutchik, R., & Weinstein, B. (1971). Psychiatric, physio- Smith, E. R., Miller, D. A., & Mackie, D. M. (2002). Effects of intergroup
logical, behavioral, and self-report measures in relation to a suicide contact and political predispositions on prejudice: Role of intergroup
attempt. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 8, 127–137. emotions. Unpublished manuscript, Purdue University.
Plutchik, R. (2002). Emotions and life: Perspectives from psychology, Smith, H. J., & Kessler, T. (2004). Group-based emotions and intergroup
biology, and evolution. Washington, DC: American Psychology Asso- behavior: The case of relative deprivation. In L. Z. Tiedens & C. W.
ciation. Leach (Eds.), The social life of emotions (pp. 292–313). Cambridge,
Ptacek, J. T., Smith, R. E., Espe, K., & Raffety, B. (1994). Limited United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.
correspondence between daily coping reports and retrospective coping Smith, R. H. (1991). Envy and the sense of injustice. In P. Salovey (Ed.),
recall. Psychological Assessment, 6, 41– 49. Psychological perspectives on jealousy and envy (pp. 79 –99). New
Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (2005). The machine in the York: Guilford.
ghost: A dual process model of defense against conscious and uncon- Smith, R. H. (2000). Assimilative and contrastive emotional reactions to
scious death-related thought. In J. P. Forgas, K. D. Williams, & S. M. upward and downward comparisons. In J. M. Suls & L. Wheeler (Eds.),
64 SMITH AND KIM

Handbook of social comparison: Theory and research (pp. 173–200). Teitelbaum, (1976). Tunisia. The leer and the loom—Social controls on
New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. handloom weavers. In C. Moloney (Ed.), The evil eye (pp. 223–279).
Smith, R. H. (2004). Envy and its transmutations. In L. Z. Tiedens & C. W. New York: Columbia University Press.
Leach (Eds.), The social life of emotions (pp. 43– 63). Cambridge, Tesser, A. (1991). Emotion in social comparison and reflection processes.
United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press. In J. M. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social comparison: Contemporary
Smith, R. H., Diener, E., & Wedell, D. H. (1989). Intrapersonal and social theory and research (pp. 115–145). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
comparison determinants of happiness: A range-frequency analysis. Tesser, A. (2000). On the confluence of self-esteem maintenance mecha-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 317–325. nisms. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 290 –299.
Smith, R. H., Kim, S. H., & Parrott, W. G. (1988). Envy and jealousy Testa, M., & Major, B. (1990). The impact of social comparison after
semantic problems and experiential distinctions. Personality and Social failure: The moderating effects of perceived control. Basic and Applied
Psychology Bulletin, 14, 401– 409. Social Psychology, 11, 205–218.
Smith, R. H., Parrott, W. G., Diener, E., Hoyle, R. H., & Kim, S. H. (1999). Thernstrom, M. (1998). Halfway heaven: Diary of a Harvard murder. New
Dispositional envy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, York: Doubleday.
1007–1020. Unamuno, M. (1996). Abel Sanchez and other short stories. New York:
Smith, R. H., Parrott, W. G., Ozer, D., & Moniz, A. (1994). Subjective Gateway Editions. (Original work published in 1917).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

injustice and inferiority as predictors of hostile and depressive feelings Veblen, T. (1994). The theory of the leisure class: An economic study of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

in envy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 705–711. institutions. New York: Penguin Classics. (Original work published in
Smith, R. H., Turner, T., Leach, C. W., Garonzik, R., Urch-Druskat, V., & 1989)
Weston, C. M. (1996). Envy and schadenfreude. Personality and Social Vecchio, R. P. (1997). It’s not easy being green: Jealousy and envy in the
Psychology Bulletin, 22, 158 –168. workplace. In R. Vecchio (Ed.), Leadership: Understanding the dynam-
Smith, T. W., & Anderson, N. (1986). Models of personality and disease: ics of power and influence in organizations (pp. 542–562). Notre Dame,
An interactional approach to Type A behavior and cardiovascular risk. IN: University of Notre Dame Press.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1166 –1173. Vecchio, R. P. (2000). Negative emotion in the workplace: Employee
Smith, T. W., Glazer, K., Ruiz, J. M., & Gallo, L. C. (2004). Hostility, jealousy and envy. International Journal of Stress Management, 7,
anger, aggressiveness, and coronary heart disease: An interpersonal 161–179.
perspective on personality, emotion, and health. Journal of Personality, Vecchio, R. P. (2005). Explorations in employee envy: Feeling envious and
72, 1217–1270. feeling envied. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 69 – 81.
Smith, T. W., Pope, M. K., Sanders, J. D., Allred, K. D., & O’Keefe, J. L. Walcot, P. (1978). Envy and the Greeks. Warminster, United Kingdom:
(1988). Cynical hostility at home and work: Psychosocial vulnerability Aris & Phillips.
across domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 22, 525–548. Walker, I., & Smith, H. J. (2002). Relative deprivation: Specification,
Stansfeld, S. A., Head, J., & Marmot, M. G. (1998). Explaining social class development, and integration. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge
differences in depression and well-being. Social Psychiatry and Psychi- University Press.
atric Epidemiology, 33, 1–9. Walker, I., Wong, N. K., & Kretzschmar, K. (2002). Relative deprivation
Stansfeld, S. A., North, F. M., White, I., & Marmot, M. G. (1995). Work and attribution: From grievance to action. In I. Walker (Ed.), Relative
characteristics and psychiatric disorder in civil servants in London. deprivation: Specification, development and integration (pp. 288 –312).
Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 49, 48 –53. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Stone, A. A., Schwartz, J. E., Neale, J. M., Shiffman, S., Marco, C. A., Waska, R. (2004). Greed and the frightening rumble of psychic hunger.
Hickcox, M., et al. (1998). A comparison of coping assessed by ecolog- The American Journal of Psychoanalysis, 64, 253–266.
ical momentary assessment and retrospective recall. Journal of Person- Watkins, P. C. (2004). Gratitude and subjective well-being. In R. A.
ality and Social Psychology, 74, 1670 –1680. Emmons & M. E. McCullough (Eds.), The psychology of gratitude (pp.
Sullivan, H. S. (1956). Clinical studies in psychiatry. New York: W. W. 167–192). New York: Oxford University Press.
Norton. Watson, D. (2000). Mood and temperament. New York: Guilford.
Suls, J. M., & Bunde, J. (2005). Anger, anxiety, and depression as risk factors Watson, D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health complaints, stress, and
for cardiovascular disease: The problems and implications of overlapping distress: Exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychological
affective dispositions. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 260 –300. Review, 96, 234 –254.
Suls, J. M., & Miller, R. L. (Eds.). (1977). Social comparison processes: Webster’s New World Dictionary of the American Language. (1982). New
Theoretical and empirical perspectives. Oxford, United Kingdom: York: Simon & Schuster.
Hemisphere. Wert, S. R., & Salovey, P. (2004). A social comparison account of gossip.
Suls, J. M., & Wheeler, L. (Eds.). (2000). Handbook of social comparison: Review of General Psychology, 8, 122–137.
Theory and research. New York: Plenum Press. Wills, T. A. (1981). Downward comparison principles in social psychol-
Tangney, J. (1995). Shame and guilt in interpersonal relationships. In J. ogy. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 245–271.
Tangney & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: Shame, guilt, Wood, J. V., & Wilson, A. E. (2003). How important is social comparison?
embarrassment, and pride (pp. 114 –139). New York: Guilford Press. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity
Tangney, J., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and guilt. New York: (pp. 344 –366). New York: Guilford Press.
Guilford Press. Zakaria, F. (2001, October 15). The politics of rage: Why do they hate us?
Tangney, J., & Salovey, P. (1999). Problematic social emotions: Shame, [Electronic version]. Newsweek, 22– 40.
guilt, jealousy, and envy. In R. M. Kowalski & M. R. Leary (Eds.), The Zizzo, D. J., & Oswald, A. (2001). Are people willing to pay to reduce
social psychology of emotional and behavioral problems: Interfaces of others’ incomes? Annales d’Economie et de Statistique, July/December,
social and clinical psychology (pp. 167–195). Washington, DC: Amer- 39 – 65.
ican Psychological Association.
Taylor, S. E., Repetti, R. L., & Seeman, T. (1997). What is an unhealthy Received July 6, 2005
environment and how does it get under the skin? Annual Review of Revision received March 20, 2006
Psychology, 48, 411– 447. Accepted March 27, 2006 䡲

You might also like