You are on page 1of 10

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/232433426

Personality and Uses of Music as Predictors of Preferences for Music


Consensually Classified as Happy, Sad, Complex, and Social

Article  in  Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts · November 2010


DOI: 10.1037/a0019210

CITATIONS READS
36 2,178

3 authors, including:

Patrick Fagan Adrian Furnham

3 PUBLICATIONS   225 CITATIONS   
University of London
1,613 PUBLICATIONS   60,469 CITATIONS   
SEE PROFILE
SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The Breast Size Satisfaction Survey: An International, Collaborative Project View project

Lay beliefs View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Adrian Furnham on 17 February 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts © 2010 American Psychological Association
2010, Vol. 4, No. 4, 205–213 1931-3896/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0019210

Personality and Uses of Music as Predictors of Preferences for Music


Consensually Classified as Happy, Sad, Complex, and Social

Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic Patrick Fagan and Adrian Furnham


University of London University College London

This study replicates the findings of a recent study (Chamorro-Premuzic, Gomà-i-Freixanet, Furnham, &
Muro, 2009) on the relationship between the Big Five personality traits and everyday uses of music or
people’s motives for listening to music. In addition, it examined emotional intelligence as predictor of
uses of music, and whether uses of music and personality traits predicted liking of music consensually
classified as sad, happy, complex, or social. A total of 100 participants rated their preferences for 20
unfamiliar musical extracts that were played for a 30-s interval on a website and completed a measure
of the Big Five personality traits. Openness predicted liking for complex music, and Extraversion
predicted liking for happy music. Background use of music predicted preference for social and happy
music, whereas emotional music use predicted preference for sad music. Finally, males tended to like sad
music and use music for cognitive purposes more than females did.

Keywords: uses of music, personality, trait EI, Big Five, music preferences/tastes

Music is one of the most celebrated yet enigmatic phenomena in (i.e., using music as background to other activities, such as study-
human society. Neanderthals are believed to have engaged in ing, driving, or working), and Openness to Experience is positively
musical activities over 30,000 years ago (Mithen, 2005), and the correlated with intellectual or cognitive use of music (i.e., experi-
significance of music today is demonstrable in terms of com- encing music in a rational way or using it for intellectual stimu-
merce—in 2006, worldwide recorded music sales totaled $31 lation).
billion and the whole music industry was valued at $130 billion Although the above findings have been replicated, quite consis-
(International Federation of the Phonographic Industry, n.d.). In- tently, across cultures, they provide only preliminary evidence for
deed, a large sample survey by Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) found the idea that major individual differences underlie different uses of
that music is one of people’s favorite and most enduring past music and potentially explain how different people use music in
times. Similarly, 94% of young adult Swedes surveyed by everyday life. First, the “catalogue” of traits that have been exam-
Bjurström and Wennhall (1991) described themselves as very or ined in connection to uses of music is far from comprehensive,
fairly interested in music, and a sample of American adolescents which explains why there remains a substantial amount of unac-
reported listening to an average of almost 5 hr of music per day counted variance in each use of music. Moreover, it would seem
between the ages of 12 and 18 years (Davis, 1985)—A figure that individual differences have not been “evenly” examined for
obtained long before the advent of the MP3 players and other each use of music. For instance, the cognitive/intellectual use of
digital media. Hence, it is easy to understand why music has been music has been examined by Openness, IQ, self-assessed intelli-
the focus of a wide breadth of psychological research (Tramo, gence, and typical intellectual engagement (Chamorro-Premuzic &
2001; Schellenberg, 2006).
Furnham, 2007; Chamorro-Premuzic, Gomà-i-Freixanet, et al.,
In a recent series of studies (see Chamorro-Premuzic, Gomà-i-
2009), but the emotional use of music has been linked only to
Freixanet, Furnham, & Muro, 2009, for a summary) Chamorro-
Extraversion and primarily, Neuroticism. Thus the current study
Premuzic, Gomà-i-Freixanet, and colleagues (2009) examined
will examine how the construct of trait emotional intelligence (EI),
psychometrically assessed (e.g., personality, cognitive ability, in-
which refers to a person’s emotional self-efficacy or their per-
terests) individual difference correlates of uses of music, that is,
ceived ability to recognize and control their own and others’
why people use music in everyday life. Their research showed that
emotions (Sevdalis, Petrides, & Harvey, 2007), relates to uses of
Neuroticism is positively correlated with emotional use of music
music.
(i.e., using music to regulate or manipulate affective states), that
Extraversion is positively correlated with background use of music Given that trait EI is assessed via self-reports, it is considered a
personality trait, related to, but different from, the Big Five per-
sonality traits (Chamorro-Premuzic, 2007). As such, it is orthog-
onal to the construct of “ability” EI (Warwick & Nettlebeck,
Tomas Chamorro-Premuzic, Department of Psychology, University of 2004), which is concerned with emotional abilities measured
London; and Patrick Fagan and Adrian Furnham, Department of Psychol-
through maximum-performance tests (Brackett & Salovey, 2006).
ogy, University College London.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Tomas In the current study, we opted for a measure of trait EI because of
Chamorro-Premuzic, Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University the current limitations associated with the objective measurement
of London, New Cross, London, SE14 6NW United Kingdom. E-mail: of the broad ability EI construct, no doubt a function of the
pss02tc@gold.ac.uk subjective nature of emotional experiences (Robinson & Clore,
205
206 CHAMORRO-PREMUZIC, FAGAN, AND FURNHAM

2002). Trait EI, on the other hand, can be measured reliably via the relationship between individual differences and uses of music,
self-reports and has been found to have significant associations to look at trait EI in relation to these, and also to extend uses of
with a number of criteria over and above the Big Five personality music to preferences for actual compositions.
traits, such as goal self-integration (Spence, Oades, & Caputi, As in the Spanish and Malaysian sample replications, structural
2004), job competencies (Van der Zee & Wabeke, 2004), and equation modeling (SEM) was used to test a model in which
recognition of facial expressions of emotion (Petrides & Furnham, personality traits (specifically Emotional Stability, Extraversion,
2003). That said, trait EI is substantially related to the Big Five Openness, and trait EI) predict uses of music, which in turn,
traits of Emotional Stability, Extraversion, and Openness predict music preferences. The hypothesized model is shown in
(Chamorro-Premuzic, 2007), which begs the question of whether Figure 1. As shown, we predicted negative effects of trait EI and
any significant correlation between trait EI and use of music Emotional Stability onto emotional use of music, which in turn,
remains significant when other Big Five traits are considered. was expected to have positive effects on both sad and happy song
There is currently very little literature relating trait EI to music. preferences. Extraversion was predicted to have positive effects on
However, as trait EI is negatively correlated with Neuroticism, one background music use, which in turn, was predicted to have
would expect that individuals with higher trait EI would be less positive effects on preference for social songs. Finally, Openness
likely to use music for emotional regulation and therefore score was expected to have positive effects on cognitive music use,
lower on the emotional use of music. which in turn, was expected to have positive effects on preference
Another area for expansion in the original Chamorro-Premuzic for complex songs.
and Furnham (2007) uses of music study was the fact that self-
reported uses of music may not necessarily correlate with actual Method
preference for appropriate music, though one would expect that
they would. As discussed, there has been a distinction in research
Participants
between musical preferences (e.g., Rentfrow & Gosling, 2003) and
uses of music (Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2007). Conse- In all, 100 participants (42 males and 58 females) took part in
quently, the present study sought to expand on previous findings this study. Opportunistic sampling was used, with the only pre-
by bridging this gap. This was done by examining participants’ requisite being fluency in English. Ages ranged from 15 to 71
preference ratings for music that could be categorized according to years, with a mean of 23.9 (SD ⫽ 7.4).
the dimensions of the Uses of Music Inventory (Chamorro-
Premuzic & Furnham, 2007)— emotional, cognitive, and back- Procedure and Measures
ground. In the case of emotional use, a limitation was previously
identified in which there was no distinction between positive and Participants were recruited over the Internet using emailing and
negative affect. Consequently, this study measured preferences for networking sites such as MySpace and Facebook. The question-
music representative of both positive and negative emotions. Con- naire was both put together (using “RiddleMeThis”) and hosted
sidering emotional music use is predominantly associated with online. Participants completed it wherever they chose and at their
lower Emotional Stability, and that individuals with neurotic ten- own pace. They were initially briefed, asked to give consent and
dencies are more susceptible to negative emotional states (Canli et informed all data was kept anonymous. After this, they were asked
al. 2001), it was expected that an emotional use of music would to provide demographic details (age and sex) to control for their
correlate positively with a preference for sad rather than happy effects on measured variables. Then they completed, in order, the
music. following measures.
Therefore, the general aim of this study was to replicate IPIP. The International Personality Item Pool (IPIP; Goldberg
Chamorro-Premuzic, Gomà-i-Frexanet, et al.’s (2009) findings on et al., 2006) consists of 50 items, of which 10 are summed (with

+ sad
- M (emot)
ES
+
-
happy
EI

+ M (back) +
social
E

O +
+
M (cog) complex

Figure 1. Hypothesized model for individual differences, uses of music, and music preferences. N ⫽ 100;
ES ⫽ emotional stability; EI ⫽ emotional intelligence; E ⫽ extraversion; O ⫽ openness; M(emot) ⫽ emotional
use of music; M(back) ⫽ background use of music; M(cog) ⫽ cognitive use of music.
PERSONALITY AND MUSIC USES AND PREFERENCES 207

some reverse scoring) to measure each of the Big Five personality 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not a lot) to 5 (very much so).
traits: Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Neuroti- These questions measured: sadness (“How sad do you find this
cism and Openness to Experience/Intellect (Agreeableness and music? How likely is it to ‘make someone sad,’ or induce sadness
Conscientiousness were not examined in the current study in line or a melancholic mood?”), happiness (“How happy do you find
with Chamorro-Premuzic, Gomà-i-Freixanet, et al., 2009). The this music? How likely is it to ‘make someone happy,’ or cheer
items involve questions about a number of typical behaviors and them up?”), complexity (“How complex do you find this music?
feelings and are based on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 How likely is it to be enjoyed those with refined or sophisticated
(very inaccurate) to 5 (very accurate). Although the authors do not musical tastes?”), and social suitability (“How suitable do you find
provide norms for comparison, the scale is widely used and has this music as background music to other social activities, such as
good reliability and validity (see Goldberg et al., 2006). studying, working, or having a conversation?”). Scores from all
TEIQue–SF. The Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire five participants were summed to give a score for each musical
Short Form (TEIQue–SF; Petrides & Furnham, 2006) was used to extract in each category. The top five highest scoring compositions
measure global trait EI. This 30-item test is based on a 5-point in each category were kept for the final study. The Cronbach’s
Likert scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely alpha showed a general increase in reliability after the removal of
agree) to gauge responses to statements such as “I’m usually able the lower rated compositions: from .85 to .93 for sad, .70 to .80 for
to find ways to control my emotions when I want to.” The item happy, .76 to .81 for complex, and .45 to .60 for background.
responses are summed (with some reverse scoring) to give a total Across all categories, alpha increased from .74 to .83 (N ⫽ 5 for
trait EI score for each participant. The measure reports good all figures). Reliability from this selection phase was thus accept-
reliability and comprehensive cover of the whole trait EI construct able in every category for musical extracts ultimately selected, and
(see Petrides & Furnham, 2006). they were consequently used in the final study.
The uses of Music Inventory. The Uses of Music Inventory The Music Preferences test included 20 compositions to be
(Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2007) was constructed to mea- rated—five happy, five sad, five complex, and five social (a
sure views about when and why one listens to music. The 15-item selection of these songs is available at http://www.thefagans
inventory is described in detail in Chamorro-Premuzic, Gomà-i- .org.uk/psychol/). For each musical composition, participants were
Freixanet, et al. (2009). asked to rate, on the same 5-point scale, the four questions outlined
Music Preferences. Initially, 40 original compositions were above. This was done to confirm each composition’s suitability to
created by an independent composer (a trained jazz guitarist its relevant category. Participants were also asked to indicate, on
known by one of the experimenters), divided equally into the the same scale, preference for each musical extract by responding
categories of sad, happy, complex, and social. The emotional to an additional item (“How much do you like this music?”). This
valence of compositions tended to be defined as outlined by test took approximately 20 to 30 min to administer. Scores were
Krumhansl (1997): Sad music generally used minor harmonies and computed to give each participant a preference score for each of
slower tempos, whereas happy generally used major harmonies, the four musical composition categories.
faster tempos, and dance-like rhythms. Complex compositions As before, all participants’ scores were summed for each ques-
were generally defined by a higher density of notes, a more tion of each composition: When the pieces were ranked according
advanced playing technique, unusual timing patterns, and sophis- to their total score for each category, they generally fit as intended.
ticated genres such as jazz and blues (Rentfrow & Gosling, 2003). The top five sad-rated musical compositions were intended to
Social music was intended to represent music that is suitable for belong in that category—the same was true of the top five happy
background to other activities (e.g., driving, studying, or working); compositions and four of the top five complex compositions. Only
hence musical extracts were composed that were emotionally one of the top five background-rated compositions belonged to its
neutral, consistent, almost monotonous, in tempo and harmony, appropriate category. However, Cronbach’s alpha (see the Appen-
and generally nonarousing. These musical extracts were also in- dix) for every preference category, including social, suggests good
tended to have little or no overlap with the emotional and cognitive reliability—alpha values higher than .6 are deemed satisfactory for
compositions outlined above. 5-item scales (Youngman, 1979).
All compositions were controlled for in terms of length (around
30-s long), volume and instrument (guitar only). Tempo was Results
controlled for as best possible and did not vary greatly from around
100 bpm; although, as mentioned, there did have to be some Means, standard deviations, and internal consistencies (Cron-
variations to achieve the intended emotional effects. Musical ex- bach’s alpha) for all composite measures are reported in Table 1.
tracts were composed with the aim of avoiding any particular The Uses of Music values were in line with previous studies, with
genre to ensure they had cognitive, emotional, and social value in means of around 14 and standard deviations in the region of 4 (see
their own right, rather than through cognitive associations such as Chamorro-Premuzic, Gomà-i-Freixanet, et al., 2009; Chamorro-
personal memories (Meyer, 1956). The pieces were composed Premuzic, Swami, Furnham, & Maakip, 2009) and all alphas were
with the intention of keeping the four categories independent so acceptable or high. Scores were subsequently standardized to a
that, for instance, complex pieces were not sad, happy, nor social mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1 (z scores).
suited. This was done to avoid overlap in composition preferences
between people of different personality types. SEM
Once these 40 pieces had been recorded, they were evaluated by
five peers for their suitability in their relevant category. For each SEM was next carried out using maximum likelihood via
piece, four questions were posed and responses recorded on a AMOS 4.0 (Arbuckle & Wothke, 1999) to: (a) account for the
208 CHAMORRO-PREMUZIC, FAGAN, AND FURNHAM

Table 1 music, as well as from age onto cognitive music use, were added.
Descriptive Statistics for Target Measures This model (shown in Figure 3) fit the data well: ␹2(55, N ⫽
100) ⫽ 71.6, p ⬍ .05, CFI ⫽ .96. As shown in Figure 3, men
Target measure ␣ No. of items M SD tended to prefer sad music and be higher on the cognitive music
Extraversion .90 10 35.4 7.9 use compared to women, whereas age had negative effects on
Openness .85 10 38.8 5.3 cognitive music use.
Emotional stability .88 10 29.9 8.0
Trait EI .91 30 99.21 16.7
Sad music .78 5 15.3 4.1 Discussion
Happy music .73 5 16.1 4.0
Complex music .70 5 15.3 3.8 The present study attempted to replicate past findings on the
Background music .68 5 13.3 3.5 relationship between broad personality traits and uses of music
M(cognitive) .77 5 12.9 4.5 (see Chamorro-Premuzic, Gomaⱊ-i-Freixanet, et al., 2009), as well
M(emotion) .71 5 17.3 4.4 as extending them by examining whether trait EI explains uses of
M(back) .72 5 15.9 4.8
music over and above the Big Five, and whether individual dif-
Note. N ⫽ 100. M (cognitive) ⫽ cognitive/intellectual use of music; M ferences and uses of music explain preferences for different types
(emotion) ⫽ emotional use of music; M (back) ⫽ background use of of music.
music. With regard to replicating the link between personality (the Big
Five) and uses of music (Chamorro-Premuzic, Gomaⱊ-i-Freixanet,
et al., 2009), results were consistent with past studies in that
overlap between different predictors, (b) account for the overlap Extraversion positively affected people’s tendency to use music as
between different criteria, (c) account for variability in preferences background to other activities, Emotional Stability negatively af-
for specific types of music (removing the variance related to fected people’s tendency to use music for emotional/mood regu-
generic preferences, i.e., for all musical extracts), and (d) assess lation, and Openness positively affected people’s tendency to use
the validity of a hierarchical model in which the same factors are music for intellectual stimulation/in a rational or cognitive way.
both predictors and criteria. Missing values (⬍5% per variable, in These effects were significant even when sex and age were taken
line with Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007) were replaced with the series into account (as shown in Figure 3) and are interpreted in line with
mean at the item level—prior to computing the factor scores. Prior Chamorro-Premuzic, Gomaⱊ-i-Freixanet, et al., 2009.
to analysis, variables (factor scores) were standardized across the In regards to trait EI, this construct was found to correlate nega-
whole sample to a mean of zero and unit variance. We used tively with emotional use of music as predicted (see Appendix).
standardized variables in our analysis because personality and However, when taking into account other personality traits, its effects
music preference scores lie on different scales, as suggested by on uses of music were trivial. This is in line with the strong overlap
Loehlin (2004). Next, we examined assumptions of multivariate between emotional intelligence and the Big Five (see Chamorro-
normality by inspecting Mardia’s coefficient (in line with Ullman, Premuzic, 2007), in particular Emotional Stability. Consequently, it
2007), which indicated that assumptions of multivariate normality may be feasible to disregard trait EI when considering other Big Five
were met and therefore, no transformation or normalization was predictors of uses of music, notably Neuroticism. That said, the
required and maximum likelihood was justified as SEM method. current sample is clearly too small to rule out the potential unique
The hypothesized model (shown in Figure 1) did not fit the data contribution of trait EI to explaining use of music.
well:1 ␹2(37, N ⫽ 100) ⫽ 86.7, p ⬍ .01, comparative fit index
(CFI) ⫽ .83. In line with modification indexes (statistical indica- Predictors of Musical Preferences
tors for model fit improvement) and Silvia and MacCallum’s
(1988) recommendations, three theoretically meaningful paths Three types of predictors of music preferences were examined,
were added in the following order, namely: from Openness to namely (from less to more distal) uses of music, personality traits,
complex music, ␹2(36, N ⫽ 100) ⫽ 77.8, p ⬍ .01, CFI ⫽ .85; and demographics (age and sex), with hypothesized associations
␹diff
2
(1) ⫽ 8.9, p ⬍ .01, from Extraversion happy music, ␹2(35, only for the link between uses of music and preference for music
N ⫽ 100) ⫽ 70.2, p ⬍ .01, CFI ⫽ .88; ␹diff 2
(2) ⫽ 16.5, p ⬍ .01, type. Two of the three hypotheses relating to the uses of music
and from background music use to happy music, ␹2(34, N ⫽ predictors of music preferences were supported (i.e., background
100) ⫽ 56.9, p ⬍ .01, CFI ⫽ .94; ␹diff 2
(3) ⫽ 29.8, p ⬍ .01. The use had positive effects on preference for social music, whereas
modified model (shown in Figure 2) fit the data well. As shown, emotional use had positive effects on preference for sad music);
three of the hypothesized paths, namely from trait EI to emotional however, contrary to predictions there were no significant effects
music use, from cognitive music use to complex music, and from of cognitive music use on complex music preference or on emo-
emotional music use to happy music, were not significant. Re- tional music use on happy music preference. Moreover, back-
moval of these paths did not significantly change model fit, ␹2(37,
N ⫽ 100) ⫽ 57.2, p ⬍ .01, CFI ⫽ .95, ␹diff 2
(3) ⫽ 0.3, p ⬎ .05. 1
The following fit indexes were used: chi-square (Bollen, 1989), which
A final model, which added sex and age, was then tested. The
tests whether an unconstrained model fits the covariance/correlation matrix
hypothesized model was the modified model shown in Figure 2 as well as the given model (although nonsignificant chi-square values
minus the nonsignificant paths (which were removed). No paths indicate good fit, well-fitting models often have significant chi-square
from sex or age onto other variables were predicted, but modifi- values); the CFI (Bentler, 1990), which compares the hypothesized model
cation indexes were examined to identify effects of these variables. with a model based on zero-correlations among all variables (values around
On that basis, effects of sex onto cognitive use of music and sad .90 indicate very good fit).
PERSONALITY AND MUSIC USES AND PREFERENCES 209

sad
-.21* M (emot) .31**
ES
.01

.02 .16*
happy
EI
.34**
.33**
M (back) social
E .25**

.26**
O
.32** complex
-.05
M (cog)

Figure 2. Modified model for individual differences, uses of music, and music preferences. N ⫽ 100; ES ⫽
emotional stability; EI ⫽ emotional intelligence; E ⫽ extraversion; O ⫽ openness; M (emot) ⫽ emotional use
of music; M (back) ⫽ background use of music; M (cog) ⫽ cognitive use of music. All coefficients are
standardized Betas; correlations between ES and E (.31ⴱⴱ), EI and ES (.66ⴱⴱ), EI and E (.41ⴱⴱ), EI and O (.19ⴱ),
M (cog) and M (emot) (.23ⴱⴱ), happy and social (.65ⴱⴱ), happy and sad (.24ⴱ), sad and social (.41ⴱⴱ), sad and
complex (.57ⴱⴱ), complex and social (.19ⴱ), not shown for simplicity. Dashed lines added in the modified model;
dotted lines are nonsignificant hypothesized paths. ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.

ground music use was found to have a positive effect of preference that the extent to which emotional users of music will prefer sad music is
for happy music, whereas Extraversion and Openness were found unrelated to their level of Emotional Stability. Moreover, males showed
to have positive effects on happy and complex music preference, higher levels of liking for sad music (irrespective of their level of Emo-
respectively (these effects had not been predicted as the model tional Stability and emotional use of music) than did females. Previous
hypothesized only indirect effects of individual differences onto findings suggest that females tend to be more concerned than males
music preferences, via uses of music). with music’s ability to aid emotional needs (North, Hargreaves, &
O’Neill, 2000) and are also more responsive than males to the emo-
Preference for Sad and Happy Music tional effects of music (Coffman, Gfeller, & Eckert, 1995; Panksepp,
Participants’ liking of sad music tended to increase as their tendency to 1995). Although this study found no gender effect on emotional use
use music in an emotional way (emotional use of music) increased. We of music, if females are more likely to experience negative emotions
find it interesting that Emotional Stability had no effects on preference for when listening to sad compositions, they may be expected to dislike
sad music even though it affected emotional use of music. This suggests sad music more than their male counterparts.

sex -.21*
-.24**
sad
-.26** M (emot) .29**
ES

happy
EI
.16*
34**

M (back) social
E .21* 34**

O .24**
.33** complex
-.23** M (cog)
age

Figure 3. Modified model including sex and age as predictors. Sex coded: 1 ⫽ males; 2 ⫽ females.
Correlations between sex and ES (⫺.28ⴱⴱ), sex and E (.18ⴱ). See note to Figure 2 for other correlations and
abbreviations. ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.
210 CHAMORRO-PREMUZIC, FAGAN, AND FURNHAM

On the other hand, liking of happy music was positively affected pected to have more refined, sophisticated taste in music and show
by individuals’ Extraversion level, which is in line with Rentfrow similar music preferences. The higher intelligence levels that is
and Gosling (2003). These authors found Extraversion to correlate emblematic of more open individuals may also mean that open
positively with preferences for their “upbeat and conventional” people are both more willing and able to understand the compli-
music dimension, which they found to be low in negative affect cated techniques of advanced performers and richer compositional
and high in positive affect—so they can be thought of as similar to structure of complex music, which is in line with the finding that
the happy music used here. Explanations for this phenomenon can intelligent people prefer complex aesthetic stimuli more than less
be found in the Extraversion facets of both positive emotionality intelligent people do (Barron, 1955; Francès, 1976; Kamman,
and low resting levels of arousal. First, if Extraversion is thought 1966). Finally, there is of course the point that those high in
of as positive emotionality (Canli et al., 2001), then it is easy to Openness to Experience would be more likely to show a prefer-
imagine that extraverts would be drawn to music that reflects this ence for music with timing, rhythms, and melodies that are less
aspect of their personality. Rentfrow and Gosling (2003) suggested familiar to them.
that individuals select music that reinforces their personal dispo-
sitions; extraverts may therefore prefer to listen to positive music Preference for Social Music
as it reflects that part of their character. Second, extraverts’ lower
resting levels of arousal (see above) and higher levels of sensation As predicted, background use of music had positive effects on
seeking (e.g., Zuckerman, Bone, Neary, Mangelsdorf, & Brust- preference for social music. In addition, background use of music
man, 1972) have been found to be predictive of preferences for predicted preferences for happy music. Because background use of
more arousing music (McNamara & Ballard, 1999). Because our music is defined partly by using music in social occasions (one
happy music generally fit Krumhansl’s (1997) criteria of having item was “I enjoy listening to music in social events”), users would
faster tempos and dance-like rhythms, extraverts may have pre- be expected to like music that may impose a positive affect on
ferred them for their attention-grabbing quality. This is echoed by people. In line, it has been found that teenagers with few friends
Rentfrow and Gosling’s (2003) positive correlation between Ex- prefer to listen to music that has themes of loneliness (Burke &
traversion and preference for their “energetic and rhythmic” di- Grinder, 1966).
mension. That said, considering the informal distribution of the survey (in
The effects of Extraversion onto happy music preference also which participants could have been working, talking to friends or
shed light on a theory proposed by Chamorro-Premuzic and Furn- flatmates, or watching TV, while completing the survey), and the
ham (2007). In their paper, they suggested that positive or negative fact that the pieces may have been listened to at the same time as
affect in emotional use was primarily driven by intentional mood rating them, all compositions may indeed have been used as
regulation—for example, listening to a sad music when one is sad background music in this context. Moreover, the classification of
or to happy music when one is happy. The results of the present music as social was the only one that did not clearly replicate the
study suggest that the choice of sad or happy music may be results of our a priori classification.
determined not only by mood, but also personality traits (which In addition to the predicted associations discussed above, the
reflect aggregate levels of mood). In particular, Neuroticism (neg- current results showed that cognitive use of music was affected by
ative emotionality) and Extraversion (positively emotionality) may both gender (males more likely to use music in a cognitive/
be valid markers of people’s general preference for sad and happy intellectual way) and age (negatively). Although these associations
music, respectively. That people would prefer to choose music that are exploratory, they seem consistent with the idea that music is
is congruent with their personality, supporting earlier theoretical used to create an external image to others (e.g., Rentfrow &
claims that individuals actively select environments that reinforce Gosling, 2003; Tarrant, North, & Hargreaves, 2000), especially in
their attitudes and beliefs (Buss, 1987). males, when both playing and listening to music (North et al.,
2000). Along similar lines, evolutionary theory postulates that sexual
Preference for Complex Music selection of males by females depends on factors that are likely to
ensure good genes for the survival of offspring and the ability of the
Although cognitive use of music had no significant effects on male to obtain resources (Buss, 1989). One of these overarching
preference for complex music, Openness to Experience did. Thus factors is intelligence. Consequently, males may be more likely to
open participants were more likely to like complex music, but not report using music in a cognitive way due to the message of cognitive
because they used music in a more cognitive/intellectual way. This ability and inherent attractiveness it conveys to the opposite sex.
association may be indicative of the novelty or unconventional With regard to age effects, a recently proposed model of creative
features of both the music classified as “complex” and the open measures (Chamorro-Premuzic, Furnham, & Reimers, 2007) pos-
personality. It appears, then, that people do not just profess cog- tulates that individual differences predict creative preferences and
nitive use because complex music inherently appealing to them. interests, which can lead over time, to creative knowledge and
So, although a number of factors may lead to a cognitive interest refined artistic judgment. In this case, a younger age may be
in music, only Openness to Experience seems to determine important in promoting greater interest in music in adolescents/
whether individuals genuinely appreciate complex music. As young adults, which in turn, may lead to greater cognitive appre-
Openness to Experience has been found to correlate with intellec- ciation of it. However, as people get older, familiarity may cause
tual engagement (Ackerman & Goff, 1994) and intelligence (Ack- a decline in this interest, and consequently, a decrease in cognitive
erman & Heggestad, 1997), open individuals could be more likely use of music.
than nonopen ones to respectively seek out and retain musical Naturally, the single-wave nature of the current design (which
knowledge. Consequently, those high in Openness may be ex- assessed all constructs at one time rather than longitudinally), as
PERSONALITY AND MUSIC USES AND PREFERENCES 211

well as the small, albeit cross-sectional, sample, make these de- generic factor of musical preferences can be removed. Indeed,
velopmental hypotheses speculative at most. Another effect to SEM is the appropriate method with multiple-outcome variables
consider is the result that females were more likely than males to and multiple predictors and intervening variables. Despite the
show a general preference for all compositions. Although this small sample size used to test this model, it is hoped that publi-
echoes previous findings that females tend to have a more positive cation of this study will encourage researchers to test this model
attitude to music (Colley, Comber, & Hargreaves, 1994; Crowther with larger samples.
& Durking, 1982), it may simply be attributed to factors within the Finally, the results need to be viewed with the consideration
musical extracts composed. All of the music had a moderate tempo that, due to the nature of the IPIP, there are no norms to compare
and were played without percussion or distortion, so they may the personality traits to. It is possible that the sample used here was
have consequently fallen into Schwartz and Fouts’s (2003) “light” higher in Openness than the general population: Open individuals
category of music, which females were found more likely to prefer were probably more likely to participate in this voluntary experi-
than males—possibly due to it reflecting themes with which fe- ment. Consequently further analysis should use more rigid sam-
males are more concerned, such as relationships and socialization pling methods to obtain a wider range personality profiles. It
(Larson, 1995). would also be important to provide evidence for the test–retest
reliability of the Music Preferences test we employed; this would
Limitations no doubt reduce error variance in the endogenous variables of the
model.
Owing to the recency of research into personality and uses of
music and the lack of past studies on the purposely designed
Conclusions
auditory measure of music preferences, the current study is largely
exploratory and suffers from a number of limitations, which could Despite these limitations, the current results replicate Chamorro-
be addressed by further research. Premuzic and Furnham’s (2007) findings with regard to personal-
First, further studies may do well to expand our model by ity and uses of music, as well as offering valuable insight into the
including a measure of creativity (as suggested by Chamorro- nature of consequent music preferences. We believe that the cur-
Premuzic et al., 2009). The current data were obtained by using a rent results provide support for the growing literature suggesting
basic web-based survey but more advanced software will enable us that variations in music use and musical taste are to some extent
to also include timed, open-ended, text items, such as those used to the reflection of individual differences in personality, and add to it
measure ideational fluency or divergent thinking processes. In by suggesting how uses and tastes relate to each other. Moreover,
addition, cognitive ability could also be examined. Both IQ and the current findings also demonstrate how both uses and prefer-
creativity are particularly relevant with regard to understanding the ences can vary as a function of age and gender. Of particular
link between Openness and cognitive use of music as well as interest is the implication that choice of positive and negative
improving our prediction of preference for complex music. affect in music may be determined as much by personality as mood
Second, the current results cannot reveal the causal direction and that preference for complex or novel music may be driven by
underlying many of the associations identified (in fact, only gender personality. It would be interesting to assess to what extent these
and age effects on other variables can be regarded as causal). findings can be extrapolated to the wider field of media prefer-
Social cognition theories suggest that chronic exposure to attitudes ences, that is, investigate whether individuals select other forms of
in media may lead to greater accessibility (e.g., Bargh, 1984), so it media that reflects their own personality traits (Buss, 1987). Ac-
could be that music preferences are determined by other, unexam- cordingly, our results have a number of applied implications, such
ined variables like peer influence—and music listened to conse- as informing the selection of different kinds of music (and other
quently determines personality traits. Longitudinal studies into media) for different types of consumers; and, from a clinical point
music preferences and personality traits could be carried out to of view, they could guide music-based therapies depending on
asses this. individual needs.
Third, the current analyses via SEM were carried out on a
relatively small sample. There is an extensive literature on the
sample size requirements for SEM, and although even a brief References
summary of that literature is beyond the scope of this article, a Ackerman, P. L., & Goff, M. (1994). Typical intellectual engagement and
number of experts seem to agree that 100 cases are the minimum personality: Reply to Rocklin (1994). Journal of Educational Psychol-
required for a design with 10 to 15 variables. Bentler and Chou ogy, 86, 150 –153.
(1987) requested five cases per parameter estimates (with our Ackerman, P. L., & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and
design that would mean a sample of at least 80 participants); Kline interests: Evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin, 121,
(1998) considered sample sizes over 100 to be tenable, while both 219 –245.
Loehlin (1992) and Hoyle (1995) recommended at least 100 cases. Arbuckle, J., & Wothke, W. (1999). AMOS 4 user’s reference guide.
Needless to say, we would have welcomed a larger sample size but Chicago, IL: Smallwaters.
Bargh, J. A. (1984). Automatic and conscious processing of social infor-
collecting the present 100 cases was a time-consuming and effort-
mation. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social
ful venture given the lack of incentives for participants and the cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 1– 43). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
length of the experiment. In the current study, the choice of SEM Barron, F. X. (1955). The disposition toward originality. Journal of Ab-
(over regression analysis) was driven mainly by the prospect of normal and Social Psychology, 5, 399 – 414.
testing a theoretical model in which variables are treated simulta- Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psy-
neously as predictors and criteria, and the variance attributed to a chological Bulletin, 107, 238 –246.
212 CHAMORRO-PREMUZIC, FAGAN, AND FURNHAM

Bentler, P. M., & Chou, C. P. (1987). Practical issues in structural mod- interest and preference in adults of different educational levels. Journal
eling. Sociological Methods and Research, 16, 78 –117. of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 62–79.
Bjurström, E., & Wennhall, J. (1991). Ungdomar och musik. Årsbok om Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C.,
ungdom 1991. (Youth and music. Yearbook on youth 1991.) Stockholm, Cloninger, C. R., & Gough, H. C. (2006). The international personality
Sweden: Statens ungdomsråd. item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. Journal
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New of Research in Psychology, 40(1), 84 –96.
York, NY: Wiley. Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C.,
Brackett, M. A., & Salovey, P. (2006). Measuring emotional intelligence with Cloninger, C. R., & Gough, H. C. (2006). The international personality
the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MS–CEIT). Psi- item pool and the future of public-
cothemia, 18(Suppl.), 34 – 41. Hoyle, R. H. (Ed.). (1995). Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues,
Burke, R., & Grinder, R. E. (1966). Personality-oriented themes and and applications. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
listening patterns in teen-age music and their relation to certain academic International Federation of the Phonographic Industry. (n.d.). The broader
and peer variables. School Review, 74, 196 –211. music industry. Retrieved January 14 2009, from http://www.ifpi.org/
Buss, D. M. (1987). Selection, evocation, and manipulation. Journal of content/library/the-broader-music-industry.pdf
Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1214 –1221. Kamman, R. (1966). Verbal complexity and preferences in poetry. Journal
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolu- of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 5, 536 –540.
tionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation mod-
12, 1– 49. eling. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Byrne, B. M. (2006). Structural equation modeling with EQS: Basic Krumhansl, C. L. (1997). An exploratory study of musical emotions and
concepts, applications, and programming (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erl- psychophysiology. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51,
baum. 336 –352.
Canli, T., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Kang, E., Gross, J., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. Larson, R. (1995). Secrets in the bedroom: Adolescents’ private use of
(2001). An fMRI study of personality influences on brain reactivity to media. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 24, 535–550.
emotional stimuli. Behavioral Neuroscience, 115, 33– 42. Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Latent variable models: An introduction to factor,
Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2007). Personality and individual differences. path, and structural analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Oxford, England: Blackwell. Loehlin, J. C. (2004). Latent variable models: An introduction to factor,
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2007). Personality and music: path, and structural equation analysis. Mahwah, NJ: L. Erlbaum Asso-
Can traits explain how people use music in everyday life? British ciates.
Journal of Psychology, 98, 175–185. McNamara, L., & Ballard, M. E. (1999). Resting arousal, sensation seek-
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Furnham, A., & Reimers, S. (2007, February). ing, and music preference. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology
The artistic personality. The Psychologist, 20, 84 – 87. Monographs, 125, 229 –250.
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Gomà-i-Freixanet, M., Furnham, A., & Muro, A. Meyer, L. B. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. Chicago, IL: Uni-
(2009). Personality, self-estimated intelligence and uses of music: A versity of Chicago Press.
Spanish replication and extension using structural equation modelling. Mithen, S. (2005). The singing Neanderthals: The origins of music, lan-
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity and the Arts, 3, 149 –155. guage, mind, and body. London, England: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Reimers, S., Hsu, A., & Ahmetoglu, G. (2009). North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., & O’Neill, S. A. (2000). The importance
Who Art thou? Personality predictors of artistic preferences in a large of music to adolescents. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70,
UK sample: The importance of Openness. British Journal of Psychol- 255–272.
ogy, 100(3), 501–516. Panksepp, J. (1995). The emotional sources of chills induced by music.
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Reimers, S., Hsu, A., & Ahmetoglu, G. (in press). Music Perception, 13, 171–207.
Who Art thou? Personality predictors of artistic preferences in a large Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2003). Trait emotional intelligence:
UK sample: The importance of Openness. British Journal of Psychol- Behavioural validation in two studies of emotion recognition and reac-
ogy. tivity to mood induction. European Journal of Personality, 15, 425– 448.
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Swami, V., Furnham, A., & Maakip, I. (2009). Petrides, K. V., & Furnham, A. (2006). The role of trait emotional intel-
The Big Five personality traits and music: A replication in Malaysia ligence in a gender-specific model of organizational variables. Journal
using structural equation modelling. Journal of Individual Differences, of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 552–569.
30, 20 –27. Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2003). The do re mi’s of everyday life:
Coffman, D. D., Gfeller, K., & Eckert, M. (1995). Effect of textual setting, The structure and personality correlates of music preferences. Journal of
training, and gender on emotional response to verbal and musical infor- Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 1236 –1256.
mation. Psychomusicology, 14, 117–136. Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence for
Colley, A., Comber, C., & Hargreaves, D. J. (1994). Gender effects in an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological Bulletin,
school subject preferences: A research note. Educational Studies, 20, 128, 934 –960.
13–18. Schellenberg, E. G. (2006). Long-term positive associations between music
Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality lessons and IQ. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 457– 468.
Inventory (NEO–PI–R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO–FFI): Schwartz, K. D., & Fouts, G. T. (2003). Music preferences, personality
Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. style, and development issues of adolescents. Journal of Youth and
Crowther, R., & Durkin, K. (1982). Sex- and age-related differences in the Adolescence, 32, 205–213.
musical behaviour, interests and attitudes towards music of 232 second- Sevdalis, N., Petrides, K., & Harvey, N. (2007). Trait emotional intelli-
ary school students. Educational Studies, 8, 131–139. gence and decision related emotions. Personality and Individual Differ-
Davis, S. (1985). Pop lyrics: A mirror and molder of society. Et Cetera, ences, 42, 1347–1358.
42(Summer), 167–169. Silvia, E. S. M., & MacCallum, R. C. (1988). Some factors affecting the
Davis, S. (1985, Summer). Pop lyrics: A mirror and molder of society. Et success of specification searches in covariance structure modeling. Mul-
Cetera, 167–169. tivariate Behavioral Research, 23, 297–326.
Francès, R. (1976). Comparative effects of six collative variables on Spence, G., Oades, L. G., & Caputi, P. (2004). Trait emotional intelligence
PERSONALITY AND MUSIC USES AND PREFERENCES 213

and goal self-integration: Important predictors of emotional well-being? Van der Zee, K., & Wabeke, R. (2004). Is trait emotional intelligence
Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 449 – 461. simply or more than just a trait? European Journal of Personality, 18,
Tabachnick, B., & Fidell, L. (Eds.). (2007). Using multivariate statistics. 243–263.
New York, NY: Harper Collins. Warwick, J., & Nettlebeck, T. (2004). Emotional intelligence is . . . ?
Tarrant, M., North, A. C., & Hargreaves, D. J. (2000). English and Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1091–1100.
American adolescents’ reasons for listening to music. Psychology of Youngman, M. B. (1979). Analyzing social and educational research data.
Music, 28, 166 –173. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Tramo, M. J. (2001). Music of the hemispheres. Science, 291, 54 –56. Zuckerman, M., Bone, R. N., Neary, R., Mangelsdorf, D., & Brustman, B.
Ullman, J. B. (2007). Structural equation modeling. In B. Tabachnick & L. (1972). What is the sensation seeker? Personality trait and experience
Fidell (Eds.), Using multivariate statistics, 5th ed. New York, NY: correlates of the sensation seeking scale. Journal of Consulting and
Harper Collins. Clinical Psychology, 39, 308 –321.

Appendix

Intercorrelations Among Target Measures

Target measure 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

1. Sexa ⫺.16 .19 .01 ⫺.27ⴱⴱ .02 ⫺.04 .18 .19 .24ⴱ ⫺.23ⴱ ⫺.02 ⫺.00
2. Age .04 .06 .17 .18 ⫺.05 ⫺.09 ⫺.16 ⫺.04 ⫺.14 .11 ⫺.10
3. Extraversion .20ⴱ .31ⴱⴱ .45ⴱⴱ ⫺.06 .21 ⴱ
.14 .05 .16 ⫺.06 .21ⴱ
4. Openness .07 .27ⴱⴱ .14 .04 .29ⴱⴱ ⫺.05 .35ⴱⴱ .16 .02
5. Emotional Stability .66ⴱⴱ ⫺.14 ⫺.12 ⫺.20ⴱ ⫺.14 .08 ⫺.25ⴱ .04
6. Trait EI ⫺.17 ⫺.02 ⫺.07 ⫺.02 .02 ⫺.20ⴱ .06
7. Sad music .36ⴱⴱ .57ⴱⴱ .47 ⴱⴱ
.03 .36 ⴱⴱ
.16
8. Happy music .33ⴱⴱ .72ⴱⴱ ⫺.02 .10 .38ⴱⴱ
9. Complex music .37ⴱⴱ .00 .10 .26ⴱ
10. Social music ⫺.17 .14 .37ⴱⴱ
11. M(cognitive) .23ⴱ .04
12. M(emotion) .08
13. M(back)

Note. N ⫽ 100. M (cognitive) ⫽ cognitive/intellectual use of music; M (emotion) ⫽ emotional use of music; M (back) ⫽
background use of music.
a
1 ⫽ male; 2 ⫽ female.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01.

Received March 22, 2009


Revision received December 18, 2009
Accepted December 30, 2009 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like