You are on page 1of 11

Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain © 2015 American Psychological Association

2015, Vol. 25, No. 1, 58 – 68 0275-3987/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pmu0000078

Music in Mind? An Experience Sampling Study of What and When,


Towards an Understanding of Why
Freya Bailes
University of Western Sydney

Imagining music in the course of everyday life is commonplace, and recent studies have begun to reveal
what we imagine, and to ask why. However, research methods that rely on retrospective reports are not
sensitive to the transience of imagined musical experience. In 2007, Bailes used experience sampling
methods instead, to understand the prevalence and nature of imagined music episodes among music
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

students. The current study extends this research to a larger and broader sample of the general public
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(N ⫽ 47, with 1,415 episodes), to determine what people imagine, when, and why. Respondents were
contacted by SMS 6 times a day, for the period of a week. On contact, they filled out an experience
sampling form surveying current location, activity, mood, and details of any musical experience, heard
or imagined. Open questions elicited reasons for imagining particular music, and probed the nature of the
experience. Specific hypotheses linking musical imagery to thought incursions and mood regulation were
tested. A positive relationship between the frequency of imagining music and transliminality was found,
as well as mood congruence between heard and imagined music episodes. Suggestions are made for
further research into the potential influence of chronobiology, arousal, and attention on everyday musical
imagery.

Keywords: musical imagery, experience sampling methods, mood congruence, transliminality, INMI

Imagining music in the course of everyday life has only recently thought suppression and schizotypy predict experiences of INMI
graduated from anecdote to the subject of scientific investigation. (Beaman & Williams, 2013). Müllensiefen et al. (2014) found a
Although recent empirical studies concern Involuntary Musical relationship between obsessive– compulsive attributes in a non-
Imagery (INMI, Liikkanen, 2008), or “earworms” that stick or clinical population and INMI disturbance and frequency, specu-
repeat in mind, this article adopts a broader perspective in its lating that deliberate attempts to suppress unwanted INMI might in
concern for any form of imagined music that might occur, whether fact enhance its occurrence. However, the authors note a potential
involuntary or voluntary, persistent or fleeting. An experience weakness with their reliance on retrospective questionnaires,
sampling study of university music students’ everyday imaginings which might have led to a greater focus on INMI than would occur
of music (Bailes, 2006, 2007) found that musical imagery occurred with the use of time-sensitive methods such as experience sam-
both spontaneously and as an involuntary corollary of a voluntary pling. Consistent with this, Beaty et al. (2013) report a discrepancy
musical activity such as performing or thinking about music. between their own two studies of musical imagery that they
Broader questions of volition arise. Liikkanen (2008) acknowl- attribute to method: their retrospective questionnaire study re-
edged the potential importance of voluntary control in “involun- vealed a relationship between neuroticism and reports of music
tary” musical imagery, by conceptually separating activation and being stuck in the head, whereas their experience sampling study
upkeep components; the former occurring outside of attention, and revealed no such relationship.
the latter allowing for mental control. Individual differences in There are many reasons to favor experience sampling methods
(ESM) in the study of ephemeral phenomena such as musical
imagery (see Bailes, 2006). Retrospective methods such as ques-
FREYA BAILES was appointed Senior Research Fellow at the MARCS tionnaires and diary studies are particularly prone to memory and
Institute, following postdoctoral appointments in France, the United States, reporting biases, and do not capture fine-grained longitudinal
and Australia. These research positions allowed her to build on her long- patterns. Bailes (2007) used ESM to “observe” musical imagery
standing interest in musical imagery and broaden her interests to the experiences as they occurred, rather than through retrospective
perception of emotion, and the dynamic analysis of music. Since 2012, recall. Respondents filled out an Experience Sampling Form (ESF)
Freya has returned to the United Kingdom to take up her current appoint- each time they received a signal on their mobile phone. Six signals
ment as Lecturer in Music at the University of Hull. were sent each day, at semirandom times throughout the day
Funds for the bulk SMS service were provided by MARCS Auditory (10 a.m.–10 p.m.), for a period of 7 days. The form comprised both
Laboratories. My thanks to Sarah Allen, Victoria Busuttil, Samar Dawidar,
closed and open questions, with the advantage of capturing a quick
and Asma Payara who collected the data for their PGDiploma research
project. Thank you to Professor Ronan Reilly for statistical guidance.
snapshot of each episode that could be readily compared with other
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Freya episodes by the same individual and other respondents, as well as
Bailes, who is now at the School of Drama, Music and Screen, University capturing qualitative data describing the context and feel of the
of Hull, Cottingham Road HU6 7RX, United Kingdom. E-mail: F.Bailes@ episode (qualitative detail that was not recorded in the recent
hull.ac.uk experience sampling study of musical imagery by Beaty et al.

58
MUSIC IN MIND: AN EXPERIENCE SAMPLING STUDY 59

(2013)). Respondents imagined music on average 35% of the ularly strongly with the experience of imagining music too, and
sampled time, melody and lyrics were rated as the most vivid Williamson et al. (2012) found that in their qualitative analysis of
components of their mental image, and respondents reported imag- INMI accounts, low or diffuse attention states were commonly
ining music most when interacting with others, working or trav- identified. One way to conceptualize fluctuating experiences of
eling. The number of episodes of imagined music dropped in the conscious awareness is in terms of the psychological construct
final 2-hr time period of the day (8 p.m.–10 p.m.). However, Bailes “transliminality,” akin to the permeability of a psychological mem-
(2006, 2007) was limited by a relatively small sample size (N ⫽ brane between conscious and unconscious awareness (Wammes &
11, 417 completed ESFs), but also by a focus on the experiences Baruss, 2009). Wammes and Baruss (2009) found that those with
of university music students. The aim of the current article is to a high score on Thalbourne’s “Transliminality Scale” (Lange,
expand this earlier research by using the same qualitatively rich Thalbourne, Houran, & Storm, 2000), indicative of a greater
ESM to investigate 47 members of the wider public. It will ask propensity for thoughts to cross in and out of conscious awareness,
whether findings from the previous ESM study with music stu- also scored highly on the dimensions of imagery persistence and
dents can be generalized to a wider population, and address the distraction in a factor analysis of responses to a musical imagery
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

question of what and when people imagine in everyday life. questionnaire.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The current study is novel in testing the hypothesis of an


association between transliminality and the frequency of imagin-
What Do We Imagine?
ing music using time-sensitive ESM rather than a retrospective
Regarding what is imagined, people report imagining music that questionnaire. However, it was also expected that not all music is
is familiar to them (Bailes, 2007), a form of memory (Halpern & equally susceptible to becoming a thought incursion. In addition to
Bartlett, 2011), although a minority imagine their own composed questions of musical characteristics that may lend themselves more
music (Bailes, 2006, 2007; Beaty et al., 2013; Liikkanen, 2008). or less to thought incursion, it was expected that music with which
Earworms are supposedly annoying, but research suggests individuals choose to engage would be more likely to be imagined
rather that INMI experiences are generally neutral or positively than music that was less significant for the respondent. Conse-
valenced (Beaman & Williams, 2010, 2013; Beaty et al., 2013; quently, the current study also tested the hypothesis of a positive
Halpern & Bartlett, 2011; Liikkanen, 2012; Williamson, Liik- relationship between imagining music and reports of actively
kanen, Jakubowski, & Stewart, 2014). choosing to hear it. Retrospective reports using interviews, diary
Why we mentally accompany our everyday lives with music studies, and questionnaires, suggest that musical experience is
remains a perplexing question. One possible trigger is having positively related to INMI (Beaman & Williams, 2010; Liikkanen,
recently heard it (Brown, 1999; Halpern & Bartlett, 2011; Hyman 2008, 2012; Müllensiefen et al., 2014; Williamson & Jilka, 2014),
et al., 2013; Liikkanen, 2009; Williamson et al., 2012). In the though this need not be through formal music training (Beaman &
experience sampling study of Bailes (2006, 2007), 58% of imagery Williams, 2010; Halpern & Bartlett, 2011), as a link has been
episodes named recently hearing or performing the music as the found between INMI and singing and listening to music (Müllen-
possible reason for subsequently imagining it. Indeed, 43% of siefen et al., 2014).
imagery episodes were of music that had been heard since the
previous time of contact. The current study aims to compare the
Mood Congruent Imagery
apparent impact of hearing music on imagining it for music stu-
dents with the general population. This was a similar aim of Beaty In Bailes (2007), respondents were asked to rate their mood at
et al. (2013), who contrasted the experience of a group of musi- each imagery episode. Rating scales were for positivity (happy–
cians (music majors) with nonmusicians (other students). They sad, relaxed–tense), present-mindedness (interested– bored,
found that listening to music predicted the likelihood of imagining involved– detached, connected–lonely), and arousal (alert–
music, and musicians reported musical imagery more frequently drowsy, energetic–tired). Overall, ratings were above the midpoint
than the nonmusician sample. The current study extends their for positivity and arousal during these episodes, but as no compa-
shorter experience sampling survey to additionally compare the rable mood ratings were obtained for nonimagery episodes, such
contents of heard and imagined music and to gather more detailed data are difficult to interpret. Perhaps there is an effect of mood
information about the mood of respondents (see below). It is of congruence (Mayer, McCormick, & Strong, 1995) in operation for
considerable interest to understand the relationship between heard imagined music, with the mood experienced when hearing partic-
and imagined musical experience, in part because of the contextual ular music matched by that experienced when imagining it. In
factors that distinguish them, including the apparent external ver- other words, does the way an individual feels when hearing music
sus internal locus of control. match how they feel when imagining it? To date, there has been no
measure of mood congruence between specific instances of heard
and imagined music (though Williamson et al., 2012 report ac-
Individual Differences Imagining Music
counts of mood and INMI match). The current study extends this
Recent research has drawn parallels between the characteristics past work, by measuring mood for every sampled episode, not just
of mind wandering and spontaneously imagining music (William- when respondents imagine music. Mood congruence between
son et al., 2014). For example, Hyman et al. (2013) found that heard and imagined music episodes could be indicative of the use
“intrusive songs” occurred under the same conditions of cognitive of imagined music as an emotion-regulation strategy. For example,
load as mind wandering. An important feature of Herbert’s (2011) individuals wishing to increase their positive affective state might
phenomenological account of everyday listening is the diffuse imagine a song that makes them happy. Beaty et al. (2013) asked
attention described by listeners. Such an account resonates partic- ESM respondents to report the extent that they were happy, sad, or
60 BAILES

worried during imagery episodes. They found that when people Materials
felt happy, they reported deliberately imagining music. Emotion
Experience sampling form. An ESF was devised for partic-
regulation can also operate to maintain or decrease a particular
ipants to complete when messaged (see Appendix). As in Bailes
emotional state (Koole, 2009). The current study investigates
(2006), the ESF occupied two sides of a sheet of paper, and
whether one of the many possible reasons to imagine music during
comprised an introductory section asking for the date, time con-
the course of daily life is to regulate mood. That is not to say that
tacted, and time filled out. Subsequent questions directed partici-
evidence of mood congruence between heard and imagined music
pants to the section(s) of the form to fill out if they were hearing,
episodes can be interpreted as emotion regulation, as the impact of
imagining, or hearing and imagining music at the time of contact.
inadvertently imagining music might well be to unintentionally In all cases, participants were required to answer part A, asking
affect one’s mood. However, as emphasized by Västfjäll, Juslin, about mood at the time of contact, location, main activity, and the
and Hartig (2012), listening to music to boost well-being is inex- presence of others.
pensive, amenable to personalization, portable, and potentially Part B of the ESF was to be completed if participants were
independent of time and location. These qualities are only in- hearing music at the time of contact. This section was up-dated
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

creased by moving away from a reliance on actual music, to the from Bailes (2006) to include laptops and MP3 players as possible
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

realm of the imagination. sources of the music. Stylistic categories to describe the heard
In summary, the current study aims to extend Bailes (2006, music were also updated, to include trance/house/techno, country,
2007) to a broader and larger sample than the music students who blues, urban (rap, R&B, hip hop), and gospel.
responded to the original survey. ESM are used to gain a rich Changes to Part C, to be completed when imagining music,
cross-sectional and longitudinal understanding of the context and include the same stylistic category updates as for Part B. Since
qualia of imagining music in everyday life, captured in real-time, Bailes (2006, 2007) devised an ESF appropriate for music stu-
circumventing retrospective reporting biases. It was expected that, dents, the current study had to make a number of changes to
just as for music students, the broader sample of respondents accommodate respondents without any musical training. Accord-
would cite recently hearing familiar music as a principal reason for ingly, when asking respondents to rate how sharp their mental
subsequently imagining it. The study also sought to begin to image of various musical elements might be, harmony and texture
understand why we imagine music in everyday life, by exploring were excluded. Timbre was expressed as “Instruments/Voice,”
the hypothesis that people might imagine music as a form of “dynamics” were expressed as “loudness,” and “expression” was
emotion regulation, and that high transliminality scores relate to altered to “emotion/expression.” Finally, “Tempo/Rhythm” was
the frequency of imagining music, indicating a propensity for added as a potentially important musical element. A question from
musical thoughts to frequently cross into conscious awareness. Bailes (2006, 2007) about the physical memory of playing the
imagined music was removed from the current ESF.
Revised Transliminality Scale. The revised transliminality
Method scale (Lange et al., 2000) was administered, comprising 17 state-
ments requiring a true or false answer.
ESM were used to observe the musical experiences of respon-
Background questionnaire. A background questionnaire col-
dents from the general population. The method closely followed
lected demographic information about the participants (age, gen-
that used by Bailes (2006).
der) and recorded details of any self-reported hearing impairment.
Remaining questions were taken from the OMSI (Ollen, 2006,
Participants 2009), which is designed to classify participants as more
(score ⬎500) or less (score ⬍500) musically sophisticated (scale
The first group of participants included 27 volunteers from 0 –1000).
greater Western Sydney, Australia, who were recruited by word of
mouth or via an announcement on a social networking site. Par- Procedure
ticipation was not remunerated, and a condition for participation
The study was conducted in two rounds, corresponding to the
was the possession of a mobile phone. The sample ranged in age participant groups described above. The procedure was identical
from 18 to 53 years (M ⫽ 25) and included 12 men. Participants for each round. Participants attended a briefing session in which
ranged in their Ollen Musical Sophistication Index (OMSI) from the study requirements were explained, and informed consent was
39 to 944 (M ⫽ 187). Twenty of 27 respondents returned fewer sought (study approved by the University of Western Sydney
than the total possible number of ESFs, but unless otherwise Human Research Ethics Committee, H6930). The background
stated, their partial data are included in the analyses. questionnaire was administered followed by the revised translimi-
The second group of participants included 20 undergraduate nality questionnaire.
psychology students from the University of Western Sydney, who In line with Bailes (2006, 2007), participants were given a pack
participated in the study in exchange for course credit. A condition of 42 ESFs, comprising one form to be filled out each time they
for participation was the possession of a mobile phone. The sample received an SMS from the researchers (6 contact times ⫻ 7 days).
ranged in age from 18 to 38 years (M ⫽ 22) and included nine The researchers noted any specific times over the coming testing
men. Participants had an OMSI between 11 and 596 (M ⫽ 97). week at which participants would not be able to respond to SMS
Eighteen of 20 respondents returned fewer than the total possible cues. As discussed in Bailes (2006), a week is taken to incorporate
number of ESFs, but unless otherwise stated, their partial data are the activities the participants would typically experience during
included in the analyses. both week days and weekends, for a wide range of possible
MUSIC IN MIND: AN EXPERIENCE SAMPLING STUDY 61

musical imagery patterns. Participants were instructed to set the would be more likely to be imagined. However, a correlation
SMS reception signal of their phone to a nonmusical cue, to avoid between personal choice rating and times imagined did not reach
artificially priming a musical image of the signal. significance, r(164) ⫽ .106, p ⫽ .17. Moreover, there was no
A bulk SMS provider (AMD Telecom A.E.) was used to sched- difference in personal choice rating between heard pieces that
ule the sending of the message “Please fill out your form” to respondents imagined versus not (U ⫽ 866.500, N1 ⫽ 152, N2 ⫽
participants six times a day, over a period of a week (7 days), 14, p ⫽ .226, two-tailed).
between 9 a.m. and 9 p.m. The schedule was quasi-random, with Respondents were asked to explain, if possible, why they might
one signal scheduled to occur at any point within each 2-hr time have been imagining particular music. Table 1 summarizes re-
period. sponses to this question, showing that having recently heard the
On receipt of the SMS, participants were to fill out a blank ESF music was the most common answer (38%), while the next most
as soon as possible, answering questions with respect to the time common answer was not knowing why (12%).
of contact, rather than the time of filling out the form. The ESF Those who reported responding to the SMS within 30 min of its
included space to note both of these times, in case of discrepancy. receipt were able to name on average 66% of the music featured in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

their imagery episodes. One hundred twenty-five different musical


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Analysis items were named, and only four of these (3%) were imagined on
multiple occasions, indicative of the great variety of music imag-
Descriptive statistics of the dataset use all submitted ESF data, ined by the respondents of this study. Of the four items imagined
from all completed ESFs. More detailed analyses exclude any on multiple occasions, three were imagined by more than one
episodes that were reported more than 30 min after the time of respondent (see Table 2). Although the recurrence of imagining the
signal, as their content could not be considered to provide a same item between and within respondents was rare, there was
reliable account of the specific characteristics of the experience. more commonality with respect to the artists who were imagined
on multiple occasions. As can be seen in Table 3, 11 artists were
Results imagined more than once, with nine of those imagined by different
respondents. The study was conducted shortly after the death of
Respondents returned a median of 34 complete ESFs (range
Michael Jackson, meaning that his music was widely played, and
1– 42), for a total of 1,415 ESFs (out of a possible 1,974). Seven-
explaining the relatively large number of respondents (six) imag-
teen percent of these episodes were reports of imagining music.
ining his songs. What is perhaps more surprising is that none of the
Most episodes involved “no music” (52%), with 35% accompanied
reported Michael Jackson imagery episodes were of the same song.
by the presence of music (either heard music alone (31%), or heard
Eight respondents imagined their own composition (four male,
and imagined music concurrently (4%)). There were considerable
OMSI 42–248), for a median of 21% of episodes. A Mann–
interindividual differences in the time reported in each state. In-
Whitney test revealed that there was no statistically significant
dividuals varied most on the proportion of episodes in which they
difference in the OMSI scores of those who reported imagining
experienced both heard and imagined music at the same time
(coefficient of variation, CV1 ⫽ 1.25). There was also consider- their own compositions compared with those who didn’t (U ⫽
able individual difference with respect to the proportion of imag- 138.5, N1 ⫽ 35, N2 ⫽ 8, p ⫽ .96, two-tailed).
ery episodes reported (CV ⫽ 1.01), but less for heard (CV ⫽ 0.57) In order to understand the phenomenological experience of how
and no music episodes (CV ⫽ 0.44). complete the musical image was, only descriptions of episodes
purportedly recorded within 30 min of receiving the SMS were
considered. Of these imagery episodes, the vast majority described
What Do We Imagine? a repeated fragment (40 episodes), a fragment (12), being repeated
A series of analyses was performed to investigate links between (4), a repeated line (3), or a repeated section (2). The word “loop”
the music that respondents heard and imagined. These analyses was often employed, as in “I heard the theme in a loop,” “I heard
begin to explore why people imagine music, by focusing on the the beginning of the tune and lyrics in a loop.” This predominant
potential impact of having heard music on subsequently imagining experience was contrasted by many reports of music playing as a
it. Considering only those ESFs that respondents claimed to have whole piece (21 episodes), and “I was singing it in my head from
filled out within 30 min of receiving the SMS cue, a recency effect beginning to end.” Indeed, one participant described imagined
was found whereby music that had actually been heard since the music that was not so much quantitatively complete, but qualita-
previous cue was imagined 38% of the time. An examination of tively so, “I heard the entire theme, all instruments and harmony
items and artists who were either heard or imagined more than very clear, then back.” Another explained “for a moment I was
once, found no evidence for a linear relationship between the there with the music—very full in mind.”
number of times that they were heard and imagined (Items: To compare the current study with the experiences of the music
r(15) ⫽ ⫺.097; Artists: r(26) ⫽ ⫺.143). student sample in Bailes (2007), one question on the ESF asked
Relating to what music people imagine, a paired t test (two- respondents to rate how sharp their mental image of the melody,
tailed) of the proportion of episodes in which respondents could lyrics (if appropriate), instruments/voice, tempo/rhythm, loudness,
name the music shows that significantly more imagined music
(M ⫽ 66%) was named than heard music (M ⫽ 55%), t(37) ⫽ 1
The coefficient of variation divides the standard deviation by the mean,
2.03, p ⬍ .00052, confirming previous findings (Bailes, 2007) that providing a normalized measure of dispersion.
the perceptual source of the imagined music is highly familiar. It 2
This analysis excluded 10 participants with no instances of either heard
was thought that music that respondents actively choose to hear or imagined music being reported within 30 min of the cue.
62 BAILES

Table 1 Table 3
References to Different Reasons for Imagining the Music Artists Imagined More Than Once

Node References % of references Artist Number of episodes Number of respondents

Recently heard 61 37.7 Michael Jackson 7 6


Don’t know why 19 11.7 Beyonce 4 4
Stickiness 11 6.8 Akon 4 3
TV 7 4.3 Jordin Sparks 4 3
Spontaneity 7 4.3 Pink 4 3
Recently imagined 6 3.7 Australian All Stars 4 1
Value judgment 5 3.1 The Dubliners 4 1
Musical features 5 3.1 Britney Spears 2 2
Favorite music 5 3.1 Lady Gaga 2 2
Visual cue 4 2.5 Kings of Leon 2 2
Recently sung/played 3 1.9 ACDC 2 2
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Imagery on waking 3 1.9


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Intentional imagining 3 1.9


Sentimental/nostalgia 3 1.9
Other 20 12.3
served to check whether previous findings relating to music stu-
dents (Bailes, 2007) might be generalized to the wider population.
and emotion/expression was. A one-way ANOVA comparing the A total of 1,253 cases were analyzed and the full model signifi-
ratings for each of these features was significant, F(5, 190) ⫽ 3.66, cantly predicted musical state (omnibus chi-square ⫽ 174.38, df ⫽
M ⫽ 4.72, p ⬍ .005. The results are plotted in Figure 1, and this 57, p ⬍ .005). The model accounted for between 13% and 14.5%
ordering of the features is in line with results from Bailes (2007), of the variance.
except that the current study included a question about mental Time of day did not reliably predict overall musical state. Nor
imagery for tempo and/or rhythm, and lyrics and instruments/voice did the presence or absence of others. Although the location of
(named as timbre for the music students of the previous study) respondents at the time of contact (p ⬍ .02) and their activity at the
have swapped positions. As can be seen, “tempo and/or rhythm” time (p ⬍ .005) were significant predictors of overall musical
was rated as being above average sharpness, and tempo and state, model coefficients for location and activity while imagining
rhythm were named as predominant features of the imagery of did not support the previous finding (Bailes, 2007) of an associa-
some respondents, with comments such as “syncopation from bass tion between imagining music and being in transit or traveling.
and drums,” “it is very dynamic, groovy music,” “It’s a joyful, The gender of the respondents was significantly related to their
swinging tune,” and “just the beat and rhythm.” musical state (p ⬍ .005). An analysis of model coefficients re-
vealed that the likelihood of imagining music compared with
Individual Differences Imagining Music neither hearing nor imagining music increased significantly for
men. The likelihood of imagining music also increased signifi-
To test the association between transliminality and the fre- cantly between 11 a.m. and 1 p.m. compared with “no music”
quency of imagining music, a one-tailed Pearson’s correlation was contexts.
performed between respondents’ proportion of reported imagery The incidence of both hearing and imagining music at the same
episodes and their transliminality score. This revealed a positive time was small (4%), and its odds were found to decrease signif-
relationship (r(43) ⫽ .25, p ⫽ .05), supporting the hypothesis.3 icantly when respondents were working, interacting with others,
In order to determine the relationship between contextual factors being at leisure, watching TV, or simply “being,” compared with
and respondents’ musical state at the time of contact (hearing neither hearing nor imagining music (p ⬍ .005).
music, imagining music, both hearing and imagining music, nei- A significant correlation was found between respondents’ age
ther hearing nor imagining music), a multinomial logistic regres- and proportion of imagery episodes (out of each participant’s total
sion analysis was run, with musical state at the time of contact as reported episodes; rs (43) ⫽ .37, p ⫽ .02). The proportion of
the dependent variable. Predictor variables entered into the model imagery episodes recorded for each individual was correlated with
included time of day (coded as six discrete 2-hr periods), location various measures of musical engagement, to determine whether
at the time of contact, activity at the time of contact, interaction musical background might shape the experience of imagining
(alone or with others), and the respondents’ gender. This analysis music in everyday life. No significant correlation was found be-
tween imagery proportion and years of private music lessons (rs
(42) ⫽ .01, p ⫽ .57). However, years of practice was positively
Table 2
correlated with reported proportion of imagery episodes (rs (42) ⫽
Items Imagined More Than Once
.30, p ⫽ .05). Those who were categorized as “more musically
Number of Number of sophisticated” by the more comprehensive OMSI (score ⬎500)
Name of item Artist episodes respondents reported a significantly greater proportion of imagery (M ⫽
Battlefield Jordin Sparks 4 3
Criss Cross Australian All Stars 4 1 3
To rule out artefacts relating to few ESF returns, those individuals who
Sweet dream Beyonce 3 3
returned a total of 7 or fewer complete ESFs were excluded from the
Beautiful Akon 3 2
analysis.
MUSIC IN MIND: AN EXPERIENCE SAMPLING STUDY 63

35.75%) than those categorized as “less musically sophisticated” Table 4


(M ⫽ 19.79%; U ⫽ 18.5, N1 ⫽ 4, N2 ⫽ 29, p ⫽ .025, two-tailed)4. Correlations Between Mood Ratings for Items That Were Both
Heard and Imagined
Musical Imagery and Mood
Correlation for Correlation for
To test for mood congruence when hearing and imagining the imagined item with imagined item with
Mood pair same heard item different heard item
same music, ratings of mood when individuals heard and imagined
the same piece were significantly correlated (with the exception of Alert/Drowsy .425 .948ⴱⴱ
the mood pair alert/drowsy), suggesting a mood congruent effect Happy/Sad .951ⴱⴱ .713ⴱ
Lonely/Connected .774ⴱ .649
(see Table 4). To ensure that this was specific to the piece of
Energetic/Tired .761ⴱ .753ⴱ
music, the mood ratings for the imagined item were correlated with Involved/Detached .716ⴱ .162
a different randomly selected heard item for these participants. Tense/Relaxed .805ⴱ .225
Although there was some evidence of consistency in the mood of Interested/Bored .739ⴱ .595
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

respondents when hearing and imagining any music, correlations Note. df ⫽ 5.



p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .0005.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

existed for a minority of mood ratings only (see Table 4), suggest-
ing that there is a degree of consistency of mood within individuals
across time when hearing and imagining music, but that this is
greater when the music is the same. energetic nor tired” (Energetic/Tired mood pair) was a significantly
In order to understand any relationship of mood to respondents’ negative predictor of imagining music.
musical state (hearing music, imagining music, neither hearing nor Model coefficients relative to the likelihood of actually hearing
imagining music), a second multinomial logistic regression analysis music reveal a similar pattern to those for imagining music: once
was run, again using musical state at the time of contact5 as the again, reports of being “drowsy” or “neither energetic nor tired” were
dependent variable. Predictor variables entered into the model were significantly negative predictors for hearing music.
ratings along the mood pairs that featured in Part A of the ESF, It might be that the quality of imagined music, rather than its
namely, Alert/Drowsy, Happy/Sad, Lonely/Connected, Energetic/ frequency, relates to mood. To test whether respondents’ mood pre-
Tired, Involved/Detached, Tense/Relaxed, and Interested/Bored. This dicted the sharpness of their musical imagery, the ratings for the
exploratory analysis enables an initial investigation into when and sharpness of each musical parameter were averaged to produce a
why people might imagine music (i.e., what is their mood?) through mean sharpness rating, against which mood ratings recorded during
comparison with the two alternative music states of “hearing music” the episode were regressed in a multiple regression analysis (see Table
or “experiencing no music.” A total of 915 cases were analyzed and 5 for a correlation matrix of these variables). Using the enter method,
the full model significantly predicted musical state (omnibus chi- a significant model emerged: F(7, 154) ⫽ 3.776, p ⫽ .001. This
square ⫽ 129.86, df ⫽ 68, p ⬍ .005). The model accounted for model explains 10.8% of the variance (Adjusted R2 ⫽ .108). Table 6
between 13.2% and 15.2% of the variance. gives information for the predictor variables entered into the model.
Only ratings of the mood pairs Alert/Drowsy (p ⫽ .01) and Lonely/ Two of the three mood ratings that relate to present-mindedness (see
Connected (p ⫽ .05) reliably predicted global musical state. An Bailes, 2007), namely, Interested/Bored and Involved/Detached, were
analysis of the coefficients revealed that ratings of “drowsy” or significant predictors of imagery sharpness. However, these ratings do
“neither alert nor drowsy” were negative predictors of imagining not align in their predictions, as sharpness increased with ratings of
music. This means that respondents were unlikely to be imagining interest, but tended to decrease with ratings of involvement.
music when they felt either drowsy or not alert. A rating of “quite
connected” significantly predicted imagining music. Feeling “neither Discussion
The current research extends the ESM study by Bailes (2006, 2007)
on music students’ everyday experiences of imagining music to a
larger sample of the general public. The results will first be discussed
in terms of the similarities and differences between these two different
populations, before focusing on what respondents imagined, and
beginning to ask why, considering how the findings support the
concept of emotion regulation through musical imagery, and the
possible roles of attention and arousal in musical imagination.

Similarities and Differences


In common with Bailes (2006, 2007), imagined music was mostly
familiar to the respondents, as evidenced by the proportion of imag-

4
Note that there were no significant differences in the total number of
Figure 1. Mean sharpness ratings of dimensions of the musical image, ESFs completed between these two OMSI groups (p ⫽ .14).
averaged across all respondents, with standard error bars. The range is from 5
The proportionally few cases of both hearing and imagining music at
1 ⫽ absent, to 7 ⫽ very sharp. Lyrics were only rated if respondents the same time were excluded from this analysis, as their inclusion led to
imagined a song. errors of quasi-complete separation.
64 BAILES

Table 5
Correlation Matrix for Mean Image Sharpness and Mood Pair Ratings

Sharpness Alert/Drowsy Happy/Sad Lonely/Connected Energetic/Tired Involved/Detached Tense/Relaxed Interested/Bored

Sharpness 1.00 .264 .194 .152 .224 .159 .032 .333


Alert/Drowsy 1.00 .579 .176 .684 .567 ⫺.096 .569
Happy/Sad 1.00 .312 .517 .456 .264 .529
Lonely/Connected 1.00 .199 .364 .213 .300
Energetic/Tired 1.00 .552 .046 .596
Involved/Detached 1.00 .050 .671
Tense/Relaxed 1.00 ⫺.028
Interested/Bored 1.00
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ined music that could be named (66%). Once again, the most common to explicitly think about music, and lesser reflection about the music
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

reason participants gave for imagining specific music was having that is both actually heard in the environment and also imagined. The
recently heard it (38%), even though this reason featured less fre- data from the current study support the latter, with a low conscious
quently than for the music students of the earlier study (46%). As awareness as to why respondents were imagining music: 12% of
before, respondents imagined a large variety of different music, with imagery episode responses indicated that the respondents “don’t know
very little overlap from one episode to another. Current respondents why” they were imagining that music, while half of that proportion
rated the sharpness of aspects of the imagined sound very similarly to (6%) had “no idea” in the earlier study of music students (Bailes,
the music students of Bailes (2007), with melody the most sharp 2007).
dimension and loudness the least sharp dimension. Music students of Beyond differences that seem to relate to the musical experience of
the earlier study gave free descriptions of rhythmic characteristics of the populations sampled, some unexpected differences emerged. In
their imagery episodes, prompting the addition of a rating scale for Bailes (2007), traveling was associated with more imagined music
tempo/rhythm in the current study. Respondents rated its sharpness than heard music, whereas in the current study, there was no signif-
above average, suggesting that tempo/rhythm is an important feature icant relationship between participants traveling and their musical
in imagined music. As before, respondents tended to imagine repeat- state. MP3 players have become ubiquitous since the earlier study,
ing fragments or loops of music. However, a substantial number of making their use during travel quite likely, while respondents might
imagery episodes involved the “hearing” of a whole piece of music, not have had such a rich source of portable music in Bailes (2007),
from start to finish. We cannot know whether participants were with participants instead imagining music. The finding in Bailes
imagining long musical passages voluntarily or involuntarily, but (2007) of a relationship between musical state (hearing, imagining, no
studies of everyday INMI that limit their scope to repetitive “ear- music) and being in the presence or absence of others was hard to
worm” phenomena, cannot account for imagining all the way through explain, and has not been replicated here (see also Beaty et al., 2013).
a piece in this way. However, the failure to replicate these two findings might be attrib-
Music students are likely to be highly engaged with music in their utable to low statistical power, which could be determined in future
everyday lives. Those in Bailes (2006, 2007) reported experiencing an work.
average of 35% imagery episodes (47% heard music episodes) while In the earlier study, the proportion of imagined music stayed fairly
the broader sample of the current study report a much lower propor- constant throughout the day, with a drop-off coinciding with an
tion of 17% imagery episodes (35% heard music). It is remarkable increase in actually hearing music at the end of the day. In the current
that this current percentage of imagery episodes is identical to that study, a different pattern emerged, since between 11 a.m. and 1 p.m.,
found by Beaty et al. (2013), who also used experience sampling the likelihood of imagining music relative to experiencing no music
methods to derive their measure. Candidate reasons for the lower was significantly higher. Because the respondents comprised mem-
proportion of imagined music across this broader cross-section of the bers of the general public rather than the exclusively student popula-
public might include hearing (and performing) less music resulting in tion sampled in Bailes (2007), their activity levels in the morning
a lower potential for material to “get stuck” in the mind, fewer reasons (e.g., going to work) could expose them to many potential triggers to
imagining music by this time. It is plausible that typically high
wakefulness at this time of day (Dijk & Lockley, 2002) in conjunction
Table 6
with exposure to triggers might have combined to raise imagined
Regression Coefficients for the Mood Pair Variables Included in
activity at this time. The odds of imagining music compared with
the Model of Imagery Sharpness
hearing music or experiencing no music were higher for men than for
Variable B SE B ␤ women in the current study. However, this finding may result from a
skew in the data, as one male respondent reported many episodes of
Alert/Drowsy 0.20 0.11 0.21 imagining (63% of 41 completed ESFs).
Happy/Sad ⫺0.09 0.13 ⫺0.07
Lonely/Connected 0.10 0.09 0.09
Energetic/Tired ⫺0.01 0.10 ⫺0.01 Individual Differences
Involved/Detached ⫺0.23 0.12 ⫺0.21ⴱ
Tense/Relaxed 0.08 0.09 0.07 As respondents increased in age, the proportion of imagery epi-
Interested/Bored 0.38 0.12 0.37ⴱⴱ sodes that they reported also increased. However, it is possible that
ⴱ ⴱⴱ
p ⫽ .06. p ⫽ .001. this can be accounted for by the positive correlation between age and
MUSIC IN MIND: AN EXPERIENCE SAMPLING STUDY 65

years of music practice (r(42) ⫽ .28, p ⫽ .03, one-tailed); a variable “Sentimental/nostalgia” 1.9%; see also Bailes, 2012), which is com-
that also related to the proportion of imagery episodes reported. parable with the percentage of codes for “affective state” reported by
Substantial differences were found between individuals in the propor- Williamson et al. (2012).
tion of time that they heard music, imagined music, or were in a “no As hypothesized, a positive relationship was found between
music” state. Respondents varied twice as much in the amount of time transliminality and the frequency of imagery episodes (Wammes &
spent imagining music as hearing music or being without music. Baruss, 2009). This suggests that the propensity for thought to cross
Individual differences with respect to what was being imagined were in and out of awareness can lead to a greater consciousness of
also apparent, with only four out of 125 imagined songs being imagined phenomena such as musical imagery. In this study, as in
imagined by more than one individual. Bailes (2006, 2007), some respondents reported both hearing and
More musically sophisticated respondents (categorized using the imagining music concurrently. This mentally intensive experience
OMSI) reported a greater proportion of musical imagery than others was less likely to occur than experiencing “no music” when working,
(see also Beaty et al., 2013). It is noteworthy that although formal interacting with others, at leisure, watching TV, or simply “being.”
musical training did not relate to the incidence of imagining music (cf. A somewhat puzzling finding from the regression of mood ratings
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Müllensiefen et al., 2014), engagement through music practice, and on musical state was an association between feeling connected and
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the composite measure (i.e., the OMSI) of formal musical training, experiencing musical imagery. This result is hard to interpret, as we
informal music experience, and musical engagement did. This is cannot be sure whether respondents felt connected to the music they
further evidence (Beaman & Williams, 2010; Beaty et al., 2013; imagined, the imagery experience itself, their physical environment,
Müllensiefen et al., 2014) for the view that it is exposure to music, and their current activity, or the people they were with at the time. Further
the degree to which it is valued that is associated with spontaneous research is warranted to consider the relationship of imagery to trance,
endogenous musical representation. Irrespective of musical back- and to elucidate whether imagining music is experienced as a form of
ground, individuals reported imagining their own music. Future re- absorption or dissociation (Herbert, 2011). Parallels have been made
search could usefully investigate the creative potential of everyday between involuntary musical imagery and mind wandering (Hyman et
musical imaginings, as even those without formal musical experience al., 2013; Williamson et al., 2014). A dominant theme in the INMI
report composing original music in mind. Indeed, there is evidence to reports analyzed by Williamson et al. (2012) was that of low attention
suggest that musical training is less important than the personality states, including mind wandering, in which “the start of the INMI
attribute of “openness” when it comes to the everyday generation of episode is marked by a period of absent-mindedness or engagement in
original musical imagery (Beaty et al., 2013). monotonous tasks” (p. 272). Such moments of “thoughtlessness” are
a characteristic occurrence in the so-called “Basic Rest Activity Cycle
(BRAC),” which describes a 90-min cycle of fluctuating states of
Mood, Arousal, and Attention
consciousness (Duchniewsk & Kokoszka, 2003). In experimental
Respondents’ musical state (hearing music, imagining music, not tasks designed to be low in cognitive load, the reappearance of
experiencing music) related to their ratings along the mood scales of “intrusive songs” was reported (Hyman et al. 2013). In the current
alert/drowsy and lonely/connected. A closer analysis of mood in study, respondents’ ratings of present-mindedness also predicted the
relation to only imagery episodes revealed that mood pairs that vary sharpness of their imagery: imagery episodes increased in sharpness
in arousal (alert/drowsy, energetic/tired) predict the likelihood of along with interest, but decreased in sharpness along with involve-
imagining music. Specifically, drowsy respondents, or respondents ment. However, caution is needed to interpret this finding, as we
who are at neither end of the alert/drowsy and energetic/tired scales, cannot be sure what aspect of their experience the respondents were
are not likely to have been imagining music. It is noteworthy that rating for interest and involvement: their mood, their engagement with
ratings of mood pairs that vary in positivity (e.g., happy/sad) did not the imagery, or their broader circumstances.
predict the likelihood of imagining music. However, looking at the Evidence of mood congruence between hearing and imagining the
level of specific pieces of music, the correlation between happy/sad same piece of music was presented above. However, such an effect
ratings for heard and imagined episodes is evidence in support of need not be explained in terms of emotion regulation. Indeed, very
mood congruence. Note that mood congruence here merely describes few affective reasons for imagining music were offered by the re-
commonalities in mood when hearing and imagining the same piece spondents, in line with Williamson and Jilka (2014), whose interview-
of music, without stipulating that imagining a particular piece will ees described only mildly valenced experiences of involuntary musi-
make one feel the same way as when that music is heard, or vice cal imagery. There is reason to suppose that music that is liked is
versa. Although mood congruence might suggest the use of musical better attended to than other music. In turn, music that has been better
imagery as an emotion-regulation strategy, going some way toward attended is more likely to stay in memory and subsequently be
answering the question of why we imagine music, no causal direction accessed as a mental image. Of the two affective dimensions most
can be inferred from a correlation. There were other parallels between commonly referenced in the literature (Russell, 1980), valence (or
the mood of participants when hearing and imagining music: the same “positivity” in the current study) is logically the most concerned with
relationship between arousal and musical experience was found for such an appetitive engagement, while arousal is typically concerned
heard musical episodes as for imagined musical episodes, with par- with the modulation of attention, which could include endogenous
ticipants less likely to be hearing music than to be experiencing no attention to imagined experience. The results of the current study are
music when they felt drowsy, or neither energetic or tired. Further suggestive, paving the way for future research to test whether valence
research is needed to determine whether people imagine music in and arousal play distinctive roles in the imaginative processing of
order to regulate their emotional state. Participants volunteered a music. People were expected to imagine music that they like, and
relatively small percentage of affective reasons for imagining music consequently it was hypothesized that a relationship would be found
(8.1%—see Table 1: “Value judgment” 3.1%, “Favorite music” 3.1%, between music that participants reported choosing to hear and their
66 BAILES

imagining of it. Although no evidence for the role of choice in heard Halpern, A. R., & Bartlett, J. C. (2011). The persistence of musical
music on the contents of musical imagination was found, this could be memories: A descriptive study of earworms. Music Perception, 28,
attributable to a lack of statistical power, or the acknowledged blunt- 425– 432. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/mp.2011.28.4.425
ness of a sampling method that captures the minority of an individ- Herbert, R. (2011). Everyday music listening: Absorption, dissociation and
trancing. Farnham, England: Ashgate Publising Limited.
ual’s experiences throughout the week.
Hyman, I. E., Jr., Burland, N. K., Duskin, H. M., Cook, M. C., Roy, C. M.,
McGrath, J. C., & Roundhill, R. F. (2013). Going gaga: Investigating,
Conclusion creating, and manipulating the song stuck in my head. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 27, 204 –215. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/acp.2897
The current research has replicated many of the findings from
Koole, S. L. (2009). The psychology of emotion regulation: An integrative
Bailes (2006, 2007), which suggests that rather than being a rarefied review. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 4 – 41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
mental exercise for the musically trained, imagining music is com- 02699930802619031
monplace and experienced in similar ways across different popula- Lange, R., Thalbourne, M. A., Houran, J., & Storm, L. (2000). The revised
tions. That music students in the earlier study had demonstrated a transliminality scale: Reliability and validity data from a Rasch top-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

greater awareness of reasons for imagining music than the current down purification procedure. Consciousness and Cognition: An Inter-
national Journal, 9, 591– 617. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/ccog.2000.0472
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sample lends support to the use of ESM: if nonexperts are less prone
to reflect on their musical imagery than experts, it is all the more Liikkanen, L. A. (2008). Music in everymind: Commonality of involuntary
important to capture their experience as it happens, before the mem- musical imagery. Paper presented at the 10th International Conference
on Music Perception and Cognition, Sapporo, Japan.
ory subsides. Further research is needed to determine whether imag-
Liikkanen, L. A. (2009). How the mind is easily hooked on musical imagery.
ining music is an effective means of emotional self-regulation, and to
Paper presented at the 7th Triennial Conference of European Society for the
explore its chronobiology. By relating musical imagery to other forms Cognitive Sciences of Music, University of Jyväskylä, Finland.
of spontaneous cognition, we can improve our understanding of why Liikkanen, L. A. (2012). Musical activities predispose to involuntary
such a relatively large proportion of our time is spent representing musical imagery. Psychology of Music, 40, 236 –256. http://dx.doi.org/
music in our mind’s ear. 10.1177/0305735611406578
Mayer, J. D., McCormick, L. J., & Strong, S. E. (1995). Mood-congruent
References memory and natural mood: new evidence. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin, 21, 736 –746.
Bailes, F. (2006). The use of experience-sampling methods to monitor Müllensiefen, D., Fry, J., Jones, R., Jilka, S. R., Stewart, L., & Williamson,
musical imagery in everyday life. Musicae Scientiae, 10, 173–190. V. J. (2014). Individual differences predict patterns in spontaneous
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/102986490601000202 involuntary musical imagery. Music Perception, 31, 323–338. http://dx
Bailes, F. (2007). The prevalence and nature of imagined music in the .doi.org/10.1525/mp.2014.31.4.323
everyday lives of music students. Psychology of Music, 35, 555–570. Ollen, J. (2006). A criterion-related validity test of selected indicators of
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0305735607077834 musical sophistication using expert ratings. (Doctor of Philosophy).
Bailes, F. (2012). Arousal, valence and the involuntary musical image. Columbus: Ohio State University.
Paper presented at the ICMPC-ESCOM 2012, Thessaloniki, Greece. Ollen, J. (2009). Cross-validation of a model for classifying musical
Beaman, C. P., & Williams, T. I. (2010). Earworms (stuck song syn- sophistication. Paper presented at the Society for Music Perception and
drome): Towards a natural history of intrusive thoughts. British Cognition Conference, Indianapolis, IN.
Journal of Psychology, 101, 637– 653. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/ Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and
000712609X479636 Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0077714
Beaman, C. P., & Williams, T. I. (2013). Individual differences in mental Västfjäll, D., Juslin, P. N., & Hartig, T. (2012). Music, subjective wellbe-
control predict involuntary musical imagery. Musicae Scientiae, 17, ing, and health: The role of everyday emotions. In R. MacDonald, G.
398 – 409. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1029864913492530 Kreutz, & L. Mitchell (Eds.), Music, health, and wellbeing (pp. 405–
Beaty, R. E., Burgin, C. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Kwapil, T. R., Hodges, D. A., 423). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. http://dx.doi.org/
& Silvia, P. J. (2013). Music to the inner ears: Exploring individual 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586974.003.0027
differences in musical imagery. Consciousness and Cognition: An In- Wammes, M., & Baruss, I. (2009). Characteristics of spontaneous musical
ternational Journal, 22, 1163–1173. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.concog imagery. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16, 37– 61.
.2013.07.006 Williamson, V. J., & Jilka, S. R. (2014). Experiencing earworms: An
Brown, S. (1999). The perpetual music track: A new psychological phe- interview study of involuntary musical imagery. Psychology of Music,
nomenon. Paper presented at the Conference on Musical Imagery, Oslo. 42, 653– 670.
Dijk, D.-J., & Lockley, S. W. (2002). Integration of human sleep-wake Williamson, V. J., Jilka, S. R., Fry, J., Finkel, S., Müllensiefen, D., &
regulation and circadian rhythmicity. Journal of Applied Physiology, 92, Stewart, L. (2012). How do “earworms” start? Classifying the everyday
852– 862. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00924.2001 circumstances of Involuntary Musical Imagery. Psychology of Music,
Duchniewsk, K., & Kokoszka, A. (2003). The protective mechanisms of 40, 259 –284. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0305735611418553
the basic rest-activity cycle as an indirect manifestation of this rhythm in Williamson, V. J., Liikkanen, L. A., Jakubowski, K., & Stewart, L. (2014).
waking: Preliminary report. The International Journal of Neuroscience, Sticky tunes: How do people react to involuntary musical imagery?
113, 153–163. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207450390162001 PLoS One, 9, e86170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0086170
MUSIC IN MIND: AN EXPERIENCE SAMPLING STUDY 67

Appendix
Experience Sampling Form (ESF)

Date: Time Contacted: a.m./p.m. Time Filled Out: a.m./p.m.

As you were contacted, were you hearing any music? YES NO


If YES, please fill out parts A & B of this form with respect to the time you were contacted

As you were contacted, were you imagining any music? YES NO


If YES, please fill out parts A & C of this form with respect to the time you were contacted

NB. If you were both hearing music and imagining music, please fill out parts A, B & C of the form

If you were neither hearing nor imagining music at the time you were contacted, please just fill in Part A with
respect to the time you were contacted.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

PART A
For each pair of moods, tick the category that most closely describes the way you were feeling when you were
contacted.
Very Quite Somewhat Neither Somewhat Quite Very
Alert Drowsy
Happy Sad
Lonely Connected
Energetic Tired
Involved Detached
Tense Relaxed
Interested Bored

Where were you?


What was the MAIN thing you were doing?

Who were you with? (Circle as many as are applicable)


Alone Partner Person/people you live with
Family member(s) Friend(s) Professional(s) (e.g. dentist)
Acquaintance(s) Strangers Person/people you work with
If not alone, how many people were you with?

PART B (Hearing music)


Where was the music coming from?
Radio TV mp3 player PA System (public place)
Record/CD/Laptop Concert Busker Rehearsal/Practice
Whistling/Humming Ring tone Other

Can you name the music you were hearing? YES NO


If YES, please name it
If NO, please circle all style categories that best describe the music you were hearing

Top 40/pop Rock Urban (rap, R&B, hip hop)


Dance/trance/house/techno Jazz/Blues Classical
Country Folk Gospel
Other (please specify)

How much personal choice did you have in hearing the music?
None at all Completely own choice
1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Not at all Very much so


How much were you concentrating on the music? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Was the music important to the activity/moment? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Do you wish you had been hearing different music? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Would you have rather not been hearing music? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

(Appendix continues)
68 BAILES

Was there anything in the music that you found particularly important or noticeable?

PART C (Imagining music)


Was the imagined music your own composition? YES NO
Can you name the music you were imagining? YES NO
If YES, please name it
If NO, please circle all style categories that best describe the music you were imagining

Top 40/pop Rock Urban (rap, R&B, hip hop)


Dance/trance/house/techno Jazz/Blues Classical
Country Folk Gospel
Other (please specify)

Had you actually heard this music since the last time you were contacted? YES NO
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

If possible, please explain why you might have been imagining that particular music
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Not at all Very much so


How aware of the imagined music were you? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
How much were you concentrating on the music? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Do you wish you had been imagining different music? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Would you have rather not been imagining music? 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Was there anything in the music that you found particularly important or noticeable?

Please describe how sharp your mental image of the following musical elements was at the time you were
contacted:
Absent Very sharp
Melody 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Lyrics (if applicable) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Instruments/Voice 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Tempo/Rhythm 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Loudness 1 2 3 4 5 6 7
Emotion/Expression 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Please give a brief description of how complete the experience was, e.g. Was the music playing in your mind
as a whole piece? Were you hearing a repeated fragment of it? Was there a visual or spatial dimension to your
musical image …?

Are you imagining the same music now? YES NO


If NO, are you imagining different music now? YES NO
If NO, are you actually hearing music now? YES NO

Please write below any additional information or comments about what was happening and/or how you
were feeling when you were imagining the music at the time you were contacted

Received July 4, 2014


Revision received February 9, 2015
Accepted March 22, 2015 䡲

You might also like