You are on page 1of 7

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 157 (2019) 114–120

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynlme

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over parietal cortex improves T


associative memory
Jovana Bjekića, , Marija V. Čolića, Marko Živanovićb, Sladjan D. Milanovića, Saša R. Filipovića

a
Department of Neuroscience, Institute for Medical Research, University of Belgrade, Serbia
b
Department of Psychology, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Belgrade, Serbia

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Keywords: Associative memory plays a key role in everyday functioning, but it declines with normal ageing as well as due to
Transcranial direct-current stimulation (tDCS) various pathological states and conditions, thus impairing quality of life. Associative memory enhancement via
Posterior parietal cortex (PPC) neurostimulation over frontal areas resulted in limited success, while posterior stimulation sites seemed to be
Cognitive enhancement more promising. We hypothesized that anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) of parietal areas
Associative memory
would lead to higher performance in associative memory due to high connectivity between posterior parietal
Face-word memory
Object-location memory
cortex (PPC) and hippocampus. Forty-two healthy adults participated in two sham-controlled cross-over ex-
periments. Anodal electrode (20 min, 1.5 mA) was placed over P3 in Experiment 1 and over P4 in Experiment 2.
During tDCS participants played a simple computer game. After each stimulation session, participants completed
parallel forms of an associative memory task (Experiment 1: face-word memory; Experiment 2: object-location
memory) and a control task (verbal fluency). In both experiments, associative memory was improved after
anodal stimulation compared to sham stimulation, while no differences were observed in the control task.
Additionally, memory performance was higher in the second than in the first trial, but the increase in perfor-
mance between the two trials did not differ between stimulation conditions. It can be concluded that a single-
session anodal tDCS over posterior parietal cortex can improve associative memory performance. The specificity,
robustness, and reproducibility of the effect suggest that PPC is a promising target for brain stimulation aiming
to enhance memory functions.

1. Introduction processes associated with the cortical region under the target electrode,
whereas cathodal stimulation has the opposite effect (Medeiros et al.,
Associative or relational memory refers to the ability to learn and 2012). Facilitatory effects of anodal tDCS have been demonstrated
remember the relationship between different and previously unrelated across multiple motor and cognitive domains in both healthy and
pieces of information in same (e.g. visual: object-location associations) clinical populations (Kuo & Nitsche, 2012). The after-effects of tDCS are
or different modalities (e.g. visual and verbal: face-name associations) considered to be NMDA-receptor dependent, and similar to the long-
(Konkel & Cohen, 2009). This type of memory plays a crucial role in term potentiation process that underlies learning and memory
everyday functioning, but it is one of the first to be affected by different (Medeiros et al., 2012). Still, the current understanding of the exact
neurological disorders (Giovanello, Verfaellie, & Keane, 2003), as well physiological consequences of tDCS on neural tissue is rudimental.
as by normal (Chalfonte & Johnson, 1996) and pathological ageing Associative memory relies on a wide network, consisting of medial
(Bastin et al., 2014). Therefore, the ability to improve associative temporal lobe structures and fronto-tempo-parietal network. Within
memory performance presents one of the most challenging tasks in that network, the hippocampus seems to be a central hub that performs
cognitive enhancement and neurorehabilitation. the binding of information from different processing modules, thus
Over the past decade, transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) creating coherent associative memories (Davachi, 2006).
has become one of the most widely used techniques for non-invasive Since memory formation and retention are achieved through high
brain stimulation. The tDCS modulates cortical excitability by deli- interconnectivity between hippocampal and anterior and posterior
vering weak electrical current between electrodes positioned over the cortical regions, many previous studies applied tDCS over frontal areas.
scalp. It is generally assumed that anodal stimulation facilitates This line of research resulted in mixed findings. A review by Kim et al


Corresponding author at: Institute for Medical Research, Dr Subotića 4, PO Box 124, Belgrade, Serbia.
E-mail address: jovana.bjekic@imi.bg.ac.rs (J. Bjekić).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2018.12.007
Received 24 July 2018; Received in revised form 9 October 2018; Accepted 12 December 2018
Available online 13 December 2018
1074-7427/ © 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
J. Bjekić et al. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 157 (2019) 114–120

Fig. 1. Procedure and associative memory tasks. Description: A – Design of experimental session; B – Face-word associative memory task; and C – Object-location
memory task.

suggested that frontal tDCS may be effective only if the stimulation is 2. Material and methods
applied during encoding (Kim, Ekstrom, & Tandon, 2016). One recent
study showed evidence of improved face-name associative memory 2.1. Participants
following anodal stimulation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, in
younger but not in older adults (Leach, McCurdy, Trumbo, Matzen, & Forty-two healthy participants aged 20–35 years were randomly
Leshikar, 2018). Two studies found the effect on recall but no effects on assigned to either first or second experiment. One participant dropped
recognition of face-name pairs (Leshikar et al., 2017; Matzen, Trumbo, out, which resulted in 20 participants (11 females, mean ± SD age:
Leach, & Leshikar, 2015), while Leach et al (Leach, McCurdy, Trumbo, 26.40 ± 3.71) taking part in Experiment 1 and another 21 (12 females,
Matzen, & Leshikar, 2016) reported no effect on recall and negative mean ± SD age: 24.15 ± 2.74) in Experiment 2. The sample size was
effect on recognition in older adults. Moreover, no effects in offline calculated a priori for the expected effect size (η2) of 0.15 and power
tDCS protocol over frontal lobe were found for episodic (Medvedeva (1 − β error probability) of 0.95. All participants were naïve to tDCS,
et al., 2018) and verbal associative memory (de Lara, Knechtges, right-handed (Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Veale, 2014) laterality
Paulus, & Antal, 2017). quotient of at least 80), had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, and
In contrast, studies that targeted posterior brain regions showed reported no history of migraines, head injuries, seizures, epilepsy and
more promising results. One research showed that tDCS over lateral other neurological or psychiatric disorders. At the time of the experi-
posterior parietal cortex (PPC), but not prefrontal cortex, decreases ment, none of the participants was pregnant or taking medication
false recognition, as well as memory bias in item and source dis- (apart from contraceptive pill), and was showing no signs of emotional
crimination (Pergolizzi & Chua, 2016). Moreover, tempo-parietal tDCS distress as measured by Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (Lovibond &
improved memory of nonword-picture pairs (Perceval, Martin, Lovibond, 1995), i.e. scores were between 0–9 for Depression, 0–7 for
Copland, Laine, & Meinzer, 2017) and object-location association in Anxiety and 0–14 for Stress. The study was conducted in accordance
older adults (Floel et al., 2012). Finally, a rTMS study showed that with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved
multiple-day stimulation of PPC has immediate and long-term effects on by the Institutional Ethics Board. All participants signed informed
associative memory, due to the high connectivity with hippocampus consent to participate and were thoroughly debriefed after completing
(Wang et al., 2014). Still, the research on the effects of tDCS over PPC the study.
on associative memory is very limited.
Here we present two experiments in which we explore the effects of
2.2. Design and procedure
anodal tDCS over PPC on the most frequently employed behavioral
associative memory paradigms. That is, following a network approach
The study adopted double-blind, cross-over sham-controlled design,
to modulating memory (Kim et al., 2016), we expect that by stimulating
with stimulation condition (anodal/sham) and trial (first/second) as
one node (i.e. PPC) of the broader network that the other cortical
within-subject factors. We opted for within-subjects design in order to
memory hubs within the network will be activated, thus enabling
control for individual differences in current responsiveness and cogni-
within-network synchronization that will lead to better memory per-
tive abilities (Li, Uehara, & Hanakawa, 2015). Within both experiments,
formance. In the first experiment we tested the effect of tDCS over left
the order of stimulation conditions was counterbalanced across parti-
PPC on face-word associative memory, and in the second, the effect of
cipants and parallel forms of associative memory tasks were used to
tDCS over right PPC on object-location memory. We hypothesize that
prevent the possible confounding effect of learning (Thair, Holloway,
anodal stimulation of parietal areas would lead to higher performance
Newport, & Smith, 2017). Experimental sessions were separated 7 days
in both associative memory tasks due to high connectivity between PPC
to minimize carryover effects (Biabani, Farrell, Zoghi, Egan, &
and hippocampus (Wang et al., 2014).
Jaberzadeh, 2017). The experiments were carried out between 10 am
and 4 pm, however, it was arranged for each participant to have both

115
J. Bjekić et al. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 157 (2019) 114–120

experimental sessions at the same time of the day to reduce the risk of 2.2.2. Object-location memory task
circadian influences (Li et al., 2015). The same software and equipment were used for the design and
In each experimental session (Fig. 1A), participants first underwent administration of stimuli as in face-memory task. The object-location
20-minute tDCS treatment (either anodal or sham), during which they task consisted of learning and recall block, as well. In the learning
played a low-demand noncompetitive computer game. The rationale block, participants were presented with color images of 15 everyday
behind introducing the game was to control for possible interference objects consecutively appearing in a 4 × 4 grid (16 positions in total)
effects of unstructured activities performed during tDCS (Horvath, for 1000 ms each. The location of the object followed pseudo-random
Carter, & Forte, 2014). In Experiment 1 participants played match-pairs pattern i.e. seven positions were unique (1 object per position), four
memory game, and in Experiment 2 they played a tile-matching puzzle positions were shared (2 objects per position) and no object appeared at
game. In match-pairs game participants need to match pairs of tiles five positions. The objects were highly familiar, i.e. met frequently in
with the same pattern, by selecting two tiles in each turn. In tile- everyday life (e.g. chair, spoon) or represented typical examples of
matching puzzle, participants had to eliminate the balls rolling around common categories (e.g. animals, musical instruments, vehicles). In the
the screen by targeting them with a color-matched ball from player’s recall block participants were presented with an image of each object in
deck. In spite of their differences, both tasks required maintaining pre-randomized order and were asked to click on its location. Thus, for
constant level of attention and manipulation of information in short- each object, there was a 6% chance of random guessing. In this task, the
term memory storage. The selection of the tasks was based on the as- measure of associative memory was the number of correctly matched
sumption that minimal engagement of same cortical circuits during objects and within-grid locations. Parallel forms of the task were de-
stimulation may promote tDCS effects (Nozari, Woodard, & Thompson- signed by 90-degrees rotation of the positions within the grid and
Schill, 2014). Different games were chosen for Experiments 1 and 2 in matching objects for category and color (e.g. apple in one form and
order to avoid proactive interference effects on the associative memory strawberry in the other).
task. In both games, participants started at the beginner’s level (e.g.
3 × 4 tiles in match-pair memory game), and progressed to upper levels 2.2.3. Verbal fluency task
each time reached an accuracy of 85%. This way, the difficulty was In the verbal fluency task, participants were presented with an al-
individually-calibrated to occupy attention, but not to exhaust partici- phabet letter and instructed to list as many unique words starting with
pants. Furthermore, every 5 min during tDCS (i.e. at minutes 3, 8, 13, the given letter. The time limit for this task was set to 45 s. This task is
and 18) participants were asked to rate the unpleasantness of the widely used to access frontal lobe functions such as executive control
treatment on a 10-point scale (1 – highly comfortable; 10 – extremely functions (Shao, Janse, Visser, & Meyer, 2014). The letters used in the
unpleasant). In Experiment 1, following tDCS participants were pre- fluency test (R and M) were randomized across sessions and stimulation
sented with the face-word memory task, which consisted of the practice conditions.
session and two trials. In the practice session, participants were pro-
vided with detailed instruction and demonstration of the task, alongside 2.3. Transcranial direct current stimulation
a few examples. Trial 1 and trail 2 of the face-word memory task were
identical, each comprising of learning and recall block, with the same The procedure followed contemporary safety standards and guide-
20 target face-word pairs. Between the two trials, participants com- lines of tDCS application in human studies (Bikson et al., 2016). The
pleted a verbal fluency task. This control task was introduced in order tDCS was delivered through constant current stimulator (Jonos-4,
to assess if the tDCS effects are stimulation site- and function-specific. Electronic Design Medical D.O.O., Belgrade, Serbia) via rubber elec-
Experiment 2 followed the same procedure, with the only difference trodes inserted in saline-soaked sponges positioned over PPC (anodal
being the associative memory task that assessed object location electrode, size: 5 × 5 cm) and contralateral cheek (reference electrode,
memory. After completing both anodal and sham condition, at the end size: 5 × 5 cm). The anodal electrode was placed over left PPC in Ex-
of the second session of the respective experiment participants were periment 1 (P3 10-20 EEG system) and right PPC in Experiment 2 (P4
asked to guess the session in which they received real stimulation in 10-20 EEG system) (Fig. 2A). In the anodal condition, the participants
order to evaluate if the blinding was successful. were administrated 1.5 mA (0.06 mA/cm2) of constant current for
20 min (15 s fade in/out), while in the sham condition the current was
administrated during first and last 30 s of the treatment period. The
2.2.1. Face-word memory task simulation of local electric fields generated by tDCS, that was per-
The task was designed in OpenSesame (Mathot, Schreij, & formed in Comets software (Jung, Kim, & Im, 2013), showed that this
Theeuwes, 2012), and presented on 23″ monitor (resolution: montage covers respective PPC areas (Fig. 2B). This particular electrode
1920 × 1080, refresh rate 60 Hz). The task consisted of learning and placement is chosen in order to target wide areas of neocortical memory
recall block. In the learning block, i.e. encoding phase, 20 face-word network with anodal electrode, thus enhancing excitability in the par-
pairs were presented for 3000 ms each in a consecutive manner. The ietal hippocampal network, and avoid any potential inhibitory effect
faces were extracted from the FEI database (Thomaz & Giraldi, 2010) under cathodal electrode as it was placed extracranially.
and were all gray-scale frontal face images (360 × 260 pixels). The
words were high-frequency and high-imaginability consisting of 2 or 3 2.4. Data analysis
syllables (e.g. banana /banana/, prozor /window/, balon /balloon/,
etc.). In the recall block, images from the first block were mixed with 30 Paired-samples t-test was used to asses differences between anodal
new distractor faces and these 50 images were consecutively presented and sham condition with regard to the unpleasantness of the tDCS
in a pre-randomized order. Participants were first asked to identify if procedure and the control task. Statistical significance for main effects
the face was presented in the learning block and if so to recall the word and interaction was determined using 2 × 2 repeated measures ANOVA
that was presented alongside it (Fig. 1B). Thus, in this block partici- with stimulation condition (anodal/sham) and trial (first/second) as
pants had first to perform item-memory task, and then proceed to as- within-subject factors; and the associative memory performance as
sociative memory free recall. The number of correctly recalled words dependent variable, i.e. the number of correctly recalled face/word
that matched correctly recognized faces was used as a measure of as- pairs in Experiment 1 and the number of correctly recalled object-lo-
sociative memory. Parallel forms of the task were designed by replacing cation pairs in Experiment 2. Moreover, MANOVAs, with follow-up
male faces with females, and vice versa, and matching words in terms of ANOVAs were used to test the effect of possible confounding factors, i.e.
frequency, imaginability, length, and category (e.g. breskva /peach/ in participants’ gender (male/female), stimulation order (1st/2nd ses-
one form and višnja /cherry/ in the other). sion), task form (A/B) on memory performance across trials and

116
J. Bjekić et al. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 157 (2019) 114–120

Fig. 2. tDCS montage. Description: A – Electrode placement in Experiment 1 (anode, marked red, over left PPC) and Experiment 2 (anode over right PPC); B –
Simulation of electric fields for 1.5 mA with PPC-contralateral cheek montage of tDCS (1 – anode; 2 – cathode), where red indicates positive electric potential.

conditions. Finally, the tDCS unpleasantness ratings were correlated Table 1


(Spearman’s rho) with associative memory performance for each con- Task performance (mean scores and standard deviations) under anodal and
dition and trial. sham stimulation in both trials.
Anodal stimulation Sham stimulation
3. Results
Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 1 Trial 2
3.1. tDCS procedure evaluation
Experiment 1 Face-word task 5.00 10.00 3.80 8.20
(2.88) (3.32) (2.19) (3.08)
Overall, participants found the procedure only moderately un- Fluency task 13.30 (3.95) 13.15 (3.36)
pleasant and rated it below 5 for all time points on 1–10 Likert type Experiment 2 Object-location 7.48 8.48 6.24 7.48
scale. The unpleasantness ratings were slightly higher for anodal than task (2.99) (3.60) (2.63) (3.40)
sham condition [Experiment 1: Manodal = 2.63, SD = 1.90; Fluency task 11.71 (2.86) 12.29 (2.74)
Msham = 2.02, SD = 1.43; t (19) = 2.221, p = .039; Experiment 2:
Manodal = 3.16, SD = 2.01; Msham = 2.04, SD = 1.04; t (20) = 2.522,
p = .020]. Still, the guessing of stimulation conditions was not above between two trials, but stimulation had no effect on learning slope, i.e.
the chance level (40% of accuracy in Experiment 1; 38% of accuracy in the difference in performance between two trials did not differ between
Experiment 2), thus confirming the successful blinding of the tDCS anodal and sham condition.
procedure. The results of the Experiment 2, followed the same pattern. The
repeated measures ANOVA with object-location recall as output vari-
3.2. Effects on associative memory able showed the effect of stimulation condition [F(1, 20) = 4.516,
p = .046, η2 = 0.184], as well as the effect of learning [F(1,
Descriptive statistics (means and standard deviations) for all out- 20) = 7.160, p = .015, η2 = 0.264], but again without interaction ef-
come measures, i.e. associative memory performance and verbal flu- fect [F(1, 20) = 0.100, p = .755].
ency are presented in Table 1.
In the Experiment 1, the repeated measures ANOVA showed the 3.3. Control task and possible confounding effects
effect of stimulation condition [F(1, 19) = 5.446, p = .031,
η2 = 0.223], as well as the effect of learning [F(1, 19) = 110.16, No effect of tDCS was observed on verbal fluency task in Experiment
p < .001, η2 = 0.853], but no interaction effect [F(1, 19) = 0.872, 1(t (19) = 0.212, p = .843), nor in Experiment 2 (t (20) = 0.890,
p = .362]. In other words, face-word memory performance was higher p = .384). In other words, the performance on control task was not
after anodal stimulation, participants showed an increase in recall modulated by tDCS in neither of the experiments.

117
J. Bjekić et al. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 157 (2019) 114–120

There were no confounding effects on memory performance in ei- conclusion and further implies that the observed effects are stimulation
ther of the experiments. The analysis showed no effect of participants' site- and task-specific. Namely, it seems that anodal stimulation over
gender [Experiment 1: F(4, 9) = 0.974, p = .467; Experiment 2: F(4, PPC affects processes that are engaged in associative memory without
10) = 2.557, p = .104], no effect of stimulation order [Experiment 1: F affecting other cognitive processes. In addition, the fact that the similar
(4, 9) = 0.371, p = .824; Experiment 2: F(4, 10) = 0.377, p = .820], effect sizes are obtained in both face-word and object-location experi-
and no effect of task form [Experiment 1: F(4, 15) = 1.172, p = .385; ment adds to the robustness of the parietal tDCS effects and supports
Experiment 2: F(4, 10) = 0.410, p = .798.]. Moreover, the tDCS un- the notion that posterior regions are highly engaged in multimodal
pleasantness ratings did not correlate with associative memory perfor- associative memory. Lastly, given that there were no effects of stimu-
mance in any of the conditions (p > .500 for both trials within both lation order, task form or participants’ gender, as well as no correlation
experiments). between unpleasantness and memory performance, we can exclude that
any of these factors mediated the tDCS effect on associative memory in
4. Discussion either of the experiments.
Another difference between previous studies and this one is the
This study investigated the effects of anodal tDCS over PPC on as- game played during tDCS. Namely, in both experiments participants
sociative memory in two separate yet closely related experiments. In performed a cognitively engaging game with non-interfering material
order to explore the robustness of the effects, the first experiment as- (e.g. color patterns and face-word pairs). There are indications that
sessed face-word associative memory, while the second tested the ob- engaging target cortical circuits during stimulation may promote the
ject-location memory performance. In both experiments, the associative after-effects of tDCS (Nozari et al., 2014). It could be argued that cog-
memory performance increased following a single session of anodal nitive activity during tDCS contributed, at least partially, to the sub-
tDCS over PPC, while performance on control task remained the same. sequent effects on associative memory.
Thus, the current study demonstrates the potential of posterior stimu- As expected, the significant effect of between-trials learning was
lation site and challenges the notion that single tDCS session has no observed – i.e. the participants showed a positive learning curve, thus
reliable effect on memory performance (Horvath, Forte, & Carter, 2015; the performance in the second trial was consistently higher than in the
Price & Hamilton, 2015). It could be argued that the inconsistencies in first. But surprisingly, tDCS had no effect on learning slope – the
findings of previous studies may stem from the choice of stimulation amount of new information encoded during the repeated presentation
site (anterior vs posterior regions), and other stimulation parameters, as of the stimuli (i.e. learning block) was equal across conditions. Since
well as the heterogeneous outcome measures used to assess relational/ this finding was consistent across both experiments, it cannot be at-
associative memory. tributed to properties of the task, such as material or relative item-
Even though face-word and object-location tasks are the two most difficulty. Furthermore, other studies that varied the number of
frequently employed associative memory paradigms, and often used learning trials also showed no interaction of stimulation condition
interchangeably, they differ in several important aspects. On one hand, (anodal/sham) and the number of stimuli presentations (once/twice)
face-word memory relies on both visual and verbal encoding of in- (Leshikar et al., 2017). One possible explanation is that the tDCS pre-
formation and requires cross-modal binding. On the other hand, object- dominantly modulated the activity of hippocampal subregions that
location memory is expected to engage visuospatial processing and underline retrieval, but not encoding of previously formed associations.
intra-modal binding. Although the underlining processes involved in This functional subdivision of hippocampus during encoding and re-
cross-modal and intra-modal binding are still a matter of debate (Del trieval of relational memories was shown in high-resolution fMRI study
Gatto, Brunetti, & Delogu, 2016), there is a great deal of evidence that (Zeineh, Engel, Thompson, & Bookheimer, 2003), but the behavioral
retrieval of different types of materials is associated with distinct brain distinction of these processes is very difficult to demonstrate in any
regions (Galli & Otten, 2011). Namely, neuroimaging studies have cognitive task. The only possible experimental dissociation would have
shown that the memory of words primarily activates frontal and tem- to rely on the assumption that multiple presentations of stimuli enhance
poral areas of the left hemisphere, whereas memory of objects and faces encoding but not the recall component of memory performance. That is,
activates posterior parts of the right hemisphere (Kim et al., 1999; repeated presentations enable better encoding, while the retrieval of
Simons, Graham, Owen, Patterson, & Hodges, 2001). Still, fMRI studies the information is only indirectly affected, as the deeper encoded in-
provide evidence for parietal old/new effect in recollection of both formation is easier to retrieve. Thus, the increased performance fol-
verbal and visual memories (Guerin & Miller, 2009). Furthermore, an lowing multiple presentation trials does not per se reveal the process
EEG-fMRI study showed that the amplitude of parietal recollection ef- that has been facilitated. Still, the difference or absence of differences
fect is associated with the activation of posterior hippocampus, para- between sham and active stimulation condition in the learning slope
hippocampal cortex and retrosplenial cortex for different types of sti- could indicate if tDCS affected both encoding and recall or just recall,
muli (Hoppstädter, Baeuchl, Diener, Flor, & Meyer, 2015). Finally, a since the number of presentations was the same in both conditions.
large body of evidence speaks to the central role of hippocampus and Bearing all this in mind, we could tentatively argue that tDCS dom-
adjacent structures in constituting relational memories across mod- inantly facilitated recall of relational memories. In order to provide
alities (Horecka et al., 2018; Konkel & Cohen, 2009; Olsen, Moses, more compelling evidence further studies that would combine high-
Riggs, & Ryan, 2012). Overall, these findings suggest that parietal definition neuroimaging techniques and neurostimulation are needed.
cortex may be the most appropriate target for neurostimulation aiming We accounted for the type of material to be retrieved by placing the
to enhance memory performance in different modalities due to its high active electrode over left PPC for the recall of verbal stimuli and over
functional connectivity with hippocampus as a key relay for cross- right for the recall of visuospatial information. The lateralization of
modal binding and memory formation. visual and verbal material processing has been well documented (Kelley
Unlike some previous studies (de Lara et al., 2017; Leach et al., et al., 1998; Weber, Fliessbach, Lange, Kügler, & Elger, 2007), espe-
2016, 2018; Leshikar et al., 2017; Matzen et al., 2015; Medvedeva cially in neuropsychological studies (Cohen, 1992; Wagner, Sziklas,
et al., 2018), the montage of electrodes in present experiments targeted Garver, & Jones-Gotman, 2009). Our study shows that unilateral tDCS
areas of posterior cortex which have been shown to play important role affects associative memory performance in a material congruent
in formation and retrieval of relational memories. Hence the positive manner. Still, some studies, challenge traditional notions on functional
effects of anodal stimulation over PPC could be attributed to the fa- lateralization and provide evidence for bilateral activation while pro-
cilitation of modality-general binding process and subsequent retrieval cessing verbal, as well as visual material (e.g. (Guerin & Miller, 2009;
of relational memories within the hippocampal-parietal circuit. The Rugg & Vilberg, 2013). Further studies are needed to test the re-
consistent absence of the effect on verbal fluency supports this lationship between left/right stimulation site and the type of associative

118
J. Bjekić et al. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 157 (2019) 114–120

memory material. 2015.01.400.


Jung, Y.-J., Kim, J.-H., & Im, C.-H. (2013). COMETS: A MATLAB toolbox for simulating
local electric fields generated by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS).
5. Conclusion Biomedical Engineering Letters, 3(1), 39–46. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13534-013-
0087-x.
A single-session of anodal tDCS over posterior parietal cortex can Kelley, W. M., Miezin, F. M., McDermott, K. B., Buckner, R. L., Raichle, M. E., Cohen, N.
J., ... Petersen, S. E. (1998). Hemispheric specialization in human dorsal frontal
induce function-specific changes in associative memory performance in cortex and medial temporal lobe for verbal and nonverbal memory encoding. Neuron,
both face-word and object-location task. This study produced evidence 20(5), 927–936. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80474-2.
in favor of parietal cortex as a promising stimulation target for memory Kim, J. J., Andreasen, N. C., O’Leary, D. S., Wiser, A. K., Boles Ponto, L. L., Watkins, G. L.,
& Hichwa, R. D. (1999). Direct comparison of the neural substrates of recognition
enhancement. memory for words and faces. Brain, 122(6), 1069–1083. https://doi.org/10.1093/
brain/122.6.1069.
Acknowledgments Kim, K., Ekstrom, A. D., & Tandon, N. (2016). A network approach for modulating
memory processes via direct and indirect brain stimulation: Toward a causal ap-
proach for the neural basis of memory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 134(Part
This work was supported by a Grant [OI175012] from the Ministry A), 162–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2016.04.001.
of Education, Science and Technological Development of Republic of Konkel, A., & Cohen, N. J. (2009). Relational memory and the hippocampus:
Serbia. The authors would like to thank Jelena Vujičić (Experiment 1) Representations and methods. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 3(SEP), 166–174. https://doi.
org/10.3389/neuro.01.023.2009.
and Milena Dimitrijević (Experiment 2) for technical support and as- Kuo, M.-F., & Nitsche, M. A. (2012). Effects of transcranial electrical stimulation on
sistance in data collection. cognition. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 43(3), 192–199. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1550059412444975.
Leach, R. C., McCurdy, M. P., Trumbo, M. C., Matzen, L. E., & Leshikar, E. D. (2016).
Declaration of interest Transcranial stimulation over the left inferior frontal gyrus increases false alarms in
an associative memory task in older adults. Healthy Aging Research, 5. https://doi.
Authors confirm that there are no known conflicts of interest asso- org/10.1097/01.HXR.0000491108.83234.85.
Leach, R. C., McCurdy, M. P., Trumbo, M. C., Matzen, L. E., & Leshikar, E. D. (2018).
ciated with this publication and there has been no significant financial Differential age effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on associative
support for this work that could have influenced its outcome. memory. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1093/
geronb/gby003.
Leshikar, E. D., Leach, R. C., Mccurdy, M. P., Trumbo, M. C., Sklenar, A. M., Frankenstein,
References A. N., & Matzen, L. E. (2017). Neuropsychologia Transcranial direct current stimu-
lation of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during encoding improves recall but not re-
Bastin, C., Bahri, M. A., Mievis, F., Lemaire, C., Collette, F., Genon, S., ... Salmon, E. cognition memory. Neuropsychologia, 106(June), 390–397. https://doi.org/10.1016/
(2014). Associative memory and its cerebral correlates in Alzheimer’s disease: j.neuropsychologia.2017.10.022.
Evidence for distinct deficits of relational and conjunctive memory. Neuropsychologia, Li, L. M., Uehara, K., & Hanakawa, T. (2015). The contribution of interindividual factors
63(1), 99–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.023. to variability of response in transcranial direct current stimulation studies. Frontiers in
Biabani, M., Farrell, M., Zoghi, M., Egan, G., & Jaberzadeh, S. (2017). Crossover design in Cellular Neuroscience, 9, 181. https://doi.org/10.3389/fncel.2015.00181.
transcranial direct current stimulation studies on motor learning: Potential pitfalls Lovibond, S. H., & Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual for the depression anxiety stress scales.
and difficulties in interpretation of findings. Reviews in the Neurosciences. https://doi. Sydney, N.S.W.: Psychology Foundation of Australia.
org/10.1515/revneuro-2017-0056. Mathot, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical
Bikson, M., Grossman, P., Thomas, C., Zannou, A. L., Jiang, J., Adnan, T., ... Woods, A. J. experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2),
(2016). Safety of transcranial direct current stimulation: Evidence based update 314–324. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7.
2016. Brain Stimulation, 9(5), 641–661. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2016.06.004. Matzen, L. E., Trumbo, M. C., Leach, R. C., & Leshikar, E. D. (2015). Effects of non-
Chalfonte, B.l., & Johnson, M. K. (1996). Feature memory and binding in young and older invasive brain stimulation on associative memory. Brain Research, 1624, 286–296.
adults. Memory & Cognition, 24(4), 403–416. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200930. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2015.07.036.
Cohen, M. (1992). Auditory/verbal and visual/spatial memory in children with complex Medeiros, L. F., de Souza, I. C. C., Vidor, L. P., de Souza, A., Deitos, A., Volz, M. S., ...
partial epilepsy of temporal lobe origin. Brain and Cognition, 20(2), 315–326. https:// Torres, I. L. S. (2012). Neurobiological effects of transcranial direct current stimu-
doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(92)90024-G. lation: A review. Frontiers in Psychiatry. Frontiers, 22, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/
Davachi, L. (2006). Item, context and relational episodic encoding in humans. Current fpsyt.2012.00110.
Opinion in Neurobiology, 16(6), 693–700. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CONB.2006.10. Medvedeva, A., Materassi, M., Neacsu, V., Beresford-Webb, J., Hussin, A., Khan, N., &
012. Galli, G. (2018). Effects of anodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the
de Lara, G. A., Knechtges, P. N., Paulus, W., & Antal, A. (2017). Anodal tDCS over the left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex on episodic memory formation and retrieval. Cerebral
DLPFC did not affect the encoding and retrieval of verbal declarative information. Cortex, (March), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhx347.
Frontiers in Neuroscience, 11(AUG), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2017.00452. Nozari, N., Woodard, K., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2014). Consequences of cathodal
Del Gatto, C., Brunetti, R., & Delogu, F. (2016). Cross-modal and intra-modal binding stimulation for behavior: When does it help and when does it hurt performance? PLoS
between identity and location in spatial working memory: The identity of objects ONE, 9(1), https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0084338.
does not help recalling their locations. Memory, 24(5), 603–615. https://doi.org/10. Olsen, R. K., Moses, S. N., Riggs, L., & Ryan, J. D. (2012). The hippocampus supports
1080/09658211.2015.1034137. multiple cognitive processes through relational binding and comparison. Frontiers in
Floel, A., Suttorp, W., Kohl, O., Korten, J., Lohmann, H., Breitenstein, C., & Knecht, S. Human Neuroscience, 6(May), 1–13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00146.
(2012). Non-invasive brain stimulation improves object-location learning in the el- Perceval, G., Martin, A. K., Copland, D. A., Laine, M., & Meinzer, M. (2017). High-defi-
derly. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(8), 1682–1689. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. nition tDCS of the temporo-parietal cortex enhances access to newly learned words.
neurobiolaging.2011.05.007. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-17279-0.
Galli, G., & Otten, L. J. (2011). Material-specific neural correlates of recollection: Objects, Pergolizzi, D., & Chua, E. F. (2016). Transcranial direct current stimulation over the
words, and faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(6), 1405–1418. https://doi. parietal cortex alters bias in item and source memory tasks. Brain and Cognition, 108,
org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21442. 56–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2016.06.009.
Giovanello, K. S., Verfaellie, M., & Keane, M. M. (2003). Disproportionate deficit in as- Price, A. R., & Hamilton, R. H. (2015). A re-evaluation of the cognitive effects from single-
sociative recognition relative to item recognition in global amnesia. Cognitive, session transcranial direct current stimulation. Brain Stimulation, 8(3), 663–665.
Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 3(3), 186–194. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2015.03.007.
3.3.186. Rugg, M. D., & Vilberg, K. L. (2013). Brain networks underlying episodic memory re-
Guerin, S. A., & Miller, M. B. (2009). Lateralization of the parietal old/new effect: An trieval. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(2), 255–260. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
event-related fMRI study comparing recognition memory for words and faces. conb.2012.11.005.
NeuroImage, 44(1), 232–242. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.08.035. Shao, Z., Janse, E., Visser, K., & Meyer, A. S. (2014). What do verbal fluency tasks
Hoppstädter, M., Baeuchl, C., Diener, C., Flor, H., & Meyer, P. (2015). Simultaneous EEG- measure? Predictors of verbal fluency performance in older adults. Frontiers in
fMRI reveals brain networks underlying recognition memory ERP old/new effects. Psychology, 5, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00772.
NeuroImage, 116, 112–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.026. Simons, J. S., Graham, K. S., Owen, A. M., Patterson, K., & Hodges, J. R. (2001).
Horecka, K. M., Dulas, M. R., Schwarb, H., Lucas, H. D., Duff, M., & Cohen, N. J. (2018). Perceptual and semantic components of memory for objects and faces: A pet study.
Reconstructing relational information. Hippocampus, 28(2), 164–177. https://doi. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(4), 430–443. https://doi.org/10.1162/
org/10.1002/hipo.22819. 08989290152001862.
Horvath, J. C., Carter, O., & Forte, J. D. (2014). Transcranial direct current stimulation: Thair, H., Holloway, A. L., Newport, R., & Smith, A. D. (2017). Transcranial direct current
Five important issues we aren’t discussing (but probably should be). Frontiers in stimulation (tDCS): A beginner’s guide for design and implementation. Frontiers in
Systems Neuroscience, 8, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00002. Neuroscience, 11, 641. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2017.00641.
Horvath, J. C., Forte, J. D., & Carter, O. (2015). Quantitative review finds no evidence of Thomaz, C. E., & Giraldi, G. A. (2010). A new ranking method for Principal Components
cognitive effects in healthy populations from single-session transcranial direct current Analysis and its application to face image analysis. Image and Vision Computing, 28(6),
stimulation (tDCS). Brain Stimulation, 8(3), 535–550. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs. 902–913.

119
J. Bjekić et al. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 157 (2019) 114–120

Veale, J. F. (2014). Edinburgh Handedness Inventory – Short Form: A revised version and associative memory. Science, 345(6200), 1054–1057. https://doi.org/10.1126/
based on confirmatory factor analysis. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and science.1252900.
Cognition, 19(2), 164–177. https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2013.783045. Weber, B., Fliessbach, K., Lange, N., Kügler, F., & Elger, C. E. (2007). Material-specific
Wagner, D. D., Sziklas, V., Garver, K. E., & Jones-Gotman, M. (2009). Material-specific memory processing is related to language dominance. NeuroImage, 37(2), 611–617.
lateralization of working memory in the medial temporal lobe. Neuropsychologia, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.022.
47(1), 112–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.010. Zeineh, M. M., Engel, S. A., Thompson, P. M., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (2003). Dynamics of
Wang, J. X., Rogers, L. M., Gross, E. Z., Ryals, A. J., Dokucu, M. E., Brandstatt, K. L., ... the hippocampus during encoding and retrieval of face-name pairs. Science,
Voss, J. L. (2014). Targeted enhancement of cortical-hippocampal brain networks 299(5606), 577–580. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1077775.

120

You might also like