You are on page 1of 13

Sex Roles (2011) 65:854–866

DOI 10.1007/s11199-011-0048-8

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

The Effects of Sample Characteristics and Experience


with Infidelity on Romantic Jealousy
Colleen M. Varga & Christina B. Gee & Geoffrey Munro

Published online: 28 July 2011


# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Abstract Past research has suggested that men are more replicable gender difference across the lifespan may be
upset by imagined sexual than emotional infidelity, and explained by two related factors: age and actual experience
women are more upset by imagined emotional infidelity with infidelity.
than sexual infidelity. However, experience with infidelity,
methodology, and age and gender of the sample may help to Keywords Jealousy . Evolutionary theory . Continuous-scale
explain inconsistent results. Two hundred ninety-four methodology . Gender differences
English-speaking undergraduate students and 325 non-
college adults in a large mid-Atlantic urban area of the U.
S. completed forced-choice or continuous-scale anonymous Introduction
questionnaires regarding jealousy over a mate’s hypotheti-
cal infidelity. Chi-square and MANOVA analyses replicated Gender differences in jealousy have been hotly debated for
previous findings of the expected gender difference in all two decades, since Buss et al.’s (1992) seminal study with
hypothetical forced-choice scenarios. However, results for U.S. college-age students promulgated the evolutionary view
those participants who reported experience with actual that men are more jealous over a mate’s sexual infidelity
infidelity demonstrated little support for the traditional while women are more jealous over a mate’s emotional
evolutionary model, as there were no gender differences in infidelity. Since that time, debate has focused largely on the
which aspect of hypothetical infidelity was reported to be existence of these possible gender differences, disputing
more distressing, and no gender differences at the college questions of theoretical soundness (e.g. Buss et al. 1996;
level in terms of which aspect of infidelity received the DeSteno and Salovey 1996; Harris and Christenfeld 1996;
greatest focus. These findings, extrapolated from both Hupka and Bank 1996) and study methodology (e.g.
undergraduates and adults and accounting for the impact DeSteno et al. 2002; Hofhansl et al. 2004). Much of the
of actual, primed memory of experience of infidelity on earlier work has focused on reactions to hypothetical
hypothetical jealousy scenarios, raise important questions scenarios of infidelity with college-age samples, which are
about the validity of hypothetical scenarios of jealousy as of questionable validity in resolving the debate. Little
proxies for real reactions to actual infidelity. The results of research in the U.S. has focused on experience of infidelity
the present study suggest that the lack of a consistent, in jealousy and sample characteristics across the lifespan or
how these factors impact the question of gender differences.
In response, the current study utilized both forced-choice and
C. M. Varga (*) : C. B. Gee
continuous-scale methodologies to examine the effect of
Department of Psychology, The George Washington University,
2125 G St. NW, age and gender, as well as actual experience of infidelity
Washington, DC 20052, USA in U.S. undergraduate students and non-college age
e-mail: cvarga@gwmail.gwu.edu adults. Notably, all studies reviewed here are based on
U.S. undergraduate samples using hypothetical scenarios
G. Munro
Department of Psychology, Towson University, unless otherwise noted. The present study builds on past
Towson, MD, USA research to explore methodological contributing factors
Sex Roles (2011) 65:854–866 855

and individual differences in a U.S. sample, allowing a Social role theorists have noted that a strong version of
more thorough and nuanced understanding of the the parental investment model would always find the
complex factors contributing to the relationship between majority of men selecting hypothetical sexual infidelity as
gender and jealousy in response to infidelity. more upsetting and the majority of women selecting
emotional infidelity as more upsetting (Harris 2003b;
Hypothetical Reactions to Infidelity Hupka and Bank 1996). Such a scenario would be
indicative of a consistent strategy particular to each gender.
The vast majority of studies examining gender differences However, several hypothetical scenario studies with
in reactions to infidelity have asked participants to respond college-age samples (Buss et al. 1999; Buunk et al. 1996;
to hypothetical scenarios of infidelity, without determina- Harris 2002; Murphy et al. 2006) and one study with an
tion of past experience of infidelity or recall and measure- adult sample (mean age = 26 years: Sheets and Wolfe 2001)
ment of participants’ response when they experienced past found that although more men than women reported that
infidelity. These hypothetical reactions to infidelity have hypothetical sexual infidelity would be upsetting, the
been widely studied from two different perspectives; that of majority of men still felt that emotional infidelity would
the evolutionary model and the social role model. The be more upsetting than sexual infidelity. In sum, testing of
evolutionary model espoused by Buss and colleagues stems the traditional hypothetical forced-choice scenario has
from the parental investment model, which holds that consistently demonstrated a gender difference whereby
females are more discriminant in mating due to the larger men report that they would be more upset by imagined
investment required in parenting, while males compete for sexual infidelity than women report they would be,
access to females (Trivers 1972). As males risk investing although the degree of expected distress over sexual
resources in offspring without paternal certainty, Buss et al. infidelity for men has not always been higher than distress
(1992) extended the logic of parental investment to apply to over emotional infidelity.
jealousy in humans. In particular, Buss et al. (1992) Research using hypothetical continuous-scale measures
proposed that men are more sexually jealous because they to test the parental investment model has produced mixed
possess an innate, biological tendency to advance their results, but overall, consistent gender differences have not
reproductive success by attempting to ensure the fidelity of been identified. An unpublished meta-analysis of romantic
their female mate. Females, on the other hand, are posited jealousy studies with both adult and college-age samples
to be more emotionally jealous because they need to secure found that continuous-scale data did not replicate forced-
the emotional interest, resources, and protection of a choice data across 77 studies (Hofhansl et al. 2004). For
powerful male in order to propagate their progeny (Buss example, DeSteno et al. (2002) found with a college-age
et al. 1992; Wiederman and Allgeier 1993; Wiederman and sample that both men and women reported that they would
Kendall 1999). According to this theory, jealousy is an be more upset by hypothetical sexual infidelity, and no
adaptive emotion which evolved as a “coping device” gender differences were found when using a Likert measure
passed down by ancestral humans to help humans survive of agreement/disagreement and a checklist of target feeling
and reproduce (Buss 2000, p. 5). This gender-specific adjectives. Several other studies have replicated this lack of
difference model has also been referred to as an evolved consistent gender differences in college-age samples
jealousy mechanism (EJM), sexual strategies theory, or (Dantzker and Eisenman 2005; Eisenman and Dantzker
jealousy as a specific innate module (JSIM) theory (Harris 2006; Takahashi et al. 2006) and one adult sample with a
2003b; Schutzwohl 2007). mean age of 48 years (Green and Sabini 2006).
Using two hypothetical forced-choice scenarios, Buss et The inconsistent results found when using hypothetical
al. (1992) found that 60% of U.S. college-age men reported forced-choice versus continuous-scale methodologies have
that they would have greater distress over imagined sexual not yet been fully explained. Several researchers have
infidelity than over imagined emotional infidelity, while suggested that the conflicting results may be due to women
83% of college-age women reported that they would have having a greater tendency to report any emotions in a
greater distress over imagined emotional infidelity than relationship context more strongly than do men (Penke and
over imagined sexual infidelity. These findings have been Asendorpf 2008; Sagarin and Guadagno 2004). However,
replicated extensively in the U.S. using hypothetical Hofhansl et al. (2004) found that gender differences in
scenarios (Brase et al. 2004; Buunk et al. 1996; Hupka sexual jealousy typically disappeared when tested with
and Bank 1996; Levy and Kelly 2010; Murphy et al. 2006; continuous-scale methodologies, while differences in emo-
Shackelford et al. 2002). In a meta-analysis based on 72 tional jealousy remained in about half of the studies
samples, including both college-age and adult samples and reviewed. Thus, emotional jealousy may be uniquely
unpublished studies, Hofhansl et al. (2004) reported a predicted by gender, at least in part, while the evidence
moderate effect size of .64 for overall gender differences. for a consistent difference in sexual jealousy across
856 Sex Roles (2011) 65:854–866

multiple methodologies is less substantial (Hofhansl et al. studies yielded differing results; one study found no age
2004; Penke and Asendorpf 2008). attenuation within an adult sample, although this adult sample
While considerable research has been devoted to had a mean age of 26 and included both college students and
methodological explanations for differing constructs of non-students (Edlund et al. 2006). Sabini and Green (2004)
male and female sexual and emotional jealousy, the found that an undergraduate sample demonstrated a gender
literature has by and large ignored the validity of examining difference, but within an adult sample (mean age of 41 years
this construct by use of hypothetical scenarios. This lacuna old), both genders reported that they would be more upset by
is problematic for several reasons. First, responses to hypothetical emotional infidelity than hypothetical sexual
hypothetical scenarios vary by methodology within indi- infidelity.
viduals (e.g. DeSteno et al.’s 2002 study with a college-age In regards to hypothetical continuous-scale studies, the
sample). Second, individuals’ responses to hypothetical data are stronger for an age effect. One study completed
scenarios do not correspond with actual experience. For with a representative adult sample found no gender
example, the expected gender difference was found when difference (Green and Sabini 2006), while other studies
participants were asked to imagine a hypothetical forced- with college-age samples demonstrated a gender difference
choice scenario consistent with Buss et al.’s (1992) survey, in the expected direction (e.g. Becker et al. 2004; Sagarin
but no gender difference was found when participants were and Guadagno 2004). Two meta-analyses also found an age
asked to recall a specific incidence of experienced infidelity effect; for example, in one meta-analysis; the average effect
in either a college-age (Berman and Frazier 2005) or an size was .74 for student samples and .45 for community
adult sample with a mean age of 37 years (Harris 2002). samples (Harris 2003b; Hofhansl et al. 2004). Thus, there is
Third, responses to hypothetical scenarios do not corre- insufficient evidence that U.S. adult samples reflect the
spond with physiological responses; several studies have same gender difference in sexual versus emotional jealousy
produced inconsistent results regarding physiological arous- as is typically found in hypothetical jealousy studies with
al in response to imagined infidelity in college-age samples undergraduate students. The current study examined both
(Buss et al. 1992; Harris 2000; Pietrzak et al. 2002). In fact, U.S. undergraduate students and adults to fill a gap in the
one study of college-age participants produced contradic- literature by allowing direct comparison of the multiple
tory results; based on electrodermal activity, men showed factors affecting romantic jealousy.
the greatest reactance to imagined emotional infidelity,
while women reacted most to imagined sexual infidelity Experience with Infidelity
(Grice and Seely 2000).
Very few studies have examined how the actual experience
Age as Moderator of infidelity affects report of jealousy in response to
hypothetical scenarios, and even fewer have examined
Another concern in the examination of gender differences individuals’ immediate jealousy response following an
using hypothetical scenarios is the overwhelming use of identified experience of infidelity. A scan of applicable
college-age samples. According to Harris’ (2003b) meta- databases (Academic Search Premier, PsycInfo, JSTOR,
analysis, of 29 attempted replications of the original Buss et and Google Scholar) identified four peer-reviewed studies
al. (1992) study conducted with heterosexual populations, and one unpublished dissertation, all using U.S. samples,
24 studies used undergraduates. The overwhelming use of which examined gender differences in responses to hypo-
such a restricted sample is problematic because under- thetical scenarios for individuals with actual experience
graduates’ relative inexperience with long-term emotional with infidelity. Of these, every study replicated the
and sexual relationships may cause them to have different predicted gender difference when using the forced-choice
responses to infidelity than older, more experienced hypothetical scenario with participants who had not
populations (Sabini and Green 2004). Perhaps as a experienced infidelity, whereas four out of five found no
consequence of the “hook-up” culture common to college gender differences in jealousy when examining the
campuses (Garcia and Reiber 2008), two studies with responses of participants who had actual experience with
undergraduates found that both men and women in short- infidelity (Berman and Frazier 2005; Edlund et al. 2006;
term relationships were more threatened by hypothetical Harris 2002, 2003a; Kimeldorf 2009). Harris (2002, 2003a)
sexual infidelity, while men and women in long-term examined actual experience with infidelity in both an
relationships were more threatened by emotional infidelity undergraduate and adult sample using continuous measures;
(Mathes 2005; Wade and Fowler 2006). both men and women reported that they focused more on
Research results have been inconsistent in regard to the the emotional aspect of imagined infidelity. However, as
effects of age as a sample characteristic on sexual and discussed in Edlund et al. (2006), Harris’s (2002) compar-
emotional jealousy. Two hypothetical forced-choice scenario ison of hypothetical scenarios and actual experience may
Sex Roles (2011) 65:854–866 857

have been confounded by a wording change from what traditional gender difference for forced-choice scenarios,
“distressed or upset” participants more to what participants but the traditional gender difference would not be replicated
focused on more. Berman and Frazier (2005) used forced- when examining adult populations and those who have
choice questions with an undergraduate sample, finding that experienced infidelity, particularly when using continuous-
both genders who had experienced actual infidelity reported scale scenarios instead of forced-choice. The present study
a slightly greater focus on the sexual aspects of imagined addressed a gap in the literature by examining larger
infidelity; however, due to the use of a more limited samples of both undergraduate and non-college age adult
definition of infidelity experience, the sample size for those participants, with and without experience with actual
who had experienced infidelity was only 64. Finally, an infidelity, who responded to both continuous-scale and
unpublished dissertation examined sexual and emotional forced-choice hypothetical measures of distress over emo-
distress in a sample of 68 undergraduates who reported that tional and sexual jealousy.
they had experienced infidelity (broadly defined, either Based on the existing literature, the following hypothe-
emotional or sexual) during the past week (Kimeldorf ses were proposed regarding the intersection of gender,
2009). No gender differences were found for how distressed experience with infidelity, and age on emotional and sexual
individuals were by imagined sexual or emotional infidelity. jealousy in response to infidelity. We hypothesized that
In contrast with these four studies, Edlund et al. (2006) among individuals who had not experienced infidelity: (1)
found the expected gender differences in response to actual Using a hypothetical forced-choice scenario, male and
infidelity experiences using both continuous-scale and female undergraduate and adult responses would replicate
forced-choice hypothetical measures of jealousy. Two the expected gender difference (i.e., males would report that
studies, one with an undergraduate sample of 110 partic- they would be more upset by sexual infidelity and women
ipants and one with an adult sample of 69 mixed students would report that they would be more upset by emotional
and non-students (average age: 26), found that men infidelity); and (2) Using continuous-scale measures, both
reported that they would be more upset by hypothetical undergraduate and adult females would be more upset by
sexual infidelity than emotional infidelity, while women hypothetical emotional infidelity than undergraduate and
displayed the opposite pattern. In addition, Edlund et al. adult males would be.
(2006) examined the effect of age on gender differences for We also hypothesized that among individuals who had
participants who had experienced infidelity and reported no experienced infidelity: (3) All groups (college-age and adult,
age attenuation. As Edlund et al. (2006) used methods and males and females) would report more distress in response to
questions very similar to those of Harris (2002, 2003a), it is imagined sexual infidelity than emotional infidelity on forced-
unclear why these studies produced opposite outcomes. The choice scenarios; (4) All groups (college-age and adult, males
present study built on the questions raised here by and females) would report more distress in response to
comparing a notably larger undergraduate sample with an imagined sexual infidelity than emotional infidelity on
older, non-student adult sample within the same study. continuous-scale scenarios; (5) For both adults and under-
graduates, males would report higher levels of focus on
Summary and Study Goals imagined sexual infidelity than would females; and (6) Male
and female undergraduate and adult responses to the forced-
While a large number of hypothetical, forced-choice studies choice question of whether the sexual or emotional aspect of
conducted with undergraduate U.S. samples have found actual infidelity distressed/upset them more would replicate
that men reported that they would be more distressed by the expected gender difference.
hypothetical sexual infidelity and women reported that they
would be more distressed by hypothetical emotional
infidelity (e.g. Buss et al. 1992; Hupka and Bank 1996; Method
Levy and Kelly 2010), very few studies have examined
emotional and sexual jealousy responses within adult Participants
samples or individuals who have experienced infidelity.
Of these studies, the majority found no gender differences, The participants in this study included 294 heterosexual
particularly when using continuous-scale measures, calling English-speaking undergraduate students at a four-year
into question whether or not hypothetical forced-choice mid-Atlantic state university (age: M=18.48, SD=1.23;
scenarios are valid measures of reactions to actual infidelity range: 17–22 years) and 325 heterosexual non-college
(Berman and Frazier 2005; Harris 2002, 2003a; Kimeldorf adults in the same large urban area (age: M=38.12, SD=
2009). One possibility that has not yet been examined in the 12.60; range: 23–76 years). The final sample was com-
literature is that a lack of experience with infidelity and/or prised of 394 women and 225 men (See Table 1 for
college-age sampling would drive the replication of the descriptive statistics).
858 Sex Roles (2011) 65:854–866

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for study variables by sample and gender

Variables Total N M Range SD Sex distress df Test statistic p

Age (3, 616) F=238.03 .000***


College Males 91 18.63 17–22 1.27
College Females 203 18.35 17–22 1.11
Adult Males 134 39.66 22–76 13.41
Adult Females 191 36.55 23–64 11.82
Had Relationship Experience (3, 616) χ2 =24.89 .000***
College Males 91 87
College Females 203 92
Adult Males 134 100
Adult Females 191 97
Experience with Infidelity (3, 616) χ2 =28.56 .000***
College Males 91 39
College Females 203 39
Adult Males 134 55
Adult Females 191 63

Sex distress = Percent of forced-choice respondent more upset by sexual infidelity


***p<.001, **p<.01, *p<.05

Procedure Measures

The survey was anonymous and took approximately Demographic Information


10 min to complete. Half of the participants were
randomly given questionnaires including the forced- Participants provided minimal demographic information,
choice scenarios, and half were given questionnaires reporting only age, biological gender, and sexual orienta-
including the continuous-scale scenarios. The college-age tion. Participants were also asked to indicate if they had
sample was a convenience sample collected by giving ever been involved in a romantic relationship.
questionnaire packets to students in introductory psy-
chology classes at a mid-Atlantic university; these Romantic Jealousy: Forced-Choice Scenarios
questionnaires were completed either in class or on
participants’ own time and returned anonymously to the Half of the participants in each sample (undergraduate and
researcher or to a sealed box in the psychology adult) received Buss et al.’s (1992) original two item Buss
department. The comparison convenience sample of Infidelity Questionnaire. Using the same wording as Buss
non-college age adults was recruited by handing out et al. (1992), the participant was asked to think of a serious
1,000 surveys (half including only forced-choice scenar- committed romantic relationship that he or she has had in
ios and half including only continuous-scale scenarios) the past, currently had, or would like to have. The
between 7:00 and 8:30 am at two different subway participant was then asked to imagine that this person has
stations in a major mid-Atlantic urban area, within 50 become interested in someone else and answer the question,
miles of the university where surveys were collected. “what would distress or upset you more?” The first item
Nearly 400 participants mailed questionnaire packets allowed one of two responses: (A) imagining your partner
back to the first author in self-addressed stamped forming a deep emotional attachment to that person, or (B)
envelopes. Incomplete surveys and participants who imagining your partner enjoying passionate sexual inter-
identified themselves as gay, lesbian or bisexual were course with that other person (Buss et al. 1992, p. 252). In
removed, resulting in a final sample of 294 college-age the second item, the participant was presented with the
participants and 325 adult participants. Of the 294 same scenario and asked to select if: (A) imagining your
college-age participants, 203 were female and 91 were partner trying different sexual positions with that other
male. Of the 325 adult participants, 191 were female and person, or (B) imagining your partner falling in love with
134 were male. that other person, would be more upsetting.
Sex Roles (2011) 65:854–866 859

Romantic Jealousy: Continuous-Scale Scenarios (A) I focused more on the emotional aspects of my partner’s
infidelity, or (B) I focused more on the sexual aspects of my
Half of the participants in each sample (undergraduate and partner’s infidelity. In addition, participants were asked,
adult) responded to the same two questions as above, but “Which aspect of your partner’s infidelity distressed or upset
instead of a forced-choice scenario, participants responded you more?” and responded either: (A) I was more distressed
using a 7-point likert-type scale indicating their degree of or upset by the emotional aspects of my partner’s infidelity, or
upset (1 = not at all upset to 7 = very upset) if their partner (B) I was more distressed or upset by the sexual aspects of my
told them of this infidelity. As described above, the first partner’s infidelity. As with actual experience of infidelity,
item asked about their partner enjoying passionate sexual analysis of degree of upset versus focus was restricted to 109
intercourse and trying different sexual positions with that males (36 college-age males and 73 adult males) and 199
person and the second item asked about forming a deep females (79 college-age females and 120 adult females).
emotional attachment, or falling in love with that person. All N′s are presented in Table 4.

Experience of Infidelity
Results
A question was included from Harris (2002) to find out if
participants had ever experienced infidelity. Using the same Demographic information indicated that males and females
wording as Harris (2002), participants were asked, “Have you evidenced similar age ranges and means for undergraduate
ever had any experiences in which someone you were males and females as well as for adult males and females,
romantically involved with “cheated” on you?” This question with a significant difference between the two groups (see
was included on both the continuous-scale and forced-choice Table 1). Age was normally distributed for both sample
surveys, after the initial set of forced-choice or continuous- groups; both skew and kurtosis fell within acceptable
scale hypothetical scenarios. Consequently, all forced-choice bounds (+/− 1.0). Differences existed in relationship
and continuous-scale questions were administered in a experience such that more adults than undergraduates
hypothetical format, but in analysis, continuous-scale and reported having been involved in a romantic relationship.
forced-choice responses were grouped according to whether Significant differences were also present for experience of
or not each participant reported actual experience with actual infidelity; adult males and females reported higher
infidelity. Of the final sample, 60% of adults (55% of male levels of experienced infidelity than college-age males and
adults and 63% of female adults) and 39% of undergraduates females, with adult females reporting the highest rate of
(39% of both undergraduate males and females) reported that experience with infidelity at 63% (See Table 1 for
they had experienced a partner’s infidelity (See Table 1). Thus, descriptive statistics on all demographic variables).
analysis of participants who reported previous experience of
infidelity was restricted to 109 males (36 college-age males No Experience of Infidelity: Responses to Hypothetical
and 73 adult males) and 199 females (79 college-age females Infidelity Scenarios
and 120 adult females). These numbers were further
subdivided by type of survey received, forced-choice or Forced-Choice Hypothetical Scenarios
continuous-scale. For example, of the 73 adult males who
reported previous experience of infidelity, 33 completed the Hypothesis 1 predicted that male and female undergraduates
forced-choice questions and 40 completed the continuous- and adults who had never experienced infidelity would
scale questions. All N′s are presented in Tables 2 and 3. replicate the expected gender differences in response to the
hypothetical forced-choice scenario. This hypothesis was fully
“Upset” Versus “Focus” Regarding Actual Infidelity supported for undergraduates and partially supported for adults.
Chi-square analyses found that for scenario one, 48% of
A second group of questions suggested by Sagarin (2005) college-age males compared to 38% of adult males, 20% of
and Edlund et al. (2006) were answered only by individuals college-age females, and 13% of adult females reported that the
who reported that they had experienced infidelity. These sexual infidelity would distress them more, χ2(3, N=146)=
questions were designed to see if participants were 11.22, p=.011 (See Table 2). Main effects were also found
responding to the choice between sexual and emotional such that college-age males were more distressed by
infidelity based on a differing understanding of the words sexual infidelity than were college-age females, χ2(1, N=
“upset or distressed” versus which aspect was focused on 86)=5.67, p=.017, and adult males were more distressed
more. Using the same wording as Sagarin (2005) partic- by sexual infidelity than were adult females, χ2(1, N=60)=
ipants were asked, “Which aspect of your partner’s 5.40, p=.020. For scenario two, 38% of college-age males
infidelity did you focus on more?” and responded either: compared to 24% of adult males, 14% of college-age females,
860 Sex Roles (2011) 65:854–866

Table 2 Participants who have not experienced infidelity: responses to hypothetical infidelity scenarios by sample and gender

Variables Total N N Sex distress % Sex distress M SD df Test p

Forced-choice Item 1
College Males 21 10 47.6
College Females 65 13 20.0
Adult Males 30 11 36.7
Adult Females 31 4 12.9
Main Effect for Gender: College (1, 86) χ2 =5.67 .017*
Main Effect for Gender: Adult (1, 61) χ2 =5.40 .020*
Gender*Sample Interaction (3, 147) χ2 =11.22 .011*
Forced-choice Item 2
College Males 21 8 38.1
College Females 65 9 13.8
Adult Males 30 7 23.3
Adult Females 31 3 9.7
Main Effect for Gender: College (1, 85) χ2 =7.34 .007**
Main Effect for Gender: Adult (1, 60) χ2 =1.57 .210
Gender*Sample Interaction (3, 145) χ2 =8.35 .039*
Continuous-scale Sexual Distressa
College Males 34 6.71 .68
College Females 59 6.69 .74
Adult Males 31 6.34 .90
Adult Females 40 6.76 .64
Main Effect for Gender (1, 164) F=2.73 .100
Gender*Sample Interaction (1, 164) F=3.22 .074
Continuous-scale Emotional Distressa
College Males 34 5.94 1.10
College Females 59 6.41 .80
Adult Males 31 6.06 .88
Adult Females 40 6.76 .60
Main Effect for Gender (1, 164) F=12.99 .000***
Gender*Sample Interaction (1, 164) F=.67 .411

N Sex distress = Number of forced-choice respondents more upset by sexual infidelity


a
7-pt scale (1 = not at all upset, 7 = very upset)
***p<.001, **p<.01, *p<.05

and 10% of adult females reported that the sexual infidelity were found for either adults or undergraduates regarding
would distress them more, χ2(3, N=145)=8.35, p=.039. A sexual jealousy, and neither interaction term was significant
main effect was found such that college-age males were more for differences among sample and gender. However, a main
distressed by sexual infidelity than were college-age females, effect for gender was found in which females reported greater
χ2(1, N=85)=7.34, p=.007, but no main effect for gender levels of distress over emotional infidelity than did men F
differences emerged in adults. (1,164)=12.99, p=.000 (see Table 2).

Continuous-Scale Hypothetical Scenarios Experience of Infidelity: Responses to Hypothetical


Infidelity Scenarios
Hypothesis 2 predicted that on continuous-scale measures of
hypothetical jealousy, both undergraduate and adult females Forced-Choice Hypothetical Scenarios
would be more upset by emotional infidelity than males
would be. Results of a multivariate ANOVA suggested that Hypothesis 3 predicted that among individuals who had
this hypothesis was fully supported. No gender differences experienced infidelity, all groups (college-age and adult,
Sex Roles (2011) 65:854–866 861

Table 3 Participants who have experienced infidelity: responses to hypothetical infidelity scenarios by sample and gender

Variables Total N N Sex distress % Sex distress M SD df Test p

Forced-choice Item 1
College Males 20 9 45.0
College Females 40 8 20.0
Adult Males 33 9 27.3
Adult Females 66 8 12.1
Main Effect for Gender: College (1, 60) χ2 =6.37 .012*
Main Effect for Gender: Adult (1, 99) χ2 =3.42 .064
Gender*Sample Interaction (3, 159) χ2 =12.28 .006**
Forced-choice Item 2
College Males 20 9 45.0
College Females 40 3 7.5
Adult Males 33 13 39.4
Adult Females 66 5 7.6
Main Effect for Gender: College (1, 60) χ2 =11.72 .001***
Main Effect for Gender: Adult (1, 90) χ2 =14.97 .000***
Gender*Sample Interaction (3, 159) χ2 =26.88 .000***
Continuous-scale Sexual Distressa
College Males 16 6.88 .50
College Females 39 6.74 .55
Adult Males 40 6.51 .99
Adult Females 54 6.70 .67
Main Effect for Gender (1, 149) F=.31 .860
Gender*Sample Interaction (1,149) F=1.47 .228
Continuous-scale Emotional Distressa
College Males 16 6.31 .87
College Females 39 6.29 .96
Adult Males 40 6.28 1.08
Adult Females 54 6.51 .82
Main Effect for Gender (1, 149) F=.36 .551
Gender*Sample Interaction (1, 149) F=.54 .465

Sex distress = Percent of forced-choice respondent more upset by sexual infidelity


a
7-pt scale (1 = not at all upset, 7 = very upset)
***p<.001, **p<.01, *p<.05

males and females) would report more distress in response reported that the sexual infidelity would distress them more,
to sexual infidelity on forced-choice scenarios. This χ2(3, N=159)=26.88, p=.000. Main effects were also
hypothesis was not supported by chi-square analyses. For found such that college-age males were more distressed
scenario one, 47% of college-age males compared to 27% by sexual infidelity than were college-age females, χ2(1, N=
of adult males, 17% of college-age females, and 12% of 61)=11.72, p=.001, and adult males were more distressed by
adult females reported that the sexual infidelity would sexual infidelity than were adult females, χ2(1, N=98)=
distress them more, χ2(3, N=159)=12.28, p=.006 (See 14.97, p=.000.
Table 3). A main effect was found such that college-age
males reported that they would have more distress in Continuous-Scale Hypothetical Scenarios
response to sexual infidelity compared to college-age
females, χ2(1, N=61)=6.37, p=.012, but no main effect Hypothesis 4 predicted that on continuous-scale measures,
for gender differences emerged in adults. For scenario two, all groups (college-age and adult, males and females) would
45% of college-age males compared to 39% of adult males, report more distress in response to sexual infidelity than in
8% of college-age females, and 18% of adult females response to emotional infidelity. Results of a multivariate
862 Sex Roles (2011) 65:854–866

Table 4 Participants who have experienced infidelity: responses to real infidelity by sample and gender

Variables Total N N Sex distress % Sex distress df Test p

Forced-choice: Focused More


College Males 36 18 50.0
College Females 79 31 39.2
Adult Males 73 31 42.5
Adult Females 120 28 23.3
Main Effect for Gender: College (1, 115) χ2 =1.23 .237
Main Effect for Gender: Adult (1, 193) χ2 =7.56 .006**
Gender*Sample Interaction (3, 308) χ2 =12.35 .006**
Forced-choice: Distressed/Upset More
College Males 36 16 44.4
College Females 79 33 41.8
Adult Males 73 23 31.5
Adult Females 120 28 23.3
Main Effect for Gender: College (1, 115) χ2 =.30 .583
Main Effect for Gender: Adult (1, 193) χ2 =1.51 .219
Gender*Sample Interaction (3, 308) χ2 =9.65 .022*

Sex distress = Percent of forced-choice respondent more upset by sexual infidelity


***p<.001, **p<.01, *p<.05

ANOVA suggested that this hypothesis was fully sup- In response to which aspect of actual infidelity dis-
ported (see Table 3). All sample groups reported greater tressed/upset them more, hypothesis 6 suggested that both
distress in response to sexual infidelity. No gender differ- male and female undergraduate and adult responses to the
ences were found for either adults or undergraduates forced-choice question of whether the sexual or emotional
regarding sexual or emotional jealousy, and neither aspect of actual infidelity distressed/upset them more would
interaction term was significant for differences among replicate the expected gender difference. This hypothesis
sample and gender. was not supported. While an interaction between gender
and age group was identified such that 46% of college-age
Responses to Real Infidelity males compared to 32% of adult males, 41% of college-age
females, and 23% of adult females reported that they were
The next sections focus on reactions to real infidelity as more distressed or upset by the sexual aspect of the
opposed to hypothetical infidelity, which was the focus of experienced infidelity, χ2(3, N=302)=9.65, p=.022, no
all previous analyses, after being questioned about past main effects were found for gender differences either in
infidelity experience. adults or undergraduates (See Table 4).

“Upset More” Versus “Focus More”


Discussion
Hypothesis 5, which stated that both adult and undergrad-
uate males who had experienced infidelity would report The present research makes several important contributions
higher levels of focus on sexual infidelity than would to the burgeoning literature on gender differences in
females, was partially supported. An interaction was found hypothetical jealousy, suggesting ways in which experience
such that 50% of college-age males compared to 44% of of infidelity, age of the respondent, and biological gender
adult males, 39% of college-age females, and 24% of adult may influence participants’ report of romantic jealousy in
females reported that they focused more on the sexual response to hypothetical sexual or emotional infidelity.
aspect of the experienced infidelity, χ2(3, N=302)=12.35, First, the current work replicated Buss et al.’s (1992) results
p=.006 (See Table 4). Adult males reported focusing more very closely, adding to a considerable body of literature
on sexual infidelity than adult women, χ2(1, N=187)=7.56, finding gender differences in sexual and emotional jealousy
p=.006, but no difference was found for college-age males when using hypothetical, forced-choice scenarios; the data
and females. were especially strong in regards to forced-choice scenarios
Sex Roles (2011) 65:854–866 863

with college-age participants. However, the results of the Sagarin et al. 2003), the actual experience of infidelity did
present study obtained by using the alternative method of not appear to activate hypothetical sexual jealousy in adult
continuous-scale measures and by examining both college- men in the present study. This change across time and
age and adult populations do not fit well with Buss et al.’s experience may interact with a separate pattern for women.
(1992) theory suggesting that the parental investment All data indicating a gender difference across hypothetical
model can explain proposed gender differences in human forced-choice and continuous-scale scenarios for partici-
jealousy. pants who had not experienced infidelity suggested that
Results for those participants who reported experience women were more upset by emotional infidelity than men
with infidelity demonstrated little support for the traditional were. However, for participants in the current study who
evolutionary model, as there were no gender differences in had experienced infidelity, no gender difference emerged
which aspect of hypothetical infidelity was more distress- for distress over hypothetical emotional infidelity when
ing, and no gender difference at the college level in terms of continuous-scale data were examined. Perhaps for women,
which aspect of infidelity received the greatest focus. These the idea of emotional infidelity may be more upsetting than
findings, extrapolated from both undergraduates and adults hypothetical sexual infidelity before they have actually
and accounting for the impact of actual, primed memory of experienced infidelity, but once that experience occurs, they
experience of infidelity on hypothetical jealousy scenarios, are equally as distressed as men by both scenarios.
raise important questions about the validity of hypothetical Further, based on differing patterns of results among
questions about jealousy as a proxy for real reactions to college-age and older adult samples, these results support
actual infidelity. Additional inconsistencies emerged when the assertion that the overwhelming use of college-age
examining the breadth of forced-choice and continuous- student samples may have skewed the current research
scale responses across populations that have and have not literature toward demonstrating more of a gender difference
had experience with infidelity. For participants who had no than exists in the general population. Some might argue that
experience with infidelity, a strong interaction effect the college-age or adolescent group is the best indicator of
emerged for forced-choice questions, along with consistent the true nature of men and women in regards to jealousy
gender differences for college students and inconsistent because they are in their prime child-bearing years; women
gender differences for adults. A gender difference also in particular are most fertile during what are now the high
emerged for continuous-scale questions, but only as a main school and college years. However, two points bear notice
effect for hypothetical emotional infidelity. In contrast, regarding college-age groups. First, women in modern
when participants who reported actual experience with western industrialized societies experience first birth nearly
infidelity responded to these same hypothetical measures, 10 years later than women in preindustrial environments
continuous-scale hypothetical questions revealed no gender and possibly in ancestral environments as well (Mathews
differences at all. For forced-choice questions among and Hamilton 2002). Paired with the “hook-up” culture of
participants who reported experience of infidelity, the college campuses and the phenomenon of extended
expected gender difference was replicated among college- adolescence, cultural norms may be a factor in that the
age males and females, but among adult males and females, combination of delayed adulthood and the salience of sex
there was no gender difference found in one of the two for men and emotion for women may contribute to an age-
forced-choice hypothetical scenarios. based gender difference in Western countries (Green and
These results suggest that the lack of a consistent, Sabini 2006). The frequency of short-term relationships and
replicable gender difference across the lifespan may be relative inexperience with infidelity may lead to men
explained by two related factors: age and actual experience expressing more distress over sexual infidelity than do
with infidelity. Across scenarios, younger males were the women in college-age samples, but in the larger framework,
group that reported the least experience with relationships it is evident that both men and women seek both intimacy
and infidelity, while simultaneously demonstrating the most and sexual relations (Garcia and Reiber 2008). It would be
consistent pattern of greater distress over hypothetical theoretically valuable to replicate this study in developing
sexual infidelity. In contrast, older males reported more countries where 18-year-olds function as adult, procreating
actual experience with relationships and infidelity. These members of the community and are exposed to different
results reflect the conclusions of two previous meta- cultural norms.
analyses, providing support for the suggestion that younger
individuals predict that they will be more upset by Limitations and Suggestions for Future Study
hypothetical sexual infidelity (Harris 2003b; Hofhansl et
al. 2004). Thus, while previous research has suggested that It should be acknowledged that there are limits to the
relationship experience activates sexual jealousy in men generalizability of this study. The sample was not com-
(Becker et al. 2004; Buss et al. 1992; Murphy et al. 2006; pletely random, as the adult sample was recruited during
864 Sex Roles (2011) 65:854–866

morning rush hour in a business district of a major utilized a number of different methods as well as a latent
metropolitan area. As a result, the men and women in the variable approach to suggest that the ceiling effect on this
adult population who responded to the survey were likely to scale does not obscure any reliable differences between
be in a higher income bracket and steadily employed, which men and women.
may have unanticipated effects on the data. However, at the
same time, this study extended the sample beyond the Conclusion
typical undergraduate students and created a direct com-
parison to an adult sample. In addition, the hypothetical, In summary, the present research offers a small but notable
forced-choice results are in accordance with a number of contribution to our understanding of male and female
studies from countries including Australia (Ward and jealousy and the relevance of sample characteristics such
Voracek 2004, college-age), Spain (Fernandez et al. 2007, as age and actual experience of infidelity in this context.
college-age), Brazil (de Souza et al. 2006), and Germany The parental investment model suggests that an innate
(Penke and Asendorpf 2008). The limitations of the forced- biological mechanism has led to a gender difference
choice methodology may also be generalized to other whereby men feel greater distress over sexual infidelity
countries, as studies in Germany, the Netherlands, China, and women feel greater distress over emotional infidelity.
Japan and Austria found that between 60% and 82% of men The present study can be seen as additional evidence that
chose emotional infidelity as most upsetting (Buss et al. the standard hypothetical forced-choice scenario consistently
1999; Buunk et al. 1996; Geary et al. 2001; Voracek 2001). supports the traditional evolutionary model. Yet, the
Second, the adult sample was limited to individuals results produced from data driven by actual experience
motivated enough to complete the survey and return it in of infidelity, especially from continuous-scale data and
the mail; while a return rate of 40% is fairly high, particularly in regard to sexual infidelity, are inconsistent
participants who did respond may differ in important ways with this type of an evolutionary model. If this gender
from those participants who did not respond. For example, difference could be attributed to an innate biological
it is possible that a bias exists in using data only from mechanism, research should show a clear gender differ-
highly motivated individuals with a certain degree of ence in a majority of males and females, regardless of
literacy. This study also did not collect data on marital experience with infidelity or age; such has not been the
status, though it is quite likely that reactions to sexual or case. Consequently, in conjunction with Levy and Kelly
emotional infidelity could be different in the context of a (2010), a case can be made that sexual jealousy may
marital relationship. Future research should collect such covary with biological gender but is not specifically or
data so as to determine whether married individuals are exclusively driven by it, possibly for reasons at least
more upset by infidelity in general than non-married partially attributable to age and experience with infidelity.
individuals and if one aspect of infidelity is more upsetting The present study adds to evidence for a gender difference
than the other. One additional concern with the set-up of the in emotional jealousy for individuals who have not
questionnaire is that participants completed the hypothetical experienced infidelity (Becker et al. 2004; Penke and
scenarios, responded to a question regarding their experi- Asendorpf 2008). Further, results particularly support
ence of infidelity, then answered two additional questions previous research indicating that infidelity experience
about what was more distressing about the infidelity and may alter or erase gender differences in sexual and emotional
what aspect they focused on more. In response to this jealousy (Berman and Frazier 2005; Harris 2002, 2003a).
question order, some participants might have changed one As emotional and sexual jealousy demonstrate different
set of answers to correspond with the other. Future research patterns across individual factors such as age, relationship
should focus on first priming individuals with a memory of experience, and experience with infidelity, future research
infidelity and then asking questions about degree of distress should examine these two perspectives together, while
over different aspects of that actual infidelity. continuing to build on the literature differentiating between
Another limitation may lie in the design of the hypothetical and real infidelity scenarios. It is likely that
continuous-scale data. Similar to previous studies, the vast additional social and ecological factors play a role in
majority of respondents scored degree of distress over both reactions to sexual and emotional infidelity; perhaps
emotional and sexual infidelity as a six or a seven on a 7- parental experience of infidelity, parental divorce, current
point scale, which may suggest that respondents cannot relationship status, or an individual’s view of the opposite
differentiate effectively between their degree of upset over gender might help explain aspects of this complex
emotional and sexual infidelity. Edlund et al. (2006) suggest construct. For example, recent research has found that the
that failures to find gender differences with continuous relationship between sexual and emotional jealousy and
rating scales may be attributable to a ceiling effect in the biological gender is moderated by attachment style (Levy
data. However, DeSteno et al. (2002) have previously and Kelly 2010), chronic levels of jealousy (Miller and
Sex Roles (2011) 65:854–866 865

Maner 2009), and education (Penke and Asendorpf 2008). Eisenman, R., & Dantzker, M. L. (2006). Gender and ethnic differ-
ences in sexual attitudes at a Hispanic-serving university. The
The results of the present study suggest that an examination
Journal of General Psychology, 133, 153–162. doi:10.3200/
of how and why jealousy changes across the lifespan may GENP.133.2.153-162.
add to our understanding of the factors that determine Fernandez, A. M., Vera-Villarroel, P., Sierra, J. C., & Zubeidat, I.
experience of romantic jealousy. (2007). Distress in response to emotional and sexual infidelity:
Evidence of evolved gender differences in Spanish students. The
Journal of Psychology, 14, 17–24. doi:10.3200/JRLP.141.1.17-24.
Garcia, J. R., & Reiber, C. (2008). Hook-up behavior: A biopsy-
References chosocial perspective. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and
Cultural Psychology, 2, 192–208. Retrieved from http://www.
jsecjournal.com/articles/volume2/issue4/NEEPSgarciareiber.pdf.
Becker, D. V., Sagarin, B. J., Guadagno, R. E., Millevoi, A., & Geary, D. C., DeSoto, M. C., Hoard, M. K., Sheldon, M. S., &
Nicastle, L. D. (2004). When the sexes need not differ: Cooper, M. L. (2001). Estrogens and relationship jealousy.
Emotional responses to the sexual and emotional aspects of Human Nature, 12, 299–320. doi:10.1007/s12110-001-1001-2.
infidelity. Personal Relationships, 11, 529–538. doi:10.1111/ Green, M. C., & Sabini, J. (2006). Gender, socioeconomic status, age
j.1475-6811.2004.00096.x. and jealousy: Emotional responses to infidelity in a national
Berman, M. I., & Frazier, P. A. (2005). Relationship power and sample. Emotion, 6, 330–334. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.2.330.
betrayal experience as predictors of reactions to infidelity. Grice, J. W., & Seely, E. (2000). The evolution of sex differences in
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1617–1627. jealousy: Failure to replicate previous results. Journal of
doi:10.1177/0146167205277209. Research in Personality, 34, 348–356. doi:10.1006/
Brase, G. L., Caprar, D. V., & Voracek, M. (2004). Sex differences in jrpe.2000.2284.
responses to relationship threats in England and Romania. Harris, C. R. (2000). Psychophysiological responses to imagined
Journal of Personal and Social Relationships, 21, 763–764. infidelity: The specific innate modular view of jealousy recon-
doi:10.1177/0265407504047836. sidered. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 1082–
Buss, D. M. (2000). The dangerous passion: Why jealousy is as 1091. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.6.1082.
necessary as love and sex. New York: Free. Harris, C. R. (2002). Sexual and romantic jealousy in heterosexual
Buss, D. M., Larsen, R. J., Westen, D., & Semmelroth, J. (1992). Sex and homosexual adults. Psychological Science, 13, 7–12.
differences in jealousy: Evolution, physiology, and psychology. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00402.
Psychological Science, 3, 251–255. doi:10.1111/j.1467- Harris, C. R. (2003a). Factors associated with jealousy over real and
9280.1992.tb00038.x. imagined infidelity: An examination of the social-cognitive and
Buss, D. M., Larsen, R. J., & Westen, D. (1996). Sex differences in evolutionary psychology perspectives. Psychology of Women
jealousy: Not gone, not forgotten, and not explained by Quarterly, 27, 319–329. doi:10.1111/1471-6402.00112.
alternative hypotheses. Psychological Science, 7, 373–375. Harris, C. R. (2003b). A review of sex differences in sexual
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00392.x. jealousy, including self-report data, psychophysiological
Buss, D. M., Shackelford, T. K., Kirkpatrick, L. A., Choe, J. C., Lim, responses, interpersonal violence, and morbid jealousy. Per-
H. K., Hasegawa, M., Hasegawa, T., & Bennett, K. (1999). sonality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 102–128.
Jealousy and the nature of beliefs about infidelity: Tests of doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0702_102-128.
competing hypotheses about sex differences in the United States, Harris, C. R., & Christenfeld, N. (1996). Gender, jealousy, and reason.
Korea, and Japan. Personal Relationships, 6, 121–150. Psychological Science, 7, 364–366. doi:10.1111/j.1467-
doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.1999.tb00215.x. 9280.1996.tb00390.x.
Buunk, B., Angleitner, A., Oubaid, V., & Buss, D. M. (1996). Sex Hofhansl, A., Voracek, M., & Vitouch, O. (2004). Sex differences in
differences in jealousy in evolutionary and cultural perspective: jealousy: A meta-analytical reconsideration. Paper presented at
Tests from the Netherlands, Germany, and the United States. the 16th Annual Meeting of the Human Behavior and Evolution
Psychological Science, 7, 359–363. doi:10.1111/j.1467- Society, Berlin, Germany.
9280.1996.tb00389.x. Hupka, R. B., & Bank, A. L. (1996). Sex differences in jealousy:
Dantzker, M. L., & Eisenman, R. (2005). Sexual attitudes among Evolution or social construction? Cross-Cultural Research, 30,
Hispanic students at a border university: Gender differences and 24–59. doi:10.1177/106939719603000102.
failure to support Buss’s jealousy theory. Journal of Evolutionary Kimeldorf, M. B. (2009). Reactions to infidelity: Individual, gender,
Psychology, 26, 71–80. and situational predictors of relationship outcome and forgive-
de Souza, A. A. L., Verderane, M. P., Taira, J. T., & Otta, E. (2006). ness. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B. Sciences
Emotional and sexual jealousy as a function of sex and sexual and Engineering, 69(7-B), 4427.
orientation in a Brazilian sample. Psychological Reports, 98, Levy, K. N., & Kelly, K. M. (2010). Sex differences in jealousy: A
529–535. doi:10.2466/PR0.98.2.529-535. contribution from attachment theory. Psychological Science, 21,
DeSteno, D. A., & Salovey, P. (1996). Evolutionary origins of sex 166–173. doi:10.1177/0956797609357708.
differences in jealousy? Questioning the “fitness” of the model. Mathes, E. W. (2005). Relationship between short-term sexual
Psychological Science, 7, 367–372. doi:10.1111/j.1467- strategies and sexual jealousy. Psychological Reports, 96, 29–
9280.1996.tb00391.x. 35. doi:10.2466/pr0.96.1.29-35.
DeSteno, D. A., Bartlett, M. Y., Braverman, J., & Salovey, P. (2002). Mathews, T. J., & Hamilton, B. E. (2002). Mean age of mother, 1970–
Sex differences in jealousy: Evolutionary mechanism or artifact 2000. National vital statistics reports, 51. Hyattsville: National
of measurement? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Center for Health Statistics. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/
83, 1103–1116. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.5.1103. nchs/data/databriefs/db21.pdf.
Edlund, J. E., Heider, J. D., Scherer, C. R., Farc, M., & Sagarin, B. J. Miller, S. L., & Maner, J. K. (2009). Sex differences in response to
(2006). Sex differences in jealousy in response to actual sexual versus emotional infidelity: The role of individual differ-
infidelity. Evolutionary Psychology, 4, 462–470. Retrieved from ences. Personality and Individual Differences, 46, 287–291.
http://www.epjournal.net/filestore/ep04462470.pdf. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.10.013.
866 Sex Roles (2011) 65:854–866

Murphy, M. M., Vallacher, R. R., Shackelford, T. K., Bjorklund, D. F., Shackelford, T. K., Buss, D. M., & Bennett, K. (2002). Forgiveness or
& Yunger, J. L. (2006). Relationship experience as a predictor of breakup: Sex differences in responses to a partner’s infidelity.
romantic jealousy. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, Cognition and Emotion, 16, 299–307. doi:10.1080/
761–769. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.09.004. 02699930143000202.
Penke, L., & Asendorpf, J. B. (2008). Evidence for conditional sex Sheets, V. L., & Wolfe, M. D. (2001). Sexual jealousy in hetero-
differences in emotional but not in sexual jealousy at the sexuals, lesbians, and gays. Sex Roles, 44, 255–276. doi:10.1023/
automatic level of cognitive processing. European Journal of A:1010996631863.
Personality, 22, 3–30. doi:10.1002/per.654. Takahashi, H., Matsuura, M., Yahata, N., Koeda, M., Suhara, T., &
Pietrzak, R. H., Laird, J. D., Stevens, D. A., & Thompson, N. S. Okubo, Y. (2006). Men and women show distinct brain activations
(2002). Sex differences in human jealousy: A coordinated study during imagery of sexual and emotional infidelity. NeuroImage, 32,
of forced-choice, continuous rating-scale, and physiological 1299–1307. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.049.
responses on the same subjects. Evolution and Human Behavior, Trivers, R. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B.
23, 83–94. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00078-2. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the descent of man: 1871–
Sabini, J., & Green, M. C. (2004). Emotional responses to sexual 1971 (pp. 136–179). Chicago: Aldine.
and emotional infidelity: Constants and differences across Voracek, M. (2001). Marital status as a candidate moderator variable
genders, samples, and methods. Personality and Social of male-female differences in sexual jealousy: The need for
Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1375–1388. doi:10.1177/01461672 representative population samples. Psychological Reports, 88,
04264012. 553–566. doi:10.2466/PR0.88.2.553-566.
Sagarin, B. J. (2005). Reconsidering evolved sex differences in jealousy: Wade, T. J., & Fowler, K. (2006). Sex differences in response to sexual
Comment on Harris (2003). Personality and Social Psychology and emotional infidelity: Considerations of rival attractiveness
Review, 9, 62–75. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0901_5. and financial status. Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary
Sagarin, B. J., & Guadagno, R. E. (2004). Sex differences in the Psychology, 4, 37–50. doi:10.1556/JCEP.4.2006.1.3.
context of extreme jealousy. Personal Relationships, 11, 319– Ward, J., & Voracek, M. (2004). Evolutionary and social cognitive
328. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.2004.00085.x. explanations of sex differences in romantic jealousy. Australian
Sagarin, B. J., Becker, D. V., Guadagno, R. E., Nicastle, L. D., & Journal of Psychology, 56, 165–171. doi:10.1080/
Millevoi, A. (2003). Sex differences (and similarities) in 00049530412331283381.
jealousy: The moderating influence of infidelity experience and Wiederman, M. W., & Allgeier, E. R. (1993). Gender differences in
sexual orientation of the infidelity. Evolution and Human sexual jealousy: Adaptionist or social learning explanation?
Behavior, 24, 17–23. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00106-X. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 115–140. doi:10.1016/0162-
Schutzwohl, A. (2007). Decision strategies in continuous ratings of 3095(93)90011-6.
jealousy feelings elicited by sexual and emotional infidelity. Wiederman, M. W., & Kendall, E. (1999). Evolution, sex, and jealousy:
Evolutionary Psychology, 5, 815–828. Retrieved from http:// Investigation with a sample from Sweden. Evolution and Human
www.epjournal.net/filestore/ep05815828.pdf. Behavior, 20, 121–128. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00046-4.

You might also like