You are on page 1of 28

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/260674678

Attitudes and social cognition as social psychological siblings

Chapter · January 2013

CITATIONS READS

2 570

2 authors:

Duane T. Wegener Richard E Petty


The Ohio State University The Ohio State University
116 PUBLICATIONS   14,579 CITATIONS    402 PUBLICATIONS   60,562 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Predicting the Effectiveness of Different Strategies of Advertising Variation: A Test of the Repetition-Variation Hypothesis View project

Bias Correction View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Duane T. Wegener on 21 December 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


CH A PT E R

Attitudes and Social Cognition as


7 Social Psychological Siblings

Duane T. Wegener and Richard E. Petty

Abstract

Attitudes and social cognition have many common conceptual roots. This chapter reviews the inter-
woven history of the attitudes and social cognition areas. It discusses the separation of the approaches
in the early days of the social cognition movement when social cognition borrowed heavily from mod-
els of cold cognition in cognitive psychology. The chapter compares the development of research on
attitudes, attitude change, and persuasion with that in social cognition during the past 30 to 40 years.
In that time, attitudes and social cognition researchers have created prominent dual and multiple pro-
cess theories, and common themes have resonated with attitudes and social cognition researchers.
Finally, the chapter discusses the broadening of the social cognition area into a general approach to
social psychological research. There are a number of ways in which the maturation of the social cogni-
tion perspective has brought it closer to the attitudinal roots that were at least partially rejected in
the early days of social cognition.
Key Words: attitudes, attitude change, persuasion, social cognition

Since the late 1970s, Attitudes and Social However, Greenwald was instrumental in bringing
Cognition have shared a section of the Journal of cognitive approaches to social psychology in his early
Personality and Social Psychology. There are good rea- work on the self (Greenwald, 1980) and subliminal
sons to house these research areas under the same persuasion (Greenwald, Spangenberg, Pratkanis, &
roof. At the same time, like siblings in the same Eskenazi, 1991), and as we note shortly, especially
house, there have also been certain tensions between in his more recent work on implicit measures of atti-
the areas, perhaps especially as the burgeoning area tudes (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998).
of social cognition grew and sought conceptual and Ostrom conducted attitudes research into the
methodological independence in the late 1970s and 1970s, edited attitudes books in the Ohio State series
early 1980s. in the late 1960s and early 1980s (i.e., Greenwald,
Both of the current authors took courses (one of Brock, & Ostrom, 1968; Petty, Ostrom, & Brock,
us in “person perception” and the other in “social 1981), and taught attitude measurement into the
cognition”) from one of the founders of contem- mid-1990s. Thus, Tom’s direct involvement in
porary social cognition, Thomas Marshall Ostrom. attitudes research came before the development of
Tom helped to found the social psychology program contemporary dual and multiple process models,
at Ohio State in the mid-1960s with Timothy Brock such as the elaboration likelihood model (ELM;
and Anthony Greenwald (and, soon thereafter, Bibb Petty & Cacioppo, 1986a, 1986b) and the heuris-
Latané). Greenwald was not as exclusively identi- tic/systematic model (HSM; Chaiken, Liberman,
fied with the “social cognition” moniker as Ostrom. & Eagly, 1989). Yet, he had conducted a healthy

118

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 118 4/9/2013 9:55:44 AM


amount of attitudes research, especially dealing with to Thurstone’s declaration that “Attitudes Can Be
issues of attitude structure (Ostrom, 1969; Ostrom Measured” (Thurstone, 1928), the focus of much
& Brock, 1969), measurement (Ostrom, 1973; attitudes research was on assessing attitudes (e.g.,
Ostrom & Upshaw, 1968), and change (Ostrom, Guttman, 1944; Likert, 1932; Thurstone & Chave,
1970; Steele & Ostrom, 1974). Tom also founded 1929). Following World War II, Carl Hovland
the Social Cognition Research Group at Ohio State assembled a group of researchers at Yale University
and was active in developing the international focused on attitude change. Hovland and col-
Person Memory Interest Group (PMIG)—one of leagues relied on the cognitive/learning theories of
the original venues for discussions of social cogni- the time to understand persuasion using a broad set
tion research, and now a popular preconference of assumptions referred to as the Message Learning
held each year before the Society of Experimental Approach (Hovland, Janis, & Kelley, 1953).
Social Psychology meeting. In this chapter, we pres- Throughout the 1950s, 1960s, and early 1970s,
ent an Ostromian view of early social cognition. attitudes work could be characterized as driven by
Like the field more generally, if Tom were still with a variety of individual theories postulating a central
us, he would likely hold different views today about process that would produce attitude effects. These
what does or does not qualify as social cognition. theories, some of which were cognitive, but oth-
We wish he were still with us, but we will try to ers of which were motivational, included classical
construct as accurate a set of recollections as we can conditioning (Staats & Staats, 1958), cognitive dis-
(with apologies to those who knew some of these sonance (Festinger, 1957, 1964), balance (Heider,
details more intimately than we do). 1958), congruity (Osgood & Tannenbaum, 1955),
We begin our chapter by providing a historical social judgment (Sherif, Sherif, & Nebergall 1965),
backdrop for the development of social cognition, cognitive response (Greenwald, 1968), information
including traditional research on attitudes/persua- integration (Anderson, 1971), and expectancy-value
sion and impression formation as well as responses (Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975) approaches. Late in the
to crises in these fields in the 1970s. We then dis- 1960s and into the 1970s, dissonance (Festinger,
cuss the early days of the social cognition movement 1964) and self-perception (Bem, 1967) theories
when cold cognition initially ruled and social cogni- vied with one another as competing explanations
tion borrowed heavily from models in cognitive psy- for effects of behavior on attitudes. Some research-
chology. We continue to compare the development ers lamented the ability of each theory to account
of attitudes and social cognition research during for results predicted by the other and suggested
the past 30 to 40 years. We focus our comparison that empirical attempts to differentiate the theories
on prominent dual and multiple process theories would fail (Greenwald, 1975).
of attitude change and of impression formation. In the same era (i.e., the late 1960s and early
The next part of the chapter identifies a number of 1970s), the accumulated attitudes literature was
themes that have resonated with both attitudes and coming under attack. In particular, research dealing
social cognition researchers, though these common with attitude–behavior relations was criticized by
themes have sometimes been studied under differ- Wicker (1969, 1971) and others as indicating woe-
ent names. Finally, we discuss the maturation and fully low predictive ability for attitudes. Soon there-
broadening of the social cognition area into a gen- after, Sherif (1977) and others bemoaned the lack
eral approach to social psychological research and of generalizable principles in the literature on atti-
theorizing. In so doing, we point out some ways in tude change. Many findings from the Hovland era,
which this maturation of the social cognition per- such as source credibility effects on persuasion, for
spective has brought it closer to some of the attitu- example, occurred in some studies but not in oth-
dinal roots that were at least partially rejected in the ers. In fact, other researchers at the time questioned
early days of social cognition. whether influences on attitudes were real or epiphe-
nomenal (a product of asking people questions in
Backdrop of Traditional Research on the lab), observing that many attitude changes in
Attitudes and Attitude Change the lab were no longer present when research partic-
The study of attitudes and attitude change can be ipants took up their daily lives (Cook & Flay, 1978).
thought of as a series of (partially overlapping) eras Of course, some research on attitudes had identified
in which particular questions or developments took lasting persuasion (e.g., Freedman, 1965; Mann
center stage (see Briñol & Petty, 2012, for a history & Janis, 1968), though many effects were, in fact,
of attitudes research). In the early days, tracing back short-lived. Yet, as of the mid-1970s, no theoretical

Wegener, Pet t y 119

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 119 4/9/2013 9:55:46 AM


framework had been developed to account for (or processes. This research approach paralleled
predict) such variation, either in persuasive effects stimulus–response approaches from the learning
or persistence of the observed changes. In many tradition (that also formed the foundation for early
respects, this time of crisis ushered in the modern attitudes research; Hovland et al., 1953) and was
dual/multiple process era, not only in attitude theo- viewed by early social cognition researchers as pro-
rizing (e.g., Petty & Cacioppo, 1986b) but also, for viding relatively little leverage on the cognitive pro-
some similar reasons, in the development of social cesses at work. Furthermore, because traits or other
cognition. information were typically presented to research
participants who were then directly asked for a
An Impression Formation Context for judgment, concerns arose that responses to such
the Development of Social Cognition questions might not reflect what people do when
Ostrom’s background was thoroughly rooted in they learn about similar information in the real
attitudes research. He was the intellectual grand- world (e.g., by observing behaviors and spontane-
son of Louis Thurstone—through Harry Upshaw, ously inferring traits; Winter & Uleman, 1984; cf.,
Ostrom’s graduate school advisor. Ostrom also had Cook & Flay, 1978). Ostrom (1977) argued that it
direct connections to research on impression forma- would be beneficial if impression formation theories
tion. Indeed, work on context effects in impression began to rely on advances in cognitive psychology
formation had direct connections to psychophysical to make more specific predictions about the mecha-
judgment (and Thurstone’s work on psychophysical nisms underlying change of meaning effects (includ-
judgment laid the foundation for his research on atti- ing more direct study of the content, structure, and
tude measurement; Thurstone, 1928). Many early processing of cognitions about the target person).
social cognition researchers initially identified with To this end, Ostrom suggested supplementing the
research on impression formation, and many still do traditional trait-rating paradigms to include tech-
(see Chapter 2). Similar to the attitudes domain, by niques such as reaction times (Anderson & Hastie,
the mid-1970s, there was growing frustration with 1974), recall (Lingle & Ostrom, 1981), and free
existing theories of impression formation. Like the association (Deese, 1965). Furthermore, presaging
dissonance/self-perception competition in the atti- later theoretical developments, Ostrom (1977) sug-
tudes domain, the general approaches of meaning gested that a more fruitful theoretical stance to the
change (Asch, 1946) and information integration dueling-theories approach of the previous decade
(Anderson, 1966, 1970) had been viewed as com- would be to suggest that each basic type of process
peting theories that provided different explanations (i.e., integration of components and meaning shifts)
for context effects in impression formation. In the contributes to impression formation, but with spe-
meaning change approach, the connotations of spe- cific circumstances determining the relative impor-
cific traits differed depending on the constellation of tance of each type of process.
other traits or information available about the tar-
get person. In contrast, the information integration Responses to Crisis
approach suggested that perceptions of component The attitudes and social cognition areas devel-
traits could be constant, but that different overall oped in remarkable ways following the crises of the
impressions would result from differential weight- 1970s. On the attitude side, the puzzle of attitude–
ing of the component traits in forming the overall behavior relations was addressed on the measure-
impression. Ostrom (1977) suggested that neither ment front (e.g., Fishbein & Ajzen, 1974) and by
the change of meaning approach nor the informa- theoretical approaches that emphasized mediation
tion integration approach was sufficiently specified of attitude effects (by behavioral intentions; Fishbein
to make clear predictions that could be differentiated & Ajzen, 1975) or moderation of attitude effects
from one another. That is, Ostrom (1977) suggested (by features of the attitude, such as direct experi-
that each approach was sufficiently ambiguous that ence, Fazio & Zanna, 1981, knowledge, Davidson
the approaches could be adjusted to account for vir- et al., 1985, and other attitude strength properties;
tually any result after the fact, but that each position see Petty & Krosnick, 1995). The theories of disso-
made precious few a priori predictions (for similar nance and self-perception were successfully distin-
criticisms of the information integration approach guished by focusing on the hypothesized discomfort
to attitude change, see Petty & Cacioppo, 1981). associated with dissonance but absent from self-
Impression formation studies of the 1960s and perception (Fazio, Zanna, & Cooper, 1977; Zanna
1970s presented traits as the input to impression & Cooper, 1974; see Wegener & Carlston, 2005,

120 At titudes and Social Cognition as Social Psychological Siblings

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 120 4/9/2013 9:55:46 AM


for discussion). Moreover, theoretical models were change literature, in part, because information inte-
developed that specified when relatively thought- gration was more of a description than a theory. That
ful versus nonthoughtful processes would influ- is, whereas the information integration equations
ence attitudes. These models used the moderators could potentially identify the differential weights
of motivation and ability to think carefully about associated with the impact of particular information
attitude-relevant information (e.g., Chaiken, 1980; on impressions or attitudes, information integration
Chaiken, Liberman, & Eagly, 1989; Petty, 1977; per se did not predict what these weights should
Petty & Cacioppo, 1979, 1986a, 1986b), and vari- be, leaving many researchers unsatisfied (including
ous processes initially studied in the 1950s, 1960s, Ostrom, 1977). With this search in cognitive psy-
and early 1970s—including dissonance and self-per- chology for tools to study impression formation, the
ception—could be placed at different points along sibling discipline of social cognition was born. (At
the elaboration continuum (Petty & Cacioppo, least that is one version of the story.)
1981; Petty & Wegener, 1998). The multiple process
approach quickly became dominant in the attitudes The Early Days of Social Cognition
field. In addition to organizing past attitude pro- Like a younger sibling attempting to escape the
cesses and persuasion effects, the multiple process shadow of the older sibling, many topics that had
approach postulated that the amount of thought been common constructs in the attitudes domain,
about the attitude object moderated the extent to such as the role of emotions or motivation in evalu-
which the attitude persisted over time and had last- ation, became almost taboo in social cognition.
ing impact (e.g., Petty & Cacioppo, 1986a). Whether this was intentional or simply a conse-
Alongside the criticisms of attitude–behavior quence of the focus on cognitive mechanisms per
and attitude change research, researchers (includ- se is unclear. Our guess is that the vision of mental
ing Ostrom) who had been examining person processes of the day within cognitive psychology was
impressions using Anderson’s information integra- simply a vision of “cold” cognition that eschewed the
tion approach were also becoming disenchanted more messy “hot” cognition constructs (Abelson,
with that approach. Indeed, at one point (if our 1963). If motivation and emotion were discussed
memories for Tom’s stories serve us well), a num- at all in the early days of social cognition, they were
ber of researchers, including Ostrom, made a pact described in cold cognitive terms.
to never again conduct an information integration Lots of interesting and influential work captured
study. Against this backdrop, the modern era of the imagination of the first generation of social cog-
social cognition research commenced. In the new- nition researchers. Much of that work addressed the
style social cognition research, participants often canonical cognitive categories of activation (acces-
encountered descriptions of behaviors rather than sibility), automaticity, and mental representation
traits (also present in the attribution research that (for reviews, see Bargh, 1994; Carlston & Smith,
might be viewed as a bridge between early impres- 1996; Higgins, 1996). Indeed, we recall a student of
sion formation studies and social cognition; see Ostrom’s quipping that, “If you are not dealing with
Chapter 6). In addition, judgmental outcomes were encoding, storage, and retrieval, you are not deal-
often accompanied by measurements of reaction ing with social cognition.” Methodologically, some
times or recall/recognition of traits or behaviors. researchers we knew complained that editors like
These measures were intended to tap into the cog- Ostrom (then editing the Journal of Experimental
nitive processes that linked perception of behaviors Social Psychology) didn’t view work as being social
to trait inferences, judgments, or behaviors toward cognition unless the dependent measure was reac-
the target. Emphasis was placed on the use of mod- tion time or ARC scores (a measure of clustering
els and methods from cognitive psychology as tools in free recall). Indeed, these were two of Ostrom’s
to help researchers understand how social informa- favorite outcome measures, whether or not they
tion (especially information about people) came formed an operational definition of which research
to be processed, remembered, and integrated into qualified as social cognition. At any rate, the early
judgments. days of social cognition were heady days full of
From an attitude researcher’s point of view, it much enthusiasm—enthusiasm that snowballed
is easy to understand why early social cognition into a thriving new approach to social psychology
researchers had become disenchanted with the (see Chapter 2).
information integration approach. This approach Without a doubt, this enthusiasm was also
had not taken a central position within the attitude fueled by small research meetings of social

Wegener, Pet t y 121

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 121 4/9/2013 9:55:46 AM


cognition researchers, perhaps the most prominent Similarities and Dissimilarities in How
being the PMIG meetings prior to the Society of Attitudes and Social Cognition Developed
Experimental Social Psychology meetings in the since the Mid-1970s
fall and the summer meetings in Nags Head, North As noted previously, social cognition research
Carolina (and later, the Duck Conference on Social responded to dissatisfaction with stimulus–response
Cognition). Like the area of social cognition more types of theorizing by orienting themselves toward
generally, the PMIG and Duck meetings have methods and models in cognitive psychology. The
continued to become broader over time. In their intent, of course, was to more directly address
early days, however, these separate meetings (per- the cognitive mechanisms underlying the result-
haps especially the PMIG meeting) contributed to ing impressions or behaviors. Therefore, in some
views of social cognition researchers as somewhat respects, one might think of the social cognition
exclusive and, perhaps, arrogant. Ostrom surely response to the concerns of the day as focusing on
contributed to perceptions of social cognition cognitive mediation. Some social cognition research
research as arrogant and rather imperialistic with also focused directly on the issue of what social per-
his lead chapter in the 1984 Handbook of Social ceivers do naturally (such as spontaneously form
Cognition volume titled, “The Sovereignty of Social trait inferences, Winter & Uleman, 1984; see also
Cognition” (Ostrom, 1984). This was one of the Carlston & Skowronski, 1994). In comparison,
great paradoxes of Tom Ostrom. He was extremely although some research in the attitudes domain
inclusive and friendly, especially in gathering and focused on straight mediation (e.g., theory of rea-
connecting those he viewed as kindred spirits. Yet, soned action, Fishbein & Ajzen, 1975; cognitive
he could also lead the charge to claim the supe- responses, Greenwald, 1968), most of the attitudes
riority of his preferred view of the world (and, in response to the crises of the 1970s focused on iden-
the process, at least to some readers, the inferior- tification of moderators. Over time, these modera-
ity of all other approaches). At times, some have tors coalesced into overall models of attitude change
questioned whether Ostrom was simply trying to (e.g., Chaiken, 1980; Chaiken et al., 1989; Petty,
be provocative with his chapter title or whether he 1977; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986a) and attitude–
even believed what he was saying at the time (or behavior relations (Fazio, 1986, 1990). These mod-
just being extreme to try to make a point). But 10 els proposed factors that determined when various
years later, in his preface to the 1994 Handbook of influences on attitudes would have their greatest
Social Cognition (and, unfortunately, the year of impact, how long resulting attitudes would persist
Tom’s passing), he headed a section of the preface over time (if unchallenged), how easily they could
with “Is Social Cognition Sovereign?” and began be changed (when challenged), and which attitudes
the paragraph with “Of course it is.” would be most likely to influence behaviors. As
It is difficult to say whether any of this “separat- described in more detail shortly, a core idea in these
ist” sentiment in the early days of social cognition models was that sometimes people engage in little
paralleled the common motivations of younger sib- thought prior to making judgments, in which case
lings to demonstrate to their parents, older siblings, simple and quick inferences and cues determine
and the world at large that they are autonomous evaluations, whereas at other times people engage in
and fully functioning individuals. Many areas of much thought, in which case deliberative processes
social psychology might be characterized as having and careful analysis of information determine judg-
less cognitive roots than the attitudes domain, so ments. These dual and multiple process frameworks
this attempt to differentiate social cognition from presaged later development of dual systems models
other areas of inquiry was likely viewed as much of judgment within social cognition (e.g., Deutsch
broader than simply differentiating from research & Strack, 2006; Smith & DeCoster, 2000; see Petty
on attitudes. However, the early choices to focus & Briñol, 2006b).
social cognition on cold cognition (ushered in by To be sure, characterizing 1970s and 1980s social
the computer analog of the brain that came with cognition as emphasizing mediation and attitudes
the development of cognitive psychology more work as emphasizing moderation is a simplifica-
generally) marked a clear departure from attitudes tion. Emphases on cognitive mediators naturally
research. The tradition and ongoing character of implied certain moderators of the proposed cogni-
attitudes research included the study of cognitive tive mechanisms. Likewise, moderators of influ-
processes but certainly also considered emotional ences on attitudes were hypothesized to result from
and motivational processes. moderation of the cognitive processes (mediators) at

122 At titudes and Social Cognition as Social Psychological Siblings

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 122 4/9/2013 9:55:46 AM


work. Thus, although people did not use the term sense, the attitudes literature progressed by embrac-
in those days, each literature included elements of ing the variety of past findings and attempting to
moderated mediation (Muller, Judd, & Yzerbyt, account for them. In contrast, early social cogni-
2005; Wegener & Fabrigar, 2000; see Chaiken & tion research rejected previous means of studying
Maheswaran, 1994, and Petty, Schumann, Richman, the questions of interest (clearing the decks, so to
& Strathman, 1993, for persuasion examples). Still, speak) and started from scratch with a new empha-
the relative focus on mediating cognitive mecha- sis on cognitive process (often defined as specifically
nisms versus moderators to organize existent effects dealing with encoding, representation in memory,
did serve to carve out distinct paths for social cog- or retrieval—in the process also rejecting alternative
nition and attitudes researchers. These paths also “cognitive” measures).
fit with certain distinctive features of how the areas Whether intended or not, these different points
came to define themselves. That is, by emphasizing of departure have had important consequences for
evaluation as the core defining feature of what con- the two literatures ever since. For example, in the
stituted attitudes research, no particular emphasis on 35 years since the development of the ELM (Petty,
any one type of process became emblematic of an 1977) and HSM (Chaiken, 1978), the various over-
“attitudinal” approach. In a sense, all mechanisms arching theories in the attitude change domain have
were fair game. In contrast, although social cogni- provided central shared structures that have guided
tion has significantly broadened since its inception much of the research. When the research has not
(see Wegener & Carlston, 2005), its early empha- tested particular tenets of those core theories, the
sis on cold cognitive processes did make the type of accumulated literature surrounding those theo-
process emblematic of a “social cognition” approach ries has provided a rich context for identifying the
(regardless of whether one was studying evaluative or conditions in which particular variables are likely
nonevaluative outcome judgments or behaviors). to have certain effects. Because so much research
Thus, one way in which the attitudes and social has either directly concerned these core theories or
cognition paths diverged in the late 1970s and early used them as a salient backdrop for the research, we
1980s was in terms of the breadth of potential medi- believe that this could lead to the false impression
ators and independent variables addressed. As we that research is not developing as quickly or chang-
have already noted, early social cognition research- ing as much in the attitudes domain as in other
ers focused intently on cognitive mediators, often areas (like social cognition) that have not developed
to the exclusion of motivational or emotional con- the same kinds of core, overarching theories.
structs. In traditional attitudes work, many different Although many specific cognitive principles
mechanisms and independent variables were con- have been identified over the years, a large propor-
sidered. A partial list of processes includes learning tion of the social cognition research has seemed to
(e.g., conditioning, Staats & Staats, 1958; effectance fall outside of broad overarching theories. This is
motivation, Byrne & Clore, 1967) and cognitive not to suggest that broad social cognition theories
consistency processes (e.g., dissonance, Festinger, have been absent. Perhaps most salient in this regard
1957; congruity, Osgood & Tannenbaum, 1955; are overarching theories of impression formation
tripartite attitude structure, Rosenberg & Hovland, (e.g., Brewer, 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990) and
1960) as well as other motivational (e.g., attitude some broad but straightforward notions, such as
functions, Katz, 1960, Smith, Bruner, & White, schemas (Hastie, 1981) and associative networks
1956; ego-involvement, Sherif & Hovland, 1961; (Anderson, 1980; Rumelhart, Lindsay, & Norman,
reactance, Brehm, 1966) and perceptual (e.g., adap- 1972), developed early on and were used to explain
tation level, Helson, 1964; assimilation-contrast, a wide variety of phenomena. Yet, in many areas
Sherif et al., 1965; variable perspective, Ostrom & of social cognition, significant work proceeded to
Upshaw, 1968; for reviews, see Briñol & Petty, 2012, address questions lying somewhat outside the con-
Ostrom, 1968) processes. Attitude researchers did fines of the overarching theories (such as spontane-
not discard the variables or mechanisms involved ous trait transference in the impression formation
in these domains or recast them in purely cognitive domain; Skowronski, Carlston, Mae, & Crawford,
terms. Rather, attitude researchers sought to inte- 1998). Perhaps, in part, because of the weaker
grate those processes into overarching theories that role that overarching theories played in (especially
provided a context for when each of the proposed early) social cognition research, interested readers
mechanisms might have the strongest effects (see may have perceived each new piece of evidence as
Petty & Cacioppo, 1986a, 1986b). Therefore, in a another separate log that brightened the blaze of

Wegener, Pet t y 123

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 123 4/9/2013 9:55:46 AM


social cognition. In contrast, research building on To test effects of elaboration (or other attitude-
existing overarching theories can be viewed as less strength-related variables) on persistence, resistance,
groundbreaking. It is an interesting philosophy of or behavior prediction, researchers have to equate
science question to ask how the presence of broad the extremity of attitudes across the crucial high
organizing theories influences perceptions of prog- versus low thinking conditions. By doing this, atti-
ress in developing areas of science. tude extremity is not confounded with the level of
elaboration (or other dimension of attitude strength
Dual and Multiple Process Theories of that is being tested). Therefore, much work com-
Attitude Formation and Change paring relatively nonthoughtful and thoughtful pro-
Broad theories of attitude formation and change cesses in attitude change has identified settings in
were based on the notion that people are not always which more and less elaborative processes result in
willing and able to think carefully about attitude- the same judgmental impact (i.e., in the same atti-
relevant information (Chaiken, 1980; Chaiken tudinal judgments). For example, Petty et al. (1993)
et al., 1989; Petty & Cacioppo, 1979, 1986a). tested the effects of happy versus neutral mood on
Therefore, these theories were oriented around attitudes when processing of information was rela-
the level of motivation and ability people pos- tively high or low. Happy mood led to more favor-
sessed in a given evaluative setting as determinants able attitudes than neutral mood and did so to the
of the level of elaboration or systematic processing same degree across high and low levels of processing
in which people engage. These theories addressed (equating the level of extremity across conditions).
the varied effects of source, message, recipient, and More importantly, when processing was high, the
context/channel factors (Lasswell, 1948) by sug- valence of generated thoughts about the persua-
gesting that low levels of information processing sive appeal was significantly affected by mood,
allowed peripheral cues (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986b; and the difference in the favorability of thoughts
or aspects of a persuasive appeal associated with across conditions mediated the impact of mood
available cognitive heuristics, Chaiken, 1987) to on attitudes (i.e., mood-biased processing; see also
have the dominant effect on resulting attitudes. In DeSteno, Petty, Rucker, Wegener, & Braverman,
contrast, higher levels of processing led to greater 2004; Wegener, Petty, & Klein, 1994). However,
effects of the effortfully assessed merits of the per- at low levels of processing, mood had no effect on
suasive appeal. the thoughts produced (i.e., mood did not bias
Under the rubric of “multiple roles,” the ELM thoughts, but rather was used as a cue or heuristic).
also proposed that a given variable can influence atti- Conceptually similar effects—showing equal
tudes for different reasons at different levels of elabo- effects on attitudes across levels of processing—have
ration. That is, a variable can act as a cue and have a been demonstrated for variables such as the expertise
relatively direct effect on attitudes when motivation of a message source (Chaiken & Maheswaran, 1994)
or ability to think is low (i.e., when elaboration like- and also on nonevaluative dependent measures, such
lihood is low). However, when people are motivated as stereotypes influencing trait ratings (Wegener,
and able to elaborate, the same variable can influ- Clark, & Petty, 2006) or numerical anchors influ-
ence attitudes through more elaborative processes, encing numerical estimates (Blankenship, Wegener,
such as serving as an argument, biasing processing of Petty, Detweiler-Bedell, & Macy, 2008). Because the
available information, or validating thoughts about attitudes or nonevaluative judgments in these stud-
the attitude object. Finally, the same variable may ies have been equally extreme across levels of elabo-
often influence the extent of elaboration if neither ration, the studies have appropriately set the stage
motivation nor ability is constrained to be high or for examination of elaboration effects on the conse-
low (for reviews see Petty & Cacioppo, 1986a; Petty quences of those judgments (such as persistence over
& Briñol, 2012; Petty & Wegener, 1999). The ELM time, resistance to change, and the like). Consistent
addressed not only 1970s criticisms concerning with the ELM notion that elaboration will lead to
unexplained variation in persuasion effects (Sherif, enhanced impact of the judgments over time, more
1977) but also concerns that attitude effects were thoughtful stereotyping and more thoughtful versions
fleeting (e.g., Cook & Flay, 1978) by noting that of numerical anchoring have been shown to resist
the amount of elaboration moderated the extent to attempts at social influence better than nonthought-
which attitudes endured over time, resisted change, ful versions of each effect (Blankenship et al., 2008;
and predicted behavior (Petty et al., 1995; Wegener, Wegener et al., 2006). Similarly, thoughtful anchor-
Petty, Smoak, & Fabrigar, 2004). ing persists longer over time than nonthoughtful

124 At titudes and Social Cognition as Social Psychological Siblings

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 124 4/9/2013 9:55:47 AM


anchoring (despite the initial anchored judgments settings, there may be some level of intention to
looking the same—being equally extreme across form an impression, but there is a lack of motivation
elaboration conditions—Blankenship et al., 2008). to put effort into forming that impression. Similarly,
Also, other ELM-inspired studies have created equal although an automatically activated attitude might
initial attitudes through more versus less elaborative be perfectly capable of influencing judgments or
means (e.g., through thoughtful influences of mes- behaviors in low-thought settings, the automatically
sage arguments in one condition and nonthoughtful activated attitude could also be used in elaborative
influences of message sources or the sheer number processes of assessing the merits of the object.
of arguments in another condition) and produced If persuasion researchers had endeavored to index
differences in consequences of the resulting attitudes the amount of elaboration through the accessibility
across levels of elaboration (see Petty et al., 1995; of resulting attitudes (one of a number of cognitive
Wegener et al., 2004). outcomes of high levels of elaboration; Petty et al.,
In the current context, it is interesting to note 1995), perhaps ELM and HSM research would have
that the term elaboration was chosen for reasons been more likely to be considered as social cogni-
that seem quite compatible with the social cogni- tion research. We suspect that Ostrom would have
tion rumblings that were beginning when the ELM been more likely to view it as such. Indeed, research
was developed (Petty, 1977). In contrast with the on attitude accessibility (e.g., Fazio, Powell, & Herr,
Message Learning approach of the 1950s, elabo- 1983), which used the language of associative net-
ration reflects the idea that scrutiny of an attitude work models, was regarded as falling more clearly
object goes beyond passive receipt or memoriza- into the social cognition category. However, ELM
tion of presented information. Rather, elaboration researchers identified other ways to assess the extent
includes attention to any presented information, of elaboration in processing of persuasive commu-
attempts to access relevant information from both nications. The most popular procedure has been to
external (message) and internal (knowledge) sources manipulate the quality of the arguments contained
(including one’s previous evaluations), attempts in a message and to gauge the extent of elaboration
to scrutinize and make inferences about attitude- by the relative size of the argument quality effect on
relevant information in light of other available postmessage attitudes (e.g., Petty et al., 1976; Petty,
knowledge and standards, drawing conclusions Cacioppo, & Goldman, 1981). Research has sup-
about merits of the attitude object or recommen- ported the idea of high elaboration being associated
dation, and derivation of an overall evaluation that with greater attitude accessibility (Bizer, Tormala,
combines the outputs of these efforts (see Petty & Rucker, & Petty, 2006; Kokkinaki & Lunt, 1999;
Cacioppo, 1986a). Thus, as discussed by Petty and Rennier, 1988; Priester & Petty, 2003). However,
Cacioppo (1986a), the concept of elaboration was little research to date has examined accessibility as
an extension of the concepts of depth of process- a mediator of elaboration effects on consequences,
ing and elaboration from cognitive psychology such as persistence over time, resistance to change, or
(Craik & Lockhart, 1972; Craik & Tulving, 1975; impact on behavior (though some research has related
where depth of processing and elaboration each attitude accessibility to an attitude’s persistence over
involved connecting to-be-remembered material to time, Zanna, Fazio, & Ross, 1994; or resistance to
other knowledge structures). In common parlance, change, Bassili & Fletcher, 1991, without directly
though terms such as scrutiny, effortful processing, addressing the level of elaboration involved).
and careful thinking can be used as synonyms, ELM
researchers considered elaboration as best capturing Dual Process Approaches to
the range of cognitive activities involved. Impression Formation
The amount of elaboration is correlated with, Similar to the concept of elaboration, the core
but not synonymous with, distinctions such as distinction between automatic and controlled pro-
automatic versus controlled processing used in both cesses was evident from early social cognitive stud-
cognitive (Schneider & Shiffrin, 1977) and social ies of stereotyping (e.g., Devine, 1989) and also in
psychology (Devine, 1989), or spontaneous versus prominent models of impression formation (e.g.,
intentional impression formation (Uleman, 1999). Brewer 1988; Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). For exam-
For example, processes that lie toward the low end ple, the continuum model (Fiske & Neuberg, 1990)
of an amount-of-thinking continuum could involve differentiated between less effortful category-based
many settings in which people have no intention to assessments of people and more effortful assess-
form an impression. However, in many low-thought ments based on piecemeal processing (i.e., attribute-

Wegener, Pet t y 125

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 125 4/9/2013 9:55:47 AM


by-attribute analysis) of individuating information. person perception (Wegener et al., 2006) will or will
When a target is first encountered, one starts by not motivate more impression formation research
categorizing the person based on salient features aimed at examining lasting impact of the resulting
(e.g., skin color, body shape). If the target is of little impressions.
interest or importance, then the social perceiver has When the ELM addresses biases in processing,
little motivation to engage in more effortful (piece- such biases can be either motivational or ability based.
meal) processing and relies on the initial categoriza- That is, biases can be based in biased perceptions
tion. However, given sufficient motivation or lack or interpretations of information that follow from
of categorical fit, one is likely to assess the target on motives to end up with particular views of oneself
a more effortful attribute-by attribute (piecemeal) or the issue (e.g., the motivational “reactance” bias
basis. In general, these models predict that stereo- can lead people to counterargue a message; Petty &
typing will be less likely when piecemeal processing Cacioppo, 1979). Alternatively, the bias can follow
occurs, either because the category is set aside for from existing knowledge. For example, the person
attribute-by-attribute processing or because the cat- could have more knowledge consistent rather than
egory ends up being treated simply as one attribute inconsistent with his or her current attitude, and
among many that contribute to the overall impres- that knowledge could produce attitude-consistent
sion (Fiske, Lin, & Neuberg, 1999). Consistent biases in related judgments—an ability-based bias
with such theoretical frameworks, the majority of (e.g., Lord, Ross, & Lepper, 1979; Wood, 1982; cf.,
research on the judgmental effects of stereotypes has Kunda, 1990, on motivational biases).1
emphasized the possible use of stereotypes as heu- Research on outcome dependency (i.e., situations
ristics or shortcuts to judgment (e.g., Bodenhausen, in which one’s outcomes depend on the actions of
1990; Bodenhausen, Sheppard, & Kramer, 1994; another person) forms much of the research that
Macrae, Milne, & Bodenhausen, 1994). underlies the continuum model of impression for-
mation. However, research on outcome dependency
Comparing the Models has been variously characterized as suggesting that
It is difficult to say whether the earlier dual or outcome dependency creates motivational biases
multiple process models of attitude change had (i.e., motivations for one’s own outcomes to be
any direct impact on the subsequent dual process positive, e.g., Klein & Kunda, 1992; Kunda, 1990)
models of impression formation (though there or simply creates differences in amount of process-
was clearly a developing zeitgeist of the time; com- ing (with outcome dependency increasing extent of
pare Brewer, 1988; Chaiken et al., 1989; Fiske & processing, Devine, Sedikides, & Fuhrman, 1989;
Neuberg, 1990; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986a, 1986b; Neuberg & Fiske, 1987; much like increases in
see Chaiken & Trope, 1999; Smith & DeCoster, personal relevance increase processing of persuasive
2000, for reviews). There are many similarities messages, Petty & Cacioppo, 1979).
across these models. Perhaps the most notable is Clark and Wegener (2008) used impression
the idea that target evaluation can sometimes be formation materials fashioned after persuasive
relatively thoughtful, but, at other times, can be sig- message argument quality manipulations to argue
nificantly less cognitively demanding. Despite these that outcome dependency can both increase pro-
similarities, the focus on consequences of attitudes cessing and positively bias processing, depending
(i.e., lasting over time, resisting change, and predict- on the setting. That is, when people expected to
ing behavior) has led to more ELM-based research meet a person and have a conversation with him
on the multiple ways that the same judgments can or her (in a study presumably examining the fac-
occur and on the differences in persistence, resis- tors that lead to a pleasant interaction), they pro-
tance, or impact on other thinking or behavior that cessed information about the target person more
is associated with different levels of elaboration con- than when they did not expect to meet the person.
tributing to those judgments (see Petty et al., 1995; This increase in mere amount of processing led to
Wegener et al., 2006). This is not to say that theories more favorable thoughts about the person and to
of impression formation cannot allow for thought- more favorable judgments of the person’s qualities
ful effects of categories. However, the emphasis on if the provided information was positive, but led
different sizes of the category effects across levels of to less favorable thoughts about the person and to
thinking might have made such studies less likely. It less favorable judgments of the person’s quali-
remains to be seen whether recent work applying a ties if the provided information was less positive.
more ELM-inspired approach to stereotyping and However, expecting to interact with a person in a

126 At titudes and Social Cognition as Social Psychological Siblings

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 126 4/9/2013 9:55:47 AM


setting where substantial awards could be received memory/reasoning systems involved in social judg-
for a smooth interaction led to positive biases in ment and decision making (Rydell & McConnell,
processing. That is, thoughts and judgments of 2006; Strack & Deutsch, 2004), and many others
the person’s qualities showed the same differences discussed throughout this volume.
across the target quality manipulation (consistent As interest in implicit measures and implicit/
with equally high levels of processing when expect- explicit discrepancies have grown in both the atti-
ing to meet the person and when expecting to tudes and social cognition areas (e.g., see Petty,
meet the person with the opportunity for substan- Briñol, & Johnson, 2012), theories to address those
tial rewards for smooth interaction). In addition, phenomena can scarcely be categorized as attitude
thoughts and judgments were more positive when theories or social cognition theories, and that is
substantial awards for a later smooth interaction perhaps as it should be. From the very beginnings
were available than when the research participant of the social cognition movement, the attitudes
simply expected to interact with the person or do and social cognition domains have shared various
so with minimal reward for a smooth interaction. interests and research questions. Yet, those interests
and questions have often been addressed in rather
More Recent Theoretical Developments distinct ways.
Both the attitudes and social cognition areas
have seen development of new theories in recent Common Themes
years. In the attitudes domain, some have suggested Despite the different approaches that attitudes
taking an approach that harkens back to the syllo- and social cognition researchers took in the late
gistic and probabilistic theories of the 1960s and 1970s and 1980s, there continued to be a number
1970s (e.g., McGuire, 1960; Wyer, 1974), treating of questions and themes that appeared and reap-
all attitude-relevant information as evidence and all peared across both of these areas. In the following
persuasion as due to inferential reasoning about that sections, we will note some of these shared research
evidence (e.g., Kruglanski & Thompson, 1999; for questions, and in many cases, related answers to
application of this idea to impression formation, see those research questions.
Erb et al., 2003). Even in this suggestion, however,
motivation and ability to think carefully about the Distinctions between Content and Rating
evidence at hand are said to result in assessment of of Perceptions
the relevance of the evidence to the evaluation (with When asking research participants to provide
greater weighting given to relevant over irrelevant judgments (of evaluations, traits, or other percep-
information when motivation and ability to think tions), both attitude researchers and social cognition
are high; Pierro, Mannetti, Kruglanski, & Sleeth- researchers have acknowledged that the judgments
Keppler, 2004). This proviso makes the theory might not always directly reflect the person’s percep-
highly compatible with the ELM notion that high tions. In their variable perspective theory, Ostrom
levels of motivation and ability result in scrutiny of and Upshaw (1968) suggested that ratings of the
the merits of all available attitude-relevant informa- perceptions of social perceivers reflect not only the
tion (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986a; see Wegener & content of their views but also their translation
Carlston, 2005). Other theories have more directly of that content onto the rating scale. Key to this
spoken to issues of attitude representation (e.g., translation was the perspective used to define the
Lord & Lepper, 1999; Petty, Briñol, & DeMarree, meaning of the scale anchors. In other words, when
2007) and discrepancies between implicit and rating a subjective scale, such as “good” to “bad”
explicit indices of attitudes (e.g., Gawronski & (Osgood, Suci, & Tannenbaum, 1957) or “light” to
Bodenhausen, 2006; Petty, Briñol, & DeMarree, “heavy” (Sherif, Taub, & Hovland, 1958), respon-
2007; Rydell & McConnell, 2006). dents call to mind examples of objects that could
In the social cognition area, new theories have represent the scale anchors, and this perspective on
addressed mechanisms that drive responses to reac- the anchors then forms the context for their transla-
tion time tasks (Conrey, Sherman, Gawronski, tion of the content of their views to be reported on
Hugenberg, & Groom, 2005), the role of the active the scale. Thus, if something in the research setting
self concept in priming effects on judgments and expands a person’s perspective by calling to mind
behaviors (Wheeler, DeMarree, & Petty, 2007), more extreme exemplars for the anchor (such as a
the various forms in which impressions might be heavier weight to anchor the “heavy” scale anchor,
represented in memory (Carlston, 1994), different Sherif et al., 1958, or a more favorable behavior or

Wegener, Pet t y 127

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 127 4/9/2013 9:55:47 AM


instance of the object category to anchor the “good” social desirability and other motives for people to
scale anchor), this could lead to a change in rating hide their true attitudes, indirect measures were
without any necessary change in content of the per- developed in an attempt to index people’s attitudes.
ception. Ostrom (1970; Steele & Ostrom, 1974) Indirect measures typically took advantage of attitu-
used variable perspective theory to examine belief dinal processes, such as activation and use of knowl-
and attitude change (see Petty & Cacioppo, 1981). edge that is directionally consistent with the attitude
However, as the weight example suggests, this point (e.g., Hammond, 1948), the tendency to like others
of view is directly applicable to a variety of non- more when they held similar rather than dissimilar
evaluative judgments. attitudes (e.g., Hendrick & Seyfried, 1974), or the
For example, a person could view a 5’10” man tendency to behave more positively toward people,
and a 5’10” woman as equally tall in a perceptual objects, or causes one likes rather than dislikes (e.g.,
sense and rate them the same on a “content” inches Byrne, Ervin, & Lamberth, 1970; Milgram, Mann,
scale, but rate the man as average in height and the & Harter, 1965). More recently, with the influence
woman as quite tall on the same “short” to “tall” of social cognition, indirect measures have more
subjective “rating” scale. From the point of view of often been referred to as implicit measures (loosely
variable perspective theory, this could be because dif- following the concept in cognitive psychology of
ferent content perspectives come to mind to anchor implicit memory—whereby the person cannot
the rating scales for men and women. This difference explicitly remember a piece of information, but
in perspective bears close resemblance to the use of performance on other tasks shows that the informa-
shifting standards across stereotyped groups (e.g., tion is still in memory; see Roediger, 2003).2 This
Biernat, Manis, & Nelson, 1991; Biernat, Vescio, & new generation of indirect (implicit) measures also
Manis, 1998) but with some differences in empha- makes use of attitudinal processes—especially of
sis. For instance, Biernat and colleagues have sug- spreading activation of the evaluation, such that,
gested that objective measures might often result in after activation of the attitude, respondents are bet-
an assimilation of judgments toward the stereotypes ter prepared to identify like-valenced stimuli rather
of the groups. Assimilation to the stereotypes of than stimuli that mismatch the valence of the atti-
men and women would result in judgments of taller tude (e.g., Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & Williams,
men than women, even when the exemplars in the 1995; Wittenbrink, Judd, & Park, 1997).
study are equated for height (Biernat et al., 1991). Generally, the critical difference between
However, subjective judgments (such as a “tallness” “explicit” (direct) and “implicit” (indirect) mea-
scale, rather than judgments of feet and inches) sures is that the latter assess the activation of an
make use of different standards (different perspec- attitude without asking the respondents to report
tives) for the different groups. These different stan- their attitude. Because of this, on implicit mea-
dards or perspectives can eliminate or sometimes sures, respondents are relatively unaware that
reverse the differences in judgments (e.g., judging their attitude is being assessed (or, in some cases,
the women to be “taller” than the men, similar to respondents might become aware of the measure-
the perspective theory example). ment attempt, but have some difficulty controlling
The variable perspective distinction between the impact of the attitude on responses, as in the
content and rating has also influenced discussions Implicit Association Test, or IAT; Greenwald et al.,
of survey responding and attitude measurement 1998). Whether a given implicit measure primarily
(e.g., Tourangeau & Rasinski, 1988), and the scale taps one’s attitude per se is another matter, how-
anchoring component of variable perspective theory ever, and, when controversial attitude objects are
has played a role in theories of assimilation and con- addressed, one cannot simply use the direct mea-
trast (especially in the inclusion/exclusion model of sure as a comparison for construct validity pur-
Schwarz & Bless, 1992, 2007). poses. However, in many instances, various indirect
(implicit) measures fail to relate closely to one
Indirect (Implicit) Measures another or to direct (explicit) measures, even when
Not too long after it was clear that attitudes addressing less controversial issues. Thus, it is not
could be measured, attitude researchers developed surprising that criticisms that the indirect measures
concerns that respondents to direct (explicit) mea- might measure something other than the attitude
sures might screen their judgments or be unwilling (e.g., Kidder & Campbell, 1970) seem to resurface
to accurately report their evaluations (Doob, 1948; with each new generation of indirect measures (see
Hovland et al., 1953). Because of concerns about Fazio & Olson, 2003).

128 At titudes and Social Cognition as Social Psychological Siblings

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 128 4/9/2013 9:55:47 AM


In the case of contemporary indirect (implicit) opinions, but could result from dissimulation, dis-
measures, there have been debates about the rela- tortion, or lying). Thus, the attitude construct was
tive contributions of “personal” and “extrapersonal” connected very early on with implicit processes and
(normative) associations to popular measures, such potential for lack of awareness of its effects on other
as the IAT (Greenwald & Nosek, 2009; Karpinski thinking and behavior.
& Hilton, 2001; Olson & Fazio, 2004). Even Consistent with this observation, it is interest-
beyond these debates, it seems worth highlighting ing to note that many of the examples provided in
one additional issue related to the constructs being the landmark Greenwald and Banaji (1995) paper
addressed. Because contemporary indirect measures on implicit social cognition addressed evaluative
often rely on spreading activation, they primarily processes. The research on mere exposure is a good
tap the influences of relatively strong (accessible) example. That is, when novel objects are encoun-
attitudes (Fazio, 1995). Thus, at least part of the tered repeatedly, people often evaluate the objects
lack of relation between direct and indirect mea- more favorably (Zajonc, 1968), even if the people
sures (even with noncontroversial topics) is likely cannot say whether or not they have previously
that indirect measures tap into both the evaluation seen the object (Kunst-Wilson & Zajonc, 1980).
and its accessibility, whereas direct measures tap Bornstein (1989; Bornstein & D’Agostino, 1994)
into the evaluation (relatively) separately from its explained such exposure effects as due to increases
accessibility. Furthermore, direct measures might in perceptual fluency, which perceivers might
also tap into the perceived validity of any evaluation attribute to liking for the object, but which they
that automatically comes to mind (Petty, Briñol, might also attribute to other stimulus dimensions
& DeMarree, 2007). Consistent with these obser- (Mandler, Nakamura, & Shebo Van Zandt, 1987),
vations, tapping stronger (especially more acces- perhaps including disliking if the stimulus is neg-
sible) attitudes (e.g., LeBel, 2010; Nosek, 2005) atively valenced (Klinger & Greenwald, 1994).
and encouraging people to view their automatic Similar to other demonstrations of misattribu-
or gut reactions as valid (e.g., Jordan, Whitfield, tion, when familiarity can be attributed to previ-
& Zeigler-Hill, 2007; Loersch, McCaslin, & Petty, ous presentations rather than liking, mere exposure
2011) increase the relation between implicit and effects are diminished. Thus, mere exposure effects
explicit measures. are reduced when exposure lasts for longer periods
of time (Bornstein & D’Agostino, 1992) or when
Implicit/Automatic versus Explicit/ people are told that the stimuli have been presented
Controlled Processes previously (Bornstein & D’Agostino, 1994).
Closely related to the work on implicit measures The interplay of implicit (automatic) and explicit
is the more general attention given to unconscious (deliberative) processes in the formation of attitudes
or automatic processes versus explicit (conscious) is a very active current area of research in models
or controlled processes. On some level, attention to such as the associative–propositional evaluation
automatic/implicit processes has become a hallmark (APE) model (Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006)
of social cognition research (even though many pro- and the metacognitive model (MCM) of attitude
cesses may possess only some features of automatic- structure (Petty, Briñol, & DeMarree, 2007). One
ity; Bargh, 1994). That is, if one has any question could certainly draw parallels between associative
about whether a given research question is social processes and the heuristic or cue-based processes
cognitive in nature, one can confidently answer of the HSM or ELM and between propositional
in the affirmative if the question addresses uncon- processes and the systematic processing or elabo-
scious or automatic processes (especially if indexed ration in the HSM or ELM. However, associa-
by reaction time measures). Although certain tools tive processes could be involved in both relatively
for examining such questions have developed along thoughtful (biased processing) and nonthoughtful
with the blossoming of social cognition, similar (cue-based) mechanisms. Similarly, propositional
research questions had been present in the atti- processes could be applied in relatively thoughtful or
tudes arena for some time. Indeed, Hovland et al. nonthoughtful ways. The APE couches the associa-
(1953) conceptually distinguished among attitudes tive and propositional processes in thoroughly social
(that were implicit tendencies to approach or avoid cognitive terms and has inspired a variety of stud-
a given attitude object), opinions (that were verbal- ies that address differences in evaluative outcomes
izable anticipations, expectations, and evaluations), when indexed through implicit (associative) versus
and overt responses (that were often consistent with explicit (propositional) means. In our reading, the

Wegener, Pet t y 129

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 129 4/9/2013 9:55:47 AM


available evidence indicates that change on both these short-SOA conditions. Automatic activation
implicit and explicit attitude measures can be medi- of attitudes was present when attitude objects were
ated by both implicit and explicit processes (Petty, identified as associated with relatively strong object-
& Briñol, 2010; Whitfield & Jordan, 2009). evaluation associations (by measuring reaction times
Perhaps one of the earliest and best single exam- to dichotomous evaluative reports) or were manipu-
ples of attitudes research making use of tools and lated to have strong object-evaluation associations
concepts from contemporary cognitive and social (through repeated expression of the attitude; Fazio,
cognitive psychology is the research on attitude Sanbonmatsu, Powell, & Kardes, 1986).
accessibility (i.e., the speed and ease with which atti- This research might also constitute the best
tudes come to mind upon encountering the attitude example of a program of research that served to
object). Following up on early research on direct advance both the attitudes and social cognition
experience and attitude confidence as factors that areas. It served as one of the earliest adaptations
increase attitude–behavior consistency (e.g., Fazio of sequential priming techniques to social stimuli.
& Zanna, 1978; Regan & Fazio, 1977), Russ Fazio More generally, the evaluative priming outcomes,
and his colleagues turned their attention to attitude moderated by attitude accessibility, also illustrated
accessibility for a more process-oriented explanation that abstract socially relevant concepts can be
of why particular attitudes have greater influence on spontaneously activated when related objects are
behaviors. Early research in this program identified encountered (which is a key factor in various types
attitudes formed through direct experience as com- of situated cognition; Robbins & Aydede, 2009).
ing to mind more quickly upon encountering the In addition, research from this program suggested
attitude object and also showed that more accessible that objects toward which people hold accessible
attitudes lead to higher attitude–behavior consis- attitudes receive more immediate visual attention
tency (Fazio, Chen, McDonel, & Sherman, 1982). (Roskos-Ewoldsen & Fazio, 1992) and that mul-
Soon thereafter, Fazio adapted the work on concept tiply categorizable targets are more readily identi-
priming (e.g., Higgins, Rholes, & Jones, 1977) to fied as members of categories toward which people
examine evaluative priming. That is, spontaneous have accessible attitudes (Smith, Fazio, & Cejka,
activation of one’s attitude upon encountering the 1996). The research on attitude accessibility also
attitude object (even if one was not asked to evalu- led to development of a prominent model of atti-
ate the object) influenced judgments of the motives tude–behavior relations (i.e., the Motivation and
underlying a target person’s behaviors, and this Opportunity as DEterminants of attitude–behavior
evaluative priming was more likely when the person processes, or MODE; Fazio, 1990). In addition to
already possessed an accessible attitude toward the attitude–behavior relations, the MODE model also
object (Fazio et al., 1983). forms a prominent approach to conceptualizing
Although this research suggested that strong sources of discrepancy between implicit and explicit
object-evaluation associations facilitate spontane- attitude measures (see Olson & Fazio, 2009), and
ous activation of attitudes, it did not directly address a version of the sequential priming technique has
whether the spontaneous activation of attitudes served as a prominent implicit measure of racial
qualifies as being automatic rather than controlled attitudes (Fazio et al., 1995).
(Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977). To address this ques-
tion more directly, Fazio and his colleagues adapted Role of Memory in Evaluations
sequential priming techniques from cognitive psy- and Impressions
chology in which presentation of a category label An early question presaging later social cognitive
facilitates identifying a target word as a word when emphases on memory addressed the possible links
that target is semantically related to the category between memory and evaluations. This question
(e.g., Neely, 1977). The automaticity of the attitude formed a key component of the message learning
activation was addressed by setting the stimulus onset approach to attitude change (Hovland et al., 1953)
asynchrony (SOA; i.e., the delay between presenta- and was addressed in a number of ways. Although
tion of the attitude object and the target adjective) some studies identified parallel effects on retention
to be too short (i.e., 300 milliseconds) for controlled of information from a persuasive message and result-
processing of the attitude object. Automatic acti- ing attitudes (e.g., Eagly, 1974; McGuire, 1957),
vation of the attitude was evidenced by speeded other studies failed to find relations between mem-
identification of the valence of like-valenced adjec- ory for message arguments and resulting attitudes
tives compared with opposite-valenced adjectives in (e.g., Hovland & Weiss, 1951; Insko, 1964; Miller

130 At titudes and Social Cognition as Social Psychological Siblings

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 130 4/9/2013 9:55:47 AM


& Campbell, 1959; Osterhouse & Brock, 1970) or change the attitude (belief ) to fit the friend’s opin-
found that changes in attitudes were often not accom- ion, deny the disagreement (by viewing the friend
panied by changes in memory for related informa- as actually holding an agreeable point of view), or
tion (or vice versa; e.g., Watts & McGuire, 1964). cognitively differentiate between the part of the
By the 1960s, attitude researchers were no longer person responsible for the disagreement (and dislike
emphasizing memory for the verbatim information that part of the person) and the part of the person
in a persuasive message. Instead, researchers treated that one likes overall. Abelson (1959) more directly
message recipients’ idiosyncratic interpretations of addressed the microprocesses potentially involved
the information or their cognitive responses to the and suggested that mechanisms such as denial of
information as more important determinants of the the disagreement, bolstering of one’s own opinion,
extent of persuasion or resistance (e.g., Festinger & and differentiation of the other varied in the extent
Maccoby, 1964; Greenwald, 1968). Alternatively, to which the perceiver had to exert effort in that
some researchers weighted memory for persuasive mode of inconsistency resolution. Relatively little
information by message recipients’ propensity to research in attitudes directly addressed the various
yield to that information, rather than assuming that modes of inconsistency resolution and often fixed
memory of the information alone would predict the conditions to make attitude change the most
resulting attitudes (e.g., McGuire, 1968). likely mode (but see Rosenberg & Abelson, 1960;
This approach to attitude change has many paral- Simon, Greenberg, & Brehm, 1995). However,
lels with the postulated independence of impressions the cognitive differentiation mode of inconsistency
and memory for impression-relevant information resolution bears a good deal of resemblance to sub-
(Lingle & Ostrom, 1979, 1981). A number of typing research in stereotyping, where people split
social cognition theorists suggested that this inde- out a subtype (e.g., working women) from a larger
pendence is especially likely when social perceivers category (e.g., women) as a means to diminish the
form impressions online (as social information is inconsistency within the category and avoid pres-
received) but is less likely when perceivers cannot or sures to change beliefs about the category as a whole
do not form impressions until later (and use what (Johnston & Hewstone, 1992; Weber & Crocker,
they remember to create memory-based impressions; 1983). Similar subtyping effects have been observed
e.g., Hastie & Park, 1986; Tormala & Petty, 2001). on both implicit and explicit measures of attitudes
In the attitudes domain, the key variables from the (Barden, Maddux, Petty, & Brewer, 2004).
ELM and HSM (i.e., motivation and ability to pro-
cess attitude-relevant information) were found to Assimilation and Contrast
moderate the extent to which evaluations correlated Early research on attitude change incorporated
with memory for message arguments (with larger the idea that people’s views of attitude-related stimuli
memory-attitude correlations following low moti- can be distorted by comparisons of the stimuli with
vation or low ability when receiving the persuasive their own attitudes (e.g., in social judgment theory;
message; e.g., Haugtvedt & Petty, 1992; Mackie & Sherif & Hovland, 1961). These judgmental distor-
Asuncion, 1990). Interestingly, these differences tions could be assimilation (i.e., viewing the evalua-
also helped to account for ELM-based predictions tive stimuli as more similar to one’s own attitude) or
regarding the likelihood of primacy effects versus contrast (i.e., viewing the evaluative stimuli as less
recency effects in persuasion (e.g., Petty, Tormala, similar to one’s own attitude; Hovland, Harvey, &
Hawkins, & Wegener, 2001), a previously unsolved Sherif, 1957). In social judgment research, evaluative
puzzle from early attitudes research (e.g., Hovland, stimuli that fell relatively close to the person’s own
1957; Miller & Campbell, 1959; see Haugtvedt & attitude (i.e., within one’s latitude of acceptance)
Wegener, 1994, for discussion). were assimilated toward one’s attitude, whereas
evaluative stimuli that fell relatively far away from
Subtyping the person’s own attitude (i.e., in one’s latitude of
When people receive information that does not rejection) were contrasted even farther away from
fit with existing beliefs or attitudes, what do they one’s attitude. In the early 1980s, similar principles
do? Early cognitive consistency theories suggested were applied to nonevaluative judgments (e.g., size),
that there can be a number of different types of reac- such that more extreme contexts (i.e., farther from
tions. For example, if a person learns that a friend the target) created contrast, but less extreme con-
disagrees with him or her about some attitude object, texts (i.e., closer to the target) created assimilation
Heider (1958) suggested that the person could either (Herr, Sherman, & Fazio, 1983).

Wegener, Pet t y 131

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 131 4/9/2013 9:55:47 AM


Over the years, in addition to the variable per- et al., 1983), recent research suggests that overlap
spective approach mentioned earlier, a number of or lack thereof in both context and target ranges
social cognitive accounts of assimilation and con- should influence the direction of context effects.
trast developed. The majority of early social cogni-
tive research on assimilation and contrast dealt with Metacognition
assimilation toward or contrast away from concept One growing topic in both the attitudes and
primes (e.g., Lombardi, Higgins, & Bargh, 1987). social cognition areas is metacognition (i.e., think-
Prominent theories in this area initially treated ing about thinking). Work on metacognition
assimilation of judgments toward the primes (i.e., emerged in cognitive psychology with a focus on
typical priming effects) as the default, and contrast perceptions of people’s own memories (e.g., feeling
away from the primes as efforts to exclude or par- of knowing; Costermans, Lories, & Ansay, 1992)
tial out reactions to the target that were attributed and was brought into prominence within social
to the primes (e.g., the set/reset approach, Martin, cognition in the late 1990s (see Jost, Kruglanski,
1986; Martin, Seta, & Crelia, 1990; and the inclu- & Nelson, 1998; Yzerbyt, Lories, & Dardenne,
sion/exclusion approach, Schwarz & Bless, 1992). 1998). Petty, Briñol, Tormala, and Wegener (2007)
Later research on theory-based correction suggested listed a number of types of thoughts one can have
that either assimilation or contrast could be the about one’s own thoughts, including the target of
default bias (consistent with Herr et al., 1983), but the thought (what the thought is about), the origin
that corrections could then proceed in either direc- (source) of the thought, the valence of the thought
tion and in amounts guided by the extent to which (regarding whether the thought conveys positive or
judgments were perceived as influenced by the con- negative qualities of the target), perceived amount
text (e.g., Petty & Wegener, 1993; Wegener & Petty, of thinking, evaluation of the thought (whether the
1995; see Wegener & Petty, 1997, for a review). thought is desirable or not, appropriate or not, etc.),
This theoretical approach was also broadly appli- and confidence in the thought.
cable to many potentially biasing stimuli, including In the attitudes domain, metacognition, though
source characteristics in persuasive messages (Kang not initially labeled as such, was of widespread inter-
& Herr, 2006; Petty, Wegener, & White, 1998). est as a factor related to the strength of attitudes.
Priming-based work on assimilation and con- That is, a number of traditional strength-related
trast also began to address different types of priming properties of attitudes can be considered metacog-
effects (such as exemplar primes leading to contrast, nitions (i.e., perceptions of one’s attitudes). These
Herr, 1986, but trait primes leading to assimila- properties include the confidence (Allport, 1924)
tion, Srull & Wyer, 1979). Eventually, research also or subjective ambivalence (Tourangeau, Rasinski,
addressed mindsets that could determine the extent Bradburn, & D’Andrade, 1989) with which the
to which the same stimuli led to assimilation or con- attitude is held and the amount of perceived knowl-
trast effects (e.g., local vs. global processing, Förster, edge underlying the attitude (Davidson et al., 1985;
Liberman, & Kuschel, 2008; similarity vs. dissimi- see Wegener, Downing, Krosnick, & Petty, 1995,
larity testing, Mussweiler, 2003; reflection vs. evalu- for a review). These strength-related properties
ation, Markman & McMullen, 2003). Coming of attitudes have been associated with many out-
somewhat full circle is recent work on determinants comes, including increased persistence of the atti-
of when people might be likely to test similarities or tudes over time, resistance to change, and guiding
differences. One approach is to focus on the poten- of future thinking and behavior. Also, when people
tial dimensional overlap between the contextual and possessing these attitudes encounter additional per-
target stimuli, such that assimilation occurs when suasive information, the properties of the attitude
the ranges of potential values for the context and also influence the amount of processing of that new
target overlap, but contrast occurs when the ranges information (e.g., see Clark & Wegener, 2013).
of potential values for the context and target do not Unfortunately, a full review of these effects could
overlap (Chien, Wegener, Hsiao, & Petty, 2010). fill the current volume, so we are unable to provide
That is, whereas social judgment theory addressed a complete review of metacognition and attitudes
contexts (existing attitudes) as representing ranges (see Briñol & DeMarree, 2012; Petty & Krosnick,
of potential values (i.e., latitudes of acceptance; 1995; Petty et al., 2007,for reviews). Recently,
Sherif & Sherif, 1967) and feature-matching views people’s perceptions of their own degree of elabo-
incorporated ambiguity of the target (i.e., different ration (Barden & Petty, 2008) have been added to
sizes of potential ranges of target qualities, Herr the traditional measures. Perceptions of amount of

132 At titudes and Social Cognition as Social Psychological Siblings

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 132 4/9/2013 9:55:48 AM


elaboration have accounted for links between tra- the more people like that object or issue (Haddock,
ditional measures of amount of elaboration (e.g., Rothman, & Schwarz, 1996; Tormala, Petty, &
number of thoughts listed) and the confidence with Briñol, 2002; Wänke, Bless, & Biller, 1996), and
which the attitude is held. ease of generation also makes people more confi-
Research on thought confidence (i.e., self-vali- dent in their attitudes (Haddock, Rothman, Reber,
dation; Petty, Briñol, & Tormala, 2002) has dem- & Schwarz, 1999) and their thoughts (Tormala
onstrated that many factors, including message et al., 2007). Ease of retrieval can also influence
recipient emotion, source credibility, and power other judgments, such as likelihood estimates (e.g.,
of the message recipient can influence confidence Hirt, Kardes, & Markman, 2004; Wänke, Schwarz,
in thoughts that people have previously generated & Bless, 1995) and risk assessments (Grayson &
when processing a persuasive message under high- Schwarz, 1999). Some of this research also suggests
elaboration conditions (see Briñol & Petty, 2009, that ease effects can be mediated by confidence (per-
for a review). In each of these cases, the validating ceptions of validity) associated with the easily gen-
factor increases confidence in thoughts regardless of erated thoughts (e.g., Tormala et al., 2002; see also
whether the thought is relatively favorable or unfa- Wänke & Bless, 2000).
vorable (i.e., regardless of whether the message con- The earlier discussion of bias correction also
sists of relatively strong or weak arguments). relates to metacognition because assessments
Interestingly, when people form impressions of of thought content (valence and source of the
others, it appears that another form of self-valida- thought), evaluation, and validity likely all come
tion can occur. When people generate initial reac- into play (see Petty et al., 2007). Before closing this
tions to a target person under conditions conducive section, it is important to note that metacognition
to relatively thoughtful processing of impression- has also recently taken a role in the mental repre-
relevant information, confidence in these initial sentation of attitudes. Specifically, in the MCM of
reactions is higher after later learning that the per- attitudes, perceptions of the attitude’s validity are a
son is a member of a group stereotyped as consis- part of the structure of the attitude (Petty & Briñol,
tent with the initial impression. This increase in 2006a; Petty, Briñol, & DeMarree, 2007). The
confidence leads to stronger influences of the initial MCM represents attitudes as including associations
(stereotype-consistent) impressions on related judg- between the attitude object and both positive and
ments and recommendations regarding the target negative evaluations. Along with these associations,
(Clark, Wegener, Briñol, & Petty, 2009). This type however, the MCM includes validity tags for these
of “matching-based” validation creates an instance evaluative associations. When evaluative responses
in which high levels of processing produce another are relatively nondeliberative (even automatic),
high-thought form of stereotyping that is influen- such responses may be guided by activated evalu-
tial and potentially difficult to overcome (because of ative associations. However, when they are more
the associated high level of elaboration; cf., Wegener deliberative (i.e., when people think about them
et al., 2006). more carefully), these responses may be influenced
Much of the earliest metacognitive research in important ways by the perceptions of validity of
in social cognition addressed the ease with which the positive versus negative evaluations.
information comes to mind (similar to the research
on perceptual fluency – see earlier discussion of Embodied Cognition
mere exposure research and Alter & Oppenheimer, A final area in which the domains of attitudes
2009, Claypool, Mackie, & Garcia-Marques, in and social cognition have had separate histories but
press, for reviews). In particular, ease of generation have come together more recently is with respect to
has been shown to influence the impact of informa- the use of one’s own body in influencing thoughts
tion on judgments of self and others. For example, and judgments, an area known as embodied cogni-
when people can easily generate a few instances of tion (see Briñol & Petty, 2008; Semin & Smith,
when they have been assertive in the past, they judge 2008). It is obvious that the mind and mental states
themselves as being more assertive than when they influence the body in many ways (e.g., happiness
must generate many instances of being assertive leading to smiling), but a core notion of embodied
(and have a difficult time doing so; Schwarz et al., (Lakoff & Johnson, 1999) or grounded (Barsalou,
1991). Many effects of ease have influenced evalua- 2008) cognition is that the movement and place-
tions. For example, the easier it seems for people to ment of one’s body can influence the mind and
generate positive thoughts about an object or issue, mental states as well. Within cognitive psychology,

Wegener, Pet t y 133

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 133 4/9/2013 9:55:48 AM


numerous embodiment effects have been shown, Although much work on embodiment in social
including people remembering more of a story psychology has focused on linking behaviors directly
when they physically act it out (Scott, Harris, & to judgments, more recent work has attempted to
Rothe, 2001). understand the mechanisms by which evaluations
Within attitudes and social cognition, interest stem from the body. Perhaps not surprisingly, work
in embodied cognition stems from two distinct within the ELM framework has shown that a per-
sources. The first is Darwin’s theory of evolution. son’s bodily movements or responses, like other
Indeed, the link between the attitude concept and variables, can influence attitudes by affecting one or
bodily responses has a long history going back to more of the core influence processes noted earlier.
the use of the term attitude to refer to the posture For example, simple cue effects of the body (e.g.,
of one’s body (Galton, 1884) and to expressive based on arm flexion) are more likely to have a
motor behaviors (e.g., a scowling face was said to direct impact on judgments when thinking is low
indicate a hostile attitude; Darwin, 1965/1872). (Priester, Cacioppo, & Petty, 1996). When thinking
Although much early attitudes research focused is high, bodily movements such as head nodding
on how the body or bodily movements could (Briñol & Petty, 2003) or sitting in an erect rather
reflect one’s attitudes (e.g., Hess & Polt, 1960; than slumped posture (Briñol, Petty, & Wagner,
Solarz, 1960), more contemporary studies have 2009) can validate one’s thoughts. And, if elabora-
examined how the body can affect one’s evalua- tion is not already constrained to be high or low
tions. For example, researchers have shown that by other variables, body-relevant factors can affect
(1) nodding one’s head in a vertical rather than a the extent of information processing. For example,
horizontal manner during presentation of a strong people are less likely to think carefully about a mes-
persuasive message can increase the persuasiveness sage when they are placed in a powerful or confident
of that message (Wells & Petty, 1980), (2) holding (standing) rather than a powerless and vulnerable
a pen between one’s teeth (which facilitates a facial (reclining) posture (Petty, Wells, Heesacker, Brock,
expression similar to smiling) versus holding a pen & Cacioppo, 1983), and they are more likely to
between one’s lips (which inhibits smiling) can think about a message extensively when it is pre-
enhance the perceived humor in cartoons (Strack, sented on a heavy (signifying weighty or important)
Martin, & Stepper, 1988), and (3) information rather than a light clipboard (Jostmann, Lakens, &
presented while performing an approach behavior Schubert, 2009). In addition to examining the basic
(e.g., using one’s hands to pull up from underneath mechanisms by which embodiment works, research
a table) is evaluated more positively than informa- in both attitudes and social cognition is likely to
tion presented during an avoidance behavior (e.g., focus in the future on whether embodiment effects
pushing down on a table top surface; Cacioppo, are the same as or different from other forms of con-
Priester, & Berntson, 1993; cf. Seibt, Neumann, ceptual priming.
Nussinson, & Strack, 2008).
A second important influence on contempo- Maturation and Broadening of
rary studies of embodiment comes from work in Social Cognition
cognitive psychology and linguistics on metaphors As noted earlier, especially in the 1970s and early
(Lakoff & Johnson, 1980). Embodied metaphors 1980s, the emphasis in social cognition was on cold
would include examples such as people verbalizing cognition (i.e., cognition without extra-cognitive
that someone is “above” them or has control “over” motives or emotion). This emphasis might have
them. These metaphors likely stem from early expe- partially come from developments in the 1960s and
riences in childhood when adults were taller than early 1970s in areas such as causal attribution (e.g.,
children. Another example would be associating Jones & Davis, 1965; Kelley, 1967; see Chapter 6)
warm temperatures with caring. This metaphor and aided by the computer analogy and mathemati-
might stem from a childhood of snuggling close to cal/computational models of cognitive (often evalu-
a nurturing parent. These early experiences can then ative) systems and processes (e.g., Fishbein, 1963;
lead adults to feel less powerful when other people McGuire, 1960; Wyer, 1974). In comparison, it is
are literally placed higher in physical location than certainly true that social cognition of the 1990s and
they are (Briñol, Petty, Valle, Rucker, & Becerra, 2000s became considerably more diverse. The tradi-
2007) and to view another person as more caring tional attitudinal topics of motivation and emotion
after simply touching a hot coffee mug (Williams have been rediscovered (though couched in terms
& Bargh, 2008). of cognitive antecedents, processes, and, in many

134 At titudes and Social Cognition as Social Psychological Siblings

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 134 4/9/2013 9:55:48 AM


cases, consequences). This does not mean, however, attitudes research has included many sorts of cog-
that social cognition researchers have embraced nitive processes and constructs, such as compre-
traditional attitudes research as part of the social hension, retention, balance, cognitive dissonance,
cognition enterprise. In some ways, this seems and cognitive response. Yet, many of the original
unfortunate because the development of contempo- treatments of these processes were not identified as
rary attitude theories and the development of the “social cognition” in the early days of that approach
social cognition movement were, in certain respects, because the original attitude theories did not directly
responses to similar concerns. It is also unfortunate address the core cognitive processes of encoding,
because, at least in our “attitudinal” view, it stems storage (representation in memory), or retrieval. We
in part from the original rather narrow (often can’t help but wonder if many such questions and
unstated) view of what qualified as social cogni- methods might have been embraced by early social
tion. This view has changed to some extent as social cognition researchers if the questions and methods
cognition has become more of an approach than an had not predated the beginnings of the earnest social
area of research (originally almost synonymous with cognition movement. That is, if the attitudinal ques-
impression formation or person memory, hence the tions and measures had developed alongside or after,
development of the PMIG meeting). Still, there is rather than prior to, the pact to abandon informa-
a bit of an “I know it when I see it” quality to iden- tion integration research, perhaps contemporary
tifying when one uses a social cognition approach, attitudes and social cognition domains would have
and some of the telltale signs of the approach con- developed more like twins (with potentially differ-
tinue to involve use of a reaction time or memory ent interests, but the same basic genetics), instead of
measure to talk about concept activation, represen- older and younger siblings attempting to carve out
tation in memory, or automatic processes. To be their own independent identities.
sure, the number and type of measures that both
attitudes and social cognition researchers use has Notes
expanded over time. However, addressing activation 1. Kunda (1990) described some research originally formu-
or automatic processes (still most typically identi- lated as an ability-based bias (e.g., attitudinal schemas biasing
fied through use of reaction time measures) remains assimilation of new information; Lord et al., 1979) in more
motivational terms, consistent with her focus on making the case
a nearly sufficient criterion for judging research to for motivational bias. Regarding that focus, it is interesting to
be social cognition. note that Kunda (1990) characterized the most extensive litera-
As noted by Wegener and Carlston (2005), the ture on directional motives biasing processing as coming from
current operational definition of “cognitive process” work on cognitive dissonance (Festinger, 1957, 1964). Indeed,
in social psychology has become quite broad and this research might also constitute some of the strongest evi-
dence of such biases. At least in our reading, many of the other
applicable to research across many domains and oft-cited studies (e.g., examining judgments of self vs. others)
using many methods. Indeed, in contemporary seemed more vulnerable than dissonance experiments to cogni-
social psychology, one could often drop the “cogni- tive (ability-based) alternative explanations (because of knowl-
tive” from the term “cognitive process” with little edge content differences when dealing with oneself rather than
change in meaning. The term cognitive is used very another person).
2. It is interesting to note that Roediger (2003), among oth-
broadly, virtually as a synonym for “psychologi- ers, has come to prefer the terms “direct” and “indirect” mea-
cal” or “mental.” If the human brain is involved, a sures over “explicit” and “implicit” measures because, clearly,
process is cognitive. Because the brain is almost implicit measures can be influenced by explicit processes, and
always involved, few activities fall outside the cog- explicit measures can be influenced by implicit processes. Thus,
nitive umbrella (including habits, directly primed at least some cognitive psychologists have come back around to
the terminology that was common in earlier research on attitude
behaviors, and other phenomena that once would measurement.
have been viewed as not particularly cognitive).
Moreover, traditional alternatives to cognition such
References
as motivation and emotion are treated as having Abelson, R. P. (1959). Modes of resolution of belief dilemmas.
cognitive antecedents, as operating on knowledge Journal of Conflict Resolution, 3, 343–352.
structures stored in memory, and as having cogni- Abelson, R. P. (1963). Computer simulation of “hot cognitions.”
tive consequences (Wegener & Carlston, 2005; see In S. S. Tomkins & S. Mesick (Eds.), Computer simulation of
also Markus & Zajonc, 1985). personality. New York: Wiley.
Allport, F. H. (1924). Social psychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
During this transformation in social cognition, Alter, A. L., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2009). Uniting the tribes
definitions of what people consider to be a “cog- of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Personality and
nitive approach” have shifted. As discussed earlier, Social Psychology Review, 13, 219–235.

Wegener, Pet t y 135

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 135 4/9/2013 9:55:48 AM


Anderson, J. R. (1980). Cognitive psychology and its implications. Bornstein, R. F., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1994). The attribution
San Francisco: Freeman. and discounting of perceptual fluency: Preliminary tests of a
Anderson, J. R., & Hastie, R. (1974). Individuation and refer- perceptual fluency/attributional model of the mere exposure
ence in memory: Proper names and definite descriptions. effect. Social Cognition, 12, 103–128.
Cognitive Psychology, 6, 495–514. Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. San
Anderson, N. H. (1966). Component ratings in impression for- Diego, CA: Academic Press.
mation. Psychonomic Science, 6, 179–180. Brewer, M. B. (1988). A dual process model of impression for-
Anderson, N. H. (1970). Functional measurement and psy- mation. In T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer (Eds.), Advances in social
chophysical judgment. Psychological Review, 77, 153–170. cognition (Vol. 1, pp. 1–36). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Anderson, N. H. (1971). Integration theory and attitude change. Briñol, P., & DeMarree, K. (Eds.) (2012). Social metacognition.
Psychological Review, 78, 171–206. New York: Psychology Press.
Asch, S. E. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. Journal Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E (2003). Overt head movements and
of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 258–290. persuasion: A self-validation analysis. Journal of Personality
Barden, J., Maddux, W. W., Petty, R. E., & Brewer, M. B. (2004). and Social Psychology, 84, 1123–1139.
Contextual moderation of racial bias: The impact of social Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2008). Embodied persuasion:
roles on controlled and automatically activated attitudes. Fundamental processes by which bodily responses can impact
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 5–22. attitudes. In G. R. Semin & E. R. Smith (Eds.), Embodiment
Barden, J., & Petty, R. E. (2008). The mere perception of elabo- grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific
ration creates attitude certainty: Exploring the thoughtful- approaches (pp. 184–207). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
ness heuristic. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, University Press.
489–509. Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2009). Persuasion: Insights from the
Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, self-validation hypothesis. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In R. S. experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press.
Wyer, Jr., & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2012). The history of attitudes and
(2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 1–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. persuasion research. In A. Kruglanski & W. Stroebe (Eds.),
Barsalou L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Handbook of the history of social psychology. New York:
Psychology, 59, 617–645. Psychology Press.
Bassili, J. N., & Fletcher, J. F. (1991). Response-time mea- Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., Valle, C., Rucker, D. D., & Becerra,
surement in survey research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55, A. (2007). The effects of message recipients’ power before
331–346. and after persuasion: A self-validation analysis. Journal of
Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1040–1053.
of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74, Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & Wagner, B. (2009). Body posture effects
183–200. on self-evaluation: A self-validation approach. European
Biernat, M., Manis, M., & Nelson, T. E. (1991). Stereotypes Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 1053–1064.
and standards of judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Byrne, D., & Clore, G. L. (1967). Effectance arousal and attrac-
Psychology, 60, 485–499. tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 1–18.
Biernat, M., Vescio, T. K., & Manis, M. (1998). Judging and Byrne, D., Ervin, C. R., & Lamberth, J. (1970). Continuity
behaving toward members of stereotyped groups: A shifting between the experimental study of attraction and “real life”
standards perspective. In C. Sedikides, J. Schopler, & C. A. computer dating. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Insko (Eds.), Intergroup cognition and intergroup behavior 16, 157–165.
(pp. 151–175). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Berntson, G. G. (1993).
Bizer, G. Y., Tormala, Z. L., Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E. (2006). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes II: Arm flexion and
Memory-based versus on-line processing: Implications for extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of
attitude strength. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 5–17.
42, 646–653. Carlston, D. E. (1994). Associated Systems Theory: A system-
Blankenship, K. L., Wegener, D. T., Petty, R. E., Detweiler- atic approach to the cognitive representation of persons and
Bedell, B., & Macy, C. L. (2008). Elaboration and conse- events. In R. S. Wyer (Ed.) Advances in Social Cognition:
quences of anchored estimates: An attitudinal perspective Vol. 7. Associated Systems Theory (pp. 1–78). Hillsdale, NJ:
on numerical anchoring. Journal of Experimental Social Erlbaum.
Psychology, 44, 1465–1476. Carlston, D. E., & Skowronski, J. J. (1994). Savings in the
Bodenhausen, G. V. (1990). Stereotypes as judgmental heu- relearning of trait information as evidence for spontane-
ristics: Evidence of circadian variations in discrimination. ous inference generation. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychological Science, 1, 319–322. Psychology, 66, 840–856.
Bodenhausen, G. V., Sheppard, L. A., & Kramer, G. P. (1994). Carlston, D. E., & Smith, E. R. (1996). Principles of mental
Negative affect and social judgment: The differential impact representation. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.),
of anger and sadness. European Journal of Social Psychology, Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 194–210).
24, 45–62. New York: Guilford Press.
Bornstein, R. F. (1989). Exposure and affect: Overview and Chaiken, S. (1978). The use of source versus message cues in persua-
meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987. Psychological Bulletin, sion: An information processing analysis. Unpublished doctoral
106, 265–289. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA.
Bornstein, R. F., & D’Agostino, P. R. (1992). Stimulus recogni- Chaiken, S. (1980). Heuristic versus systematic information pro-
tion and the mere exposure effect. Journal of Personality and cessing in the use of source versus message cues in persuasion.
Social Psychology, 63, 545–552. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752–766.

136 At titudes and Social Cognition as Social Psychological Siblings

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 136 4/9/2013 9:55:48 AM


Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In role of emotion-induced expectancies. Journal of Personality
M. P. Zanna, J. M. Olson, & C. P. Herman (Ed.), Social influ- and Social Psychology, 86, 43–56.
ence: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 5, pp. 3–39). Hillsdale, Deutsch, R., & Strack, F. (2006). Duality-models in social psy-
NJ: Erlbaum. chology: From opposing processes to interacting systems.
Chaiken, S., Liberman, A., & Eagly, A. H. (1989). Heuristic and Psychological Inquiry, 17, 166–172.
systematic information processing within and beyond the Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic
persuasion context. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social
Unintended thought (pp. 212–252). New York: Guilford. Psychology, 56, 5–18.
Chaiken, S., & Maheswaran, D. (1994). Heuristic processing can Devine, P. G., Sedikides, C., & Fuhrman, R. W. (1989). Goals
bias systematic processing: Effects of source credibility, argu- in social information processing: The case of anticipated
ment ambiguity, and task importance on attitude judgment. interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 460–473. 680–690.
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.) (1999). Dual process theories in Doob, L. W. (1948). Public opinion and propaganda. New York:
social psychology. New York: Guilford Press. Holt.
Chien, Y. W., Wegener, D. T., Hsiao, C. C., & Petty, R. E. (2010). Eagly, A. H. (1974). Comprehensibility of persuasive arguments
Dimensional range overlap and context effects in consumer as a determinant of opinion change. Journal of Personality and
judgments. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 530–542. Social Psychology, 29, 758–773.
Clark, J. K., & Wegener, D. T. (2008). Unpacking outcome Erb, H. P., Kruglanski, A. W., Chun, W. Y., Pierro, A., Mannetti,
dependency: Differentiating effects of dependency and L., & Spiegel, S. (2003). Searching for commonalities in
outcome desirability on the processing of goal-relevant human judgment: The parametric unimodel and its dual mode
information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, alternatives. European Review of Social Psychology, 14, 1–49.
586–599. Fazio, R. H. (1986). How do attitudes guide behavior? In R. M.
Clark, J. K., & Wegener, D. T. (2013). Message position, Sorrentino, & E. T. Higgins (Ed.), Handbook of motivation
information processing, and persuasion: The Discrepancy and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (pp. 204–243).
Motives Model. In P. Devine & A. Plant (Eds.), Advances in New York: Guilford Press.
Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 47, pp. 189–231). San Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide
Diego, CA: Academic Press. behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. In
Clark, J. K., Wegener, D. T., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2009). M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
Discovering that the shoe fits: The self-validating role of ste- (Vol. 23, pp. 75–109). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
reotypes. Psychological Science, 20, 846–852. Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitudes as object-evaluation associations:
Claypool, H. M., Mackie, D. M., & Garcia-Marques, T. (in Determinants, consequences, and correlates of attitude
press). Fluency and attitudes: Effects on evaluation and pro- accessibility. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude
cessing. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. strength: Antecedents and consequences (pp. 247–282).
Conrey, F. R., Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Hugenberg, K., Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
& Groom, C. J. (2005). Separating multiple processes in Fazio, R. H., Chen, J., McDonel, E. C., & Sherman, S. J.
implicit social cognition: The Quad Model of implicit task (1982). Attitude accessibility, attitude-behavior consistency,
performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, and the strength of the object-evaluation association. Journal
469–487. of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 339–357.
Cook, T. D., & Flay, B. R. (1978). The persistence of experi- Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J.
mentally induced attitude change. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 1–57). measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of
New York: Academic Press. Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1013–1027.
Costermans, J., Lories, G., & Ansay, C. (1992). Confidence level Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social
and feeling of knowing in question answering: The weight cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of
of inferential processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Psychology, 54, 297–327.
Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 18, 142–150. Fazio, R. H., Powell, M. C., & Herr, P. M. (1983). Toward a
Craik, F. I., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A process model of the attitude-behavior relation: Accessing
framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning one’s attitude upon mere observation of the attitude object.
and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671–684. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 723–735.
Craik, F. I., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R.
the retention of words in episodic memory. Journal of (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 268–294. Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229–238.
Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of emotions in man and ani- Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1978). On the predictive validity
mals. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. (Original of attitudes: The roles of direct experience and confidence.
work published 1872.) Journal of Personality, 46, 228–243.
Davidson, A. R., Yantis, S., Norwood, M., & Montano, D. E. Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1981). Direct experience and atti-
(1985). Amount of information about the attitude object tude behavior consistency. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in
and attitude-behavior consistency. Journal of Personality and experimental social psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 161–202). New
Social Psychology, 49, 1184–1198. York: Academic Press.
Deese, J. (1965). The structure of associations in language and Fazio, R. H., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1977). Dissonance
thought. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press. and self-perception: An integrative view of each theory’s
DeSteno, D., Petty, R. E., Rucker, D. D., Wegener, D. T., & proper domain of application. Journal of Experimental Social
Braverman, J. (2004). Discrete emotions and persuasion: The Psychology, 13, 464–479.

Wegener, Pet t y 137

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 137 4/9/2013 9:55:48 AM


Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evanston, Briñol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit mea-
IL: Row, Peterson. sures (pp. 65–82). New York: Psychology Press.
Festinger, L. (1964). Conflict, decision, and dissonance. Stanford, Greenwald, A. G., Spangenberg, E. R., Pratkanis, A. R., &
CA: Stanford University Press. Eskenazi, J. (1991). Double-blind tests of subliminal self-
Festinger, L., & Maccoby, N. (1964). On resistance to persuasive help audiotapes. Psychological Science, 2, 119–122.
communications. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Guttman, L. (1944). A basis for scaling qualitative data. American
68, 359–366. Sociological Review, 9, 139–150.
Fishbein, M. (1963). An investigation of the relationship between Haddock, G., Rothman, A. J., Reber, R., & Schwarz, N. (1999).
beliefs about an object and the attitude toward that object. Forming judgments of attitude certainty, intensity, and
Human Relations, 16, 233–240. importance: The role of subjective experiences. Personality
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1974). Attitudes toward objects as pre- and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 771–782.
dictors of single and multiple behavioral criteria, Psychological Haddock, G., Rothman, A. J., & Schwarz, N. (1996). Are (some)
Review, 81, 59–74. reports of attitude strength context dependent? Canadian
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and Journal of Behavioral Science, 24, 313–317.
behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Reading, Hammond, K. R. (1948). Measuring attitudes by error choice:
MA: Addison-Wesley. An indirect method. Journal of Abnormal and Social
Fiske, S. T., Lin, M., & Neuberg, S. L. (1999). The continuum Psychology, 43, 38–48.
model: Ten years later. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Hastie, R. (1981). Schematic principles in human memory. In E.
Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 231–245). New T. Higgins, C. P. Herman, & M. P. Zanna (Eds.), Social cog-
York: Guilford Press. nition: The Ontario symposium (Vol. 1, pp. 39–88). Hillsdale,
Fiske, S. T., & Neuberg, S. L. (1990). A continuum of impression NJ: Erlbaum.
formation, from category-based to individuating processes: Hastie, R., & Park, B. (1986). The relationship between
Influences of information and motivation on attention and inter- memory and judgment depends on whether the judgment
pretation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social task is memory-based or on-line. Psychological Review, 93,
psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 1–74). New York: Academic Press. 258–268.
Förster, J., Liberman, N., & Kuschel, S. (2008). The effect of Haugtvedt, C. P., & Petty, R. E. (1992). Personality and persua-
global versus local processing styles on assimilation versus sion: Need for cognition moderates the persistence and resis-
contrast in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social tance of attitude changes. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 94, 579–99. Psychology, 63, 308–319.
Freedman, J. L. (1965). Long-term behavioral effects of cogni- Haugtvedt, C. P., & Wegener, D. T. (1994). Message order effects
tive dissonance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 1, in persuasion: An attitude strength perspective. Journal of
145–155. Consumer Research, 21, 205–218.
Galton, F. (1884). Measurement of character. Fortnightly Review, Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. New
42, 179–185. York: Wiley.
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and Helson, H. (1964). Adaptation-level theory: An experimental and
propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review systematic approach to behavior. New York: Harper & Row.
of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, Hendrick, C., & Seyfried, B. A. (1974). Assessing the validity of
132, 692–731. laboratory-produced attitude change. Journal of Personality
Grayson, C. E., & Schwarz, N. (1999). Beliefs influence infor- and Social Psychology, 29, 865–870.
mation processing strategies: Declarative and experiential Hess, E. H., & Polt, J. M. (1960). Pupil size as related to interest
information in risk assessment. Social Cognition, 17, 1–18. value of visual stimuli. Science, 132, 349–350.
Greenwald, A. G. (1968). Cognitive learning, cognitive response Herr, P. M. (1986). Consequences of priming: Judgment and
to persuasion, and attitude change. In A. G. Greenwald, T. behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51,
C. Brock, & T. M. Ostrom (Ed.), Psychological foundations of 1106–15.
attitudes (pp. 147–170). New York: Academic Press. Herr, P. M., Sherman, S. J., & Fazio, R. H. (1983). On the
Greenwald, A. G. (1975). On the inconclusiveness of “crucial” consequences of priming: Assimilation and contrast effects.
cognitive tests of dissonance versus self-perception theories. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 323–340.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 490–499. Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, appli-
Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication cability, and salience. In. E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski
and revision of personal history. American Psychologist, 35, (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp.
603–618. 133–168). New York: Guilford.
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cog- Higgins, E. T., Rholes, W. S., & Jones, C. R. (1977).
nition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Category accessibility and impression formation. Journal of
Review, 102, 4–27. Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 141–154.
Greenwald, A. G., Brock, T. C., & Ostrom, T. M. (Eds.), (1968). Hirt, E. R., Kardes, F. R., & Markman, K. D. (2004). Activating a
Psychological foundations of attitudes. New York: Academic mental simulation mind-set through generation of alternatives:
Press. Implications for debiasing in related and unrelated domains.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 374–383.
Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Hovland, C. I. (1957). Summary and explications. In
implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social C. I. Hovland (Ed.), The order of presentation in persuasion
Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. (pp. 129–157). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Greenwald, A. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Attitudinal dissocia- Hovland, C. I., Harvey, O. J., & Sherif, M. (1957). Assimilation
tion: What does it mean? In R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. and contrast effects in reactions to communications and

138 At titudes and Social Cognition as Social Psychological Siblings

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 138 4/9/2013 9:55:48 AM


attitude change. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The
55, 244–252. embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. New
Hovland, C. I., Janis, I. L., & Kelley, H. H. (1953). York: Basic Books.
Communication and persuasion: Psychological studies of opin- Lasswell, H. D. (1948). The structure and function of commu-
ion change. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. nication in society. In L. Bryson (Ed.), The communication of
Hovland, C. I., & Weiss, W. (1951). The influence of source ideas: Religion and civilization series (pp. 37–51). New York:
credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Opinion Harper & Row.
Quarterly, 15, 635–650. LeBel, E. P. (2010). Attitude accessibility as a moderator of
Insko, C. A. (1964). Primacy versus recency in persuasion as a implicit and explicit self-esteem correspondence. Self and
function of the timing of arguments and measures. Journal of Identity, 9, 195–208.
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 69, 381–391. Likert, R. (1932). A technique for the measurement of attitudes.
Johnston, L., & Hewstone, M. (1992). Cognitive models of ste- Archives of Psychology, 140, 44–53.
reotype change: 3. Subtyping and the perceived typicality of Lingle, J. H., & Ostrom, T. M. (1979). Retrieval selectivity in
disconfirming group members. Journal of Experimental Social memory-based impression judgments. Journal of Personality
Psychology, 28, 360–386. and Social Psychology, 37, 180–194.
Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: Lingle, J. C., & Ostrom, T. M. (1981). Principles of memory
The attribution process in person perception. In L. Berkowitz and cognition in attitude formation. In. R. E Petty, T. M.
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. Ostrom, & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Cognitive responses in persua-
219–266). New York: Academic Press. sion (pp. 399–420). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Jordan, C. H., Whitfield, M., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2007). Intuition Loersch, C., McCaslin, M. J., & Petty, R. E. (2011). Exploring
and the correspondence between implicit and explicit self-esteem. the impact of social judgeability concerns on the interplay
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1067–1079. of associative and deliberative attitude processes. Journal of
Jost, J. T., Kruglanski, A. W., & Nelson, T. O. (1998). Social Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 1029–1032.
meta-cognition: An expansionist review. Personality and Lombardi, W. J., Higgins, E. T., & Bargh, J. A. (1987). The role of
Social Psychology Review, 2, 137–154. consciousness in priming effects on categorization: Assimilation
Jostmann, N. B., Lakens, D., & Schubert, T. W. (2009). Weight versus contrast as a function of awareness of the priming task.
as an embodiment of importance. Psychological Science, 20, Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 13, 411–429.
1169–1174. Lord, C. G., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Attitude representation
Kang, Y. S., & Herr, P. M. (2006). Beauty and the beholder: theory. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
Toward an integrative model of communication source psychology (Vol. 31, pp. 265–343). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 33, 123–130. Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimila-
Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the implicit tion and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on
association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and
81, 774–788. Social Psychology, 37, 2098–2109.
Katz, D. (1960). The functional approach to the study of atti- Mackie, D. M., & Asuncion, A. G. (1990). On-line and memo-
tudes. Public Opinion Quarterly, 24, 163–204. ry-based modification of attitudes: Determinants of message
Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. In recall-attitude change correspondence. Journal of Personality
D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 15, and Social Psychology, 59, 5–16.
pp. 192–238). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press. Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (1994).
Kidder, L. H., & Campbell, D. T. (1970). The indirect testing Stereotypes as energy-saving devices: A peek inside the cogni-
of social attitudes. In G. F. Summers (Ed.), Attitude measure- tive toolbox. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66,
ment (pp. 333–385). Chicago: Rand McNally. 37–47.
Klein, W. M., & Kunda, Z. (1992). Motivated person percep- Mann, L., & Janis, I. L. (1968). A follow-up study on the long-
tion: Constructing justifications for desired beliefs. Journal of term effects of emotional role playing. Journal of Personality
Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 145–168. and Social Psychology, 8, 339–342.
Klinger, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (1994). Preferences need Mandler, G., Nakamura, Y., & Shebo Van Zandt, B. J. (1987).
no inferences? The cognitive basis of unconscious mere expo- Nonspecific effects of exposure on stimuli that cannot be
sure effects. In P. M. Niedenthal & S. Kitayama (Eds.), The recognized. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
hear’s eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention Memory, and Cognition, 13, 646–648.
(pp. 67–85). San Diego: Academic Press. Markman, K. D., & McMullen, M. N. (2003). A reflection and
Kokkinaki, F., & Lunt, P. (1999). The effect of advertising mes- evaluation model of comparative thinking. Personality and
sage involvement on brand attitude accessibility. Journal of Social Psychology Review, 7, 244–267.
Economic Psychology, 20, 41–51. Markus, H., & Zajonc, R. B. (1985). The cognitive perspective
Kruglanski, A. W., & Thompson, E. P. (1999). Persuasion by a in social psychology. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), The
single route: A view from the unimodel. Psychological Inquiry, handbook of social psychology (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 137–230).
10, 83–109. New York: Random House.
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Martin, L. L. (1986). Set/reset: Use and disuse of concepts in
Bulletin, 108, 480–498. impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social
Kunst-Wilson, W. R., & Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Affective discrim- Psychology, 51, 493–504.
ination of stimuli that cannot be recognized. Science, 207, Martin, L. L., Seta, J. J., & Crelia, R. A. (1990). Assimilation
557–558. and contrast as a function of people’s willingness and abil-
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, ity to expend effort in forming an impression. Journal of
IL: University of Chicago Press. Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 27–37.

Wegener, Pet t y 139

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 139 4/9/2013 9:55:48 AM


McGuire, W. J. (1957). Order of presentation as a factor in “con- Ostrom, T. M. (1973). The bogus pipeline: A new ignis fatuus?
ditioning” persuasiveness. In C. I. Hovland (Ed.), The order Psychological Bulletin, 79, 252–259.
of presentation in persuasion (pp. 98–114). New Haven, CT: Ostrom, T. M. (1977). Between-theory and within-theory con-
Yale University Press. flict in explaining context effects in impression formation.
McGuire, W. J. (1960). A syllogistic analysis of cognitive rela- Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 492–503.
tionships. In C. I. Hovland, & M. J. Rosenberg (Ed.), Ostrom, T. M. (1984). The sovereignty of social cognition. In
Attitude organization and change: An analysis of consistency R. S. Wyer, Jr. & T. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition
among attitude components (pp. 65–111). New Haven, CT: (Vol. 1, pp. 1–38). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Yale University Press. Ostrom, T. M., & Brock, T. C. (1969). Cognitive bonding to
McGuire, W. J. (1968). Personality and attitude change: An central values and resistance to a communication advocating
information-processing theory. In A. G. Greenwald, T. C. change in policy orientation. Journal of Experimental Research
Brock, & T. M. Ostrom (Ed.), Psychological foundations of in Personality, 4, 42–50.
attitudes (pp. 171–196). New York: Academic. Ostrom, T. M. & Upshaw, H. S. (1968). Psychological perspec-
Milgram, S. L., Mann, L., & Harter, S. (1965). The lost-letter tive and attitude change. In A. G. Greenwald, T. C. Brock,
technique: A tool of social science research. Public Opinion & T. M. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological foundations of attitudes
Quarterly, 29, 437–438. (pp. 217–242). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Miller, N., & Campbell, D. T. (1959). Recency and primacy in Petty, R. E. (1977). A cognitive response analysis of the temporal
persuasion as a function of the timing of speeches and measure- persistence of attitude changes induced by persuasive commu-
ments. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59, 1–9. nications. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Ohio State
Muller, D., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2005). When mod- University, Columbus, OH.
eration is mediated and mediation is moderated. Journal of Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2006a). A meta-cognitive approach
Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 852–863. to “implicit” and “explicit” evaluations: Comment on
Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judg- Gawronski and Bodenhausen (2006). Psychological Bulletin,
ment: Mechanisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 132, 740–744.
110, 472–489. Petty, R. E. & Briñol, P. (2006b). Understanding social judg-
Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexi- ment: Multiple systems and processes. Psychological Inquiry,
cal memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and 17, 217–223.
limited-capacity attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2010). Attitude structure and change:
General, 106, 225–254. Implications for implicit measures. In B. Gawronski &
Neuberg, S. L., & Fiske, S. T. (1987). Motivational influences B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition:
on impression formation: Outcome dependency, accuracy- Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 335–352). New
driven attention, and individuating processes. Journal of York: Guilford Press.
Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 431–444. Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2012). The Elaboration Likelihood
Nosek, B. A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between Model. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A. Kruglanski, & E. T.
implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol.
Psychology: General, 134, 565–584. 1, pp. 224–245). London, UK: Sage.
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Reducing the influence Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., & DeMarree, K. G. (2007). The meta-
of extrapersonal associations on the implicit association cognitive model (MCM) of attitudes: Implications for atti-
test: Personalizing the IAT. Journal of Personality and Social tude measurement, change, and strength. Social Cognition,
Psychology, 86, 653–667. 25, 657–686.
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2009). Implicit and explicit mea- Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., & Johnson, I. (2012). Implicit ambiva-
sures of attitudes: The perspective of the MODE model. In lence. In B. Gawronski, & F. Strack (Eds.), Cognitive con-
R. E. Petty, R. H. Fazio, & P. Briñol (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights sistency: A fundamental principle in social cognition (pp.
from the new implicit measures (pp. 19–63). New York: 178–201). New York: Guilford Press.
Psychology Press. Petty, R. E., Briñol, P. & Tormala, Z. L. (2002). Thought con-
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The mea- fidence as a determinant of persuasion: The self-validation
surement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press. hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82,
Osgood, C. E., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1955). The principle of 722–741.
congruity in the prediction of attitude change. Psychological Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., Tormala, Z. L., & Wegener, D.T.
Review, 62, 42–55. (2007). The role of metacognition in social judgment. In
Osterhouse, R. A., & Brock, T. C. (1970). Distraction increases A. W. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology:
yielding to propaganda by inhibiting counterarguing. Journal Handbook of basic principles (2nd ed., pp. 254–284). New
of Personality and Social Psychology, 15, 344–358. York: Guilford.
Ostrom, T. M. (1968). The emergence of attitude theory: 1930– Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1979). Issue involvement can
1950. In A. G. Greenwald, T. C. Brock, & T. M. Ostrom increase or decrease persuasion by enhancing message-rel-
(Eds.), Psychological foundations of attitudes (pp. 1–32). New evant cognitive responses. Journal of Personality and Social
York: Academic Press. Psychology, 37, 1915–1926.
Ostrom, T. M. (1969). The relationship between the affective, Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Attitudes and persuasion:
behavioral, and cognitive components of attitude. Journal of Classic and contemporary approaches. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C.
Experimental Social Psychology, 5, 12–30. Brown.
Ostrom, T. M., (1970). Perspective as a determinant of atti- Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986a). Communication and per-
tude change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 6, suasion: Central and peripheral routes to persuasion. New York:
280–292. Springer-Verlag.

140 At titudes and Social Cognition as Social Psychological Siblings

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 140 4/9/2013 9:55:48 AM


Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986b). The Elaboration Rennier, G. A. (1988). The strength of the object-evaluation asso-
Likelihood Model of persuasion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), ciation, the attitude-behavior relationship, and the Elaboration
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 19, pp. 123– Likelihood Model of persuasion. Unpublished doctoral disser-
205). New York: Academic Press. tation, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO.
Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Goldman, R. (1981). Personal Robbins, P., & Aydede, M., (2009) (Eds.). The Cambridge
involvement as a determinant of argument-based persuasion. handbook of situated cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 847–855. University Press.
Petty, R. E., Haugtvedt, C. P., & Smith, S. M. (1995). Elaboration Roediger, H. L. (2003). Reconsidering implicit memory. In J. S.
as a determinant of attitude strength: Creating attitudes that Bowers & C. J. Marsolek (Eds.),, Rethinking implicit memory
are persistent, resistant, and predictive of behavior. In R. E. (pp. 3–18). New York: Oxford University Press.
Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength: Antecedents Rosenberg, M. J., & Abelson, R. P. (1960). An analysis of cogni-
and consequences (pp. 93–130). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. tive balancing. In C. I. Hovland, & M. J. Rosenberg (Eds.),
Petty, R. E., & Krosnick, J. A. (Eds.) (1995). Attitude strength: Attitude organization and change: An analysis of consistency
Antecedents and consequences. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. among components (pp. 112–163). New Haven, CT: Yale
Petty, R. E., Ostrom, T. M., & Brock, T. D. (Eds.) (1981). University Press.
Cognitive responses in persuasion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Rosenberg, M. J., & Hovland, C. I. (1960). Cognitive, affective,
Petty, R. E., Schumann, D. W., Richman, S. A., & Strathman, A. and behavioral components of attitudes. In M. Rosenberg,
J. (1993). Positive mood and persuasion: Different roles for C. Hovland, W. McGuire, R. Abelson, & J. Brehm (Eds.),
affect under high- and low-elaboration conditions. Journal of Attitude organization and change (pp. 1–14). New Haven,
Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 5–20. CT: Yale University Press.
Petty, R. E., Tormala, Z., Hawkins, C., & Wegener, D. T. Roskos-Ewoldsen, D. R., & Fazio, R. H. (1992). On the ori-
(2001). Motivation to think and order effects in persua- enting value of attitudes: Attitude accessibility as a determi-
sion: The moderating role of chunking. Personality and Social nant of an object’s attraction of visual attention. Journal of
Psychology Bulletin, 27, 332–344. Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 198–211.
Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1993). Flexible correction pro- Rumelhart, D. E., Lindsay, P. H., & Norman, D. A. (1972). A
cesses in social judgment: Correcting for context-induced process model for long-term memory. In E. Tulving & W.
contrast. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 29, Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 197–246).
137–165. New York: Academic Press.
Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1998). Attitude change: Multiple Rydell, R. J., & McConnell, A. R. (2006). Understanding implicit
roles for persuasion variables. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, & G. and explicit attitude change: A systems of reasoning analysis.
Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 995–1008.
pp. 323–390). New York: McGraw-Hill. Schneider, W., & R. M. Shiffrin. (1977). Controlled and auto-
Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1999). The Elaboration matic human information processing: 1. Detection, search,
Likelihood Model: Current status and controversies. In S. and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66.
Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psy- Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1992). Constructing reality and its
chology (pp. 41–72). New York: Guilford Press. alternatives: An inclusion/exclusion model of assimilation
Petty, R. E., Wegener, D. T., & White, P. (1998). Flexible cor- and contrast effects in social judgment. In L. L. Martin & A.
rection processes in social judgment: Implications for persua- Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments (pp. 217–
sion. Social Cognition, 16, 93–113. 245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Petty, R. E., Wells, G. L., & Brock, T. C. (1976). Distraction Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (2007). Mental construal processes:
can enhance or reduce yielding to propaganda: Thought dis- The inclusion/exclusion model. In D. A. Stapel & J. M. Suls
ruption versus effort justification. Journal of Personality and (Eds.), Assimilation and contrast in social psychology (pp. 119–
Social Psychology, 34, 874–884. 141). New York: Psychology Press.
Petty, R. E., Wells, G. L., Heesacker, M., Brock, T. C., & Schwarz, N., Bless, H., Strack, F., Klumpp, G., Rittenauer-
Cacioppo, J. T. (1983). The effects of recipient posture on Schatka, H., & Simons, A. (1991). Ease of retrieval as infor-
persuasion: A cognitive response analysis. Personality and mation: Another look at the availability heuristic. Journal of
Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 209–222. Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 195–202.
Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sleeth-Keppler, Scott, C. L., Harris, R. J., & Rothe, A. R. (2001). Embodied
D. (2004). Relevance override: On the reduced impact of cognition through improvisation improves memory for a
“cues” under high-motivation conditions of persuasion stud- dramatic monologue. Discourse Processes, 31, 293–305.
ies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 251–264. Seibt, B., Neumann, R., Nussinson, R., & Strack, F. (2008).
Priester, J. R., Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1996). The influ- Movement direction or change in distance? Self- and object-
ence of motor processes on attitudes toward novel versus related approach-avoidance motions. Journal of Experimental
familiar semantic stimuli. Personality and Social Psychology Social Psychology, 44, 713–720.
Bulletin, 22, 442–447. Semin, G. R., & Smith, E. R. (Eds.) (2008). Embodiment ground-
Priester, J. R., & Petty, R. E. (2003). The influence of spokes- ing: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches.
person trustworthiness on message elaboration, attitude Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
strength, and advertising effectiveness. Journal of Consumer Sherif, C. W., Sherif, M., & Nebergall, R. E. (1965). Attitude and
Psychology, 13, 408–421. attitude change: The social judgment-involvement approach.
Regan, D. T., & Fazio, R. H. (1977). On the consistency Philadelphia: Saunders.
between attitudes and behavior: Look to the method of atti- Sherif, M. (1977). Crisis in social psychology: Some remarks
tude formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, towards breaking through the crisis. Personality and Social
28–45. Psychology Bulletin, 3, 368–382.

Wegener, Pet t y 141

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 141 4/9/2013 9:55:49 AM


Sherif, M., & Hovland, C. I. (1961). Social judgment: Assimilation perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12,
and contrast effects in communication and attitude change. New 1599–1612.
Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Tormala, Z., Petty, R. E., & Briñol, P. (2002). Ease of retrieval
Sherif, M., & Sherif, C. W. (1967). Attitude as the individual’s effects in persuasion: The roles of elaboration and thought
own categories: The social judgment-involvement approach confidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28,
to attitude and attitude change. In C. W. Sherif & M. Sherif 1700–1712.
(Eds.), Attitude, ego-involvement, and change (pp. 105–139). Tourangeau, R., & Rasinski, K. A. (1988). Cognitive pro-
New York: Wiley. cesses underlying context effects in attitude measurement.
Sherif, M., Taub, D., & Hovland, C. I. (1958). Assimilation and Psychological Bulletin, 103, 299–314.
contrast effects of anchoring stimuli on judgments. Journal of Tourangeau, R., Rasinski, K. A., Bradburn, N., & D’Andrade,
Experimental Psychology, 55, 150–155. R. (1989). Belief accessibility and context effects in attitude
Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and auto- measurement. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25,
matic human information processing: II. Perceptual learn- 401–421.
ing, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Uleman, J. S. (1999). Spontaneous versus intentional inferences
Review, 84, 127–190. in impression formation. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.),
Simon, L., Greenberg, J., & Brehm, J. (1995). Trivialization: Dual-process theories in social psychology (pp. 141–160). New
The forgotten mode of dissonance reduction. Journal of York: Guilford.
Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 247–260. Wänke, M., & Bless, H. (2000). The effects of subjective ease
Skowronski, J. J, Carlston, D. E., Mae, L., & Crawford, M. T. of retrieval on attitudinal judgments: The moderating role
(1998). Spontaneous trait transference: Communicators take of processing motivation. In H. Bless & J. P. Forgas (Eds.),
on the qualities they describe in others. Journal of Personality The message within: The role of subjective experience in social
and Social Psychology, 74, 837–848. cognition and behavior (pp. 143–161). Philadelphia: Taylor
Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in & Francis.
social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and Wänke, M., Bless, H., & Biller, B. (1996). Subjective experi-
links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social ence versus content of information in the construction of
Psychology Review, 4, 108–131. attitude judgments. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Smith, E. R., Fazio, R. H., & Cejka, M. A. (1996). Accessible 22, 1105–1113.
attitudes influence categorization of multiply categoriz- Wänke, M., Schwarz, N., & Bless, H. (1995). The availability
able objects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, heuristic revisited: Experienced ease of retrieval in mundane
888–898. frequency estimates. Acta Psychologica, 89, 83–90.
Smith, M. B., Bruner, J. S., & White, R. W. (1956). Opinions Watts, W. A., & McGuire, W. J. (1964). Persistence of induced
and personality. New York: Wiley. opinion change and retention of the inducing message contents.
Solarz, A. K. (1960). Latency of instrumental responses as a Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 233–241.
function of compatibility with the meaning eliciting verbal Weber, R., & Crocker, J. (1983). Cognitive processes in the revi-
signs. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 239–245. sion of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social
Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (1979). The role of category acces- Psychology, 45, 961–977.
sibility in the interpretation of information about persons: Wegener, D. T., & Carlston, D. E. (2005). Cognitive pro-
Some determinants and implications. Journal of Personality cesses in attitude formation and change. In D. Albarracin,
and Social Psychology, 37, 1660–1672. B. Johnson, & M. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of attitudes
Staats, A. W., & Staats, C. K. (1958). Attitudes established (pp. 493–542). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
by classical conditioning. Journal of Abnormal and Social Wegener, D. T., Clark, J. K., & Petty, R. E. (2006). Not all
Psychology, 57, 37–40. stereotyping is created equal: Differential consequences of
Steele, C. M., & Ostrom, T. M. (1974). Perspective-mediated thoughtful versus nonthoughtful stereotyping. Journal of
attitude change: When is indirect persuasion more effec- Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 42–59.
tive than direct persuasion? Journal of Personality and Social Wegener, D. T., Downing, J., Krosnick, J. A., & Petty, R. E.
Psychology, 29, 737–741. (1995). Measures and manipulations of strength-related
Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive deter- properties of attitudes: Current practice and future direc-
minants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology tions. In R. E. Petty & J. A. Krosnick (Eds.), Attitude strength:
Review, 8, 220–247. Antecedants and consequences (pp. 455–487). Mahwah, NJ:
Strack, F., Martin, L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facili- Erlbaum.
tating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test Wegener, D. T. & Fabrigar, L. R. (2000). Analysis and design
of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and for nonexperimental data: Addressing causal and noncausal
Social Psychology, 54, 768–777. hypotheses. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook
Thurstone, L. L. (1928). Attitudes can be measured. American of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp.
Journal of Sociology, 33, 529–544. 412–450). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Thurstone, L. L., & Chave, E. J. (1929). The measurement of atti- Wegener, D. T., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Flexible correction pro-
tude. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. cesses in social judgment: The role of naive theories in cor-
Tormala, Z. L., Falces, C., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2007). Ease rections for perceived bias. Journal of Personality and Social
of retrieval effects in social judgment: The role of unrequested Psychology, 68, 36–51.
cognitions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, Wegener, D. T., & Petty, R. E. (1997). The flexible correction
143–157. model: The role of naive theories of bias in bias correction. In
Tormala, Z. L., & Petty, R. E. (2001). On-line versus memory M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
based processing: The role of “need to evaluate” in person (Vol. 29, pp. 141–208). New York: Academic Press.

142 At titudes and Social Cognition as Social Psychological Siblings

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 142 4/9/2013 9:55:49 AM


Wegener, D. T., Petty, R. E., & Klein, D. J. (1994). Effects of Winter, L., & Uleman, J. S. (1984). When are social judg-
mood on high elaboration attitude change: The mediating ments made? Evidence for the spontaneousness of trait
role of likelihood judgments. European Journal of Social inferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47,
Psychology, 24, 25–43. 237–252.
Wegener, D. T., Petty, R. E., Smoak, N. D., & Fabrigar, L. R. Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1997). Evidence for
(2004). Multiple routes to resisting attitude change. In E. S. racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationship with
Knowles & J. A. Linn (Eds.), Resistance and persuasion (pp. questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social
13–38). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Psychology, 72, 262–274.
Wells, G. L., & Petty, R. E. (1980). The effects of overt head Wood, W. (1982). Retrieval of attitude-relevant information
movements on persuasion: Compatibility and incompatibility from memory: Effects on susceptibility to persuasion and
of responses. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 1, 219–230. on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social
Wheeler, S. C., DeMarree, K. G., & Petty, R. E. (2007). Psychology, 42, 798–810.
Understanding the role of the self in prime-to-behavior Wyer, R. S., Jr. (1974). Cognitive organization and change: An
effects: The active self account. Personality and Social information-processing approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Psychology Review, 11, 234–261. Yzerbyt, V. Y., Lories, G., & Dardenne, B. (1998). Metacognition:
Whitfield, M., & Jordan, C. H. (2009). Mutual influences of Cognitive and social dimensions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
explicit and implicit attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Zajonc, R. B. (1968). Attitudinal effects of mere exposure.
Psychology, 45, 748–759. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monograph
Wicker, A. W. (1969). Attitudes versus actions: The Relationship Supplements, 9, 1–27.
of verbal and overt behavioral responses to attitude objects. Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1974). Dissonance and the pill: An
Journal of Social Issues, 25, 41–78. attribution approach to studying the arousal properties of
Wicker, A. W. (1971). An examination of the “other-variables” dissonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29,
explanation of attitude-behavior inconsistency. Journal of 703–709.
Personality and Social Psychology, 19, 18–30. Zanna, M. P., Fazio, R. H., & Ross, M. (1994). The persistence
Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Experiencing physical of persuasion. In R. C. Schank & E. Langer (Eds.), Beliefs,
warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322(5901), reasoning, and decision making: Psychologic in honor of Bob
606–607. Abelson (pp. 347–362). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Wegener, Pet t y 143

07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 143 4/9/2013 9:55:49 AM


07_Carlston_Ch07.indd 144 4/9/2013 9:55:49 AM
View publication stats

You might also like