You are on page 1of 13

EDITOR'S COMMENTS: CONSTRUCT CLARITY IN THEORIES OF MANAGEMENT AND

ORGANIZATION
Author(s): Roy Suddaby
Source: The Academy of Management Review , July 2010, Vol. 35, No. 3 (July 2010), pp.
346-357
Published by: Academy of Management

Stable URL: https://www.jstor.org/stable/25682418

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide
range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and
facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at
https://about.jstor.org/terms

Academy of Management is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
The Academy of Management Review

This content downloaded from


146.50.98.29 on Mon, 26 Oct 2020 10:18:06 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
? Academy of Management Review
2010. Vol. 35, No. 3. 346-357.

EDITOR'S COMMENTS:
CONSTRUCT CLARITY IN THEORIES OF
MANAGEMENT AND ORGANIZATION

One of the more commonly cited reasons for narrowly constituted on empirical questions of
rejecting a manuscript at AMR is that reviewers operationalization and measurement.
feel the submission lacks "construct clarity." Yet Nor is my intent to discuss the broader ques
reviewers (and editors) often find it difficult to tion of what constitutes "good" theory. This topic
has already received substantial prior, more
articulate precisely what construct clarity is. In
deed, in contrast to other social sciences, such skilled attention (i.e., Bacharach, 1989; Sutton &
as sociology and psychology, where the nature Staw, 1995; Weick, 1989). While recognizing that
and role of constructs are subjects of consider strong, clear constructs contribute to good the
able debate, the field of management seems ory, my goal here is more modest. I simply in
unusually silent on the subject. The absence of tend to focus the discussion on why we need
an open discussion about theoretical constructs clear constructs in developing theories of man
is somewhat surprising given their widespread agement and how best to accomplish this.
use in and undeniable importance to manage This essay proceeds in four parts. In the first I
discuss what constitutes a theoretical construct
ment theory.
The purpose of this essay, thus, is twofold. My and how to best create clarity in our constructs.
first objective is pragmatic. I hope to offer some Second, I outline why we need clear constructs
degree of clarification about how the issue of in management theory. In the third part I outline
how the term consfrizcf means different things to
construct clarity is dealt with at AMR. I do so by
different kinds of researchers, and I explore how
offering a review and synthesis of prior writing
standards of construct clarity vary across epis
on the subject in management journals and in
temological and methodological divisions. Fi
journals from related social science disciplines.
nally, I present a more normative argument
Ideally, this will assist authors of prospective
about the need for more open dialogue about the
AMR manuscripts to improve the clarity of their
role of constructs in our discipline.
theoretical constructs. My second objective is
less pragmatic but, arguably, more important. I
hope to open a dialogue within the AMR com
munity about the role and use of constructs in WHAT ARE CONSTRUCTS ... AND WHAT IS
developing theories. CONSTRUCT CLARITY?
Before doing this, however, I should be clear Constructs are conceptual abstractions of
about the scope of this essay. The intent is not to phenomena that cannot be directly observed
discuss issues of construct validity. This is a (MacCorquodale & Meehl, 1948). Kerlinger de
subsidiary topic of high importance that has fines a construct as a concept that has "been
received and continues to receive considerable deliberately and consciously invented or
attention (i.e., Bagozzi & Edwards, 1998; Cook & adopted for a special scientific purpose" (1973:
Campbell, 1979; Schwab, 1980). Questions of 29). Constructs are not reducible to specific ob
construct clarity and validity are quite distinct servations but, rather, are abstract statements of
(Bacharach, 1989). Issues of construct validity, categories of observations (Priem & Butler, 2001).
which flows from the ability to crisply and pre Clear constructs are simply robust categories
cisely describe theoretical constructs, are more that distill phenomena into sharp distinctions
that are comprehensible to a community of
researchers?that is, animal, mineral, or vege
table; gas, liquid, or solid.
I thank Bob Gephart, Bob Hinings, Dave Whetten, and the
editor and associate editors of the Academy of Management
Constructs are the foundation of theory. Bach
Review for their helpful and stimulating comments on ear arach defines theory as a "system of constructs
lier versions of this essay. ... in which the constructs are related to each
346
Copyright of the Academy of Management, all rights reserved. Contents may not be copied, emailed, posted to a listserv, or otherwise transmitted without the copyright
holder's express written permission. Users may print, download, or email articles for individual use only.
This content downloaded from
146.50.98.29 on Mon, 26 Oct 2020 10:18:06 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
2010 Editor's Comments 347

other by propositions" (1989: 498). Just as con Perhaps the most common definitional issue
structs are the building blocks of strong theory, in manuscripts is that authors simply fail to
clear and accurate terms are the fundament of define their constructs. Authors often use terms
strong constructs. As Sutton and Staw (1995) re described as constructs and assume that the
mind us, constructs are not a substitute for the reader understands the intended meaning. This
ory. They are, however, essential to the process is clearly problematic since any word has both a
of building strong theory. Constructs, therefore, denotative and connotative meaning. In The
are a necessary but insufficient condition for Structure of Complex Words, famous literary
theory. critic William Empson (1995/1951) demonstrates
The essence of construct clarity comprises that even individual words like "knowledge"
four basic elements. First, definitions are impor and "honest" contain a complex "inner gram
tant. Construct clarity involves the skillful use of mar" that can generate multiple and sometimes
language to persuasively create precise and contradictory interpretations of the same word.
parsimonious categorical distinctions between Offering definitions of key terms and constructs,
concepts. Second, construct clarity requires the thus, is a bare minimal standard of construct
author to delineate the scope conditions or con clarity.
textual circumstances under which a construct A good definition should accomplish several
will or will not apply. Third, not only must the tasks. First, the definition should effectively
theorist offer clear conceptual distinctions, but capture the essential properties and character
he or she must also show their semantic rela istics of the concept or phenomenon under con
tionship to other related constructs. Finally, the sideration.
theorist must demonstrate a degree of coher Second, a good definition should avoid tautol
ence or logical consistency of the construct in ogy or circularity. This occurs when a theorist
relation to the overall theoretical argument he uses elements of the term being defined in the
or she is trying to make. definition or incorporates antecedent or out
Reviewers are quick to reject a manuscript come variables as part of his or her definition.
where the core constructs are weakly defined, where Thus, defining a "transformational leader" as a
contextual conditions are not specified, or where "leader who transforms organizations" is an
their connection to other constructs and to the empty definition because it uses the construct in
overall theory is not clear. Unfortunately, the the definition. Similarly, defining "cognitive
typical rejection letter offers little space to con ability" as "a capability that enables people to
textualize or elaborate these conditions. How do learn more effectively in contexts that are dy
these constituent elements contribute to clear namic or complex" creates confusion because it
construct development? How do they contribute incorporates, as part of the definition, anteced
to theory? More significantly, what can I, as an ent variables (i.e., complex and dynamic con
author, do to ensure that the constructs used in texts) that are likely causally related to the con
my theoretical argument meet the requisite struct being defined.
standard for clarity and precision? My intent is Third, a good definition should be parsimoni
to address these questions in the balance of this ous. That is, it should try to capture as concisely
section. I begin by elaborating each of the four as possible the essential characteristics of a
subcomponents of construct clarity described phenomenon or concept. The challenge here is
above under the following four headings; defi twofold. On the one hand, the definition should
nitions, scope conditions, relations between focus the meaning of the term as narrowly as
constructs and coherence. possible. On the other hand, there is a danger of
overshooting the mark?offering a construct
Definitions definition that is so narrow it lacks relevance
and cannot be generalized.
Theory construction relies on the ability of These three characteristics of a good defini
theorists to accurately abstract empirical phe tion are intended to help fix the meaning of a
nomena into robust conceptual generalizations. theoretical term. Meanings, however, are notori
Accomplishing this requires an unusual skill in ously difficult to specify, for a variety of reasons.
translating abstract concepts into crisply de One reason is that the meanings of words are
fined theoretical constructs. never fixed or permanent. When different re

This content downloaded from


146.50.98.29 on Mon, 26 Oct 2020 10:18:06 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
348 Academy of Management Review July

searchers apply an existing construct to a new inition?which they then recalibrate by trim
empirical context, they often change the mean ming away the surplus meaning of prior uses
ing of the term, however slightly. Over time and and introducing their own three salient at
over multiple empirical applications, the defini tributes (power, legitimacy, and urgency). In this
tion of a construct tends to drift?that is, it ac way the authors demonstrate their command of
quires substantial "surplus meaning" (MacCor the relevant literature by illustrating the prior
quodale & Meehl, 1948) or meaning beyond the accumulation of surplus meaning in a term and
parameters of its original intended definition. then impose some order on the construct by in
Therefore, it is incumbent on the theorist to first troducing a new, refined definition of the term.
demonstrate the prior uses of the term and then The most common error in developing con
to illustrate, as exhaustively as possible, prior structs is making them too general. There is,
variation in how the term has been used. however, considerable danger in extending this
Similarly, new constructs are often given logic too far. That is, on occasion constructs can
names used in common speech?for example, be presented too narrowly. Recall that a key
"organizational performance." In this case the function of constructs is to create robust catego
term performance has acquired substantial sur rizations of phenomena. If the categories are
plus meaning as a result of its use in literature expressed too narrowly, the theoretical rele
and advertising, as well as its everyday use. The vance of a construct will be compromised (Ast
term has a depth of connotation, some of which ley, 1985). The creative capacity of theoretical
works well for the intended theory and some of constructs rests on the tension between defini
which does not. As a result, researchers working tional accuracy and ideational scope. Effective
at both the organizational (Hansen & Wernerfeldt, constructs create broad categories and, thus,
1989) and individual (Rogers & Wright, 1998) levels should not be reducible to narrow empirical ob
of analysis have expressed concern about the dif servations. Some degree of linguistic ambiguity
ficulty of constraining the definition of perfor is therefore a useful component of any theoreti
mance as a construct. The construct of "family cal construct (Astley & Zammuto, 1992). The chal
business" also suffers from definitional clarity lenge is to create constructs that are sufficiently
as a result of its surplus meaning acquired from narrow enough to strip away unintended conno
everyday use. Sharma, Chrisman, and Chua tations and surplus meaning but are conceptu
(1996) found thirty-four different definitional ally broad enough to capture the underlying es
uses of the term in management literature. sence of the phenomenon.
In any case, it is critically important for the
theorist to attempt to strip away the extraneous
Scope Conditions
meaning that has become attached to a con
struct. He or she can accomplish this by offering In contrast to the physical sciences, few con
a contextually specific and clear definition of structs in organization theory have universal
the term. But achieving this is no simple task, application. Rather, organizational constructs
particularly where, as in most instances of the tend to be highly sensitive to and contingent on
ory development, there is no clear agreement on contextual conditions. So, for example, con
the substantive definitional content of a structs developed from research on large, pub
construct. licly traded corporations may have little rele
There are, however, excellent illustrative ex vance for closely held corporations (Shane &
amples of how this can be done. Consider, for Venkataraman, 2000) or family businesses. Sim
example, how Mitchell, Agle, and Wood (1997) ilarly, organizational constructs are highly cul
develop their definition of the term stakeholder turally sensitive. Constructs formulated by
in their groundbreaking AMR article. They begin studying North American corporations may not
by acknowledging the "maddening variety" of exhibit the same characteristics in Asian orga
uses of the term. They then catalog these defi nizations (Gibson & Zellmer-Bruhn, 2001; Shen
nitions across various theories, including kar & von Glinow, 1994; White, 2002).
agency, behavioral, ecological, institutional, re There is also a noted tendency within man
source dependence, and transaction cost theo agement scholarship for researchers to "borrow"
ries of the firm. The authors go on to offer a new concepts from other disciplines, such as psy
definition of the term?a purposefully broad def chology or biology. In addition, organizational

This content downloaded from


146.50.98.29 on Mon, 26 Oct 2020 10:18:06 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
2010 Editor's Comments 349

researchers often take constructs developed at level of analysis under which a proposed con
one level of analysis, such as the individual, struct will apply (Rousseau, 1985). As Klein,
and apply them to another level of analysis, Dansereau, and Hall remind us,
such as the group, team, or organization (Floyd, No construct is level free. Every construct is tied
2009). While the practice of borrowing constructs to one or more organizational levels or entities,
can be beneficial, it is often done unreflectively, that is, individuals, dyads, groups, organizations,
without considering how the borrowed construct industries, markets, and so on. To examine orga
might vary as a result of the distinctive nature of nizational phenomena is thus to encounter levels
issues. Levels issues create particular problems
organizations (Whetten, Felin, & King, 2009). In when the level of theory, the level of measure
the process of borrowing constructs, researchers ment, and/or the level of statistical analysis are
often assume universality and neglect to clarify incongruent (1994: 198).
whether the essential characteristics of a con
struct that may have been present in the origi So, for example, employee performance is a con
struct that is highly dependent on the level of
nal context are equally present in the new one.
analysis within the organization where observa
Because organizational constructs lack uni
tions are made. An individual might be perform
versality, it is very important for theorists to
ing extremely well in reference to his or her past
spell out the contextual conditions under which
a proposed construct will or will not adhere (Du
performance (individual level) but below aver
bin, 1969). Failure to specify the "boundary lim
age relative to group performance and merely
its" or "scope conditions" of a construct exposes
average at the organizational level.
one's theoretical argument to almost certain re Organizational constructs are subject to con
straints of time because organizational phenom
jection. When an author claims universal appli
cation of a construct, it is usually very easy for a
ena tend to be temporal, and as a result,
reviewer to identify at least one exception to the
changes in time may affect the expression of
any construct (Avital, 2000; Zaheer, Albert, & Za
hypothetical abstraction. Indeed, as Walker and
heer, 1999). Critics, however, have observed that
Cohen observe, "One can easily find exceptions
to most of the propositions which are advanced management theorists tend to ignore the tempo
as general ... principles" (1985: 288). ral boundaries of phenomena and assume in
Finding a single exception is often fatal to a variance over time in key constructs. George
construct because it implies that any proposi and Jones offer two key examples:
tion associated with the construct is false. Re So, for example, any conceptualization of motiva
viewers may take this position even in cases tion must contain reference to its inherently sub
where there is substantial positive empirical jective and changing nature and definitions
should not be constrained by viewing it through
support for a construct, largely because most the lens of standard time. As another example, it
reviewers have been oversocialized to accept should be recognized that the desire to act oppor
falsification as the basis of scientific truth. An tunistically can be viewed as a state of mind that
easy resolution to this problem, however, is sim can change, often quickly; however opportunism
ply to avoid overgeneralizing the nature of your is often treated as a stable tendency that exists
across people and situations (2000: 667).
constructs by placing scope conditions on
them?that is, carefully outlining the contextual Another example, recounted by Zaheer et al.
conditions under which the constructs will or (1999: 726), is that the antecedents and nature of
will not apply. trust seem to vary over different time scales.
There are three general categories or types of When observed over short time frames, trust ap
scope conditions: space, time, and values (Bach pears to be based on stereotypical features of a
arach, 1989). The first two are relatively straight potential alliance partner. But when measured
forward and easy to address. Constraints of over longer time frames, trust is based on more
space, discussed above, refer to the fact that specific or individualized elements of potential
constructs may apply differently in different partners.
types of organizations, at different levels of or Often, organizational constructs implicitly as
ganizational analysis, under different cultural sume boundary conditions of time, without mak
conditions, or in varying environmental circum ing them explicit. George and Jones (2000: 662)
stances. Perhaps the most common omission in point to research on job satisfaction as an illus
theory manuscripts is a failure to specify the tration of this, where research typically mea

This content downloaded from


146.50.98.29 on Mon, 26 Oct 2020 10:18:06 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
350 Academy of Management Review July

sures job satisfaction at time 1 and then mea should provide answers to what the constructs
sures absenteeism at time 2?usually one year are, how and why they are related, who the
later. The methodology implies certain assump constructs apply to, and when and where they
tions about the temporal scope conditions of job are applicable.
satisfaction as a construct?that isf that job sat
isfaction is stable over that time period but ab
Relationships Between Constructs
senteeism is incremental. Job stress, similarly,
has been viewed as a construct with both incre With apologies to John Donne, no construct is
mental temporal scope conditions?it increases an island. Constructs exist only in referential
incrementally as an employee encounters in relationships, either explicit or implicit, with
creasing levels of stressors?and discontinuous other constructs and with the phenomena they
temporal scope conditions?a specific event in are designed to represent. New constructs are
creases stress temporarily but the stress then rarely created de novo. Rather, they are usually
subsides. Both conceptualizations of the con the result of creative building upon preexisting
struct of stress are accurate, but they operate constructs, which themselves refer to other ex
under different boundary conditions of time. The tant constructs, in an ongoing web of referential
onus is on the researcher to clearly state the relationships. Constructs, thus, are the outcome
temporal scope conditions under which he or of a semantic network of conceptual connections
she assumes the construct to operate. to other prior constructs. Psychologists refer to
Constraints of value are more complex and this as the nomoiogical network (Cronbach &
arguably more difficult to address. Constraints Meehl, 1955) and semioticians as the system of
of value refer to scope conditions of a theoretical signification (Saussure, 2000). While these two
construct that arise as a result of the assump groups of researchers may not share much in
tions or world view of the researcher. So, for terms of epistemology, they both seem to under
example, Pierce, Gardner, Cummings, and Dun stand that theoretical constructs are suspended
ham (1989) note that most of the constructs de in a complex web of references to and relation
veloped by human resources theorists, such as ships with other constructs.
turnover, climate, and citizenship, tend to "priv Part of the task in demonstrating construct
ilege" or adopt the point of view of the employee clarity, thus, is to draw out these relationships
and his or her role within the organization. in a fashion that the reader can understand.
These authors point out the implications of Describing the historical relationships between
these value assumptions and how they might the proposed new construct and the prior histor
limit, theoretically, the depth of each construct. ical constructs on which it was built is a critical
They go on to suggest that researchers might component of the literature review of any theo
correct for their "employee bias" by adopting a retical manuscript. Theorists need to "acknowl
broader set of assumptions that are "anchored edge the stream of logic on which they are draw
in an organizational frame of reference" (Pierce ing and to which they are contributing" (Sutton
et al., 1989: 624). & Staw, 1995: 372). Similarly, theorists also need
Because constructs are subject to conditions of to carefully describe the logical connections be
value, researchers must make their best efforts tween the proposed new construct and other ex
to explicate the hidden assumptions that they tant constructs, a process Bacharach (1989) sug
bring to the theorization of a construct. As orga gests is usually carried out in the form of
nizational theorists, we must adopt an ongoing propositions.
position of critical reflexivity about how our in The key observation here is that the clarity of
dividual point of view, our often taken-for a construct is only partly achieved by the preci
granted assumptions, and our institutional biog sion of its definition. The notion of clarity ex
raphy might introduce bias and distortion into tends beyond this to include clarity in how the
how we conceptualize and abstract reality. theorist describes the complex relationships
In sum, clearly stating the scope conditions of that exist befween constructs. One effective way
constructs contributes directly to building of addressing this is for the theorist to demon
strong theory. Whetten (1989) summarizes this strate the historical lineage of a new construct
relationship neatly in his description of the four and position that construct on the horizon of
essential conditions of a strong theory. A theory extant related constructs.

This content downloaded from


146.50.98.29 on Mon, 26 Oct 2020 10:18:06 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
2010 Editor's Comments 351

Constructs may be "relational" in a different ship to other constructs must all make sense.
sense, however. Some constructs are relational That is# they must all cohere or "hang to
not just because they are derived from other gether" in a logically consistent manner. In
constructs but because they are embedded in part, the need for coherence derives from the
processes that involve other constructs. That is, inherently multidimensional nature of man
some constructs are processual in nature be agement research. Most of the constructs we
cause they are derived from process data (Lang use are highly contextually sensitive, and over
ley, 1999). Constructs in process theory are qual time constructs developed in management re
itatively different from constructs derived from search tend to consist of a number of interre
variance theory (Mohr, 1982). Process data are lated attributes or dimensions that may vary
inherently "messy," in part because they are somewhat in different organizational contexts
collected in real time through direct observa but still meaningfully capture a comprehen
tions in the field (Langley, 1999). Constructs de sive element of organizational experience. As
rived from processes, therefore, tend to be rela a result, constructs often become multidimen
tional inasmuch as they focus on events that are sional. That is, they describe abstract con
multidimensional, temporally embedded, and cepts that are themselves composed of multi
often spanning multiple levels of analysis (Lang ple attributes.
ley, 1999; Van de Ven, 1992). So, for example, So, for example, organizational citizenship
some theorists have identified stories or narra behavior (OCB) is a recognized construct that
tives as distinct constructs in process theory has been based on five distinct foundational
(Pentland, 1999). elements: civic virtue, sportsmanship, altru
A critical question that arises when assessing ism, conscientiousness, and courtesy (Law,
the clarity of constructs used in process theory is Wong, & Mobley, 1998). Each of these founda
what the tipping point is at which complex pro tional elements is based on distinct measures
cess constructs should be broken down into and may vary somewhat across different or
more concise theoretical units. Addressing this ganizational contexts in terms of its propor
issue is difficult in that it illustrates, in part, how tionate contribution to the "umbrella con
different epistemological assumptions produce struct" of OCB. However, the umbrella
different standards of construct clarity (dis construct retains an overall coherence or con
cussed in more detail below). Some general sistency that is more than the sum of its foun
principles of construct clarity, however, may dational parts. Law et al. (1998) describe this
shed some insight here. Notwithstanding the internal coherence of the umbrella construct
complexity and multidimensionality of con as a "latent model" and the summated ele
structs used in process theory, authors should ments as an "aggregate model." They also use
still strive to capture the essential characteris the term profile model to describe the different
tics of phenomena with constructs that balance profiles that occur when the elements vary as
accuracy (comprehensiveness) with simplicity a result of different contextual conditions. The
(parsimony) and generality (Langley, 1999). Con key element of their argument, however, is
structs derived from process theory might well the understanding that the core construct is
be more densely embedded in relation to other greater?that is, more resilient?than its foun
constructs. The goal of the researcher, however, dational elements. This perhaps best illus
should still be to strive for clarity, parsimony, trates the notion of coherence in that a true
and precision in capturing the essential ele multidimensional construct demonstrates
ments of the construct and in mapping out the greater resilience than its component ele
relationships between the focal construct and ments.
other constructs within which the focal construct Often, the issue presented by questions of co
is embedded. herence is the ability of the theorist to use the
constructs to create logically consistent and the
Coherence oretically integrated arguments. As Sutton and
Staw (1995) have observed, theorists will regu
A final component of construct clarity is the larly offer up complex schematics or intricate
notion that the construct, its definition, its process flow diagrams in lieu of coherence. The
scope conditions, its lineage, and its relation diagrams are a good start, but ultimately, the

This content downloaded from


146.50.98.29 on Mon, 26 Oct 2020 10:18:06 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
352 Academy of Management Review July

power of coherence of such an assembly of con search. I elaborate each of these points in the
structs can only be provided by a compelling balance of this section.
and coherent explanation.
Coherence is a difficult characteristic of con
struct clarity to explain, in part because it re
Clarity Facilitates Communication
veals the recursive or dialectical relationship
that exists between constructs and theory. That Construct clarity allows us to build on prior
is, in large part constructs gain their coherence, research by providing the research community
both internally and in relation to other con with a common language. A common language
structs, as a result of the theory in which they is an essential prerequisite for a community of
are embedded. It is difficult to understand the scholars interested in the same or similar phe
construct of legitimacy independent of one's nomena to exchange ideas and build knowl
knowledge of institutional theory (Suchman, edge. The ability to precisely articulate the key
1995). Kaplan calls this the "paradox of concep elements that underpin an idea helps us to un
tualization," noting that "proper concepts are derstand the degree to which ideas overlap or
needed to formulate good theory, but we need a differ. Moreover, the advancement of theory and
good theory to arrive at proper concepts" (1964: knowledge relies on the ability of new research
501). ers to build on the work of prior researchers. If
Coherence, thus, is a somewhat intuitive as new and old researchers cannot agree on or
sessment of whether the various attributes of a communicate the basic elements of a phenome
phenomenon are adequately contained within a non, the accumulation of knowledge cannot
construct?that is, do these attributes hang to occur.
gether in a logical and empirically convincing The ability to precisely define the essence
way? Is the construct plausible, given one's ex an abstraction in such a way that different
perience in similar contexts (Weick, 1989)? Are it from other similar abstractions produce
the relationships described or implied by the ous advantages for a scholarly commun
construct plausible? Does the construct make Foremost, it avoids the proliferation of diff
sense? terms and labels for similar phenom
Collectively, these four characteristics (defini problem that is often colloquially describ
tions, scope conditions, relationships between putting "old wine in new bottles." Tru
constructs, and coherence) capture the essential Kelley, one of the founders of the Stan
elements of construct clarity. It is perhaps trite Achievement Test, called the proliferation o
to note that the characteristics mutually rein ferent labels for the same underlying const
force each other. It is difficult, for example, to the "jangle fallacy." Kelley (1927) observed
demonstrate scope conditions without first pro when researchers used different words?"
viding a sound definition. Similarly, coherence ligence" or "achievement"?to describe
is highly dependent on clearly stated scope con same underlying construct of general in
ditions. Construct clarity requires considerable gence, there was a tendency to start treatin
skill in crafting each of these essential terms as completely different constructs,
elements. though the overlap in individual differ
that underpin the two terms was over 90 pe
(Lubinski, 2004).
When researchers use different terms for
WHY DO WE NEED CONSTRUCT CLARITY?
ilar phenomena, it produces confusion?"
There are three main justifications for clear founding effects"?that impede the abilit
and concise constructs, each of which builds on members of a research community to comm
the core idea that such clarity is critical to the cate with each other or to accumulate k
accumulation of knowledge. First, clear con edge. The creation of a common vocab
structs facilitate communication between schol avoids the "Tower of Babel" effect, in which
ars. Second, improved clarity of constructs en communities of researchers have no com
hances researchers' ability to empirically means of communication. In the absence of
explore phenomena. Third, clear constructs al mon and well-articulated constructs, the bo
low for greater creativity and innovation in re aries between subcommunities become m

This content downloaded from


146.50.98.29 on Mon, 26 Oct 2020 10:18:06 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
2010 Editors Comments 353

sharply defined and organizational knowledge sence of empiricism is the ability to create clear
becomes increasingly fragmented. classifications of phenomena that structure ex
Clear constructs can and should also extend perience into meaningful categories (Hacking,
the scope of knowledge beyond the academic 1975). Clear constructs are simply robust catego
community to include practitioners. Manage ries that organize experience. Moreover, clear
ment scholars have expressed considerable dis constructs help researchers identify anomalies
may about the failure of academic research to or phenomena that defy categories and force
penetrate the practitioner community (Rynes, researchers to reevaluate their theories.
2007; Rynes, Bartunek, & Daft, 2001). In part, this
is the result of weakly articulated constructs or
Construct Clarity Enhances Creativity
constructs that are so narrowly defined they
lack relevance. Effective constructs, however, Clearly defined theoretical constructs serve a
can help immensely in bridging that gap. As creative heuristic purpose in the elaboration of
Astley and Zammuto note, it is at the abstract theory. Like metaphors, a well-crafted construct
level of ideational constructs "where much of can capture the essential elements or character
the knowledge transfer between scientific and istics of a phenomenon and, simultaneously,
practitioner domains occurs" (1992: 444). An ef highlight both its similarities to and differences
fective construct, thus, navigates a narrow path from related phenomena. Constructs are care
between definitional accuracy and communica fully articulated abstractions that, if effectively
ble generality?that is, it is precisely and ac crafted, expand the range of phenomena and
curately constructed, but in a way that broad relationships they capture. Effective constructs,
audiences can understand and participate in thus, can enhance research creativity by "allow
the process of empirical elaboration and ing managers to redefine problems in ways that
exploration. are more amenable to resolution" (Astley & Zam
muto, 1992: 455). Constructs are conceptual
frames, and clear constructs expose a phenom
Clarity Assists Empirical Analysis
enon to multiple perspectives.
Construct clarity aids in the empirical appli A clear construct, thus, not only serves as a
cation of theory. For positivists, construct clarity useful means of description but can stimulate
helps them test theory, since precisely defined insights into additional possible relationships,
constructs are easier to operationalize and test related constructs, and often related theories.
(Schwab, 1980) and it is easier for researchers to Like a well-chosen metaphor, a carefully crafted
compare and contrast results (Bagozzi & Ed construct is a powerful creative tool that en
wards, 1998). For constructivists, construct clar hances theory development.
ity is not intended to lead to precise operation
alization and measurement but is still critical
HOW DOES CONSTRUCT CLARITY VARY?
(perhaps even more essential) for capturing and
communicating with precision the often subjective Thus far, I have presented the notion of con
meaning and interpretation of an abstraction by struct clarity in a somewhat catholic fashion?
individual subjects. Berger and Luckmann, for that is, with an implicit assumption that its
example, argue that construct clarity helps con importance and basic characteristics are uni
structivist researchers avoid the positivist di versally accepted across the various epistemo
lemma of reification or "confusing its own con logical and ontological regimes that comprise
ceptualizations with the laws of the universe" the Academy of Management scholars. Clearly,
(1967: 187). Similarly, the success of grounded that is not the case, and at various points in the
theory research lies, in large part, in the ability discussion above, I have made some effort to
of researchers to clearly identify and express foreshadow that, for example, positivists and
"concepts or constructs that are grounded in ac social constructionists might hold different
tors' meaning-in-use, rather than categories or views as to what might constitute a good defi
constructs that are imposed by the researcher" nition. Indeed, the term construct itself is likely
(Bob Gephart, personal communication). to be contested by nonpositivists, based on the
In sum, careful description of phenomena is connotations of hypothesis testing and opera
the fundament of empirical exploration. The es tionalization typically associated with the term.

This content downloaded from


146.50.98.29 on Mon, 26 Oct 2020 10:18:06 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
354 Academy of Management Review July

For researchers using a constructivist perspec disconfirm or build upon existing theories_We
tive, concepr might be a more acceptable value argue that the classic case study approach has
neutral term. been extremely powerful because these authors
have described general phenomena so well that
Different traditions of research have very dif others have little difficulty seeing the same phe
ferent understandings of what construct clarity nomena in their own experience and research.
is and how constructs might best be used in We return to the classics because they are good
building theory. Let me illustrate through two stories, not because they are merely clear state
ments of a construct. Indeed the very clarity of the
examples. The first comes from Eisenhardt's constructs stems from the story that supports and
(1989) classic paper on how to use case studies demonstrates them (1991: 617).
to build theory. Here Eisenhardt adopts a very
positivist view of what constructs are and how On its surface, the debate between Eisenhardt
they should be used in building theory: and Dyer and Wilkins seems to reflect two op
A priori specification of constructs can also help posing and irreconcilable epistemological posi
to shape the initial design of theory-building, re tions. On closer examination, however, they
search. Although this type of specification is not simply illustrate different assumptions regard
common in theory-building studies to date, it is ing the role of constructs in the research process.
valuable because it permits researchers to mea Eisenhardt (1989) sees constructs as lenses
sure constructs more accurately. If these con
structs prove important as the study progresses, through which data can be analyzed in the the
then researchers have a firmer empirical ground ory-building process. Dyer and Wilkins (1991)
ing for the emergent theory (1989: 536). see constructs as emerging from the data. Both,
however, seem to acknowledge the need for
Note that Eisenhardt sees constructs as essen
clear constructs; they simply differ on their role
tial to theory building but acknowledges that in the process of building theory.
the researcher will bring preexisting constructs In fact, Eisenhardt (1989) and Dyer and Wilkins
into the research to be "tested" through empiri (1991) are assuming two distinct roles in an on
cal application. Eisenhardt qualifies this posi going dialectic or tension over theoretical con
tion somewhat with an admonition to the re
structs that Hirsch and Levin (1999) describe as
searcher to keep an open mind about the the "umbrella advocates" versus the "validity
possibility of refining the construct in accor police." The term umbrella advocates refers to
dance with the data as the research progresses: those researchers who argue that constructs
Although early identification of the research should be viewed as large buckets or broad con
question and possible constructs is helpful, it is cepts loosely defined because this better cap
equally important to recognize that both are ten tures the inherent complexity and messiness of
tative in this type of research. No construct is
the empirical world we study. The term validity
guaranteed a place in the resultant theory, no
matter how well it is measured (1989: 536). police refers to those researchers who argue
that constructs should be small buckets nar
Eisenhardt's (1989) view of the role of con rowly defined in order to bring more scientific
structs in theory building is not universally ac rigor and validity to the study of organizations.
cepted and was subsequently challenged by re Hirsch and Levin (1999) argue that the tension
searchers who thought that entering the field between these two regimes creates a distinct
with narrowly defined constructs would inter life cycle for theoretical constructs in organiza
fere with the researcher's ability to create new tion studies, where umbrella advocates first
constructs or enrich our understanding of exist introduce a new construct, which then succumbs
ing ones. Drawing from a research tradition that to demands from the validity police to "clean up
encourages more creativity and flexibility in the the concept" (for a recent example of this stage,
research process and one that might adopt the see Briner, Denyer, & Rousseau, 2009). In some
term concepf in place of consfrucf, in their re instances constructs become so clearly defined,
joinder to Eisenhardt, Dyer and Wilkins suggest measurable, and operationalized over time that
that researchers ought to aim for "good stories" they lose relevance with the empirical world
rather than "good constructs": and, ultimately, reappear under a different
Eisenhardt's approach [is] to start with ... con name. Hirsch and Levin (1999) illustrate this phe
structs and measurement instruments. Such an nomenon through an analysis of the emergence,
approach leads the case researcher to confirm, clarification, and disappearance of the theoret

This content downloaded from


146.50.98.29 on Mon, 26 Oct 2020 10:18:06 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
2010 Editor's Comments 355

ical construct "organizational effectiveness." lined in this essay still applies. Phenomenolo
Hirsch and Levin (1999) remind us that this gists may not agree with the notion that con
construct was itself a replacement for a prior structs must be made measureable, but they
similar construct called "organizational would not argue with the notion that the con
performance." cepts they derive from their research must be
In contrast to the evident disagreement in thecommunicated clearly, with appropriate limit
debate between Eisenhardt (1989) and Dyer and ing conditions and assumptions and with some
Wilkins (1991) about the proper role of theoreti explanation of how these concepts fit in relation
cal constructs, Hirsch and Levin argue that this to other concepts used in similar research. Sim
tension between broad and narrow interpreta ilarly, while a researcher using grounded theory
tions of constructs is not only healthy but is might be striving for novel insights from his or
necessary for the advancement of knowledge: her data, when writing his or her theory, the
researcher must still bear the burden of demon
Though each of us may have his or her own lean
strating how his or her insights fit on the horizon
ings, the field as a whole probably needs both
broad (umbrella) and narrow (policing) perspec of prior knowledge of the subject under study,
tives, for this dialectic can be useful for under even those drawn from other research traditions.
standing and explaining the underlying issues of
organizational life. This struggle thus enables
the field as a whole to balance its competing WHY WE NEED AN ONGOING
needs to be both scientific and relevant (1999: CONVERSATION ABOUT THEORETICAL
209). CONSTRUCTS IN ORGANIZATION STUDIES
The standards for the meaning and use of con I have tried to demonstrate how construct clar
structs, thus, appear to vary considerably across ity lies at the heart of theory building. Clearly
different research traditions, epistemologies, defined conceptual categories encourage re
and ontological positions within organization searchers to generate more effective research
studies. questions, apply appropriate and epistemologi
While different research traditions may have cally consistent methods, and identify excep
different interpretations of how constructs are tions to the categories that open opportunities
constituted and how they should be used in re for future research. All of this serves to
search, the need for clarity and precision in the strengthen our understanding of phenomena.
description of constructs remains intact. For pos Construct clarity also aids in the communica
itivists, precise language is necessary to cap tion and accumulation of knowledge. Clear con
ture, as effectively as possible, the essence of ceptual categories can help overcome fragmen
the subject matter under study. The challenge is tation in the field, make our research relevant to
to use language to create constructs that accu broader audiences, and enhance the legitimacy
rately represent reality. For nonpositivists, pre of management as a research discipline.
cise language is equally important, not to cap What surprises me is how unusually mute our
ture reality or to enable the measurement of discipline seems to be on so important a subject.
constructs but, rather, to recognize that linguis While management journals devote some space
tic constructs are themselves the embodiment of to discussions of constructs and their role in
knowledge (Gergen, 1982). As Astley observes, theory development (i.e., Astley & Zammuto,
for nonpositivists, "Language is not simply a 1992; Hirsch & Levin, 1999; Preim & Butler, 2001),
vehicle for transmitting information. Rather it is the coverage seems disproportionate to the im
the very embodiment of truth. . . . Scientific portance of the topic. This anomaly is reflected,
fields are word systems created and maintained somewhat, in how we train graduate students,
through a process of negotiation between adher where considerable time is devoted to under
ents to alternative theoretical languages" (1985: standing how constructs are measured and opera
499). tionalized but substantially less time is devoted
So, while the interpretation of what a con to understanding how constructs are created
struct means might vary across subdisciplines and used in the research process.
of organizational research, I believe that the One clear conclusion from this essay is that
requirement for clarity of description as well as construct clarity is highly dependent on a theo
the four key elements of construct clarity out rist's facility with language. Good constructs ef

This content downloaded from


146.50.98.29 on Mon, 26 Oct 2020 10:18:06 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
356 Academy of Management Review July

fectively balance some competing (if not contra Design and analysis issues for field setting. Chicago:
Rand McNally.
dictory) tensions. So, for example, constructs
must strip away surplus meaning but not be Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. 1955. Construct validity in
made too narrow. Constructs should offer clear psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52: 281-302.
boundaries and scope conditions but also be Dubin, R. 1969. Theory building. New York: Free Press.
sufficiently "linguistically ambiguous" to spark Dyer, W. G., & Wilkins, A. L. 1991. Better stories, not better
new connotative relationships. These are no constructs, to generate better theory: A rejoinder to
small tasks, even for those trained in the skillful Eisenhardt. Academy of Management Review, 16: 613?
619.
use of language. Yet even though we under
stand that language matters to effective theory Eisenhardt, K. 1989. Building theory from case study re
search. Academy of Management Review, 14: 532-550.
development in the same way it matters to phi
losophy (Hacking, 1975), we devote considerably Empson, W. 1995. (First published in 1951.) The structure of
less time to training new researchers in under complex words. London: Penguin Books.
standing the nuances of language than we do to Floyd, S. W. 2009. Borrowing theory: What does this mean
understanding the nuances of statistical mea and when does it make sense in management scholar
surement. ship? Journal of Management Studies, 46: 1059-1075.

Our silence on the subject of use of constructs George, J. M., & Jones, G. R. 2000. The role of time in theory
in management may be a pragmatic effort to and theory building. Journal of Management, 26: 657
684.
avoid the "paradigm wars" of the past, or it may
simply reflect the ongoing fragmentation of our Gergen, K. J. 1982. Toward transformation in social knowl
edge. New York: Springer.
field. My hope is that this essay will not only
help aspiring theorists understand how to more Gibson, C. B., & Zellmer-Bruhn, M. E. 2001. Metaphors and
meaning: An intercultural analysis of the concept of
effectively develop constructs but will also re
teamwork. Administrative Science Quarterly, 46: 274
new the conversation on constructs in manage 303.
ment theories and focus discussion on tech
Hacking, I. 1975. Why does language matter to philosophy?
niques for improving their clarity, increasing Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
our understanding of their role in bridging re
Hansen, G. S., & Wernerfeldt, B. 1989. Determinants of firm
search disciplines, and improving both the rel
performance: The relative importance of economic and
evance and rigor of organizational research. organizational factors. Strategic Management Journal,
10: 399-411.
Hirsch, P. M., & Levin, D. Z. 1999. Umbrella advocates versus
REFERENCES
validity police: A life-cycle model. Organization Sci
Astley, W. G. 1985. Administrative science as socially con ence, 10: 199-212.
structed truth. Administrative Science Quarterly, 30:497
Kaplan, A. 1964. The conduct of inquiry. San Francisco:
513. Chandler.
Astley, W. G.( & Zammuto, R. F. 1992. Organization science,
Kelley, T. L. 1927. Interpretation of educational measure
managers and language games. Organization Science, ments. New York: World Book.
3: 443-460.
Kerlinger, F. N. 1973. Foundations of behavioral research.
Avital, M. 2000. Dealing with time in social inquiry: A tension New York: Reinhart & Winston.
between method and lived experience. Organization
Science, 11: 665-673. Klein, K. J., Dansereau, F., & Hall, R. J. 1994. Levels issues in
Bacharach, S. B. 1989. Organizational theories: Some criteria
theory development, data collection, and analysis.
for evaluation. Academy of Management Journal, 14: Academy of Management Review, 19: 195-229.
496-515. Langley, A. 1999. Strategies for theorizing from process data.
Bagozzi, R. P., & Edwards, J. R. 1998. A general approach for Academy of Management Review, 24: 691-710.
representing constructs in organizational research. Or Law, K. S., Wong, C, & Mobley, W. H. 1998. Toward a taxon
ganizational Research Methods, 1: 45-87. omy of multidimensional constructs. Academy of Man
Berger, P. L., & Luckmann, T. 1967. The social construction of agement Review, 23: 741-755.
reality: A treatise on the sociology of knowledge. New Lubinski, D. 2004. Introduction to the special section on cog
York: Anchor Books. nitive abilities: 100 years after Spearman's (1904) "Gen
Briner, R. B., Denyer, D., & Rousseau, D. M. 2009. Evidence eral intelligence," objectively determined and mea
based management: Concept cleanup time? Academy of sured. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86:
Management Perspectives, 23(4): 19-32. 96-111.
Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. 1979. Quasi-experimentation: MacCorquodale, K., & Meehl, P. E. 1948. On a distinction

This content downloaded from


146.50.98.29 on Mon, 26 Oct 2020 10:18:06 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms
2010 Editor's Comments 357

between hypothetical constructs and intervening vari Shane, S., & Venkataraman, S. 2000. The promise of entre
ables. Psychological Review, 55: 95-107. preneurship as a field of research. Academy of Manage
ment Review, 25: 217-226.
Mitchell, R. K., Agle, B. R., & Wood, D. J. 1997. Toward a theory
of stakeholder identification and salience: Defining the Sharma, P., Chrisman, J. J., & Chua, J. H. 1996. A review and
principle of who and what really counts. Academy of annotated bibliography of family business studies. Bos
Management Review, 22: 853-886. ton: Kluwer Academic.

Mohr, L. B. 1982. Explaining organizational behavior. San Shenkar, O., & von Glinow, M. A. 1994. Paradoxes of orga
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. nizational theory and research: Using the case of China
to illustrate national contingency. Management Sci
Pentland, B. T. 1999. Building process theory with narrative:
ence, 40: 56-69.
From description to explanation. Academy of Manage
ment Review, 24: 711-724. Suchman, M. C. 1995. Managing legitimacy: Strategic and
institutional approaches. Academy of Management Re
Pierce, J. L., Gardner, D. G., Cummings, L. L., & Dunham, R. B.
view, 20: 571-610.
1989. Organization-based self-esteem: Construct defini
tion, measurement, and validation. Academy of Man Sutton, R. I., & Staw, B. M. 1995. What theory is not. Admin
agement Journal, 32: 622-648. istrative Science Quarterly, 40: 371-384.
Priem, R. L., & Butler, J. E. 2001. Tautology in the resource Van de ven, A. H. 1992. Suggestions for studying strategy
based view and the implications of externally deter process: A research note. Strategic Management Jour
mined resource value: Further comments. Academy of nal, 13: 169-188.
Management Review, 26: 57-66. Walker, H. A., & Cohen, B. P. 1985. Scope statements: Imper
Rogers, E. W., & Wright, P. M. 1998. Measuring organization atives for evaluating theory. American Sociological Re
al performance in strategic human resource manage view, 50: 288-301.
ment: Problems, prospects and performance informa Weick, K. E. 1989. Theory construction as disciplined imagi
tion markets. Human Resource Management Review, nation. Academy of Management Review, 14: 516-531.
8: 311-331.
Whetten, D. A. 1989. What constitutes a theoretical contribu
Rousseau, D. M. 1985. Issues of level in organizational tion? Academy of Management Review, 14: 490-495.
research: Multi-level and cross level perspectives. Re
search in Organizational Behavior, 7: 1-37. Whetten, D. A., Felin, T., & King, B. 2009. The practice of theory
borrowing in organizational studies: Current issues and
Rynes, S. L. 2007. Let's create a tipping point: What academ
future directions. Journal of Management, 35: 537-563.
ics and practitioners can do, alone and together. Acad
emy of Management Journal, 50: 1046-1054. White, S. 2002. Rigor and relevance in Asian management
research: Where are we and where can we go? Asia
Rynes, S. L., Bartunek, J. M., & Daft, R. L. 2001. Across the
Pacific Journal of Management, 19: 287-352.
great divide: Knowledge creation and transfer between
practitioners and academics. Academy of Management Zaheer, S., Albert, S., & Zaheer, A. 1999. Time scales and
Journal, 44: 340-355. organizational theory. Academy of Management Re
view, 24: 725-741.
Saussure, F. 2000. Course in general linguistics. Peru, IL:
Open Court.
Schwab, D. P. 1980. Construct validity in organizational be Roy Suddaby
havior. Research in Organizational Behavior, 2: 3-43. Associate Editor

This content downloaded from


146.50.98.29 on Mon, 26 Oct 2020 10:18:06 UTC
All use subject to https://about.jstor.org/terms

You might also like