You are on page 1of 27

Jonathan Tarbox· Dennis R.

Dixon
Peter Sturmey ·Johnny L. Matson
Editors

Handbook of Early
Intervention for Autism
Spectrum Disorders
Research, Policy, and Practice

~Springer
Editors
Jonathan Tarbox Peter Sturmey
Center for Autism and Related Department of Psychology
Disorders Queens College City University
Tarzana, California of New York Flushing, New York
USA USA

Dennis R. Dixon Johnny L. Matson


Center for Autism and Related Department of Psychology
Disorders Louisiana State University
Tarzana, California Baton Rouge, Louisiana
USA USA

ISSN 2192-922X ISSN 2192-9238 (electronic)


ISBN 978-1-4939-0400-6 ISBN 978-1-4939-0401-3 (eBook)
DOl 10.1007/978-1-4939-0401-3
Springer New York Heidelberg Dordrecht London

Library of Congress Control Number: 2014933217

© Springer Science+ Business Media New York 2014


This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved by the Publisher, whether the whole
or part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation; reprinting. reuse of
illustrations, recitation, broadcasting. reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way,
and transmission or infom1ation storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software.
or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter devel9ped. Exempted from this
legal reservation are brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis or material
supplied specifically for the purpose of being entered and executed on a computer system, for
exclusive use by the purchaser of the work. Duplication of this publication or parts thereof is
permitted only under the provisions of the Copyright Law of the Publisher's location, in its cur-
rent version, and permission for use must always be obtained from Springer. Permissions for use
may be obtained through Rights Link at the Copyright Clearance Center. Violations are liable to
prosecution under the respective Copyright Law.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks. service marks, etc. in this
publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are ex-
empt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
While the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of
publication, neither the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legal responsibil-
ity for any errors or omissions that may be made. The publisher makes no warranty, express or
implied, with respect to the material contained herein.

Printed on acid-free paper

Springer is part of Springer Science+ Business Media (www.springer.com)


Contents

Part I Diagnosis and Background

1 Evolution of Autism: From Kanner to the DSM-V.:................... 3


B. Andrew Adler, Noha F. Minshawi and Craig A. Erickson

2 Autism Spectrum Disorders: Several Disorders


on a Continuum or One?............................................................... 21
Brian Reichow, Daniel Campbell and Fred R. Volkmar

4 Related Disorders..... ...................................................................... 39


Joyce Suh and Deborah Fein

4 Assessment ofthe Core Features of ASD ..................................... 65


Raphael Bernier

5 Measures Used to Screen and Diagnose ASD


in Young Children .......................................................................... 87
Megan Sipes and Johnny L. Matson

6 Psychological Theories of Childhood Autism.............................. I 05


Patrick M. Ghezzi, Janice K. Doney and Jennifer A. Bonow

7 Family Adaptation to a Diagnosis of Autism


Spectrum Disorder......................................................................... 117
Lisa M. Negri and Lia L Castorina

Part II Intervention

8 Ethical Issues in Early Intervention...................... ....................... 141


Alan Poling and Timothy L. Edwards

9 Economics of Autism Spectrum Disorders:


An Overview of Treatment and Research Funding.................... 165
Julie Komack, Angela Persicke, Paige Cervantes,
Jina Jang and Dennis Dixon

ix
x Contents

10 Designing Curriculum Programs for Children with Autism ..... 179


Adel C. Najdowski, Evelyn R. Gould, Taira
M. Lanagan and Michele R. Bishop

11 Reinforcement Arrangements for Learners


with Autism Spectrum Disorder................................................... 205
lser G. DeLeon, Richard B. Graff, Michelle A. Frank-Crawford,
Griffin W. Rooker and Christopher E. Bullock

12 Discrete Trial Teaching and Discrimination


Training............................................................ ............................... 229
Svein Eikeseth, Dean P. Smith and Lars Klintwalt

13 Natural Environment Ttaining..................................................... 255


Sarah Dufek and Laura Schreibman

14 Picture Activity Schedules............................................................. 271


Patricia J. Krantz and Lynn E. McClannahan

15 Use of Visual Supports with Young


Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders................................. 293
Kara Hume, Connie Wong, Joshua
Plavnick and Tia Schultz

16 Teaching Verbal Behavior to Children with


Autism Spectrum Disorders.......................................................... 315
Danielte L. LaFrance and Caio F. Miguel

17 Social Skills and Play in Children with Autism........................... 341


Melaura Erickson Tomaino, Catherine A. Miltenberger
and Marjorie H. Charlop

18 Teaching Cognitive Skills to Children with Autism.................... 355


Jonathan Tarbox and Adel C. Najdowski

19 Teaching Independent Living Skills to Children with ASD ....... 373


Bridget A. Taylor, Jaime A. DeQuinzio and Jaime Stine

20 Developmental Approaches to Treatment of.Young


Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder................................... 393
Amy Lee Wagner, Katherine S. Wallace and
Sally J. Rogers

21 Recovery and Prevention............................................................... 429


Doreen Granpeesheh, Jonathan Tarbox and
Angela Persicke
Contents xi

22 Mainstream Education for Children with Autism


Spectrum Disorders....................................................................... 447
Phil Reed and Lisa A. Osborne

23 Controversial Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorders....... 487


Julie A. Worley, Jill C. Fodstad and Daniene Neal

24 Families of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders:


Intervention and Family Supports............................................... 511
Wendy Machalicek, Robert Didden, Russell Lang,
Vanessa Green, Jenna Lequia, Jeff Sigafoos,
Giulio Lancioni and Mark F. O'Reilly

25 Management of General Medical Conditions.............................. 533 ·


Paul S. Carbone, Ann M. Reynolds and Lynne M. Kerr

26 Behavioral Disorders in Young Children with


Autism Spectrum Disorder ........................................................... 563
John Huet~, Jonathan Schmidt and Carmen L6pez-Arvizu

27 Mental Health Disorders in Young Children with


Autism Spectrum Disorders.......................................................... 591
Luc Lecavalier, Aaron J. Kaat and Elizabeth A. Stratis

28 Training and Supporting Caregivers in


Evidence-Based Practices.............................................................. 613
Rebecca C. Shaffer and Noha F. Minshawi

29 Multicultural and Minority Issues............................................... 637


Kyong-Mee Chung, Jina Jang and Hilary L. Adams

Index...................................................................................................... 651
Assessment of the Core Features
of ASD 4
Raphael Bernier

Keywords
Assessment · Evaluation

History of ASD Assessment in Young had yet to standardize diagnostic criteria for the
Children “psychoses of infancy,” which created contradic-
tion and controversy in the assessment, diagnosis,
In his groundbreaking paper, “Autistic Distur- and treatment of these disorders (Rutter 1967).
bances of Affective Contact,” psychiatrist Leo It was not until the 1970s that “infantile autism”
Kanner first described a group of children who was identified as a distinct disorder apart from
shared a cluster of clinical symptoms character- schizophrenia and soon thereafter earned its own
ized by impairment in social affect and skills set of diagnostic criteria, which was standardized
coupled with resistance to change and unusual, in the DSM-III (APA 1980; Rutter and Schopler
stereotyped behaviors. He noted that the funda- 1987; Volkmar and Klin 2005). DSM-III placed
mental problem in these children was their “in- infantile autism in the new general category of
ability to relate themselves in the ordinary way Pervasive Developmental Disorders (PDD),
to people and situations from the beginning of which was indicative of a key reconceptualiza-
life” (Kanner 1943). Kanner borrowed the term tion of autism as a developmental, rather than a
“autistic” from Dr. Euguene Blueler who used psychiatric, disorder (Volkmar and Klin 2005).
the term to describe the withdrawal into one’s Diagnostic criteria were broadened in the DSM-
self observed in individuals with schizophrenia. III-R (APA 1987) under the new term “Autistic
Despite the fact that Kanner specifically noted Disorder” in recognition of children who devel-
differences between the 11 children he studied oped the symptoms of “infantile autism” at later
and those with schizophrenia, many profession- stages of development and who presented with
als in the early field of child psychiatry viewed varying levels of symptom severity (Volkmar
these children as being severely mentally ill with and Klin 2005; Wing 2005). Pervasive Develop-
early onset adult psychosis (Rutter and Schopler mental Disorder, Not Otherwise Specified (PDD-
1987). In the 1960s, the field of child psychiatry NOS) was also added at this time to account for
children with significant symptoms who did not
meet full criteria for Autistic Disorder. Howev-
R. Bernier () er, the DSM-III-R criteria for Autistic Disorder
Departments of Psychiatry and Psychology,
University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA proved to be too broad, which led to overdiag-
e-mail: rab2@u.washington.edu nosis of children with intellectual disability and

J. Tarbox et al. (eds.), Handbook of Early Intervention for Autism Spectrum Disorders, 65
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4939-0401-3_4,
© Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014
66 R. Bernier

underdiagnosis of higher functioning children have noticed regarding their child’s development.
with ASD (Volkmar and Klin 2005). The DSM- For example, speech delay is a significant warn-
IV (APA 1994) refined and narrowed the diag- ing sign that is often readily detected by parents
nostic criteria for Autistic Disorder and added and often leads to a discussion of their concerns
Asperger’s Disorder to the category of Pervasive with their primary care physician (PCP). For
Developmental Disorders. As of this writing, the other children, early social deficits such as being
APA is considering another major revision in unresponsive to people, focusing intently on one
the forthcoming DSM-V which would subsume object for long periods of time, or delayed or ab-
Autistic Disorder, Asperger’s Disorder and PDD- sent joint attention (JA) can be important signs.
NOS into one overarching category called ASD However, these cues are often more subtle and
(Lord et al. 2011). difficult to detect without formal screening and
At the root of the controversies in ASD di- evaluation.
agnostic criteria were fundamental differences Given there is no specific biological or genetic
in the conception and nature of autism among marker for ASD, currently assessment must focus
experts in the field. Some experts believed that on the screening and evaluation of behavior (Fili-
Kanner’s description of symptoms represented pek et al. 1999; Zwaigenbaum et al. 2009). Al-
“true” autism, while others believed that Kan- though there is room for ongoing improvement
ner’s description was too narrow and did not ac- and expansion, screening and assessment efforts
count for the wide range of children with the dis- in young children are in high demand and are
order. Thus, early assessment instruments were becoming more commonplace. Practice param-
not standardized and tended to emphasize some eters for the detection and assessment of ASD
key deficits while ignoring others, depending on have been published by many organizations such
the theoretical orientation of the author (Parks as the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP;
1988). As diagnostic criteria for ASD have been American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on
refined and altered over time, so too have assess- Children with Disabilities 2006; Johnson, Myers,
ment procedures and protocols. and American Academy of Pediatrics, Council on
Children with Disabilities 2007), the American
Academy of Neurology (Filipek et al. 2000b),
Clinical Assessment of Autism the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Spectrum Disorders Psychiatry (Volkmar et al. 1999), and a consen-
sus panel with representation from various pro-
Early screening  Given the wide heterogeneity fessional societies (Filipek et al. 1999). These
of features in children and the significant variance parameters highlight two levels of the evaluation
in presentation of the three core deficits currently process: Level 1 which entails developmental
implicated in ASD, making the diagnosis of ASD surveillance and screening, and Level 2 which in-
is particularly challenging. There is no one iden- volves comprehensive multidisciplinary diagnos-
tifiable descriptor, genetic marker, or biological tic assessments and evaluations by professionals
feature that can definitively indicate the presence who have significant experience with ASD.
of an ASD. However, with increasing awareness
of ASD in the media and through the efforts of Level 1 surveillance and screening  According
research and public agencies, a growing number to the AAP (American Academy of Pediatrics
of caregivers and providers are becoming more Committee on Children with Disabilities 2006;
astute to the indicators of ASD. Johnson and Blasco 1997; Johnson et al. 2007;
Often the assessment process begins with par- American Academy of Pediatrics, Council on
ents raising concerns about their child’s develop- Children with Disabilities 2007) “surveillance”
ment. During regular office visits or well-child is the flexible and ongoing process of identify-
checks, parents may share their observations, ing children who could be at risk for develop-
videotapes, or written notes about red flags they mental delays, while “screening” is the use of
4  Assessment of the Core Features of ASD 67

standardized tools at specific time points to ver- Screens (Brigance 1986; Glascoe 1996) which
ify risk and define it further. It is recommended assesses general knowledge, speech-language,
that surveillance occur at every preventative visit fine and gross motor, graphomotor develop-
during childhood and should involve attending ment, and reading and math; The Child Develop-
to parent’s concerns, gathering a developmental ment Inventories (CDI; Ireton 1992; Ireton and
history, making observations of the child, iden- Glascoe 1995) which screen for social, self-help,
tifying possible risk and protective factors, and behavior, and health problems, as well as lan-
keeping good records of this process and the guage motor, cognitive, and pre-academic skills;
findings. Screeners should be administered at and The Parents’ Evaluation of Developmental
any time point when concerns are raised through Status (PEDS, Glascoe 1998) which assesses par-
observations or during the surveillance process to ents’ concerns about delays and disabilities (See
identify potential developmental delays or defi- Filipek et al. 1999 for a more detailed review).
cits in communication, language, motor, social, It is important to note that many screeners
and play skills. The AAP also recommends that used by pediatricians or primary care physicians
screening with standardized assessment tools be at well-child checks may not always differentiate
used at specific time points (i.e., 9, 18, 24, 30 children with ASD from children with other de-
month visits) even if developmental concerns velopmental concerns (Filipek et al. 2000b; John-
have not been raised. son et al. 2007; American Academy of Pediatrics,
PCPs often use screeners that assess for a Council on Children with Disabilities 2007), so it
wide variety of developmental concerns, medical is important for physicians to administer follow-
issues, and childhood disorders, most of which up screeners more specific to ASD if indicated
involve parent report via paper and pencil ques- as part of the Level 1 process. Some examples of
tionnaires. The following is a brief summary of autism-based screening tools include The Check-
two of the traditional developmental screeners list for Autism in Toddlers (CHAT; Baron-Cohen
and a few of the standardized developmental et al. 1992), The Modified Checklist for Autism
screening instruments that are used in primary in Toddlers (M-CHAT; Robins et al. 2001), and
care practices. The Denver-II (DDST-II; Fran- The Pervasive Developmental Disorders Screen-
kenburg et al. 1992) is a traditional tool for de- ing Test-II Primary Care Screener (Siegel 2004).
velopmental screening for children from birth to If scores are elevated on screeners and/or red
6 years of age. It obtains samples of receptive and flags are identified by observations or parent
expressive language, articulation, as well as fine report, it is important that the PCP continue to
motor, gross motor, adaptive, and personal-social Level 2 of the evaluation process and refer the
skills. The Revised Denver Pre-Screening Devel- child for a full multidisciplinary evaluation.
opmental Questionnaire (R-DPDQ; Frankenburg During surveillance and screening, another
1986) was created to identify a subtest of chil- crucial component to the assessment process is
dren who need further screening from birth to providing parent education along the way (John-
age 6. Parents are asked to answer 10 to 15 items son et al. 2007). By being knowledgeable about
which cover a wide range of domains. Accord- the characteristics and clinical symptoms of
ing to Filipek et al. (1999), because the DDST-II ASD, the evaluation process will likely run more
and R-DPDQ lack sensitivity and specificity, bet- smoothly and efficiently, as parents will have
ter standardized measures must be used during more information about what to expect from the
Level 1 surveillance and screening to aid in the process and be better able to articulate and de-
assessment of ASDs. scribe their child’s presentation and symptoms.
Examples of standardized parent report ques-
tionnaires with acceptable psychometric proper- Level 2 comprehensive evaluation by multi-
ties include: The Ages and Stages Questionnaire, disciplinary team  Findings suggestive of ASD
Second Edition (ASQ; Bricker and Squires 1994, observed during Level 1 surveillance and screen-
1999; Squires et  al. 1997); The BRIGANCE® ing warrant Level 2 multidisciplinary assessment.
68 R. Bernier

Key disciplines that should be involved in the history taken from the child’s caregivers. Ad-
Level 2 evaluation process include clinical psy- ditionally, physical examination may include
chologists, school psychologists, social work- searching for medical issues, co-morbid disor-
ers, speech and language pathologists, physical ders, dysmorphic features, acquired brain inju-
therapists, occupational therapists, audiologists, ries, and neurological abnormalities.
primary care physicians, neurologists, and devel- Developmental and psychometric evaluations
opmental pediatricians. Within multidisciplinary are key factors in determining a child’s overall
teams it is important that one individual serves level of functioning, cognitive abilities, adaptive
as the point person, which entails organizing skills, language skills, motor skills, and any po-
the team of evaluators, communicating with tential behavioral or emotional concerns. Profes-
the family and assessment providers to clarify sionals with specific training in clinical assess-
and understand the referral questions, planning ments, psychometrics and test design, and test
the components of the assessment, and relaying administration techniques should utilize stan-
information to parents and treatment providers in dardized tools and assessments with acceptable
the community who will carry out treatment rec- psychometric properties to aid in the diagnostic
ommendations (Ozonoff et al. 2005). process. These multidisciplinary assessments
There is a strong professional consensus that include observations, behavioral measures, and
interviewing parents about developmental mile- specific cognitive, academic, language, and
stones and detailed symptoms of ASD, as well as motor skill assessments. Both strengths and
direct child observation, preferably with the as- weaknesses should be assessed. Whenever pos-
sistance of standardized measures, are essential sible, interviews with and questionnaires from
components of the evaluation process (Johnson multiple sources such as teachers, providers, and
et al. 2007; NIMH 2008; Ozonoff et al. 2005). family members should be obtained to provide
The following is a brief overview of specific in- a well-rounded picture of the child’s behaviors,
terview and behavioral observation components abilities, and functioning in a variety of contexts.
that should be included in a comprehensive eval- As previously stated, parents are a key com-
uation. ponent in the evaluation process. It is important
A thorough intake should be conducted with to continue the assessment of the parent’s knowl-
the parents or primary caregivers of the child. edge of ASD, coping skills, and their available
An intake includes an in-depth interview about resources throughout the process and to make
the child’s prenatal, neonatal, and postnatal pe- adjustments accordingly to ensure they and their
riods; developmental milestones reached and not children are being fully supported throughout
reached; health and medical history; and behav- the process. Further, the evaluation team should
ioral and mental health histories. Critical aspects work closely with parents to provide them with
of history taking include gathering information regular updates with information about the as-
about the child’s communication, social, and sessment processes, review the assessment out-
behavioral development, as well as motor skills comes, discuss recommendations for interven-
and adaptive functioning abilities. It is also im- tion, and begin to provide education about inter-
perative to listen to and review parent’s current vention options.
concerns, which may involve reviewing audio, In addition to the early screening and assess-
visual, or written documentation. If available, ment of specific characteristics associated with
review of past and current medical, psychiatric, ASD, it is also important to address factors out-
assessment, treatment, and academic records will side of the three core domains of ASD to obtain a
provide rich information from multiple perspec- complete and holistic picture of the child. Infor-
tives and across various settings and contexts. mation about the child’s overall level of function-
Physical examination by a PCP, pediatrician, ing in multiple areas such as language, cognitive,
or pediatric neurologist can provide observation- and adaptive abilities; age; environmental and
al information of the child and includes medical family influences such as cultural factors and fam-
4  Assessment of the Core Features of ASD 69

ily mental health histories; and comorbid medi- fines impairment in social interaction for Autistic
cal and mental health issues should be obtained Disorder and Asperger’s Disorder as follows: (a)
and considered to address factors that might be marked impairment in the use of multiple non-
contributing to the child’s overall presentation, to verbal behaviors, such as eye-to-eye gaze, facial
identify strengths and weaknesses, and to assist expression, body postures, and gestures, to regu-
with differential diagnoses. For example, many late social interaction; (b) failure to develop peer
diagnoses should be considered in the evaluation relationships appropriate to developmental level;
process such as intellectual disability, borderline (c) a lack of spontaneous seeking to share enjoy-
intellectual functioning, various developmen- ment, interests, or achievements with other peo-
tal disorders including developmental language ple (e.g., by lack of showing, bringing or point-
disorders (e.g., expressive language disorder), ing out objects of interest); and (d) lack of social
schizophrenia, selective mutism, and stereotypic or emotional reciprocity. Diagnostic criteria for
movement disorder (APA 2000). ASD were generated over time by research which
The multidisciplinary diagnostic evaluation indicated that children with ASD exhibit difficul-
entails assessment of a wide range of behaviors ties in specific social behaviors including eye
and domains of functioning in addition to the gaze, social orienting, joint attention, face per-
core domains of ASD. Many of the assessment ception, imitation, empathy, and social reciproc-
tools used in a clinical evaluation focus on evalu- ity. These key social features will be explored in
ation of the three core domains of impairment in the following section.
ASD. There has been a significant amount of sci-
entific work conducted in the exploration and de- Key features in the social domain  One of the
velopment of appropriate and accurate evaluative more striking features of many, though certainly
measures related to the domains of impairment not all, children with ASD is a lack of gaze or the
in ASD. In the next section we provide detailed tendency not to look directly into others’ eyes.
descriptions of the core domains of impairment Orientation to others via eye contact is a critical
in ASD and the observational assessments, inter- biological trait that has evolved in humans as a
view measures, questionnaires, and experimental regulator of myriad social encounters. Typically
procedures that have been developed to inform developing infants establish eye contact with
our characterization of ASD. their caregivers early and often; however, infants
with ASD often fail to develop this skill (Carter
et al. 2005). Furthermore, this failure to establish
Assessment of the Three Core mutual gaze with caregivers appears specific to
Domains of ASD ASD and is not usually observed in children with
other developmental delays including intellec-
Social Domain tual disability. It is important to note, however,
that not all children with ASD display the same
Definition  At its core, ASD is a disorder of so- impairment in eye gaze. Some children with ASD
cial interaction, marked by a lack of social ini- express a clear preference not to make eye con-
tiation and reciprocity. This fact is reflected in tact to the point that parents may have to hold
DSM-IV-TR (APA 2000) criteria for Autistic their face to catch their eyes; other children with
Disorder and Asperger’s Disorder, which require ASD may exhibit milder symptoms, such as brief
individuals to exhibit more behaviors indicative eye contact or eye contact that is poorly modu-
of impairment in social behavior than they are re- lated in social situations (Filipek et al. 1999).
quired to exhibit in the domains of communica- Many children with ASD have impairments
tion and restrictive, repetitive behaviors. In order in social orienting skills. Social orienting may
to assess young children who may be at risk for be defined as one’s behavioral response to audi-
ASD, it is important to understand the unique so- tory or visual social stimuli, typically indicated
cial impairments that define it. DSM-IV-TR de- by a head turn or eye gaze towards the stimulus.
70 R. Bernier

Studies of social orienting focusing on visual complex skills, ranging from social smiling to
and auditory preference in children with ASD language acquisition. Typically developing chil-
have found that children with ASD, compared dren may be observed mimicking their caregiv-
to matched controls, prefer nonsocial to social ers’ facial expressions as infants and, later, their
sounds (Dawson et al. 1998), prefer nonspeech nonverbal gestures, such as waving, pointing, and
sounds over speech sounds (Kuhl et al. 2005), blowing kisses. As they get older, these children
prefer geometric patterns over social scenes with learn to play simple games like peek-a-boo and
other children (Pierce et al. 2011), and show a begin to imitate the actions of their caregivers.
lack of preference for point-light biological mo- Many studies have shown that children with ASD
tion (e.g., a figure running) compared to random tend to have difficulty imitating simple actions
point-light motion (Klin et al. 2009). All of these involving objects, body movements, and facial
studies indicate that children with ASD seem to expressions and that these deficits are associated
possess a neurodevelopmental predisposition to with impaired social development (Rogers et al.
attend to nonsocial stimuli over social stimuli. 2005).
Joint attention is another key social deficit in Another core feature of many children with
ASD and is defined as the sharing of an object ASD is “aloofness” and lack of understanding
or event with another person (Carter et al. 2005). of and empathy towards others. This lack of em-
Typically developing infants are able to follow pathy manifests itself in atypical behaviors. For
a visual cue, such as pointing or a shift in gaze, example, a child with ASD may run over an-
between 6 and 18 months of age (Mundy and other child, as if the child were an object, to get
Burnette 2005). However, children with ASD to a slide on a playground. Similarly, this child
exhibit marked impairment in joint attention as may not respond or offer comfort when another
evidenced by a lack of response to shifts in gaze, child is hurt or may laugh at socially inappropri-
pointing, or other nonverbal gestures, as well as ate times. One theory that seeks to explain this
a lack of initiation of joint attention with others. social impairment in autism is “theory of mind,”
Thus, joint attention embodies two complementa- proposed in 1995 by Simon Baron-Cohen. Dr.
ry concepts: the response to joint attention (RJA) Cohen hypothesized that the inability to infer
and the initiation of joint attention (IJA). Most others’ mental states (e.g., intentions) and to at-
typically developing children will follow a care- tribute mental states to self and others was a core
giver’s point to something of interest in the dis- feature of autism. He later expanded his theory
tance (RJA) or they might try to point out some- of mind to include deficits in empathizing, not-
thing themselves in order to share their interest ing that if one is unable to infer others’ mental
with another person (IJA). Joint attention skills states, then one is also unable to have an affec-
form a basis of social sharing and engagement tive response (e.g., a facial expression) that is ap-
with others and they are significantly impaired propriate to others’ mental states (Baron-Cohen
in children with ASD. While some children with et al. 2005).
ASD may point at an object of interest in the dis- ASD is perhaps best understood as a disorder
tance, they usually do not coordinate their gaze of social reciprocity. Reciprocity is defined sim-
back to another person to share the experience ply as “mutual exchange” and is a cornerstone
with them. Also, children with ASD may point or of our growth and survival as a species. Human
gesture at something, but typically this gesture is lives are interconnected, and we have developed
not well coordinated with eye contact and serves highly specialized social skills to navigate the
to satisfy a need rather than to initiate a social en- many complicated exchanges we experience with
counter with another person (Baron-Cohen 1989; others on a daily basis. Reciprocity is the act of
Carter et al. 2005). giving and taking in partnership with others and
Many children with ASD also exhibit im- to each other’s mutual benefit. Typically devel-
pairments in imitation. Imitation is the vehicle oping children learn about giving and taking in
through which children develop simple to highly the earliest stages of life. Parents show and share
4  Assessment of the Core Features of ASD 71

Table 4.1   Behaviors warranting further assessment


Behaviors between birth and 1 year of age Behaviors from 1 to 3 years of age
Limited ability to anticipate being picked up Abnormal eye contact
Low frequency of looking at people Limited range of facial expressions
Little interest in interactive games Limited social referencing
Little affection towards familiar people Limited sharing of affect/enjoyment
Content to be alone Limited interest in other children
Little interest in interactive games
Limited social smile
Limited functional play; no pretend play
Low frequency of looking at people
Limited motor imitation

objects with their children early on to their de- on the autism spectrum, symptom presentation
light and wonder. Children soon learn that when will vary depending on the developmental stage
they show and share objects to their parents, they and cognitive ability of the child. Clinicians who
are rewarded with smiles, giggles, and affection. assess social functioning in young children at
Some children with ASD do not learn to show or risk or suspected of having ASD must possess
share with others or do so inconsistently or on knowledge of typical child development in order
a limited basis. Similarly, many children with to ascertain whether key social milestones are
ASD have difficulty engaging in social games being met. Chawarska and Volkmar (2005) sum-
that require imitation, such as peek-a-boo or the marized the following behaviors in early social
tickle game, and, if they do participate, do so in development that distinguish children with ASD
a non-reciprocal way (e.g., by being tickled only from typically developing and developmentally
and not tickling back). As children mature, the delayed peers. Table 4.1 highlights behaviors
social landscape becomes even more complex indicating further assessment is warranted.
and children who do not learn the skills of so- Many instruments are now available for the
cial reciprocity have great difficulty developing assessment of ASD in young children and will be
friendships with their peers. Several studies of highlighted in the instruments listed below.
young children with ASD indicate that they initi-
ate social interaction with peers less frequently Observational methods One of the most com-
than other children and are less responsive to oth- monly used observational measures in the
ers’ initiations (Travis and Sigman 1998). assessment and diagnosis of ASD is the Autism
Many studies have illustrated that children Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord
with ASD have significant deficits in face per- et al. 1999). The ADOS is a semi-structured
ception. Schultz (2005) identifies two types of standardized assessment instrument that creates
face perception in ASD research: (1) Recognition many opportunities for an examiner to observe
of a person’s identity via the structure of the face any social difficulties through the use of play and
and (2) Recognition of the internal affective state activities designed to foster social communica-
(emotion) of another person via changes in facial tion with a child such as blowing bubbles, look-
expression. These skill deficits may have obvious ing at pictures, and reading stories. The ADOS
negative repercussions in social development as comprises four different modules, which have
perception of faces is a springboard for social ini- been carefully designed to match the language
tiation, friendship, and a foundation for empathy ability and developmental level of the child,
(Dawson et al. 2005). ranging from preverbal/single words to fluent
speech. A toddler version of the ADOS (ADOS-
Assessment of the social domain  While impair- T; Luyster et al. 2009) has been developed to
ment in social functioning is universal for children ascertain deficits in children under 24 months.
72 R. Bernier

All versions of the ADOS can be administered (e.g., does the child engage in turn-taking inter-
in 30–45 min. For younger, more cognitively and actions with others). The assessment also differ-
verbally impaired children, key social behaviors entiates whether the behaviors are child-initiated
assessed include showing, pointing, coordina- versus responses to the examiner’s bids.
tion of gaze, frequency of vocalizations directed The Communication and Symbolic Behavior
to others, and joint attention. For older children Scales Developmental Profile (CSBS DP; Weth-
with fewer to no verbal or cognitive limitations, erby and Prizant 2002) is a screening tool admin-
key social behaviors assessed include insight istered by a trained professional that utilizes di-
into the nature of interpersonal relationships, the rect observation to assess social communication
amount of reciprocal social communication, and skills in young children. The social domain is
quality of social response. All individuals, no divided into three major sections that assess spe-
matter their age, verbal, or developmental level cific behaviors: (1) emotion and eye gaze (gaze
are also assessed in the following social skill shifts, shared positive affect, gaze/point follow-
areas: unusual eye contact, facial expressions ing), (2) communication (rate of communicating,
directed to others, gestures, shared enjoyment in behavior regulation, social interaction, and joint
interaction, quality of social overtures, and over- attention), and (3) gestures (conventional ges-
all quality of rapport. tures, distal gestures).
The Childhood Autism Rating Scale, Second Other standardized, normed assessments of
Edition (CARS2; Schopler et al. 1988; Schopler related social abilities include measurements
et al. 2010) is an observational rating scale that of face recognition (NEPSY-II, Korkman et al.
is used to assess behaviors associated with ASD 2007) and face memory (Children’s Memory
in children 2 years and older. The CARS2 con- Scales: Face Memory Subscales; Cohen 1997).
sists of 15 items on which the child is rated by
a trained clinician using a 4-point scale based Interview formats  The Autism Diagnostic Inter-
on their interactions with and observations of view-Revised (ADI-R; Lord et al. 1994) is an
the child. The ratings take into consideration the extended parent interview used in the assess-
frequency, intensity, peculiarity, and duration of ment of ASD that typically takes 1.5 to 2.5 h to
the behavior. There are three forms included in administer by a trained professional. The ADI-R
the CARS2: the Standard Version Rating Book- consists of 93 items, 17 of which are grouped
let (CARS2-ST) appropriate for children under 6 into the “social development and play” category.
years of age, the High-Functioning Version Rat- Specific items from this category are then chosen
ing Booklet (CARS2-HF) appropriate for chil- for the ADI-R diagnostic algorithm, which mir-
dren over 6 years of age, and the Questionnaire rors DSM-IV-TR criteria in the social domain for
for Parents or Caregivers (CARS2-QPC) which Autistic Disorder and Asperger’s Disorder. Thus,
aids in scoring both the ST and HF versions. DSM-IV-TR criterion (a) “marked impairment
Specific social skills assessed using the CARS2 in the use of multiple nonverbal behaviors” is
include interpersonal relationships, emotional re- assessed by questions probing for a child’s use
sponses, and imitation. of direct gaze, social smiling, and range of facial
The Early Social Communication Scales expression; criterion (b) “failure to develop peer
(ESCS; Mundy et al. 2003) is an observational relations” is assessed by questions which ask
measure designed to assess nonverbal social- about the child’s interest in and response to other
communication skills. The three main behaviors children, group play with peers, and friendships;
of interest are joint attention behaviors (i.e., does criterion (c) “lack of spontaneous seeking to
the child use nonverbal behaviors to share their share enjoyment” is assessed by questions which
experiences pertaining to objects and events), be- probe whether a child is actively showing things
havioral requests (i.e., does the child use nonver- of interest, offering to share things with others,
bal behaviors to request help during events or to or seeking to share his/her own enjoyment with
obtain objects), and social interaction behaviors others; criterion (d) “lack of social emotional
4  Assessment of the Core Features of ASD 73

reciprocity” is assessed by questions which probe related disorders. The PDDBI comes in a stan-
for quality of social overtures, inappropriate dard form (124 items; 20–30 min) and extended
facial expressions (i.e., those that are incongruent form (180–188 items; 30–45 min), depending on
to the situation and indicate a lack of understand- the needs of the assessor. The standard form fo-
ing of others’ affective states), appropriateness of cuses on behaviors specific to ASD in the three
social response, and the act of offering comfort core domains and the extended form includes be-
when others when are hurt or ill. haviors that are not solely related to ASD, such
The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Sec- as aggression and specific fears. Key social skills
ond Edition (VABS-II; Sparrow et al. 2005) is a assessed include social pragmatics and social ap-
30–45 min parent interview that assesses a child’s proach.
adaptive functioning in the domains of commu- The Infant/Toddler Social Emotional Assess-
nication, daily living skills, socialization, motor ment (ITSEA; Carter and Briggs-Gowan 2000)
skills, and maladaptive behaviors. The VABS- is a 166-item parent/caregiver scale used to as-
II is used frequently with children suspected of sess developmental strengths and weaknesses in
ASD, intellectual disability, and developmental young children. It may be completed by a parent/
delay. Within the socialization domain, children caregiver or administered as a structured inter-
are assessed in the areas of interpersonal relation- view. The ITSEA comprises four broad domains:
ships, play and leisure time, and coping skills. externalizing, internalizing, dysregulation, and
competence. The competence domain includes
Questionnaires  Questionnaires that assess social the social skills of compliance, attention, imita-
functioning can be very useful in assessing the tion/play, mastery motivation, empathy, and pro-
social domain. The Social Communication Ques- social peer relations. Elevated scores in any sub-
tionnaire (SCQ; Berument et al. 1999; Rutter domain are classified as “Of Concern” and may
et al. 2003a) is a 40-item “yes/no” questionnaire indicate the need for early intervention services.
that can be completed by a parent or caregiver in The Social Skills Improvement System Rat-
about 10 min. The questions directly mirror those ing Scales (SSIS; Gresham and Elliott 2008)
of the ADI-R and provide evidence of social defi- measures social skills, problem behaviors, and
cits as well as challenges in communication and academic competence in children ages 3–18. It
behavior. The SCQ has both Lifetime and Current may be completed by a parent, caregiver, or by
forms which can be used to focus on a child’s the student. The social skills of communication,
developmental history or present functioning, cooperation, assertion, responsibility, empathy,
respectively, in the three core domains of impair- engagement, and self-control are assessed. An
ment in ASD. “Autism Spectrum” subscale was added to the
The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS; Con- newest published version.
stantino 2002) is a 65-item scale that assesses the
severity of symptoms associated with ASD, has Experimental approaches A wide variety of
both parent and teacher report forms, and can be experimental measures have been employed to
completed in about 15 min. Behaviors are divid- quantify the social functioning of children in
ed into the following five subscales: receptive, ASD. A description of all the behavioral, psycho-
cognitive, expressive, and motivational aspects physiological, and imaging paradigms used to
of social behavior, as well as autistic preoccupa- characterize the social challenges noted in ASD
tions. Besides subscale scores, the SRS generates is beyond the scope of this chapter, but a few
a total score indicative of overall social impair- experimental measures will be briefly described
ment. to provide the reader with some insight into the
The PDD Behavior Inventory (PDDBI; Cohen tools available to scientists.
et al. 2003) is a parent/teacher rating scale that Atypical eye gaze is one of the primary fea-
was designed to aid professionals in evaluating tures noted in ASD. Eye-tracking technology has
the treatment progress of children with ASD and elucidated significant differences between the
74 R. Bernier

use of gaze in children with ASD and matched The assessment of empathy has been assessed
controls. Many studies indicate that while typi- experimentally using the response to distress task
cally developing children focus on the eyes of (Sigman et al. 1992). In this paradigm, while
others, children with ASD tend to focus instead seated across from a child, the experimenter pre-
on the mouth, body, or even objects (Klin et al. tends to bang a finger with a toy hammer and
2002). Findings using this technological ap- then proceeds to cry for a short period of time.
proach have indicated the utility of eye-tracking The amount of time the child spends attending
paradigms to assess social impairments in ASD to the crying experimenter is tallied offline by
and suggest that a toddler’s failure to orient to a coders blind to child group status. Children with
caregiver’s gaze is an early disruptor of socializa- ASD have been found to attend less to crying ex-
tion and language acquisition (Jones et al. 2008). perimenters than their typically developing peers
As described above, children with ASD show (Sigman et al. 1992).
reduced attention to information in the social The use of facial expressions in ASD has been
world. Dawson et al. (1998) illustrated this key assessed with the Maximally Discriminative
deficit in a novel experiment in which children Facial Movement Coding System (MAX; Izard
with autism were compared to children with 1979). Results from the use of this assessment in-
Down syndrome and typical development in strument indicated children with ASD were more
their ability to orient towards auditory social neutral in their facial expressions and displayed
stimuli and nonsocial stimuli. Results indicated more ambiguous expressions than comparison
that children with autism were significantly more children (Yirmiya et al. 1989).
impaired than the other children in responding to Several experimental measures have been de-
both types of stimuli, and their lack of response veloped to assess “theory of mind” abilities in
to social stimuli was even more pronounced. In young children with ASD. In the Sally and Anne
this social orienting assessment a child sits across task (Baron-Cohen 1985) the child observes
from an experimenter and is presented with au- a model put an object in one location and then
ditory stimuli. From four locations around the watches the object be moved by another without
room, a second experimenter delivers social the model being aware of the move. The child
(e.g., calling child’s name, clapping hands) and must then identify where the model would look
nonsocial (e.g., car horn honking, kitchen timer) for the object. The Smarties task (Perner et al.
sounds. Each sound is presented for approxi- 1989) calls one’s own experience into the sce-
mately 6 s, at the same decibel level, and once in nario. A child is shown a Smarties box that con-
the child’s left and right visual field and once 30 tains another object and then asked what others
degrees behind the child to the left or right. The would think would be in the box. The Charlie
number of times the child orients to the sound is Test (Baron-Cohen et al. 1995) utilizes a nonver-
summed. bal approach in which a child looks at a picture
Since DeMeyer and colleagues’ first report of of Charlie looking at one of four tasty treats. The
imitation deficits in ASD (DeMeyer et al. 1972), child must infer from Charlie’s gaze which sweet
a number of experimental tools have been devel- Charlie likes the most. Children with ASD show
oped to assess imitation abilities in young chil- impairments in these three theory of mind tasks.
dren with ASD (Smith et al. 2006). Of the ex- Face processing impairments have been de-
perimental measures, the Motor Imitation Scale scribed using electrophysiological paradigms.
(MIS; Stone et al. 1997), a 16-item scale based Electrophysiological studies require only pas-
on Piaget’s developmental sequence, shows good sive viewing, rendering language and behavioral
psychometric properties. Several studies with responses unnecessary, and making these para-
ASD have also utilized the gestural imitation bat- digms appropriate for young children of all func-
tery from Uzgiris and Hunt’s sensorimotor scales tioning levels. The presentation of faces elicits a
(Uzgiris and Hunt 1975). well-described pattern of activation in the brain,
4  Assessment of the Core Features of ASD 75

Table 4.2   Red flags in the Red flags


communication domain No babbling, pointing, or other gesture by 12 months
No single words by 16 months
No two-word spontaneous (non-echolalic) phrases by 24 months
Loss of language or social skills at any age

or event related potential (ERP). The latency tic criteria for the communication impairment
and amplitude of select ERP components, such component of Autistic Disorder requires at least
as the face specific, negative going wave that one of the following: (a) delay in, or total lack
is observed approximately 170 ms after view- of, the development of spoken language (not ac-
ing a face can then be analyzed as a measure of companied by an attempt to compensate through
face processing brain activation. Findings from alternative modes of communication such as
studies employing these paradigms indicate in- gesture or mime); (b) in individuals with ad-
dividuals with ASD show atypical activation to equate speech, marked impairment in the ability
neutral and fearful faces (Dawson et al. 2004; to initiate or sustain a conversation with others;
Webb et al. 2006) and upright and inverted faces (c) stereotyped and repetitive use of language or
(McPartland et al. 2004). idiosyncratic language; and (d) lack of varied,
There are many options available for clini- spontaneous make-believe play or social imita-
cians and scientists to utilize in the assessment tive play appropriate to developmental level.
of social abilities in young children with ASD.
These measures range from standardized ques- Key features in the communication domain 
tionnaires with good psychometric properties to The communication domain encompasses a large
experimental, psychophysiological paradigms variety of speech, language, communication, and
conducted with small samples and limited control play-related deficits. Speech delays or language
groups. Given the heterogeneity in presentation deficits, particularly difficulties with expressive
of children with ASD, broad-based measures as language, are the most common concerns shared
well as assessments focused on specific aspects by parents about children between age 1 and 5
of social cognition are all needed to contribute to years (Filipek et al. 1999). Other common com-
the understanding of the social deficits in ASD. munication concerns that parents report include:
difficulties with sharing needs, not pointing
or using other common gestures such as wav-
Communication Domain ing, and regression in the use of words (Filipek
et al. 1999). Early communication deficits also
Definition A second core domain of ASD is include accompanying behaviors or difficulties
communication. Communication is a broad term with pragmatics such as lack of appropriate gaze
that refers to the giving and receiving of informa- integrated with communication, lack of recipro-
tion through spoken language and sounds, writ- cal (alternating to-and-fro pattern) vocalizations
ten language, gestures, sign language, and body between child and caregiver, lack of or decreased
language (Paul and Wilson 2009). It is important use of gestures to communicate wants and needs,
to consider the different components of commu- and delayed babbling after 9 months of age
nication as the evaluation of communication, lan- (Johnson et al. 2007). Table 4.2 lists several com-
guage, and speech overlap and can involve their munication-based red flags that warrant immedi-
own processes and assessment measures. ate evaluation for possible ASD as identified by
Impairment in communication can range from the American Academy of Neurology and Child
total lack of language, or an absence of an ap- Neurology Society (Filipek et al. 2000b).
parent desire to communicate, to excessive or In addition to the early signs of communication
formal speech with poor reciprocal conversation deficits, there are several other communication,
abilities. The DSM-IV-TR (APA 2000) diagnos- language, or speech difficulties observed in ASD.
76 R. Bernier

For example, some children use spoken language, Assessment of the communication domain  As
but demonstrate atypical use of language, such stressed previously, the evaluation of communi-
as employing more formal or articulated speech, cation skills and deficits should ideally include
echolalia (immediate or delayed repetition of a comprehensive, multidisciplinary assessment
others’ speech), or atypical tonal or rhythm quali- that involves behavioral observations, parent
ties. Some children also have difficulties with report and interview, questionnaires completed
using correct pronouns or may use neologisms by individuals familiar with the child, and stan-
(a made up word) or literal idiosyncratic phrases. dardized instruments to assess abilities. Addi-
Children who have adequate speech may show tionally, if there are red flags within the domain
communication deficits through impairment in of communication, it is especially important for
initiating or sustaining conversations with oth- the child to be evaluated by a speech and lan-
ers or staying on topic of mutual interest (Filipek guage therapist or pathologist and to undergo
et al. 1999; Johnson et al. 2007). an audiological evaluation if indicated, as these
Regression of language, speech, or commu- professions are highly specialized in the assess-
nication is another significant indicator of ASD. ment of hearing, communication, language, and
It is estimated that 25 to 30 % of children with speech. Next is a review of possible tools that can
ASD have exhibited language for some period of be used for the assessment of the communication
time but then stop, typically between 15 and 24 domain of ASDs.
months of age (Tuchma and Rapin 1997; Turner
et al. 2006). Regression can be sudden or gradual Observational methods  Structured behav-
and can be accompanied by other losses such ioral observations and standardized measures
adaptive functioning abilities, loss of communi- of behavior provide specific opportunities for
cative gestures (e.g., pointing), or loss of social children to demonstrate their communication,
skills such as eye contact (Rogers 2004). speech, language, sign and gesture abilities.
Play skills are also captured under the com- More specifically, observations allow clinicians
munication domain of ASD. Some children with to look for the presence or absence of a variety
ASD may play with toys, miniature objects, or of communication skills by creating opportuni-
dolls in a repetitive and mechanical way and ties for conversation, social interaction, play or
demonstrate less flexible use or representation of other scenarios (in clinical or laboratory settings)
objects. Other children may use toys or objects where communication of some sort would typi-
appropriately in a functional manner, but struggle cally be present. There are very few standardized
with engaging in creative and imaginative play observational measures designed specifically for
such as having dolls or action figures interact as the assessment of the communication domain of
agents or pretending that a block is a cup. Very ASD. Below is summary of the most well-known
verbal children may create fantasy worlds where measures to date.
certain topics become the center of their play and As described above in the social domain, the
they struggle to play anything else (Filipek et al. ADOS examines components of communication
1999). in addition to the social and behavioral domains.
To capture the wide variety of possible com- Language and communication use, speech, prag-
munication deficits in ASD, multiple assessment matics, and play are all coded after careful ad-
procedures including observations, parent report, ministration of the instrument. First, the ADOS
questionnaires, standardized language assess- provides opportunity to assess a child’s overall
ments, and experimental methods can be utilized. level of language (i.e., does he/she use mostly
The following is a brief summary of assessment single words, no words, two-three word phrases,
tools that are available to evaluate a child’s overall or phrase speech), the amount of verbal social
communication abilities, specific communication overtures and verbal maintenance of the exam-
and language-related strengths and weaknesses, iner’s attention, and whether echolalia is present
and possible red flags and indicators of ASD. (the immediate or delayed repetition of the last
4  Assessment of the Core Features of ASD 77

statement or series of statements heard). Con- impairments and delays in addition to examining
versation ability is also assessed by focusing on social communication skills. The CSBS has 18
whether the child verbally shares information, subscales that measure various aspects of com-
asks questions, engages in social chat, and how munication including communicative functions,
well he/she is able to build and carry on to-and- use of gestures and vocals, gaze shifts, affect, and
fro conversations. Second, the ADOS allows the reciprocity, as well as four scales that measure
examiner to listen to aspects of speech patterns symbolic development such as constructive and
and abnormalities that are associated with ASD symbolic play. The Developmental Play Assess-
such as flat or exaggerated intonation, little varia- ment Instrument (Lifter 2000) taps into the play
tion in tone or pitch, unusual volume, or a slow or skills component of the communication domain
quick rate of speech. Additionally, the examiner of ASD by investigating a child’s level of pretend
evaluates the presence of highly repetitive utter- play and the frequency of a variety of play activi-
ances with consistent intonation patterns (stereo- ties. The Play Assessment Scale (Fewell 1986) is
typed or idiosyncratic use of words). Third, the a play-based measure that can be administered
ADOS assesses for pragmatic aspects of commu- by a teacher, parent, researcher or another adult
nication. The use of gestures such as pointing, as familiar with the child and the measure. It con-
well as descriptive (holding arms out to indicate sists of two conditions: one involves examining
size), conventional (clapping for “well done”), the child’s spontaneous play with one set of toys,
instrumental, or informational gestures (shrug- while the other entails eliciting “a higher level”
ging, head nodding, or head shaking) are ob- of play behavior in response to verbal prompts
served. Fourth, functional and imaginative/cre- (e.g., will the child offer a fork in response to an
ative play are also assessed through observations “I’m hungry” prompt).
and interactions during the ADOS. The examiner There are also several standardized assess-
looks for whether the child spontaneously plays ment options that offer direct assessment of com-
with a variety of toys, how the child plays with munication and language and can provide valu-
the toys (i.e., uses the toys in a cause-and-effect able information about a child’s current abilities
or functional manner, imitates use of toys, or uses and weaknesses. These measures are typically
figures as agents of action), and how flexible and administered in a clinical or research setting and
creative is the use of toys. Overall, the ADOS is are administered by trained psychologists, school
considered to be a “gold standard” for the obser- psychologists, or speech and language profes-
vational and interactive assessment of ASD and sionals. The following is a brief summary of a
provides an opportunity to obtain a snapshot of a small selection of the many communication and
child’s overall communication skills. language assessment measures that are common-
As described above, The Childhood Autism ly used in the assessment of communicative abil-
Rating Scale, Second Edition (CARS2) provides ities in ASD. The Comprehensive Assessment
observational information regarding ASD. In re- of Spoken Language (CASL; Carrow-Woolfolk
gard to the communication domain the CARS2 1999) is an oral assessment of language for ages
provides a measure of both verbal and nonver- 3 to 21 that measures lexical/semantic language,
bal communication skills including functional syntax, supralingustic abilities, and pragmatics.
speech, echolalia, pronoun reversal, peculiar The Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamen-
words or jargon, and gestures such as pointing. tals—Preschool, 2nd Edition (CELF-P2; Semel
Similarly, as described above, The Early Social et al. 2003) and The Preschool Language Scales,
Communication Scales (ESCS) is a structured 5th Edition (PLS-5; Zimmerman et al. 2011) also
observation-based measure designed to assess assess a broad range of language skills for pre-
nonverbal social communication skills in young school children. The Peabody Picture Vocabu-
children while The Communication and Symbolic lary Test—Fourth Edition (PPVT-4; Dunn and
Behavior Scales (CSBS) uses direct observation Dunn 1997) assesses receptive language skills
and parent interview to assess for communication through the use of a variety of pictures while
78 R. Bernier

its co-normed companion, The Expressive Vo- Questionnaires  There are a number of question-
cabulary Test, Second Edition (EVT-2; Williams naires that address various aspects of the com-
2007) tests expressive vocabulary and word re- munication and language impairments associated
trieval. The Test of Early Language Develop- with ASD (along with the other two domains)
ment—Primary: Third Edition (Newcomer and including the following: The Social Communica-
Hammill 1997) also measures receptive and ex- tion Questionnaire (SCQ), The Social Respon-
pressive language and yields an overall spoken siveness Scale (SRS), The PDD Behavior Inven-
language score. tory (PDD-BI), The Children’s Communication
Checklist—2nd Edition (CCC-2; Bishop 2006),
Interview format Parents are often the first to and The Gilliam Autism Rating Scale (Gilliam
identify problems with speech or language and 1995). The communication component of these
parents are the best resource for information questionnaires inquire about a child’s skills or
about their child’s language milestones, current deficits in the areas of quantity and quality of lan-
abilities, any language loss, as well as social guage and gestures, give-and-take conversations,
communication and pragmatic skills. The ADI-R, the ability to keep up with the flow of conversa-
described above in social assessments, dedicates tions, communication of feelings, being able to
21 items to investigating language and commu- answer questions, and tone of voice. Additionally,
nication skills and deficits. Clinicians using the The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Develop-
ADI-R assess a child’s overall level of language ment Inventories—3rd Edition (MCDI; Fenson
by asking questions about the child’s comprehen- et al. 2007) is a communication-specific ques-
sion of spoken and overall language ability. Items tionnaire that assesses emerging language skills
on the ADI-R address the presence of abnormal by asking parents about what spoken words and
language such as stereotyped and repetitive pat- sentences, as well as gestures, their child uses.
terns of verbal and nonverbal language (e.g.,
neologisms, idiosyncratic language, verbal ritu- Experimental approaches  The examination
als). The ADI-R interviewer also asks parents to of the language and communication deficits
describe their child’s speech and various deficits observed in ASD has largely utilized the variety
that could be present (e.g., articulation/pronun- of standardized questionnaires, observations, and
ciation difficulties, intonation/rate/tone volume interviews available. However, many experimen-
of speech, pronominal reversal). Social aspects tal measures assessing language and communica-
of communication such as the frequency and tion in ASD have been reported in the literature.
quality of social chat, reciprocal conversation, While a review of all the experimental measures
and inappropriate statements are investigated. is beyond the scope of this chapter, we will high-
Clinicians using the ADI-R also gather informa- light some of the experimental measures that
tion about nonverbal aspects of communication. have been described in the literature.
For example, parents are asked whether their At the behavioral level, pragmatic language
child uses another’s body to communicate (e.g., use has been assessed using the pragmatic rating
using another person’s hand to perform some sort scale (Landa et al. 1992) which provides a coding
of task like opening a door). The frequency and system for rating a variety of behaviors based on
quality of gestures such as pointing to express communicative interactions. Although originally
interests, nodding and head shaking, and conven- developed for use with family members of indi-
tional and instrumental gestures (e.g., blowing a viduals with ASD, the instrument has highlighted
kiss, clapping, finger to lips) are also discussed. the impairments in reciprocity and intonation in
As described above, The Vineland Adaptive adolescent children with ASD (Paul et al. 2009).
Behavior Scales, Second Edition (VABS) is a The coding of contingent utterances based on
parent interview that provides information about recorded samples of spontaneous speech has
a child’s adaptive skills. Within the communica- also been utilized to assess pragmatics (Tager-
tion domain, the VABS assesses expressive, re- Flusberg and Anderson 1991). The Profiling Ele-
ceptive, and written language abilities. ments of Prosodic Systems in Children (PEPS-C)
4  Assessment of the Core Features of ASD 79

task has been utilized to assess prosody in autism The DSM-IV-R (APA 2000) diagnostic crite-
(Peppe et al. 2007). The task assesses prosodic ria for the restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped
skills at the basic level of auditory discrimination behaviors, interests, and activities component
and production of prosodic change, and at the of Autistic Disorder requires at least one of the
level of using prosody to communicate through following: (a) encompassing preoccupation with
the child’s responses to questions pertaining to one or more stereotyped and restricted patterns
visual and auditory stimuli. of interest that is abnormal either in its intensity
Electrophysiological and imaging paradigms or focus; (b) apparently inflexible adherence to
have also been developed to assess aspects of specific, nonfunctional routines or rituals; (c) ste-
communication in ASD. Examination of Mis- reotyped and repetitive motor mannerisms (e.g.,
match Negativity (MMN), an event related po- hand or finger flapping or twisting, or complex
tential component that responds to an odd stimu- whole-body movements); (d) persistent preoccu-
lus among a sequence of similar stimuli, has been pation with parts of objects.
used to assess phonological processing abilities.
With this approach, children with ASD have Key features in the restricted/repetitive inter-
failed to show the expected MMN response to ests and behavior domain Restrictive and
changes in syllables suggestive of phonological repetitive behaviors (RRBs) in ASD can range
processing deficits (Kuhl et al. 2005). In an ex- from extreme and obvious to subtle and infre-
amination of word boundary identification, high quent. Although many children with ASD can
functioning boys with ASD and controls listened appear physically normal, many exhibit odd
to two artificial languages which contained either repetitive movements that distinguish them
statistical cues or statistical and prosodic cues to from other children (NIMH 2008). One com-
indicate word boundaries. The boys with autism mon restrictive and repetitive behavior (RRB)
did not show the expected reduction in fronto- in ASD is an encompassing preoccupation with
temporal-parietal circuit activation with the in- a stereotypic or restricted pattern of interest that
crease in word boundary cues nor did they show is abnormal in its intensity or focus. Restrictive
the learning related neural activation increases to interests involve topics, items, or hobbies that
the languages over time (Scott-Van Zeeland et al. a child might be particularly enamored with or
2010). The wide range of experimental assess- have intense interest in learning about such as
ments utilized in studies of language in ASD mir- mechanical (e.g., fixing tires or vacuum cleaners)
rors the breadth of experimental tools available or cognitive themes (e.g., train schedules, dino-
for assessment of the social and behavioral do- saurs, video games). Some children may ask the
mains. However, relative to these two domains, same question repeatedly or share information
there have been fewer standardized assessment about their interests regardless of the interests of
instruments developed to assess the behavioral or responses given by others.
domain in young children with ASD. Preoccupations with parts of objects and sen-
sory interests also fall under this domain. Exam-
ples include chewing on chords or strings, lining
Repetitive and Restrictive Interests up toys in a particular way or pattern, spinning
and Behaviors Domain wheels on toys, or watching ceiling fans spin
around and around. Some children may collect
Definition  The third core domain of ASD con- things or objects for no particular purpose. Others
cerns restrictive interests and repetitive or stereo- may engage in repetitive actions such as opening
typic behaviors. Repetitive and restrictive inter- or closing doors or turning light switches on and
ests and behaviors encompass qualitative deficits off. Repetitive touching, sniffing, or mouthing of
in a variety of behaviors such as repetitive or ste- objects may also occur.
reotyped movements, inflexible routines, intense Another component of the RRB category is
interests, or preoccupation with parts of objects. inflexible adherence to certain nonfunctional
80 R. Bernier

routines or rituals. Inflexibility and rigidity may example, Lord et al. (2006) revealed that RRB
include adhering to certain routines in home and domain scores at age 2 were predictive of ASD
school environments. Many children are preoc- at age 9. In a sample of 2-year-olds, Richler
cupied with sameness and keeping their every- et al. (2007) found children with ASD had higher
day routine consistent with little change. Some rates of repetitive sensory motor (RSM) behav-
children engage in mealtime, dressing, or bed- iors (approximately three) than children with
time rituals that are abnormal in their intensity. developmental delays and typically developing
Significant resistance to change is also common children (about one or less). These researchers
for some children with ASD. Tantrums or refus- proposed having one RSM behavior may not be
als to do an activity are not uncommon reactions indicative of ASD, but having several or severe
to transitions or changes in routines. RSM might be suggestive of ASD. Additional
Stereotyped and repetitive motor mannerisms studies are needed to identify and evaluate RRBs
or whole complex body movements are also seen in young children. Second, RRBs may not always
in some, but not all children with ASD (Filipek occur every day or be observable in short clinical
et al. 1999). Odd repetitive motions otherwise or research sessions. Third, RRBs are varied, on
known as stereotypies may include arm flapping, a continuum from mild to severe, and may not
hand clapping, or finger flicking. Some children manifest in the same way for each child, mak-
may spin in circles, rock back and forth, run aim- ing RRBs particularly difficult to assess. Addi-
lessly, or walk on their toes. tionally, RRBs are often not stable over time and
Additionally, research has been devoted to in- may change in type or frequency (Militerni et al.
vestigating the function of RRBs, which suggests 2002). Therefore, if possible, it is imperative that
RRBs may have a variety of purposes including a variety of assessments are utilized, that obser-
sensory stimulation, perceptual reinforcement, vations are made in multiple contexts, and that
situation avoidance, and attention seeking (Ken- information is obtained from many sources. The
nedy et al. 2000; Lovaas et al. 1987). following is a summary of observation-based,
interview, questionnaire, and experimental mea-
Assessment of repetitive and restrictive inter- sures used for the assessment of RRBs associated
ests and behaviors Although the assessment with ASD.
of the social and communication domains of
ASD is challenging, the assessment of repetitive Observation  The ADOS allows for the observa-
and restrictive behaviors can be more compli- tion of RRBs during the variety of structured and
cated for a number of reasons. First, repetitive play-based activities. Possible RRBs that can be
and restricted behaviors are often common in a evaluated if they occur include repetitive hand,
variety of disorders other than ASD, including, finger, and other complex mannerisms; self-
obsessive compulsive disorder, Tourette’s syn- injurious behavior; repetitive interests and ste-
drome, and various mood and anxiety disorders reotyped behaviors, and unusual sensory inter-
(Bodfish et al. 2000; Cuccaro et al. 2003; Lewis ests in play materials or persons. However, it is
and Bodfish 1998; Mahone et al. 2004). Although important to note there are no specific “presses”
RRBs are common in many disorders, research (created opportunities) specifically for repeti-
suggests RRBs may occur more frequently and tive or restricted behaviors, thus they cannot
may be more severe in some individuals with be reliably assessed (Ozonoff et al. 2005). Sub-
ASD (Bodfish et al. 2000; Carcani-Rathwell sequently, additional observations and reports
et al. 2006; Osterling et al. 2002). Although many from multiple sources may be needed in order
studies indicate in general, social and communi- to confirm or rule out the possibility of these
cation impairments are more common than RRBs behaviors. Repetitive and restricted behaviors
in very young children with ASD, there is emerg- may be observed through informal behavioral
ing evidence that some RRBs are evident in chil- observations throughout the evaluation process
dren as young as age 2 (Richler et al. 2007). For and should be noted.
4  Assessment of the Core Features of ASD 81

The Repetitive and Restricted Behaviour other complex body mannerisms such as repeti-
Scale (RBS; Bodfish et al. 1999) and The Re- tively spinning in circles is collected.
petitive and Restricted Behaviour Scale-Revised
(RBS-R; Bodfish et al. 2000; Lams and Aman Questionnaires  There are few published ques-
2007) purport to be the only known tools specifi- tionnaires that focus solely on the assessment
cally designed for the assessment of RRBs. The of repetitive and restricted behaviors in ASD.
current version of the RBS-R (Bourreau et al. The Aberrant Behavior Checklist (ABC; Aman
2009) employs a descriptive approach that entails and Singh 1986) contains a stereotypic behav-
a professional providing behavioral ratings after ior subscale that inquires about the presence of
the observation of the child in multiple contexts. repetitive hand, body or head movements, stereo-
This version has 35 items which are evaluated on typed repetitive movements, and odd and bizarre
a 5-point Likert scale. If the rater is not able to behaviors; however, it does not have a normative
obtain enough information through observations, sample for children under 6 years of age. The
supplemental information may be obtained from Sensory Profile (Dunn 1999) assesses how well
a family member who knows the child well. Par- children aged 3 to 10 process sensory informa-
ents often fill out this scale as a questionnaire as tion in everyday situations and to what extent
well. sensory factors impact functioning.
Other measures focus on all three domains
Interview  Given that RRBs are not always characteristic of ASD including repetitive and
present or readily observable during clinical or restricted behaviors. Some of the more common
research assessments, obtaining information and questionnaires that provide information about
descriptions about these behaviors through par- the type, frequency, or severity of repetitive and
ent report is essential. As with social and com- restricted behaviors include: The Gilliam Au-
munication skill impairments, ADI-R is used tism Rating Scale which inquires about stereo-
to gather information about the third domain of typed behaviors; The PDD Behavior Inventory
ASD through parent interview which includes 13 (PDDBI) which provides parent and teacher rat-
questions that are grouped in the “Interests and ings of ritualistic/repetitive activities and resis-
Behaviors” category. RRBs referenced include tance to change; and The Social Responsiveness
unusual preoccupations (interests that are odd Scale (SRS) which devotes questions to the pres-
or peculiar in quality); unusual attachments to ence of hand and complex body mannerisms, as
objects; circumscribed interests that are unusual well as the presence of rigidity, inflexibility, dif-
in intensity, circumscribed nature, nonsocial qual- ficulties with coordination, and unusual sensory
ity, and lack of progression over time; repetitive interests.
use of objects or interests in parts of objects (e.g.,
shaking strings, turning wheels and dials, open- Experimental approaches  The inconsistent, spo-
ing and closing toy car doors); and compulsions radic, and heterogeneous presentation of RRBs
or rituals (e.g., turning all lights off). The “Inter- has made the assessment of behaviors in this
ests and Behaviors” category also includes ques- domain challenging. While questionnaires, rating
tions about unusual sensory interests, sensitivity scales, and interviews have been widely used,
to everyday noises, and abnormal and predictable experimental measures have also been developed
responses to specific sensory stimuli. Difficulties to assess behaviors in this domain. The Interests
with changes in a child’s routine or environment, Scale is a parent rating form that assesses the
including resistance to minor changes in the envi- intensity, degree of interference, frequency of
ronment (e.g., furniture moved, someone wears involvement, and involvement of others in inter-
a hat who typically does not) are also captured. ests for 39 typical childhood interests using mul-
Finally, information about the presence of rapid, tiple choice and open-ended questions (Turner-
voluntary, and repetitive hand and finger manner- Brown et al. 2011). The Interview for Repetitive
isms (e.g., twisting fingers, hand flapping) and Behavior is a structured clinical interview to
82 R. Bernier

assess forms of repetitive behavior that are spe- Functional and structural imaging paradigms
cific to a given individual based on items that have provided significant insight into the neuro-
are endorsed on the RBS-R (Turner-Brown et al. logical contributions of the observed behaviors
2011). A number of behavioral coding systems in ASD. These technologies will, no doubt, con-
have been used to quantify and assess repetitive tinue to enhance our understanding and contrib-
behaviors in ASD (Gardenier et al. 2004; Symons ute assessment paradigms to evaluate the skills
et al. 2005). Goldman et al. (2009) coded the and deficits in the core domains of ASD in young
movements of children with ASD during 15-min children.
videotaped play sessions. All movements were
assigned to one of eight categories, but duration
and amplitude of movements were unable to be References
scored because of challenges in establishing reli-
ability concerning the start, end and intensity of Aman, M. G., & Singh, N. N. (1986). Aberrant behavior
checklist manual. East Aurora: Slosson Educational
the movements. Although the majority of experi- Publications.
mental measures have consisted of behavioral American Academy of Pediatrics, Council on Children
coding schemes, advances in motion capture with Disabilities, Section on Developmental and
technology suggest new possibilities on the near Behavioral Pediatrics, Bright Futures Screening Com-
mittee, Medical Home Initiatives for Children with
horizon for methods to capture repetitive and Special Needs Project Advisory Committee. (2006).
restricted behaviors and interests in ASD. Identifying infants and young children with develop-
mental disorders in the medical home: An algorithm
for developmental surveillance and screening. Pediat-
rics, 118, 405–420.
Future Directions American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.).
There have been vast improvements in the assess- Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.
ment of ASD since Leo Kanner’s first descrip- American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.,
tion of 11 individuals with autism and we can text rev). Washington DC: American Psychiatric Asso-
anticipate continued growth in our assessment ciation.
and measurement of ASD. The measures used to American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic
assess the core features of ASD will continue to and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).
Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association.
be refined and new measures will be developed American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic
just as our conceptualization and characterization and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.,
of the core domains of impairment become more text rev.). Washington DC: American Psychiatric
refined. A perfect example of this refinement is Association.
Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Perceptual role taking and pro-
the movement away from conceptualizing ASD todeclarative pointing in autism. British Journal of
as a disorder marked by impairments in three Developmental Psychology, 7, 113–127.
domains to a disorder defined by impairments in Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the
two domains: social communication and restrict- autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21,
37–46.
ed and repetitive interests and behaviors. As the Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A.,
focus of ASD transitions away from social and Grant, J., & Walker, J. (1995). Are children with
language deficits to social communication defi- autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the
cits, tools to assess the social use of language will eyes? British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
13, 379–398.
continue to expand. Baron-Cohen, S., Allen, J., & Gillberg, C. (1992). Can
Additionally, as technologies become more autism be detected at 18 months? The needle, the hay-
widely available so will their use. Electrophysi- stack, and the CHAT. British Journal of Psychiatry,
ology and eye-tracking approaches have helped 161, 839–843.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Lawson, J., Griffin,
to contribute to our understanding of the core R., Ashwin, C., et al. (2005). Empathizing and sys-
impairments observed in ASD and increasingly temizing in autism spectrum conditions. In F. R. Volk-
more common in studies with young children. mar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.) Handbook of
4  Assessment of the Core Features of ASD 83

autism and pervasive developmental disorders: Vol. 1. autism diagnostic interview-R. Child Psychiatry and
Diagnosis, development, neurobiology, and behavior Human Development, 34, 3–17.
(pp. 628–639). Hoboken: Wiley. Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J. &
Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bai- Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient
ley, A. (1999). Autism screening questionnaire: Diag- to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism
nostic validity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, and Developmental Disorders, 28, 479–485.
444–451. Dawson, G., Webb, S., Carver, L., Panagiotides, H. &
Bishop, D.V.M. (2006). Children’s Communication McPartland, J. (2004). Young children with autism
Checklist-2, U.S. Edition. Bloomington: Pearson. show atypical brain responses to fearful versus neu-
Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F. J., & Lewis, M. H. (1999). The tral facial expressions of emotion. Developmental Sci-
Repetitive Behavior Scales (RBS). Western Carolina ence, 7, 340–59.
Center Research Reports. Dawson, G., Webb, S., & McPartland, J. (2005). Under-
Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F. J., Parker, D. E., & Lewis, M. standing the nature of face processing impairment in
H. (2000). Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: autism: insights from behavioral and electrophysi-
Comparisons to mental retardation. Journal of Autism ological studies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27,
and Developmental Disorders, 30, 237–243. 403–24.
Bourreau, Y., Roux, S., Gomot, M., Bonnet-Brilhault, F., DeMeyer, M., Alpern, G., Barons, S., DeMyer, W.,
& Barthélémy, C. (2009). Validation of the repetitive Churchill, D., Hingtgen, J., Bryson, C., Pontius, W.
and restricted behaviour scale in autism spectrum dis- & Kimberlin, C. (1972). Imitation in autistic, early
orders. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, schizophrenic, and non-psychotic subnormal chil-
18, 675–682. dren. Journal of Autism and Child Schizophrenia, 2,
Bricker, D., & Squires, J. (1994). Ages and stages ques- 264–287.
tionnaire. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Dunn, W. (1999). The sensory profile manual. San Anto-
Bricker, D., & Squires, J. (1999). The ages and stages nio: The Psychological Corporation.
questionnaires (2nd ed.). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary
Brigance, A. (1986). The BRIGANCE® screens. North test—Third edition. Circle Pines: American Guidance
Billerica: Curriculum Associates. Service.
Carcani-Rathwell, I., Rabe-Hesketh, S., & Santosh, P. Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S.,
(2006). Repetitive and stereotyped behaviours in per- Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates
vasive developmental disorders. The Journal of Child communicative developmental inventories users guide
Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 47, and technical manual (2nd Ed.). Baltimore: Brookes.
573–581. Fewell, R. R. (1986). Play assessment scale (5th revi-
Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1999). Comprehensive assessment sion). Seattle: University of Washington.
of spoken language. Circle Pines: American Guidance Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Baranek, G. T., Cook, E.
Service. H., Jr., Dawson, G., Gordon, B., et al. (1999). The
Carter, A. S., Davis, N. O., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. R. screening and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders.
(2005).Social development in autism. In F. R. Volk- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29,
mar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook 439–484.
of autism and pervasive developmental disorders: Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Ashwal, S., Baranek, G. T.,
Vol. 1. Diagnosis, development, neurobiology, and Cook E. H., Jr., Dawson, G., et al. (2000a). Practice
behavior (pp. 312–334). Hoboken: Wiley. parameter: Screening and diagnosis of autism. Neurol-
Chawarska, K., & Volkmar, F. R. (2005). Autism in ogy, 55, 468–479.
infancy and early childhood. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Ashwal, S., Baranek, G. T.,
A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and Cook E. H., Jr., Dawson, G., et al. (2000b). Practice
pervasive developmental disorders: Vol. 1. Diagnosis, parameter: Screening and diagnosis of autism: Report
development, neurobiology, and behavior (pp. 223– of the quality standards subcommittee of the Ameri-
246). Hoboken, NJ: John Willey & Sons. can Academy of Neurology and the Child Neurology
Cohen, M. (1997). Children’s memory scale. San Antonio: Society. Neurology, 55, 468–479.
The Psychological Corporation. Frankenburg, W. K. (1986). Revised Denver pre-screen-
Cohen, I. L., Schmidt-Lackner, S., Romanczyk, R., & ing developmental questionnaire. Denver: Denver
Sudhalter, V. (2003). The PDD behavior inventory: Developmental Materials.
A rating scale for assessing response to intervention Frankenburg, W. K., Dodds, J., Archer, P., Shapiro, H., &
in children with pervasive developmental disorder. Bresnick, B. (1992). The Denver II: A major revision
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, and restandardization of the Denver Developmental
31–45. Screening Test. Pediatrics, 89, 91–97.
Constantino, J. N. (2002). The social responsiveness scale. Gardenier, N., MacDonald, R., & Green, G. (2004). Com-
Los Angeles: Western Psychological Association. parison of direct observational methods for measuring
Cuccaro, M. L., Shao, Y., Grubber, J., Slifer, M., Wolpert, stereotypic behavior in children with autism spectrum
C. M., Donnelly, S. L., et al. (2003). Factor analysis of disorders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 25,
restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism using the 99–118.
84 R. Bernier

Gilliam, J. E. (1995). Gilliam autism rating scale second als with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism
edition (GARS-2). Austin: Pro-Ed. and Developmental Disorders, 37, 855–866.
Glascoe, F. P. (1996). A validation study and the psycho- Landa, R., Piven, J., Wzorek, M., Gayle, J., Chase, G.,
metric properties of the BRIGANCE® screens. North & Folstein, S. (1992). Social language use in parents
Billercia: Curriculum Associate. of autistic individuals. Psychological Medicine, 22,
Glascoe, F. P. (1998). Collaborating with parents: Using 245–54.
parents’ evaluation of developmental status to detect Lewis, M. H., & Bodfish, J. W. (1998). Repetitive behav-
and address developmental and behavioral problems. ior disorders in autism. Mental Retardation and Devel-
Nashville: Ellsworth & Vandermeer. opmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 4, 80–89.
Goldman, S., Wang, C., Salgado, M., Greene, P., Kim, M. Lifter, K. (2000). Linking assessment to intervention for
& Rapin, I. (2009). Motor stereotypies in children with children with developmental disabilities or at-risk for
Autism and other developmental disorders. Develop- developmental delay: The DPA. In K. Gitlin-Weiner,
mental Medicine and Child Neurology, 51, 30–38. A. Sandgrund, & C. E. Schaefer (Eds.), Play diagnosis
Gresham, F., & Elliott, S. (2008). Social skills improve- and assessment (2nd ed., pp. 228–261). NY: Wiley
ment system: Rating scales manual. Minneapolis, Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism
MN: Pearson Assessments. diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a
Ireton, H. (1992). Child development inventories. Minne- diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with
apolis: Behavior Science Systems. possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal
Ireton, H., & Glascoe, F. P. (1995). Assessing children’s of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.
development using parents’ reports. The child devel- Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., & Risi, S. (1999).
opment inventory. Clinical Pediatrics, 34, 248–255. Autism diagnostic observation schedule—WPS
Izard, C. (1979). The maximally discriminative facial (ADOS-WPS). Los Angeles: Western Psychological
movement coding system. Newark: Institutional Services.
Resource Center, University of Delaware. Lord, C., Risi, S., DiLavore, P., Schulman, C., Thurm,
Johnson, C. P., & Blasco, P. A. (1997). Infant growth and A., & Pickles, A. (2006). Autism from two to nine.
development. Pediatrics in Review, 18, 224–242. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 694–701.
Johnson, C. P., Myers, S. M., & American Academy of Lord, C., Petkova, E., Hus, V., Gan, W., Lu, F., Martin,
Pediatrics, Council on Children with Disabilities. D. M., Ousley, O., Guy, L., Bernier, R., Gerdts, J.,
(2007). Identification and evaluation of children with Algermissen, M., Whitaker, A., Sutcliffe, J. S., War-
autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 120, 1183–1215. ren, Z., Klin, A., Saulnier, C., Hanson, E., Hundley,
Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of pref- R., Piggot, J., Fombonne, E., Steiman, M., Miles, J.,
erential looking to the eyes of approaching adults pre- Kanne, S. M., Goin-Kochel, R. P., Peters, S.U., Cook,
dicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers E. H., Guter, S., Tjernagel, J., Green-Snyder, L. A.,
with autism spectrum disorder. Archives of General Bishop, S., Esler, A., Gotham, K., Luyster, R., Miller,
Psychiatry, 65(8), 946–954. F., Olson, J., Richler, J., Risi, S. (2011). A multisite
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective con- study of the clinical diagnosis of different autism spec-
tact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250. trum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, Nov 7.
Kennedy, C. H., Meyer, K. A., Knowles, T., & Shukla, [Epub ahead of print].
S. (2000). Analyzing the multiple functions of stereo- Lovaas, O. I., Newsom, C., & Hickman, C. (1987). Self-
typical behavior for students with autism: Implica- stimulatory behavior and perceptual reinforcement.
tions for assessment and treatment. Journal of Applied Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 45–68.
Behavior Analysis, 33, 559–571. Luyster, R., Gotham, K., Guthrie, W., Coffing, M., Petrak,
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, R., Pierce, K., Bishop, S., Esler, A., Hus, V., Oti, R.,
D. (2002).Visual fixation patterns during viewing of Richler, J., Risi, S., & Lord, C. (2009). The autism
naturalistic social situations as predictors of social diagnostic observations schedule-toddler module.
competence in individuals with autism. Archives of Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809–816. 39 (9), 1305–1320.
Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. Mahone, E. M., Bridges, D., Prahme, C., & Singer, H. S.
(2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to nonsocial (2004). Repetitive arm and hand movements (complex
contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature, motor stereotypies) in children. Journal of Pediatrics,
459(7244), 257–261. 145, 391–395.
Korkman, M., Kirk, U. & Kemp, S. (2007). NEPSY—Second McPartland, J., Dawson, G., Webb, S., Panagiotides, H.,
Edition. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation. & Carver, L. (2004). Event-related brain potentials
Kuhl, P., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., & Dawson, G. reveal anomalies in temporal processing of faces in
(2005). Links between social and linguistic processing autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychol-
of speech in preschool children with autism: Behav- ogy and Psychiatry, 45, 1235–45.
ioral and electrophysiological measures. Developmen- Militerni, R., Bravaccio, C., Falco, C., Fico, C., & Pal-
tal Science, 8, F1–F12. ermo, M. T. (2002). Repetitive behaviors in autistic
Lams, K. S., & Aman, M. G. (2007). The repetitive behav- disorder. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
ior scale revised: Independent validation in individu- 11, 210–218.
4  Assessment of the Core Features of ASD 85

Mundy, P., & Burnette, C. (2005). Joint attention and neu- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31,
rodevelopmental models of autism. In F. R. Volkmar, 131–144.
R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of Rogers, S. (2004). Developmental regression in autism
autism and pervasive developmental disorders: Vol. 1. spectrum disorders. Mental Retardation and Devel-
Diagnosis, development, neurobiology, and behavior opmental Disabilities Research Review, 10, 139–143.
(pp. 650–681). Hoboken: Wiley. Rogers, S. J., Cook, I., & Meryl, A. (2005). Imitation and
Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Venezia, M., Hogan A., play in autism. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, &
& Siebert, J. (2003). A manual for the abridged early D. Cohen (Eds.) Handbook of autism and pervasive
social communication scales (ESCS). Coral Gables: developmental disorders: Vol. 1. Diagnosis, devel-
University of Miami. opment, neurobiology, and behavior (pp. 382–405).
National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), & National Hoboken: Wiley.
Institutes of Health (NIH) Publication No. 08–5511. Rutter, M., & Schopler, E. (1987). Autism and pervasive
(2008). Autism spectrum disorders: Pervasive devel- developmental disorders: Concepts and diagnostic
opmental disorders. http://www.nimh.nih.gov/ issues. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disor-
health/publications/autism/nimhautismspectrum.pdf. ders, 17(2), 159–186.
Accessed 15 Oct 2012. Rutter, M., & Lockyer, L. (1967). A five to fifteen year
Newcomer, P. L., & Hammill, D. D. (1997) Test of lan- follow-up study of infantile psychosis: I. description
guage development—primary: Third edition (TOLD- of sample. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 113,
P:4). Austin: PRO–ED. 1169–1182.
Osterling, J. A., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. A. (2002). Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Lord, C., & Berument, S. K.
Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism (2003a). Social communication questionnaire. Los
spectrum disorder versus mental retardation. Develop- Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
mental Psychology, 14, 239–251. Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1988).Child-
Ozonoff, S., Goodlin-Jones, B. L., & Solomon, M. (2005). hood autism rating scale (CARS). Los Angeles: West-
Evidence-based assessment of autism spectrum disor- ern Psychological Services.
ders in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Schopler, E., Van Bourgondien, M. E., Wellman, G. J.,
Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 523–540. & Love, S. R. (2010). Childhood autism rating scale,
Parks, S.L. (1988). Psychometric instruments available second edition (CARS2). Los Angeles: Western Psy-
for the assessment of autistic children. In E. Schopler chological Services.
& G. Mesibov (Eds.), Diagnosis and assessment in Schultz, R. T. (2005). Developmental deficits in social
autism (pp. 123–136). New York: Plenum. perception in autism: The role of the amygdala and
Paul, R., & Wilson, K. P. (2009). Assessing speech, lan- fusiform face area. International Journal of Develop-
guage, and communication in autism spectrum dis- mental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 125–141.
orders. In S. Goldstein, J. A. Naglieri, & S. Ozonoff Scott-Van Zeeland, A., McNealy, K., Wang, A., Sigman,
(Eds.), Assessment of autism spectrum disorders M., Bookheimer, S., & Dapretto, M. (2010). No neu-
(pp. 171–208). New York: The Guilford. ral evidence of statistical learning during exposure to
Paul, R., Orlovski, S., Marcinko, H., & Volkmar, F. artificial languages in children with autism spectrum
(2009). Conversational behaviors in youth with high- disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 68, 345–51.
functioning ASD and asperger syndrome. Journal of Semel, E., Wiig, E. & Secord, W. (2003). The clinical
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 115–25. evaluation of language fundamentals—Preschool (2nd
Peppé, S., McCann, J., Gibbon, F., O’Hare, A., & Ruther- edition (CELF-P2). San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment.
ford, M. (2007). Receptive and expressive prosodic abil- Siegel, B. (2004). The pervasive developmental disorders
ity in children with high-functioning autism. Journal of screening test II (PDDST-II). San Antonio: Harcourt
Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 50, 1015–28. Assessment.
Perner, J., Frith, U., Leslie, A., & Leekam, S. (1989). Sigman, M., Kasari, C., Kwon, J., & Yirmiya, N. (1992).
Exploration of the autistic child’s theory of mind: Responses to the negative emotions of others by autis-
Knowledge, belief, and communication. Child Devel- tic, mentally retarded, and normal children. Child
opment, 60, 689–700. Development, 63, 796–807.
Pierce, K., Conant, D., Hazin, R., Stoner, R., & Desmond, Smith, I., Lowe-Pearce, C., & Nichols, S. (2006). Assess-
J. (2011). Preference for geometric patterns early in ment of imitation abilities in autism: Conceptual and
life as a risk factor for autism. Archives of General methodological issues. In S. Rogers & J. Williams
Psychiatry, 68, 101–109. (Eds). Imitation and the social mind (pp. 377–398).
Richler, J., Bishop, S. L., Kleinke, J. R., & Lord, C. New York: Guilford.
(2007). Restricted and repetitive behaviors in young Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Balla, D. (2005) Vineland
children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of adaptive behavioral scales (2nd Edition (Vineland-
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 73–85. II)). Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.
Robins, D., Fein, D., Barton, M., & Green, J. A. (2001). Squires, J., Bricker, D., & Potter, L. (1997). Revision of a
The modified checklist for Autism in Toddlers parent-completed developmental screening tool: Ages
(M-CHAT): An initial investigation in the early detec- and Stages Questionnaires. Journal of Pediatric Psy-
tion of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. chology, 22, 313–328.
86 R. Bernier

Stone, W., Ousley, O., & Littleford, C. (1997). Motor Volkmar, F. R., Cook, E. H. J., Pomeroy, J., Realmuto, G.,
imitation in young children with autism: What’s the & Tanguay, P. (1999). Practice parameters for autism
object? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of the
475–485. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychia-
Symons, F., Sperry, L., Dropik, P., & Bodfish, J. (2005). try, 38, 32s–54s.
The early development of stereotypy and self-injury: Webb, S., Dawson, G., Bernier, R., & Panagiotides, H.
a review of research methods. Journal of Intellectual (2006). ERP evidence of atypical face processing in
Disabilities Research, 49, 144–58. young children with autism. Journal of Autism and
Tager-Flusberg, H, & Andersons, M. (1991). The devel- Developmental Disorders, 36, 881–890.
opment of contingent discourse ability in autistic chil- Wetherby, A., & Prizant, B. (2002). Communication
dren. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, and symbolic behavior scales developmental profile-
1123–1134. first normed edition (CSBS DP).Baltimore: Paul H.
Travis, L. L., & Sigman, M. (1998). Social deficits and Brookes.
interpersonal relationships in autism. Mental Retar- Williams, K. (2007). Expressive vocabulary test (2nd Edi-
dation and Developmental Disabilities Research tion (EVT-2)). Circle Pines: AGS.
Reviews, 4, 65–72. Wing, L. (2005). Problems of categorical classifica-
Tuchman, R. F., & Rapin, I. (1997). Regression in per- tion systems. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, &
vasive developmental disorders: Seizures and epilep- D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive
tiform electroencephalogram correlatives. Pediatrics, developmental disorders: Vol. 1. Diagnosis, devel-
99, 560–566. opment, neurobiology, and behavior (pp. 583–605).
Turner, L. M., Stone, W. L., Pozdol, S. L., & Coonrod, E. Hoboken: Wiley.
E. (2006). Follow-up of children with autism spectrum Yirmiya, N., Kasari, C., Sigman, M., & Mundy, P. (1989).
disorders from age 2 to age 9. Autism, 10, 243–265. Facial expressions of affect in autistic, mentally
Turner-Brown, L., Lam, K., Holtzclaw, T., Dichter, G., & retarded and normal children. Journal of Child Psy-
Bodfish, J. (2011). Phenomenology and measurement chology and Psychiatry, 30, 725–735.
of circumscribed interests in autism spectrum disor- Zimmerman, I., Steiner, V., & Pond, R. (2011). Preschool
ders. Autism, 15, 437–456. language scales (5th Edition (PLS-5)). San Antonio:
Uzgiris, I., & Hunt, J. (1975). Assessment in infancy: The Psychological Corporation.
Ordinal scales of psychological development. Urbana: Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Lord, C., Rogers, S.,
University of Illinois Press. Carter, A., Carver, L., et al. (2009). Clinical assess-
Volkmar, F. R., & Klin, A. (2005).Issues in the classifica- ment and management of toddlers with suspected
tion of autism and related conditions. In F. R. Volkmar, autism spectrum disorder: Insights from studies of
R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of high-risk infants. Pediatrics, 123, 1383–1391.
autism and pervasive developmental disorders: Vol. 1.
Diagnosis, development, neurobiology, and behavior
(pp. 5–41). Hoboken: Wiley.

You might also like